Journal of Social Issues, Vol. 71, No. 1, 2015, pp. 123--138 doi: 10.1111/josi.12100 Using Celebrity News Stories to Effectively Reduce Racial/Ethnic Prejudice Srividya Ramasubramanian ∗ Texas A&M University This article argues that exposure to admirable media celebrities from racial/ethnic outgroups is an effective, proactive, and viable strategy for prejudice reduction and intergroup harmony. It uses mediated contact and exemplification theories to demonstrate that reading news stories about likable outgroup media personalities who serve as counter-stereotypic exemplars can subtly modify racial attitudes, which are malleable and context-sensitive. Specifically, results from a between-participants experiment (N = 88) show that exposure to news stories about counter-stereotypic African American media personalities as compared to stereotypical ones reduces stereotypical perceptions and symbolic racist beliefs of White Americans about African Americans. Furthermore, these favorable attitudes translate into an increased willingness to support affirmative action policies. Intergroup conflict, which stems from prejudice and discrimination, is a major global issue today. It is the root cause for genocide, ethnic cleansing, and terrorism whose costs are staggering in terms of deaths, illnesses, and destruction. Even after these conflicts are seemingly resolved, their impact on societies can be long-lasting. In the United States, for instance, interracial tensions and discrimination continue to exist even 40 or more years after the civil rights movement. Mistrust, racial discord, and anti-Black prejudice are still prevalent in so-called color-blind, postracial America. Recent controversies such as the Trayvon Martin murder, the racial profiling of Prof. Henry Louis Gates Jr., the Jena Six case, and the shooting of Reshida McBride show that racial/ethnic enmity remains a deep-seated problem in the United States. Under these circumstances, ∗ Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Srividya Ramasubramanian, Department of Communication, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX 77843-4234. Tel: (979) 845-5178 [e-mail: srivi@tamu.edu.]. I sincerely thank Amanda Martinez, Robin Bedenbaugh, and Jonathan Chacon for their assistance with data collection. I am grateful to Karen Dill-Shackleford for feedback on an earlier draft of this article. 123 C 2015 The Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues 124 Ramasubramanian the promotion of intergroup harmony through reduction of symbolic racist beliefs and increased support for public policies that encourage racial equality remains an important goal for scholars, social activists, and educators. Media scholars studying intergroup processes are committed to exploring the role of mass media, which offer a rich range of salient vivid exemplars, in promoting positive intergroup relations. Media-based positive contact with well-liked members of outgroups offers a less intrusive and anxiety-ridden approach than direct, face-to-face interracial contact. Using mediated contact and exemplification theories, it would be expected that because racial/ethnic attitudes are malleable and context-sensitive, exposure to news stories about positive media celebrities from racial/ethnic outgroups can serve as counter-stereotypic exemplars that activate favorable attitudes toward outgroups. Specifically, the findings from the present between-participants experiment demonstrate that exposure to news stories about counter-stereotypical, admirable African American media celebrities (compared to negative, stereotypical presentations) reduces stereotypical perceptions and symbolic racist beliefs about African Americans, among White American participants. Importantly these favorable attitudes further translate into an increased willingness to support affirmative action policies that provide greater opportunities for racial/ethnic outgroups to access societal resources. This study contributes and builds on existing scholarship on media’s role in racial/ethnic stereotyping processes in several ways. Whereas the bulk of existing literature examines how media portrayals influence the formation and maintenance of cultural stereotypes, the current research contributes to the important but sparse literature on media-based strategies for prejudice reduction. Moreover, it extends intergroup contact theory by applying it to media contexts by suggesting that positive vicarious contact with outgroup members via media can help encourage favorable outgroup attitudes. Additionally, this study expands the scope of the scholarship on mediated counter-stereotypic exemplars by examining their effects on subtle symbolic racism and affirmative action policy opinions. Finally, it provides rare experiment-based evidence that links exposure to media exemplars with viewers’ policy opinions. Media Priming and Racial/Ethnic Attitudes Content analyses of news media document that African Americans have been historically depicted in negative ways such as dangerous, lazy, unintelligent (Dixon & Linz, 2000; Entman & Rojecki, 2000), hypersexual and relatively low social and professional status (Tukachinsky, Mastro & Yarchi, 2015). Scholars have studied the priming effects of these stereotypical news portrayals on audiences’ person perceptions, social judgments, endorsement of explicit stereotypes (e.g., Gilliam & Iyengar, 2000; Hurley, Jensen, Weaver, & Dixon, 2015; Oliver & Fonash, 2002). However, the current media scenario offers a more complex and diverse Celebrity News and Prejudice Reduction 125 array of exemplars about African Americans beyond just negative stereotypes. For instance, entertainment genres such as sitcoms and noncrime dramas are gradually presenting characters from African Americans in a more favorable light (Children Now, 2003; Mastro & Greenberg, 2000). Not surprisingly, there is a growing research interest in understanding the effects of more progressive portrayals on audience attitudes (Bodenhausen, Schwarz, Bless, & Waenke, 1995; Fujioka, 1999; Mastro & Tukachinsky, 2011; Power, Murphy, & Coover, 1996; Ramasubramanian, 2007, 2011; Ramasubramanian & Oliver, 2007; Schiappa, Gregg, & Hewes, 2005). For example, Power et al. (1996) use the priming paradigm to show how counter-stereotypic media information increased sympathetic ratings towards African Americans. Similarly, Bodenhausen et al. (1995) found that exposure to either likable positive African American role models as compared to neutral or negative media personalities primed positive attitudes, which they labeled generalized appraisal, of White audiences toward the entire outgroup. This study applies the priming perspective to examine whether more auspicious media portrayals of African Americans can decrease biases and encourage favorable outcomes towards them. Celebrity News Stories and Mediated Vicarious Contact One of the best researched approaches for promoting intergroup harmony is the contact hypothesis. It theorizes that greater intergroup contact increases positive attitudes, especially when such contact involves mutual respect and cooperation in an egalitarian environment with institutional support from authorities (Allport, 1954; Brewer & Miller, 1988; Hewstone & Brown, 1986; Pettigrew & Tropp, 2000). However, meaningful long-term intergroup contact in the real world is difficult to achieve due to societal constraints such as continued segregation in housing, schooling, and working environments. There is also the problem of selection bias such that more prejudiced individuals tend to avoid interpersonal contact with other racial/ethnic groups. Research also shows that both Blacks and Whites experience inter-racial anxiety that prevents initiating face-to-face social exchanges, as they fear race-based rejection (Richeson & Shelton, 2007). This article contends that vicarious mediated contact with positive outgroup media celebrities offers a practical and effective approach to prejudice reduction that could serve as an alternative or supplement to traditional face-to-face direct contact. Mediated contact is particularly effective with individuals who otherwise have minimal or no direct contact with outgroup members (Armstrong, Blake, & Neuendorf, 1989; Fujioka, 2005). In the context of television programs, Schiappa et al. (2005) found that when heterosexual audiences developed strong emotional bonds through para-social relationships with admired gay television characters, their overall attitudes toward homosexuality was improved. 126 Ramasubramanian This article applies the idea of vicarious mediated contact to celebrity news media contexts. Celebrity news has evolved as a popular subgenre of news media that appeals to a broad range of audiences by combining entertainment and news through infotainment outlets such as television, magazines, Internet sites, and social media. By reading celebrity news stories, readers often feel a sense of closeness and intimacy with the media personalities whom they venerate (Cashmore, 2006). Media serve as important sites through which audiences learn vicariously about admired media characters, which abstractly shapes their real-world perceptions and behaviors (Bandura, 1994). This article argues that reading news stories about outgroup media celebrities can serve as a vicarious contact experience that influences outgroup attitudes. Exemplification Theory and the Drench Hypothesis Exemplification theory posits that media exemplars act as concrete and vivid cues in influencing attitudes toward specific issues or social groups (Zillmann & Brosius, 2000). Racial attitudes are not stable evaluations but are context-sensitive such that specific salient exemplars that are momentarily primed at the time of making judgments can play a significant role in influencing attitudes (Krupat, Smith, Leach, & Jackson, 1997; Nisbett & Ross, 1980). Unlike associative network and prototyping models of stereotypes that suggest that attitudes cannot be altered and that only the most typical and strongest traits associated with a racial/ethnic group are stored in schemas, exemplar-based models posit that both stereotypic and counter-stereotypic exemplars coexist such that those activated most recently and continuously are most likely to influence subsequent attitudes (Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006). Prior studies show that exposure to counter-stereotypic exemplars leads to prejudice reduction, especially when the exemplars are not extreme outliers and when they are primed subtly (Blair & Banaji, 1996; Bodenhausen et al., 1995; Dasgupta & Greenwald, 2001). Drench hypothesis suggests that vivid, interesting, and engaging counter-stereotypic exemplars about outgroups can be very powerful, even if such portrayals are fairly uncommon (Greenberg, 1988). Media Exemplars, Symbolic Racism, and Support for Affirmative Action In order to be effective, strategies to reduce prejudice should not only target explicit stereotypical perceptions but also subtle, symbolic prejudice. Racial prejudice often manifests in implicit and aversive ways rather than old-fashioned blatant racism (Devine & Elliot, 2000; Entman & Rojecki, 2000). In particular, symbolic racism that emphasizes the ideology of meritocracy and the American Celebrity News and Prejudice Reduction 127 dream is a pervasive form of subtle prejudice in the United States (McConahay, 1986; Sears & Henry, 2003). Those who endorse symbolic racism believe that racial discrimination is no longer an issue in present-day contexts and that racetargeted public policies provide unjustified advantages to racial/ethnic outgroups. Considering that racism is increasingly expressed in subtle ways, measuring symbolic racism is invaluable in media stereotyping scholarship; yet only a few studies in media contexts have looked at effects on subtle prejudice (Busselle & Crandall, 2002; Entman & Rojecki, 2000; Mastro, Behm-Morawitz, & Kopacz, 2008). One of the paradoxes of contemporary racism is that although there appears to be a greater acceptance of the notion of racial equality, in practice such principles have not necessarily converted into tangible support for policies such as affirmative action or welfare. Motivational approaches to prejudice reduction often focus on self-enhancement goals of the perceivers but such efforts may not lead to helpful attitudes to accommodate, support, and promote interests of racial/ethnic outgroups. The current study contends that in order to be effective, any prejudice reduction strategy should also lead to more support for public policies that promote outgroup interests. Prior research on the role of televised stereotypes in influencing audiences’ support for policies such as affirmative action and welfare programs have largely been correlational in nature (Pan & Kosicki, 1998; Tan, Fujioka, & Tan, 2000). This study provides rare experiment-based evidence for the effectiveness of media counter-stereotypes in increasing support for policies that promote outgroup interests. The Current Investigation Using media priming, mediated contact, and exemplification approaches, this study argues that exposure to news stories about likable African American media celebrities serve as counter-stereotypical exemplars that can be used effectively to reduce symbolic racist beliefs and increase support for policies that encourage outgroup interests. Specifically, an experiment was conducted to understand the complex causal chain of relationships linking exposure to news stories about African American media celebrities with heuristic policy decision-making relating to affirmative action policy. Specifically, the following hypothesis was tested: Hypothesis: Exposure to news stories featuring counter-stereotypic African Americans as compared to stereotypic ones amongst White American participants will reduce real-world stereotypical perceptions about African Americans, lessen symbolic racist beliefs about African Americans, and increase support for affirmative action policies. 128 Ramasubramanian Method Design A between-participant experimental design was used in this study to compare the effects of type of media exemplar (stereotypical or counter-stereotypical) on the dependent variables (a) stereotypical perceptions about African Americans, (b) symbolic racist beliefs about African Americans, and (c) support for affirmative action. Each participant was randomly assigned to either the stereotypical stories condition (N = 48) or the counter-stereotypic condition (N = 40). They read through a news booklet that contained four news stories, two of which were manipulated to be about either stereotypical or counter-stereotypical African American media personalities. They then completed a second purportedly unrelated “Quality of Life” study that, among other filler questions and demographic variables, assessed the dependent measures. Participants Participants were recruited from two mass lecture communication courses at a large public university. They participated voluntarily in exchange for course credit and had the option of completing an alternative writing assignment. They were between the ages of 19 and 30, with the average age being 21.0 years (SD = 1.54). They represented many different educational backgrounds and community settings (urban, semirural and rural). Women comprised the majority of the sample at 66%. Note that 75.4% of participants were non-Hispanic Whites, 12% were Hispanic, 7.8% marked Other, 3% were Asian, 1.2% were African American, and 0.6% were Pacific Islanders. Only those participants who self-identified as non-Hispanic Whites were included in the final analysis since the study focused on prejudice reduction amongst majority group members. Procedure Students were asked to participate in two unrelated studies: “News Enjoyment Study” and “Quality of Life Study.” In the News Enjoyment Study, participants each read a news booklet containing four short news stories and a few brief items that assessed their recall and enjoyment of each story. Once the first study was completed, participants were invited to continue on to the second study called the “Quality of Life” study. In this mini-study, they completed a “Group Traits and Characteristics” section that asked about their opinions about several groups in society, including African Americans. Then, participants were asked to complete some filler questions about general beliefs pertaining to the justice system, politics, and education in the United States within which were embedded the items Celebrity News and Prejudice Reduction 129 relating to affirmative action policy opinions and symbolic racism. Finally, participants completed demographic information such as gender, age, race/ethnicity, and media usage. Participants were thanked at the end of the session and asked to fill out open-ended questions about their experience participating in the two studies. Stimuli The news stories were formatted with a headline, embedded photos of approximately the same size, and columned text for a consistent and realistic presentation. All stories were approximately one-page long with 300–350 words. Each condition included four news stories, two pertaining to media personalities and two neutral stories. The two neutral news stories were about the galaxy and about pumpkin pie; the neutral stories were placed between the experimental news stories in each survey. In both experimental conditions, the participants were exposed to two news stories about African Americans, one about a male African American exemplar and the other about a female. Headshot photos with little background context and neutral facial expressions (i.e., not smiling or frowning) of the media personality accompanied each news story. These news stories were edited from existing stories from popular online news sites such as Yahoo News and Entertainment Weekly. Prior to the experiment, two pilot tests were conducted to assess the appropriateness and effectiveness of the stimuli. Participants (N = 48 for pilot 1; N = 250 for pilot 2) were enrolled in mass lecture communication classes at a large public university. In the first pilot, the researchers surveyed mainstream popular media, current events, and engaged in informal conversations with racially diverse personal contacts to generate a list of well-known male and female media personalities, organized by racial category and gender. The media celebrities perceived as neutral were eliminated and those rated positive and negative, respectively, were included for the next pilot. This test assessed the chosen media personality images alongside short news stories including a set of neutral “fluff” news stories to serve as a control in the final experiment. Participants were instructed to read each short news story and then respond to questions about recognition and judgment of the news story’s content and tone apart from the media personality’s overall persona and reputation. An open-ended response box prompted additional thoughts after each news story. Based on the inputs from these two pilot tests, the control and manipulated news stories for the final experiment were determined. The stereotypical stories focused on common cultural stereotypes of African Americans such as aggressiveness and promiscuity. For example, one news story used in the current study featured Kanye West and emphasized his angry outbursts at the awards shows where he “cut the teen singer off, grabbed the mic . . . ” and “lost his temper after he was displeased.” In contrast, the counter-stereotypic 130 Ramasubramanian news stories highlighted traits such as calmness, which opposes traditional African American stereotypes. For instance, one news story featured Morgan Freeman and described him as a man who “embodies a calm authority that demands respect for the character.” Those who read news stories with counter-stereotypic exemplars were exposed to stories about Morgan Freeman (male) and Beyonce Knowles (female). The new stories with stereotypical exemplars were about Kanye West (male) and Mo’Nique Imes-Jackson (female). Participants were asked to: “Please indicate your responses to the story you just read.” They were presented with a seven-point Likert-scale (Strongly disagree to Strongly agree) asking the extent to which they found each story informative, enjoyable, boring, and interesting. Dependent Variables Stereotypical perceptions about African Americans (M = 4.56, SD = 0.91). Prior research on prevalent cultural stereotypes about African Americans reveals that the predominant negative stereotypes revolve around violence, criminality, poverty, and laziness (Ramasubramanian, 2011; Tan et al., 2000). Accordingly, participants responded to the question: “Indicate the extent to which you agree that the following traits describe African Americans/Black people in general. Please be honest in your responses.” A seven-point Likert-type scale, Strongly disagree to Strongly agree, allowed participants to choose an appropriate number that was closest to their perceptions and assessment of African Americans (apart from other social groups that were not of interest to the current research). The negative stereotypical traits included in this index were: criminal, violent, lazy, poor, uneducated, welfare recipient, drug dealer, and drug user (Cronbach’s α = .89). Symbolic racism beliefs (M = 3.61, SD = 1.08). The items used to measure symbolic racism beliefs were derived from prior research and comprise a blend of antiblack effect, conservative values, and individualism. The items thematically relate to the following subcategories on symbolic racism and stereotypes: excessive demands, denial of discrimination, and undeserved (dis)advantage (Sears & Henry, 2003; Tarman & Sears, 2005). Specifically, participants were asked to: “Please indicate the extent to which you agree with the following statements about African Americans. Please be honest in your responses.” Participants responded to five items on a seven-point Likert-type scale, Strongly disagree to Strongly agree. Some sample items were as follows: “African Americans are getting too demanding in their push for equal rights;” “Discrimination against African Americans is no longer a problem in the United States;” “Over the past few years African Americans have gotten more economically and politically than they deserve” (Cronbach’s α = .70). Celebrity News and Prejudice Reduction 131 Support for affirmative action policies (M = 3.16, SD = 1.29) . Items used to measure participants’ policy support for affirmative action were modified from existing studies (Ramasubramanian, 2010; Tan et al., 2000). The measure consisted of three items in two sections entitled “Beliefs about Justice.” Participants were asked to respond on a 7-point Likert-type scale, Strongly disagree to Strongly agree, to their beliefs about the justice system in the United States: Affirmative action is one good way to deal with past racial discrimination; Universities should not consider an applicant’s race in the admission process (reverse-coded); and, Affirmative action policies are unfair to White participants (Cronbach’s α = .63). Results Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA) Results A multivariate analysis of variance was used to examine the effects of the independent variable (type of media exemplars: stereotypical or counter-stereotypic) on the dependent variables (stereotypical perceptions, symbolic racist beliefs, and affirmative action support). Type of media exemplars had a statistically significant effect on stereotypical perceptions and symbolic racist beliefs but not on affirmative action support; Wilk’s λ = 0.91, F (3, 84) = 2.74; p ࣘ.05; R2 = .05. Specifically, participants who read African American counter-stereotypical news stories (M = 4.36; SD = 0.87) reported significantly lower levels of stereotypical perceptions as compared to those who read the stereotypical ones (M = 4.73; SD = 0.91); F (1, 86) = 3.69; p ࣘ .05; R2 = .04. Similarly, those who read counter-stereotypical news stories (M = 3.32; SD = 1.08) reported significantly lower levels of symbolic racist beliefs as compared to those who read the stereotypical ones (M = 3.86; SD = 1.02); F (1, 86) = 5.91; p ࣘ .05; R2 = .06. However, those who read counter-stereotypical news stories (M = 3.20; SD = 1.32) did not significantly differ in terms of affirmative action policy support as compared to those who read the stereotypical ones (M = 3.13; SD = 1.27); F (1, 86) = .07; ns. Path Analysis Results A path analysis was then conducted using the statistical package AMOS to simultaneously examine the direct and indirect effects of the exogenous variable (type of media exemplars: stereotypical or counter-stereotypic) on the endogenous variables (stereotypical perceptions, symbolic racist beliefs, and policy support). The exogenous variables were dummy-coded such that: 0 = stereotypical exemplars and 1 = counter-stereotypical exemplars. The initial model used the observed variable approach to hypothesize direct and indirect correlations among all endogenous variables with the exogenous variable. The model eliminated all paths that are not statistically significant at p ࣘ .05. Goodness of fit of the final model 132 Ramasubramanian Fig. 1. Model relating type of media exemplars in news stories about African Americans (stereotypical or counter-stereotypic) with stereotypical perceptions about African Americans, symbolic racist beliefs about African Americans, and support for affirmative action policies. was determined using a variety of indicators such as χ 2 /df value of more than 0.05, the root mean square of approximation (RMSEA) less than .06, and a comparative fit index (CFI) higher than .95 (Hu & Bentler, 1999). The final developed path model has a good fit with the data as evidenced by χ 2 = 3.14; df = 2; p = .86; CFI = 1.00; NFI = .99; RMSEA = .00 (.00 to .12). If we look at the individual paths in the final model generated in Figure 1, we notice two paths connecting type of media exemplars to symbolic racist beliefs. The direct path from type of media exemplars to symbolic racist beliefs shows a negative, significant correlation (β = –.49; p ࣘ .05). The indirect path is via “stereotypical perceptions,” which reveals a significant, negative correlation (β = −.37; p ࣘ .05) between type of media exemplars and stereotypical perceptions followed by a positive, marginally significant relationship between stereotypical perceptions and symbolic racist beliefs (β = .23; p ࣘ .10). In other words, when participants read news stories about African Americans with counter-stereotypic rather than stereotypic exemplars, negative stereotypical perceptions about African Americans as a whole were reduced, which influenced symbolic racist beliefs. In the next step of the path diagram, we notice that symbolic racist beliefs and affirmative policy support are negatively correlated (β = –.39; p ࣘ .001). That is, as symbolic racist beliefs decrease, there is increased support for affirmative action policy. Discussion Findings from this study support the assertion that celebrity news stories can serve as an effective tool in racial/ethnic prejudice reduction. The results are Celebrity News and Prejudice Reduction 133 in line with mediated contact theory, media priming, and exemplification perspectives and provide evidence that reading news stories functions as a mediated vicarious contact experience with counter-stereotypic outgroup exemplars, which prime favorable attitudes towards the outgroup as a whole. Specifically, the results show that participants who read news stories about counter-stereotypic African American media celebrities rather than stereotypical ones reported less stereotypical perceptions of African Americans in general and reduced subtle symbolic racist beliefs about African Americans, which in turn increased support for affirmative action policies. It is to be noted that these findings do not support alternative theories regarding counter-stereotypes that suggest that perceivers will disregard and discount them while making judgments in ways that preserve or strengthen existing stereotypes. Although the enlightened racism perspective speculates that counter-stereotypic media portrayals will lead to a negative shift through the denial of contemporary racism (Jhally & Lewis, 1992), this article does not find evidence for it. In contrast, similar to Bodenhausen et al. (1995), it supports the generalized appraisal perspective that a few atypical media exemplars are able to shape perceptions about the entire group. There is also no evidence to suggest that participants used subtyping models of stereotype change, which contend that individuals might perceive counter-stereotypic exemplars as too atypical and therefore ignore them while making their assessments about the entire group (Johnston & Hewstone, 1992). With regard to exemplification effects, the present research applies this theory to an entirely new context of media stereotyping. Like exemplification theory, it is argued that when participants are subtly exposed to news stories about entertainment celebrities as counter-stereotypical exemplars, they are not deliberately or consciously aware of how these exemplars shape their attitudes. Distinct from existing exemplification research however, this study contends (and the data support) that these contextual cues can improve outgroup perceptions and attitudes; to the extent that they present positive, counter-stereotypic exemplars. Zillmann and Brosius (2000) have largely considered exemplification and the use of exemplars (instead of base-rate statistical information) as a problem to be contended with when exposed to media content. However, these findings suggest that the exemplification process can also function to enhance social justice goals by increasing the presence of vivid, atypical, and interesting counter-stereotypic exemplars strategically in news stories. Future studies could further test the conditions and circumstances under which the frequency, recency, atypicality, vividness, and perceived realism of media exemplars might influence their accessibility and effects on racial/ethnic attitudes (Busselle & Shrum, 2003). Celebrity news is an increasingly popular media genre that uses multiple media formats such as social media, internet, magazines, and television to reach a wide, diverse audience. Although celebrity news is often seen as trivial “fluff” 134 Ramasubramanian compared to hard news stories, this article argues that given its popularity and broad reach, it might have a significant role to play in media-based strategies for prejudice reduction. Such vicarious contact with outgroup media characters offers a less intrusive and less anxiety-provoking alternative to traditional face-to-face, direct intergroup contact. The outcomes of this experiment support the idea that celebrity news media serve as an important virtual site for contact amongst racial/ethnic groups, which can be a practical and viable tool in promoting intergroup harmony. It is encouraging to note that in line with drench hypothesis and exemplification theory, even short-term, brief exposure to a couple of positive counterstereotypical celebrity news stories was able to elicit beneficial audience responses. The findings from this study make a case for the continued efforts of media scholars and policy-makers to encourage more progressive and diverse portrayals of racial/ethnic groups in the media. The role of media and art in society should not be to simply mirror prevailing racial dynamics but to challenge existing inequities to serve social justice ends. In the same vein, promoting greater diversity in the media workplace (in terms of journalists, scriptwriters, and media characters) promises to play a key role in combating cultural stereotypes in mainstream media by opening the door to alternative ways of reimagining intergroup relations and portraying marginalized groups. The study of counter-stereotypes is still very new within media effects literature. Future research should continue to clarify the differences between positive and negative exemplars apart from stereotypical versus counter-stereotypic exemplars. Counter-stereotypes were defined in this study as challenging or opposing existing cultural stereotypes. For instance, calmness and nonaggression was exemplified in the media celebrities in counter-stereotypical story condition. These are not the same as (seemingly) positive stereotypes, such as athleticism stereotypes associated with African Americans or model minority stereotypes of Asian Americans, which can lead to negative effects such as benevolent prejudice and symbolic racism (Ramasubramanian & Oliver, 2007). Mediated counterstereotypes, positive stereotypes, and negative stereotypes shape implicit and explicit racial attitudes in complex and sometimes conflicting ways, especially in contemporary contexts when racism is expressed by subtle, covert, and symbolic methods. Although mediated contact might serve as a proactive solution to the more reactive motivational approaches, there is no guarantee that majority, White American audiences would actively seek media content about outgroups. With the proliferation of media formats, news outlets, television channels, and online websites, there is a constant competition to gain audience’s attention. Given multiple media usage and multitasking, it is likely that even when audiences are exposed to counter-stereotypical news stories about outgroups, they might process them in less deliberate ways than seen in lab-based settings where they do not have other distractions. Apart from encouraging positive media portrayals, it is also important to focus on promoting critical viewing skills amongst audiences. A combination Celebrity News and Prejudice Reduction 135 of audience-centered approaches such as media literacy training along with exposure to positive outgroup media content can work in conjunction with one another to maximize prejudice reduction effects (Ramasubramanian, 2007). In the real-world media scenario, the more auspicious portrayals of African American characters happen mainly in genres such as drama and sitcoms, especially those targeted at Black audiences. Unless we see a sustained effort from media producers and content creators to continue to present a more diverse range of characters in all media genres, the potential for mediated contact to serve as an effective prejudice reduction strategy in the real world will not be optimally effective. Media diversity should also go beyond race to include representations from a variety of age groups, body sizes, sexualities, nationalities, and social classes. Future research should examine how intersectionality of identities in media characters and audiences influence the complex relationships between media content and attitudes. By including other racial groups and nonstudent samples, researchers can understand the audience-related characteristics that moderate the relationship between media exposure and policy opinions. Moreover, future research should include a control condition to clarify if the counter-stereotypes are reducing racism or if stereotypes are increasing them or if it is a combination of both processes. Despite these limitations, this study makes a valuable contribution to the sparse literature on media-based strategies for prejudice reduction by providing new experimental evidence in the context of popular celebrity news stories that shows that even a brief exposure to news stories about well-liked African American media personalities is able to prime more favorable attitudes, reduce symbolic racism and increase support for affirmative action policies among White viewers. It makes the case that scholarship on media entertainment and celebrity journalism offers an important contribution to understanding subtle stereotyping and public policy opinion-formation processes as we tackle complex global issues such as intergroup conflict. References Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Oxford, England: Addison-Wesley. Armstrong, G. B., & Neuendorf, K. A. (1992). TV entertainment, news, and racial perceptions of college students. Journal of Communication, 42(3), 153–176. Bandura, A. (1994). Social cognitive theory of mass communication. In D. Zillmann & J. Bryant (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Blair, I., & Banaji, M. (1996). Automatic and controlled processes in stereotype priming. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(6), 1142–1163. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.70.6.1142. Bodenhausen, G. V., Schwarz, N., Bless, H., & Waenke, M. (1995). Effects of atypical exemplars on racial beliefs: Enlightened racism or generalized appraisals? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 31, 48–63. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1995.1003. Brewer, M. B., & Miller, N. (1988). Contact and cooperation: When do they work? In P. A. Katz & D. A. Taylor (Eds.), Eliminating racism: Profiles in controversy (pp. 315–326). New York: Plenum Press. 136 Ramasubramanian Busselle, R. W., & Crandall, H. (2002). Television viewing and perceptions about race differences in socioeconomic success. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 46, 265–282. doi: 10.1207/s15506878jobem4602_6. Busselle, R. W., & Shrum, L. J. (2003). Media exposure and exemplar accessibility. Media Psychology, 5, 255–282. doi: 10.1207/S1532785XMEP0503_02. Cashmore, E. (2006). Celebrity/culture. New York, NY: Routledge. Children Now. (2003). Fall colors: Prime time diversity report. In C. Now (Ed.): San Francisco, CA: Children Now. Dasgupta, N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2001). On the malleability of automatic attitudes: Combating automatic prejudice with images of admired and disliked individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 800–814. doi: 10.1037/0022–3514.81.5.800. Devine, P. G., & Elliot, A. J. (2000). Are racial stereotypes really fading? The Princeton trilogy revisited. In C. Stangor (Ed.), Stereotypes and prejudice: Essential readings. Key readings in social psychology (pp. 86–99). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press. Dixon, T. L., & Linz, D. (2000). Overrepresentation and underrepresentation of African Americans and Latinos as lawbreakers on television news. Journal of Communication, 50(2), 131–154. doi: 10.1093/joc/50.2.131. Entman, R. M., & Rojecki, A. (2000). The Black image in the White mind: Media and race in America. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Fujioka, Y. (1999). Television portrayals and African American stereotypes: Examination of television effects when direct contact is lacking. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 76, 52–75. doi: 10.1177/107769909907600105. Fujioka, Y. (2005). Black media images as a perceived threat to African American ethnic identity: Coping responses, perceived public perception, and attitudes towards affirmative action. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 49(4), 450–467. Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological bulletin, 132(5), 692–731. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.692. Gilliam, F. D., & Iyengar, S. (2000). Prime suspects: The influence of local television news on the viewing public. American Journal of Political Science, 44, 560–573. doi 10.2307/2669264. Greenberg, B. S. (1988). Some uncommon television images and the drench hypothesis. In S. Oskamp (Ed.): Applied social psychology annual (Vol. No. 8): Television as a social issue. (pp. 88–102). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Hewstone, M., & Brown, R. (1986). Contact is not enough: An intergroup perspective on the ‘contact hypothesis.’ In M. Hewstone & R. Brown (Eds.), Contact and conflict in intergroup encounters: Social psychology and society (pp. 1–44). Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell. Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118. Hurley, R. J., Jensen, J., Weaver, A. & Dixon, T. (2015). Viewer ethnicity matters: Black crime in TV News and its impact on decisions regarding public policy. Journal of Social Issues, 71(1), 155–170. Jhally, S., & Lewis, J. (1992). Enlightened racism: The Cosby Show, audiences, and the myth of the American dream. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Johnston, L., & Hewstone, M. (1992). Cognitive models of stereotype change: III. Subtyping and the perceived typicality of disconfirming group members. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 28(4), 360–386. Krupat, E., Smith, R. H., Leach, C. W., & Jackson, M. A. (1997). Generalizing from atypical cases: How general a tendency? Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 19, 345–361. doi: 10.1207/15324839751037002. Mastro, D. E., Behm-Morawitz, E., & Kopacz, M. A. (2008). Exposure to television portrayals of Latinos: The implications of aversive racism and social identity theory. Human Communication Research, 34(1), 1–27. doi: 10.1111/j.1468–2958.2007.00311.x. Celebrity News and Prejudice Reduction 137 Mastro, D. E., & Greenberg, B. S. (2000). The portrayal of racial minorities on prime time television. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 44, 690–703. doi: 10.1207/s15506878jobem4404_10. Mastro, D., & Tukachinsky, R. (2011). The influence of exemplar versus prototype-based media primes on racial/ethnic evaluations. Journal of Communication, 61, 916–937. doi: 10.1111/j.14602466.2011.01587.x. McConahay, J. B. (1986). Modern racism, ambivalence, and the Modern Racism Scale. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination, and racism (pp. 91–125). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Oliver, M. B., & Fonash, D. (2002). Race and crime in the news: Whites’ identification and misidentification of violent and nonviolent criminal suspects. Media Psychology, 4, 137–156. doi: 10.1207/S1532785XMEP0402_02. Pan, Z., & Kosicki, G. (1998). Assessing news media influences on the formation of Whites’ racial policy preferences. Communication Research, 23(2), 147–178. doi: 10.1177/009365096023002001. Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2000). Does intergroup contact reduce prejudice: Recent metaanalytic findings. In S. Oskamp (Ed.), Reducing prejudice and discrimination. ‘The Claremont Symposium on Applied Social Psychology’ (pp. 93-114). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Power, J. G., Murphy, S. T., & Coover, G. (1996). Priming prejudice: How stereotypes and counter-stereotypes influence attribution of responsibility and credibility among ingroups and outgroups. Human Communication Research, 23(1), 36–58. doi: 10.1111/j.1468– 2958.1996.tb00386.x. Ramasubramanian, S. (2007). Media-based strategies to reduce racial stereotypes activated by news stories. Journalism & Mass Comm Quarterly, 84(2), 249–264. doi: 10.1177/107769900708400204. Ramasubramanian, S. (2010). Television viewing, racial attitudes, and policy preferences: Exploring the role of social identity and intergroup emotions in influencing support for affirmative action. Communication Monographs, 77(1), 102–120. Ramasubramanian, S. (2011). The impact of stereotypical versus counterstereotypical media exemplars on racial attitudes, causal attributions, and support for affirmative action. Communication Research, 38(4), 497–516. doi: 10.1177/0093650210384854. Ramasubramanian, S., & Oliver, M. B. (2007). Activating and suppressing hostile and benevolent racism: Evidence for comparative media stereotyping. Media Psychology, 9(3), 623–646. doi: 10.1080/15213260701283244. Richeson, J. A., & Shelton, J. N. (2007). Negotiating interracial interactions: Costs, consequences, and possibilities. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 316–320. Schiappa, E., Gregg, P. B., & Hewes, D. E. (2005). The Parasocial Contact Hypothesis. Communication Monographs, 72(1), 92–115. doi: 10.1080/0363775052000342544. Sears, D. O., & Henry, P. J. (2003). The origins of symbolic racism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 259–275. doi: 10.1037/0022–3514.85.2.259. Tan, A. S., Fujioka, Y., & Tan, G. (2000). Television use, stereotypes of African Americans and opinions on affirmative action: An affective model of policy reasoning. Communication Monographs, 67, 362–371. doi: 10.1080/03637750009376517. Tarman, C., & Sears, D. O. (2005). The conceptualization and measurement of symbolic racism. The Journal of Politics, 67(3), 731–761. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2508.2005.00337.x. Tukachinsky, R., Mastro, D., & Yarchi, M. (2015). Documenting portrayals of race/ethnicity on primetime television over a 20-year span and their association with national-level racial/ethnic attitudes. Journal of Social Issues, 71(1), 17–38. Zillmann, D., & Brosius, H. B. (2000). Exemplification in communication: The influence of case reports on the perception of issues. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. DR. SRIVIDYA RAMASUBRAMANIAN, PhD, Penn State University, is an Associate Professor of Communication and the Associate Dean for Climate & 138 Ramasubramanian Inclusion at Texas A&M University. Her research interests are in media psychology, stereotyping processes, cultural diversity, media literacy, and positive media effects. She has published in journals such as Communication Research, Media Psychology, Communication Monographs, Sex Roles, Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, Howard Journal of Communications, and the Journal of International & Intercultural Communication. She serves on the editorial board of Communication Monographs, Journal of Applied Communication Research, Journal of International & Intercultural Communication, Southern Journal of Communication and Journal of Creative Communications.