Achievement Gains in Elementary and High School

advertisement
ACHIEVEMENT G AINS IN ELEMENTARY AND HIGH SCHOOL
by Laura LoGerfo*, Austin Nichols*, and Sean F. Reardon**
Abstract
We estimate how much students are learning at different points in school across the United States, as
measured by reading and mathematics tests, and how these rates of learning differ for students of different
social backgrounds. We find our results depend on which of several plausible estimates of achievement is
used, but results differ less dramatically when achievement gains are measured in standard deviation units.
We find that students in kindergarten or first grade make much larger gains, on average, than students in
later grades, and students make larger gains early in high school than late in high school. The extent to
which this finding is an artifact of the tests used is unclear. Achievement gaps across race, income, and
home language groups exist at the start of kindergarten, and typically increase in the first two years of
school, but seem somewhat more stable afterwards. Given these two findings, we suspect that
interventions targeted on earlier grades may produce "more bang for the buck," though other
interpretations are possible. Finally, we argue that our estimates of average gains measured in standard
deviation units can be used as benchmarks that policymakers or researchers can use to estimate average
gains in reading or math in a baseline scenario in future experimental interventions, to judge the relative
importance of measured effect sizes, or to conduct power analyses.
Acknowledgements
The authors gratefully acknowledge research support from Sarah Cohodes and Joe Gasper. Many thanks
to Duncan Chaplin, Larry Hedges, Kim Reuben and Jesse Rothstein for extremely helpful comments and
suggestions. This report was written under the supervision of Jane Hannaway, Director of the Education
Policy Center of the Urban Institute.
* Urban Institute, Washington DC
** Stanford University, Stanford CA
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Chapter I: Introduction.............................................................................................................................. 1
Research Questions................................................................................................................................ 2
Methods................................................................................................................................................. 2
Data and Samples................................................................................................................................... 5
Chapter II: Average Learning Rates ......................................................................................................... 12
Elementary School............................................................................................................................... 12
Secondary School................................................................................................................................. 17
Summary of Elementary and High School Estimates............................................................................ 20
Chapter III: Differences in Learning Rates............................................................................................... 22
Elementary School............................................................................................................................... 22
Secondary School................................................................................................................................. 59
Summary of Elementary and High School Estimates............................................................................ 83
Chapter IV: Comparison with Theta Scores............................................................................................. 84
Elementary School............................................................................................................................... 84
High School ......................................................................................................................................... 95
Changes in Learning Rates Over Time ............................................................................................... 101
Summary ............................................................................................................................................ 113
Chapter V: Locally Standardized Differences in Learning Rates............................................................. 114
Brief Description of Method.............................................................................................................. 114
Advantage of Method Over Linear Models........................................................................................ 114
Limitations of the LSD Method ......................................................................................................... 116
Locally Standardized Difference Estimates......................................................................................... 116
Summary ............................................................................................................................................ 119
Chapter VI: Discussion and Implications............................................................................................... 121
Achievement Gains Over Time.......................................................................................................... 121
Gaps in Achievement Gains, and Gaps in Achievement..................................................................... 122
Methodological Caveats ..................................................................................................................... 124
Policy Recommendations................................................................................................................... 124
Future Work ...................................................................................................................................... 125
Conclusion......................................................................................................................................... 126
References ............................................................................................................................................. 127
Appendix A: Details of Data and Methods ............................................................................................ 128
Variables Used ................................................................................................................................... 130
Analytic Methods............................................................................................................................... 135
Appendix B: Rescaling Issues................................................................................................................. 140
Appendix C: Standard Deviations.......................................................................................................... 144
Effect Size.......................................................................................................................................... 144
IRT and Theta Score Distributions .................................................................................................... 146
Appendix D: Comparison of IRT and Theta Scores............................................................................... 155
Appendix E: Locally Standardized Growth Rate Differences................................................................. 161
* Urban Institute, Washington DC
** Stanford University, Stanford CA
CHAPTER I: INTRODUCTION
Educational research often attempts to explain student achievement by estimating the effects of individual
ability, home environment, and teacher and school quality (Burkam et al., 2004; Cooper et al., 1996;
Entwisle & Alexander, 1992; Ferguson, 1998; Fryer & Levitt, 2002; Goldhaber & Brewer, 2000; Lee &
Burkam, 2002; Nye et al., 2004; Reardon, 2003). Rather than isolate what factors account for learning, this
report steps back to ask two basic and crucial questions.
First, how much are students learning per grade in reading and mathematics? Second, how do these rates
of learning differ for students of different social backgrounds? We address these questions for students in
elementary school and high school, taking advantage of two nationally representative, longitudinal datasets
sponsored by the National Center for Education Statistics (NCES)—the Early Childhood Longitudinal
Study (ECLS-K) and the National Education Longitudinal Study (NELS:88).
Lessons learned can set benchmarks for researchers interested in experimental and quasi-experimental
designs, inform policymakers about the likely impacts of potential policy reforms, and help educators and
the general public understand what students and schools can be expected to accomplish in an academic
year. In sum, our results will serve as reference points for future research on achievement gains.
The report is organized into six chapters. This first chapter explains the importance of the study and
outlines what results are presented. The second chapter presents baseline estimates for how much students
learn per grade on average in both reading and math. The third chapter examines the relative learning rates
for specific student subgroups (gender, race/ethnicity, language background, and economic status). The
fourth chapter explores learning per grade and by subgroup using a different metric than the previous two
chapters. The fifth chapter presents results from specialized analyses that re-examine differences in
learning rates across time and across subgroups. The sixth and final chapter summarizes the findings and
draws implications for research and policy. Appendices document the data and analytic methods used in
the report.
1
Research Questions
Question 1: How much do students learn?
To address this question we estimate the average gains in reading and math for students in elementary
school and students in high school. To produce such estimates, we use two nationally representative and
longitudinal datasets, the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study—Kindergarten cohort (ECLS-K) for
elementary school and the National Education Longitudinal Study of 1988 (NELS:88) for high school. In
ECLS-K the typical student is a child who was a kindergartner in 1998, and in NELS:88 the typical student
was an eighth-grader in 1988.
Participants in these studies took a battery of tests in reading and mathematics at the start of their relevant
school transition. The elementary school children were first tested in the first term of kindergarten and last
tested three years later, for a total of five testing times. The high school students were first tested during
eighth-grade, typically the last grade before high school, and last tested four years later, for a total of three
times. Results from this report indicate how much students learned during the intervals.
To study learning, we want to measure achievement, but what we have are several versions of these
reading and math test scores. As definitions of achievement, each version has strengths and weaknesses,
which are discussed in the next subsection. In general, achievement is defined for our purposes as
probable performance on a reliable test, which represents knowledge or skills at a point in time. Gains in
achievement are thus increases in probable performance over time and represent learning.
Question 2: Do learning rates differ for different types of students?
We examine if, when, and for whom differences in learning rates are evident. This report sets benchmarks
for the yearly gains in achievement made by: 1) male students and female students; 2) students of different
racial/ethnic groups and language backgrounds; and 3) higher-income students and low-income students.
Early evidence from ECLS-K suggests that economically disadvantaged and minority children enter
kindergarten with lower average achievement than socio-economically advantaged and white children
(Burkam et al., 2004; Downey et al., 2004; Fryer & Levitt, 2004; Lee & Burkam, 2002). Studies with the
NELS:88 data suggest the same pattern of academic disadvantage for low-income and minority students at
the beginning of high school (Phillips, Crane, and Rouse, 1998). Phillips and colleagues attribute
achievement gaps between black and white students at the end of high school to differences in initial skills.
Over the school years, the initial gap widens. Our analyses estimate the learning rates for these groups to
explore what group differences exist and if the differences change over time.
Methods
We address each of these two research questions with several different analytic approaches. If the different
techniques produce similar results, we are more confident that our findings are robust and accurate. We
apply two different methods—growth curve analysis and locally standardized differences—and use three
different metrics—IRT scale score points, effect sizes, and theta scores. The different methods applied to
these different metrics all measure growth in achievement test scores. One approach is not more accurate
than the other. This section explains the analytic methods.
Growth Analysis
In Chapters 2 and 3, we report on regression analyses that use piecewise linear growth models1 to estimate
learning in elementary school and in high school.2 In elementary school, gains in reading and math
1
The analytic methods used in this report are explained in more detail in Appendix A.
2
achievement are estimated during: 1) kindergarten; 2) the summer between kindergarten and first grade; 3)
first grade; and 4) the time between the end of first grade and the end of third grade. High school analyses
estimate gains in reading and math achievement between eighth and tenth grades and between tenth and
twelfth grades. In Chapter 2, we look at results by grade level, and in Chapter 3, we focus on the learning
of subgroups defined by gender, race/ethnicity, and economic status.
Findings are reported in three different metrics to facilitate interpretation of the results. Each is in
common use by researchers and policy analysts, and each has both drawbacks and advantages. Using one
metric or the other simply reflects preferences in interpretability and not differences in accuracy. The
metrics we use are: (1) estimated changes in points on the test; (2) effect sizes; and (3) estimated changes in
theta scores.
IRT scores
Gain is measured in points on the tests (for the ECLS-K analyses, the scale score points are rescaled as of
the third grade assessment—see Appendix B for details on rescaling). The points are not the actual
number right on the assessment as administered, but rather the number that item response theory (IRT)
predicts the student would have answered correctly if s/he had been administered all the questions in the
ECLS-K kindergarten through third grade item pools. At any given test administration, a student was
administered only a subset of these items—a subset that corresponded to their grade level and skill level as
estimated by an initial set of routing items.
The IRT model does not increase students’ scores for correct guesses (NCES, 2005), so the score is more
accurate than a pure sum of correct responses. This IRT process allows each student’s performance to be
put on a common scale at each point in time, and over time. The IRT scores represent the best available
measure of knowledge at a specific time point, and therefore offer some hope of quantifying learning rates.
However, there are three major challenges to face before using the IRT scale scores. First, the range of
possible IRT scores is still a somewhat arbitrary component of the test design. In ECLS-K, for example,
the math test had fewer questions than the reading test, so a mid-range score on reading may appear quite
high for math. Thus, IRT scale scores across these different subjects are not comparable. This is easy to
rectify by converting all point scores into percent right, that is, by dividing by the maximum score on the
test. However, this solution would mask differences in the test introduced by the subsequent addition of
more difficult questions in later rounds, so we choose not to change the point scores in this way.
Second, this scale is assumed to be an interval scale, meaning that point differences are consistent in
numeric and substantive value throughout the distribution. But this assumption is tenuous at best. The
IRT scale score is an interval-scaled measure of the number of items right on a specific test, which is
necessarily one of many possible interval-scaled measures of achievement, one for each possible test.
Different tests, each of which might have some desirable properties, could produce different results. On
any one test, there are subtle distinctions in the difficulty of questions asked that complicates comparing
IRT scores from different points in the distribution of scores within the same subject.3 This complicates
comparisons of learning by the same students across time (e.g. is learning multiplication over 9 months in
third grade faster or slower than learning addition in six months during first grade?) and comparisons of
two students at different levels at the same point in time (e.g. is a gain of 16 points for a higher income
The program used to test the growth models, Hierarchical Linear Modeling (HLM) 6.0 clusters multiple test scores within
individual students. The program also can group students within schools so as to test for school effects on learning trajectories.
Future research will explore differences in school characteristics that may explain differences in student achievement gains.
3 See Appendix B for details.
2
3
student whose initial score was 20 points a greater gain than a gain of 14 points for a lower income student
whose initial score was 14 points?). This issue is further explicated in Appendix B.
Third, the IRT scale scores tend to be positively skewed in the earliest grades (indicating that the lowest
performing students may have higher scores than they would have had on a longer test) and negatively
skewed in third grade (indicating that the highest performing students may have lower scores than they
would have had on a longer test). On the ECLS, this reflects the fact that the tests included relatively few
questions4 that were very easy for most kindergarteners, the type of questions which would better capture
differences in achievement among low-achieving kindergartners, so these low achievers tend to be
“clumped” together at a higher achievement level than accurately reflects the achievement of the lowestachieving among them. The tests included relatively few questions that were extremely difficult for most
third graders, so these high achievers tend to be “clumped” together at a lower achievement level than
accurately reflects the achievement of the highest-achieving among them. To address these and related
issues, we standardize the change in points over time to construct effect sizes, and compare across two
metrics (test performance measured by scale scores and theta scores), as discussed in the following
sections.
Effect sizes
Effect sizes measure the magnitude of a relationship by calculating the point gains made relative to the
baseline variation. Effect sizes can be compared across tests that have not been designed to be compared
and that differ in the difficulty range of questions asked. For these reasons, we report estimates from
models using IRT scores in points per month, divided by the standard deviation of the initial (baseline)
score at the start of the time period. This translates point gains into standard deviation gains per month.5
In other words, effect sizes measure how far children’s scores progress along the time 1 test distribution
by time 2. The gain is measured relative to the distribution of test scores at time 1 so the rescaling issues
discussed in Chapter 5 and Appendix B are less problematic. For students with test scores at the median at
time 1, a one standard deviation gain means that their time 2 test scores would put them roughly at the 84 th
percentile, instead of the 50 th percentile, in the time 1 distribution. Details on how we estimate standard
deviations are available in Appendix C.
These effect sizes facilitate comparisons of learning rates across different tests and different populations
(e.g. compare average learning by the population of US kindergartners in 1998 to average learning by the
population of US kindergartners in some other year or in some other geographic unit). We offer some
comparisons at the end of Chapters 2 and 3 between results from the ECLS-K analyses and results from
the NELS:88 analyses.
Neither points nor effect sizes directly describe the specific skills students are actually learning. However,
IRT points can be linked to those skills. Each graph in Chapters 2 and 3 maps IRT scores onto the
proficiency level (which corresponds to a set of skills) that students are learning most rapidly at that score6
(for example, a score of 50 in the ECLS-K IRT score metric corresponds to a skill level at which students
are primarily learning to add and subtract). Using this method adds substantive meaning to the IRT scores
reported as levels, and also clarifies what gains students make at different points in time. Stating a gain of 5
More details on the implications of test design appear in Chapter 5 and Appendix C.
One month is the largest unit of calendar time smaller in size than every interval, and we use the conversion factor one month
equal to 30.4375 days (the average length of one month in days).
6 Specifically, each proficiency level is mapped onto the IRT scale score at which the probability of proficiency in that level is
one half; in general, this corresponds to the score at which students’ proficiency in that skill grows most rapidly with gains in
scale scores.
4
5
4
points on a math test is not as meaningful or informative as stating a gain in skill from identifying numbers
to solving word problems.
Theta scores
The IRT process combines an individual’s pattern of responses (right, wrong, omitted) with characteristics
of the items on a test to estimate individual ability, known as theta. First, the IRT model estimates the
theta for each student and the item parameters (difficulty, discrimination, and guess-ability). Then these
theta and IRT parameters are transformed in a non-linear, monotonic function to construct the IRT scale
scores (in the “estimated number right” metric).
Using the theta scores offers a distinct advantage. In contrast to the more skewed scale scores, the theta
scores are more symmetrically distributed, because theta is on an absolute scale, independent from the
particular set of questions that are asked (see Appendix B for more details). However, like the IRT scale
associated with these theta scores, the theta scale is still mathematically arbitrary.
We construct models that are identical to the IRT scale score models, but using theta scores as the
outcome variable. The theta score is often referred to as a measure of ability in the subject area. However,
this does not imply that ability is a fixed characteristic of the test-taker (Hambleton, Swaminathan, &
Rogers, 1991). The notion of ability measured by theta is the capacity to answer questions on a kind of test
(e.g. reading or math) at a point in time, and does not correspond to any notion of unformed or genetic
potential. The ability represented by theta scores can change over time, and the change in theta scores is
simply another measure of achievement or skill level to contrast with changes in IRT scale scores. In
Chapter 4, we discuss how the results from the IRT scale score models compare with results from the
theta models.
Locally Standardized Difference
In Chapter 5, we use a method pioneered by Sean Reardon, an associate professor at Stanford University
and a co-author of this report, called locally standardized difference (LSD), to analyze differences in gains
over time between subgroups. The method generates estimates of differences in learning rates that change
less in response to certain changes in test design than do findings from our analyses in Chapters 2 through
4, where IRT and theta scores are the dependent variables.
The LSD technique compares the learning rates of students in different categories in the vicinity of some
initial score (either IRT score or theta score), normalized by the difference in the pooled standard
deviation (roughly, the average within-group standard deviation among population subgroups). Each of
these standardized estimates of gaps in learning rates is local to a particular baseline score, and averaging
across the entire distribution of baseline IRT scores produces an estimate of the average locally
standardized difference in growth rates.
This LSD approach offers a distinct advantage in that it was developed to address issues of rescaling in the
ECLS-K data. The mean of the local estimates measures the differences in gains across subgroups in a way
that is more robust to subsequent rescaling. This rescaling affects ECLS-K most, so our LSD analyses
focus only on young children. The first set of results from the LSD models is produced in the IRTestimated number right metric, and the second set of results is in the theta metric.
Data and Samples
ECLS-K Data
The ECLS-K study followed students from kindergarten through third grade. In addition to test score
data, information on children’s gender, race and ethnicity, language status, and family background were
also gathered.
5
Data were collected at five points in time: fall and spring of kindergarten, fall and spring of first grade, and
spring of third grade. Not all students were assessed at every time point for several reasons: 1) sample
attrition; 2) random subsampling in the fall of third grade; and 3) insufficient English fluency. The third
reason has the most important implications for our analyses.
In the rounds of data collection during kindergarten and first grade, children from a language minority
background first took a screening test for English fluency called the Oral Language Development Scale
(OLDS). If children did not pass this test, they could not take the reading assessment in that round.
Spanish speakers who failed this test, however, could take a Spanish translation of the math assessment.
Students classified as fluent at one wave were deemed fluent at all subsequent waves; students not fluent at
one wave were re-administered the English OLDS at each subsequent wave until a passing score was
obtained. When children demonstrated sufficient proficiency in English on the OLDS at any point, they
then took both the reading and math assessments.
Due to this language assessment process, the sample of Hispanic students with valid scores on the reading
assessment increases over time. At wave 1, in the fall of kindergarten, 30 percent of Hispanic students
were not assessed in reading (Table 2). By the spring of first grade, 10 percent of Hispanic students were
missing reading scores, and by the spring of third grade almost all Hispanic students were able to take the
reading assessment in English.
As a result of the changing sample of Hispanic students with reading scores, comparisons of average
reading scores by subgroup over time must be done with caution. If, for example, the reading score gap
(the difference in average reading scores) between non-Hispanic white students and Hispanic students
grows over time, this may not indicate slower average rates of reading skill gain among Hispanic students,
but rather reflect the addition of students with lower-than-average English reading skills to the sample
over time.
TABLE 1.1: P ERCENTAGES OF STUDENTS MISSING R EADING SCORES , BY RACE/ ETHNICITY AND W AVE
RACE/ETHNICITY
FALL K
SPRING K
FALL 1ST
SPRING 1ST
SPRING 3RD
White
0.9
0.5
0.4
0.3
1.0
Black
0.5
0.4
0.0
0.1
3.3
Asian
22.8
12.9
7.4
3.2
0.6
Hispanic (Total)
30.0
20.9
20.7
10.4
1.4
Total
7.5
5.0
4.9
2.5
1.5
The same problem is not manifest in the math assessment, since virtually all Hispanic students took the
math assessment at each wave—some in Spanish, some in English. Nearly a quarter of Asian students did
not take the math assessment at wave 1, since the math assessment was administered only in English and
Spanish. Thus, Asian math achievement gap patterns must be interpreted with similar caution as outlined
above.
6
TABLE 1.2: P ERCENTAGES OF STUDENTS MISSING MATH SCORES , BY RACE/ ETHNICITY AND W AVE
RACE/ETHNICITY
FALL K
SPRING K
FALL 1ST
SPRING 1ST
SPRING 3RD
White
0.9
0.5
0.4
0.4
0.6
Black
0.6
0.7
0.3
0.1
1.4
Asian
22.6
12.9
7.8
3.2
0.6
Hispanic (Total)
0.9
0.4
0.6
0.6
0.7
Total
1.7
1.0
1.0
0.6
0.7
ECLS-K sample
Our analyses are based on the 21,059 children in the ECLS-K restricted sample who have at least one
reading or math test score.7 Slightly fewer than 5 percent of the children in the sample were repeating
kindergarten in 19988 and 54.86 percent attended full-day kindergarten. Complete details on which
students are enrolled in half-day versus full-day kindergarten are available in Appendix A. Table 1.1
presents descriptive statistics for the sample.
Excluding children with only one test score does not change the results significantly.
Children who were repeating kindergarten in the fall of 1998 are included in this sample to represent who enrolls in
kindergarten. Children who repeated kindergarten in the fall of 1999–when the majority of children in the sample progressed to
first grade–are not included. We dropped the students who were retained in kindergarten in the spring of 1999, because we
wanted to ensure that we were capturing the achievement gain made in a given grade. If some students were in kindergarten at
the same time as the majority had moved onto first grade, then the gains would not be defined consistently.
7
8
7
TABLE 1.3: SAMPLE SIZES—ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
Percent of
Sample
Male
Female
Missing Gender
White
Black
Hispanic
Asian
Other
Missing Race
English Speaking Home (EH)
Non-English Speaking Home (NEH)
Hispanic Non-EH
Hispanic-EH
Asian Non-EH
Asian-EH
Missing Race*EH
Low Income
Higher Income
Missing Income Status
Sample Size
(N=21,059)
10,760
10,275
24
11,643
3,192
3,744
1,303
1,106
71
17,905
3,132
1,957
1,787
812
491
71
8,417
11,840
802
White-Low Income
White-High Income
Black-Low Income
Black-High Income
Hispanic-Low Income
Hispanic-High Income
Asian-Low Income
Asian-High Income
Other-Low Income
Other-High Income
Missing Race*Economic Status
3,008
8,635
2,046
964
2,286
1,316
497
693
574
513
822
14.28
41.00
10.11
4.76
11.30
6.50
2.46
3.42
2.73
2.44
3.90
50.99
49.01
0.11
55.29
15.16
17.78
6.19
5.25
0.34
85.11
14.89
9.32
8.51
3.87
2.34
0.34
41.55
58.45
3.81
White students make up 55.3 percent of this analytic sample (these descriptive frequencies are unweighted
and are not intended to be representative of the population), followed by Hispanic children (17.8 percent)
and black children (15.2 percent). Asian students (6.2 percent) and children classified as other (5.3
percent)—mixed race, American Indian, Native Hawaiian—complete the sample. Approximately 15
percent of the children in the analytic sample speak a language other than English at home. The Asian
students and Hispanic students are almost evenly divided between those who come from homes where
English is spoken and from homes where another language is spoken. In this sample, 41.6 percent of the
children are eligible for the federal free and reduced-price lunch program. 9,10
Results from analyses that compare the analytic sample to the survey sample are discussed in Appendix A.
Eligibility for the federal free and reduced price lunch program is determined by calculating an income-to-needs ratio, a
family’s income as a proportion of the official federal poverty line for a family of that size. A family with income at the poverty
line has a ratio of 1.00. Free and reduced price lunch eligibility extends to those with a ratio of 1.85. These data come from the
fall kindergarten round of data collection. We use information from the first data collection, because we expect that the
socioeconomic level at which students start school plays an important role in predicting subsequent learning rate. Changes in
9
10
8
NELS:88 Data
NELS:88 collected data from a nationally representative sample of 24,599 eighth graders and followed
them through high school. Data were collected at three time points: spring of eighth grade, spring of tenth
grade, and spring of twelfth grade. Descriptive information about students was also recorded, including
their gender, race/ethnicity, language status, and family background.
NELS:88 sample
Analyses include students who participated in the base year, first follow-up, and second follow-up of
NELS:88. We exclude students who dropped out or were retained in high school.11,12,13 Of the full sample
of NELS:88 participants, 4.42 percent dropped out between grades 8 and 10 and 11.19 percent between
grades 10 and 12. Thus, our final analytic sample consists of 14,078 respondents (again, these descriptive
frequencies are unweighted and thus not representative of the population).14 Table 1.2 presents the
demographic composition of the analytic sample.
socioeconomic status may affect achievement status and learning rate, but that compelling and critical question is not the focus
of this report.
11We use two sources to determine whether a student dropped out. First, we use the created variable F2EVDOST, which
indicates whether the student ever dropped out at least once during the base year through second follow-up, regardless of
whether they ever returned. This variable is constructed from non-transcript sources. Second, we use the variable F2TROUT,
which indicates whether a student dropped out based on information collected from transcripts. If either of these variables
indicates dropout, then the student was excluded from the analyses. To test how much our analytic results changed by including
and excluding dropouts and retained students, we conducted analyses with these students. The results changed only slightly. A
more thorough discussion of this process and results is in Appendix A.
12The variable G12COHRT determines whether a student is on time in the twelfth grade.
13 We selected this sample to retain a true longitudinal sample. The weights we use to ensure generalizability over time was
constructed for the sample selected.
14A small number of respondents (n=104) were missing data on race. These respondents are included in the analytic sample
when possible because most of them had valid cognitive test scores at all three time points as well as data on other key
demographic characteristics.
9
TABLE 1.4: SAMPLE SIZES—S ECONDARY SCHOOL
Male
Female
White
Black
Hispanic
Asian
Native American
Sample Size
(N=14,078)
6,882
7,196
10,186
1,247
1,514
911
116
Percent of
Total Sample
48.88
51.12
72.35
8.86
10.75
6.47
0.82
English Speaking Home (EH)
Non-English Speaking Home (NEH)
Missing EH Status
Hispanic Non-English
Hispanic-English
Asian Non-English
Asian-English
Missing Race*EH
Low Income
Higher Income
Missing Income Status
12444
1521
113
735
768
421
439
166
1,944
10,740
1394
89.11
10.89
0.80
5.22
5.46
2.99
3.12
1.18
13.81
76.29
9.90
White-Low Income
White-High Income
Black-Low Income
Black-High Income
Hispanic-Low Income
Hispanic-High Income
Asian-Low Income
Asian-High Income
Native American-Low Income
Native American-High Income
Missing Race*Income Status
852
8424
435
673
460
851
145
668
32
58
1480
6.76
66.87
3.45
5.34
3.65
6.76
1.15
5.30
0.25
0.46
10.5
Black students represent 8.9 percent of the sample, and 10.8 percent of the sample is Hispanic. Asian
students account for 6.5 percent of the sample, Native Americans for just 0.8 percent, and the remainder
are white students. Of the sample, 10.9 percent speak a language other than English in their homes. As in
ECLS-K, the Asian and Hispanic students are divided about evenly between those from non-English
speaking homes and from English-speaking homes. Based on our definition of low-income (eligibility for
the free and reduced price lunch program), 13.8 percent of the analytic sample qualifies as low-income.15
Analyses are weighted so that the samples become nationally representative, in that results from the
analytic models can be generalized to two groups of students. First, the ECLS-K findings can be
generalized to children across America who entered kindergarten in the fall of 1998. Second, the NELS:88
findings can be generalized to adolescents across America who were enrolled in eighth grade in the spring
of 1988. The findings presented in this report provide a broad overview of the patterns in achievement for
young children and high school students. Though 1988 seems long ago and high school reforms have
The proportion of high school students eligible for the free and reduced price lunch program (less than 15 percent) differs
dramatically from the proportion of eligible elementary school students in the ECLS-K sample for a number of reasons. One of
the most important is that eligibility is determined through completing an application sent home with or to students. A risk of
humiliation or a lack of interest in signing up for the program may prevent more eligible students from applying.
15
10
come and gone in the meantime, this report offers a baseline of what learning can be achieved. We think
the findings are still relevant today and will serve as useful reference points for future research.
11
CHAPTER II: A VERAGE LEARNING RATES
This chapter presents answers to the first research question: how much are students learning over
particular time intervals? Analyses estimate gains in reading and mathematics for a typical elementary
school student who was in kindergarten in 1998 and for a typical high school student who was in the
eighth grade in 1988. We present reading and math results, first for elementary school students16 and then
for secondary school students.
Elementary School
This section presents findings from the elementary school analyses. Results for reading and math are
presented for each time interval in terms of gain per month, effect size per month, gain per time period,
and accumulated gain. Reading results are shown in Table 2.1 and math results are shown in Table 2.2. All
differences discussed in the text are statistically significant unless otherwise specified. We also present
findings that convert achievement scores to skill proficiencies.17 This provides a more substantive
interpretation of how much children gain, not in points but in specific skills and knowledge. Figure 2.1
shows this conversion for reading. Figure 2.2 shows this conversion for math.
Reading
Starting point: Entering kindergarten
The estimated score on the reading assessment at the start of kindergarten is 22.75 points. As illustrated in
Figure 2.1, this means that the average child is learning to recognize letters at the start of formal schooling.
Kindergarten
During kindergarten, children, on average, gain 1.81 points per month on the reading assessment (moving
0.196 standard deviations up the distribution of scores at the beginning of kindergarten). On average,
kindergartners primarily learn beginning sounds, and by the end of the kindergarten year, they are learning,
on average, how to identify ending sounds.
Summer
No gains occur, on average, during the summer between kindergarten and first grade. Indeed, there is a
0.171 point loss per month, suggesting a slight summer “slide” effect. Summer slide refers to a dip in
children’s cognitive development in the summer months when children are not exposed to stimulation at
school. However, this loss is very small.18
First grade
During first grade, children are gaining 3.28 points per month (moving 0.210 standard deviations up the
distribution of scores at the beginning of first grade) in reading skills, on average. The average first grader
moves from learning ending sounds to learning how to read words in context. The slightly larger gain in
first grade compared to kindergarten may reflect traditional emphasis of teaching basic reading skills such
All results are based on the twice-rescaled test scores. We expect that the advent of the thrice-rescaled scores from the fifth
grade data will slightly alter the findings. More discussion about the impact of rescaling in subsequent rounds of ECLS-K is
provided in Appendix B.
17 Appendix A provides a table for converting points to skill proficiencies.
18 We do not discuss issues of summer learning extensively for several reasons. First, the summer analysis cannot be replicated
with the subsequent years of data. Second, others have conducted research focusing on the summer months with these ECLS-K
data (Burkam et al., 2004; Downey et al., 2004). Third, this report focuses on what students learn in a given year, not how the
summer learning rate compares with the school year learning rate.
16
12
as phonics in the first grade curriculum. Or, the larger increase may derive from issues with the test design,
discussed in Chapter 1 and further discussed in Appendix B (see Figure B.1).
Second and third grades19
Over the next two grades, children are gaining 1.58 points per month, on average. This represents a gain of
just 0.075 SD on the reading assessment. During second and third grade, children are learning to identify
words on sight, understand words in context, and draw literal inferences. By the end of third grade, the
average student is learning how to extrapolate information from text.
TABLE 2.1: READING GAINS FOR STUDENTS IN KINDERGARTEN IN 1998
Time Period
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
At End of Period
22.75
1.81
(0.0106)
-0.171
(0.0462)
3.28
(0.0187)
1.59
(0.0065)
0.196
17.06
39.80
-0.0126
-0.44
39.36
0.210
30.89
70.26
0.0749
38.17
108.43
Note: Standard errors for the estimated coefficients are presented in the first column of the table in parentheses below the
corresponding coefficient. All coefficients are significantly different from zero. To calculate effect sizes, we divide the gain per
month by the estimated standard deviation of the base-period test at the start of each time period.
We cannot determine whether the learning rate is the same in second and third grades, because assessment data are collected
only at the end of third grade and not during second grade. So we can discuss and compare the kindergarten and first grade
rates explicitly, but we cannot distinguish the second and third grade learning rates. The second-grade learning rate could
plausibly be similar to either the learning rate in first grade or the learning rate in third grade.
19
13
FIGURE 2.1: READING GAINS FOR STUDENTS IN KINDERGARTEN IN 1998
ECLS Reading Scores, All Students
110
7-EXTRAPOLATION
70
5-WORD IN CONTEXT
4-SIGHT WORDS
50
Score
90
6-LITERAL INFERENCE
3-ENDING SOUNDS
30
2-BEGINNING SOUNDS
10
1-LETTER RECOGNITION
Kindergarten
1st Grade
2nd and 3rd Grades
Mathematics
Starting point: Entering kindergarten
Findings for mathematics learning rates are presented in Table 2.2. At the start of kindergarten, children
score, on average, 17.52 points on the mathematics assessment. This mean score implies that many
students already know how to count and to identify shapes (see Figure 2.2). These are the math skills that
many children typically learn before formal schooling begins.
Kindergarten
In kindergarten, the monthly average gain is 1.62 points per month (moving 0.196 standard deviations
each month up the distribution of scores at the start of kindergarten). Children, on average, are learning
relative size near the beginning of kindergarten and learning ordinality and sequences near the end of
kindergarten.
Summer
Children exhibit slow growth in scores over the summer months, but still make modest gains. Instead of
losing their math skills, on average, they gain roughly a half point per month (0.491 points, moving 0.042
standard deviations each month up the distribution of scores at the end of kindergarten).
First grade
In first grade, children gain 2.37 points per month, on average, moving 0.191 standard deviations (each
month) up the distribution of scores at the beginning of first grade. In this year, children, on average, learn
how to add and subtract.
14
Second and third grades
During second and third grades, children advance their math performance at an average rate of 1.20 points
per month. This rate is equivalent to moving 0.077 standard deviations (each month) up the distribution of
scores at the beginning of first grade. Over these years, children are learning multiplication and division,
on average, and may begin to learn more advanced skills like place value by the end of the period. Though
the test includes questions on using the concepts of rate and measurement, children are not yet learning
those skills (on average, though the highest-performing students have mastered those skills by the middle
of third grade).
TABLE 2.2: MATH GAINS FOR STUDENTS IN KINDERGARTEN IN 1998
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
At End of Period
17.52
1.62
(0.0090)
0.491
(0.0446)
2.37
(0.0158)
1.20
(0.0048)
0.196
15.21
32.73
0.0422
1.26
34.00
0.191
22.28
56.28
0.0767
28.87
85.15
Note: Standard errors for the estimated coefficients are presented in the first column of the table in parentheses below the
corresponding coefficient. All coefficients are significantly different from zero. To calculate effect sizes, we divide the gain per
month by the estimated standard deviation of the base-period test at the start of each time period.
15
FIGURE 2.2: MATH GAINS FOR STUDENTS IN KINDERGARTEN IN 1998
ECLS Math Scores, All Students
90 100
7-RATE & MEASUREMENT
50
60
5-MULTIPLY/DIVIDE
4-ADD/SUBTRACT
30
3-ORDINALITY, SEQUENCE
20
2-RELATIVE SIZE
10
40
Score
70
80
6-PLACE VALUE
1-COUNT, NUMBER, SHAPE
Kindergarten
1st Grade
2nd and 3rd Grades
Summary
Children make bigger gains in reading and mathematics in kindergarten and first grade than in second and
third grades.20 In reading, children are gaining about 0.20 standard deviations (each month) on the baseline
distribution of scores (at the beginning of the period) over the first two years of school. By the end of
third grade, their gain has dropped to a third of the earlier pace, to 0.07 standard deviations per month. In
math, a similar pattern emerges.
Examining the estimated learning rates measured in points, the rate in first grade is much faster than the
rate in kindergarten. However, the distribution of the first grade scores is more widely spread than that of
the kindergarten scores. Thus, when these gains are converted into effect sizes, the learning rates across
kindergarten and first grade are nearly the same.
In previous work with the ECLS-K data conducted by NCES, the average gain children made from the
beginning of kindergarten through the end of third grade equaled 85.55 points in reading and 64.53 points
in math (NCES, 2004, Appendix A: Table A-6). Our analyses find a gain of 85.68 points in reading and
67.63 points in math over the same time period.21 Our results are thus very similar to results produced by
NCES, when looking at the average gains made by all students.
These differences in growth may derive from a number of sources, including different test scaling as well as different rates of
gain. So this interpretation of these differences should be taken as the result of several factors, not only that children gain more
ground earlier in elementary school than later.
21 Differences between our results and those in NCES (2004) most likely derive from NCES’ use of regression analysis, our use
of growth curve modeling and precision weights, and a more inclusive sample of heretofore unreleased restricted data.
20
16
Secondary School
This section presents findings from the secondary school analyses with the NELS:88 data, following the
same format as the previous section. Tables 2.3 and 2.4 present the findings in the same four metrics as
the ECLS-K findings, and graphs depict the relationship between the numerical findings and the
corresponding proficiencies.
Reading
On average, students make slightly larger gains on the reading test earlier in high school than they do later
in high school. In the spring of eighth grade, the average reading achievement is 28.25 points. Students
gain an average of 3.66 points between the spring of eighth grade and tenth grade and 2.17 points between
tenth grade and twelfth grade. The effect sizes are small, suggesting a slow rate of learning. Between eighth
and tenth grades, students move 0.02 standard deviations (each month) up the distribution of scores at the
end of eighth grade on the reading assessment; between tenth and twelfth grades, this drops to 0.01
standard deviations per month, reflecting both slower mean gain in points on the test and a greater
standard deviation on the test at the end of tenth grade than at the end of eighth.
TABLE 2.3: READING GAINS FOR STUDENTS IN EIGHTH GRADE IN 1988
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
At End of Period
28.25
0.152
(0.0033)
0.0903
(0.0042)
0.0199
3.66
31.91
0.00978
2.17
34.07
Note: Standard errors for the estimated coefficients are presented in the first column of the table in parentheses below the
corresponding coefficient. All coefficients are significantly different from zero. To calculate effect sizes, we divide the gain per
month by the estimated standard deviation of the base-period test at the start of each time period.
17
45
FIGURE 2.3: READING GAINS FOR STUDENTS IN EIGHTH G RADE IN 1988
NELS Reading Scores, All Students
30
2-Simple Inferences
20
25
Score
35
40
3-Complex Inferences
8-10
10-12
Mathematics
On average, students make slightly larger gains in math achievement earlier in high school than they do
later in high school. The average eighth grader scores 38.16 points on the math assessment. Students gain
an average of 7.78 points (moving 0.029 standard deviations, each month, up the distribution of scores at
the end of eighth grade) between eighth and tenth grades and 4.28 points (moving 0.014 standard
deviations, each month, up the distribution of scores at the end of tenth grade) between tenth and twelfth
grades.
TABLE 2.4: MATH GAINS FOR STUDENTS IN EIGHTH GRADE IN 1988
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size per
Month
Gain Per Period
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
At End of Period
38.16
0.324
(0.0038)
0.178
(0.0038)
0.0291
7.78
45.94
0.0136
4.28
50.21
Note: Standard errors for the estimated coefficients are presented in the first column of the table in parentheses below the
corresponding coefficient. All coefficients are significantly different from zero. To calculate effect sizes, we divide the gain per
month by the estimated standard deviation of the base-period test at the start of each time period.
18
FIGURE 2.4: MATH GAINS FOR STUDENTS IN EIGHTH G RADE IN 1998
60
NELS Math Scores, All Students
50
4-Intermediate Level Math
40
Score
3-Simple Problem Solving
30
2-Fractions and Exponents
20
1-Single Operations
8-10
10-12
Summary
Gains made during high school appear slow relative to elementary school. In the two years between eighth
and tenth grade, high school students, on average, gain just 0.02 to 0.03 SD per month in reading and
math respectively. This is equivalent to about 0.48 SD over this time period in reading and about 0.70 SD
in math during these two years. The gains between tenth and twelfth grades are even smaller. From tenth
grade to twelfth grade, students gain less than a quarter of a standard deviation (0.23 SD) in reading and
about a third of a standard deviation in math. The gain per period between the first half of high school is
nearly double the gain made during the second half of high school.
There are at least four plausible explanations for the apparent slowdown of learning, only one of which
actually implies a slower mean rate of growth in high school. First, the reading and math assessments
include basic questions about concepts and skills that students may no longer encounter in their classes, so
students are not improving their scores. High school may be where students gain knowledge about social
studies and chemistry, not about reading or basic math. Second, these are not the same tests, nor the same
children—the ECLS tests children who were in kindergarten in 1998 and the NELS tests children who
were in eighth grade in 1988—two different cohorts receiving two different tests, so the results may not be
comparable for a host of reasons. Third, the underlying variation in math and reading skills may be much
greater in high school than in elementary school, so that gains expressed in standard deviation units appear
smaller relative to the variation in the population. Finally, it may be that there are decreasing returns to
instruction, and more students learn at a lower rate once they have learned most of the material taught
prior to high school (so they are on the “flatter” part of their individual learning curves). This last
explanation is the only one of these four explanations that implies slower learning (though both the first
and last imply a slower mean rate of growth in reading and math) in high school.
19
Summary of Elementary and High School Estimates
In elementary school, children are gaining more on math and reading tests in kindergarten and first grade
than in second and third grade. By the time students enter high school, their achievement gains in these
subjects decrease substantially.
In elementary school, children make quadruple the achievement gains high school students make. In
kindergarten, children gain about 1.9 SD in reading and in math, and first graders gain nearly 2.0 SD in
reading and about 1.8 SD in math. The gain between second and third grade is on a similar magnitude to
kindergarten gain (about 1.8 SD in both reading and math), but this gain is made over two years, not just
one year. In high school, the gain per period drops drastically to just 0.48 SD over the first two years in
reading and to 0.70 SD in math. The second half of high school witnesses further decreases in
achievement gain; students gain about a quarter of a standard deviation in reading and a third of a standard
deviation in math over this time period.
In comparing across subjects, the gain in reading and math is about the same during kindergarten, as
measured in effect sizes. During first through third grades, children gain more on the reading test than on
the math test. Then in high school, the gain in math exceeds the gain in reading. This makes sense if we
consider that the primary grades typically emphasize literacy, so children may pick up more reading skills
than math skills. In subsequent years, math learning may depend more on classroom instruction, especially
in later grades when advanced math is more likely taught.
Table 2.5 compares two measures of effect size gains, one that divides point gains by the estimated
standard deviation on the test at the start of the period, and one that divides point gains by the estimated
standard deviation on the test at the end of the period. The first measures progress along the distribution
of scores at the initial time period, for example, how far the typical student would move during
kindergarten up the distribution of scores from the beginning of kindergarten. The second measures
progress along the distribution of scores at the end of the period, for example, how far the typical student
would move during kindergarten up the distribution of scores taken from the end of kindergarten. Both
have a similar interpretation, and both are in some sense scale-free, but give slightly different impressions
of relative rates of gain.
The gain per period in elementary school seem quite large, measured in standard deviation units (regardless
of whether we use the standard deviation from the first-period test, which we refer to as effect sizes, or the
alternative measure). Students gain more than one standard deviation per grade in kindergarten and first
grade and just under one standard deviation per grade in second and third grades. In comparison, Kane
(2003) finds effect sizes of only 0.25 and 0.5 during elementary school. However, Kane was looking at
math and reading gains during fifth grade. This would be consistent with a pattern of decreasing effect
sizes across grade-levels after first grade. Indeed, our findings suggest an average gain in second and third
grades slower than the first grade gain, but faster than Kane’s fifth grade gains. In addition, our estimates
suggest that by high school students are making gains of about two to three tenths of a standard deviation
per school year between eighth and tenth grade and more than one tenth but less than two tenths of a
standard deviation per school year between tenth and twelfth grades.
20
TABLE 2.5: COMPARING R EADING AND MATH GAINS ACROSS TIME
(IN TWO DIFFERENT EFFECT SIZE PER PERIOD MEASURES )
Using SD at Start of Period
Using SD at End of Period
Time Period
Reading
Math
Reading
Math
Kindergarten
First Grade
1.84
1.98
1.85
1.80
1.25
1.46
1.31
1.42
2nd and 3rd Grades
1.80
1.84
1.86
1.60
8th Grade to 10th Grade
0.478
0.698
0.396
0.592
10th Grade to 12th Grade
0.235
0.325
0.218
0.306
There are several possible explanations for the apparent decline in learning rates. First, there is likely a shift
in instruction, away from basic skills such as reading and math, toward more specialized topics such as
social studies or physical sciences. The reading and math assessments used in these analyses do not focus
on such topics. Second, some students may in high school reach the level of proficiency in reading that is
their lifetime maximum, and additional instruction has no effect on these students. In general, we expect
there to be decreasing returns to instruction in any topic, and many students are likely on the “flat” part of
their learning curve in reading by tenth grade.
A third possible explanation for the decreasing effect size between elementary school and high school is
the changing population. The demographic composition of the samples may shift in ways that increase the
standard deviation in the distribution of test scores between elementary and high school. For example, if
the fraction of black and Hispanic students increased substantially between 1988 (when the NELS study
started) and 1998 (when the ECLS-K study started), this could cause the standard deviation in test scores
overall to go up.22 Indeed, the ECLS-K data contain a much higher fraction of black and Hispanic students
than the NELS:88 data. Thus, based on differences in demographics alone, we expect larger standard
deviations in the ECLS-K data and, consequently, smaller effect sizes.
Another possible explanation for the decreasing effect size between elementary school and high school is
simply that the standard deviation of test scores increases as children move to higher grade levels. Indeed,
the standard deviations in ECLS-K increase consistently as children move from kindergarten to third grade
and a similar pattern is seen in the NELS:88 data as students move from eighth to twelfth grade.23
However, the ECLS-K scale scores are not comparable to the NELS:88 scale scores.
In sum, the rate of gain in elementary school appear to differ substantially from the rate of gain in high
school, as measured by gains in points relative to the distributions on tests administered in ECLS and
NELS. A quick rate of gain in reading and math emerges in kindergarten and first grade, which then drops
off during second and third grades. By high school, the rate of gain slows even more dramatically. This
pattern is manifest through our analyses using the IRT scale score metric as well as effect sizes. However,
we cannot conclude that different rates of gains on these tests, even measured in effect sizes so they are
more comparable across tests, correspond to different rates of learning at different points on time.
This assumes that the (within-group) standard deviation in scores for Black students and Hispanic students is similar to (or
larger than) the standard deviation for other students but that the Black students and Hispanic students have much lower scores
on average.
23 See Appendix C for a discussion on the standard deviations and a corresponding table.
22
21
CHAPTER III: D IFFERENCES IN LEARNING RATES
The previous chapter established the average baseline learning rates for elementary school students and for
secondary school students. Focusing on the average, however, can mask vast differences in learning rates
across different student subgroups. Several studies have found substantial gaps in achievement by
race/ethnicity and by socioeconomic status, starting from before kindergarten through the end of high
school (Fryer & Levitt, 2002; Hedges & Nowell, 1998; Lee & Burkam, 2002; Phillips et al., 1998). This
chapter presents differences in learning rates by four dimensions of student background: gender,
race/ethnicity, language background, and economic status. For each of these dimensions, reading and
mathematics results are separately presented. Relevant tables and graphs follow the text.
Elementary School
Gender
Reading
Female students begin kindergarten with higher scores than male students and maintain their slight
advantage through kindergarten, as shown in Table 3.1 and Figure 3.1. At the start of kindergarten, girls
are predicted to score nearly a point higher on the reading assessment. During kindergarten and first grade,
girls gain very slightly more than boys (0.136 points per month or 0.0146 SD in kindergarten; 0.00925 SD
in first grade), but the difference is statistically significant.
After first grade, the gain per year on reading tests is essentially identical across genders, but due to the
initial differences, the slight advantage for females (in terms of overall points earned on the assessment)
remains. Girls finish third grade with an average reading score nearly 4 points higher than boys.
This advantage is seen in Figure 3.1. In kindergarten, the lines representing gains are quite close, and they
separate by first grade with the line representing females’ learning very slightly steeper. During the second
and third grades, the lines that identify male and female learning rates are parallel, with the gain for females
slightly higher than the gain for boys. But, in terms of substance, by the end of third grade, both boys and
girls are learning literal inference and not yet learning extrapolation.
22
TABLE 3.1: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY G ENDER—ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
M ALE STUDENTS
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
22.29
1.75
(0.0147)
-0.230
(0.0651)
3.21
(0.0271)
1.59
(0.0094)
0.189
16.44
38.73
-0.0168
-0.59
38.14
0.206
30.24
68.38
0.102
38.25
106.63
FEMALE STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM MALE S TUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
0.94
0.136
(0.0211)
0.120
(0.0923)
0.144
(0.0374)
-0.00694
(0.0129)
0.0146
1.28
2.22
0.00880
0.31
2.53
0.00925
1.36
3.88
-0.000445
-0.17
3.72
These analyses are based on students who have at least one reading or math test score in five rounds of ECLS-K data. Each
estimate in bold is significantly different from the corresponding estimate for male students at the 5 percent level. Descriptions
of models are provided in Appendix A.
23
FIGURE 3.1: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY G ENDER —ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
ECLS Reading Scores by Gender
110
7-EXTRAPOLATION
70
5-WORD IN CONTEXT
50
Score
90
6-LITERAL INFERENCE
4-SIGHT WORDS
3-ENDING SOUNDS
30
2-BEGINNING SOUNDS
10
1-LETTER RECOGNITION
Kindergarten
1st Grade
2nd and 3rd Grades
Female
Male
Math
Male and female students start kindergarten with very similar math scores, but in sharp contrast to the
reading results, male students begin to edge out girls in first grade (see Table 3.2). Also in contrast to the
reading results, the gap continues to widen over time. In kindergarten, boys and girls start with similar
math scores and make similar gains on the math assessment. In first grade, girls begin to make less gain in
math (-0.0078 SD). By the third grade assessment, girls have earned 2.79 points less on the math
assessment than boys. But this does not translate to a great difference in skill attainment. Both male and
female students are learning place value by the end of third grade, as presented in Figure 3.2.
24
TABLE 3.2: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY G ENDER—ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
M ALE STUDENTS
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
17.53
1.63
(0.0132)
0.469
(0.0649)
2.41
(0.0229)
1.23
(0.0069)
0.198
15.37
32.91
0.0403
1.21
34.11
0.195
22.72
56.83
0.0998
29.68
86.51
FEMALE STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM MALE S TUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-0.03
-0.0350
(0.0180)
0.0464
(0.0890)
-0.0959
(0.0316)
-0.0684
(0.0097)
-0.00425
-0.33
-0.36
0.00399
0.12
-0.24
-0.00776
-0.90
-1.15
-0.00553
-1.64
-2.79
These analyses are based on students who have at least one reading or math test score in five rounds of ECLS-K data. Each
estimate in bold is significantly different from the corresponding estimate for male students at the 5 percent level. Descriptions
of models are provided in Appendix A.
25
FIGURE 3.2: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY G ENDER —ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
7-RATE & MEASUREMENT
10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100
Score
ECLS Math Scores by Gender
6-PLACE VALUE
5-MULTIPLY/DIVIDE
4-ADD/SUBTRACT
3-ORDINALITY, SEQUENCE
2-RELATIVE SIZE
1-COUNT, NUMBER, SHAPE
Kindergarten
1st Grade
2nd and 3rd Grades
Female
Male
Race/Ethnicity
Reading
Even before school begins, learning differences by race/ethnicity emerge, and these differences persist
during the early school years. Table 3.3 presents differences in learning rates by race/ethnicity, and Figure
3.3 maps these learning rates onto skill proficiency levels.
Black children begin kindergarten more than 3 points behind white children and trail in their reading
learning rates during the first three years of school. Black children gain 0.031 SD less per month during
kindergarten and 0.037 SD less during first grade. Thus the initial difference is compounded by a slower
learning rate. Figure 3.3 illustrates the widening of the difference in points gained per time period.
Hispanic children start kindergarten with the lowest average score on the reading assessment. This deficit
increases during school, because Hispanic students make fewer gains in reading than white students.
Compared to white children, Hispanic children gain 0.0201 SD less per month in kindergarten and 0.044
SD less per month in first grade. In second and third grades, Hispanic children are still gaining significantly
less per month than white children. But Hispanic children are not as disadvantaged in their reading gains
as black children. The deficit that Hispanic children face is significantly smaller than what black children
face in kindergarten and in second and third grades.24
Asian children start kindergarten with 1.51 more points than white children and learn significantly more
quickly than white children in kindergarten, gaining an average 0.240 more points per month or 0.0259 SD
We tested selected subgroup comparisons to determine if differences in subgroup differences from White students (slopes in
HLM parlance) were significant.
24
26
more. Surprisingly, unlike other subgroups, Asian children on average make gains in reading during the
summer (0.611 points per month or 0.0448 SD). However, during first through third grades, the pattern
reverses; Asian students learn significantly less in reading than white students. Figure 3.3 shows the
narrowing of these differences by the end of first grade and the crossover in the second and third grade
time period as the cumulative average score for white students begins to exceed the cumulative score for
Asian students.
Figure 3.3 depicts the racial/ethnic learning differences. The learning rates for black and Hispanic children
are below those for white and Asian children in kindergarten and remain so through the spring of third
grade. Black and Hispanic children end third grade on average learning literal inference, a skill that Asian
and white children have already learned.
27
TABLE 3.3: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY RACE/ ETHNICITY—ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of
Period
W HITE STUDENTS
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
24.22
1.90
(0.0137)
-0.231
(0.0602)
3.53
(0.0251)
1.64
(0.0083)
0.205
17.86
42.08
-0.0169
-0.60
41.49
0.227
33.27
74.75
0.0773
39.41
114.16
BLACK STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-3.17
-0.283
(0.0281)
-0.0288
(0.1240)
-0.576
(0.0507)
-0.185
(0.0195)
-0.0306
-2.66
-5.83
-0.00211
-0.07
-5.91
-0.0370
-5.43
-11.33
-0.00871
-4.44
-15.77
HISPANIC STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-4.92
-0.186
(0.0304)
0.268
(0.1350)
-0.687
(0.0499)
-0.0578
(0.0176)
-0.0201
-1.75
-6.67
0.0196
0.69
-5.98
-0.0440
-6.47
-12.45
-0.00273
-1.39
-13.84
ASIAN STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
1.51
0.240
(0.0595)
0.611
(0.3020)
-0.289
(0.1020)
-0.250
(0.0274)
0.0259
2.26
3.77
0.0448
1.57
5.34
-0.0185
-2.72
2.63
-0.0118
-6.02
-3.39
Each estimate in bold is significantly different from the corresponding estimate for white students at the 5 percent level.
Descriptions of models are provided in Appendix A.
28
FIGURE 3.3: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY RACE/ETHNICITY—ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
ECLS Reading Scores by Race
110
7-EXTRAPOLATION
70
5-WORD IN CONTEXT
50
Score
90
6-LITERAL INFERENCE
4-SIGHT WORDS
30
3-ENDING SOUNDS
2-BEGINNING SOUNDS
10
1-LETTER RECOGNITION
Kindergarten
1st Grade
Asian
Hispanic
2nd and 3rd Grades
Black
White
Math
Table 3.4 presents the learning rates in mathematics for children who enrolled in kindergarten in 1988. As
in reading, Asian and white children score higher than black and Hispanic children in mathematics.
At the start of kindergarten, black children score about 4.5 points lower than white children on the math
assessment. By the end of third grade, black children trail white students by a total of about 15.5 points.
Hispanic children start kindergarten with the lowest score in math, and compared to white children gain
less in math over the first few years of elementary school. The difference in monthly math gains decreases
over time, but Hispanic students continue to learn at lower rates during the later periods. The initial gap
and the slower learning pace result in Hispanic children scoring more than 10 points less than white
students (on average) at the end of third grade.
Asian children and white children start kindergarten with nearly equivalent scores and gain a similar
amount of points on the math assessment during kindergarten. White children, however, experience
slightly larger gains during first grade, and Asian children make larger gains in second and third grades. By
the end of third grade, the difference between the subgroups accumulated math gain is less than a quarter
of a point.
Figure 3.4 depicts the differences in learning rates by race/ethnicity. The lines representing the learning
rates of white and Asian children overlap and are substantially above those representing the other
subgroups. White and Asian children begin kindergarten learning relative size and end third grade learning
place value, but black and Hispanic children are nearly one grade level behind throughout these grades (for
29
example, they are learning to multiply and divide in third grade, but white and Asian students are learning
this skill in second grade). The differences magnify slightly for black children over these primary school
years, while Hispanic students seem to maintain a constant disadvantage relative to white and Asian
students.
30
TABLE 3.4: DIFFERENCES IN MATHEMATICS LEARNING RATES BY RACE/ ETHNICITY — ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
W HITE STUDENTS
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
19.53
1.73
(0.0122)
0.473
(0.0624)
2.49
(0.0225)
1.23
(0.0062)
0.210
16.26
35.79
0.0407
1.22
37.01
0.202
23.48
60.49
0.0784
29.51
90.00
BLACK STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-4.64
-0.361
(0.0237)
-0.0965
(0.1200)
-0.379
(0.0424)
-0.147
(0.0144)
-0.0439
-3.40
-8.04
-0.00829
-0.25
-8.29
-0.0306
-3.57
-11.85
-0.00938
-3.53
-15.38
HISPANIC STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-5.63
-0.253
(0.0230)
0.125
(0.1160)
-0.222
(0.0399)
-0.0275
(0.0130)
-0.0307
-2.38
-8.01
0.0108
0.32
-7.68
-0.0180
-2.09
-9.78
-0.00176
-0.66
-10.44
ASIAN STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
0.56
-0.0341
(0.0467)
0.443
(0.2770)
-0.362
(0.0818)
0.0941
(0.0208)
-0.00414
-0.32
0.23
0.0381
1.14
1.38
-0.0293
-3.41
-2.03
0.00601
2.26
0.23
Each estimate in bold is significantly different from the corresponding estimate for white students at the 5 percent level.
Descriptions of models are provided in Appendix A.
31
FIGURE 3.4: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY RACE/ETHNICITY—ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
7-RATE & MEASUREMENT
10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100
Score
ECLS Math Scores by Race
6-PLACE VALUE
5-MULTIPLY/DIVIDE
4-ADD/SUBTRACT
3-ORDINALITY, SEQUENCE
2-RELATIVE SIZE
1-COUNT, NUMBER, SHAPE
Kindergarten
1st Grade
2nd and 3rd Grades
Asian
Hispanic
Black
White
Language Status
Reading
We define language status as a dichotomous variable indicating whether children come from homes in
which the primary language spoken is English (EH) or not English (NEH).25 Table 3.5 presents the results
for reading learning by language status, and Figure 3.5 aligns these learning rates with gains in skill
proficiency levels. Students included in the language assessments speak English with sufficient fluency to
qualify to take the reading assessment. Please refer to Appendix A for a discussion comparing achievement
between those students who qualified to take the reading assessment and those who did not.26
Children from homes where English is not the primary language start kindergarten with reading scores
4.12 points below those of their peers whose home language is English. During kindergarten, NEH
Children who were administered the oral language screening test are classified as NEH, since children whose home language
was not English were administered the oral language screening test to determine if they could take the assessments. By the third
grade assessment, no child was excluded from taking the assessment for not speaking English with sufficient fluency, so this is
more a measure of potential limited English proficiency (LEP) at some point in time, than of actual contemporaneous LEP.
26
Hispanic students who failed the OLDS screening test start kindergarten with the lowest average reading score and continue
to earn less than White students on the reading assessment throughout early elementary school. Asian students who failed the
OLDS start off at the same reading score as White students on average, but gain less in almost every time period than White
students. Asian students who never failed the OLDS fare better than White students until second and third grades. Hispanic
never-failed students essentially keep pace with White students. Students who never failed the OLDS are more likely included in
early estimates of reading learning. Children who failed the OLDS at least once enter the models after the first time period and
therefore may bias the later estimates. Their entrance biases the initial level upward (students weaker in English reading are
excluded initially), but biases the level at the subsequent time point downward, especially for the subgroups with language
minority members, such as Asian students and Hispanic students (students who are weaker are now included in average
estimates). This leads to a lower average estimated growth rate. See Appendix A for more details.
25
32
students gain slightly less per month than EH students, and this learning rate gap grows even larger in first
grade.
By the end of first grade, the cumulative point difference in reading is already more than 10 points. In
second and third grades, the difference in reading gains is significant, but small. Thus the 10-point
difference widens only slightly.
This is illustrated in Figure 3.3 where the lines representing learning rates visibly diverge over the first
grade time period yet stay parallel throughout the second and third grades. On average, NEH children are
learning literal inference by the end of third grade, while EH children have generally advanced past this
skill.
TABLE 3.5: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY LANGUAGE STATUS —ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
23.16
1.84
(0.0110)
-0.205
(0.0482)
3.36
(0.0199)
1.59
(0.0070)
0.199
17.30
40.46
-0.0150
-0.53
39.94
0.215
31.59
71.53
0.0752
38.33
109.86
NON -ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME S TUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-0.154
(0.0398)
0.154
(0.1680)
-0.543
(0.0571)
-0.0217
(0.0181)
-4.17
-0.0166
-1.45
-5.62
0.0113
0.40
-5.22
-0.0348
-5.11
-10.34
-0.00102
-0.52
-10.86
Each estimate in bold is significantly different from the corresponding estimate for EH students at the 5 percent level.
Descriptions of models are provided in Appendix A.
33
FIGURE 3.5: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY LANGUAGE STATUS —ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
ECLS Reading Scores by Language Status
110
7-EXTRAPOLATION
70
5-WORD IN CONTEXT
50
Score
90
6-LITERAL INFERENCE
4-SIGHT WORDS
30
3-ENDING SOUNDS
2-BEGINNING SOUNDS
10
1-LETTER RECOGNITION
Kindergarten
1st Grade
2nd and 3rd Grades
EH
Non-EH
Math
The math assessment includes more children who may not speak English at home, because the math test
was translated into Spanish for those participants who did not pass the language screening test. Results for
math by language status are presented in Table 3.6 and represented in Figure 3.6 as changes in skill
proficiency levels.
Children from different language backgrounds learn math at different rates in kindergarten and in first
grade. NEH children begin kindergarten more than five points behind their peers on the math assessment.
The difference in learning rates is slight; NEH students gain just 0.200 points less per month (-0.02 SD) in
kindergarten. The deficit declines in first grade to 0.0773 points less per month (-0.00627 SD). By second
and third grades, there is a slight, but significant, difference in math learning rates between the two groups,
with the difference favoring the NEH students. The reversal in second and third grades cannot eliminate
the gap, however, and NEH students’ scores remain behind EH students’ scores.
34
TABLE 3.6: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY LANGUAGE STATUS —ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
23.16
1.84
(0.0110)
-0.205
(0.0482)
3.36
(0.0199)
1.59
(0.0070)
0.199
17.30
40.46
-0.0150
-0.53
39.94
0.215
31.59
71.53
0.0752
38.33
109.86
NON -ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME S TUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-0.154
(0.0398)
0.154
(0.1680)
-0.543
(0.0571)
-0.0217
(0.0181)
-4.17
-0.0166
-1.45
-5.62
0.0113
0.40
-5.22
-0.0348
-5.11
-10.34
-0.00102
-0.52
-10.86
Each estimate in bold is significantly different from the corresponding estimate for EH students at the 5 percent level.
Descriptions of models are provided in Appendix A.
35
FIGURE 3.6: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY LANGUAGE STATUS —ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
7-RATE & MEASUREMENT
10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100
Score
ECLS Math Scores by Language Status
6-PLACE VALUE
5-MULTIPLY/DIVIDE
4-ADD/SUBTRACT
3-ORDINALITY, SEQUENCE
2-RELATIVE SIZE
1-COUNT, NUMBER, SHAPE
Kindergarten
1st Grade
2nd and 3rd Grades
EH
Non-EH
Race and Language
Reading
Table 3.7 presents reading skills by racial/ethnic categories and by whether English is the primary language
spoken at home.27,28
As seen in an earlier table (Table 3.3), Hispanic students experience lower growth rates in reading than
white students. In Table 3.7, we see that this pattern holds for both Hispanic EH and NEH students.
However, the differences are much larger for NEH children. In first grade, the deficit for Hispanic NEH
children balloons. Most likely this is due to the entrance of students previously excluded from the
assessment because they failed to pass the OLDS screening test. Far more Hispanic NEH students
qualified to take the reading assessment in first grade than in kindergarten. These newly-included students
may demonstrate weaker-than-average English reading skills and thus depress the reading scores in the
first grade spring data. This helps to explain the lower scores and gains in first grade for Hispanic NEH
children. Hispanic EH children move from being about 3 points behind white children at the beginning of
kindergarten to more than 8 points behind by the end of third grade. In contrast, Hispanic NEH children
shift from a gap that starts at 8.15 points to a much wider gap of over 19 points.
We remind readers that the language minority sample with reading scores is somewhat more advantaged than the sample
excluded from the assessment process for language reasons. This caveat does not apply in the same way to mathematics tests.
Hispanic LEP students who do not pass the screening test take a Spanish form of the math assessment. There were no
translations of the math test into Asian languages, so Asian LEP students were excluded.
28 Children from homes where a language other than English is predominantly spoken are labeled NEH, for non-English
speaking home. Children from homes where English is the primary language are labeled EH, for English-speaking home.
27
36
Asian EH children start kindergarten with higher average reading scores than white children but learn at
similar rates to white children during kindergarten and first grade. In second and third grades, Asian EH
children’s learning rate is slightly but significantly slower than white children’s learning rate. Thus, Asian
EH students end third grade with about the same accumulated gain as white students.
Asian NEH children start kindergarten with an average reading score less than a point behind white
children. These Asian children then learn more than white children in kindergarten, but significantly less in
the later grades. Thus Asian NEH students end third grade with cumulatively fewer points on the reading
assessment than white students. The more these students are exposed to school, the slower their learning
rate seems to become.
On average, all groups except Hispanic NEH begin kindergarten with skills in letter recognition. By the
end of kindergarten, white and Asian children (regardless of language at home) have learned beginning and
ending sounds. By the end of third grade, all groups are learning literal inference, though white and Asian
EH children are beginning to learn how to extrapolate (see Figure 3.7).
37
TABLE 3.7: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY RACE AND LANGUAGE—ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
W HITE STUDENTS
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
24.22
1.90
(0.0137)
-0.231
(0.0602)
3.53
(0.0251)
1.64
(0.0083)
0.205
17.86
42.08
-0.0169
-0.60
41.49
0.227
33.27
74.76
0.0773
39.41
114.17
BLACK STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-3.17
-0.283
(0.0281)
-0.0290
(0.1240)
-0.576
(0.0507)
-0.185
(0.0195)
-0.0306
-2.66
-5.83
-0.00212
-0.07
-5.91
-0.0370
-5.43
-11.33
-0.00870
-4.44
-15.77
HISPANIC ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-3.36
-0.0567
(0.0380)
0.366
(0.1770)
-0.437
(0.0676)
-0.0648
(0.0254)
-0.00613
-0.53
-3.89
0.0268
0.94
-2.95
-0.0280
-4.12
-7.07
-0.00305
-1.56
-8.62
HISPAN IC NON -ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-6.98
-0.293
(0.0450)
0.0404
(0.1790)
-0.880
(0.0629)
-0.0309
(0.0213)
-0.0317
-2.76
-9.74
0.00296
0.10
-9.64
-0.0564
-8.28
-17.92
-0.00146
-0.74
-18.67
Table Continues on Next Page
38
TABLE 3.7(CONT .): DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY RACE AND LANGUAGE—ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
ASIAN ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
3.85
0.312
(0.0817)
0.758
(0.3720)
-0.213
(0.1290)
-0.273
(0.0408)
0.0337
2.94
6.79
0.0556
1.95
8.75
-0.0136
-2.00
6.74
-0.0129
-6.56
0.18
ASIAN NON -ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME S TUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
0.221
(0.0837)
0.501
(0.4730)
-0.327
(0.1560)
-0.235
(0.0350)
-0.56
0.0239
2.08
1.51
0.0367
1.29
2.81
-0.0210
-3.08
-0.27
-0.0111
-5.64
-5.92
Estimates in bold are significantly different from the corresponding estimate for white students at the 5 percent level.
Description of models is provided in Appendix A.
39
FIGURE 3.7: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY RACE AND LANGUAGE—ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
ECLS Reading Scores by Race and Language
110
7-EXTRAPOLATION
70
5-WORD IN CONTEXT
50
4-SIGHT WORDS
3-ENDING SOUNDS
2-BEGINNING SOUNDS
30
Score
90
6-LITERAL INFERENCE
10
1-LETTER RECOGNITION
Kindergarten
1st Grade
2nd and 3rd Grades
Asian EH
Black
Hispanic Non-EH
Asian Non-EH
Hispanic EH
White
Math
Table 3.8 breaks out race and ethnic groups by their language proficiency. An earlier table (Table 3.4)
shows Hispanic students experiencing slower math growth between the beginning of kindergarten and the
end of third grade than white students. In Table 3.8, this pattern generally holds true regardless of the
language spoken at home. Hispanic NEH students, however, make slightly less gain in math than Hispanic
EH students.
As presented earlier in Table 3.4, Asian and white students learn math at about the same rate between the
beginning of kindergarten and the end of third grade, with some vacillation. In Table 3.8, the same pattern
emerges for Asian NEH children. Asian NEH children start kindergarten with an average test score that
does not differ significantly from that of white children but gain less than white children during
kindergarten. Nearly 25 percent of Asian students did not take the math assessment in kindergarten but
did qualify in first grade. The introduction of these students to the sample may drop the average math
score for this subgroup in first grade, thus explaining the larger deficit. In second and third grades, Asian
NEH students are making greater gains and shrinking the gap that widened during first grade.
Asian EH students start kindergarten with an average math score almost two points higher than white
students. In kindergarten, Asian EH children and white children gain similar points on the math
assessment. Unlike Asian NEH children, Asian EH children gain significantly more than white children in
the summer between kindergarten and first grade. Like Asian NEH students, Asian EH students fall
behind in math during first grade, but regain the advantage in second and third grades.
40
By the end of third grade, white students a nd both groups of Asian students are, on average, learning place
value, as illustrated in Figure 3.8. In sum, by the end of third grade, Asian EH students have scores similar
to white students in both reading and math, and Asian NEH students are behind white students only in
reading. Black and Hispanic students fall behind in gaining math skills, and so end third grade on average
not yet learning place value.
41
TABLE 3.8: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY RACE AND LANGUAGE —- ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
W HITE STUDENTS
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
24.22
1.90
(0.0137)
-0.231
(0.0602)
3.53
(0.0251)
1.64
(0.0083)
0.205
17.86
42.08
-0.0169
-0.60
41.49
0.227
33.27
74.76
0.0773
39.41
114.17
BLACK STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-3.17
-0.283
(0.0281)
-0.0290
(0.1240)
-0.576
(0.0507)
-0.185
(0.0195)
-0.0306
-2.66
-5.83
-0.00212
-0.07
-5.91
-0.0370
-5.43
-11.33
-0.00870
-4.44
-15.77
HISPANIC ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-0.0567
(0.0380)
0.366
(0.1770)
-0.437
(0.0676)
-0.0648
(0.0254)
-3.36
-0.00613
-0.53
-3.89
0.0268
0.94
-2.95
-0.0280
-4.12
-7.07
-0.00305
-1.56
-8.62
HISPANIC NON -ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-0.293
(0.0450)
0.0404
(0.1790)
-0.880
(0.0629)
-0.0309
(0.0213)
-0.0317
-2.76
-9.74
0.00296
0.10
-9.64
-0.0564
-8.28
-17.92
-0.00146
-0.74
-18.67
Table Continues on Next Page
42
-6.98
TABLE 3.8 (C ONT.): DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY RACE AND LANGUAGE —- ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
ASIAN ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
3.85
0.312
(0.0817)
0.758
(0.3720)
-0.213
(0.1290)
-0.273
(0.0408)
0.0337
2.94
6.79
0.0556
1.95
8.75
-0.0136
-2.00
6.74
-0.0129
-6.56
0.18
ASIAN NON -ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME S TUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-0.0567
(0.0380)
0.366
(0.1770)
-0.437
(0.0676)
-0.0648
(0.0254)
-3.36
-0.00613
-0.53
-3.89
0.0268
0.94
-2.95
-0.0280
-4.12
-7.07
-0.00305
-1.56
-8.62
Estimates in bold are significantly different from the corresponding estimate for white students at the 5 percent level.
Descriptions of models are provided in Appendix A. Some of the numbers in the end of period column are off by 0.01 points
due to rounding.
43
FIGURE 3.8: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY RACE AND LANGUAGE—ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
7-RATE & MEASUREMENT
10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100
Score
ECLS Math Scores by Race and Language
6-PLACE VALUE
5-MULTIPLY/DIVIDE
4-ADD/SUBTRACT
3-ORDINALITY, SEQUENCE
2-RELATIVE SIZE
1-COUNT, NUMBER, SHAPE
Kindergarten
1st Grade
2nd and 3rd Grades
Asian EH
Black
Hispanic Non-EH
Asian Non-EH
Hispanic EH
White
Economic Status29
Reading
The findings presented in Table 3.9 show that low-income students begin kindergarten more than 5 points
behind higher-income students on the reading assessment. Low-income children are learning to recognize
letters at the beginning of kindergarten (see Figure 3.9). Higher-income students are learning beginning
sounds to words in the first half of kindergarten, on average, but low-income children are learning
beginning sounds at the end of kindergarten.
The learning rates differ slightly in kindergarten and continue to diverge significantly in the summer and in
first grade. By the end of first grade, low-income children are just learning how to infer meaning from text,
whereas higher-income children are beginning to learn the next more advanced skill, extrapolation (see
Figure 3.9). The gap between low-income and higher-income children in reading widens from kindergarten
through third grades. Because of the initial advantage and faster gain for higher-income children, lowincome children continue to lag behind. By the end of third grade, higher-income children are starting to
learn extrapolation skills, whereas their low-income peers are on average still learning the less advanced
skill of literal inference.
Low-income status is defined as having family income less than 1.85 times the poverty line, representing the cutoff for
eligibility in the federal free and reduced price lunch program. Our measure of low-income status therefore includes many
students whose families live above the poverty line, but these families are considered low-income by the federal government.
29
44
TABLE 3.9: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY ECONOMIC STATUS —ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
HIGHER -INCOME S TUDENTS
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
25.03
1.93
(0.0140)
-0.0636
(0.0623)
3.52
(0.0247)
1.61
(0.0083)
0.209
18.21
43.25
-0.00466
-0.16
43.08
0.225
33.11
76.20
0.103
38.79
114.98
LOW -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM HIGHER -INCOME S TUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-0.277
(0.0209)
-0.273
(0.0917)
-0.541
(0.0375)
-0.0665
(0.0132)
-5.37
-0.0299
-2.61
-7.98
-0.0200
-0.70
-8.68
-0.0347
-5.09
-13.77
-0.00427
-1.60
-15.37
Each estimate in bold is significantly different from the corresponding estimate for higher-income students at the 5 percent
level. Descriptions of models are provided in Appendix A.
45
FIGURE 3.9: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY ECONOMIC STATUS —ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
ECLS Reading Scores by Economic Status
110
7-EXTRAPOLATION
70
5-WORD IN CONTEXT
50
Score
90
6-LITERAL INFERENCE
4-SIGHT WORDS
3-ENDING SOUNDS
30
2-BEGINNING SOUNDS
10
1-LETTER RECOGNITION
Kindergarten
1st Grade
2nd and 3rd Grades
Higher-Income
Low-Income
Math
As with reading, low-income students begin kindergarten more than 5 points behind their peers in
mathematics. Figure 3.10 shows that the average higher-income student is learning to compare sizes at
kindergarten entry. In contrast, low-income students are learning to compare sizes halfway through
kindergarten, on average.
The difference between the learning rates of low-income and higher-income students in kindergarten and
first grade is about a quarter of a point per month. Although this difference may seem small, it
accumulates over nine months of school, and compounds the initial deficit with which low-income
students enter kindergarten. The math learning gap between the economically advantaged and
disadvantaged exists on the first day of school and persists through third grade, so that low-income
students are nearly a grade behind throughout these grades. For example, higher-income students are on
average learning the concept of place value in second grade, whereas low-income students are learning
about place value in third grade.
46
TABLE 3.10: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY ECONOMIC STATUS —ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
HIGHER -INCOME S TUDENTS
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
19.91
1.72
(0.0120)
0.514
(0.0626)
2.48
(0.0221)
1.23
(0.0061)
0.209
16.24
36.15
0.0441
1.32
37.48
0.201
23.33
60.81
0.0999
29.68
90.49
LOW -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM HIGHER -INCOME S TUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-0.248
(0.0180)
-0.0677
(0.0881)
-0.252
(0.0313)
-0.0805
(0.0100)
-5.50
-0.0301
-2.34
-7.83
-0.00582
-0.17
-8.01
-0.0204
-2.37
-10.38
-0.00651
-1.94
-12.32
Each estimate in bold is significantly different from the corresponding estimate for higher-income students at the 5 percent
level. Descriptions of models are provided in Appendix A.
47
FIGURE 3.10: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY ECONOMIC STATUS —ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
7-RATE & MEASUREMENT
10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100
Score
ECLS Math Scores by Economic Status
6-PLACE VALUE
5-MULTIPLY/DIVIDE
4-ADD/SUBTRACT
3-ORDINALITY, SEQUENCE
2-RELATIVE SIZE
1-COUNT, NUMBER, SHAPE
Kindergarten
1st Grade
2nd and 3rd Grades
Higher-Income
Low-Income
Race and Income
Reading
Previous analyses with just income and just race indicated large disparities in average reading scores and
growth rates. Combining the two highlights which subgroups experience the greatest challenges in
learning. The analyses of learning differences by race/ethnicity and income status are presented in Table
3.11 and illustrated in Figure 3.11.
The reading gains made by white low-income children differ from those made by higher-income white
children. White low-income children begin kindergarten with a 4.57-point disadvantage on the reading
assessment. The difference in gains widens slightly in kindergarten and is widest in first grade. Thus the
accumulated disadvantage grows to more than 10 points by the end of third grade.
Comparing the white low-income disadvantage and the black low-income disadvantage indicates the
importance of race. Despite similar economic backgrounds, white low-income children are significantly
better off than black low-income children, when both are compared to white higher-income children. In
first through third grades, white low-income children have deficits in reading gain half the size of black
low-income children’s deficits. White low-income children do not fall as far behind white higher-income
children in learning reading as do black low-income children.30
Early in school, low-income children of black, Hispanic, and Asian ethnicities gain substantially less than
not only white-higher income children but also higher-income children of the same ethnicity. Black lowincome children start kindergarten with lower reading scores and gain significantly less in kindergarten
30
This comparison was tested explicitly in the HLM program.
48
than black higher-income children. 31 In first through third grades, however, black children at both income
strata learn at very similar rates.
Hispanic higher-income students start kindergarten with a deficit on the reading assessment compared to
white higher-income students. During elementary school, this deficit doubles in size. Although Hispanic
higher-income children gain reading skills at a similar pace as white higher-income children in kindergarten
and second and third grades, they gain significantly less in first grade. 32 This then leads to the large 8-point
accumulated deficit by the end of third grade.
Hispanic low-income students start kindergarten with the most substantial deficit on the reading
assessment, compared to white higher-income students. In each grade, the gap (the difference in
achievement levels) between Hispanic low-income students and white higher-income students increases
(reflecting lower average growth among Hispanic students). The gap is widest in first grade, the year that
many Hispanic students excluded for a lack of English proficiency take the reading assessment for the first
time. In second and third grades, the gap shrinks dramatically. However, the overall gap remains large. By
the end of third grade, Hispanic low-income students’ original deficit almost triples in size and matches the
magnitude of the accumulated deficit faced by black low-income students.
The gap between higher- and low-income Asian children grows by the most and ends up being the largest
of the income gaps by race. Asian low-income children start kindergarten with a 4-point disadvantage
compared to white higher-income children on the reading assessment and a 7-point gap compared to their
higher-income Asian counterparts.
Asian higher-income children score the highest on the reading assessment in the fall of kindergarten and
make more gain in reading during kindergarten. In first grade, however, Asian higher-income children
make less gain in reading than white high-income children in first grade. 32 In second and third grades,
Asian higher-income students manifest the slowest reading gains of all the subgroups. Nevertheless,
higher-income Asian students end up almost 10 points ahead of their low-income Asian counterparts but
still behind white higher-income students.
Black higher-income children start kindergarten fewer than 2 points behind white higher-income children
on the reading assessment. However, the deficit increases over the first four years of elementary school. By
the end of third grade, black higher-income children have gained 21 points less than white higher-income
children. Interestingly, black higher-income children score higher than white low-income children on the
initial assessment, but by the end of third grade have fallen behind, due in part to a dramatically lower rate
of learning in first grade and a slightly lower rate in second and third grades.
Figure 3.11a focuses on the higher-income children, with separate lines to represent the learning rate of
each race/ethnicity. The reading gains made by Asian higher-income children outpace the rest of the
higher-income children. However, in second and third grades, white students catch up to Asian students.
Black higher-income and Hispanic higher-income children make slower progress than white students and
Asian students of the same socioeconomic stratum. All the higher-income children are advancing towards
learning extrapolation, with white students and Asian students making the quickest gain.
Figure 3.11b focuses on low-income children. Again, black and Hispanic students do not gain as quickly as
white and Asian students. But unlike in the higher-income cluster, among the low-income students, Asian
This comparison was tested explicitly in the HLM program.
Again, this is partially attributable to the introduction of students who passed the OLDS English-language screening test in
first grade and may exhibit weaker than average reading skills.
31
32
49
students keep pace with white students. The difference in gain between Asian and white students is smaller
among the low-income students than among the higher-income students. Among low-income students,
white students not only catch up to Asian students in second and third grades, but also begin to make very
slightly stronger gains. By the end of third grade, Asian and white low-income children are advancing
towards learning extrapolation (like most of the higher-income students).
50
TABLE 3.11: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY RACE AND INCOME—ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
W HITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
25.51
1.95
(0.0163)
-0.159
(0.0723)
3.64
(0.0290)
1.64
(0.0095)
0.210
18.32
43.83
-0.0116
-0.41
43.42
0.233
34.25
77.67
0.0774
39.47
117.15
W HITE LOW -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-0.169
(0.0296)
-0.242
(0.1300)
-0.384
(0.0575)
-0.0202
(0.0193)
-4.57
-0.0183
-1.59
-6.17
-0.0177
-0.62
-6.79
-0.0246
-3.61
-10.41
-0.000952
-0.49
-10.89
BLACK HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-0.164
(0.0515)
0.337
(0.2310)
-0.626
(0.0877)
-0.171
(0.0377)
-1.62
-0.0177
-1.54
-3.17
0.0247
0.87
-2.30
-0.0401
-5.89
-8.19
-0.00806
-4.11
-12.30
BLACK LOW -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-5.69
-0.405
(0.0321)
-0.311
(0.1390)
-0.699
(0.0591)
-0.193
(0.0219)
51
-0.0437
-3.81
-9.50
-0.0228
-0.80
-10.30
-0.0448
-6.58
-16.88
-0.00910
-4.64
-21.52
TABLE 3.11 (CONT .): DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY RACE AND I NCOME—ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
HISPANIC HIGHER -INCOME S TUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-3.38
-0.0322
(0.0460)
0.280
(0.2080)
-0.394
(0.0782)
-0.0518
(0.0267)
-0.00348
-0.30
-3.69
0.0205
0.72
-2.96
-0.0252
-3.71
-6.67
-0.00244
-1.25
-7.91
HISPANIC LOW -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-0.337
(0.0381)
0.133
(0.1660)
-1.00
(0.0587)
-0.0621
(0.0220)
-7.87
-0.0364
-3.18
-11.05
0.00977
0.34
-10.70
-0.0643
-9.44
-20.14
-0.00293
-1.49
-21.64
ASIAN HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
0.342
(0.0801)
0.531
(0.3880)
-0.211
(0.1290)
-0.281
(0.0366)
3.16
0.0369
3.22
6.38
0.0389
1.37
7.74
-0.0135
-1.99
5.76
-0.0133
-6.77
-1.01
ASIAN LOW -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE HIGHER -INCOME S TUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-0.00597
(0.0799)
0.628
(0.4420)
-0.657
(0.1430)
-0.208
(0.0397)
-4.72
-0.000644
-0.06
-4.77
0.0460
1.62
-3.15
-0.0422
-6.19
-9.34
-0.00981
-5.00
-14.34
Each estimate in bold is significantly different from the corresponding estimate for white higher-income students at the 5
percent level. Descriptions of models are provided in Appendix A.
52
FIGURE 3.11A: READING L EARNING RATES BY RACE (HIGHER INCOME)—ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
ECLS Reading Scores by Race, Higher Inc
110
7-EXTRAPOLATION
70
5-WORD IN CONTEXT
50
4-SIGHT WORDS
30
3-ENDING SOUNDS
2-BEGINNING SOUNDS
1-LETTER RECOGNITION
10
Score
90
6-LITERAL INFERENCE
Kindergarten
1st Grade
2nd and 3rd Grades
Asian
Hispanic
Black
White
53
FIGURE 3.11B: READING L EARNING RATES BY RACE(LOW -INCOME)—ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
ECLS Reading Scores by Race, Low Income
110
7-EXTRAPOLATION
70
5-WORD IN CONTEXT
50
Score
90
6-LITERAL INFERENCE
4-SIGHT WORDS
30
3-ENDING SOUNDS
2-BEGINNING SOUNDS
10
1-LETTER RECOGNITION
Kindergarten
1st Grade
Asian
Hispanic
2nd and 3rd Grades
Black
White
Math
Results from math models that combine race/ethnicity and income show how being low-income and
belonging to a minority group is associated with compounded disadvantages in learning.
Earlier, we showed that low-income students gain about two or three hundredths of a standard deviation
less per month in math than higher-income students. Table 3.4 showed that black students gain less than
white students throughout these early grades, though the difference shrinks with time (-0.04 SD in
kindergarten, -0.03 SD in first grade, and -0.01 SD in second and third grades). Hispanic students show
even less of a deficit with white students (-0.03 SD in kindergarten; -0.02 SD in first grade, and -0.001 SD
in second and third grades).
When breaking these findings out by race and income, black and Hispanic low-income students are at an
even greater disadvantage. Black low-income and Hispanic low-income children begin kindergarten with
the lowest scores on the math assessment and make the slowest gain in kindergarten. Each of the learning
rate differences discussed in the previous paragraph increases for low-income children. Black low-income
children gain 0.05 SD less than white higher-income children during kindergarten, 0.04 SD less in first
grade, and 0.01 SD less in second and third grades. Hispanic low-income children experience a similar
compounding effect in their gap relative to white higher-income children. Hispanic low-income children’s
learning rate is lower than white higher-income students by 0.05 SD per month in kindergarten, 0.03 SD
per month in first grade, and 0.004 SD per month in second and third grade.
In the fall of kindergarten, black higher-income children’s math scores look like those of white low-income
children. But their achievement diverges over time. Black higher-income students make less gain in math
54
than white low-income students during every time period. By the end of third grade, black higher-income
children are behind white children of both income strata.
At the start of kindergarten, Hispanic higher-income students earn lower math scores than white and black
higher-income students, but face less of a deficit than black students throughout the primary school years.
Hispanic children from higher-income families finish third grade with cumulative average scores higher
than black higher-income children and their Hispanic low-income peers. However, these scores are nearly
7 points behind those of white higher-income children.
Asian higher-income children start school with an average math score more than a point higher than white
higher-income children. Their learning rates are even during kindergarten. But Asian students gain points
on the math assessment at a slower pace in first grade and a faster pace in second and third grades, relative
to white students. By the end of third grade, Asian higher-income children are 1.67 points ahead of white
higher-income children.
Asian low-income children start with a lower average math score than white higher-income children.
During kindergarten, they make less gain in math than white higher-income students. In first grade, they
make even slower gains in learning math, but gain faster than white higher-income students in second and
third grades, relative to white students. In second and third grades, Asian children, of both income
categories, are the only students making greater math gains on average than white higher-income children.
Thus Asian higher-income students retain their original advantage on the math assessment by the end of
third grade.
The finding that Asian students learn less in math during first grade than white students but later learn
more than white students is not perplexing when taken in context of the change in sample. Many Asian
students who were excluded from the first rounds of data collection for lack of English proficiency were
included in the first grade data collection. These students may demonstrate below-average math skills and
push down the gain estimated during that period (see Appendix A for more on sample selection).
Figures 3.12a and 3.12b illustrate the learning rates in math for higher-income and low-income children
respectively. Figure 3.12a shows that black students, despite their higher-income background, have a
distinctly slower learning rate than the Asian, white, and Hispanic higher-income groups. Figure 3.12b
suggests a similarity between the learning rates of black and Hispanic low-income children; both are
behind Asian and white low-income children’s learning rates. Figures 3.12a and 3.12b illustrate that Asian
and white students learn at nearly the same rate, regardless of income status. Higher-income students are
already learning place value (see Figure 3.12a), whereas the low-income black and Hispanic students are
not yet there.
55
TABLE 3.12: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY RACE AND INCOME—ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
W HITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
20.76
1.77
(0.0142)
0.524
(0.0766)
2.54
(0.0273)
1.24
(0.0071)
0.215
16.64
37.39
0.0450
1.35
38.74
0.206
23.93
62.68
0.0791
29.77
92.45
W HITE LOW -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE HIGH ER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-0.138
(0.0272)
-0.177
(0.1290)
-0.176
(0.0477)
-0.0412
(0.0147)
-4.35
-0.0167
-1.30
-5.64
-0.0152
-0.46
-6.10
-0.0143
-1.66
-7.76
-0.00263
-0.99
-8.75
BLACK HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-0.303
(0.0409)
-0.0632
(0.2010)
-0.384
(0.0692)
-0.0942
(0.0260)
-3.67
-0.0368
-2.86
-6.53
-0.00543
-0.16
-6.69
-0.0310
-3.61
-10.30
-0.00602
-2.27
-12.57
BLACK LOW -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-0.443
(0.0278)
-0.187
(0.1440)
-0.445
(0.0514)
-0.185
(0.0167)
-0.0538
-4.17
-10.99
-0.0160
-0.48
-11.47
-0.0360
-4.19
-15.66
-0.0118
-4.45
-20.11
Table Continues on Next Page
56
-6.82
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
HISPANIC HIGHER -INCOME S TUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-4.18
-0.108
(0.0360)
-0.198
(0.1770)
-0.129
(0.0628)
0.00648
(0.0187)
-0.0131
-1.02
-5.20
-0.0170
-0.51
-5.71
-0.0104
-1.21
-6.92
0.000414
0.16
-6.77
HISPANIC LOW -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-8.25
-0.388
(0.0278)
0.226
(0.1460)
-0.343
(0.0487)
-0.0628
(0.0165)
-0.0472
-3.66
-11.91
0.0194
0.58
-11.33
-0.0277
-3.23
-14.55
-0.00401
-1.51
-16.06
ASIAN HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
1.64
-0.00469
(0.0611)
0.212
(0.3500)
-0.281
(0.1010)
0.0906
(0.0276)
-0.000569
-0.04
1.59
0.0182
0.55
2.14
-0.0227
-2.64
-0.51
0.00579
2.18
1.67
ASIAN LOW -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE HIGHER -INCOME S TUDENTS)
Before Kindergarten
During Kindergarten
Summer K-1st
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-4.55
-0.144
(0.0687)
0.754
(0.3970)
-0.617
(0.1230)
0.0745
(0.0302)
-0.0175
-1.36
-5.91
0.0648
1.94
-3.97
-0.0499
-5.81
-9.78
0.00476
1.79
-7.99
Each estimates in bold is significantly different from the corresponding estimate for white higher-income students at the 5
percent level. Descriptions of models are provided in Appendix A.
57
FIGURE 3.12A: MATH L EARNING RATES BY RACE (HIGHER INCOME)—ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
7-RATE & MEASUREMENT
10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100
Score
ECLS Math Scores by Race, Higher Inc
6-PLACE VALUE
5-MULTIPLY/DIVIDE
4-ADD/SUBTRACT
3-ORDINALITY, SEQUENCE
2-RELATIVE SIZE
1-COUNT, NUMBER, SHAPE
Kindergarten 1st Grade
2nd and 3rd Grades
Asian
Hispanic
Black
White
FIGURE 3.12B: MATH L EARNING RATES BY RACE (LOW-I NCOME)—ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
7-RATE & MEASUREMENT
10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100
Score
ECLS Math Scores by Race, Low Income
6-PLACE VALUE
5-MULTIPLY/DIVIDE
4-ADD/SUBTRACT
3-ORDINALITY, SEQUENCE
2-RELATIVE SIZE
1-COUNT, NUMBER, SHAPE
Kindergarten
1st Grade
2nd and 3rd Grades
Asian
Hispanic
Black
White
58
Summary
Differences in learning by gender, race/ethnicity, and economic status have already begun when children
enter formal schooling in kindergarten and tend to continue or even grow during elementary school.
Race/ethnicity is a substantial factor in these learning rate differences. Black and Hispanic students are
mostly lagging behind white and Asian students, with black students consistently worst off.
In reading and math, low-income children start school with substantially lower test scores than their more
advantaged peers, and learn at a slightly lower rate in each grade. The learning deficit decreases in later
grades (that is, low-income students learn at a lower rate throughout, but their learning rate is closer to
their higher-income peers in second and third grade), but the achievement gap grows throughout the early
school years.
The analyses interacting race/ethnicity with income suggest the relative importance of race in reading and
math gaps in achievement and learning. Black and Hispanic children, whether in higher-income or lowincome families, gain consistently less in reading and math than Asian and white peers of similar economic
background. The analyses that explore the interaction of race/ethnicity and language status suggest a
similar conclusion. Black children start kindergarten with lower scores and improve at a slower rate, on
average, than either Asian or Hispanic students in non-English homes.
Secondary School
Gender
Reading
In high school, females hold a significant initial advantage in reading achievement. Findings presented in
Table 3.13 show that female students score nearly 2 points higher in eighth grade than their male peers.
Females make reading gains similar to those of males early in high school and slightly higher between tenth
and twelfth grades, leaving them with about the same point advantage at the end of high school as they
had at the beginning. Both groups are, on average, moving from learning simple to more complex
inferences.
TABLE 3.13: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY G ENDER—S ECONDARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
M ALE STUDENTS
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
27.30
0.156
(0.0049)
0.0809
(0.0066)
0.0204
3.74
31.04
0.00876
1.94
32.98
FEMALE STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM MALE S TUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
1.87
-0.00670
(0.0066)
0.0190
(0.0083)
-0.000875
-0.16
1.71
0.00206
0.46
2.17
Each estimate in bold is significantly different from the corresponding estimate for male students at the 5 percent level.
Descriptions of models are provided in Appendix A.
59
45
FIGURE 3.13: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY G ENDER—S ECONDARY SCHOOL
NELS Reading Scores by Gender
30
2-Simple Inferences
20
25
Score
35
40
3-Complex Inferences
8-10
10-12
Female
Male
Math
Perhaps not surprisingly, given the elementary school student results presented earlier in Table 3.2, females
start high school with slightly lower math achievement than their male peers (a difference of about a half
point). These differences, presented in Table 3.13, do not increase between eighth and tenth grades but do
increase between tenth and twelfth grades when female students gain about 0.84 points less per period on
the math test than boys. This gap in gain represents about 0.01 of a standard deviation on the tenth-grade
test. By the end of high school, the initial male advantage on the math assessment increases to an
advantage of almost 2 points.
60
TABLE 3.14: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY G ENDER—S ECONDARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
M ALE STUDENTS
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
38.44
0.330
(0.0058)
0.200
(0.0057)
0.0296
7.92
46.36
0.0152
4.81
51.17
FEMALE STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM MALE S TUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-0.56
-0.0117
(0.0076)
-0.0442
(0.0076)
-0.00105
-0.28
-0.84
-0.00336
-1.06
-1.90
Each estimate in bold is significantly different from the corresponding estimate for male students at the 5 percent level.
Descriptions of models are provided in Appendix A.
FIGURE 3.14: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY G ENDER—S ECONDARY SCHOOL
60
NELS Math Scores by Gender
50
4-Intermediate Level Math
40
2-Fractions and Exponents
30
Score
3-Simple Problem Solving
20
1-Single Operations
8-10
10-12
Female
Male
Race/Ethnicity
Reading
White students hold an initial advantage in reading achievement over black and Hispanic students (see
Table 3.15) but not Asian students. Black students score 5.49 points lower than white students and
61
Hispanic students score 4.83 points lower than white students. These differences in initial status are
compounded by differences in reading gains made during high school.
There are statistically significant race/ethnicity differences in reading gains during high school. Between
ninth and tenth grades, white students gain very slightly more than black students and Hispanic students
but less than Asian students. Between tenth and twelfth grades, white students gain at a slightly faster rate
than black students but at a slower rate than Hispanic students and Asian students. By the end of high
school, black students and Hispanic students are learning simple inference and abstract points, which
white students and Asian students, on average, learned in eighth grade.
TABLE 3.15: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY RACE/ETHNICITY — SECONDARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
W HITE STUDENTS
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
29.35
0.157
(0.0038)
0.0885
(0.0049)
0.0205
3.76
33.10
0.00959
2.12
35.23
BLACK STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-5.49
-0.0360
(0.0114)
-0.0129
(0.0140)
-0.00471
-0.86
-6.36
-0.00140
-0.31
-6.67
HISPANIC STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-4.83
-0.0122
(0.0114)
0.0191
(0.0133)
-0.00160
-0.29
-5.12
0.00207
0.46
-4.66
ASIAN STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-0.26
0.0231
(0.0127)
0.0477
(0.0190)
0.00302
0.55
0.29
0.00516
1.14
1.43
Each estimates in bold is significantly different from the corresponding estimate for white students at the 5 percent level.
Descriptions of models are provided in Appendix A.
62
NELS Reading Scores by Race
3-Complex Inferences
30
2-Simple Inferences
20
25
Score
35
40
45
FIGURE 3.15: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY RACE/ ETHNICITY—SECONDARY SCHOOL
8-10
10-12
Asian
Hispanic
Black
White
Math
Table 3.16 shows the significant race/ethnic differences in math achievement at the start of high school.
In eighth grade, white students have an initial advantage over black and Hispanic students. However,
Asian students have an initial 2.71 advantage over white students and keep pace with white students
throughout high school. The deficits increase early in high school.
Between eighth and tenth grade, black students and Hispanic students make slower gains in math than
white students, with black students falling farthest behind. Asian students gain 2.71 more points than white
students during this time period. Some of these differences in gains persist later in high school. Between
tenth and twelfth grades, white students gain more than black students, and Asian students gain more than
white students. There are no significant differences in math gains between white students and Hispanic
students.
By the end of high school, gaps between groups increase slightly. For example, the initial 9-point
advantage of white students over black students increases by about a point, and the initial advantage of
Asian students over white students also increases by about a point. These changes translate into wide gaps
in skills. By the end of high school, Asian students are beginning to learn intermediate-level math
concepts, whereas black and Hispanic students are far behind, learning fractions and decimals, math
concepts that the white and Asian students learned by the start of eighth grade. These gaps can also be
compared to the gender gaps. Black and Hispanic students end twelfth grade with scores 11 and 7 points
behind those of white students, the male-female difference in math scores is only around 2 points.
63
TABLE 3.16: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY RACE/ ETHNICITY—S ECONDARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
W HITE STUDENTS
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
39.75
0.333
(0.0041)
0.176
(0.0045)
0.0299
7.99
47.75
0.0134
4.23
51.97
BLACK STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-9.13
-0.0625
(0.0168)
-0.00472
(0.0124)
-0.00560
-1.50
-10.63
-0.000359
-0.11
-10.74
HISPANIC STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-7.00
-0.0329
(0.0121)
0.0168
(0.0128)
-0.00295
-0.79
-7.79
0.00128
0.40
-7.39
ASIAN STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
2.71
0.0326
(0.0174)
0.0299
(0.0190)
0.00292
0.78
3.49
0.00228
0.72
4.20
Each estimate in bold is significantly different from the corresponding estimate for white students at the 5 percent level.
Descriptions of models are provided in Appendix A.
64
FIGURE 3.16: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY RACE/ ETHNICITY—SECONDARY SCHOOL
60
NELS Math Scores by Race
50
4-Intermediate Level Math
40
2-Fractions and Exponents
30
Score
3-Simple Problem Solving
20
1-Single Operations
8-10
10-12
Asian
Hispanic
Black
White
Language Status
Reading
Table 3.17 shows that students from homes where English is not the primary language (NEH) hold an
initial disadvantage in reading achievement compared to students from homes where English is the
primary language (EH). NEH students start high school with an average eighth grade reading score 3.96
points lower than EH students.
There is no significant difference between NEH and EH students’ learning between eighth and tenth
grades. However, between tenth and twelfth grades, NEH students actually gain about a point more than
EH students (0.004 SD per month). Figure 3.17 illustrates the slight narrowing of the learning rate
difference over tenth to twelfth grades. Similarly, in elementary school, the NEH disadvantage in learning
gains diminishes after first grade (Table 3.5), though NEH students still have slower growth rates in
second and third grades. Interestingly, the effect size per month gain for NEH students between tenth and
twelfth grades is equal in magnitude to the effect size per month loss for NEH students in second and
third grades (see Table 3.5).
65
TABLE 3.17: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY LANGUAGE STATUS —S ECONDARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
ENGL ISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS
Before High School
28.60
8th Grade to 10th Grade
0.152
(0.0035)
0.0873
(0.0044)
10th Grade to 12th Grade
0.0199
3.65
32.25
0.00945
2.09
34.34
NON -ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME S TUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
0.00368
(0.0106)
0.0406
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-3.96
0.000481
0.09
-3.87
0.00440
0.97
-2.90
(0.0132)
Each estimate in bold is significantly different from the corresponding estimate for EH students at the 5 percent level.
Descriptions of models are provided in Appendix A.
45
FIGURE 3.17: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY LANGUAGE STATUS —S ECONDARY SCHOOL
NELS Reading Scores by Language Status
25
30
2-Simple Inferences
20
Score
35
40
3-Complex Inferences
8-10
10-12
EH
Non-EH
66
Math
Table 3.18 shows that NEH students start high school with a significant disadvantage in math achievement
compared to EH students. NEH students score 4.01 points lower than English speaking students at the
end of eighth grade.
This difference does not grow early in high school, when NEH students make the same gain as EH
students. But like in reading, this pattern reverses between tenth and twelfth grades when NEH students
gain significantly more than EH students (0.75 points). By the end of high school, both groups are moving
past simple problem solving and beginning to learn more advanced math concepts. In elementary school,
we also see that the initial advantage in math learning gains of EH households observed in kindergarten
and first grade is not observed during second and third grades. This suggests that the longer the NEH
students remain in school, the more their learning rates approach those of EH students.
TABLE 3.18: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY LANGUAGE STATUS —S ECONDARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
38.52
0.325
(0.0040)
0.176
(0.0040)
0.0292
7.80
46.32
0.0133
4.21
50.53
NON -ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME S TUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
At End of Period
-0.0103
(0.0131)
0.0314
(0.0129)
-4.01
-0.000926
-0.25
-4.26
0.00238
0.75
-3.51
Each estimate in bold is significantly different from the corresponding estimate for EH students at the 5 percent level.
Descriptions of models are provided in Appendix A.
67
FIGURE 3.18: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY LANGUAGE STATUS —S ECONDARY SCHOOL
60
NELS Math Scores by Language Status
50
4-Intermediate Level Math
40
2-Fractions and Exponents
30
Score
3-Simple Problem Solving
20
1-Single Operations
8-10
10-12
EH
Non-EH
Race and Language
Reading
As seen in Table 3.15, there are significant differences in average reading achievement between white
students and racial/ethnic minority students in the spring of eighth grade. In Table 3.19, we split these
findings by language status.
Black students start high school with less of a deficit than Hispanic NEH students. But because black
students make less gain than Hispanic students, regardless of language status, black students end high
school with the deepest deficit on the reading assessment.
Hispanic students regardless of EH status start high school with a lower average reading score than white
students, but the deficit is twice as great for Hispanic NEH students than for Hispanic EH students.
Hispanic students of both NEH and EH subgroups gain points at a similar rate to white students
throughout high school.
As seen in an earlier table (Table 3.15), Asian students do not differ from white students in terms of
average reading achievement in the spring of eighth grade but gain more than white students during high
school. Breaking out the Asian subgroup by language background shows some surprising findings. Asian
EH students keep pace with white students throughout high school. From tenth to twelfth grade, Asian
NEH students make greater gains in reading than white students (2.18 points more). This runs counter to
the elementary findings where the later elementary grades showed slower reading gains for Asian students.
Here the later secondary grades prove more successful for Asian students.
68
TABLE 3.19: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY RACE AND LANGUAGE—S ECONDARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
W HITE STUDENTS
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
29.35
0.157
(0.0038)
0.0884
(0.0049)
0.0205
3.76
33.11
0.00957
2.12
35.23
BLACK STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-5.47
-0.0356
(0.0115)
-0.0129
(0.0140)
-0.00465
-0.85
-6.32
-0.00139
-0.31
-6.63
HISPANIC ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-0.0184
(0.0150)
0.00231
(0.0174)
-3.17
-0.00240
-0.44
-2.81
0.000250
0.06
-2.39
HISPANIC NON -ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-0.00517
(0.0161)
0.0324
(0.0185)
-6.21
-0.000676
-0.12
-5.83
0.00350
0.78
-5.38
ASIAN ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
0.0221
(0.0188)
0.0372
(0.0233)
0.36
0.00288
0.53
0.81
0.00402
0.89
1.37
ASIAN NON -ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME S TUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
0.0288
(0.0168)
0.0911
(0.0183)
-1.20
0.00376
0.69
-0.79
0.00986
2.18
-0.35
Each estimate in bold is significantly different from the corresponding estimate for white students at the 5 percent level.
Descriptions of models are provided in Appendix A.
69
FIGURE 3.19: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY RACE AND LANGUAGE—S ECONDARY SCHOOL
45
NELS Reading Scores by Race and Language
30
2-Simple Inferences
20
25
Score
35
40
3-Complex Inferences
8-10
10-12
Asian EH
Black
Hispanic Non-EH
Asian Non-EH
Hispanic EH
White
Math
Results presented earlier in Table 3.16 show that black and Hispanic students have lower initial math
achievement at the end of eighth grade and slower math gains throughout high school than white students,
though Hispanic students learn at a similar rate between tenth and twelfth grades. Table 3.20 teases apart
these race/ethnicity findings by language status.
Compared to white students, Hispanic NEH students start high school with a greater math deficit than
Hispanic EH students. Throughout high school, Hispanic students—both NEH and EH—make similar
gains in math as white students. This differs from the findings presented in Table 3.16 in which Hispanic
students overall learn very slightly but significantly less in eighth to tenth grades than white students. The
difference is likely because the estimates for Hispanic students by language status are less precise than the
overall results for Hispanic students because of the smaller sample sizes.
In general, Asian students have higher initial math achievement than white students and during high
school, make similar gains to white students (see Table 3.16). These patterns hold for Asian students
regardless of language status, with one exception. Asian NEH students gain math points slightly but
significantly faster than white students between tenth and twelfth grades (0.072 points per month, or 0.005
SD). This provides further support for the general theme that once NEH students have been in school for
a few years their learning rates increase.
70
TABLE 3.20: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY RACE AND LANGUAGE—S ECONDARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
W HITE STUDENTS
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
39.76
0.333
(0.0041)
0.176
(0.0045)
0.0299
8.00
47.75
0.0134
4.22
51.98
BLACK STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-9.11
-0.0623
(0.0168)
-0.00469
(0.0125)
-0.00559
-1.50
-10.61
-0.000356
-0.11
-10.72
HISPANIC ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-0.0332
(0.0151)
0.00158
(0.0154)
-5.17
-0.00298
-0.80
-5.97
0.000120
0.04
-5.93
HISPANIC NON -ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-0.0317
(0.0177)
0.0282
(0.0184)
-8.50
-0.00284
-0.76
-9.26
0.00214
0.68
-8.58
ASIAN ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
0.0245
(0.0175)
0.00612
(0.0228)
2.82
0.00220
0.59
3.41
0.000465
0.15
3.56
ASIAN NON -ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME S TUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
0.0309
(0.0300)
0.0719
(0.0216)
2.97
0.00277
0.74
3.71
0.00547
1.72
5.44
Each estimate in bold is significantly different from the corresponding estimate for white students at the 5 percent level.
Descriptions of models are provided in Appendix A.
71
FIGURE 3.20: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY RACE AND LANGUAGE—S ECONDARY SCHOOL
60
NELS Math Scores by Race and Language
50
4-Intermediate Level Math
40
2-Fractions and Exponents
30
Score
3-Simple Problem Solving
20
1-Single Operations
8-10
10-12
Asian EH
Black
Hispanic Non-EH
Asian Non-EH
Hispanic EH
White
Economic Status
Reading
In the spring of eighth grade, the average reading achievement for students from low-income families
(below 185 percent of the poverty line) is 3.62 points below that of higher-income students (see Table
3.21). This gap continues to widen early in high school when low-income students gain 0.71 points less
than higher-income students between the spring of eighth grade and the spring of tenth grade. Later in
high school, low-income students make slightly greater gain in reading than higher-income students. But
the initial gap and the difference in learning rates means that low-income students end high school with
nearly a 4-point disadvantage in cumulative math score. Both groups are learning simple inference skills,
however, higher-income students are making strides towards learning complex inference skills (Figure
3.21).
72
TABLE 3.21: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY ECONOMIC STATUS —S ECONDARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
HIGHER -INCOME S TUDENTS
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
29.52
0.163
(0.0041)
0.0825
(0.0054)
0.0213
3.90
33.42
0.00893
1.98
35.40
LOW -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM HIGHER -INCOME S TUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-0.0296
(0.0069)
0.0225
(0.0083)
-3.62
-0.00387
-0.71
-4.33
0.00244
0.54
-3.79
Each estimate in bold is significantly different from the corresponding estimate for higher-income students at the 5 percent
level. Descriptions of models are provided in Appendix A.
45
FIGURE 3.21: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY ECONOMIC STATUS —SECONDARY SCHOOL
NELS Reading Scores by Economic Status
30
2-Simple Inferences
20
25
Score
35
40
3-Complex Inferences
8-10
10-12
Higher-Income
Low-Income
Math
Findings presented in Table 3.22 show that low-income students start high school at a significant
disadvantage compared to higher-income students and continue to lose ground during high school. Low73
income students have average math scores about 5 points lower than higher-income students in the spring
of eighth grade. Low-income students gain about 1 point less than more advantaged peers in the first half
of high school and about a half point less in the second half of high school. Low-income students end
high school not yet learning simple problem solving skills, a skill higher-income students have learned by
tenth grade (Figure 3.22).
TABLE 3.22: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY ECONOMIC STATUS —S ECONDARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
HIGHER -INCOME S TUDENTS
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
40.05
0.338
(0.0049)
0.185
(0.0050)
0.0303
8.10
48.15
0.0141
4.44
52.59
LOW -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM HIGHER -INCOME S TUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-0.0383
(0.0076)
-0.0192
(0.0075)
-5.40
-0.00344
-0.92
-6.32
-0.00146
-0.46
-6.79
Each estimate in bold is significantly different from the corresponding estimate for higher-income students at the 5 percent
level. Descriptions of models are provided in Appendix A.
74
FIGURE 3.22: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY ECONOMIC STATUS —SECONDARY SCHOOL
60
NELS Math Scores by Economic Status
50
4-Intermediate Level Math
40
2-Fractions and Exponents
30
Score
3-Simple Problem Solving
20
1-Single Operations
8-10
10-12
Higher-Income
Low-Income
Race and Income
Reading
Tables 3.15 and 3.21, presented earlier, compared the initial status and learning rates across race/ethnicity
and across income status. There were stark differences across subgroups. In Table 3.23, the race and
income groups are combined to explore how both minority status and income status matter to learning.
White children of low-income backgrounds start high school with a lower average reading score (2.77
points less) than their more advantaged white peers. The white low-income students learn significantly less
than the white higher-income students between eighth and tenth grades but learn significantly more
between tenth and twelfth grades, ending with about the same deficit that they started school with.
Black students and Hispanic students, regardless of income status, have significantly lower reading scores
at the end of eighth grade than higher-income white students. Throughout high school, black students
from both income strata, make less gain on the reading assessment than white students, regardless of their
economic background. The learning deficit is greatest for low-income black students. They gain
significantly fewer points between eighth and tenth grades than any other subgroup.
Hispanic students from low-income backgrounds start high school with the largest deficit on the reading
test. However, these students gain significantly more between tenth and twelfth grades than white higherincome students and so reduce their accumulated point deficit. Hispanic students from higher-income
backgrounds start high school behind white students in reading but ma ke similar progress as white
students throughout high school.
75
Low-income Asian students earn less than higher-income white students on the initial reading assessment
at the end of eighth grade, but make similar gains to higher-income white students early in high school.
Later in high school, these less economically advantaged Asian students gain 2 points more than white
higher-income students on the reading assessment. Higher-income Asian students start high school with a
similar average eighth grade reading score and learn at the same rate throughout high school as white
higher-income students.
By the end of high school, Asian and white students from higher-income backgrounds are advancing
towards learning complex inference skills, while black and Hispanic higher-income students are learning
simple inferences (see Figure 3.23a). The lines representing learning rates for the low-income students
(shown in Figure 3.23b) indicate closer clustering among these subgroups. They all are learning simple
inferences and abstract points.
TABLE 3.23: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY RACE AND INCOME—S ECONDARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
W HITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
30.16
0.166
(0.0046)
0.0812
(0.0061)
0.0217
3.98
34.14
0.00880
1.95
36.09
W HITE LOW -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-0.0311
(0.0083)
0.0249
(0.0100)
-2.77
-0.00407
-0.75
-3.52
0.00270
0.60
-2.92
BLACK HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-0.0353
(0.0184)
-0.00530
(0.0219)
-4.60
-0.00461
-0.85
-5.45
-0.000573
-0.13
-5.57
BLACK LOW -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
At End of Period
-0.0523
(0.0143)
-0.00490
(0.0178)
-0.00683
-1.25
-8.77
-0.000531
-0.12
-8.89
Table Continues on Next Page
76
-7.52
TABLE 3.23 (CONT .): DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY RACE AND INCOME—S ECONDARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
HISPANIC HIGHER -INCOME S TUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-4.55
-0.0283
(0.0158)
0.00186
(0.0194)
-0.00370
-0.68
-5.23
0.000201
0.04
-5.19
HISPANIC LOW -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-0.0162
(0.0158)
0.0461
(0.0176)
-6.50
-0.00212
-0.39
-6.89
0.00499
1.10
-5.78
ASIAN HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
0.0327
(0.0158)
0.0419
(0.0244)
0.45
0.00427
0.78
1.23
0.00454
1.01
2.24
ASIAN LOW -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE HIGHER -INCOME S TUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-0.0307
(0.0195)
0.0862
(0.0261)
-4.53
-0.00401
-0.74
-5.27
0.00933
2.07
-3.20
Estimates in bold are significantly different from corresponding estimate for white higher-income students at the 5 percent
level. Descriptions of models are provided in Appendix A.
77
45
FIGURE 3.23A: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY RACE (HIGHER INCOME)—S ECONDARY SCHOOL
NELS Reading Scores by Race, Higher Inc
30
2-Simple Inferences
20
25
Score
35
40
3-Complex Inferences
8-10
10-12
Asian
Hispanic
Black
White
NELS Reading Scores by Race, Low Income
3-Complex Inferences
25
30
2-Simple Inferences
20
Score
35
40
45
FIGURE 3.23 B: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING L EARNING RATES BY RACE(LOW-I NCOME)—S ECONDARY SCHOOL
8-10
10-12
Asian
Hispanic
Black
White
78
Math
Previous tables (Tables 3.16 and 3.22) indicate differences in initial status and learning by race/ethnicity
and by income status. In this section, we look at differences by race and income simultaneously.
There are substantial differences in initial math achievement between low-income and higher-income
white students. White low-income students start high school with an average math score 4.1 points less
than white higher-income students. During high school, white low-income students fall farther behind,
because they gain slightly less than white higher-income students.
Black low-income students start high school at the greatest disadvantage in math. On average, black lowincome students earn 12 fewer points on the math assessment at the end of eighth grade than white
higher-income students. This deficit increases in early high school, during which black low-income
students gain 2.04 points less than these white students. The math learning rates between black lowincome and white higher-income students do not differ significantly later in high school. Black higherincome students start high school with a 7.74-point deficit on the math assessment compared to white
higher-income students but both groups learn at similar rates during high school. This deficit is larger than
that faced by white low-income students, suggesting the importance of race to achievement gains.
Relative to the eighth grade math performance of white higher-income students, Hispanic low-income
students are 9.32 points behind and Hispanic higher-income students are 6.81 points behind. The Hispanic
low-income students gain 1.52 points less than white higher-income students between eighth and tenth
grades, thus expanding the initial gap. Hispanic higher-income students gain math points at about the same
pace as white higher-income students throughout all four years of high school.
Asian higher-income students earn a higher average math score at the end of eighth grade than white
higher-income students (3.11 points). Throughout high school, these Asian students make math gains that
do not significantly differ from the math gains made by white higher-income students. Asian low-income
students earn a slightly lower math score at the end of eighth grade than white higher-income students, but
gain slightly more during each time period (though this advantage is not significant).
By the end of high school, Asian higher-income students have outpaced all other subgroups and are
learning intermediate-level math concepts (see Figure 3.24a). White higher-income students are moving
towards learning these concepts but are not there yet. Black and Hispanic higher-income students are
beginning to pick up skills in the less advanced proficiency level of simple problem solving.
Figure 3.24b illustrates the learning rates of the low-income students by race/ethnicity. In this graph, black
and Hispanic students are clustered together, relatively far below the white and Asian low-income
students, who are already learning intermediate-level math concepts. In sum, black and Hispanic students,
regardless of income status, are far behind their peers in learning math.
79
TABLE 3.24: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY RACE AND INCOME—S ECONDARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
W HITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS ON L Y
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
40.95
0.341
(0.0051)
0.183
(0.0057)
0.0306
8.19
49.14
0.0140
4.40
53.54
W HITE LOW -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-0.0274
(0.0086)
-0.0240
(0.0087)
-4.10
-0.00246
-0.66
-4.76
-0.00183
-0.58
-5.33
BLACK HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-0.0501
(0.0309)
-0.000609
(0.0189)
-7.74
-0.00450
-1.20
-8.94
-0.0000463
-0.01
-8.95
BLACK LOW -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-0.0851
(0.0181)
-0.0187
(0.0160)
-12.19
-0.00764
-2.04
-14.23
-0.00142
-0.45
-14.68
HISPANIC HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-6.81
-0.0123
(0.0187)
0.0179
(0.0187)
-0.00110
-0.29
-7.11
0.00136
0.43
-6.68
HISPANIC LOW -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM WHITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
-0.0634
-0.00569
(0.0150)
10th Grade to 12th Grade
0.00314
0.000238
(0.0171)
Table Continues on Next Page
80
-9.32
-1.52
-10.84
0.08
-10.76
TABLE 3.24 (CONT .): DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY RACE AND INCOME—S ECONDARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
ASIAN HIGH -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
3.11
0.0361
(0.0226)
0.0167
(0.0244)
0.00324
0.87
3.97
0.00127
0.40
4.37
ASIAN LOW -INCOME STUDENTS (DIFFERENCE FROM W HITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS)
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-0.00283
(0.0255)
0.0368
(0.0270)
-2.12
-0.000254
-0.07
-2.19
0.00280
0.88
-1.30
Each estimate in bold is significantly different from the corresponding estimate for white higher-income students at the 5
percent level. Descriptions of models are provided in Appendix A.
FIGURE 3.24A: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY RACE (HIGHER INCOME)—S ECONDARY SCHOOL
60
NELS Math Scores by Race, Higher Inc
50
4-Intermediate Level Math
40
30
2-Fractions and Exponents
1-Single Operations
20
Score
3-Simple Problem Solving
8-10
10-12
Asian
Hispanic
Black
White
81
FIGURE 3.24 B: DIFFERENCES IN MATH L EARNING RATES BY RACE (L OW INCOME)—S ECONDARY SCHOOL
60
NELS Math Scores by Race, Low Income
50
4-Intermediate Level Math
40
2-Fractions and Exponents
30
Score
3-Simple Problem Solving
20
1-Single Operations
8-10
10-12
Asian
Hispanic
Black
White
Summary
These secondary school analyses reveal some interesting learning differences by gender, race/ethnicity,
language status, and income background. Females start high school with a slightly higher average reading
score than males and gain faster than males later in high school. Males start high school with a very slightly
higher average math score, and gain faster than females later in high school.
Black students face substantial deficits in reading and math achievement compared to white students and
to students of other ethnicities. Overall, Hispanic students perform better than black students in reading
and in math (both initial status and learning rates). In general, the initial status and the gains for students
who do not speak English at home lag behind those who do speak English at home in both reading and
math. But when the groups are combined to create subgroups by race and language status, a more
complicated story emerges. Hispanic and Asian NEH students make greater gains in reading and math
during high school than EH students of the same ethnicities.
Income status plays a major role in learning rate differences. Higher-income students of black and
Hispanic ethnicities finish eighth grade with average reading and math scores less than those of white
higher-income students. However, all three subgroups make similar amounts of reading and math gain
during high school. Low-income black and Hispanic students have the lowest average reading and math
achievement at the end of eighth grade. However, Hispanic low-income students either keep pace or
outpace white higher-income students between tenth and twelfth grades. Black low-income students
consistently fall farther behind white higher-income students by learning at a slower rate during high
school.
82
Summary of Elementary and High School Estimates
The analyses in this chapter have examined differences in reading and math learning across gender,
race/ethnicity, and economic status during elementary and high school. There are larger differences in
initial achievement and gains between males and females in elementary school than in high school. This
holds true for both reading and math results.
These analyses confirm how important race is to academic achievement in America. In both the
elementary school and secondary school analyses, regardless of subject matter, black students on average
start behind and finish even farther behind white students due to slower learning rates. Hispanic students
are also at a disadvantage, compared to white students, both in terms of starting point at the beginning of
kindergarten and their average learning rate during elementary school. Over time, this difference in
learning rate fades away, but the differences in achievement levels remain. By high school, Hispanic
students are still behind in achievement levels, but are keeping pace with white students’ learning rate.
Economic background appears important for students’ reading and math gains. In elementary school, the
reading and math learning differences between low-income and higher-income students is large at first
then becomes smaller over time. In high school, the gap is significant only in their initial status and then
later, from tenth to twelfth grade. Students from families with more middle class incomes start higher and
make slightly faster gains than students from lower-income families.
A great deal of research has focused on achievement differences by race/ethnicity. Clearly these
differences are closely related to income status. In elementary school, black and Hispanic children begin
kindergarten with significantly lower reading and math scores than white children. Minority children from
low-income backgrounds start even farther behind and gain even less. Minority children from higherincome backgrounds start school with less of a deficit compared to the average white student. However,
the higher-income black children start higher—but gain more slowly than white low-income children,
falling behind white low-income children by the end of third grade.
The advantage of higher income to children’s reading and math fall kindergarten test scores may derive
from better resources available at home. Not surprisingly, black higher income students gain at faster rates
than black low-income students in both math and reading. Hispanic higher-income students also fare
better than Hispanic low-income students in terms of growth rates during elementary school, but only as
well as white low-income students in math and reading. In high school, black and Hispanic higher-income
students end eighth grade with large deficits in reading and math test scores compared to white higherincome students. But during high school, these students keep pace with the white higher-income students.
Black and Hispanic low-income students continue to fall behind and compound their initial deficit, again
highlighting the joint importance of income and race.
Family income is an important indicator of better performance, predicting both a higher initial level and a
faster rate of growth on reading and math tests. However, race seems to be a more important indicator,
even conditional on family income. The apparent influence of race may reflect many factors not explored
here, such as differences in neighborhoods33 or schools, which we hope to explore further in future
research. Nevertheless, the simple fact that higher-income black children start higher but gain more slowly
than white low-income children, and have lower mean achievement than low-income white students after
the third grade, confirms the importance of race as an indicator of where our greatest challenges lie in the
education system.
33
Though recent research by Sanbonmatsu et al. (2006) suggests little effect of neighborhood characteristics.
83
CHAPTER IV: COMPARISON WITH THETA SCORES
All of the analytic models presented in Chapter 2 and 3 were also estimated using theta scores as the
outcome. In this section, we compare findings using the IRT scale scores (used as the outcome variable in
the Chapter 2 and 3 analyses) to those using the theta scores.34 The theta scores have at least two
advantages over the scale scores. First, the theta scores are potentially less determined by choices made in
test item selection. Second, the distribution of theta scores more closely resembles the normal distribution;
the distribution is more symmetric and is less truncated or compressed at the tails, which better matches
the assumptions that underlie our statistical modeling. The main drawbacks of theta scores are that they
may not be available for all tests (so results using theta scores are potentially less comparable across tests),
and they are measured in arbitrary units (in ECLS, from –5 to 5, and in NELS, from 0 to 100). In general,
the theta scores do not represent a more or less accurate metric tha n the IRT scale scores from the prior
chapter, but are simply different.
Because the IRT scale and theta results use different metrics, there is no guarantee that the results will look
similar, and in fact they differ in several important ways, as can be seen in the tables that follow in the next
section. However, this potential and actual difference offers an important test of our use of “standardized”
rates of learning or “effect sizes,” in which we divide mean growth rates by the standard deviation at the
base period to construct a measure of growth in standard deviation units. If the “effect sizes” do actually
offer the potential of comparing across different tests using different metrics, the IRT scale and theta score
results should look more similar when presented as effect sizes, and we find they do—especially when
using end-of-period standard deviations to estimate standardized gaps, as in the final section of this
chapter. As such, the results in this chapter support the findings from Chapters 2 and 3., and offer a
compelling reason to focus on effect size results in those chapters.
Tables in this chapter present the IRT results from four models next to the theta results from the same
four models. The four selected models in this comparative analysis are: 1) a base model with only variables
measuring time spent in each specified time period used as predictors; 2) the base model augmented with
race/ethnicity indicators; 3) the base model augmented with race/ethnicity indicators fully interacted with
language status; and 4) the base model augmented with race/ethnicity indicators fully interacted with
income status.
Elementary School
Base model
Results using the ECLS data, presented in Table 4.1, indicate that the effect sizes look reasonably similar
across the two metrics we examine—IRT scale scores and theta scores. A priori, we need not expect that
these two metrics would produce similar results. The IRT scale score distribution is multi-modal and
skewed; the theta score distribution is more normal (see Appendix C for more detail). But comparing the
effect sizes for the two metrics, in the third and sixth columns of estimates in Table 4.1, we can conclude
they seem similar and suggest a broad agreement of results. The only glaring discrepancy is for reading
gains (or losses) over the summer between kindergarten and first grade, but the gains over this period are
very imprecisely estimated, due in part to the smaller sample size at the beginning of first grade, so neither
estimate differs significantly from zero (i.e. the null hypothesis of no change over summer cannot be
rejected). The estimates for kindergarten and second and third grades are more robust, and seem
reasonably close in magnitude.
The IRT scores are nonlinear transformations of the theta scores, including information on the set of test item characteristics.
The IRT model uses an individual’s pattern of item responses (right, wrong, omitted) and dimensions of the items to estimate
each individual’s probability of answering each item correctly, and the individual’s “ability” parameter theta (similar to a student
fixed effect in a logistic regression).
34
84
Second, a comparison of gain as measured in IRT scale score points suggests that first grade gains exceed
kindergarten gains. However, the theta scores and the effect sizes using IRT scale score indicate that the
first grade rate is similar to—or perhaps smaller than—the kindergarten rate. In Table 4.2, the ratio of first
grade to kindergarten estimated growth rates, and the ratio of second and third grade to first grade
estimated growth rates, are presented for both metrics. The ratios for the unstandardized growth estimates
indicate that IRT scale reading scores increase nearly twice as fast in first grade as in kindergarten (1.812
times as fast) but theta reading scores increase about nine tenths as fast in first grade as in kindergarten
(0.936 times as fast). Looking at the same two metrics using effect sizes, however, the rate of increase
appears just as fast in first grade as in kindergarten (both 1.071 and 0.966 are close to one, indicating parity
or near-parity of learning rates).
TABLE 4.1: ESTIMATED GROWTH RATES IN ECLS-K: COMPARISON OF IRT AND T HETA, ALL STUDENTS
Time Period
Starting
Level
IRT
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
Starting
Level
Theta
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
SCORES
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
22.8
39.8
39.4
70.3
1.81
-0.171
3.28
1.59
0.196
-0.0126
0.210
0.0749
-1.34
-0.372
-0.349
0.559
0.103
0.00889
0.0964
0.0309
0.175
0.0160
0.169
0.0614
M ATHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
17.5
32.7
34.0
56.3
1.62
0.491
2.37
1.20
0.196
0.0422
0.191
0.0767
-1.24
-0.342
-0.265
0.552
0.0955
0.0301
0.0867
0.0317
0.163
0.0545
0.154
0.0638
Third, the gain per month and the effect sizes measure results on the theta scores do not diverge as
dramatically across time periods as the same results based on the IRT scale scores. This is primarily
because the size of the standard deviation in the theta metric is fairly stable while the standard deviations
for the IRT scores vary more widely (see Appendix C). Thus, comparisons of ratios in Table 4.2 range
between roughly four tenths and one for effect sizes (and for raw theta scores), but range between one half
and 1.8 for IRT scale scores.
TABLE 4.2: RATIOS OF ESTIMATED GROWTH RATES IN ECLS-K: COMPARISON OF IRT AND T HETA, ALL STUDENTS
Time Period
IRT
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Theta
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
RATIOS
READING
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
1.812
0.485
1.071
0.357
0.936
0.321
0.966
0.363
1.463
0.506
0.974
0.402
0.908
0.366
0.945
0.414
M ATHEMATICS
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
85
Race/ethnicity model
Even disaggregated by race/ethnicity, the effect sizes look reasonably similar across the two metrics we
examine—IRT scale scores and theta scores. While individual time periods for some groups seem to have
different estimated growth rates, an overall comparison of the effect sizes for the two metrics, in the third
and sixth columns of estimates in Table 4.3, suggests the results are broadly similar. The major difference
seems to be that black and Hispanic students are predicted to experience faster growth in scores than
white students, using the theta metric, when results using the IRT scale metric predict faster growth rates
for white students.
86
TABLE 4.3: ESTIMATED GROWTH RATES IN ECLS-K: COMPARISON OF IRT AND T HETA, BY RACE
Time Period
Starting
Level
IRT
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
Starting
Level
Theta
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
W HITE STUDENTS
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
24.2
42.1
41.5
74.8
1.90
-0.231
3.53
1.64
0.205
-0.0169
0.227
0.0773
-1.23
-0.263
-0.247
0.668
0.103
0.00609
0.0972
0.0310
0.174
0.0109
0.170
0.0616
M ATHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
19.5
35.8
37.0
60.5
1.73
0.473
2.49
1.23
0.210
0.0407
0.202
0.0784
-1.08
-0.189
-0.121
0.683
0.0945
0.0267
0.0853
0.0315
0.161
0.0482
0.151
0.0635
BLACK STUDENTS , DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE STUDENTS
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-3.17
-5.83
-5.91
-11.3
-0.283
-0.0288
-0.576
-0.185
-0.0306
-0.00211
-0.0370
-0.00871
-0.244
-0.276
-0.277
-0.271
-0.00332
-0.000396
0.000639
-0.00131
-0.00562
-0.000711
0.00112
-0.00260
M ATHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-4.64
-8.04
-8.29
-11.9
-0.361
-0.0965
-0.379
-0.147
-0.0439
-0.00829
-0.0306
-0.00938
-0.363
-0.398
-0.397
-0.367
-0.00374
0.000578
0.00314
-0.00113
-0.00639
0.00105
0.00555
-0.00227
HISPANIC STUDENTS , DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE STUDENTS
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-4.92
-6.67
-5.98
-12.4
-0.186
0.268
-0.687
-0.0578
-0.0201
0.0196
-0.0440
-0.00273
-0.373
-0.311
-0.265
-0.301
0.00660
0.0179
-0.00384
0.00122
0.0112
0.0321
-0.00671
0.00242
M ATHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-5.63
-8.01
-7.68
-9.78
-0.253
0.125
-0.222
-0.0275
-0.0307
0.0108
-0.0180
-0.00176
-0.461
-0.404
-0.355
-0.303
0.00606
0.0189
0.00554
0.00100
0.0103
0.0342
0.00981
0.00202
ASIAN STUDENTS, DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE STUDENTS
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
1.51
3.77
5.34
2.63
0.240
0.611
-0.289
-0.250
0.0259
0.0448
-0.0185
-0.0118
0.0789
0.107
0.136
0.0456
0.00295
0.0113
-0.00959
-0.00463
0.00500
0.0204
-0.0168
-0.00920
M ATHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
0.556
0.235
1.38
-2.03
-0.0341
0.443
-0.362
0.0941
-0.00414
0.0381
-0.0293
0.00601
0.0233
-0.00936
0.0198
0.0645
-0.00347
0.0145
-0.00986
0.00307
-0.00592
0.0263
-0.0175
0.00619
87
Black students’ learning rates are consistently higher relative to white students when looking at theta scores
instead of scale scores. In particular, black students actually make larger gains on the reading test in first
grade, and larger gains on the math test in both first and the summer before, relative to white students.
While black students start out farther behind (about four tenths of a standard deviation behind white
students at the beginning of kindergarten) when using the theta metric, they wind up slightly less far
behind by the end of first grade when using the theta metric. However, when the gaps a re measured in
standard deviation units (see the final section of this chapter), the differences between the theta and scale
score results at a point in time are quite small indeed. This is partly due to differences in how the standard
deviation on the test changes over time for the two different metrics, and partly to the order of magnitude
of the gaps in learning rates, which are quite small relative to the gap in levels. That is, the rate of change in
the gap over relatively short periods of time like first grade or the summer between kindergarten and first
is less relevant to the size of the gap than its starting size and its rate of change over longer periods of
time, such as the period encompassing second and third grades.
A much larger difference appears for Hispanic students, who gain relative to non-Hispanic white students
in every period (i.e. the coefficients measuring the rates of gain on the mathematics test, in the third
column of estimates, are all positive) when looking at the theta score results. Hispanic students seem to
lose ground relative to non-Hispanic white students in every period except the summer between
kindergarten and first grade when looking at the scale score results (i.e. three of the four coefficients
measuring the rates of gain, in the first column of estimates, are negative). Thus a researcher using theta
scores would find that Hispanic students seem to start at a large disadvantage and gradually narrow that
gap, while a researcher using scale scores would find that Hispanic students seem to start at a smaller
disadvantage that gradually increased in size.
88
Language model
TABLE 4.4: ESTIMATED GROWTH RATES IN ECLS-K: COMPARISON OF IRT AND T HETA, BY LANGUAGE
Time Period
Starting
Level
IRT
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
Starting
Level
Theta
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
ENGLISH AT HOME STUDENTS
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
23.1
40.3
39.8
71.3
1.89
-0.236
3.53
1.64
0.206
-0.0174
0.228
0.0775
-1.32
-0.345
-0.328
0.585
0.103
0.00612
0.0973
0.0310
0.174
0.0110
0.170
0.0616
M ATHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
18.0
33.4
34.7
57.0
1.72
0.471
2.49
1.23
0.210
0.0406
0.202
0.0785
-1.20
-0.306
-0.231
0.576
0.0945
0.0266
0.0855
0.0316
0.162
0.0481
0.151
0.0634
-0.00563
0.000328
0.000480
-0.00254
-0.00707
0.00390
0.00458
-0.00214
NON -ENGLISH AT HOME STUDENTS
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-4.12
-5.53
-5.18
-10.1
-0.282
-0.0129
-0.588
-0.186
-0.0306
-0.000952
-0.0379
-0.00880
-0.309
-0.276
-0.235
-0.245
-0.00332
0.000183
0.000274
-0.00128
M ATHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-4.51
-6.40
-6.40
-7.13
-0.367
-0.0594
-0.392
-0.146
-0.0448
-0.00512
-0.0318
-0.00932
-0.384
-0.339
-0.298
-0.226
-0.00414
0.00216
0.00259
-0.00107
Race/ethnicity by language model
Disaggregating further, by race/ethnicity and by whether students have ever failed the OLDS assessment
(indicating poor English skills), the effect sizes still look broadly similar across IRT scale scores and theta
scores. While estimated growth rates differ in individual time periods for some groups, the effect sizes for
the two metrics reported in the third and sixth columns of estimates in Table 4.5 seem broadly similar.
Again, black students seem to gain a bit more on white students when looking at the theta results, but the
difference in learning rates is negligible relative to the size of the initial gap. The major difference is again
for Hispanic students, who are predicted to experience faster growth in scores than white students using
the theta metric, when results using the IRT scale metric predict faster growth rates for white students, and
when we disaggregate by language status, this difference is very similar for both types of Hispanic students.
The difference in results for Hispanic students is thus robust to the particular type of disaggregation
reported in Table 4.5, and there is no obvious explanation for why the rates of gain would be so different
looking at scale scores or theta scores. Because many Hispanic students who took the OLDS test a t some
point in time (whom we classify as belonging to non-English homes, or NEH) have missing test scores on
the reading test in early periods, we might expect the reading test results to look somewhat different,
especially for the subgroup of students that we classify as NEH. This is not the case, however—in fact, the
larger difference is observed in the math test, where Hispanic students are not missing scores at an
exceptionally high rate.
89
TABLE 4.5: ESTIMATED GROWTH RATES IN ECLS-K: COMPARISON OF IRT AND T HETA, BY RACE AND LANGUAGE
Time Period
Starting
Level
IRT
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
Starting
Level
Theta
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
W HITE STUDENTS
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
24.2
42.1
41.5
74.8
1.90
-0.231
3.53
1.64
0.205
-0.0169
0.227
0.0773
-1.23
-1.263
-0.247
0.668
0.103
0.00612
0.0972
0.0310
0.174
0.0110
0.170
0.0616
M ATHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
19.5
35.8
37.0
60.5
1.73
0.473
2.49
1.23
0.210
0.0407
0.202
0.0784
-1.08
-0.189
-0.121
0.682
0.0945
0.0266
0.0853
0.0315
0.161
0.0481
0.151
0.0635
BLACK STUDENTS , DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE STUDENTS
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-3.17
-5.83
-5.91
-11.3
-0.283
-0.0290
-0.576
-0.185
-0.0306
-0.00212
-0.0370
-0.00870
-0.244
-0.276
-0.277
-0.271
-0.00332
-0.000396
0.000639
-0.00131
-0.00562
-0.000711
0.00112
-0.00260
M ATHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
T ABLE CONTINUES ON NEXT P AGE
-4.64
-8.04
-8.29
-11.9
-0.367
-0.0594
-0.392
-0.146
-0.0448
-0.00512
-0.0318
-0.00932
-0.363
-0.398
-0.396
-0.36
-0.00371
0.000578
0.00314
-0.00113
-0.00633
0.00105
0.00555
-0.00227
90
Time Period
Starting
Level
IRT
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Starting
Level
Theta
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
HISPANIC ENGLISH SPE AKING HOME STUDENTS, DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE STUDENTS
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-3.36
-3.89
-2.95
-7.07
-0.0567
0.366
-0.437
-0.0648
-0.00613
0.0268
-0.0280
-0.00305
-0.257
-0.174
-0.136
-0.174
0.00883
0.0148
-0.00402
-0.0000913
0.0150
0.0265
-0.00703
-0.000182
M ATHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1s t Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-3.79
-5.27
-4.76
-6.95
-0.160
0.191
-0.228
-0.0144
-0.0196
0.0165
-0.0185
-0.000919
-0.295
-0.249
-0.211
-0.210
0.00490
0.0146
0.000152
0.000517
0.00836
0.0265
0.000270
0.00104
HISPANIC NON -ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS , DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE STUDENTS
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-6.98
-974
-9.64
-17.9
-0.293
0.0404
-0.880
-0.0309
-0.0317
0.00296
-0.0564
-0.00146
-0.526
-0.469
-0.419
-0.432
0.00606
0.0194
-0.00134
0.00286
0.0103
0.0348
-0.00234
0.00569
M ATHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-7.39
-10.6
-10.4
-12.5
-0.340
0.0829
-0.221
-0.0376
-0.0413
0.00712
-0.0178
-0.00240
-0.621
-0.551
-0.486
-0.392
0.0251
0.0251
0.00998
0.00146
0.0453
0.0453
0.0177
0.00294
ASIAN ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS , DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE STUDENTS
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
3.85
6.79
8.75
6.74
0.312
0.758
-0.213
-0.273
0.0337
0.0556
-0.0136
-0.0129
0.196
0.212
0.247
0.127
0.00170
0.0136
-0.0128
-0.00548
0.00289
0.0245
-0.0224
-0.0109
M ATHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
2.09
2.63
4.51
1.51
0.0574
0.731
-0.319
0.0657
0.00697
0.0628
-0.0258
0.00420
0.120
0.0947
0.151
0.0364
-0.00265
0.0220
-0.0122
0.00177
-0.00452
0.0397
-0.0216
0.00355
ASIAN NON -ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME S TUDENTS, DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE STUDENTS
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-0.564
1.51
2.81
0.274
0.221
0.501
-0.327
-0.235
0.0239
0.0367
-0.0210
-0.0111
-0.0207
0.0280
0.0519
-0.0114
0.00517
0.00928
-0.00673
-0.00402
0.00876
0.0167
-0.0118
-0.00799
M ATHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-0.638
-1.52
-1.13
-4.55
-0.0937
0.153
-0.363
0.114
-0.0114
0.0131
-0.0294
0.00731
-0.0492
-0.0856
-0.0682
-0.136
-0.00387
0.00673
-0.00724
0.00402
-0.00659
0.0122
-0.0128
0.00809
91
Income model
TABLE 4.6: ESTIMATED GROWTH RATES IN ECLS-K: COMPARISON OF IRT AND T HETA, BY INCOME
Time Period
Starting
Level
IRT
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
Starting
Level
Theta
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
HIGHER -INCOME S TUDENTS
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
25.0
43.3
43.1
76.2
1.89
-0.236
3.53
1.64
0.206
-0.0174
0.228
0.0775
-1.17
-0.215
-0.189
.701
0.103
0.00612
0.0973
0.0310
0.174
0.0110
0.170
0.0616
M ATHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
19.9
36.2
37.5
60.8
1.72
0.471
2.49
1.23
0.210
0.0406
0.202
0.0785
-1.05
-0.173
-0.99
0.693
0.0945
0.0266
0.0855
0.0316
0.162
0.0481
0.151
0.0634
-0.00563
0.000328
0.000480
-0.00254
-0.00707
0.00390
0.00458
-0.00214
LOW -INCOME STUDENTS
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-5.37
-7.98
-8.68
-13.77
-0.282
-0.0129
-0.588
-0.186
-0.0306
-0.000952
-0.0379
-0.00880
-0.401
-0.362
-0.370
-0.328
-0.00332
0.000183
0.000274
-0.00128
M ATHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-5.50
-7.83
-8.01
-10.4
-0.367
-0.0594
-0.392
-0.146
-0.0448
-0.00512
-0.0318
-0.00932
-0.437
-0.388
-0.382
-0.323
-0.00414
0.00216
0.00259
-0.00107
Race/ethnicity by income model
Disaggregating in a different way, by race/ethnicity and by whether students have family income low
enough to qualify for free or reduced-price lunch (1.85 times poverty), the overall pattern of effect sizes
still looks reasonably similar across IRT scale scores and theta scores. While estimated growth rates differ
in individual time periods for some groups, the effect sizes for the two metrics reported in the third and
sixth columns of estimates in Table 4.7 seem broadly similar.
Again, the biggest differences between scale score and theta score results tend to observed among the
estimates of gains for Hispanic children, though the results are much closer after we break the Hispanic
sample into low-income and higher-income groups. The one subgroup with relatively large differences
between scale score and theta score results is low-income Hispanic students, and this difference is mainly
observed in mathematics growth rates. It is possible that the group of students who cannot be assigned to
the low-income or the higher-income group, due to missing income or household composition data, is
driving the large observed differences between scale score and theta score results for Hispanic students in
Tables 4.3 and 4.5. Further research is warranted to check if this inconsistency is primarily a data quality
issue, or in fact reflects undesirable features of the test design.
92
TABLE 4.7: ESTIMATED GROWTH RATES IN ECLS-K: COMPARISON OF IRT AND T HETA, BY RACE AND I NCOME
Time Period
Starting
Level
IRT
Gain Per
Month
SD Per
Month
Starting
Level
Theta
Gain Per
Month
SD Per
Month
W HITE HIGHER INCOME STUDENTS
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
25.5
43.8
43.4
77.7
1.95
-0.159
3.64
1.64
0.210
-0.0116
0.233
0.0774
-1.14
-0.186
-0.166
0.736
0.101
0.00767
0.0958
0.0305
0.171
0.0138
0.168
0.0606
M ATHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
20.8
37.4
38.7
62.9
1.77
0.524
2.54
1.24
0.215
0.0450
0.206
0.0791
-0.987
-0.114
-0.040
0.749
0.0928
0.0285
0.0839
0.0314
0.158
0.0515
0.149
0.0632
W HITE LOW INCOME STUDENTS, DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE HIGHER INCOME STUDENTS
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-4.57
-6.17
-6.79
-10.41
-0.169
-0.242
-0.384
-0.0202
-0.0183
-0.0177
-0.0246
-0.000952
-0.331
-0.271
-0.283
-0.244
0.00645
-0.00502
0.00423
0.00161
0.0109
-0.00903
0.00740
0.00321
MaTHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-4.35
-5.64
-6.10
-7.76
-0.138
-0.177
-0.176
-0.0412
-0.0167
-0.0152
-0.0143
-0.00263
-0.325
-0.267
-0.284
-0.236
0.00609
-0.00645
0.00511
0.000365
0.0104
-0.0117
0.00906
0.000735
BLACK HIGHER INCOME STUDENTS, DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE HIGHER INCOME STUDENTS
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-1.62
-3.17
-2.30
-8.19
-0.164
0.337
-0.626
-0.171
-0.0177
0.0247
-0.0401
-0.00806
-0.122
-0.146
-0.126
-0.190
-0.00253
0.00767
-0.00676
-0.00219
-0.00428
0.0138
-0.0118
-0.00436
M ATHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-3.67
-6.53
-6.69
-10.3
-0.303
-0.0632
-0.384
-0.0942
-0.0368
-0.00543
-0.0310
-0.00602
-0.271
-0.313
-0.317
-0.306
-0.00447
-0.00167
0.00122
-0.000852
-0.00763
-0.00303
0.00216
-0.00172
BLACK LOW INCOME S TUDENTS, DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE HIGHER INCOME STUDENTS
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-5.69
-9.50
-10.3
-16.9
-0.405
-0.311
-0.699
-0.193
-0.0437
-0.0228
-0.0448
-0.00910
-0.431
-0.442
-0.459
-0.403
-0.00110
-0.00679
0.00590
-0.000183
-0.00186
-0.0122
0.0103
-0.000363
M ATHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
T ABLE CONTINUES ON NEXT P AGE
-6.82
-11.0
-11.5
-15.7
-0.443
-0.187
-0.445
-0.185
-0.0538
-0.0160
-0.0360
-0.0118
-0.534
-0.544
-0.547
-0.489
-0.000974
-0.00116
0.00609
-0.00107
-0.00166
-0.00209
0.0108
-0.00214
93
TABLE 4.7: ESTIMATED GROWTH RATES IN ECLS-K: COMPARISON OF IRT AND T HETA, BY RACE AND I NCOME
Time Period
Starting
Level
IRT
Gain Per
Month
SD Per
Month
Starting
Level
Theta
Gain Per
Month
SD Per
Month
HISPANIC HIGHER INCOME STUDENTS, DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE HIGHER INCOME STUDENTS
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-3.38
-3.69
-2.96
-6.67
-0.0322
0.280
-0.394
-0.0518
-0.00348
0.0205
-0.0252
-0.00244
-0.244
-0.161
-0.137
-0.155
0.00883
0.00910
-0.00192
-0.000335
0.0150
0.0164
-0.00336
-0.000666
M ATHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-4.18
-5.20
-5.71
-6.92
-0.108
-0.198
-0.129
0.00648
-0.0131
-0.0170
-0.0104
0.000414
-0.312
-0.244
-0.247
-0.208
0.00721
-0.00143
0.00414
0.000974
0.0123
-0.00259
0.00733
0.00196
HISPANIC LOW INCOME S TUDENTS, DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE HIGHER INCOME STUDENTS
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-7.87
-11.1
-10.7
-20.1
-0.337
0.133
-1.00
-0.0621
-0.0364
0.00977
-0.0643
-0.00293
-0.601
-0.516
-0.461
-0.487
0.00901
0.0212
-0.00277
0.00295
0.0153
0.0381
-0.00485
0.00587
M ATHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-8.25
-11.9
-11.3
-14.6
-0.388
0.226
-0.343
-0.0628
-0.0472
0.0194
-0.0277
-0.00401
-0.679
-0.604
-0.532
-0.452
0.00807
0.0278
0.00852
0.00119
0.0138
0.0502
0.0151
0.00239
ASIAN HIGHER INCOME STUDENTS , DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE HIGHER INCOME STUDENTS
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
3.16
6.38
7.74
5.76
0.342
0.531
-0.211
-0.281
0.0369
0.0389
-0.0135
-0.0133
0.179
0.186
0.190
0.108
0.000761
0.00143
-0.00871
-0.00542
0.00129
0.00257
-0.0152
-0.0108
M ATHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
1.64
1.59
2.14
-.051
-0.00469
0.212
-0.281
0.0906
-0.000569
0.0182
-0.0227
0.00579
0.087
0.045
0.048
-0.024
-0.00444
0.00143
-0.00764
0.00295
-0.00758
0.00259
-0.0135
0.00594
ASIAN LOW INCOME STUDENTS , DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE HIGHER INCOME STUDENTS
READING
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
M ATHEMATICS
During Kindergarten
Summer Kindergarten-1st Grade
During 1st Grade
After 1st Grade, into 3rd Grade
-4.72
-0.000644
0.0460
-0.0422
-0.00981
-0.344
-4.77
-3.15
-9.34
-0.00597
0.628
-0.657
-0.208
-0.209
-0.141
-0.220
0.0143
0.0263
-0.00834
-0.00207
0.0243
0.0473
-0.0146
-0.00412
-4.55
-5.91
-3.97
-9.78
-0.144
0.754
-0.617
0.0745
-0.0175
0.0648
-0.0499
0.00476
-0.325
-0.283
-0.198
-0.293
0.00444
0.0328
-0.0101
0.00362
0.00758
0.0593
-0.0178
0.00729
94
High School
Base Model
First, the effect sizes are essentially the same—IRT scale score and theta score. Again, there is no reason to
think that these results should match so closely. Yet the gains per month reported in standard deviation
units (effect size columns, the third and sixth columns in Table 4.8) are very similar. Even the IRT scale
and theta score gains per month (reported in the second and fifth columns of estimates in Table 4.8) are
very similar. However, looking at Table 4.9, we can see that the ratio of the mean growth rate in the
second half of high school to the mean growth rate in the first half of high school is much more similar
when measured in standard deviation units, lending support to our focus on effect sizes in prior chapters.
TABLE 4.8: ESTIMATED GROWTH RATES IN NELS:88: COMPARISON OF IRT AND T HETA, ALL STUDENTS
Time Period
IRT
Starting
Level
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
Theta
Starting
Gain Per
Level
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
SCORES
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
28.3
31.9
0.152
0.0903
0.0199
0.00978
47.6
51.4
0.156
0.0971
0.0205
0.0102
38.2
45.9
0.324
0.178
0.0291
0.0136
46.6
52.04
0.227
0.133
0.0300
0.0145
M ATHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
TABLE 4.9: RATIOS OF ESTIMATED GROWTH RATES IN NELS:88: COMPARISON OF IRT AND T HETA, ALL STUDENTS
Time Period
IRT
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Theta
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
RATIOS
READING
10th to 12th Grade/8th to 10th Grade
0.594
0.498
0.622
0.491
M ATHEMATICS
10th to 12th Grade/8th to 10th Grade
0.549
0.467
0.586
0.483
Race/ethnicity model
The comparison of results using IRT scale scores and theta scores in Table 4.10 is less straightforward.
Looking only at effect size estimates, every estimate of the mean growth rate for white students and
comparison to this base group (chosen because they represent the largest fraction of students) has a similar
magnitude, and every difference from the base group has the same estimated sign using either IRT scale
scores or theta scores. However, it seems that black and Hispanic students slightly compare less favorably
to white and Asian students when using theta scores (positive coefficients measuring the difference from
white students tend to be smaller, and negative point estimate tend to be larger) in reading, and slightly
more favorably in math. White students compare less favorably to Asian students when using theta scores
(every positive point estimate for the difference of Asian students from white students is larger). Further
research would be required to pinpoint the source of these minor differences between results using IRT
scale scores and theta scores.
95
TABLE 4.10: ESTIMATED G ROWTH RATES IN NELS:88: COMPARISON OF IRT AND THETA, BY RACE
Time Period
IRT
Starting
Level
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
Theta
Starting
Gain Per
Level
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
W HITE STUDENTS ONLY
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
29.4
33.1
0.157
0.0885
0.0205
0.00959
48.7
52.6
0.161
0.0959
0.0212
0.0101
M ATHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
39.8
47.8
0.333
0.176
0.0299
0.0134
47.8
53.3
0.231
0.132
0.0305
0.0145
BLACK STUDENTS , DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE STUDENTS
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-5.49
-6.36
-0.0360
-0.0129
-0.00471
-0.00140
-5.44
-6.49
-0.0440
-0.0212
-0.00580
-0.00223
M ATHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-9.13
-10.6
-0.0625
-0.00472
-0.00560
-0.000359
-6.61
-7.41
-0.0331
-0.0106
-0.00438
-0.00115
HISPANIC STUDENTS , DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE STUDENTS
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-4.83
-5.12
-0.0122
0.0191
-0.00160
0.00207
-4.75
-5.22
-0.0197
0.0168
-0.00259
0.00178
M ATHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-7.00
-7.79
-0.0329
0.0168
-0.00295
0.00128
-4.99
-5.30
-0.0131
0.00594
-0.00174
0.000649
ASIAN STUDENTS, DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE STUDENTS
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-0.265
0.289
0.0231
0.0477
0.00302
0.00516
0.557
1.44
0.0232
0.0601
0.00306
0.00634
M ATHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
2.71
3.49
0.0326
0.0299
0.00292
0.00228
1.86
2.52
0.0277
0.0301
0.00366
0.00329
96
Language model
TABLE 4.11: ESTIMATED G ROWTH RATES IN ECLS-K: COMPARISON OF IRT AND T HETA, BY LANGUAGE
Time Period
Starting
Level
Theta
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
0.0199
0.00945
48.9
52.9
0.0205
0.00988
3.734
2.245
0.325
0.0292
0.176
0.0133
NON -ENGLISH AT HOME STUDENTS
46.9
52.3
0.227
0.131
0.0300
0.0143
Starting
Level
IRT
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
ENGLISH AT HOME STUDENTS
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
28.6
32.2
M ATHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
38.5
46.3
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-3.96
-3.87
0.00368
0.0406
0.000481
0.00440
-3.54
-4.40
-0.000533
0.00482
-0.097
1.096
M ATHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-4.01
-4.26
-0.0103
0.0314
-0.000926
0.00238
-5.86
-2.89
-0.000944
0.0224
-0.000125
0.00245
0.152
0.0873
Race/ethnicity by language model
Again, the gains per month reported in standard deviation units (effect size columns, the third and sixth
columns of estimates in Table 4.9) are very similar. Even the IRT scale and theta score gains per month
(reported in the second and fifth columns of estimates in Table 4.12) are very similar. Again white students
compare less favorably to Asian students when using theta scores than when using scale scores, but the
clear pattern is observed only for NEH Asian students. Most differences between the estimates of IRT
scale and theta score gains per month in Table 4.12 are quite small.
97
TABLE 4.12: ESTIMATED G ROWTH RATES IN NELS:88: COMPARISON OF IRT AND THETA, BY RACE AND LANGUAGE
Time Period
Starting
Level
IRT
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
Starting
Level
Theta
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
W HITE STUDENTS
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
29.4
33.1
0.157
0.0884
0.0205
0.00957
48.73
52.60
0.161
0.0957
0.0212
0.0101
M ATHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
39.8
47.5
0.333
0.176
0.0299
0.0134
47.8
53.3
0.231
0.132
0.0306
0.0145
BL ACK STUDENTS , DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE STUDENTS
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-5.47
-6.32
-0.0356
-0.0129
-0.00465
-0.00139
-5.41
-6.45
-0.0436
-0.0212
-0.00574
-0.00224
M ATHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-9.11
-10.6
-0.0623
-0.00469
-0.00559
-0.000356
-6.60
-7.39
-0.0331
-0.0107
-0.00437
-0.00116
HISPANIC ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS, DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE STUDENTS
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-3.17
-3.61
-0.0184
0.00231
-0.00240
0.000250
-3.13
-3.66
-0.0222
-0.00317
-0.00293
-0.000334
M ATHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-5.17
-5.97
-0.0332
0.00158
-0.00298
0.000120
-3.70
-4.05
-0.0146
-0.00569
-0.00193
-0.000622
HISPANIC NON -ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS , DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE STUDENTS
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-6.21
-6.34
-0.00517
0.0324
-0.000676
0.00350
-6.10
-6.48
-0.0158
0.0320
-0.00208
0.00338
M ATHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-8.50
-9.26
-0.0317
0.0282
-0.00284
0.00214
-6.03
-6.30
-0.0113
0.0145
-0.00150
0.00158
ASIAN ENGLISH SPEAKING HOME STUDENTS , DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE STUDENTS
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
0.361
0.890
0.0221
0.0372
0.00288
0.00402
0.366
0.812
0.0186
0.0513
0.00245
0.00542
M ATHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
2.82
3.41
0.0245
0.00612
0.00220
0.000465
1.88
2.45
0.0236
0.0138
0.00313
0.00151
ASIAN NON -ENGLISH SPEAKING HO ME S TUDENTS, DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE STUDENTS
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-1.20
-0.506
0.0288
0.0911
0.00376
0.00986
-1.13
-0.428
0.0293
0.107
0.00385
0.0113
M ATHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
2.97
3.71
0.0309
0.0719
0.00277
0.00547
2.07
2.74
0.0276
0.0603
0.00365
0.00659
98
Income model
TABLE 4.13: ESTIMATED G ROWTH RATES IN ECLS-K: COMPARISON OF IRT AND T HETA, BY LANGUAGE
Time Period
Starting
Level
Theta
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
0.0199
0.00945
48.9
52.9
0.0205
0.00988
3.734
2.245
0.325
0.0292
0.176
0.0133
NON -ENGLISH AT HOME STUDENTS
46.9
52.3
0.227
0.131
0.0300
0.0143
Starting
Level
IRT
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
ENGLISH AT HOME STUDENTS
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
28.6
32.2
M ATHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
38.5
46.3
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-3.96
-3.87
0.00368
0.0406
0.000481
0.00440
-3.54
-4.40
-0.000533
0.00482
-0.097
1.096
M ATHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-4.01
-4.26
-0.0103
0.0314
-0.000926
0.00238
-5.86
-2.89
-0.000944
0.0224
-0.000125
0.00245
0.152
0.0873
Race/ethnicity by income model
The gains per month reported in standard deviation units (effect size columns, the third and sixth columns
of estimates) in Table 4.14 are very similar, and the IRT scale and theta score gains per month are also
close. Again white students compare less favorably to Asian students when using theta scores than when
using scale scores, but the pattern is observed only for higher-income Asian students.
99
TABLE 4.14: L EARNING RATE DIFFERENCES ACROSS GRADE IN NELS:88: COMPARISON OF IRT AND T HETA, RACE
AND I NCOME
Time Period
Starting
Level
IRT
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
Starting
Level
Theta
Gain Per
Month
Effect
Size Per
Month
W HITE HIGHER INCOME STUDENTS
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
30.2
34.1
0.166
0.0812
0.0217
0.00880
49.5
53.6
0.172
0.0903
0.0226
0.00953
M ATHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
41.0
49.1
0.341
0.183
0.0306
0.0140
48.6
54.3
0.236
0.140
0.0312
0.0153
W HITE LOW INCOME STUDENTS, DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE HIGHER INCOME STUDENTS
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-2.77
-3.52
-0.0311
0.0249
-0.00407
0.00270
-2.72
-3.58
-0.0360
0.0191
-0.00474
0.00202
-4.10
-4.76
-0.0274
-0.0240
-0.00246
-0.00183
-2.84
-3.28
-0.0185
-0.0239
-0.00244
-0.00261
MaTHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
BLACK HIGHER INCOME STUDENTS, DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE HIGHER INCOME STUDENTS
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-4.60
-5.45
-0.0353
-0.00530
-0.00461
-0.000573
-4.58
-5.59
-0.0421
-0.0135
-0.00555
-0.00142
M ATHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-7.74
-8.94
-0.0501
-0.000609
-0.00450
-0.0000463
-5.53
-6.25
-0.0300
-0.00600
-0.00397
-0.000655
BLACK LOW INCOME S TUDENTS, DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE HIGHER INCOME STUDENTS
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
M ATHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
T ABLE CONTINUES ON NEXT P AGE
-7.52
-8.78
-0.0523
-0.00490
-0.00683
-0.000531
-7.40
-8.93
-0.0421
-0.0135
-0.00555
-0.00142
-12.2
-14.2
-0.0851
-0.0187
-0.00764
-0.00142
-8.82
-9.90
-0.0453
-0.0246
-0.00599
-0.00269
100
TABLE 4.14 (CONTINUED). L EARNING RATE DIFFERENCES ACROSS G RADE IN NELS:88: COMPARISON OF IRT AND
THETA, RACE AND INCOME
Time Period
Starting
Level
IRT
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
Starting
Level
Theta
Gain Per
Month
Effect Size
Per Month
HISPANIC HIGHER INCOME STUDENTS, DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE HIGHER INCOME STUDENTS
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-4.56
-5.23
-0.0283
0.00186
-0.00370
0.000201
-4.59
-5.38
-0.0330
0.0000913
-0.00434
0.00000964
-6.81
-7.11
-0.0123
0.0179
-0.00110
0.00136
-4.87
-4.86
0.000426
0.00642
0.0000564
0.000702
M ATHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
HISPANIC LOW INCOME S TUDENTS, DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE HIGHER INCOME STUDENTS
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-6.50
-6.89
-0.0162
0.0461
-0.00212
0.00499
-6.30
-6.98
-0.0283
0.0402
-0.00373
0.00425
M ATHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-9.32
-10.8
-0.0634
0.00314
-0.00569
0.000238
-6.58
-7.38
-0.0333
-0.00797
-0.00441
-0.000872
ASIAN HIGHER INCOME STUDENTS , DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE HIGHER INCOME STUDENTS
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
0.448
1.23
0.0327
0.0419
0.00427
0.00454
0.489
1.31
0.0342
0.0590
0.00450
0.00622
M ATHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
3.11
3.97
0.0361
0.0167
0.00324
0.00127
2.16
2.98
0.0342
0.0180
0.00453
0.00197
ASIAN LOW INCOME STUDENTS , DIFFERENCE RELATIVE TO W HITE HIGHER INCOME STUDEN TS
READING
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
M ATHEMATICS
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
-4.53
-0.00401
0.00933
-4.47
-5.27
-0.0307
0.0862
-5.39
-0.0382
0.0824
-0.00503
0.00869
-2.12
-2.19
-0.00283
0.0368
-0.000254
0.00280
-1.54
-1.65
-0.00466
0.0299
-0.000616
0.00327
Changes in Learning Rates Over Time
The rates presented in Tables 4.1 and 4.6 suggest that learning rates decline over time, as do the ratios in
Tables 4.2 and 4.9. For example, Table 4.6 shows a much larger drop in learning rates after first grade than
between kindergarten and first grade. Even in the more disaggregated results, the growth rate in second
and third grades, as measured in the effect size metric, is often less than half the first grade growth rate. In
comparison, the growth rates in first grade are generally much closer to or even larger than the
kindergarten rates. For example, across analyses, the ratio of the first grade gain to the kindergarten gain is
typically around 1.0 (in IRT scale effect sizes or theta scores), ranging from 0.812 (IRT, Asian EH) to
1.143 (theta, black low-income). This suggests that the kindergarten and first grade gains are similar in
magnitude, whereas the second and third grade gains are consistently and substantially smaller than the
first grade gains (thus the ratios hovering around 0.30 to 0.50). Gains in high school are substantially
101
smaller, in standard deviation units. However, there are a number of possible explanations for this pattern,
and only a small subset of them imply that students are actually learning less. See the conclusion of
Chapter 3, and Chapters 1 and 6, for more on this subject.
Changes in Learning Rates and Gaps Over Time
While we cannot directly compare learning rates within a type of student across time, we can more
reasonably compare the estimated gap across different types of students, and track changes in the size of
this gap over time. In this section, we use estimates from Chapter 3 and the tables earlier in this chapter to
construct estimates of the gap in scores at the end of each time period, scaled by the standard deviation of
scores at the end of each time period (from Appendix C), to measure the gap in contemporaneous
standard deviation units. Thus, girls are about a tenth of a standard deviation ahead of boys on Reading
tests at the start of kindergarten, and about two tenths of a standard deviation ahead of boys on Reading
tests at the end of third grade.
We compute these gaps for the Male-Female comparison (female students measured relative to male
students), the black-white comparison (black students measured relative to white students), the Low-High
Income comparison (low-income students measured relative to higher-income students), and the NonEnglish-English comparison (non-English-speaking students measured relative to English-speaking
students). We then graph these estimated gaps, in standard deviation units, over the ages at which
individuals are expected to reach at each of these time periods, in Figures 4.1 through 4.4.
We can compare the size of gaps at each point in time by comparing the level of each line to the light-grey
zero line (marked 0 on the ordinate, representing no difference from the reference group), or differences
in learning rates by comparing the slopes of the lines. We can also compare the gaps over time, though by
doing this later comparison, we are conflating results from two different surveys, representing different
populations of students. In addition, the estimates for the end of third grade are out-of-sample predictions
(which can be improved on using fifth grade data available in 2006). Given all these caveats, the gaps are
remarkably stable over time, and seem to indicate broad trends of interest. We focus here on a few
representative findings on gender, race, income, and language.
Girls seem to take an early lead over boys on reading tests (see Figure 4.1), and to maintain that lead from
the end of first grade through the end of high school. In contrast, boys gain a small advantage over girls on
Math tests (see Figure 4.2) in early grades. Boys begin high school with a lesser advantage, but make faster
gains, so they finish high school more that a tenth of a standard deviation ahead. These results are quite
robust whether using IRT scale scores or theta scores.
Black students start school at a large disadvantage relative to white students (Figures 4.3 and 4.4), in both
Reading and Math, and fall farther behind in elementary school, and then rebound somewhat by the end
of high school. Hispanic students (Figures 4.5 and 4.6) start out behind black students, more than half a
standard deviation behind white students in reading, and about three quarters of a standard deviation
behind white students in math, but they do not lose ground as do black students. In high school, Hispanic
students gain on white students, and end high school about half a standard deviation behind on both tests.
Black students do not make comparable gains on their white peers in high school, ending high school
about three quarters of a standard deviation behind on both tests.
The time path of the gap between low-income students and higher-income students (Figures 4.7 and 4.8)
is very similar to the path of the gap between black and white students, though low-income students start
with a bigger gap and have a smaller gap by the end of high school than do black students. Both gaps are
similar across Reading and Math tests, and across models using IRT scale scores or theta scores as the
outcome measure.
102
FIGURE 4.1 READING ACHIEVEMENT GAPS IN STANDARD D EVIATION U NITS, FEMALE COMPARED TO MALE STUDENTS
(BOTH SCALE AND THETA M ETRICS, USING ECLS AND NELS DATA)
.1
0
-.1
-.3 -.2
-.4
SD units
.2
.3
.4
Reading Gaps, Female vs. Male Students
Beg K
End 1
End 3
End 8
End 10
Grade Level
Reading Theta
103
Reading Scale
End 12
FIGURE 4.2 MATH ACHIEVEMENT GAPS IN STANDARD D EVIATION U NITS, FEMALE COMPARED TO MALE STUDENTS
(BOTH SCALE AND THETA M ETRICS, USING ECLS AND NELS DATA)
.1
0
-.1
-.2
-.3
-.4
SD units
.2
.3
.4
Math Gaps, Female vs. Male Students
Beg K
End 1
End 3
End 8
End 10
Grade Level
Math Theta
104
Math Scale
End 12
FIGURE 4.3 READING ACHIEVEMENT GAPS IN STANDARD D EVIATION U NITS, BLACK COMPARED TO W HITE STUDENTS
(BOTH SCALE AND THETA M ETRICS, USING ECLS AND NELS DATA)
-.8 -.7 -.6 -.5 -.4 -.3 -.2 -.1 0 .1 .2 .3 .4
SD units
Reading Gaps, Black vs. White
Beg K
End 1
End 3
End 8
End 10
Grade Level
Reading Theta
105
Reading Scale
End 12
FIGURE 4.4 MATH ACHIEVEMENT GAPS IN STANDARD D EVIATION U NITS, BLACK COMPARED TO W HITE STUDENTS
(BOTH SCALE AND THETA M ETRICS, USING ECLS AND NELS DATA)
-.9 -.8 -.7 -.6 -.5 -.4 -.3 -.2 -.1 0 .1 .2 .3 .4
SD units
Math Gaps, Black vs. White
Beg K
End 1
End 3
End 8
End 10
Grade Level
Math Theta
106
Math Scale
End 12
FIGURE 4.5 READING ACHIEVEMENT GAPS IN STANDARD D EVIATION U NITS, HISPANIC COMPARED TO W HITE STUDENTS
(BOTH SCALE AND THETA M ETRICS, USING ECLS AND NELS DATA)
-.7 -.6 -.5 -.4 -.3 -.2 -.1 0 .1 .2 .3 .4
SD units
Reading Gaps, Hispanic vs. White
Beg K
End 1
End 3
End 8
End 10
Grade Level
Reading Theta
107
Reading Scale
End 12
FIGURE 4.6 MATH ACHIEVEMENT GAPS IN STANDARD D EVIATION U NITS, HISPANIC COMPARED TO W HITE STUDENTS
(BOTH SCALE AND THETA M ETRICS, USING ECLS AND NELS DATA)
-.8 -.7 -.6 -.5 -.4 -.3 -.2 -.1 0 .1 .2 .3 .4
SD units
Math Gaps, Hispanic vs. White
Beg K
End 1
End 3
End 8
End 10
Grade Level
Math Theta
108
Math Scale
End 12
FIGURE 4.7 READING ACHIEVEMENT GAPS IN STANDARD D EVIATION U NITS, LOW-INCOME COMPARED TO HIGHER
INCOME STUDENTS (BOTH SCALE AND T HETA M ETRICS, USING ECLS AND NELS DATA)
-.8 -.7 -.6 -.5 -.4 -.3 -.2 -.1 0 .1 .2 .3 .4
SD units
Reading Gaps, Low-Income vs. Higher Income
Beg K
End 1
End 3
End 8
End 10
Grade Level
Reading Theta
109
Reading Scale
End 12
FIGURE 4.8 MATH ACHIEVEMENT GAPS IN STANDARD D EVIATION U NITS, LOW-INCOME COMPARED TO HIGHER I NCOME
STUDENTS (B OTH SCALE AND T HETA METRICS, USING ECLS AND NELS DATA)
-.8 -.7 -.6 -.5 -.4 -.3 -.2 -.1 0 .1 .2 .3 .4
SD units
Math Gaps, Low-Income vs. Higher Income
Beg K
End 1
End 3
End 8
End 10
Grade Level
Math Theta
110
Math Scale
End 12
FIGURE 4.9 READING ACHIEVEMENT GAPS IN STANDARD D EVIATION U NITS, NEH COMPARED TO EH STUDENTS
(BOTH SCALE AND THETA M ETRICS, USING ECLS AND NELS DATA)
-.6 -.5 -.4 -.3 -.2 -.1 0 .1 .2 .3 .4
SD units
Reading Gaps, NEH vs. EH
Beg K
End 1
End 3
End 8
End 10
Grade Level
Reading Theta
111
Reading Scale
End 12
FIGURE 4.10 MATH ACHIEVEMENT GAPS IN STANDARD D EVIATION U NITS, NEH COMPARED TO EH STUDENTS
(BOTH SCALE AND THETA M ETRICS, USING ECLS AND NELS DATA)
-.7 -.6 -.5 -.4 -.3 -.2 -.1 0 .1 .2 .3 .4
SD units
Math Gaps, NEH vs. EH
Beg K
End 1
End 3
End 8
End 10
End 12
Grade Level
Math Theta
Math Scale
In both data sets, the “Non-English at Home” (or NEH) variable measures whether a language other than
English spoken in the home. Among kindergartners in 1998, this is a strong predictor of limited English
proficiency (LEP), but among eighth-grade students in 1988, a language other than English spoken in the
home may represent a different bundle of individual characteristics. The two data source represent distinct
populations in at least two ways: first, the population of immigrants has changed over time in the US, and
second, the effect of a language other than English spoken at home almost certainly changes with age, and
probably has changed over the last decades. Nevertheless, the gap seems to exhibit a smooth trend over
time (Figures 4.9 and 4.10), with NEH students apparently starting school half a standard deviation or
more behind their peers in both Math and Reading tests, and gradually gaining until they are only about
three tenths of a standard deviation behind.
The population of students is not exactly comparable across the two surveys, since one survey represents
students who were in kindergarten in late 1998 and one represents students at the end of eighth grade in
early 1988 (implying that the two cohorts are nearly twenty years apart in age). Given that the preceding
graphs conflate estimates from many models and two different surveys representing different populations,
too much credence should not be placed in any specific conclusions drawn from them. They are intended
more to be an illustration of the types of results that may easily be constructed from the estimates
contained in this report. Future waves of the ECLS may offer more reliable estimates in later school years,
and could plausibly confirm or contradict these suggestive patterns.
112
Summary
Theta analyses in this chapter produced results that are generally more similar to the results in Chapters 2
and 3 that were based on effect sizes than to those based on the IRT scale scores before standardization.
They offer some support for the use of effect sizes in comparing outcomes across types of students, across
tests, and to a lesser extent, across time. Gaps across types of students measured in standard deviation
units seem to be exacerbated in elementary school, but to diminish somewhat by the end of high school,
though further research is needed to confirm this finding.
113
CHAPTER V: LOCALLY STANDARDIZED D IFFERENCES IN LEARNING RATES
In this chapter, we report estimates of the average locally standardized difference (LSD) in growth rates
between student subgroups. This approach measures differences in learning rates in a way that is less
sensitive to the details of test design than our regression analyses from Chapters 2 and 3, where IRT scores
are the dependent variable.
Brief Description of Method
The locally standardized difference technique compares the average learning rates during each time period
of students in different subgroups who start the time period with similar initial test scores (hence the ‘local’
feature of the estimates). This difference in learning rates at each initial test score level is then standardized
by dividing the difference by the pooled within-group standard deviation of growth rates (this pooled
standard deviation, too, is estimated locally). Details of the estimation are in Appendix E.
Each of these standardized estimates of learning rate differences is local to a particular baseline test score;
averaging across the entire distribution of baseline scores produces an average locally standardized
difference in growth rates. This estimate can be interpreted as the expected difference in test score growth
rates between two students of different subgroups who have the same initial test scores, expressed as a
fraction of the standard deviation of test score growth rates among students starting with this same initial
score.
Advantage of Method Over Linear Models
The advantage of this method of describing differences in growth rates is that it is robust to differences
across tests: regardless of what test metric the scores are reported in, we obtain virtually the same estimates
of locally standardized growth rate differences. The same cannot be said of the regression growth model
estimates reported in Chapter 3, as these estimates could be quite different if we used a different version of
the test metric. Prior to reporting the results from this approach, we briefly describe its rationale.
Any comparison of growth rates in math and reading between different population subgroups relies on the
assumption that test scores are interval-scaled—meaning that a one-point difference in scores at the high
end of the test scale, for example, represents a difference in skills equivalent to that indicated by a onepoint difference in scores at the low end of the scale. Without this assumption, it is difficult to determine
whether a one-point gain in average scores for a group that begins with low average scores represents a
gain in skills greater than, equal to, or less than that indicated by a one-point gain in average scores for a
group that starts with high average skills.
To make this more concrete, consider the following stylized example. Consider two students: student A,
who can reliably add one-digit numbers but cannot subtract, multiply, or divide; and student B, who can
reliably perform simple addition, subtraction, and multiplication, but not division. Suppose we administer
two arithmetic tests to each student at the beginning of the school year. The first test—test I—has a lot of
simple addition and subtraction items on it, and few multiplication and division items; the other—test II—
has few addition and subtraction items and many simple multiplication and division items on it. Clearly
student B will do better, on average, on both tests than will student A. This is illustrated in Figure 5.1: at
the start of the year, student A and B are at the skill levels labeled A1 and B1, respectively, at the start of
the year. Regardless of which test we use, student B scores higher than student A.
114
FIGURE 5.1: COMPARING STUDENT PERFORMANCE ON TWO H YPOTHETICAL MATH TESTS
Comparison of Two Hypothetical Arithmetic Tests
B2
test II
division
B1
multiplication
subtraction
addition
A2
A1
addition
subtraction
test I
multiplication division
Now suppose that student A becomes proficient in subtraction during the school year, but still cannot
multiply and divide well; and suppose that student B learns to perform simple division during the school
year. At the end of the school year, we administer the same two tests to each student—both of whom have
been in math classes during the interval. Student A, having learned to perform simple subtraction during
the interval, performs far better on test I than at time 1, but little better on test II (since he has yet to learn
to multiply and divide). In contrast, student B, because she already could add and subtract with
proficiency, performs little better on test I than at time 1, but performs much better on test II (since she
has learned division during the school year). In Figure 5.1, the end of the year scores of students A and B
will correspond to the points A2 and B2, respectively.
So which student has learned more during the interval between tests? As a practical matter, the answer will
depend on which test we give. According to test I, which weights subtraction skills more heavily than
division skills, student A ‘learned more’ (i.e., showed a greater gain in test score points) than student B
during second grade. According to test II, however, which weights division skills more than subtraction,
student B ‘learned more.’ And which test we decide to report depends on whether we think learning to
subtract represents a greater increase in math skills than does learning to divide. While there may be
arguments that resolve this dilemma on content grounds, test scores are often reported without specific
detail about the content and relative weight of the test items. Instead, the test is simply assumed to be an
interval-scaled measure of students’ skills in a broad domain, and the uncertainty about whether the
comparison of gain scores is meaningful is not noted because only a single test metric is generally available.
This stylized example suggests that any comparison of achievement growth rates is intricately tied up with
the metric of the test used to measure achievement. This issue has significant consequences for the
interpretation growth rates in achievement, since the dependence of the inferences on the test metric calls
into question any conclusions one might draw from a single test. This leaves us with a bit of a problem: we
want to know whether different subgroups have different learning rates, but our answer depends on the
115
generally unjustifiable assumption that the test metric is interval-scaled. Moreover, in a study like ECLS-K,
where test scores are available in several metrics, we will get different answers (as was evident in Chapter
4) depending on which metric we use—a highly unsatisfactory result.
So how are we to address this issue? Returning to the example above, consider as well a third student C,
who begins the school year knowing how to add, but not to subtract, just as does student A (at the skill
level indicated by A1). Student C, however, learns not only subtraction, but also multiplication and division
over the course of the school year, and so at the end of the school year has arithmetic skills equivalent to
student B (at B2). Now, regardless of which test we use, it will be clear that student C has learned more
than student A. Moreover, this will be evident on any test of arithmetic skills which has a monotonic
relationship to tests I and II. The important point here is that, because students C and A start with the
same skills, any test that ranks multiplication and division skills higher than subtraction skills will lead us to
conclude that student C learned more than student A during the school year. This insight suggests that we
could make unambiguous comparisons of growth rates if we restrict our comparisons to students with the
same initial scores. We use such an approach in this chapter.
Limitations of the LSD Method
While the locally standardized difference methods has the advantage of producing results that are much
less dependent on the assumption that test scores are interval-scaled measures of achievement, it is not
without its own drawbacks. First, the method assumes that initial scores are measured without error; this
is, of course, unlikely to be true. The consequence of measurement error in the test scores is twofold: 1) it
will bias estimates of the difference in growth rates upward (away from zero); and 2) it will bias estimates
of the local standard deviation upward. Since the locally standardized difference is the ratio of these two,
the two biases will tend to at least partially cancel one another out, though not necessarily completely. So
the resulting estimates may be biased, though the direction of bias is not clear. Second, the method is less
familiar, and hence, the results less interpretable perhaps. In brief, the average locally standardized
difference is the estimated average difference in growth rates between two students from different
subgroups who start with the same initial test score, expressed in terms of the standard deviation of
growth rates among individuals starting with the same initial scores. Third, the method does not make full
use of the longitudinal nature of the data.
Locally Standardized Difference Estimates
Gender
Reading
Growth rate differences in reading by gender are presented in Table 5.1. Over the course of kindergarten
through third grade, female students develop reading skills, on average, at a rate one-tenth of a standard
deviation faster than male students who start kindergarten with similar initial reading skills. The reading
growth differences between males and females are most pronounced in kindergarten, and are small or
nonexistent in the later grades.
Math
In contrast to reading patterns, over the course of kindergarten through third grade, female students
develop math skills, on average, at a rate roughly one-quarter of a standard deviation slower than male
students who start kindergarten with similar initial math skills. There is no significant math learning rate
difference between males and females in kindergarten, but growth rates begin to diverge slightly in first
grade and then diverge substantially in second and third grade, so that during the period from the end of
first grade to the start of third grade, female students’ math learning rates are, on average, one-fifth of a
standard deviation slower than those of male students who have similar math skills at the end of first
grade.
116
TABLE 5.1: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING AND MATH L EARNING RATES BY G ENDER—ELEM ENTARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Reading
(R2 Metric)
Reading
(Theta Metric)
FEMALE-M ALE LOCALLY STANDARDIZED LEARNING RATE DIFFERENCE
Overall K-3rd
0.104
During K
Summer K-1st
During 1 st
After 1st, into 3 rd
0.113
0.052
0.017
0.065
Math
(R2 Metric)
Math
(Theta Metric)
0.105
-0.269
-0.270
0.124
0.054
0.034
0.061
-0.043
-0.040
-0.092
-0.211
-0.037
-0.029
-0.087
-0.209
These analyses are based on all students in the ECLS-K who had valid scores at the two waves corresponding to the endpoints
of each period. Estimates in bold are significantly different from zero at the 5 percent level. Descriptions of models are
provided in Appendix E.
Race/Ethnicity
Reading
Table 5.2 indicates substantial differences in learning rates in reading skills among race/ethnic groups.
Over the course of kindergarten through third grade, black students develop reading skills, on average, at a
rate almost two-thirds of a standard deviation slower than white, non-Hispanic students who start
kindergarten with similar initial reading skills. The reading growth differences between black and white
students are most pronounced in second and third grade, but are substantial through kindergarten and first
grade as well.
Over the course of kindergarten through third grade, Hispanic students develop reading skills, on average,
at a rate about one-seventh of a standard deviation slower than white, non-Hispanic students who start
kindergarten with similar initial reading skills. Note, however, that these estimates are based only on the 70
percent of Hispanic kindergarten students sufficiently proficient in English to be assessed in reading in
kindergarten. The estimated reading growth differences between Hispanic and white students are not
significantly different from zero in the kindergarten year (but again, these are based only on Englishproficient Hispanic kindergartners), but are moderately large in first, second, and third grades. These
differences across grades may be attributable, in part, to the fact that the later estimates are based on a
larger population of Hispanic students, since more were proficient enough by first grade to take the
reading assessments.
In the primary grades, Asian students develop reading skills, on average, at about the same rate as those of
white, non-Hispanic students who start kindergarten with similar initial reading skills. As above, these
estimates are based only on the 77 percent of Asian kindergarten students sufficiently proficient in English
to be assessed in reading in kindergarten. The estimated reading growth differences between Asian and
white students are large and positive in kindergarten, but large and negative in second and third grade. As
above, these differences across grades may be attributable, in part, to the fact that the later estimates are
based on a larger population of Asian students, since more were proficient enough by first grade to take
the reading assessments.
Math
Over the course of kindergarten through third grade, black students develop math skills, on average, at a
rate roughly one-half of a standard deviation slower than white students who start kindergarten with
similar initial math skills. In kindergarten, first grade, and second and third grades, the difference in growth
rates between black and white students is roughly one-quarter to one-third of a standard deviation, while
during the summer between kindergarten and first grade the difference in growth rates is much smaller.
117
Estimates of Hispanic-white differences in math learning rates show a somewhat puzzling pattern. Over
the full course of kindergarten through third grade, Hispanic and white students who start kindergarten
with similar initial math skills show no significant difference in learning rates. However, in each of the
school years from kindergarten through third grade, Hispanic students learn math skills at a rate roughly
one-seventh of a standard deviation slower than white students who start with the same level of math skills
at the start of the grade. This discrepancy is in part a result of the fact tha t the standard deviation of
growth rates varies across the range of initial scores, the difference in growth rates varies across the initial
range of scores, and the resulting shifts in the distribution of Hispanic students’ scores relative to white
students’ scores means that the average growth rates are computed over different parts of the score
distribution at each wave.
Over the course of kindergarten through third grade, Asian students develop math skills, on average, at a
rate slightly greater than white, non-Hispanic students who start kindergarten with similar initial reading
skills. As above, however, these estimates are based only on the 77 percent of Asian kindergarten students
sufficiently proficient in English to be assessed in math in kindergarten. The estimated math growth
differences between Asian and white students during kindergarten and the K-1 summer are similar (about
a tenth of standard deviation higher for Asian students). In first grade, however, Asian students learn math
skills at a rate a fifth to a quarter of a standard deviation slower than white students who start first grade
with comparable skill levels, while in second and third grade they learn faster than comparable white
students. As above, these differences across grades may be attributable, in part, to the fact that the later
estimates are based on a larger population of Asian students, since more were proficient enough in English
to take the math assessments in the later grades.
TABLE 5.2: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING AND MATH L EARNING RATES BY RACE/ ETHNICITY—ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Reading
(Theta Metric)
Math
(R2 Metric)
Math
(Theta Metric)
BLACK-W HITE LOCALLY STANDARDIZED LEARNING RATE DIFFERENCE
Overall K-3rd
-0.629
-0.637
-0.485
-0.503
-0.237
-0.113
-0.244
-0.586
-0.33
-0.136
-0.252
-0.327
-0.322
-0.114
-0.237
-0.332
HISPANIC-W HITE LOCALLY S TANDARDIZED LEARNING RATE DIFFERENCE
Overall K-3rd
-0.136
-0.145
-0.042
-0.052
-0.001
-0.027
-0.166
-0.341
-0.134
-0.06
-0.162
-0.134
-0.139
-0.054
-0.145
-0.149
-0.064
0.104
0.055
0.305
-0.122
0
-0.284
0.06
0.114
-0.264
0.152
0.12
0.172
-0.194
0.113
During K
Summer K-1st
During 1 st
After 1 st, into 3 rd
During K
Summer K-1st
During 1 st
After 1 st, into 3 rd
Reading
(R2 Metric)
-0.21
-0.111
-0.272
-0.587
-0.002
-0.049
-0.183
-0.34
ASIAN -W HITE LOCALLY STANDARDIZED LEARNING RATE DIFFERENCE
Overall K-3rd
-0.084
During K
Summer K-1st
During 1 st
After 1 st, into 3 rd
0.288
0.138
-0.058
-0.298
These analyses are based on all students in the ECLS-K who had valid scores at the two waves corresponding to the endpoints
of each period. Estimates in bold are significantly different from zero at the 5 percent level. Descriptions of models are
118
provided in Appendix E. Note that substantial numbers of Hispanic and Asian students did not take the reading test in the early
waves (and substantial numbers of Asian students did not take the math test in early waves) because of limited English
proficiency. Thus the differences in growth rates for these groups estimated for the early periods are based on a smaller (and
more skilled) population than those estimated for the later periods.
Economic Status
Reading
Table 5.3 presents the growth rate differences by economic status. Over the course of kindergarten
through third grade, higher-income students develop reading skills, on average, at a rate roughly two-fifths
of a standard deviation faster than low-income students who start kindergarten with similar initial reading
skills. The reading growth differences between higher- and low-income students are most pronounced in
second and third grade, but are substantial through kindergarten and first grade as well.
Math
Similarly, over the course of kindergarten through third grade, higher-income students develop math skills,
on average, at a rate roughly one-quarter of a standard deviation slower than low-income students who
start kindergarten with similar initial math skills. As with reading development, the growth differences
between higher- and low-income students are most pronounced in second and third grade, but are
substantial through kindergarten and first grade as well.
TABLE 5.3: DIFFERENCES IN R EADING AND MATH L EARNING RATES BY ECONOMIC STATUS —ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Reading
(R2 Metric)
Reading
(Theta Metric)
LOWER -INCOME - HIGHER -INCOME LOCALLY STANDARDIZED LEARNING RATE DIFFERENCE
Overall K-3rd
-0.39
-0.385
During K
Summer K-1st
During 1 st
After 1st, into 3 rd
-0.145
-0.223
-0.17
-0.42
-0.150
-0.231
-0.174
-0.415
Math
(R2 Metric)
Math
(Theta Metric)
-0.234
-0.240
-0.136
-0.171
-0.159
-0.316
-0.148
-0.170
-0.160
-0.316
These analyses are based on all students in the ECLS-K who had valid scores at the two waves corresponding to the endpoints
of each period. Estimates in bold are significantly different from zero at the 5 percent level. Descriptions of models are
provided in Appendix E.
Summary
This chapter uses a different analytic approach from the previous three chapters, yet draws similar
conclusions. For example, as presented in Chapter 3, the gender gap favors girls in reading and boys in
math. Black students’ reading and math test scores start behind and lag behind those of white students on
average. The black-white achievement gap is widest when measured during the school year and least when
measured over the summer. Asian students exceed white students’ learning in math and keep pace in
reading. As also seen in Chapter 3’s results, low-income students score lower than higher-income students
in both math and reading, and this gap widens, until it is most pronounced in second and third grades.
Where the results diverge, and what represents the advantages of this chapter’s analyses, is potentially
better estimates for differences in growth rates in second and third grade between certain subgroups. The
rescaling of the ECLS-K assessments may affect the subgroups differently, and results for the most recent
round is the most likely to be affected by rescaling issues (see Appendix B for details). The figures in
Chapter 4 illustrate that the estimated differences in growth rates in second and third grade and the levels
at the end of third grade seem to diverge somewhat from our other estimates, and they will likely be much
changed by the addition of fifth grade data. The LSD method offers the potential to produce estimates
119
more robust to rescaling, in the sense that they may change less when fifth grade ECLS data are added to
the analysis, and newly rescaled scores are used.
Using the growth models, differences in achievement gains are estimated to consistently shrink during the
second and third grade time period. However, using the LSD method, the differences in growth rates are
most pronounced during second and third grades, as would be expected in light of the figures from
Chapter 4. The race, language status, and income status gaps all become much larger between the end of
first grade and the end of third grade. Yet the estimated gaps in high school are more comparable with the
estimated gaps at the end of first grade. In the locally standardized analyses, the gaps during the second
and third grade time periods are greater than in previous grades, which suggest a more linear expansion in
the differences. This expansion seems even greater than what the high school data imply exists in the later
grades. Either the gaps legitimately widen in late elementary school then return in the middle school years,
or these larger estimated gaps at the end of third grade represent bias in the estimated gap trajectory due to
rescaling and other data-specific issues—once subsequent rounds of ECLS data are made available, the
resolution to this question may become clear.
In these locally standardized analyses, Hispanic students learn reading at the same rate as white students in
kindergarten. Differences in rates of achievement gain in reading emerge in first grade, and the difference
nearly doubles during second and third grades. However, in the growth curve analyses, Hispanic students,
from either low-income or higher-income backgrounds or from English-speaking or non-English speaking
households, start behind and their gains continue to lag behind those of white students (see Table 3.3).
The difference is most pronounced during first grade and decreases dramatically during second and third
grades.
In addition, the findings in this chapter confirm the convergence of the IRT and theta results. Using the
IRT or the theta scores in these locally standardized difference analyses present highly similar findings. In
the analysis by income, the estimates would be the same if rounded to the second significant figure. Larger
discrepancies between the theta and the IRT score results emerge in the analysis by race, where the
difference ranges from 0.01 to 0.03 in magnitude.
120
CHAPTER VI: D ISCUSSION AND IMPLICATIONS
This report examines achievement gains by U.S. students in the early elementary school grades and in the
high school grades. Our findings quantify the gains students make, on average, across one and two-year
time spans in elementary and high school. We also quantify the differences in achievement, and differences
in achievement gains, across subgroups defined by gender, race, family income, and English-speaking
homes.
These estimated achievement gains serve as important benchmarks for researchers and policymakers. They
set a context of how much ground the average student makes in reading or in math in a given year.
Researchers planning interventions need to know what magnitude of effect is realistic to expect within this
context. Policymakers who interpret findings from such interventions to determine funding require such a
context to deem a program effective or not. Estimates broken down by subgroups allow researchers and
policymakers to more accurately predict the performance of students in settings where the distribution of
students’ demographic characteristics does not conform to national averages.
Our measures of how much students gain over different time periods, and how the differences across
different types of student are magnified or minimized over time will also inform the debate over where
educational resources should be targeted. Parents, educators, administrators, policymakers, and researchers
may find suggestive evidence that directly bears on questions about what kids should be expected to learn
over time, a nd how and when some students fall behind others. The report may raise many questions, as
well, and in this chapter, we both summarize the findings and discuss some possible implications and
directions for future research.
Achievement Gains Over Time
In general, our findings indicate that students appear to make greater gains on reading and math tests in
elementary school than in secondary school. On reading and math assessments, kindergartners and firstgraders gain one and a half to two standard deviations per year. Over the two-year time span between the
end of first grade and the end of third grade, children move nearly two standard deviations up the
distribution of first-grade scores, for both reading and math scores, indicating a rate of gain just over half
that observed in the first two years. Over a two-year time span in high school between the end of eighth
grade and the end of tenth, students move about sixth tenths of a standard deviation up the distribution of
eighth-grade scores in math and about four tenths of a standard deviation up the distribution of eighthgrade scores in reading. The implied learning rate is thus one quarter or one third as fast during ninth and
tenth grades as in second and third, and the implied learning rate in eleventh and twelfth grades is half that
of ninth and tenth grades.
There are a number of plausible explanations for the apparent slowdown of learning. First, the reading and
math assessments include questions about concepts and skills that may be taught less in later grades, so
may be learning more social studies and science, rather than reading or basic math. Second, the underlying
variation in math and reading skills may increase over time, so that gains expressed in standard deviation
units appear smaller relative to the variation in the population, but we argue throughout the report that
some type of standardization is necessary to compare gains made during different time periods, or from
different initial levels. Finally, it may be that there are decreasing returns to instruction, and more students
learn at a lower rate once they have learned most of the material taught prior to high school (so they are on
the “flatter” part of their individual learning curves). Notice that the last explanation is the only one we
have considered that implies a slower learning in later grades.
121
If we accept a standardized measure of progress in reading and math as a reasonable proxy for overall
learning, we find a dramatic slowdown in learning that begins earlier than previously thought. A recent
report from the Fordham Foundation (Yecke and Finn 2005) generalized that children “do reasonably
well” in elementary school but falter in middle school and by high school, their academic performance is
weak. Our analyses here support the observations about high school, since we find a relatively quick pace
in reading and math gains early in schooling and a slower pace later in schooling. However, our findings
indicate that students may begin to falter earlier than middle school. From some of our estimates, it seems
that children gain less in second and third grades than in kindergarten and first grade, but forthcoming
ECLS-K data from the fifth grade assessment should help clarify the pattern of math and reading learning
rates over time.
Gaps in Achievement Gains, and Gaps in Achievement
The performance of the average student masks large differences between students who differ by gender,
race and ethnicity, native language, or family income. We focus on these categories because they have been
explored in prior research, and because the size and mean performance of these groups are usually easily
identifiable in school or regional data, which gives our results maximal utility for policymakers, researchers,
and educational administrators. The gap between groups is of interest in its own right, as well, and better
measures of gaps in achievement or learning at different points in time may help better direct educational
resources.
The gender gap in reading seems to be in place before school begins. Girls start kindergarten about one
tenths to two tenths of a standard deviation ahead of boys on reading assessments and the gap favoring
girls does not widen or close substantially by the end of third grade. Girls seem to be about two tenths of a
standard deviation ahead of boys on reading tests throughout high school. In math, the gender gap takes
quite a different form. Boys and girls enter kindergarten with roughly the same score on a math
assessment. But boys gain faster than girls on math tests throughout the early years of school , so that boys
are about a tenth of a standard deviation ahead in third grade (boys are about a tenth of a standard
deviation ahead throughout high school as well). Past work with small datasets that are not nationally
representative suggest that the middle school years are when gender gaps in math achievement emerge
(Eccles; Midgley). However, recently released data from the TIMMS study suggests the gender gap in math
achievement starts early in America, and our results confirm this finding.
Achievement gaps by ethnicity, by language status, and by income have made national headlines in light of
the No Child Left Behind Act of 2001. The black-white achievement gap has attracted attention for years,
but No Child Left Behind also focuses attention on Hispanic children, children who speak English as a
second language, and economically disadvantaged children. Their achievement differences compared to
white students, English speakers, and economically more advantaged students are now estimated and
publicized. This report presents achievement gain differences among these student subgroups.
Black students start kindergarten about six tenths of a standard deviation behind white students on math
tests, and about four tenths of a standard deviation behind white students on reading tests. These same
students are about three quarters of a standard deviation behind by third grade on both tests, so they have
lost ground (learned at a slower rate) relative to white students. Hispanic students, on the other hand, start
farther behind black students on average, but gain faster. On math tests, Hispanic students start
kindergarten about three quarters of a standard deviation behind white students, but are only six tenths of
a standard deviation behind white students by third grade. On reading tests, Hispanic students start
kindergarten about six tenths of a standard deviation behind white students, and are about three quarters
of a standard deviation behind white students by third grade. Black students are about eight tenths of a
standard deviation behind white students in high school math tests, whereas Hispanic students are about
122
six tenths of a standard deviation behind white students. Both black and Hispanic students are about half a
standard deviation behind white students in high school reading tests. In short, black students start school
slightly behind, but have slower growth in scores than other groups, so wind up severely disadvantaged by
high school, even compared to other disadvantaged groups.
Asian students are a minority group for which the achievement gap with white students is reversed. Asian
students begin kindergarten with a significant advantage in reading, which is no longer evident by third
grade. Asian students begin high school at similar achievement levels to white students in reading test, but
gain slightly faster, so they are more than a tenth of a standard deviation ahead by the end of high school.
On the math assessment, white and Asian students start kindergarten essentially even, and remain at
similar levels through the end of third grade. Asian students begin high school two or three tenths of a
standard deviation ahead of white students on math tests, on average, and gain faster than white students
throughout high school.
Differences by language status matter to achievement as well. Students from households where English is
not the first language start kindergarten about half a standard deviation behind other students in reading
and in math. In math, the gap shrinks significantly in both our elementary and high school samples, and
the gap is only a quarter of a standard deviation at the end of high school. In reading, students from
households where English is not the first language make no substantial gains on other students during
elementary school, but the gap is smaller at the start of high school, and shrinks to about three tenths of a
standard deviation by the end of high school.
As students from non-English households spend more time in school and with peers and teachers who
speak English, their academic disadvantage diminishes. Non-native speakers may not only improve their
language skills from immersion in a predominantly English-speaking environment but also feel more
comfortable asking for clarification in an ever more familiar environment. The narrowing of the gap
during elementary school is more evident in math than in reading. It is unclear why this would be the case,
and it is possible that some portion of this result is due to sample selection that arises from the exclusion
from the reading test of Spanish speakers who fail a placement exam. As previously excluded sample
members pass the placement exam and take the reading test, these students may reduce the mean
performance of students from non-English households in the latter grades of elementary school.
Differences by subgroup membership are often attributed to differences in home resources. And
differences by economic status are consistently wide. In both reading and math, students from
economically disadvantaged backgrounds start kindergarten with a significant, substantial disadvantage
compared to their more advantaged peers. This gap is more than half a standard deviation through
elementary school, and about half a standard deviation throughout high school. It is not clear whether lowincome students are gaining between third grade and eighth grade, since the comparability of the data
between the elementary and high school samples is subpar for income and poverty measures.
Compared to other large gaps in achievement, such as the black-white gap, the language status gap, and the
gender gap, the economic status gap is quite large at the start of kindergarten, and is of comparable size to
the gap between Hispanic and white students. Only the Hispa nic students who speak Spanish at home are
at a greater disadvantage. But over time, Hispanic students who speak Spanish at home make substantial
gains relative to white students, and low-income students seem to keep pace with higher income students,
while black students fall farther and farther behind. By the end of high school, the black-white gap is much
larger than all other achievement gaps.
These analyses by individual subgroup raise the question of what happens when students not only are
economically disadvantaged but also belong to a minority group that does not make as much reading and
123
math gain as white students. Indeed, black low-income students comprise the only subgroup for whom
reading and math achievement gaps consistently widen, from elementary school through high school. Most
fascinating and perplexing, these analyses show the limits of home financial resources to academic
achievement. Black higher-income students begin kindergarten with average test scores nearly the same as
white higher-income students, but fall behind in subsequent years of schooling. Hispanic higher-income
students start kindergarten behind white and black higher-income students, but their gap compared to
white higher-income students does not widen as much as the black-white gap.
White, black, and Hispanic low-income students all start with deep deficits in achievement compared to
white higher-income students and continue to lose ground by gaining less in eighth through tenth grades.
However, only white low-income students continue to face widening gaps later in high school and make
significantly slower average gain in math. There are several possible explanations for this finding, including
selection as white students exhibit lower dropout rates, or the higher proportion of black and Hispanic
students who fall into the low-income category, which may imply that the low-income category includes
students of higher mean ability among black and Hispanic students than white students.
Methodological Caveats
There are several caveats to interpreting this work that must be noted. First, the use of achievement tests
to capture what students know is not straightforward. Tests are more sensitive to design issues than
intuition would suggest, even after sophisticated manipulation based on Item Response Theory. Virtually
any desired change in estimates of learning rates could be generated by an appropriate reweighting of the
test toward items of certain difficulty levels.
We have focused throughout the report on measuring gains or differences in achievement across students
in terms of standard deviations, and used several methods to make these comparisons, because in this way
we can get a sense of the range of possible estimates, and reach more robust conclusions. However, the
conclusions are dependent on measures of variability as well as growth rates and gap estimates, and are still
only as good as the tests on which they are based.
Second, our results, which generalize across all students in the nation, may not be robust to inclusion of
other explanatory factors, such as school characteristics (e.g., racial/ethnic composition of schools). Future
analyses, discussed in a subsequent section, will include more factors that might influence student learning.
Despite these limitations, the results may be interpreted as the estimated achievement gains made by all
students and as the estimated learning differences among specific subgroups defined by such
characteristics as ethnicity and income status. None of the caveats mentioned in the report overwhelm the
usefulness of our findings as benchmarks for how much elementary school and high school students learn.
Policy Recommendations
It is clear from the analytic results that the achievement gaps more frequently discussed in relation to later
grades are already present at the start of kindergarten and in first grade. Most achievement gaps observed
at school entry seem to remain at similar magnitudes over time, except the black-white gap, which widens
over time. Achievement gaps that exist in third grade among students who were kindergartners in 1998
often seem similar to those observed at the start of high school in 1988. Addressing these gaps early may
be the key to closing them. Results in this report suggest that efforts should be targeted towards younger
children, when gaps tend to be smaller, and when growth rates seem highest.
124
Indeed, policymakers have endeavored for decades to provide preschool interventions to give
economically disadvantaged children a boost before they start school. Widespread federal interventions
such as Early Start and Head Start begin before school begins so that achievement gaps may be narrowed
before students reach kindergarten. Our analyses indicate that achievement gaps already exist by
kindergarten, and often widen most for the most disadvantaged subgroups.
Critics of federal preschool programs, e.g., Head Start, cite relatively weak, ephemeral impacts on academic
performance; however, Head Start attempts to compensate for more than a shortage of cognitive or
academic resources among disadvantaged children. The program’s emphasis on social development and
health practices may be valuable, but may have less impact on the academic disadvantage with which lowincome or minority students begin school.
Our results suggest that more cognitively- and academically-focused interventions should begin earlier
when learning appears to be faster and the subsequent payoff potentially richer. Getting disadvantaged
students to the point of recognizing beginning and ending sounds to words by the fall of kindergarten, for
example, could help reduce the gap in initial status on reading assessments. Continuing intensive cognitive
stimulation during school, whether through tutoring services or through after-school programs, might help
economically disadvantaged students further. Such interventions should be targeted to specific populations
and at specific timepoints. Targeted students should be those who most clearly need a boost before they
begin school and those who fall behind during school—these are not necessarily the same students.
The results of this report play their most significant and crucial role for researchers and policymakers who
wish to understand the effectiveness of interventions. Our results set benchmarks against which to
evaluate the estimated impacts of educational interventions. Young students can make on average gains of
about 2 standard deviations in reading during kindergarten and during first grade and in second and third
grades combined. An intervention that improves achievement from kindergarten to first grade by less than
a tenth of a standard deviation may not provide a sufficiently effective improvement to students’ learning.
And since the black-white achievement gap in math increases by about 0.40 standard deviations per
academic year, an intervention that improves black students’ scores by 0.05 standard deviations per year
will do little to narrow that achievement gap substantially.
These analyses also suggest that testing children in academic subjects is possible before the third grade,
when the federal education programs require initial assessment. In light of our findings, unearthing
achievement gaps in third grade may be too late to change course and close gaps. Assessing achievement in
kindergarten and first grade is possible, and there is interesting, crucial variation in both achievement and
learning rates to study.
Closing achievement gaps between subgroups is not the only policy focus suggested by these results.
During school, the early years should be acknowledged as a time of fast cognitive growth. This time
demands sufficient parental and instructional support to make sure children can make the great leaps in
reading and math skills that our results indicate are possible. Well-prepared teachers, intensive
interventions, and programs to increase parental participation in cognitive activities at home all should be
considered as factors that may bolster the achievement gains made in elementary school.
Future Work
In the 2006 calendar year, the fifth-grade data from the ECLS-K study should be made available. From
these data, we can learn what happens to children’s learning and to the gaps in learning rates as students
transition out of elementary school and into middle school. The current work shows achievement gains
125
and differential growth rates in elementary school and in high school. The future fifth-grade data can
provide a bridge between the two analyses.
Future work will consider how differences in learning trajectories may derive from differences in school
characteristics. The importance of school context cannot be ignored, and we plan to construct three-level
hierarchical linear models to determine the extent to which achievement gains vary by school.
One of the significant contributions of this work is aligning achievement gains to actual skills that students
gain. This helps to make test scores tangible and comprehensible to a lay audience. Gains in points mean
less in interpretation than gains in the actual reading and math skills the assessments are designed to test.
Our next analyses will examine the proportions of children that achieve at or above certain benchmarks,
such as mastering problem-solving in math or comprehension of complex reading passages.
Future analyses will also examine the evolution of variation in test scores, both for all students, and any
differences among the relevant subgroups. Especially given the use of standard deviation estimates to
standardize gaps and growth rates, changes in the variance in test scores across individuals could produce
equally large observed changes in estimated learning rates as any plausible intervention in schooling. Thus,
we plan to characterize the evolution of variance, and the influence of a variety of predictors of changes to
the variance in test scores across individuals over time.
Conclusion
This report presented the average achievement gains in reading and math made by nationally
representative samples of elementary school students and of high school students. Achievement gains are
estimated for all students in the sample and then are disaggregated by subgroup membership, including by
gender, ethnicity, language status, income status, and the intersections of ethnicity and language status and
very importantly, or ethnicity and income status. These achievement gains provide benchmarks for
researchers, program designers, and policymakers to understand the amount of learning to be expected in
these grades. Estimates should provide a useful context to compare impact estimates of extant programs,
to interpret children’s progress in skill attainment, to design future experimental studies, and to develop
and target future interventions.
These are good estimates based on the data available to us now. Once the fifth grade ECLS data, collected
in the spring of 2004, are analyzed, estimates using newly rescaled test scores from that wave may differ
substantially from these estimates, particularly for the period following first grade and extending into third
grade, but potentially for all elementary school grades.
126
REFERENCES
Burkam, D.T., Ready, D.D., Lee, V.E, & LoGerfo, L.F. (2004). Social-class differences in summer learning
between kindergarten and first grade: Model specification and estimation. Sociology of Education, 77(1), 1-31.
Downey, D.B., Broh, B.A., & von Hippel, P.T. (2004). Are schools the great equalizer? Cognitive
inequality during the summer months and the school year. American Sociological Review, 69(3), 613-635.
Fryer, R.G., & Levitt, S.D. (2004). Understanding the Black-White test score gap in the first two years of
school. Review of Economics and Statistics, 86(2), 447-464.
Goldhaber, D. & Brewer, D. (2000). Does teacher certification matter? High school teacher certification
status and student achievement. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 22, 129-145.
Jencks, C. & Phillips, M. (1998). The Black-White Test Score Gap. Washington, DC: Brookings.
Lee, V.E., & Burkam, D.T. (2002). Inequality at the Starting Gate: Social Background Differences in Achievement as
Children Begin School. Washington, DC: Economic Policy Institute.
Nye, B., Konstantopoulos, S., & Hedges, L.V. (2004). “How large are teacher effects?” Educational
Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 26(3), 237-257.
Reardon, Sean. (2003). “Sources of educational inequality: The growth of racial/ethnic and socioeconomic
test score gaps in kindergarten and first grade.” Population Research Institute Working Paper 03-05R.
Sanbonmatsu, Lisa, Jeffrey R. Kling, Greg J. Duncan, and Jeanne Brooks-Gunn. “Neighborhoods and
Academic Achievement: Results from the Moving to Opportunity Experiment.” NBER Working Paper
No. 11909 (available at nber.org).
Yecke, Cheri Pierson & Chester E. Finn, Jr. (2005). Mayhem in the middle: How middle schools have failed
America. Washington, DC: Thomas B. Fordham Foundation.
127
A PPENDIX A: D ETAILS OF DATA AND METHODS
This section briefly describes the methodology for the report. It opens with discussing how the analytic
sample differs from the survey sample for the ECLS-K and NELS:88 data. The variables included in the
models are described next, which is followed by a concise description of the analytic method.
ECLS-K
The ECLS-K study was constructed with a multi-stage, probability sample design (NCES, 2000). The
primary sampling units were geographic areas, from which 1,280 public and private schools offering
kindergarten in the base year were sampled. NCES sampled schools to ensure a nationally representative
sample of public, Catholic, other religious, and private schools. In each sampled public school, between
twenty and thirty kindergartners were selected to participate, and in private schools at least twelve
kindergartners were selected. Private schools were oversampled, as were Asian and Pacific Islander
children, to facilitate comparative analyses (NCES, 2000).35
Missing Data
Of the ECLS-K sample with 21,399 children, 6,152 do not have any test score data and are excluded from
our analyses. These students differ in several ways from the students in the analytic sample. The analytic
sample has a larger proportion of white children and a smaller proportion of black children than expected
from the survey sample. There are more non-English-speaking children missing from the analytic sample
than would be expected under an assumption of randomly missing data. Significantly fewer children from
the lowest SES quintile and more children from the highest SES quintile are in the analytic sample than
expected.
Effect of Missing Scores for Non-English Speakers
Hispanic and Asian children are much more likely than white, black, or other children to fail the oral
language proficiency screening exam (oral language development scale, or OLDS), and therefore are more
likely to be missing scores in earlier rounds of testing. The missing scores may lead us to overstate the true
average achievement in early rounds (particularly at the beginning of kindergarten) for Hispanic and Asian
children and to underestimate average learning rates, but the estimated gap between Hispanic or Asian
children and white children at the end of grade 3 is much less likely to be biased, since all children passed
the OLDS placement exam in the fifth round of testing.
Children who ever failed the OLDS placement exam perform worse on average by every measure (they
have lower starting points and learn at a slower rate) than those who never failed. Looking only at children
who never failed the OLDS placement exam, the estimated gap between Hispanic children and white
children at the end of grade 3 is only 59 percent as big (8.36 points instead of 14.14 points). The average
achievement of Asian students who never failed the OLDS placement exam is indistinguishable from that
of white children at the end of grade 3 (compared to a nearly 5 point gap when including children who
failed the OLDS test at some point in time). Excluding children with missing test scores in early periods
could therefore lead to much smaller estimates of gaps in achievement.
In the third round of data collection, at the fall of first grade, only a subsample of about 3,400 children were surveyed and
tested. To create this subsample, NCES drew a 30 percent equal probability subsample of ECLS-K schools and included all the
base-year responding children in those schools.
35
128
Hispanic Students (difference in IRT reading scores from White Students)
All students
Never failed OLDS
Beginning of Kindergarten
-5.22
-3.87
End of Kindergarten
End of Summer after K-Beginning
of 1st
-6.97
-4.39
-6.33
-3.56
End of 1st Grade
-12.6
-7.54
End of 3rd Grade
-14.14
-8.36
Asian Students (difference in IRT reading scores from White Students)
All students
Never failed OLDS
Beginning of Kindergarten
1.09
2.54
End of Kindergarten
End of Summer after K-Beginning
of 1st
2.59
5.57
3.83
7.46
End of 1st Grade
0.81
5.65
End of 3rd Grade
-4.88
-0.520
NELS:88
Like ECLS-K, NELS:88 used a two-stage sampling procedure. In the first stage, 815 public schools and
237 private schools were selected with probabilities proportional to their eighth grade enrollment. Twentysix students were then randomly sampled from each school in the second stage. However, the study oversampled Hispanic students and Asian students to allow for valid comparisons across racial groups. In the
first follow-up (1990) and second follow-up (1992), students who were on time in the correct grade were
in tenth and twelfth grades, respectively.
Missing Data
There are small to moderate amounts of missing data on students’ test scores that are not missing at
random (MAR) for which our weights do not adjust (Allison 2002; Little and Rubin 2002). Approximately
3 percent of students are missing test scores for the base year (grade 8), 3 percent for the first follow-up
(grade 10), and 18 percent for the second follow-up (grade 12). On average, students with low
achievement are more likely to be missing test scores than students with high achievement.36 In addition,
low-income students and black students are slightly more likely to have missing data on test scores than
higher-income and white students, respectively. The non-random nature to the missing data results in a
loss of statistical power and possible bias in parameter estimates. The use of growth curve models, which
utilize all possible test scores in estimating parameters, deals with this problem.
The analyses discussed in the text exclude students who dropped out of high school and were retained at
some point during their schooling. A cross-tabulation analysis indicates that an overwhelming majority of
students who were retained are very likely—almost certain—to be the same students who leave high
school without completing a degree. Of those who dropout, 96.1 percent are retained; of those who are
retained, 78.1 percent drop out. By twelfth grade, 807 students dropped out of high school at some point37
and have a reading test score, and 1,127 students were retained at some point and have a reading test
score. Of the dropouts, 801 have a math score, and of the students ever retained, 1,123 have a math score.
36
37
Inspection of plots of kernel density functions reveals this pattern. Results are available from the authors upon request.
Some students who dropped out may have returned to school, but we do not know this information.
129
We ran analyses that include the dropouts to see how much their inclusion changes our results. The
average reading score at the end of eighth grade is about a third of a point less with the dropouts included
than with the more select group of students. A tenth of a point separates the learning rates between the
two samples, with the inclusive sample making less gain in tenth grade. In twelfth grade, the learning rates
are very similar; the difference in gain (measured in effect size per month) is 0.00022 SD and favors the
sample that includes the dropouts. These differences do not strike us as problematic.
TABLE A.3: READING GAINS FOR STUDENTS IN EIGHTH G RADE IN 1988—EXCLUDING D ROPOUTS
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Gain Per Period
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
At End of Period
28.25
0.152
(0.0033)
0.0903
(0.0042)
0.0199
3.66
31.91
0.00978
2.17
34.07
Gain Per Period
At End of Period
R EADING GAINS FOR STUDENTS IN EIGHTH GRADE IN 1988—INCLUDING D ROPOUTS
Time Period
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Before High School
8th Grade to 10th Grade
10th Grade to 12th Grade
27.94
0.146
(0.0035)
0.0925
(0.0042)
0.0191
3.50
31.44
0.0100
2.22
33.66
Note: Standard errors for the estimated coefficients are presented in the first column of the table in
parentheses below the corresponding coefficient. All coefficients are significantly different from zero. To
calculate effect sizes, we divide the gain per month by the estimated standard deviation of the base-period
test at the start of each time period.
Variables Used
This section describes the key variables that are included in the analytic models. We discuss first the
outcome measures and second, the demographic background variables and adjustments for differences in
school exposure.
Dependent Variables
In ECLS-K, cognitive assessments were administered to children one-on-one by trained NCES staff. In
each round, children from a language minority background first took a screening test called the Oral
Language Development Scale (OLDS). If children did not pass this test but spoke Spanish, they could not
take the reading test but could take a Spanish translation of the math assessment. If children demonstrated
sufficient proficiency in English on the OLDS at any point, they then took both the reading and math
assessments.
Each assessment included two stages. First, children answered the same 12 to 20 reading and mathematics
questions whose difficulty levels varied widely. This first stage provided a rough estimate of children’s
achievement level. Children’s performances on these test questions in each domain determined the next
stage of the assessment. In the second stage, the tests included items of low, medium, and high difficulty.
The routing process tried to ensure that children were given questions in this second stage that were
130
appropriate to their level of cognitive development and more precisely measured their achievement
(NCES, 2000).
The cognitive assessments in NELS:88 followed a similar approach. However, rather than use a routing
test at the same assessment time, NCES used each student’s eighth-grade scores to tailor the appropriate
level of difficulty for the follow-up test in tenth grade (NCES, 1995). The tenth-grade performance then
influenced the form of the test for the twelfth-grade follow-up. There were two forms of the NELS:88
reading tests (low and high difficulty) and three forms of the math tests (low, average, and high difficulty).
Each successive grade level form included additional more difficult items to address students’ presumably
improving skills. The adaptive approach to testing in both ECLS-K and NELS:88 reduces the chance of
ceiling and floor effects.
Reading
Reading questions tapped basic skills, from letter recognition and the link between letters and sounds to vocabulary
and reading comprehension. Because more children than expected performed close to the ceiling on the spring K
reading assessment, NCES increased the number and difficulty of questions in first grade (NCES, 2002). Changes
between the first and third grade rounds included adding more advanced questions about literal inference,
extrapolation, and evaluation.
The NELS:88 reading tests posed questions about reading passages that varied in length from a single
paragraph to a half-page. The tests measured skills in reading comprehension, literal inference, and critical
evaluation, which represent an extension of the skills tapped by the ECLS-K tests. The high difficulty
form was differentiated from the low difficulty form by including more complex texts taken from social
studies and science.
Math
Math questions tapped skills in conceptual knowledge, procedural knowledge, and problem solving. Items range
from asking children to identify numbers to solving simple multiplication and division problems. This assessment
included the same number and difficulty of questions in kindergarten and first grade until third grade at which point
more difficult items were added. These additional, more difficult items measure skills in geometry and spatial sense,
data analysis, probability and statistics, and basic algebraic functions (NCES, 2004).
At the secondary level, the NELS:88 10 th and 12th grade math tests had three forms of difficulty. All levels
of the test tapped skills in arithmetic, similar to the skills found on the ECLS-K tests. The average and
high difficulty level tests tapped skills in algebra and geometry. The high difficulty level tests included precalculus questions and/or analytic geometry questions.
Weights
Precision weights
The precision weight accounts for differences in the quality of information across observations at each
wave. The importance of imprecise estimates are weighted down, while more precise estimates are more
heavily weighted. This precision weight is constructed by multiplying the variance in the test scores (IRT
scale score or theta, depending on the analysis) at each wave with the inverse of the reliability for the theta
scores, which is the best approximate estimate of the test reliability (NCES, 2005, p.85).
Individual weights
We created analytic weights for each child included in the level-2 part of the hierarchical linear models. In
ECLS-K, the analytic weights are derived from the school weight in the base year (SCHBSW0), a flag
indicating whether students participated in rounds 1 and 2 (R12SC_F0), and the within school child weight
(WS_CWGT), all pulled from the restricted data files. These variables are then multiplied together. To
131
check this measure, we compared the resulting weight with the child weight at round 2. As expected, they
are highly correlated.
In NELS:88, we use the panel weight for students included in all three rounds of data collection. we
weight our sample to ensure that it is representative to students across the country who were eighth
graders in 1988. The weight F2PNLWT is applied, which weights the base year through second follow-up
panel. The weights that we use in our analyses reflect not only the unequal probabilities of sampling but
also non-response adjustments.
Student Characteristics
All analyses use demographic data from the base year data collection.
Gender. Males and females may learn at different rates in elementary and secondary school. In both
datasets, gender is represented by a dummy variable.38
Ethnicity. This report focuses on different learning rates across four ethnic subgroups: White, Black,
Hispanic, and Asian.39
Language status. The models also account for subgroups with substantial fractions of non-English
households (Hispanic and Asian). In ECLS-K, families responded whether English is the primary language
spoken at their home. If not, their children were tested with the oral language screening test to determine
English proficiency. In NELS:88, whether English is the predominant language spoken in the household
was collected from students. Estimates by language status are analyzed separately from analyses that focus
on the following subgroups: Hispanic in English-Speaking Homes, Hispanic in Non-English-Speaking Homes,
Asian in English-Speaking Homes, and Asian in Non-English-Speaking Homes.40
Income status. We adjust for differences in children’s family resources by focusing on the gap in learning
between low-income and higher-income children. We characterize as low-income those children whose family’s
income-to-needs ratio41 is less than 1.85 (the same figure often used to determine eligibility for the reduced
price lunch program [NAS, 2000]).
Differences in school exposure—testing time gaps. In large-scale studies, students cannot be administered cognitive
assessments at the same time. Assessments typically occur over a span of at least two and sometimes four
months. This means that students have different levels of school exposure before they take their
assessments. And because school exposure is positively correlated with test performance, models must
adjust for these differences. We model test scores as a function of the time before a given assessment. In
other words, each time parameter measures from the beginning of the school year to the date of the test42,
For ECLS-K and NELS:88 analyses, we use a dummy variable coded 1 for female and 0 for male derived from the
GENDER variable.
39 From ECLS-K, we use the measure of WKRACETH. Children in American Indian, Native Hawaiians, and Mixed Race
subgroups are combined under one category of “Other” to estimate mean gains. Estimates of expected gains in this “Other”
group will have much higher errors, because the children do not perform in consistently similar ways on the achievement tests.
These findings are not at the focus of our report. The same applies to the NELS:88 findings for Native Americans (from
RACE). These students are not included in sufficient numbers to present reliable results, so we do not focus on them.
40 In ECLS-K, we used the variable called WKLANGST, and in NELS:88, we used the variable, BYS22.
41 The income-to-needs ratio is defined as pre-tax income divided by poverty threshold for particular family size based on
census data. For ECLS-K, we use the variables WKINCOME and P1HTOTAL and the poverty thresholds for 1998. For
NELS:88, we use the variables BYFAMINC, BYFAMSIZ, and the poverty thresholds for 1988.
42 In NELS:88, there are no dates for the start and end of school years. We assumed June 1, 1988 for the end of eighth grade
and June 1, 1990 for the end of tenth grade.
38
132
both of which are individually variable.43 Thus by including these time measures, the models account for
the variable amount of time spent in each grade, or school exposure. The coefficients for these parameters
represent the amount of learning during that grade or time period.
Differences in school exposure—kindergarten program. This report explores how much children learn over a given
year. In kindergarten, children enrolled in full-day kindergarten are exposed to more schooling than
children enrolled in half-day kindergarten. Thus if we assume that school contributes significantly to
student learning, then these full-day kindergartners may make greater gains in reading and math. These
greater gains may emerge not only in kindergarten when the difference in school exposure is most
immediate but also in subsequent grades if full-day kindergartners are better prepared for later academic
success. To test this hypothesis, we re-ran two of the models with a sample of only half-day
kindergarteners and a sample of only full-day kindergartners, then compared the results.
Disregarding the comparability of gains in IRT scores across grade levels (discussed in detail in Chapter 4
and Appendix B), some of the higher estimated gains in first grade when compared to kindergarten may
arise from the different types of instruction provided in kindergarten, and differing lengths of exposure.
Forty-four percent of the estimation sample was enrolled in half-day kindergarten, and we might expect
these students to learn less in kindergarten and more in first grade.
Table A.1 describes the two groups of kindergartners, and Table A.2 presents results for the two groups.
Indeed, students enrolled in full-day kindergarten are more than two IRT scale points ahead of their
counterparts in half-day kindergarten, on average, at the end of kindergarten, but the two groups are
indistinguishable by the end of first grade. We do not distinguish these groups for analysis, but
acknowledge the slight differences in learning.
For example, for assessments in kindergarten, the amount of time in the first and third grades equals zero. This changes as the
assessment time changes so that by third grade, the times in kindergarten and first grade are set (those grades are already
completed and spanned a fixed amount of time, approximately 286 days, including weekends). However, the time in third grade
before the assessment does not equal a full year, because at the third grade assessment, third grade is not yet completed.
43
133
44
TABLE A.1: SAMPLE SIZES—BY KINDERGARTEN PROGRAM
Half-day
Kindergarten
Male
Female
Missing Gender
Full-day
Kindergarten
n=11,012
5,537
5,432
43
White
Black
Hispanic
Asian
Other
Missing Race
5,560
2,364
1,769
558
677
84
5,204
618
1,782
733
396
51
English Speaking Home (EH)
Non-English Speaking Home (NEH)
Missing EH Status
Hispanic Non-EH
Hispanic-EH
Asian Non-EH
Asian-EH
Missing Race*EH
Low Income
Higher Income
White-Low Income
White-High Income
Black-Low Income
Black-High Income
Hispanic-Low Income
Hispanic-High Income
Asian-Low Income
Asian-High Income
Other-Low Income
Other-High Income
Missing Race*Economic Status
9,190
1,219
603
777
899
280
212
625
4,254
6,758
2,116
3,444
1,355
1,009
946
823
158
400
357
320
84
6,924
1,393
467
858
836
429
183
470
2,954
5,830
1,390
3,814
340
278
1,039
743
241
492
149
247
51
n=8,784
4,527
4,220
37
There are 1,603 students (7.49 percent of the sample) missing data on the variable that indicates full-day or half-day
kindergarten.
44
134
TABLE A.2: IRT READING SCORES AT END OF P ERIOD, BY
LENGTH OF KINDERGARTEN SCHOOL DAY
Full-day
Half-day
Kindergarten Kindergarten
Beginning of Kindergarten
22.98749
23.00523
End of Kindergarten
End of Summer after KBeginning of 1st
41.35577
38.93086
40.83949
38.73071
End of 1st Grade
69.90634
70.25758
End of 3rd Grade
110.2182
109.0318
Results presented in these tables suggest that full-day kindergartners start on par with their peers in halfday kindergarten programs, but learn slightly more during kindergarten. In subsequent years, children who
had full-day kindergarten gain less in reading than children who attended half-day kindergarten.
In models that interact full-day kindergarten with income status, a full-day kindergarten program seems to
help alleviate academic challenges associated with low-income status during kindergarten but shows no
advantage in first through third grades. Low-income students in full-day programs and in half-day
programs start kindergarten with similar deficits on the reading assessment compared to high-income
students. In kindergarten, the low-income full-day kindergartners do not lag behind high-income students
as much as the low-income half-day kindergartners. Perhaps the extra time in school compensates slightly
for the lack of resources at home.
But in first grade, low-income children regardless of kindergarten program face nearly the same deficit in
reading gains compared to their high-income peers. In second and third grades, the low-income children
who were in half-day programs actually catch up in reading gains to their high-income peers, but the
children from full-day kindergarten programs make slightly less gain. These differences might derive from
other characteristics associated with the selection of full-day versus half-day kindergarten.
Black children are much more likely to participate in full-day kindergarten programs than in half-day
kindergarten programs. Black students in full-day programs do not start kindergarten as far behind white
full-day kindergartners as do black and Hispanic students in half-day programs. In kindergarten, black and
Hispanic students in half-day kindergarten gain slightly but significantly less in reading than white and
black students in full-day programs. Hispanic students in full-day kindergartens make similar gains in
reading to their white peers who were enrolled in full-day programs. In sum, children in full-day
kindergartens gain more during kindergarten than those in half-day kindergartens, but this advantage is
offset by faster learning among children who participated in half-day kindergartens later in elementary
school.
Analytic Methods
This section outlines the analytic approach and explains the multiple metrics in which results are reported
to facilitate interpretation. To calculate average gain in reading and math across grades, we estimate the
relationship between test scores and time spent in each grade. One set of models estimates students’ test
scores at the beginning of elementary school and their learning rates from kindergarten until third grade.
Another set of models estimates students’ test scores at the beginning of high school and their learning
rates from that time point forward.
45
The unique challenge in working with the ECLS-K data is the rescaling of earlier test performance based on subsequent test
scores, an issue that is fully explained in Appendix B.
45
135
Elementary school
The elementary school analyses model test scores as a function of five time parameters—before
kindergarten entry, kindergarten, summer between kindergarten and first grade, first grade, and third grade.
By the end of third grade, time spent in kindergarten and in first grade equals, on average, 286.64 days or
9.5 months (each grade). The shortest time parameter represents the summer between kindergarten and
first grade—78.36 days, or about 2.5 months. Time spent between the end of first grade and the third
grade assessment date averages 691.28 days or 23.04 months.
Secondary school
The secondary school analyses follow the same procedure but use three time components—before high
school or eighth grade, tenth grade, and twelfth grade. In NELS:88 most test dates occurred within a small
time from of about 2 to 4 months (standard deviations equal to 1 and 2) and in regular intervals of about
24 months.46
Model Specification
Seven models are constructed for estimating learning in reading and mathematics:
1. A model with only the time parameters
2. A model with a dummy variable for female on the initial status and each of the learning slopes
3. A model with dummy variables for race/ethnicity subgroup membership on the intercept and each of the
learning slopes
4. A model with a dummy variable for language status on the initial status and each of the learning slopes
5. A model with dummy variables for low-income/higher-income status on the intercept and each of the
learning slopes
6. A model with dummy variables for ethnicity on the initial status and each of the learning slopes, with
ethnicity broken out by language minority status
7. A model with dummy variables that interact race/ethnicity with income status on the initial status and each
of the learning slopes
The first model estimates average reading and math gains for the typical student. Models 2 through 7
illustrate differences in these gains by subgroup membership. In all models, initial status and learning in
each time period are a llowed to vary randomly across students.
Metrics for results
Findings are reported in four metrics, each of which should facilitate interpretation and is explained in the following
section.
Points
Learning or growth rates are reported in points per day, per month, and per time period. These metrics
represent the most easily understood and familiar approach. But what do points mean exactly? To
understand what a 1.5-point gain per month actually means, we relate the scores to specific skills in the
accompanying graphs.
Effect sizes
We also present findings for differences in learning rates in units of standard deviations, or effect sizes.
Effect sizes measure the magnitude of a relationship and can be compared across tests with different point
ranges. For example, the magnitude of the achievement gap found between white and black students in
Exact test dates are not available for the base year of NELS:88 but are available for the first and second follow-ups. In order
to calculate the elapsed time between tests, we impute the base year test date with the median test date for the base year of April
1, 1988. If the test dates were missing for the first or second follow-ups, we imputed them with the median test dates of March
20, 1990 and February 27, 1992, respectively.
46
136
elementary school can be compared to the same gap in secondary school when results are presented in
effect sizes. Effect sizes may be a more familiar outcome metric for researchers prepared to create
experimental designs. We divide average growth rates in each period by the standard deviation of scores
from the assessment at the beginning of the period, measured at the start of the period. This makes
expected gains comparable across different test designs.
In calculating effect sizes, we could have used the standard deviation for a specific group or the standard
deviation for all. We chose the latter, so that the standard deviation used is comparable and consistent
across analyses. We can provide ratios to convert the standard deviation across all to the standard
deviation for a specific group.
Proficiency index
The point where a student’s score corresponds to a 50 percent probability of mastery of a topic or skill set
is the ability level at which children are learning the topic at the fastest rate. We refer to this type of
proficiency as the current level of achievement for a student with this score. For example, a child in the
ECLS-K study with a math score of 43.84 (in the rescaled version of the test as of third grade) has a 50
percent chance of being proficient on the topic labeled “ADD/SUBTRACT.” From this, we can plausibly
say that students with scores in the vicinity of 44 are learning to add and subtract. The next such level
"MULTIPLY/DIVIDE” occurs at an IRT score of 67.32, so students in the vicinity of 67 are learning to
multiply and divide. Gradations of ability between these two milestones (in the range from 44 to 67 points)
cannot be tied to specific na med skills, but the milestones offer a means to measure increases in an
essentially arbitrary test score metric using familiar concepts. The following table provides the key to
converting test scores to proficiency scores.
TABLE A1. SKILLS B EING L EARNED AT SPECIFIED IRT SCORE L EVELS—ECLS-K
(Assumes 50% Proficiency Level Corresponds to Point of Maximal Learning Speed)
Math Skills
IRT Score
Proficiency Type
10.05
1-COUNT, NUMBER, SHAPE
18.71
2-RELATIVE SIZE
28.46
3-ORDINALITY, SEQUENCE
43.84
4-ADD/SUBTRACT
67.32
5-MULTIPLY/DIVIDE
91.29
6-PLACE VALUE
104.41
7-RATE & MEASUREMENT
Reading Skills
IRT Score
Proficiency Type
21.41
1-LETTER RECOGNITION
30.73
2-BEGINNING SOUNDS
36.08
3-ENDING SOUNDS
51.03
4-SIGHT WORDS
68.87
5-WORD IN CONTEXT
91.63
6-LITERAL INFERENCE
112.98
7-EXTRAPOLATION
124.59
8-EVALUATION
137
TABLE A2. SKILLS B EING L EARNED AT SPECIFIED IRT SCORE L EVELS—NELS:88
(Assumes 50% Proficiency Level Corresponds to Point of Maximal Learning Speed)
Math Skills
IRT Score
Proficiency Type
15.59
1-COMPREHENSION, INCLUDING LEVEL OF DETAIL
30.65
2-SIMPLE INFERENCES AND UNDERSTAND ABSTRACT CONCEPTS
43.30
3-COMPLEX INFERENCE AND EVALUATE JUDGMENTS
Reading Skills
IRT Score
22.82
37.24
46.21
57.73
73.55
Proficiency Type
1-SINGLE OPERATIONS WITH WHOLE NUMBERS
2-FRACTIONS, DECIMALS, POWERS, AND ROOTS
3-SIMPLE PROBLEM SOLVING
4-INTERMEDIATE LEVEL MATH CONCEPTS
5-MULTI-STEP PROBLEM SOLVING AND ADVANCED MATH
Linear models
To account for different rates of learning across students, we construct growth curve models, with growth
varying by time period and done in piecewise fashion (e.g., in ECLS-K, Fall-K to Spring-K; Fall-1 to
Spring-1, etc.). Models are two-level hierarchical models, with testing times nested within students. Level-1
represents testing times, with analyses weighted by precision weights to account for measurement error.
Level-2 represents individual students, weighted to ensure generalizability of the sample (the inverse of the
probability of being selected for the sample). We report findings from these models with robust standard
errors. The model’s equations are:
Level 1
Yti = p0i + p1i ati + eti
Yti =
p0i =
ati =
eti =
observed status at time t for individual i
growth trajectory parameter for subject i at time 0
amount of time passed at time t for person i
error term
Level 2
p0i = ß00 + Sß0qXqi + r0i
p1i = ß00 + S ß1qXqi + r0i
p0i =
p1i =
ß0q =
Xq =
r0i =
initial status at time 0, constant term
growth rate for person i over the time period; the expected change during this time
the effect of Xq on the growth parameter
an individual background measure (e.g., gender, race/ethnicity)
random effect with mean of 0, assumed to be normally distributed
These hierarchical models allow the initial level and learning rate of each student to have a common
component and an individual random component. Ignoring this feature, the models are essentially a linear
138
regression of scores on the lengths of time spent in various parts of the educational system at the point the
score is measured. The estimated constant in such a model is the initial score when entering kindergarten
or at the end of eighth grade, and the coefficients on time variables are growth rates of scores in points per
day during the relevant span of time.
We compare estimates constructed in these hierarchical linear models using the twice-rescaled versions of
the reading and math tests from the fifth round of data but excluding the fifth round to those constructed
using the once-rescaled versions of the reading and math tests from the fourth round of data, and found
no substantial differences.
Locally standardized regressions
We estimate gains for different subgroups using only individuals with base year scores close to a particular
point, then normalize the gains by pooled standard deviation of gains (again only of individuals with base
year scores close to a particular point). These estimates are constructed at many points across the entire
distribution of base year scores, dividing always by the standard deviation of gains. This method is
described more extensively in Appendix D.
139
A PPENDIX B: RESCALING ISSUES
Rescaling conducted in later rounds of data substantially change any tabulations of mean point gains for
many students. This issue applies uniquely to our ECLS-K estimations. Since rescaling is conducted in
each new round, our tables measuring gains made in kindergarten in 1998 could change in 2006 when data
on fifth grade are released, and again each time a new round of survey data is made available. Thus, the
gains at a far-prior point in time could become a moving target, subject to data collection conducted in
future years. NELS:88 data were not rescaled in each round, so these concerns apply only to the ECLS-K
data.
Each assessment in ECLS-K and NELS:88 includes more items than participants actually answered.
NCES then scales the results using item response theory (IRT). IRT uses information about student ability
from the questions they did answer and item characteristics (difficulty level, discrimination power, and
likelihood of guessing the correct answer) to estimate how many questions students would have answered
correctly if they had responded to all possible questions. Some overlapping questions among test versions
allow estimators based on IRT to place all student scores on the same scale (horizontal equating).
In these cases where test scores measure the estimated number of questions a student would have
answered correctly if administered all items on the test, the assumption that test scores are interval-scaled
is difficult to justify, since the sensitivity of the test score to changes in skill levels will be highly sensitive to
the density of test items of different difficulty levels.
For example, in ECLS-K, children’s performances on the math tests in the fall and spring of first grade are
scored two different ways—the estimated number of items a student would have gotten correct had she or
he been given all 64 items on the “first grade version” of the test (this is called the R1 test score); and the
estimated number of items a student would have gotten correct had she or he been given all 124 items (the
64 first grade items and 60 additional items) on the “third grade version” of the test (this is called the R2
test score). Because the additional 60 items on the R2 test measure higher-level math skills than most of
those on the R1 test, the R2 version of the test score is based on a test with many more “difficult” items
than the R1 version of the test (Figures B1 and B2 below). As a result, if we examine the difference in the
fall-spring gain in test scores between two students—one who starts with low level math skills, and one
who starts with high level skills—the magnitude and direction of this difference may not be the same if we
use the R1 or R2 version of the test.
140
FIGURE B1:
DISTRIBUTION OF R EADING ASSESSMENT ITEM DIFFICULTY
ECLS-K Reading Assessment Item Difficulty Distributions,
Benchmarked Against Proficiency Levels
-3
Item Difficulty (Theta metric)
-1
0
1
-2
2
3
K-1 test items (R1 scale)
extrapolation
evaluation
literal inference
words in context
sight words
ending sounds
letter recognition
beginning sounds
K-3 test items (R2 scale)
Proficiency Levels
*Note: the height of the distribution at each point indicates the relative number of items at a given difficulty level on each test.
Since the K-3 test includes all 64 items on the K-1 test, plus an additional 60 items, the relative item volume on the K-3 test is
everywhere greater than (or equal to) the item volume on the K-1 test. For example, there are the same number of items
measuring relative size on both versions of the test, but more than twice as many multiplication/division questions on the K-3
test as the K-1 test.
FIGURE B2:
DISTRIBUTION OF MATH ASSESSMENT ITEM DIFFICULTY
ECLS-K Math Assessment Item Difficulty Distributions,
Benchmarked Against Proficiency Levels
-3
-2
Item Difficulty (Theta metric)
-1
0
1
2
3
K-1 test items (R1 scale)
rate & measurement
place value
multiply-divide
add-subtract
ordinality-sequence
relative size
count-number-shape
K-3 test items (R2 scale)
Proficiency Levels
*Note: the height of the distribution at each point indicates the relative number of items at a given difficulty level on each test.
Since the K-3 test includes all 93 items on the K-1 test, plus an additional 61 items, the relative item volume on the K-3 test is
everywhere greater than (or equal to) the item volume on the K-1 test. For example, there are the same number of items
measuring ending sounds on both versions of the test, but more than twice as many literal inference questions on the K-3 test
as the K-1 test.
Comparing R0 scores (IRT scores without any subsequent rescaling, available only for kindergarten) and
R1 scores (IRT scores rescaled based on K and first grade tests) suggests little difference in the
distributions. But the difference between R0 or R1 and R3 scores is quite sizeable, at least for the reading
141
test. NCES added more questions with a relatively high guessability factor (many students could guess the
correct answer) to the reading test than to the math test. In addition, few students were scoring near the
highest level on the math test, unlike on the reading test. So adding more difficult items to the math test
did not elicit more correct responses or consequently more accurate estimates of student ability. Thus
rescaling shifts the distribution of math scores only slightly. However, the rescaling in third grade shifts the
previous reading test scores substantially, especially among high achievers, and shifts up all scores for the
reading test. This is visible in Figure B3 as a shift to the right in the distribution of baseline scores (the two
smoother, single-peaked dashed and dotted lines are the distributions of scores in Spring K, in the R1 and
R3 metrics) of about 7 points.
FIGURE B3: RESCALING EFFECTS:
DISTRIBUTION OF R EADING SCORES SHIFTS RIGHT , GAINS HIGHER
30
20
0
10
0
Gain in Points/Year
40
.01
.02
.03
.04
Density of Spring K Scores
ECLS-K Reading Scores and Gains, Spring K to Spring 1
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
Baseline (Spring K) Score ...
Annual Gains in R3 Metric
Annual Gains in R1 Metric
80
90
Baseline Score Distribution, R3 Metric
Baseline Score Distribution, R1 Metric
More importantly, average gains computed using the twice-rescaled scores, shown in Figure B3 as a solid
line graphed against prior period scores, differ substantially from those calculated using the once-rescaled
scores (shown as a dashed line). Math does not show as much of an effect of rescaling in the distribution
of scores, but has an even larger problem in mean gains (Figure B4). The distribution of raw scores moves
slightly to the right, and gains are everywhere substantially higher, when using twice-rescaled scores.
142
FIGURE B4: RESCALING EFFECTS:
DISTRIBUTION OF MATH SCORES S HIFTS SLIGHTLY, GAINS M UCH HIGHER
.01
.02
.03
.04
Density of Spring K Scores
20
15
10
0
0
5
Gain in Points/Year
25
.05
ECLS-K Math Scores and Gains, Spring K to Spring 1
10
20
30
40
Annual Gains in R3 Metric
Annual Gains in R1 Metric
50
score...
60
70
80
90
Baseline Score Distribution, R3 Metric
Baseline Score Distribution, R1 Metric
The analytic models we discuss in this report attempt to deal with these issues and maximize the precision
and interpretability of the estimates. There are two main complementary goals: 1) to make estimated gains
independent of the specific design of the tests, and 2) to make estimated mean gains comparable across
different tests (e.g., ECLS-K tests and tests for NCLB requirements).
However, due to the intrinsic nature of the rescaling process, all of the results in this report, including not
only growth rates, but also projected levels, could be different when the scores are rescaled again using
fifth grade (round 6) estimates. We checked the extent of this possible difference by constructing models
pretending that we had no information from the third grade round of data. Our growth-curve estimates
(not reported here) using once-rescaled scores (based on first grade assessments collected in round 4) look
similar to estimates using twice-rescaled scores but dropping all data from round 5. This indicates that our
results may be less sensitive to future rescaling than might have been feared from an examination of mean
gains alone.
However, some effects of the rescaling process are clear. The rescaling process affects whether evidence of
a summer slide emerges. Analyses in this report and analyses with early rounds of ECLS-K data show no
significant dip in children’s reading test scores during the summer time. Despite the lack of statistical
significance, analyses with early data produce a small positive coefficient and analyses with the latest round
of data produce a small negative coefficient. This difference may not seem particularly important but do
highlight the issues with rescaling.
143
A PPENDIX C: STANDARD D EVIATIONS
We present our results in a number of metrics, including effect sizes. These are calculated by dividing
estimated effects by the standard deviation of the outcome. Not surprisingly, these effect size estimates
depend on how this standard deviation is calculated. In this appendix we discuss various alternatives and
the one we used.
Effect Size
The familiar notion of normalizing a variable divides the variable by its own standard deviation, to obtain a
variable measured in standard deviation units (analogous to a standard normal distribution, hence the verb
normalize), which is a scale-free metric47. The concept of effect size captures the notion of a “normalized”
growth rate, where the estimate of gain in levels (points in some metric) is converted into a number
representing the number of standard deviations gained over time, constructed as a ratio dividing the gain
estimate by the standard deviation of scores—and there are as many notions of effect size as there are
plausible values for the standard deviation used in the denominator of that ratio.
There are at least four obvious options for constructing effect sizes from estimates of gain in levels. One
option would use the standard deviation of all test scores over the entire time period, pooling across time.
The second would use the standard deviation of test scores in the first period of measurement, or fall of
kindergarten (fall k) in the ECLS-K data. The third would use the standard deviation of test scores in the
first period of measurement to scale gains between the first and second period, expressing gains in points
as gains in standard deviations on the baseline test. The fourth would use the standard deviation of test
scores in the second period of measurement to scale gains between the first and second period for each
pair of assessments, expressing gains in points as gains in standard deviations on the test taken after
learning new material.
We have adopted a revised version of the third method, which has the most intuitive interpretation, and
could potentially be useful to anyone having results from a test in one grade and wanting to predict likely
gains on a hypothetical test in a later period (for example, for the purposes of power analysis in the process
of designing an experiment).
Our method does not divide point gains by the raw standard deviation of test scores pooled within a
round, since tests are administered at different points in time. Instead, we estimate the standard deviation
at the beginning of the period of interest (e.g. at the beginning of first grade to compute the effect size for
gains over the course of first grade). These are obtained by shifting the intercept of the regression model
to that point in time, which does not change the slope estimates, but does change the estimated constant
term. The estimated standard deviation of scores around the intercept term is our estimate of the standard
deviation on the test at that point in time.
This is a particularly useful technique when the standard deviation of test administration dates changes
appreciably over time, though this is not the case with our data. The deviation from mean assessment
dates by round in the ECLS-K data is shown in Figure C1, and it is clear that the random variation in
assessment dates does not differ much across the rounds. However, it is still possible that different
subgroups are sampled at different times, or even that the lag between test dates differs systematically
across subgroups (particularly for the ECLS-K data, since the middle third round used a small nonrepresentative sample). It is also clear that point gains are strongly related to the number of days elapsed
The normalized variable has no units, and measurements in different scales produce the same normalized values, e.g. a
variable measuring length coded in inches, or one containing the same measurements in feet will be identical after normalizing
the two variables. In this sense the scale of measurement is irrelevant.
47
144
between tests, as shown in Figure C2 (all of the positive slopes, while small in magnitude, are significant at
the 1-10 -12 level).
These estimates pool all types of individuals (i.e. are not estimated separately by race or income or
language status) and are constructed separately for each model we run, to ensure the sample is identical.
TABLE C1. ESTIMATED STANDARD D EVIATIONS —ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
Time Period
FULL S AMPLE
Beginning of K
End of K
Beginning of 1st
End of 1st
End of 3rd
Reading
IRT
Math IRT
Reading
Theta
Math Theta
9.25822
13.64673
15.59768
21.20280
19.45075
8.23542
11.63820
12.36079
15.65170
15.62940
0.59042
0.55636
0.57151
0.50295
0.37636
0.58623
0.55331
0.56463
0.49667
0.45640
0.58919
0.55684
0.57103
0.50374
0.36730
0.5852
0.55355
0.56438
0.49768
0.45352
S AMPLES RESTRICTED TO NON - MISSING L ANGUAGE STATUS
Beginning of K
9.19347
8.19316
End of K
13.59159
11.60565
Beginning of 1st
15.50918
12.326
End of 1st
21.16788
15.63889
End of 3rd
19.23780
16.13096
TABLE C2. ESTIMATED STANDARD D EVIATIONS —S ECONDARY SCHOOL
Time Period
FULL S AMPLE
End of 8th grade
End of 10th Grade
End of 12th Grade
Reading
IRT
Math IRT
Reading
Theta
Math Theta
8.532
9.836
11.707
13.471
8.451
10.050
8.312
9.409
Tables C1 and C2 present our estimates of the standard deviation on the test at each relevant point in time
(the beginning of each period over which we estimate gains). Tables C3 and C4 present the corresponding
estimates we would have used if we had not modified the third option, that is, if we had used the raw
standard deviation of test scores pooled within a round. The difference is quite small in most cases.
TABLE C3. ACTUAL STANDARD D EVIATIONS — ELEMENTARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Reading IRT
Math IRT
Reading Theta
Math Theta
FULL S AMPLE
Beginning of K
End of K
Beginning of 1st
End of 1st
9.810306
13.09301
16.56015
20.47198
8.830328
11.43061
13.45482
15.94837
.5667186
.5538426
.573677
.5118973
.5798147
.5636462
.5838926
.5218103
S AMPLES RESTR ICTED TO NON - MISSING L ANGUAGE STATUS
Beginning of K
9.841982
8.851763
End of K
13.10695
11.44324
Beginning of 1st
16.53039
13.41443
End of 1st
20.43452
15.93634
.5672197
.5523441
.5687264
.5084035
.5803936
.5624954
.5780821
.5188048
145
TABLE C4. ACTUAL STANDARD D EVIATIONS —S ECONDARY SCHOOL
Time Period
Reading IRT
Math IRT
Reading Theta
Math Theta
FULL S AMPLE
End of 8th Grade
End of 10th Grade
8.532705
9.836779
11.70741
13.4713
8.450959
10.05063
8.312182
9.4096
In any case, using the tables, one can convert any of the effect size estimates in this report into a different
type of effect size estimate. For example, to convert the math gain made by male students in first grade
(Table 3.1, third row) into effect size units that use the raw standard deviation on the Fall Kindergarten
assessment in the denominator, multiply the estimate from the table (0.208) by the estimated standard
deviation (15.382) at the start of first grade from Table C1, then divide by the actual standard deviation of
scores in round 1 (9.810306) from Table C3.
IRT and Theta Score Distributions
One of the main differences between IRT scale and theta scores, from a practical standpoint, is that the
standard deviation of the IRT scale scores can change dramatically over time. In contrast, the standard
deviation of the theta scores remains fairly stable. This has important implications for our comparisons of
growth rates based on scale scores and effect sizes.
In the ECLS-K data, the IRT scores exhibit substantial growth of standard deviations over time. This
phenomenon is evident in the tables above, and in Figures C3 and C4. This leads to greater differences in
IRT and effect size estimates when comparing growth rates across time, and makes comparisons across
time more dependent on relatively subjective choices about the scale of measurement.
In each round, the distribution of IRT scores moves to the right, and the distance between the centroids
of these distributions represents mean learning across rounds, expressed in IRT point scores as observed
in ECLS-K. Thus, the first and second curves are distant (overlap less than the second and third, counting
left to right), reflecting the high rate of learning (growth in theta scores) between rounds 1 and 2 (roughly,
in Kindergarten). The second and third curves are quite close (overlap more than the others) reflecting the
minimal learning (growth in IRT scores) observed between rounds 2 and 3 (roughly, during the summer
between Kindergarten and first grade). The third and four distributions are slightly farther apart than the
first and second, indicating apparently faster growth in first grade, but the dispersion also increases, so
larger point gains do not translate into faster gains as measured in standard deviations.
There is also evidence in Figures C3 and C4 of truncation of the distribution in the most recent round of
testing. If not enough difficult questions are added to the test bank, and if many new questions have high
guessability, the highest performing students will be indistinguishable from moderately high performers, or
nearly (for example, the 99th percentile will be much closer to the 75th than the first is to the 25th). This
will reduce the mean gain estimated across all students, and could lead us to conclude that learning is
slower in the time between the penultimate and the last assessment, when there is no real slowing of the
rate of learning. It will be instructive to examine the most recent assessment and newly rescaled scores in
early 2006, to investigate this concern in the light of new evidence.
One of the reasons that effect size and theta score gains are similar (see the last pages of Chapter 3 for
discussion of this point) is that theta scores are already approximately normal, so renormalizing by dividing
by the standard deviation has little impact. This is plainly visible in Figures C5 and C6, where the
distribution of theta in each round is shown as the estimated density across the range of theta scores.
146
Each round has a distribution of theta that is roughly bell-shaped and the distance between the centroids
of these approximate bells represents mean learning across rounds, expressed in theta scores as observed
in ECLS-K. Thus, the first and second curves are quite distant (overlap less than the others) reflecting the
high rate of learning (growth in theta scores) between rounds 1 and 2 (roughly, in Kindergarten). The
second and third curves are quite close (overlap more than the others) reflecting the minimal learning
(growth in theta scores) observed between rounds 2 and 3 (roughly, during the summer between
Kindergarten and first grade).
Note that the thetas when pooled across all rounds need not look so bell-shaped, since the pooled theta
scores are theoretically drawn from a mixture of normals distribution, and will typically look “lumpier”
than each round’s theta distribution. Figures C7 and C8 demonstrate this point, and this exercise serves to
underline the interpretation of theta not as a measure of inborn or genetic capacity, but as a learned ability
to score well on a particular family of tests.
Figures C9 to C14 repeat the graphs C3 to C8 using the NELS data, which has somewhat different
properties. In particular, the IRT scores do not exhibit the dispersion of ECLS-K IRT scores, which is
evidence that the NELS tests likely created an artificial ceiling or floor on performance of extremely low
and high performers, truncating the distribution of scores. Also, the distributions of thetas in individual
rounds look less normal than the distribution of theta pooled across rounds, for both reading and math
tests. This may indicate that the IRT estimation model was imperfectly applied, or the tests were not welldesigned to capture changes in the learned ability measured by theta over the high school years.
147
0
.005
.01
.015
.02
.025
FiGURE C1. DISTRIBUTION OF ASSESSMENT DATES FOR EACH OF ROUNDS 1-5 IN ECLS-K
-60
-30
0
30
60
Distance from mean assessment date in days
FIGURE C2. LINEAR FIT OF IRT POINT GAINS REGRESSED ON DAYS ELAPSED B ETWEEN ASSESSMENTS
Spr K to Fall 1
Fall 1 to Spr 1
Spr 1 to Spr 3
0
30 40
20
10
0
Fitted values
10 20 30 40
Fall K to Spr K
-100
-50
0
50
100 -100
-50
0
Days Elapsed Between Assessments
Math Gain
Reading Gain
Graphs by Rounds of Assessments
148
50
100
0
.02
.04
.06
FIGURE C3. DISTRIBUTION OF R EADING IRT SCORES FOR EACH OF ROUNDS 1-5 IN ECLS-K
0
50
Reading IRT score
100
150
0
.01
.02
.03
.04
.05
FIGURE C4. DISTRIBUTION OF MATH IRT SCORES FOR EACH OF R OUNDS 1-5 IN ECLS-K
0
50
Math IRT score
149
100
150
0
.5
1
1.5
FIGURE C5. DISTRIBUTION OF R EADING T HETA SCORES FOR EACH OF ROUNDS 1-5 IN ECLS-K
-3
-2
-1
0
Reading theta
1
2
3
0
.5
1
1.5
FIGURE C6. DISTRIBUTION OF MATH T HETA SCORES FOR EACH OF R OUNDS 1-5 IN ECLS-K
-3
-2
-1
0
Math theta
150
1
2
3
0
.1
Density
.2
.3
.4
FIGURE C7. DENSITY OF R EADING THETA SCORES P OOLING ALL ROUNDS (1-5) IN ECLS-K
-3
-2
-1
0
Reading theta
1
2
3
0
.1
Density
.2
.3
.4
FIGURE C8. DENSITY OF MATH T HETA SCORES POOLING ALL ROUNDS (1-5) IN ECLS-K
-3
-2
-1
0
Math theta
151
1
2
3
0
.01
.02
.03
.04
FIGURE C9. DENSITY OF R EADING IRT SCORES FOR EACH OF ROUNDS 1-3 IN NELS
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
Reading IRT score
0
.01
.02
.03
FIGURE C10. DENSITY OF MATH IRT SCORES FOR EACH OF R OUNDS 1-3 IN NELS
0
10
20
30
40
50
Math IRT score
152
60
70
80
90
0
.01
.02
.03
.04
FIGURE C11. DENSITY OF R EADING T HETA SCORES FOR EACH OF R OUNDS 1-3 IN NELS
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
Reading Theta score
0
.01
.02
.03
.04
.05
FIGURE C12. DENSITY OF MATH T HETA SCORES FOR EACH OF R OUNDS 1-3 IN NELS
20
30
40
50
60
Math Theta score
153
70
80
90
0
.01
.02
.03
.04
FIGURE C13. DENSITY OF R EADING T HETA SCORES POOLING ALL ROUNDS (1-3) IN NELS
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
Reading Theta Score
0
.01
.02
.03
.04
FIGURE C14. DENSITY OF MATH T HETA SCORES POOLING ALL ROUNDS (1-3) IN NELS
0
10
20
30
40
50
Math Theta Score
154
60
70
80
90
A PPENDIX D: COMPARISON OF IRT AND THETA SCORES
At the end of Chapter 3, we compare findings in the IRT scale score metric with the findings in the theta score
metric. This comparison analyzes ratios of the learning rate in a subsequent grade with the learning rate in the
previous grade (e.g., first grade gain to kindergarten gain). This Appendix presents the tables on which we based our
comparisons of the learning rate ratios.
TABLE D1: RATIO OF L EARNING RATE DIFFERENCES IN ECLS-K: COMPARISON OF IRT AND THETA BY RACE
Time Period
IRT
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Theta
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
W HITE STUDENTS / READING
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
1.836
0.474
1.090
0.348
0.940
0.320
0.971
0.363
W HITE STUDENTS / M ATHEMATICS
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
1.451
0.500
0.968
0.395
0.905
0.736
0.941
0.827
BLACK STUDENTS / READING
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
1.771
0.520
1.046
0.383
0.974
0.305
1.004
0.346
BLACK STUDENTS / M ATHEMATICS
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
1.581
0.529
1.055
0.420
0.979
0.340
1.019
0.386
HISPANIC STUDENTS / READING
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
1.597
0.591
0.944
0.435
0.845
0.347
0.872
0.393
HISPANIC STUDENTS / M ATHEMATICS
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
1.571
0.546
1.047
0.433
0.908
0.355
0.945
0.402
ASIAN STUDENTS / READING
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
1.459
0.448
0.863
0.330
0.819
0.303
0.846
0.343
ASIAN STUDENTS / M ATHEMATICS
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
1.266
0.644
0.843
0.512
0.835
0.452
0.869
0.513
155
TABLE D2: RATIO OF L EARNING RATE DIFFERENCES IN ECLS-K: COMPARISON OF IRT AND THETA BY RACE AND
LANGUAGE
Time Period
IRT
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Theta
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
W HITE STUDENTS / READING
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
1.836
0.474
1.090
0.348
0.940
0.320
0.971
0.363
1.451
0.500
0.968
0.395
0.905
0.367
0.941
0.417
1.771
0.520
1.046
0.383
0.974
0.305
1.004
0.346
1.581
0.529
1.055
0.420
0.979
0.340
1.018
0.386
1.582
0.650
0.935
0.478
0.872
0.355
0.899
0.403
1.679
0.541
1.120
0.429
0.802
0.343
0.824
0.389
1.622
0.536
0.959
0.395
0.826
0.333
0.852
0.378
1.461
0.554
0.973
0.440
0.865
0.371
0.900
0.420
1.454
0.459
0.859
0.338
0.829
0.300
0.855
0.340
1.315
0.655
0.875
0.520
0.867
0.450
0.903
0.509
1.445
0.429
0.855
0.316
0.798
0.304
0.824
0.344
1.223
0.617
0.814
0.490
0.802
0.449
0.835
0.509
W HITE STUDENTS / M ATHEMATICS
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
BLACK STUDENTS / READING
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
BLACK STUDENTS / M ATHEMATICS
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
HISPANIC (ENGLISH NOT SPOKEN AT HOME) / READING
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
HISPANIC (ENGLISH NOT SPOKEN AT HOME) / M ATHEMATICS
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
HISPANIC (ENGLISH SPOKEN AT HOME) / R EADING
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
HISPANIC (ENGLISH SPOKEN AT HOME) / M ATHEMATICS
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
ASIAN (ENGLISH NOT SPOKEN AT HOME) / READING
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
ASIAN (ENGLISH NOT SPOKEN AT HOME) / M ATHEMATICS
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
ASIAN (ENGLISH SPOKEN AT HOME) / READING
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
ASIAN (ENGLISH SPOKEN AT HOME) / M ATHEMATICS
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
156
TABLE D3: RATIO OF L EARNING RATE DIFFERENCES IN ECLS-K FOR IRT AND THETA BY RACE AND INCOME
IRT
Time Period
Gain Per Month
W HITE LOW -INCOME STUDENTS / R EADING
1.765
1st Grade/Kindergarten
0.542
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
W HITE LOW -INCOME STUDENTS / M ATHEMATICS
1.480
1st Grade/Kindergarten
0.528
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
W HITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS / READING
1.840
1st Grade/Kindergarten
0.467
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
W HITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS / M ATHEMATICS
1.448
1st Grade/Kindergarten
0.497
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
BLACK LOW -INCOME STUDENTS / R EADING
1.837
1st Grade/Kindergarten
0.541
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
BLACK LOW -INCOME STUDENTS / M ATHEMATICS
Effect Size
Theta
Gain Per Month
Effect Size
1.043
0.399
0.919
0.323
0.949
0.366
0.985
0.419
0.904
0.349
0.940
0.396
1.091
0.344
0.942
0.319
0.974
0.362
0.966
0.392
0.906
0.370
0.944
0.419
1.085
0.398
1.004
0.300
1.036
0.340
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
BLACK HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS / READING
1.636
0.527
1.090
0.419
0.982
0.330
1.021
0.374
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
BLACK HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS / M ATHEMATICS
1.612
0.535
0.953
0.393
0.892
0.320
0.921
0.363
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
HISPANIC LOW -INCOME STUDENTS / R EADING
1.508
0.557
1.005
0.442
0.966
0.351
1.005
0.398
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
HISPANIC LOW -INCOME STUDENTS / M ATHEMATICS
1.548
0.670
0.913
0.494
0.836
0.362
0.863
0.410
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
HISPANIC HIGHER -INCOME S TUDENTS / READING
1.645
0.561
1.097
0.445
0.919
0.345
0.956
0.391
1.623
1st Grade/Kindergarten
0.533
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
HISPANIC HIGHER -INCOME S TUDENTS / M ATHEMATICS
0.960
0.392
0.845
0.324
0.872
0.367
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
ASIAN LOW -INCOME STUDENTS / READING
1.482
0.538
0.987
0.427
0.884
0.360
0.920
0.408
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
ASIAN LOW -INCOME STUDENTS / M ATHEMATICS
1.454
0.527
0.859
0.388
0.751
0.327
0.775
0.371
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
ASIAN HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS / READING
1.188
0.727
0.790
0.577
0.766
0.461
0.798
0.522
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
ASIAN HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS / M ATHEMATICS
1.426
0.427
0.844
0.313
0.845
0.291
0.872
0.329
1.293
0.618
0.861
0.490
1.293
0.618
0.861
0.490
1st Grade/Kindergarten
2nd and 3rd Grades/1st Grade
157
TABLE D4: RATIO OF L EARNING RATE DIFFERENCES ACROSS GRADE (10TH TO 12TH G RADE RATE DIVIDED BY 8TH TO
10TH G RADE RATE) IN NELS:88: COMPARISON OF IRT AND T HETA
Time Period
IRT
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Theta
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
ALL STUDENTS
Reading
Mathematics
0.594
0.549
0.498
0.467
0.622
0.586
0.491
0.483
TABLE D5: RATIO OF L EARNING RATE DIFFERENCES ACROSS GRADE (10TH TO 12TH G RADE RATE DIVIDED BY 8TH TO
10TH G RADE RATE) IN NELS:88: COMPARISON OF IRT AND T HETA BY RACE
Time Period
IRT
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Theta
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
W HITE STUDENTS
Reading
Mathematics
0.579
0.539
0.479
0.458
0.609
0.579
0.487
0.479
Reading
Mathematics
0.667
0.663
0.552
0.563
0.678
0.631
0.542
0.521
Reading
Mathematics
0.783
0.669
0.648
0.569
0.836
0.650
0.669
0.536
Reading
Mathematics
0.788
0.583
0.652
0.496
0.877
0.640
0.702
0.529
BLACK STUDENTS
HISPANIC STUDENTS
ASIAN STUDENTS
158
TABLE D6: RATIO OF L EARNING RATE DIFFERENCES ACROSS GRADE (10TH TO 12TH G RADE RATE DIVIDED BY 8TH TO
10TH G RADE RATE) IN NELS:88: COMPARISON OF IRT AND T HETA BY RACE AND LANGUAGE
Time Period
IRT
Gain Per Effect Size Per
Month
Month
Gain Per
Month
Theta
Effect Size Per
Month
W HITE STUDENTS
Reading
Mathematics
0.578
0.539
0.479
0.458
0.608
0.579
0.487
0.479
Reading
Mathematics
0.665
0.662
0.550
0.563
0.675
0.662
0.539
0.563
HISPANIC (ENGLISH NOT SPOKEN AT HOME)
Reading
Mathematics
0.836
0.705
0.691
0.599
0.921
0.684
0.737
0.564
0.693
0.618
0.573
0.525
0.702
0.599
0.562
0.494
1.003
0.704
0.830
0.598
1.101
0.759
0.883
0.627
0.732
0.528
0.606
0.449
0.850
0.586
0.681
0.483
BLACK STUDENTS
HISPANIC (ENGLISH SPOKEN AT HOME)
Reading
Mathematics
ASIAN (ENGLISH NOT SPOKEN AT HOME)
Reading
Mathematics
ASIAN (ENGLISH SPOKEN AT HOME)
Reading
Mathematics
159
TABLE D7: RATIO OF L EARNING RATE DIFFERENCES ACROSS GRADE (10TH TO 12TH G RADE RATE DIVIDED BY 8TH TO
10TH G RADE RATE) IN NELS:88: COMPARISON OF IRT AND T HETA BY RACE AND I NCOME
Time Period
IRT
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
Theta
Gain Per Month
Effect Size Per
Month
W HITE LOW -INCOME STUDENTS
Reading
Mathematics
0.953
0.519
0.788
0.442
0.968
0.523
0.775
0.431
W HITE HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS
Reading
Mathematics
0.539
0.543
0.447
0.462
0.571
0.590
0.458
0.487
BLACK LOW -INCOME STUDENTS
Reading
Mathematics
0.857
0.667
0.708
0.568
0.874
0.597
0.699
0.492
0.728
0.648
0.602
0.551
0.734
0.645
0.587
0.532
Reading
Mathematics
1.004
0.695
0.831
0.591
1.075
0.645
0.862
0.533
HISPANIC HIGHER -INCOME S TUDENTS
Reading
Mathematics
0.745
0.628
0.616
0.534
0.790
0.613
0.632
0.506
ASIAN LOW -INCOME STUDENTS
Reading
Mathematics
1.455
0.669
1.203
0.569
1.524
0.733
1.221
0.605
ASIAN HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS
Reading
Mathematics
0.716
0.541
0.592
0.460
0.821
0.578
0.657
0.477
BLACK HIGHER -INCOME STUDENTS
Reading
Mathematics
HISPANIC LOW -INCOME STUDENTS
160
A PPENDIX E: LOCALLY STANDARDIZED GROWTH RATE D IFFERENCES
The locally standardized difference estimates reported in Chapter 5 are estimated as follows.
We first compute each student’s growth rate between round t and round t+1 by computing the change in
test score between rounds and dividing this by the number of calendar days elapsed between the two
assessments.48 Next we divided the distribution of test scores at round t at a relatively large number of
evenly-spaced points.49 At each of these points, we computed a bias-adjusted kernel-weighted50 estimate of
the local within-group difference in growth rates and a kernel-weighted estimate of the local pooled
standard deviation51, and we then divide the estimated difference in growth rates by the estimated pooled
standard deviation, yielding an estimate of the locally standardized difference in growth rates at each point.
We then compute a standard error for the estimated standardized difference at each point.52 Finally, we
average the estimated locally standardized differences in growth rates over the range of the initial test
scores, weighting the average by the inverse of the variance of the estimate at each point.53
This process yields an estimate of the average locally standardized difference in growth rates between two
groups. This estimate can be interpreted as the expected difference in growth rates between round t and
t+1 between two students with the same score at round t, expressed in terms of the standard deviation of
growth rates among all students starting with that same score.
Since growth rates are later scaled by their standard deviations, it makes no difference whether we use these daily growth
rates, or rescale them to some other unit of time.
49 In the analyses here, we selected points in the distribution of test scores that were 0.02 standard deviations apart, though the
results were insensitive to other choices.
50 We report estimates based on a biweight kernel with halfwidth of 0.1 standard deviations of the round t score distribution,
but our results are virtually identical if we use a rectangular kernel or a different halfwidth. The biweight kernel has the form
48
 d
wij = 1 −  ij
  h



2
2

 , where dij is the distance from point j, and h is the kernel halfwidth; where dij>h, wij=0. The bias adjustment is

done by fitting a kernel-weighted regression model at each point j (i.e., fitting a model of the form Yi=b0j+b1j(groupi)+b2j(dij)+ei).
51 In computing female-male differences, we use the pooled within-gender standard deviation; in computing poor-non-poor
differences, we use the pooled within-economic group standard deviation; and in computing race/ethnic differences, we use the
pooled within race/ethnic group standard deviation.
52 The standard error of δˆ , the estimated locally standardized difference at point j, is computed as
j
( )
s.e, δˆj = δˆ j
( )
s.e. γˆ j
γˆ j 2
2
+
( )
s.e. σˆ j
σˆ j2
2
, where δˆ j =
γˆ j
, and where γˆ j and σ̂ j are the estimated difference in growth rates and the
σˆ j
estimated pooled standard deviation of growth rates at point j, respectively. Bootstrap standard errors matched these computed
standard errors very closely.
[ ( )]

53 The standard error of this average is computed as s.e. δˆ  =
 j  ∑ s.e. δˆj
   j
161
−2
−
1
 2
 .

Download