REVIEW OF THE RED TO GREEN SEQUENCE AT TRAFFIC SIGNALS Alastair Maxwell TRL Iain York TRL 1. SUMMARY At traffic signals in the UK a red-with-amber signal (‘starting amber’) denotes the impending change to green, but conveys the same prohibition as the red signal. It is fixed at two seconds. Practice varies across Europe with some countries using a starting amber and some not. The period for which the starting amber is shown also varies by country, most use either one or two seconds. Additionally some European countries have or are considering reducing the length of the starting amber. The UK Department for Transport commissioned TRL to review omitting or reducing the ‘starting amber’ both in efficiency and safety terms, compared with the current policy. The research included: an international literature review; previous accident studies at traffic signals; a review of practice in other countries; stakeholder interviews and questionnaires; video analysis of vehicle, cyclist, and pedestrian response to the current starting amber, and conflict analysis; and a TRL Driving Simulator experiment examining driver responses to 0, 1 and 2 second starting amber periods. The advantages and disadvantages of a change to the current starting amber policy are assessed, including effects on non-motorised traffic. 2. BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES While the starting amber is a requirement for the UK, it is not the case in many countries abroad. Drivers are becoming increasingly familiar with traffic signal systems and strategies used elsewhere. The last significant UK research on the starting amber was carried out in the early 1960’s. The Department for Transport finds that it is increasingly difficult to defend the status quo based upon this, given changes in vehicle and signal technology and driver experiences. As such this new research has been commissioned to investigate the consequences of omitting or amending the length of the starting amber, both in safety and efficiency terms. Note that, any views expressed are not necessarily those of the Department for Transport. © Association for European Transport and contributors 2005 3. METHODOLOGY The main strands of the research were: • An initial review including practice in other countries, an international literature review, previous accident studies at traffic signals, stakeholder interviews and questionnaires, and an initial assessment of the potential implications on road users (including pedestrians). • Comprehensive video analysis of the current UK situation, recording vehicle and cyclist movements in relation to the starting amber, conflict analysis, and investigating how pedestrians use the starting amber. • A pedestrian questionnaire, used to ascertain their perceived crossing behaviour at traffic signals and their views on a change to the starting amber. • A TRL Driving Simulator experiment examining driver’s responses to 0, 1 and 2 second starting amber periods. Drivers’ decisions were investigated when they were approaching, or already stationary at the junction. At some of the junctions the drivers could just see the signal shown by the opposing signal heads. Drivers completed questionnaires before and after the trials. Further methodology details are given in the relevant sections. 4. STARTING AMBER POLICY Pan-European policy on the use of the starting amber is laid down in the European rules concerning road traffic signs and signals (European Conference of Ministers of Transport, 1974), commonly referred to as the ‘Vienna convention’. The starting amber is optional and no timing periods are specified. The red and amber shown together means ‘that signal is about to change, but shall not affect the prohibition of passing indicated by the red light.’ The use of the starting amber varies across Europe, generally the ‘low countries’ and southern European countries do not use a starting amber, while Scandinavian and northern European countries do, as shown in Table 1. The period for which the starting amber is shown also varies by country from between one and three seconds. © Association for European Transport and contributors 2005 Table 1: European starting amber practice Starting amber Length of starting used amber (seconds) Poland 1 Finland 1 to 1.5 (2 before 1990) Sweden 1.5 (1 in the future) Greece Germany 1 Luxembourg UK 2 Netherlands Austria 2 Portugal Czech Republic 2 Italy Cyprus 2 Spain Iceland 2 Ireland Malta 2 Russia 3 Starting amber not used Belgium France Both the Finnish and Swedish decisions to reduce the starting amber were made subjectively on the basis of drivers violating the starting amber. An interviewed Finnish traffic control committee member stated that there has been no noticeable effect on delay, and both drivers and signal experts are satisfied with the practice used now. Most countries outside Europe do not utilise the starting amber, including: USA; Canada; Australia and New Zealand. Until around 1980, two Australian States, Western Australia and Victoria, had starting ambers. They were removed in line with the national standards. An interviewed representative of Victoria State Highway Authority was of the opinion that drivers easily adapt to some signals having a starting amber and some not. 5. PREVIOUS STUDIES ON THE EFFECTS OF THE STARTING AMBER 5.1 Introduction There have been three reported on-street trials where the starting amber period was omitted. The UK trials previously had a three second starting amber, the others a two second period: • • UK trials analysing 18 junctions from 1959 to 1962 (Older, 1963) Hong Kong trials covering 2 junctions and 2 pedestrian crossings in 1973 (Seneviratne, 1974). © Association for European Transport and contributors 2005 • Melbourne, Australia, trials covering 7 junctions in 1965; analysis was only undertaken at two junctions (McGill, 1970). There was also study in London in 1979 (Branston, 1979), at 10 approaches to signalised junctions, which assessed the effect of the length of the starting amber upon green starting lost time. 5.2 Effect on efficiency The starting amber provides a warning of the impending green to drivers, and this helps maximise the capacity of the junction by reducing starting lost time (the time taken from the onset of green for queued vehicles to reach maximum flow rate over the stopline (‘saturation flow’)). The trials indicated average increases in vehicular starting delay of between 1 and 1.7* seconds (*3 second starting amber) when the starting amber is omitted. The increase in starting delay was less when opposing signal heads could be seen. Older stated that if all the signals had been effectively screened, the average effect of omitting the starting amber would have been greater, and McGill (1970) found an initial 1.4 second increase in starting lost time at one study junction which fell to zero after a period of weeks when drivers adapted their behaviour to anticipate the start of green from opposing signal heads. Older calculated that the increase in lost time would reduce junction capacity by 6%, based upon the junctions used in this experiment. After the Hong Kong trials the starting amber was maintained, the effect on vehicle delay was cited as the main reason. Branston (1979) maintained the starting amber, but studied the effect of its length. The results indicated that the increase in starting lost time is approximately equal to the reduction in starting amber time, when the starting amber period is reduced. 5.3 Effect upon traffic signal violations The starting amber has the same prohibition as the red signal. However, previous surveys and work undertaken in this project indicate that there is a notable violation rate. The UK trials (Older, 1963) showed a reduction from 1.5 percent of all vehicles entering the junction in the starting amber period to 0.1 percent in the equivalent three seconds when the starting amber was omitted. However, there was an increase in the number of vehicles entering the junction in the stopping amber period, but not the following red. Prior to the omission 15 percent of the signal cycles had at least one vehicle entering the junction after the amber appeared, while after the omission this figure had risen to 20 percent (a 33% increase). Older believed that this was due to drivers © Association for European Transport and contributors 2005 becoming familiar with, and taking advantage of, the increase in start-up delay of opposing vehicles. The Hong Kong trials (Seneviratne, 1974) showed a reduction from 22.1% to 6.5% for first vehicle waiting at the lights to ‘jump the signals’. While this is a significant reduction, it still shows a notable proportion of drivers who were prepared to proceed, anticipating the onset of green, when there was not a starting amber. The Branston (1979) results indicate that the level of starting amber violations increase with the length of the starting amber period. 5.4 Effect upon safety The UK and Hong Kong trials indicated potential safety benefits from omitting the starting amber from the increase in the effective intergreen time caused by the increase in starting delay. Seneviratne noted a significant reduction in pedestrian conflicts with vehicles at pedestrian crossings (which had low clearance times and very high pedestrian flows), and the UK trials showed a significant reduction in personal-injury accidents between vehicles from different roads (47%), however this was offset by an unexplained increase in single vehicle and same direction vehicle accidents (31% increase). However, these trials had intergreen times which were well below the current recommended values, and as such these benefits would be unlikely to be shown in today’s conditions. Newby (1961) showed an 83% reduction in accidents between vehicles on different roads by adding a one or two second all-red period. Increasing the effective intergreen potentially provides more time for pedestrians to cross. However, pedestrians no longer receive a warning of the start of vehicle green from the traffic signal indications. Branston found that pedestrians were more likely to be crossing at the start of green when the traffic signal indication was impossible or difficult to see, and the UK trials found a 128% (Older 1960) increase in the number of pedestrians crossing in the period that had previously been red-with-amber – part of this increase will have also been due to a reduction in vehicles beginning to move in this period. The UK trials showed that overall effect on pedestrian accidents was negligible. Both Older and Seneviratne expressed concerns that there may be an increase due to the lack of warning. Older suggested the negligible effect on pedestrian accidents could have been due to the ‘change in sequence made pedestrians rely more on personal observation of vehicle behaviour for their safety than on the implication of the vehicle signal faces’. 5.5 Amber arrows Amber arrows have been trialled in the UK to attempt to reduce the number of false starts caused by drivers reacting to the wrong red-with-amber signal at double-headed signal arrangements using green arrows. The trials indicate a © Association for European Transport and contributors 2005 reduction in drivers responding to the wrong signal, but an increase in violations of the stopping amber and following red. It was thought that the lower visibility of the amber arrow was the likely reason for the deterioration of driver behaviour at the end of green. The amber arrow is less easy to see than a full roundel, making it more difficult to distinguish and thus increasing driver response time, and reducing general compliance to the signal (Pleydell and Gillam, 2002). A possible solution under trial is an ‘intelligent’ LED amber arrow, which shows an arrow for the starting amber but a full roundel for the stopping amber. A potential advantage of omitting the starting amber would be to prevent drivers reacting to the wrong amber roundel under green arrow arrangements, and thus negate the perceived need for amber arrows. 6. STAKEHOLDER INPUT The main aim of the stakeholder input was to help identify the potential issues arising from any change to the starting amber. A secondary aim was to get a sample of stakeholders’ views on the subject. TRL specialists covering traffic signals, cycling, motorcycling and pedestrians were interviewed, and a list of potential issues associated with the starting amber and its potential amendment drawn up. The external stakeholder input covered the committee of the Traffic Control User Group (TCUG) and a selection of Local Authorities. These stakeholders were sent a questionnaire to complete and interviewed. The stakeholders were in favour of maintaining the current two-second starting amber (a minority were unsure whether two seconds was optimal) and did not believe that a change would be worthwhile or the cost justifiable. The main concerns, with regards to changing the current policy were: • • • • • The increase in starting delay should the starting amber be omitted The effect upon co-ordination of closely spaced signals e.g. signalised roundabouts – the starting amber signals the impending green, facilitating the progression of approaching drivers. The lack of warning to pedestrians of the onset of vehicle green. Increased pressure on drivers to get away quickly at the start of green. Implementation costs of any change The only issue with the starting amber raised was that some drivers treat it as ‘go’. However, the effect of this was not regarded as being so significant as to be a safety issue, and no interviewees were aware of any cases where the intergreen time had been set above the guideline values, given in TA/16/81 General principles of control by traffic signals, due to drivers beginning to proceed in the starting amber period. © Association for European Transport and contributors 2005 7. RESULTS OF THE VIDEO SURVEYS SHOWING CURRENT RESPONSE TO THE STARTING AMBER 7.1 Introduction Videos of five junctions and two pedestrian crossings (Puffins) were analysed, covering a range of site characteristics, including urban and interurban, and low and high pedestrian flows. The data were recorded for the first vehicle waiting at the stopline. Times were recorded to 0.04 seconds (25 frames per second). The centre of the front wheel was used as the position reference for the vehicle. Distances were estimated to 0.5 metres (or less where possible) from junction plan/ measured site dimensions. A clear Perspex sheet with sample distances from the stopline was used on the screen to ensure consistency in reported distances. 7.2 Driver response to the starting amber The results exclude situations where there was a pedestrian or vehicle conflict, or blocking back within the junction i.e. where the starting movement could have been impeded. Conflicts are analysed separately in Section 7.4. The results also exclude twelve blatant red runners who did not wait for the end of the opposing phase. Most of these were cyclists (7), or vehicles at the junction ‘Camden’ (4). Camden has a double headed signal arrangement, with the studied approach being a full signalised right turn green arrow, on some occasions vehicles would proceed when the straight-ahead signal went green when the right turn was still red. This type of violation is discussed in Section 5.5. As can be seen from Table 2, a notable percentage of vehicles crossed the stopline before the onset of green (36%), especially at junctions. On average, vehicles are in front of the stopline by the time that the green starts, they start to move 0.25 seconds before the onset of green and those that have stopped behind the stopline cross it 0.38 seconds after green. Table 2: Summary of driver response to the starting amber © Association for European Transport and contributors 2005 Traffic signal type Analysis Type Waiting distance (metres) Start to move time (secs from start of redamber) Distance to stopline at the start of green (metres) Distance travelled by the start of green (metres) Cross stopline time* (secs Sample size from start of red-amber) Waiting behind stopline Cross stopline before green* Start to move before green 86% 36% 66% 83% 39% 63% 98% 27% 74% Mean 0.68 1.75 -0.47 1.15 2.38 947 15th (85th) %ile 0.00 1.04 -2.50 2.50 1.44 2.35 0.36 1.81 -0.77 Mean 1.13 755 junctions 15th (85th) %ile -0.50 1.04 -3.00 2.50 1.40 2.51 1.96 1.50 0.73 Mean 1.23 192 Puffins 15th (85th) %ile 0.00 0.88 -1.00 2.50 1.65 *Excludes vehicles that wait in front of stopline Bold and underlined: Junction and Puffin results statistically different at the 95% level Distance traveled is the 85th percentile, the rest are 15th percentiles All Comparing Puffins and junctions, the results indicate significant differences in driver behaviour at the studied traffic signal installations. At Puffins, drivers cross the stopline later and have a greater distance to the stopline at the start of green. This is primarily due to the larger waiting distance. Possible reasons for this are: • • • • Drivers avoid blocking the pedestrian crossing. Drivers avoid pressurising pedestrians. There is not an Advanced Stop Line (ASL) to abuse as there was at one of the junctions. And perhaps the closely associated secondary signal heads means that drivers get a better view from a distance back from the stopline. The quicker start to move times at Puffins is likely to be due to: • • A driver, seeing that the pedestrian has crossed, is likely to expect the end of red shortly, also on seeing the pedestrian has crossed a proportion of drivers (4%) then starting to move (See Table 3). The relatively short red times at Puffins may mean that it is more likely that the handbrake is not applied and the vehicle is still in gear. Table 3 shows the percentage of drivers starting to move and crossing the stopline in the first 0, 1, 1.5, and 2 seconds of the starting amber. Table 3: Start to move and cross stopline times relative to time in starting amber © Association for European Transport and contributors 2005 Traffic signal Total sample type size Waiting behind stopline Cross stopline during red* Cross stopline before 1sec starting amber* Cross stopline before 1.5sec starting amber* Cross stopline before green* Start to Start to Start to move before move before Start to move during 1sec move before 1.5sec red starting green starting amber amber All 947 86% 0% 4% 17% 36% 1% 14% 40% 66% junctions 755 83% 0% 4% 19% 39% 1% 14% 40% 63% Puffins 192 98% 1% 2% 10% 27% 4% 18% 42% 74% *Excludes vehicles that wait in front of stopline Bold and underlined: Junction and Puffin results statistically different at the 95% level Distance traveled is the 85th percentile, the rest are 15th percentiles As can be seen, for all traffic signal types, 4% of vehicles that waited on or behind the stopline crossed it before the first second of red-amber, and 17% crossed it before 1.5 seconds of red-amber. Figures 1 to 4 illustrate the times that vehicles start to move and cross the stopline. Figure 5 shows the distances travelled by the start of green. Figure 1: Start to move time 30% 26.50% 25.03% Percentage of vehicles 25% 20% 17.85% 15% 10.67% 10% 7.60% 5.28% 5% 2.22% 1.90% 0.11% 0.11% 0.21% 0.32% 0.21% 0.21% 0.95% 0.53% 0.21% 0.11% 0% -5.5 -4 -3 -2 -1.5 -1 0 0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 3.5 4 4.5 5 5.5 Start to move time seconds from start of red-amber © Association for European Transport and contributors 2005 Figure 2: Cumulative frequency graph of start to move time 100% 95% Percentage of vehicles that have started to move 90% 85% 80% 75% 70% 65% 60% 55% 50% 45% 40% 35% 30% 25% 20% 15% 10% 5% 0% 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 1.2 1.4 1.6 1.8 2 2.2 2.4 2.6 2.8 3 3.2 3.4 3.6 3.8 4 4.2 4.4 4.6 4.8 5 5.2 5.4 5.6 5.8 6 Seconds from start of red-amber Figure 3: Cross the stopline time (excludes those stopped in advance of the stopline) 30% 25.95% Percentage of vehicles 25% 20% 18.97% 14.93% 15% 13.10% 10.53% 10% 6.36% 5% 3.30% 3.06% 0.12% 1.71% 0.73% 0.24% 0.61% 0.24% 0.12% 5 5.5 6 6.5 0% -1 0 0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 3.5 4 4.5 Crossing the stopline time seconds from start of red-amber © Association for European Transport and contributors 2005 Figure 4: Cumulative frequency graph of time of crossing the stopline (excludes those stopped in advance of the stopline) 100% 95% 90% 85% 80% Percentage crossed the stopline 75% 70% 65% 60% 55% 50% 45% 40% 35% 30% 25% 20% 15% 10% 5% 0% 6.00 5.80 5.60 5.40 5.20 5.00 4.80 4.60 4.40 4.20 4.00 3.80 3.60 3.40 3.20 3.00 2.80 2.60 2.40 2.20 2.00 1.80 1.60 1.40 1.20 1.00 0.80 0.60 0.40 0.20 0.00 Seconds from start of red amber Figure 5: Distance travelled by the start of green 40% 36.22% 35% Percentage of vehicles 30% 24.29% 25% 21.01% 20% 15% 11.40% 10% 4.65% 5% 2.01% 0.32% 0.11% 4>5 5>6 6>7 0% 0 0>1 1>2 2>3 3>4 Distance travelled by start of green (metres) Table 4 gives a summary of the results by vehicle type. The mean and percentage results for ‘Cars and Taxis’ have been statistically assessed against the other vehicle types. © Association for European Transport and contributors 2005 Table 4: Summary of results by vehicle type Vehicle type Analysis Type Waiting distance (metres) Start to move time (secs from start of redamber) Distance to stopline at the start of green (metres) Distance travelled by start of green (metres) Cross stopline Total time* (secs sample size from start of red-amber) Mean 0.68 1.75 -0.47 1.15 2.38 15th (85th) %ile 0.00 1.04 -2.50 2.50 1.44 Cars & Mean 0.93 1.75 -0.17 1.10 2.38 taxis 15th (85th) %ile 0.00 1.04 -2.00 2.50 1.48 0.68 Mean 1.94 -0.25 0.93 2.38 LGV 15th (85th) %ile 0.00 1.06 -2.00 2.15 1.41 Mean 1.55 1.55 0.71 2.72 0.84 HGV 15th (85th) %ile 0.00 1.04 0.00 1.95 1.92 0.63 3.32 Mean 1.28 1.99 0.66 PSV 15th (85th) %ile -0.75 1.64 -1.75 1.00 2.15 -1.51 1.40 -3.91 2.41 1.59 Mean Motorbike 15th (85th) %ile -4.00 0.73 -6.00 4.00 0.87 Mean -4.42 1.67 -5.69 1.27 1.56 Bicycle 15th (85th) %ile -6.40 0.94 -8.00 2.00 1.56 *Excludes vehicles that wait in front of the stopline Bold: Other vehicle types statistically different at the 95% level Distance traveled is the 85th percentile, the rest are 15th percentiles LGV: Light Goods Vehicle. HGV Heavy Goods Vehicle. PSV Public Service Vehicle All Waiting behind stopline Cross stopline before green* Start to move before green 947 86% 36% 66% 727 91% 35% 65% 111 93% 38% 57% 18 100% 22% 56% 16 81% 8% 50% 62 31% 74% 87% 13 8% 100% 77% The results show that motorcyclists and bicyclists have particularly different characteristics from cars and taxis. The majority of both tend to wait in-front of the stopline, and are quite far advanced beyond the stopline by the start of green. Only one cyclist waited behind the stopline (this includes a junction with an ASL), and not included in this analysis are seven blatant red running cyclists. The potential for rapid acceleration of motorcycles, together with infringing the starting amber means that there is potential for conflicts in the junction unless the motorcyclists check carefully before accelerating into the junction. The conflict analysis indicates that this could be the case, with only one motorcyclist being involved in a conflict and that was due to congestion within the junction, there was no starting amber violation. The results were also assessed by time of day (peak, off-peak and lunch), generally there was little difference, except in the peak period slightly fewer drivers wait behind the stopline and vehicles tended to be slightly further in advance of the stopline at the start of green (0.70 metres). Results by site were assessed. Site plans and signal timings were collected, and site visits undertaken, particular reference was given to intervisibility (the ability for drivers at each stopline to see each other and pedestrian waiting positions) and the ability to see opposing signal heads. The results showed a fair amount of variation in driver behaviour by site. An assessment of the results was made against the site characteristics. The results would appear to indicate that drivers modify their behaviour based upon the ability to assess the risks and the potential for conflicts. Situations where intervisibility is good and the potential for conflicts is low are treated with less caution. Busy urban junctions had notably lower violation rates than open, low pedestrian flow, © Association for European Transport and contributors 2005 interurban junctions. Ahead movements also had a quicker start to move time than turning movements. Previous research indicated that the ability to see opposing signal heads is a significant factor affecting the starting delay of vehicles. The sites studied here tended to provide little opportunity for drivers to see the opposing signals and their visibility did not appear to be a particularly significant factor in this study. 7.3 Pedestrians Pedestrian arrivals and crossing decisions at the crossing facilities were recorded between the last two seconds of red and the first two seconds of green. A full day (08:00 to 18:00) was analysed at each site. The results showed that the vast majority of pedestrians were prepared to cross the road in-front of stationary traffic, 80% of all pedestrians arriving between the last two seconds red and first two seconds green chose to cross in this period if the opposing vehicle was stationary. While if the pedestrian arrives when vehicles have started to move then they are much less likely to cross; only 30% in the starting amber and 10% during the green. There was no statistically significant difference between the proportion of pedestrians crossing at the end of red (89%) and the starting amber (84%). Thus unlike the previous UK trials (Older, 1963) there is no indication from these results that the starting amber signal acts as a warning to pedestrians of the onset of green. 7.4 Conflicts A conflict is defined as a situation where two road users would collide if neither took any avoiding action, e.g. braked, swerved etc. The ‘conflict’ analysis recorded incidents occurring around the time of the starting amber in an attempt to assess whether there are safety issues associated with driver and pedestrian behaviour under a two second starting amber. Two Puffins (one a single direction approach and the other with a two-way approach) and five approaches to junctions were studied, with over 52 hours of video analysed. In total 82 conflicts were recorded, 33 being vehicle-vehicle conflicts and 49 vehicle-pedestrian conflicts; a conflict rate per hour of around 1.6 per approach/ Puffin around the start of green. Table 5 shows the types of conflict. 90% of vehicles delayed start of movement when there was a pedestrian conflict, and with a vehicle conflict the percentage was about two-thirds; with the conflict point further away vehicles were prepared to start moving and then brake if the conflict point was still not clear. There were no near misses recorded © Association for European Transport and contributors 2005 Table 5: Type of conflict Type of conflict Pedestrian Vehicle All conflicts conflicts conflicts Count % Count % Count % Obstruction – delayed start of 44 90% 22 67% 66 80% movement Precautionary braking or lane 5 10% 11 33% 16 20% change when risk is minimal Near miss 0 0% 0 0% 0 0% Very near miss 0 0% 0 0% 0 0% Collision 0 0% 0 0% 0 0% Total 49 100% 33 100% 82 100% The majority of pedestrian-vehicle conflicts recorded were associated with pedestrians beginning to cross in the starting amber period, as shown in Table 6. Many pedestrians who started to cross towards the end of red were able to clear the conflict point before a vehicle could have reached it. An assessment was made of the studied sites and it was found that on average only pedestrians crossing in the last second of red (half the length of the starting amber period) would be in potential conflict with a starting vehicle. Table 6: Period that conflicting pedestrian starts to cross Pedestrian start Percentage to cross period Red 25% Starting amber 60% Green 15% Total 100% About half of pedestrians in a potential conflict increased their walking speed. The most common combined action to avoid the conflict is for the pedestrian to proceed normally and the driver to delay starting. Only three pedestrian conflicts involved a starting amber violation, but in all cases the conflict probably would have occurred without the violation, in one the pedestrian starting crossing in green; in another the pedestrian ran, allowing the violation without a safety impact; and in the third the vehicle violated but proceeded very slowly. Vehicle-vehicle conflicts occur when a vehicle has not cleared the conflict point by the time vehicles from the next phase would have reached it. The causes of not clearing the conflict point found in this study were: • • • • Opposed right turners Blocking back/ congestion Red running Large vehicles making a difficult turning movement © Association for European Transport and contributors 2005 By far the most common event was for the starting vehicle to delay movement and the conflicting vehicle to proceed normally. Though not classified as near misses, there were four more potentially serious conflicts. All involved at least partial obscuration between the starting and conflicting vehicle due to blocking back through the junction, three were associated with apparent red runners and one an opposed right turner. Three of the four had starting amber violations. In total five vehicle conflicts involved a starting amber violation (15%, which is much lower than the 36% recorded for all starting vehicles), three of these probably would not have occurred without the violation. Two involved red running vehicles on other approaches and one an opposed right turn movement combined with congestion within the junction. Thus in conclusion where there is a visible potential conflict drivers tend not to violate the starting amber, and if they do they can use precautionary braking later, and, as shown in Section 7.3, pedestrians tend not to start crossing if vehicle movement has started. The potentially more serious conflicts were associated with the conflict being at least partially obscured from view and the conflicting vehicle movement occurring well into the green of the new stage. Of the total of 82 conflicts, 8 involved a starting amber violation and three conflicts (3.7%) probably would have been avoided without the violation. In no cases was a starting amber violation the sole or main cause of a conflict. 8. PEDESTRIAN QUESTIONNAIRE The absence of a warning of the onset of green to pedestrians was seen as a concern by interviewed stakeholders. Pedestrian behaviour was surveyed onstreet (Sections 7.3 and 7.4) and a pedestrian questionnaire devised to attempt to assess anticipated changes in behaviour. A total of 107 participants completed the questionnaire. In general pedestrians believed they would feel less safe if the starting amber was removed (73% at junctions with red/green men, and 81% at junctions without). The main reasons given were: not receiving any warning of the impending green; the starting amber gives time for drivers to check for pedestrians; and a feeling that there would be less time to cross due to the immediate change to green. For the small percentage of participants who thought that they would feel safer without a red-with-amber, one of the main underlying reasons was that they would be more cautious and consequently safer. Another reason was the inability of drivers to start off on red-with-amber. Only 6% of pedestrians stated that they always wait for the green man. When crossing against a red man 76% stated that they always or ‘usually’ check the signal indication given to vehicles before deciding to cross. Most participants © Association for European Transport and contributors 2005 (70%) considered that they would be more hesitant about crossing against a red man if the starting amber was omitted. At junctions without pedestrian phases 68% said that they would be less likely to start to cross part way through the red to traffic. In summary pedestrians consider that they would feel less safe if the red-withamber was removed and would show more caution and request pedestrian phases more often. This could possibly cause greater delays to pedestrians and traffic, but could potentially offset any negative influences on pedestrian safety, as indicated by the Older (1963) trials. 9. THE TRL DRIVING SIMULATOR EXPERIMENT TRL’s driving simulator consists of a full-size car linked to a wrap around projected screen. Motion is simulated through the suspension units of the vehicle and provides heave, pitch and roll to the car body. It was used to investigate drivers’ responses to changing the starting amber period in a safe environment. The simulated road network consisted of 15 signal controlled junctions, one kilometre apart on a 50 mph speed limit dual carriageway. There were automated cars travelling in the opposite direction and from the side roads. No automated cars were on the same side of the road as the driver. This prevented them influencing the drivers decision when to start moving forward. The traffic signals were set to change from red either on the approach to the signals (20, 30 or 150 metres from the stopline) or after a varied period of waiting at the stopline. At the signals where the drivers came to a stop, the opposing signal head indication could just be seen at half the junctions. The intergreen timings were set to six seconds at all junctions. A total of 60 drivers, 30 male and 30 female took part in the simulator experiment. Twenty participants of each gender were in the age range 25 to 55, whilst ten were older drivers. The younger drivers took part in three separate driving runs, with the order randomised. In each drive the starting amber duration was constant at all junctions and set to either: • • • 2 seconds (currently in use within the UK) 1 second 0 seconds (change straight from red to green) The older drivers were only asked to drive twice, with the first and last of these red-with-amber durations (i.e. the extreme situations). This examined whether age affected reactions to red-with-amber duration, in addition to the main sample examining the reactions in detail. © Association for European Transport and contributors 2005 The results showed that drivers waiting at the stopline were delayed by: • • • 1.4 to 1.6 seconds when the starting amber was omitted (younger drivers) 1.2 to 1.3 seconds when the starting amber was omitted (older drivers) 0.7 to 0.8 seconds when the starting amber was reduced to one second (younger drivers) This equates to a loss of junction capacity, all else being equal. For instance a three stage junction, with a cycle time of 80 seconds would have a reduced capacity of around 6% if the starting amber was omitted and about 3% if it was reduced to one second, based upon the results of the younger drivers. Given the congested nature of many UK junctions in the peak hours this could have a significant effect upon vehicle delay, especially for an omission, unless the intergreen timings are lowered in line with the increases in starting delays. The difference in ages appears to be largely due to older drivers showing more caution under current conditions. They were less likely to start to move in the red-with-amber than the younger drivers. The crossing the stopline results were similar for vehicles approaching the stopline when the signals changed, and indicate that the starting amber can be beneficial to maintain progression at closely spaced signals. For signal changes at 20 and 30 metres from the stopline younger drivers took 0.8 to 0.9 seconds longer to enter the junction when the red-with-amber duration was reduced to one second. When the red-with-amber was removed younger drivers entered the junction 1.6 to 1.7 seconds later after the start of green, and older drivers 1.2 to 1.3 seconds later. The ability to view opposing signal heads was found to be a significant determinant of the delay in starting from rest both in previous trials and in this. Younger drivers’ average delay was reduced by 0.7 seconds and older drivers’ average by 0.4 seconds when the opposing signals were visible. This reduction in delay did not depend significantly on whether or not a starting amber was used. Therefore, unlike previous trials (Older and McGill), the simulator results did not show drivers using the opposing signals more when the starting amber is omitted than when it is present. However, these were short term results and further behaviour modifications could occur with familiarity, as shown by McGill (1970) described in Section 5.2. For vehicles waiting at the stopline acceleration used was found to be slightly, but statistically significantly (at the 95% confidence level), greater when the starting amber was omitted. The maximum acceleration used was on average between 0.08 and 0.09 m/s2 greater, and the maximum acceleration was reached 0.7 to 0.8 metres earlier. There is an implication that drivers accelerated slightly harder when the starting amber was omitted, possibly as an over-reaction to an unexpected change. However, these slightly higher accelerations had little effect on speed once through the junction, with the average difference in speed only being 0.3 to 0.4 km/h greater 50 metres after the stopline. © Association for European Transport and contributors 2005 The results for average brake release time (hand or foot brake) and accelerator on times shown in the graphs below, show that drivers react quicker to a change from red to green than from red to starting amber and more uniformly (as shown by the standard deviation). © Association for European Transport and contributors 2005 Figure 6: Average time brake released after end of red Time (seconds) Time brake released after signal change 3.5 3 2.5 2 1.5 1 0.5 0 0 seconds 1 seconds 2 seconds 0 20 30 150 Distance from stop line (m) Figure 7: Average time accelerator pressed after end of red Time accelerator pressed after signal change Time (seconds) 4 Starting amber length 3 0 seconds 2 1 seconds 2 seconds 1 0 0 20 30 150 Distance from stop line (m) Figure 8: Standard deviation of brake release time after end of red Time (seconds) Standard deviation of time brake released after signal change Starting amber length 1.4 1.2 1 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 0 seconds 1 seconds 2 seconds 0 20 30 150 Distance from stop line (m) © Association for European Transport and contributors 2005 Figure 9: Standard deviation of accelerator pressed time after end of red Time (seconds) Standard deviation of time accelerator pressed after signal change Starting amber length 2.5 2 1.5 1 0.5 0 0 seconds 1 seconds 2 seconds 0 20 30 150 Distance from stop line (m) The level of signal violations was found to be very low in the simulator experiment. On investigation it was found that on average drivers waited two metres further back than on-street. It is believed that the close to perspective and visibility of the stopline in the simulator partly caused this, and this accounts for much of the difference in crossing time, but not all, reaction times also appear to be slightly slower. It is possible that drivers on the road, with other vehicles waiting behind them, feel more pressure to start moving as soon as possible. Drivers completed questionnaires before and after the simulation runs. Over three quarters identified the correct definition of the starting amber, however 20% of drivers gave definitions which implied that one could proceed with caution. Also one third of drivers did not believe it was bad practice to cross the stopline during the starting amber. Participants generally preferred the current two seconds of red-with-amber time: 69% of those aged under 55 and 55% of those aged over 55. Participants’ reasons were due to familiarity, and feeling more comfortable owing to having more time to prepare to go. 18% of the younger driver preferred a 1 second red-with-amber as they felt most comfortable with it and it reduced hesitancy and reduced waiting time. Approximately a sixth of the participants preferred removing the red-with-amber completely. It was mainly the older driver who chose this option (30% of them). Their reasons were that it was easier to understand and made them feel safer. However, overall drivers felt more hurried in their decisions without a starting amber, which could have safety implications. © Association for European Transport and contributors 2005 10. IMPLEMENTATION ISSUES In March 2000 there were an estimated 12,300 sets of traffic signal controlled junctions and 13,800 isolated traffic signal controlled pedestrian crossings (DTLR, 2001) – note that many of the crossings are ‘Pelicans’, which use a flashing amber rather than a red-with-amber. Additionally, there are portable traffic signal controllers and temporary traffic lights. The implementation costs of any change in the starting amber period would be significant, particularly as new firmware would be required in all existing controllers, as the starting amber period is fixed. The reduction in starting amber in Finland was implemented over 5 years. Most controllers had a configurable starting amber period, so it was relatively easy to modify. The intergreen timings were maintained i.e. the omitted red-with-amber was replaced by red. 11. CONCLUSIONS The starting amber provides a warning of the impending green, giving drivers time to prepare to move. This helps maximise the capacity of the traffic signals and reduces pressure to get away quickly at the start of green. It also serves to maintain progression at closely linked signals. The simulator trial showed a more uniform response to the start of green when the starting amber was omitted, and some drivers said that such a sequence was easier to understand. With a two second amber, part of the efficiency benefits are from some drivers crossing the stopline in the starting amber. While this is a concern and against the regulations, the current recommended UK intergreen timings are generally regarded as appropriate, and the safety assessment showed that drivers usually delay movement when there is a potential conflict, and only tend to proceed on the starting amber when it is safe to do so. To maintain the current efficiency levels, if the starting amber was omitted, the intergreen timings would need to be adjusted accordingly with the increase in start up delay. This has potential safety concerns, especially for vulnerable road users, as drivers would now be starting to proceed in the green period which previously was starting amber, which has been shown to be treated with caution. Reactions to green are quicker and drivers felt under more pressure. Additionally, the potential increase in drivers anticipating the end of red from opposing signals and traffic movements, is unlikely to warrant a wholesale reduction in intergreen timings. Increased anticipation also has safety concerns. Such behaviour could lead to drivers concentrating on the side roads as opposed to scanning the whole conflict zone, and could increase false starts, particularly at signals with variable stage orders. © Association for European Transport and contributors 2005 A reduction in the starting amber period would reduce the violation rate of drivers crossing the stopline before the start of green, while still providing some of the efficiency benefits. Current driver behaviour indicates that a length of 1.5 seconds would reduce the violation rate to about 17% and a one second starting amber would virtually negate violations. However, violations are likely to slightly increase with the predicted quicker response time, and the implementation costs of any change in the UK would be significant. REFERENCES Branston, D. (1979) Some factors affecting the capacity of signalised intersections. Traffic Engineering and Control (1979-08/09) 360-6. Department for Transport, Local Government and the Regions. (2001) Transport Statistics Great Britain 2001. London: The Stationery Office. European Conference of Ministers of Transport. (1974) European Rules Concerning Road Traffic Signs and Signals. Paris: European Conference of Ministers of Transport (Vienna 1968 – Geneva 1971-73). McGill, W. A., (1970) Optimising the use of amber in traffic signals. Proceedings of 5th ARRB Conference. 5(3), pp 95-110. Newby, R.F., (1961) The effect on accident frequency at signal controlled crossroads with an all-red period. Traffic Engineering and Control. 1961, 3(2) 102-103. Older, S. J. (1960) The effect of the omission of the red amber indication at traffic signals (Leicester). Research Note RN/3702/SJO. Crowthorne: Road Research Laboratory (Unpublished). Older, S. J. (1963) Omission of the red/amber period at traffic signals. Traffic Engineering and Control (November 1963) 414-417. Pleydell, M., and Gillam, J. (2002). Yellow arrows with and without intelligence. Traffic Engineering and Control (2002, 10) 354-5. Seneviratne, J. R. V. (1974) Omission of the starting amber at traffic light signals – a before and after study. Technical Report 157, Public Works Department, Traffic and Transport Survey Division, Hong Kong. © Association for European Transport and contributors 2005