"It Happened Not Too Far from Here...": A Survey of Legend Theory and Characterization Author(s): Timothy R. Tangherlini Source: Western Folklore, Vol. 49, No. 4 (Oct., 1990), pp. 371-390 Published by: Western States Folklore Society Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1499751 . Accessed: 07/05/2013 16:09 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org. . Western States Folklore Society is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Western Folklore. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 128.97.27.21 on Tue, 7 May 2013 16:09:46 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions "It Happened Not Too Far From Here ...": A Surveyof Legend Theoryand Characterization R. TIMOTHY TANGHERLINI One of themainconcernsof folklorists studying legendhas been new and classificatory systems revisingpreviousdefinideveloping tions(Hand 1965; Bodker1965:253-261).In 1975,in an articleentitled"The Legendand theSparrow"Linda D6gh observedthefailwouldbe betterspent and proposedthatefforts ingsoftheseattempts the obstacles to a characterization of the"genus" examining deriving legend (D6gh 1975:188). Both the congressesof the International conSocietyforFolk NarrativeResearchand theseriesof Sheffield ferenceson contemporary held since have made 1982 legend great headwayin addressingtheconcernsDegh voicedin herarticle.The goals of thispaper are to providea historicalsurveyof the earlier forcurrentdiscuslegendscholarshipthathas laid thegroundwork sionsand to developa characterization of thelegendgenrebased on a synthesis of previousscholarship in a mannerthatdoes notdelimit therangeof thegenrebutratherexplainsthebasiccharacteristics of thefolklegend. JacobGrimmwas perhapsthefirstto begintheprocessof legend istpoetischer, whenhe observedthat,"Das Mairchen characterization, This die Sage historischer" characterization influ(Grimm1865:v). Western Folklore49 (October 1990): 371-390 371 This content downloaded from 128.97.27.21 on Tue, 7 May 2013 16:09:46 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 372 WESTERN FOLKLORE enced the entirecourse of legend scholarship.Earlyscholarswho on literary legendfocussedtheirefforts approachesto investigated and psythe genre,withdisregardforsocialcontext,performance to examine the in preferring legend relation chologicalmotivations, narrative the folktale. tootherfolk forms, primarily Examplesofsuch approachesmaybe foundin Karl Wehrhan's(1908) earlystudyof Peuckert's legend,as wellas FriedrichRanke's(1971) and Will-Erich on In the 1950s and works scholars 1960s, (1965) legend. began to from a broader considerations considerlegend including perspective, to the 1962 congressof the of contextand performance. Referring International SocietyforFolkNarrativeResearch,Leopold Schmidt had begun(L. Schmidt declaredthata newera in legendscholarship This Peeters new era envisioned boththe develop1969:53; 1963). mentof an international legend catalogueas well as the new apto which included and psychological anthropological proaches legend Stefaan that declared Conversely, Top perspectives. legendresearch had been plungedinto a crisis(Top 1969). He was joined in this assessmentby Lutz Rohrich(1973:13). The crisiswas mainlyone of based in thedifficulty scholarsfacedwhentheytriedto classification, Most reliedheavilyon questionsof categorizelegend. categorizations to a establish content,trying legendtypeindexsimilarto theAarneindex.Catalogsofthistypenecessarily overlooked Thompsonfolktale considerations of context and mode of performance, important being of thegenre.A synthetic based on a contentual characterization view of legend,one thatconsideredlegendfromliterary, anthropological was missing. and psychological viewpoints, oflegendare themostabunLiterary approachesto thedefinition ofthesestudiesconsideraspectsoflegendformin dant.The majority relationto othertypesof folknarrative.One of the earliestdistinctionsmadewastherelativelackof formlegendexhibitedin comparison to folktale(de Boor 1928). ROhrichfurther delineatedthe distinctionbetweenthe two formswhen he examinedtheirrelative treatment of reality(Rohrich1956:9-26). Max Luithialso considers thissamequestion(Ltithi1961).Whilethefolktaleuses realityin an ironicway,legendtriesto reconstruct realityin a believablefashion. is linked narrative to outer Legend reality, opposed to theinnerreof allusions to verifiable ality folktale,makingspecific topographic featuresor historical thecore of a personages.Also,unlikefolktale, legend narrativeconsistsof a single experience (Lathi 1961:46-48). This characteristicaccounts forthe episodic natureof legend. In most instances,the legend narrationcenterson a singleeventand is mono- This content downloaded from 128.97.27.21 on Tue, 7 May 2013 16:09:46 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions A SURVEY OF LEGEND 373 episodic.Finally,whilefolktaleconsidersman himself,legendconsiderswhathappensto man(Liithi1966).Althoughbothformsmay in orientation thisdifference leads todiffering includesimilarmotifs, of folktaleand legendmotreatments of thesemotifs.The similarity morehistorical thanfolktale tifsimpliesthatlegendis notnecessarily (Heiske 1962).As similarmotifsappear in bothnarrativeforms,the is notrelationto reality, characteristic butratherpredistinguishing of protagonists. worldviewand portrayal sentationof motifs, In thecomparisonto folktale, consideredto legendwasfrequently be formless. D6gh noticed,however,thatthe observedlackof form was more a resultof impropercollectionthan an actual aspectof legend character,legends oftenbeing truncatedby editorializing fieldworkers (D6gh 1965:84).GillianBennethas addressedtheproblemsthefieldworkerfacesin collecting legendnarrative, mentioning ... and that"thenaturallegendtellingcontextis largelyinaccessible is an induced natural difficult create" context to ... (Benparticularly of"The net 1987:16).BillEllisprovidesan analysisofa performance Hook" as an exampleof boththe problemsinherentin providinga of a legendperformance verbatimtranscript as wellas the benefits in such a transcript the of analysis legendfunction, provides noting that"itis throughanalysisof wholeperformances thatwe comprehend wholelegends"(Ellis 1987:57).Whileacknowledging thediffithe field worker encounters both and trancollection culty during he of "the ... do that texts scription, posits majority printedlegend not representlegend tellingbut ratherlegend summarizing" (Ellis 1987:34).To remedythisproblem,he suggeststhatlegendcollections becauseitis in includenotationsof thesubtleaspectsof performance theselinguistic and paralinguistic detailsthatlegendmeaningis encapsulated(Ellis 1987). In 1934,Carl Wilhelmvon Sydowdifferentiated between"memorate"and "fabulate"(von Sydow1934:261).The basic tenetof this distinction was thatfirsthand accountsof supernatural experiences are a different typeof folknarrativethansecondor thirdhand accounts.To avoid the exclusionary natureof such a distinction, von considered the of movement memorate across the border into Sydow fabulate, labeling this group of cross-overnarratives"Erinnerungssage (minnessaigen)..." (von Sydow 1934:261).1 Gunnar distinction, Granbergbuilt on von Sydow'smemorate/fabulate by equating legend with the categoryfabulate and suggestingthat leg1. Von Sydow firstuses the term"minnessigner"in 1931 (von Sydow 1931:98). This content downloaded from 128.97.27.21 on Tue, 7 May 2013 16:09:46 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 374 WESTERN FOLKLORE end, like fabulate,is a "kurze, ein-episodischeErzaihlung"(Granberg 1935:120). Legend springsfromthe imaginationof the folkand exhibitsa fixed form in tradition(Granberg 1935:121). The defining featureof memorateis the personal focus of the account, even if the narrativeis related by someone other than the person who experienced it (Granberg 1935:121). Reidar Christiansenprovided a theoretical modificationof the distinctionwhen he observed that memorate derives importance from the narrator having personally experienced the related event (Christiansen 1962:99). Fabulate, on the other hand, derivesimportancefromthe narratedevent. In 1968, Juha Pentikaiinenpresented a clear summary of the memorate/ fabulate distinction,statingthat fabulates,in contrastto memorates, exhibita more fixed form,withanonymous characters(Pentikainen 1968). D6gh and Andrew Vizsonyi successfullychallenged the memorate/ fabulatedistinction,notingthe progressiveliberalizationof the memorate definition,withfirsthand accounts being supplanted by second and thirdhand accounts (Degh and Vaizsonyi1974:226-228). At any time,a fabulate can take the formof a memorate and, more importantly,vice-versa,simplyby changing narrativevoice. A primaryreason for thischange is the tendencyto performlegend as a true narrative.A legend preceded by "A friendof a friendof a friendtold me ..." has essentiallyno credibility(Degh and Vazsonyi 1974:230-231). In his discussionof the folkbeliefstory,Otto Blehr limitsthe number of transmissionallinks to two (Blehr 1974:42). Any more transmissional links seriously compromise the believabilityof the account. During performance,the narratoris thereforemotivatedto reduce the number of transmissionallinks, possibly to the extent that he would relate it as a first-handexperience. This brings into question the definitionof tradition.In thiscontext,a narrativeshould be considered in traditionwhen original authorship is no longer verifiable and transmissionis stillactivelytaking place-this could occur in as link. Any narrativethathas ever been fewas a single transmissionary in traditioncan be said to be a traditionalnarrative.Point by point, Degh and Viazsonyidiscreditedvon Sydow's distinctionof memorate and fabulate,showingthat legend cannot be characterizedby narrative voice or other internalmanifestationsof transmissionallinks.2 2. The studyof memorateshas been supported by various scholars,and may be quite useful in understanding folk belief (Honko 1964; Klintberg 1976). Concerning the relationshipbetween memorate and legend, Klintbergmentionsthat "signen och memoratetar tvAgenrer som lever tillsamansi traditioneni en standig vaxelveskan"(af Klintberg1976:269). This content downloaded from 128.97.27.21 on Tue, 7 May 2013 16:09:46 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions A SURVEY OF LEGEND 375 Legend has also been considered in relation to rumor. Both of these narrativeformsare believable and oftenpresentedas true,with a single event formingthe core of the narrative. Ernst Bernheim touched on the similarityof legend and rumor when he suggested that legend is simplya survivalof rumor (Bernheim 1920:97-108). Gordon Allport,in his celebrated workon rumor,agreed and added "Legends persistbecause theyembody undyingstatesof mind" (Allport 1947:164). Edgar Morin studied the genesis,life and disappearance of a rumor in the French cityof Orleans, showingthat temporalityand extremelocalizationare twofactorsthat,ifnot overcomeby widespread transmissionor adaptation of the narrative to a fixed form,may cause the rumor to disappear ratherthan remain in tradition (Morin 1982). A similarstudyby Carsten Bregenhoj supports many of Morin's conclusions (Bregenhoj 1978). To account for the brief life of some narratives,WilliamJansen suggested that legends which disappear quicklybe termed "short-termlegends" and those with more staying power be termed "long-termlegends" (Jansen 1976:270). This effectivelyeliminates the rumor/legenddistinction. The only remainingdistinctionis thatwhile legend is alwaysa narrative,rumor maybe but a shortstatement.As such, rumor exhibitsan even more elastic formthan legend does. Both the legend and rumor are closelyrelated to questions of belief. Rumor, like legend, relies on popular belief as a generativeimpetus (Shibutani 1966:156). Importantto the formationof rumor is ambiguityor a lack of officialinformation,which helps precipitate accounts whichreflectboth commonlyheld beliefsand interests(Shibutani 1966:157; Allport 1947:33-36). Once a rumor no longer reflectscollectivebiases,it disappears fromtradition.Bengt af Klintberg considered this reliance on folk belief in the two formsof folk narrative (af Klintberg 1976). He points out that the category,"Urban Legend," is oftena reflectionof rumor (af Klintberg1976:270). If a narrativerumor persists,it is a legend. Thus, a more suitable characterizationis that rumor narrativeexperiences a hyperactivetransmissionin a shorttimespan, oftenwithinan extremelylocalized area. Narrativerumor is a transmissionarystate of legend-a state which, despite its intensity,does not guarantee longevity. Legend style and internal aspects of composition and structure have also been examined as a means fordescribingthe legend genre. Mathilde Hain included in her view of legend considerationsof the internal landscape and localization of the narratives (Hain 1937 the internallandscape of the legend [1969]). Through itsbelievability, This content downloaded from 128.97.27.21 on Tue, 7 May 2013 16:09:46 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 376 WESTERN FOLKLORE reinforces the believabilityof the narrative (Hain 1937[1969]:99). Peuckertpursued a similarexamination of the internallandscape of natureofthenarratives, thelegendand thehistorical that suggesting thesetwo forcescombinedresultin an accountbelievableto both narratorand audience (Peuckert1938[1969]:151-152). Siegfried Beyschlagconcursin thisevaluationof theroleofa credibleinternal worldin developingthebelievability ofthenarrative (Beyschlag1941 This world in the [1969]). distinguishes portrayed legendfromthose in and folktale portrayed myth. elementshave Legend morphologyand the studyof structural in roles the of a of view playedimportant development legendand its Axel Olrik identified laws" whichgovern composition. eighteen"epic thecomposition all of folknarrative(Olrik1908 and 1921).Johann Folkersmodifiedthisapproachspecifically to fitlegends,developing a morphological to the elementsof legend, approach compositional and attempting to establisha groupof functions servingas thegrammar for the narrative(Folkers1910). OldfichSirovtka also propounded a morphological approachin a muchlaterarticle,witha focuson legend motifsto help develop a legend index (Sirovitka 1964).Unlikefolktalemotif,legendmotifis a moreautonomousentityand subjectto greatervariation(Sirovtka 1964:101).This autonomousmotifcoupledwiththetendencyof legendnarrativetowards and contentual leads to theelasticformlegincorporation instability end exhibits.Ina-MariaGreverussuggestedthata systematic analysis of type,motifand themewouldresultin a clearerunderstanding of legend, concludingthat FriedrichHebbel's (1839) "Ideen-alfabet" shouldbe applied to the analysisof legend morphology(Greverus 1965[1969]:401).VilmosVoigtalso suggesteda morphological approachas a meansfordevelopingthegrammaroflegends,suggesting thata veryshortformulacoulddescribethesyntagmatic structure of legend (Voigt 1973:80). Pentikainenlater explored the link between and legendfunction (Pentikainen 1976). He discovlegendstructure eredthatlegendstructure differs to each according particular legend function W. F. H. Nicolaisen has also consid1976:149). (Pentikaiinen eredmorphological on aspectsoflegendstructure, focusingprimarily his on William Labov's (1972) contemporary legend. Basing study vernacularnarrative, he suggeststhatlegend studyofAfro-American narrativeconsistsat a minimumlevelof threestructural elementsorientation, complicating action, and result (Nicolaisen 1987:72). However, he allows that the legend may encompass as many as six This content downloaded from 128.97.27.21 on Tue, 7 May 2013 16:09:46 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions A SURVEY OF LEGEND 377 distinct structural elementsor as fewas two(Nicolaisen1987).Despite thepromiseof thesestructural approachesto legendstudy,none has been able toestablishanysetrulesoflegendmorphology. Partof the ofthelegend failureoftheseapproachesliesin theextremeelasticity narrativeand its highdegree of ecotypification. Also, mostof the approachesare forcedto ignoreimportantaspectsof contextand on considerations ofcontent.Howperformance, centering primarily as such Nicolaisen's and Pentikainen's do provide ever,morphologies bothmuchneeded insightintothe structural between relationships and other forms of oral narrative as well as of the minlegend maps imumstructural elementsthatmakeup a legendnarrative. has alsobeenconsideredas a basis The stylistics oflegendnarrative Andr6Jolles'influential forgenredefinition. Formen includes Einfache a shortdiscussionof legend based on its formand content(Jolles however,linkslegend to conceptsof 1958:50-74). His definition, clan and blood relations,a somewhatmisleadingviewof the family, genre.A moreproductive approachconcernslegendstyleratherthan content. Friedrich-Wilhelm Schmidtexploredthe particular legend features of stylistic legend,concludingthat,likefolktale,it is an artisticfolkexpressionwitha definite form,usinga narrativeframeto the account(F. W. Schmidt1929[1969]:64).He mentions structure thatthe legend is often"episch-dramatisch" and exhibitsa poetic that Wilhelm Wisser's observation on thenature lyricism, concluding "Mitdem Inhaltder Geschichten of folktale, istdie Formtiberliefert, die gleichsam vonselbstaus diesemhervorwachst," also holdstruefor W. Schmidt Wisser (F. 1929[1969]:64-65; 1925). Carl Herlegend mannTillhagenlatersharedthissameview(Tillhagen1967). However,a majorproblemof analyzingtheformand styleof thelegend is thelackofclearand consistent fordescribing thenarterminology ratives(Ferenczi1966).It is oftenforgotten thata definition refersto an ideal type,ratherthan being an exact representation (Honko 1968). In part,it was the breakdownin the abilityto clearlyand adequatelycharacterize legendwhichled to thefailureof theinternationallegendcatalogue.In theprocess,however,a greatdeal was discoveredaboutlegendstyleand form. and survives Legendhasan elasticform;itexpands,contracts great variation.Lee Doo Hyon mentionsthatthiselasticity is one of the ofthegenre(Lee 1983:361).Partoftheprocess uniquecharacteristics of variationmaybe linkedto von Sydow'sconceptof ecotypification (von Sydow 1932[1948]:16). Herbert Halpert observed that "each This content downloaded from 128.97.27.21 on Tue, 7 May 2013 16:09:46 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 378 WESTERN FOLKLORE geographical and culturalarea tends to ascribe supernaturallegends to its dominant supernatural figure"(Halpert 1971:50). This observation is directlyrelated to Albert Eskerod's concept of "tradition dominants" (EskerSd 1947:81). Klintbergnoticed the ethno-and sociocentricnature of Swedish contemporarylegends, suggestingthat each individual culture places its own ethnicity,conventions and norms in opposition to groups whichdo not conformand are therefore threatening(af Klintberg 1976:271 and 278). This process of can be extended to the entirelegend genre, whereby ecotypification the narrativeis variatedto fitthe needs of thecultureand itstradition. Because of the effectof social and psychologicalforces,a definition based solely on contentor formcannot possiblyadequately describe the genre. However, these literaryconsiderationsmust play a role in such a finalcharacterization. The Grimmsoriginallycharacterizedthe legend as historical.The general trend in legend research in the late nineteenthand early twentiethcenturies was to consider both the historicalcore of the legend as well as the worthof legends as sources for archaeological and historicalstudies(Cederschiold 1932). In 1888, Franz Muth commented on the connection between legend and rumor, noting the focus of both narrativetypeson historicalevents,and folkinterpretation of these events (Muth 1888). KristofferNyrop continued the reasoningthatlegends are accurate reflectionsof the past,statingthat legends provide insightto the ancient past, and could be used as a means for studying ancient history(Nyrop 1907-1933). Wilhelm CederschiWldwas perhaps more perceptive in his view of legend, proposingthatthe legend containsan historicalcore whichis an accurate recordingof an historicalevent (Cederschiold 1924). The legend narrativesurroundingthiscore may distortthe historicalcontentsof the narrative(Cederschiold 1924). The belief thatoral traditionmay be used as a historicalsource was solidifiedbyJan Vansina's workon oral traditionand historicalmethodology(Vansina 1985). Unlike earlier works,he advocated caution in dealing withthese narratives,as the historicalveracityof the accounts varies greatlyand is oftennonexistent. It was not untilthe 1970s thatthe historicity of the legend was truly into Heda in brought question. Jason, writing theJournalofAmerican that the Grimms' "historischer"definitionwas Folklore,suggested flawedand should be reconsidered (Jason 1971:134). Bjarne Hodne This content downloaded from 128.97.27.21 on Tue, 7 May 2013 16:09:46 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions A SURVEY OF LEGEND 379 continued this line of examination with a thorough analysis of the historicalvalue of legends as opposed to their entertainmentvalue (Hodne 1973). At best,legends should be used as secondaryhistorical sources, since the reliabilityof their historicalvalue is questionable. Marta ?rimkovaisupported earlier suggestionsthatlegend is a reflection of folk history,in that it records what the "folk"consider to be important,pointingout that legends relyon the connectionbetween the narrativecontentand locality,both in space and time,for their survivalin tradition(?raimkovi 1975). Rudolf Schenda concurred in the folk historyhypothesis,statingthat in legend one can find a reflectionof the socio-historicalcontext one needs to understand the historyof sixtyto eightypercentof the population ignored by official history(Schenda 1982:186). While legend may appear to be a historicalnarrativebecause of referenceto readilyverifiabletopographic and geographic features, as well as a reliance on culturallycredible characters,legend has been misinterpretedas a fundamentally"true" narrative.Simplybecause it is often performedas "true," the narrativecontentitselfis not necessarily an actual reflectionof historical events. Studies such as Hodne's have shown thatthe historiccore of the legend is oftenhard to find, and then of dubious historicalvalue when finallyisolated. narrative.The process Legends are bettercharacterizedas historicized of historicizationmay be likened to diachronic ecotypification.The believabilityof the narrativeis underscored by the historicizationof the account. Connected to the historicizednature of legend is the relationbetween legend and folk belief. At the root of this relationshipis the "trueness"of the legend narrative.Legend may be characterizedas a reflectionof folk belief: commonly held values and beliefs in the communityin which a given legend exists (Domiotr 1977). Peuckert examined the relationshipbetween legend and belief, although he also viewed legend as a primarilyhistoricalnarrative(Peuckert 1965). ROhrichpresenteda more balanced view of the relationshipbetween legend and belief, centering his study on the demonic and magic (Rbhrich 1949). He suggeststhatlegend narrativeboth reiteratesand reinforcesbelief. Blehr expanded on RJOhrich's hypothesis,using accounts of both legends and beliefs to illustratetheir symbioticrelationshipin tradition(Blehr 1974). Legend and folkbelief,especially concerningthe supernatural,reinforceeach other,neitherbeing uni- This content downloaded from 128.97.27.21 on Tue, 7 May 2013 16:09:46 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 380 WESTERN FOLKLORE laterally dependenton theother.In tradition, legendderivespartof fromthefolkbeliefsit reflects, whilefolkbeliefsare itsbelievability narratives. supportedbylegend of legend,the belieflegend,was proposedas a A subcategory meansforcategorizing legendswhichdepend largelyon folkbelief. These legendsor folkbeliefstoriesoftenrelateencounterswiththe and are toldwithfirstor secondhand authority (Blehr supernatural how has shown 1965:32-47). Degh contemporary legendsincorporate beliefsabout the supernaturalin a mannerconsistentwitha worldview,ratherthan discardthosebeliefs(Degh contemporary It is 1971). possiblethatlegendswhichare noteasilyrecognizedas folkbeliefsmakeuse of thesebeliefsin a moresubtle incorporating way.Whiletheterm"folkbelief"has been limitedto beliefsconcernan analysisof leging supernaturalphenomenain pastscholarship, of tabooand genend based on beliefshouldincludeconsiderations eralizedbelief(Mullen1971).A broadviewofthisnatureis necessary to understandhow legend derivesbelievability by tappingalready a symbolicreality establishedbeliefsand values. By constructing whichencompassesthesevaluesand beliefs,thelegendnotonlymainin tradition, tainsitsvitality but also reinforces thoseverybeliefsit makesuse of.As thecomplexforcesat workon societychangebeliefs in the legendnarrative(Degh and values,the changesare reflected the its narrative loses Otherwise, 1971). viability. The psychological studyoflegendis a longneglectedfield(Dundes of leg1971). An earlypsychological approachto theunderstanding end waspresentedbyGerhardGesemannin whichhe consideredthe in legendnarraquestionof sociologicaland psychological continuity the formal of his tive,abandoning (Geseapproach contemporaries on the Alpine mann 1928). GotthilfIsler,in his 1971 dissertation is itself legend"die Sennenpuppe,"suggeststhatthelegendnarrative not the narratedexperience,but servesa deeper religiousfunction of eventscombinedwiththearche(Isler 1971:247).The singularity unconsciousundoubtedly accountforthe typesfoundinthecollective episodicnatureofthelegend(Isler 1973).Experiencescombinewith of theunconsciousand elementsof folkbeliefin thegenarchetypes esis of legend (Isler 1971:247). Legend,accordingto Isler,reflects, "die aktuellen Tendenzen des kollektivenUnbewuBten"(Isler oflegendsuggeststhatlegendis 1971:251).ThisJungianperspective a symbolic of universalsmoldedbycollectiveexperiences. reflection This content downloaded from 128.97.27.21 on Tue, 7 May 2013 16:09:46 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions A SURVEY OF LEGEND 381 The immediate reason a person tells or remembersa legend well enough to perpetuate it is psychological(Crane 1977). BeverlyCrane concentratedon the legend cycle of "The Roommate's Death," and suggested thatit persistsbecause of "itsabilityto organize a complex set of environmentalfactors,whichappear to be contributinganxiety and tension to the lives of the individuals concerned" (Crane 1977:147). Gary Alan Fine, in his discussionof "urban" legends, suggests that it is the socio-psychologicalsituation of the present-day whichimbues contemporarylegends withtheirurbanness(1980:223). Since legends are intended to be believable and believed, theyact as an unconsciousfictionizing (Ranke 1971:202-203). Legend addresses real psychologicalproblemsassociated withthe geographic and social environments,actingas a reflectionof commonlyfeltpressures.However,it is not onlyfearswhichare addressed but also desires. Much of folk narrativeis the human fantasyengaging in wishfulthinking. Legend, thus, acts as a symbolicrepresentationof collectiveexperiences and beliefs, expressing fears and desires associated with the common environmentaland social factorsaffectingboth the active and passive traditionbearers. Contemporarylegends, referredto as "urban" or "modern" legends, have become a major focus of those interestedin legend psychology.The term"modern urban legend" received broad exposure as a result of Jan Harold Brunvand's popular collections of these "urban belief tales" (Brunvand 1981, 1984, 1986 and 1989). In contrast,Klintberg,in his collectionof Scandinavian variantsof the legends, refersto them as "folksaigner" (af Klintberg1986). The debate over the suitabilityof the term,whichsuggeststhatthese legends are both modern, and thereforedivorced fromearlier tradition,and urban, and thereforedifferentfromtheirrural counterparts,has been fought out in journals throughoutthe world. One camp views the "modern urban" legend as a distinctgenre, with Daniel Barnes assertingthat "[the] everpresentimpulse to redefinethe roles of dramatispersonae-of villains,victim,hero- . .. finallymakes the urban legend ... distinctlydifferentfrom most other formsof traditional narrative"(Barnes 1984:77). Fine also defends the use of the term, assertingthat "urban" is not a geographic signifierbut rathera referent to a socio-psychologicalcondition (Fine 1980). Georgina Smith also viewsthe genre as unique, citingthe mode of performanceas the definingfeature,viewing"modern urban legend" as a narrativeform This content downloaded from 128.97.27.21 on Tue, 7 May 2013 16:09:46 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 382 WESTERN FOLKLORE detachment whichis oftentoldwitha certaindegreeof narratorial (Smith1981:171; 1979:41). On theotherend of thespectrumone findsthecampwhichviews and an unnectheterm"modernurbanlegend"as botha misnomer of the to folklore attachment genre alreadybulkybaggage essary This grouppreferstheappellation"contemporary legterminology. thenarratives end,"thereby accordingto theirpresentstate situating in tradition. Anylegendviablein traditionmaybe viewedas a conlegend.The Sheffieldconferencesmentionedabove are temporary on Contemporary Legend,revealpublished under the rubricPerspectives ing the generalorientationof thesescholarsin thisterminological debate(Nicolaisen1985:214).Noel Williamshas triedto characterize thecontemporary legend,notingthat"whatwe mayregardas a disfromtraditional tinctgenreis actuallynotdistinct legend,exceptby in thecultureof itstransas participants virtueof our ownattitudes of mission"(Williams1984:228).Nicolaisenstraddlestheviewpoints that"modernurbanlegend"be conthetwocampswell,suggesting elementsnotonlyoflegendbut sidereda "blurred"genre,containing also of the personalexperiencestory,a view also held by Bennet (Nicolaisen1985:216; Bennet 1985:222). Nevertheless, Jacqueline is a "clever transforthat legend contemporary Simpson'sproposal mation of ... old rural supernatural motif[s]into modern, urban rationalized deservesconsideration form[s]," (Simpson1981:203).In a well knownstudy,ShirleyMarchalonisprovidedthreemedieval legend,"Spidersin thehair," analoguesto a commoncontemporary of in legendtradition a exists that level even continuity high proving over greatlengthsof time(Marchalonis1976). Bennet,in a similar betweencontemporary study,showeda highlevelofcontinuity "Reptilein thestomach"legendsand earliervariants(Bennet1985).These two studiesconfirmthe process of historicization or diachronic above. mentioned ecotypification UlrikaWolf-Knuts providesthe mostconvincingrebuttalof the term"modernurbanlegend,"proposingthattheselegendsbe consideredinsteadas migratory legends(Wolf-Knuts1987). She comof definition "modernurbanlegend"to Klintberg's Brunvand's pares of notes "bothformpartof a definition legendand thesimilarities: collective tradition, theyare bothrelatedin a seriousmannerinorder to providemoreor less authenticinformation, and theyare spread orallyor by mass communication"(Wolf-Knuts1987:173). In suggesting the use of the termmigratorylegend, she pointsout that"modern This content downloaded from 128.97.27.21 on Tue, 7 May 2013 16:09:46 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions A SURVEY OF LEGEND 383 fromgeneration urbanlegendand migratory legendspreadvertically forhundredsofyearsand horizontally fromcountry to to generation wide areas" over (Wolf-Knuts 1987:178). country very geographical Ratherthanviewing"modernurbanlegend"as an isolatedexpression of a particularage, thesescholarschooseto viewthe contemporary elimByeffectively legendtradition. legendas partofthecontinuing the distinctions that "modern urban leginating proposed separate characterization end" fromlegendin general,a broad,encompassing in turn,should ofthegenreis closerat hand.Sucha characterization, in the in function of the largersocietal legend help understanding of narrative context.Understanding thefunction tradition as partof themacro-context has been labeledby Fineas thegoal of the"third forcein AmericanFolklore"(Fine 1988:352-353). The studyofcontextand socialfunction oflegendperformance is anothercriticalbutoftenneglectedaspectofthelegendgenre(Abraof conhams 1975). Arnoldvan Genneprecognizedtheimportance ' textin defining legend:"De nosjours on se demande:oit,quand et (vanGennep1912:306).He qui se raconteune historied6terminee?" also examinedthesocialfunctionand internalpsychological aspects of the narratives, as a meansforunderstanding contextforlegend genesis(van Gennep 1912:305-306).FriedrichRanke'searlydefinitionof legend was not based solelyon formand content,but also includeda consideration of performance: "VolkssagensindvolklauInhalts,der figeErz~ihlungen objektivunwahren,phantasiegeboren als tatsaichliches Geschehnisin der Formdes einfachenEreignisberichteserza*hlt wird"(Ranke 1925[1969]:4).D6gh voiced the comweretoo literarily based,and noplaintthatmostlegenddefinitions ticedthatmostlegendshad beencollectedimproperly, lackingcritical contextualinformation (D6gh 1965:78). Legendshave been printed as continuous,cohesivenarratives. In context,legendis not a neat, narrative compartmentalized performedfor a captive audience. beRather,duringperformance, legendexhibitsno predetermined infitsand starts, ginningor end,butprogresses interrupted byothers' observations or linkedtojokes,and oftenservingrhetorical purposes 1965 and 1976). In a studywhere D6gh examinedlegend (D6gh ina Hungarianemigrant hertranscriptions performance community, an view of how various reveal interesting conversational narrative formscommingleduringtransmission, the relationbeparticularly tweenjoke and legend (D~gh 1976:109). GaryButler'sstudyof Terreneuvienne legends supports the assertion that legend is a conversa- This content downloaded from 128.97.27.21 on Tue, 7 May 2013 16:09:46 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 384 WESTERN FOLKLORE tional genre (1980). During legend performance,the boundary of narratorand audience blurs, transmissiontaking place interactively. The conversationalnature of legend, in turn,adds to the believability beof the narrativeand its functionas a mechanismfor reaffirming liefssince the narrativeis not set offby any distancingformula. The proposal of an internationallegend catalogue, similarin scope to the Aarne-Thompson index, forcedthe crisisin legend researchof the 1960s (Hand 1965:441-443). One of the mostdifficultaspects of classifyinglegend was the abundance of specificallusions relevantor understandable to only relativelysmall groups of people (Tillhagen 1969). Also, it was discovered that,due to the mono-episodic nature of legend, it spanned a nearlyinfinitenumber of motifs,precluding classificationaccording only to content.Systemssuch as Julian Krzyzanoski's,whichsuggesteda divisionof legend into threemain classes, and belief,failed due to the lack of namely religious,historical/local any clear distinctionbetween these categories (Krzyianoski 1967; Hand 1965:444). C. Scott Littleton'smulti-dimensional,synchronic and diachroniccategorizationof folknarrativedid littleto help clarify since legend spanned all of his categories(Litlegend's characteristics, tleton 1965). Jason also suggested a multi-dimensionalapproach to oral literatureas a whole, plottingwhat she calls "determinants"onto the "oral literaturespace," withlittlesuccess (Jason 1969). However, the point thatlegend mustbe classifiedon multiplecriteriais one well taken.Vibeke Dahll, in her criticalanalysisof Nordic legend catalogue systems,concurred withthis assertion(Dahll 1973:182). A thorough analysis of properlycollected legends and a synthesisof earlier theoretical approaches to legend could lead to a more fundamentalunderstandingof legend characteristicson whichone could base a classificatorysystem (Sirovaitka1964). Sirovaitkaproposes the use of of a givenlegend; one could computersto help plot the characteristics (Sirovitka 1964). The need fora therebyclassifymulti-dimensionally classificationsystemis certainlyan importantissue. Tillhagen, however, made the most importantobservationon legend classification when he statedthata legend catalogue is a tool forthe studyof legend and should not be considered an end in itself(Tillhagen 1969:318). Scholars' effortsmay best be spent exploring questions of performance, motivation,functionand structureratherthan attemptingto pigeonhole the vast numbers of collected variants.Through continued studies, the most suitable classificatorysystemwill undoubtedly presentitself.The earlyattemptsat legend classificationhave brought This content downloaded from 128.97.27.21 on Tue, 7 May 2013 16:09:46 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions A SURVEY OF LEGEND 385 to light the often localized and highlyecotypifiednature of legend. Each traditioninto which a legend moves exerts a culturalinfluence on the narrative.Because legend is a reflectionof culturallybased values and beliefs,the ecotypificationprocess becomes exaggerated, problematizingthe classificationprocess. RobertGeorges, in his opening address to the American Folk Legend Symposium,suggeststhat a new definitionbased on the "nature and structureof the sets of relationshipsthat underlie" legend be developed (1971:18). Such a definitionwould have tOinclude considerations of all characteristicsof the genre, fusingthe approaches of earlier scholarship.Althougha great deal has been discovered about the underlying relationships-textual, psychological and sociological-which affectthe formand functionof legends, these discoveries have not been synthesizedintoa concise and concretecharacterization of the genre. The above surveyof legend scholarship bespeaks the need for such a syntheticcharacterization.Distillingthe main points of the major theoreticaladvances considered above providesone with the beginningsof such a characterization.Legend, typically,is a short (mono-) episodic, traditional,highlyecotypified,historicizednarrativeperformedin a conversationalmode, reflectingon a psychological level a symbolicrepresentationof folk belief and collectiveexperiences and servingas a reaffirmation of commonlyheld values of the to whose tradition it The group belongs. promisingwork on contemhas revived a field that porarylegend onlytwentyyearsago had been considered to have reached an impasse-a scholarlycrisis of paramount proportions. With this survey I have hoped to provide an overview of the main trends which have led to current debates in legend study,as well as suggestareas forcontinued study.Collection efforts,classificationsystemsand analyses of legends and legend cycles which consider the legend from multiple perspectiveswill undoubtedly help our understandingof the functionof this complex folk narrativegenre in the greater context of society.The renewed vigor in legend scholarship bespeaks the emergence of yet another "new era" in legend research. University of California Berkeley, California This content downloaded from 128.97.27.21 on Tue, 7 May 2013 16:09:46 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 386 WESTERN FOLKLORE ReferencesCited StudiaFennica Abrahams,Roger D. 1975. GenreTheoryand Folkloristics. 20:13-19. Allport,Gordon. 1947. The Psychology of Rumor.New York: Holt. UrbanLegends.Arv40:67-78. Barnes,Daniel R. 1984. Interpreting Bennet,Gillian.1985.What's"Modern"AbouttheModernLegend?Fabula 26:219-229. Urban LegBennet,Gillian.1987. Problemsin Collectingand Classifying ends: A Personal Experience. In Perspectives on Contemporary Legend2, ed. GillianBennet,Paul Smith,and D. D. A. Widdowson,pp. 15-30. Sheffield:CECTAL. Berlin: Walter Bernheim, Ernst. 1920. Einleitungin derGeschichtswissenschaft. de Gruyter. Beyschlag, Siegfried.1969.WeltbildderVolkssage.In Vergleichende Sagenfored. schung, LeanderPetzoldt,pp. 189-216. Darmstadt:Wissenschaftliche Buchgesellschaft. Oslo: Blehr, Otto. 1965. Noen synspunkter pd analysenav folketrofortellingen. Universitetets Museum. Etnografiske Oslo: Universitetsforlaget. Blehr,Otto. 1974.Folketro ogsagnforskning. Boor, Helmut de. 1928. Mairchenforschung.Zeitschrift fiir Deutschkunde 42:561-581. 8: appelsinerog folkesagn. Tradisjon Bregenhoj,Carsten.1978.Terrorisme, 65-78. Hitchhiker: AmericanUrbanLegends Brunvand,Jan Harold. 1981. TheVanishing and TheirMeanings. New York:W.W. Nortonand Co. Brunvand,Jan Harold. 1984. The ChokingDoberman:And Other"New" Urban New York:Norton. Legends. Brunvand,Jan Harold. 1986. TheMexicanPet: More "New" UrbanLegendsand SomeOld Favorites.New York: Norton. Brunvand,Jan Harold. 1989. Curses!BroiledAgain: TheHottestUrbanLegends Going.New York:Norton. Truthand Beliefin theLegend Butler,GaryR. 1980.Participant Interaction, Process. Cultureand Tradition5:67-78. of Bodker,Laurits.1965.Folkliterature (Germanic). (International Dictionary and Folklore Rosenkilde 2.) RegionalEuropeanEthnology Copenhagen: og Bagger. Cederschiold, Wilhelm.1924.Saigenochsanning.Nordisk tidskriftfor vetenskap, konstoch industri1924:449-466. Wilhelm.1932.Sant ochosantifolksdignerna. Stockholm: WahlCederschiold, stromoch Widstrand. ReidarTh. 1962.Fabulatochmemorat Christiansen, (Innledning).In Nordisk Seminar i Folkedigtning I, ed. LauritsBodker,pp. 86-106. NordiskInstitut forFolkedigtning 2. Copenhagen:Rosenkildeog Bagger. ofValue in 'The Roommate's Crane,Beverly.1977.The Structure Death':A forInterpretive of FolkLegends.Journal Methodology Analysis oftheFolkloreInstitute14:133-151. This content downloaded from 128.97.27.21 on Tue, 7 May 2013 16:09:46 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions A SURVEY OF LEGEND 387 Dahll, Vibeke. 1972. Nordiskesagnregistre (Nordisk Institut og sagn systematik. forFolkdigtning 2.) Copenhagen:NIF. 22:77-87. Laographia D6gh,Linda. 1965. Processesof LegendFormation. in The Modern Function Linda. 'Belief 1971. Form, Society: D6gh, Legend' A Symposium, ed. and Relationshipto Other Genres. In American Folklegend: oftheUCLA Centerforthe WaylandDebs Hand,pp. 55-68. (Publications of Berkeley:University Studyof ComparativeFolkloreand Mythology.) CaliforniaPress. 76:177-189. D6gh,Linda. 1975.The Legendand theSparrow.Volkskunde ofJokeand Legend:A Case of Conversational D4gh,Linda. 1976.Symbiosis Folklore. In FolkloreToday:A Festschrift forRichardDorson,ed. Linda D6gh, Press. IndianaUniversity pp. 101-122. Bloomington: 1974. The Memorateand the ProtoD6gh,Linda, and AndrewVAzsonyi. Memorate.JournalofAmericanFolklore87:225-239. 88:529-539. Domotor,Tekla. 1977.A tipizilasa nepmondaban. Ethnographia FolkLegend: of Legend.In American Dundes,Alan. 1971.On thePsychology ed. WaylandDebs Hand, pp. 21-36. (Publications of the A Symposium, UCLA Centerforthe Studyof ComparativeFolkloreand Mythology.) of CaliforniaPress. Berkeley:University of LegendsNecessary?In Ellis,Bill. 1987. WhyAre VerbatimTranscripts on Contemporary Legend2, ed. Gillian Bennet, Paul Smith,and Perspectives D. D. A. Widdowson,pp. 31-60. CECTAL ConferencePapersSeries5. Sheffield: CECTAL. studieri skirdensochjulenstrooch Eskero6d,Albert. 1947. Aretsiring.Etnologiska sed.(Nordiskamuseetshandlingar26.) Stockholm:Nordiskamuseet. 6s mfifajkrit&riumok. Ferenczi,Imri. 1966. Mondateminol6giak Neprajz6s 10:5-18. Nyelvtudomdny FriedRat: Legendsand ModernSoFine,GaryAlan. 1980.The Kentucky ciety.JournaloftheFolkloreInstitute17:222-243. Fine,GaryAlan. 1988.The ThirdForcein AmericanFolklore:FolkNarraFabula29:342- 353. tivesand SocialStructures. 1910. Ulrich. ZurStilkritik derVolkssage. Dissertation: Folkers, Kiel. Johann desldgendes. Paris:E. Flammarion. Gennep,Arnoldvan. 1912.Laformation Georges,RobertA. 1971.The GeneralConceptof Legend:Some Assump- tions to be reexamined and Reassessed. In AmericanFolklegend:A Sympo- of theUCLA Censium,ed. WaylandDebs Hand,pp. 1-20. (Publications ter for the Studyof ComparativeFolkloreand Mythology.) Berkeley: of CaliforniaPress. University ZusammenGesemann,Gerhard.1928. Soziologischeund psychologische 4:19-43. haingein der Sagenforschung.Zeitschriftfiir V61kerpsychologie GesichtsGunnar.1935.Memoratund Sage. Einigemethodische Granberg, punkte.Saga ochSed 1935:120-127. Greverus,Ina-Maria.1969. Thema, Typus und Motiv.Zur Determination ed. Leander Petder Erzaihlforschung.In Vergleichende Sagenforschung, zoldt,pp. 390-401. Darmstadt:Wissenschaftliche Buchgesellschaft. Grimm, Sagen.Berlin:NicoJacobLudwigKarl. 1905(4thedition).Deutsche laischeVerlag. This content downloaded from 128.97.27.21 on Tue, 7 May 2013 16:09:46 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 388 WESTERN FOLKLORE Hain, Mathilde. 1969. Volkssage und Sagenlandschaft.In Vergleichende Sagened. Leander Petzoldt,pp. 99-107. Darmstadt:Wissenschaftliche forschung, Buchgesellschaft. Halpert, Herbert. 1971. Definitionand Variationin Folk Legend. In American A Symposium, ed. Wayland Debs Hand, pp. 47-54. (Publications Folklegend: of the UCLA Center for the Study of Comparative Folklore and Mythology.) Berkeley: Universityof California Press. Hand, Wayland Debs. 1965. Status of European and American Legend 6:439-446. Study. CurrentAnthropology Heiske, Wilhelm. 1962. Das Marchen ist poetischer,die Sage historischer. Versuch einer Kritik.Deutschunterricht 14:69-75. Oslo: UniHodne, Bjarne. 1973. Personalhistoriske sagn. En studiei kildeverdi. versitetsforlaget. Honko, Lauri. 1964. Memoratesand the Study of Folk Beliefs.Journalofthe FolkloreInstitute1:5-19. Honko, Lauri. 1968. Genre Analysisin Folkloristicsand Comparative Religion. Temenos3:48-66. Eine Untersuchung Isler, Gotthilf.1971. Die Sennenpuppe. iiberdie religioseAlder Schweizerischen Gesellschaft fur Volkskunde.) (Schriften pensagen. Basel: Verlag G. Krebs. der Isler, Gotthilf.1973. Tiefenpsychologieund Sagenforschung.In Probleme ed. Lutz R6hrich, pp. 149-164. Freiburg im Breisgau: Sagenforschung, ForschungsstelleSage. Jansen,William Hugh. 1976. Legend: Oral Tradition in the Modern Experience. In FolkloreToday:A Festschrift for RichardDorson,ed. Linda D6gh, pp. 265-272. Bloomington: Indiana UniversityPress. Jason, Heda. 1969. A Multi-DimensionalApproach to Oral Literature.CurrentAnthropology 10:413-426. Jason, Heda. 1971. Concerning the 'Historical' and the 'Local' Legends and their Relatives.JournalofAmericanFolklore84:134-144. Jolles,Andr&. 1958 [1929]. EinfacheFormen.Tuibingen:Max Niemayer. Klintberg,Bengt af. 1976. Folksigner i dag. Fataburen1976:269-296. Klintberg,Bengt af. 1986. Rdttani pizzan. Folksagneri vdr tid. Stockholm: Norstedt. Krzyianowsky,Julian. 1967. Legend in Literature and Folklore. Fabula 9:111-117. Labov, William. 1972. Language in theInner City.Studiesin theBlack English Vernacular.(Conduct and Communication3.) Philadelphia: Universityof PennsylvaniaPress. Lee, Doo Hyon, Chang Ju Kun, and I Kwang Kyu. 1983. Hanguk Minsok Hakkaesol.Seoul: Hak Yon Sa. Littleton,C. Scott. 1965. A Two-Dimensional Scheme forthe Classificationof Narratives.JournalofAmericanFolklore78:21-27. und Volkssage. Zwei Grundformen erzdhLuthi, Max. 1961 [1966]. Volksmiirchen lenderDichtung.Bern: Francke Verlag. Luthi, Max. 1966. Aspekte des Marchens und der Sage. GermanischRomanische 16:337-350. Monatsschrift This content downloaded from 128.97.27.21 on Tue, 7 May 2013 16:09:46 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions A SURVEY OF LEGEND 389 Marchalonis,Shirley. 1976. Three Medieval Tales and Their Modern Analogues. Journalof theFolkloreInstitute13:173-184. Morin, Edgar. 1982. La Rumeurd'Orlians.Paris: Ed. du Sevil. Mullen, PatrickB. 1971. The Relationshipof Legend and Folk Belief.Journal ofAmericanFolklore84:406-413. Studie. Muth, Franz Alfred. 1888. Die deutscheSage. Eine litteraturhistorische BroschOren Frankfurt: von A. (Frankfurterzeitgemasse IX:11.) Verlag Foesser Nachfolger. Nicolaisen, W. F. H. 1985. Perspectiveson Contemporary Legend. Fabula 26:213-218. Nicolaisen, W. F. H. 1987. The LinguisticStructureof Legends. In Perspectiveson Contemporary Legend2, ed. Gillian Bennet, Paul Smith,and D. D. A. Widdowson, pp. 61-76. CECTAL Conference Papers Series 5. Sheffield: CECTAL. Nyrop,Kristoffer.1907-1933. Fortidssagn ogsange. Copenhagen: Gyldendal. Olrik, Axel. 1908. Episke love i folkedigtningen.DanskeStudier1908:69-89. Olrik, Axel. 1921. Nogle grundsaetninger (Danmarks folkefor sagnforskning. minder 23.) Copenhagen: Det Schonbergskeforlag. Peeters, K. C., ed. 1963. Tagung der 'International Societyfor Folk Narrative Research'in Antwerp (6-8 Sept.,1962). Berichtund Referate.Antwerp: Centrumvoor studie en documentatie. Pentikiinen,Juha. 1968. Grenzprobleme zwischen Memorat und Sage. Temenos3:136-167. Pentikaiinen,Juha. 1976. Signens struktur och funktion. Fataburen 1976:141-154. Peuckert,Will-Erich.1965. Sagen. Munich: E. Schmidt. Peuckert, Will-Erich. 1969. Die Welt der Sage. In Vergleichende Sagened. Leander Petzoldt,pp. 135-188. Darmstadt: Wissenschaftliforschung, che Buchgesellschaft. Ranke, Friedrich. 1969. Grundfragender Volkssagenforschung.In VergleichendeSagenforschung, ed. Leander Petzoldt,pp. 1-20. Darmstadt:WissenschaftlicheBuchgesellschaft. ed. Heinz Rank, Friedrich. 1971. Sage und Mairchen.In KleinereSchriften, Rupp and Edvard Studer, pp. 189-203. Bern: Francke Verlag. 25:251Rohrich,Lutz. 1949. Sage und Brauch. Forschungen und Fortschritte 254. Eine volkskundliche UntersuR6hrich, Lutz. 1956. Marchenund Wirklichkeit. chung.Weisbaden: Franz Steiner Verlag. ROhrich,Lutz. 1973. Was soil und kann Sagenforschungleisten?Einige aktuelle Probleme unseres Faches. In Problemeder Sagenforschung, ed. Lutz Rohrich,pp. 13-33. Freiburgim Breisgau: ForschungsstelleSage. Schenda, Rudolf. 1982. Remarques sur le contenu socio-historiquedes r&cits 16gendaires.Le MondeAlpinet Rhodanien10:185-188. Schmidt,Friedrich-Wilhelm.1929. Die Volkssage als Kunstverk.Niederdeutsche Zeitschrift 7, 129-143, 230-244. Reprinted (1969) in fiir Volkskunde ed. Vergleichende Sagenforschung, Leander Petzoldt,pp. 21-65. Darmstadt: WissenschaftlicheBuchgesellschaft. This content downloaded from 128.97.27.21 on Tue, 7 May 2013 16:09:46 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 390 WESTERN FOLKLORE Schmidt, Osterreich Leopold.1969.VoreinenneuenAraderSagenforschung. Volkskunde 29:53-74. Zeitschriftfiir News.A SociologicalStudyofRumor.InShibutani,Tamotsu. 1966. Improvised dianapolis:Bobbs-Merill. Simpson,Jacqueline.1981. RationalizedMotifsin Urban Legends.Folklore 92:203-207. Sirovtka, Oldfich. 1964. Zur Morphologieder Sage und Sagenkatalogi13:99-106. sierung. ActaEthnographica Genre. Smith,Georgina.1979.Aspectsof UrbanLegendas a Performance Loreand Language 2(10):41-44. Smith,Georgina.1981. UrbanLegend,PersonalExperienceNarrativeand Oral History:Literaland SocialTruthin Performance. Arv,37:167-173. lidov6mpopov6stiv sourasndm rmkovi, Marta.1975.Mistnia historick6 vistnikmoravskj27:62-70. VlastivAdny dnin.C. W. von. 1931.Om folkets Nordisk Kultur9:96-112. Sydow, saigner. Scandia5. Reprinted as "On Sydow,C. W. von.1932.Om traditionsspridning. theSpread of Tradition"in von Sydow.1948. Selected Paperson Folklore, pp. 11-43. Copenhagen:Rosenkildeand Bagger. der Prosa-Volksdichtung. In VolkskundliSydow,C. W. von. 1934.Kategorien che Gaben. Festschrift ed. Erich SeJ. Meierzum 70. Geburtstag dargebracht, mannand HarrySchewe,pp. 253-268. Berlin:W. de Gruyter. International. Tillhagen,C. H. 1967.Die Sage als Dichtung.In Folklore Essays in traditional literature, beliefand customin honorof WaylandDebs Hand, ed. D. K. Wilgus,pp. 211-220. Hatboro:FolkloreAssociatesInc. und ein Vorschlag Tillhagen,C. H. 1969.Was isteine Sage? Eine Definition ed. Lefir ein europaisches Sagensystem.In Vergleichende Sagenforschung, ander Petzoldt,pp. 307-318. Darmstadt:Wissenschaftliche Buchgesellschaft. Anno 1969. Vernieutwing of stagTop, Stefaan.1969. Sagenproblematiek natie.Volkskunde 70:123-165. as History. Madison:University of WisconVansina,Jan. 1985.OralTradition sin Press. der Sagen. Voigt,Vilmos.1973.Die strukturell-morphologisch Erforschung In Probleme derSagenforschung, ed. Lutz R6hrich,pp. 66-85. Freiburgim Breisgau:Forschungsstelle Sage. Wehrhan,Karl. 1908.Die Sage.Leipzig:W. Heims. Noel. 1984.Problemsin DefiningContemporary Williams, Legends.In Perspectiveson Contemporary Legend,ed. Paul Smith, pp. 216-228. CECTAL ConferencePapersSeries4. Sheffield: CECTAL. imVolksmund. Wisser,Wilhelm.1925.Das Miirchen VerHamburg:Quickborn lag. Ulrika.1987. ModernUrbanLegendsSeen as Migratory Wolf-Knuts, Legends.Arv43:167-179. This content downloaded from 128.97.27.21 on Tue, 7 May 2013 16:09:46 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions