COMM 792: Persuasion (3 credits) M 4:00pm–6:40pm, 209 COM School of Communication San Diego State University Rachael Record, PhD Assistant Professor rrecord@mail.sdsu.edu Office: 203 Communication Building Office hours: T/R 12:30pm-2:00pm or by appointment Course Website: http://blackboard.sdsu.edu (Blackboard) ABOUT THIS COURSE Persuasion is the act of influencing someone to change their knowledge, attitudes, and/or behavior. To thoroughly learn the communication field of persuasion this course will be split into three sections: theories, influences, and contexts. The first half of the semester will be focused on theoretical perspectives of persuasion and communication influences on the persuasion process. We will examine three classifications of models of persuasion. Psychological models of persuasion involve understanding the cognitive factors that may lead an individual to behave a certain way. Social models of persuasion focus on how an individual’s relationships to other people or groups may influence her or him. Cultural and Environmental models of persuasion examine how the broader world in which an individual lives affect her or his thoughts and behavior. Together, these three classifications of persuasion models offer a comprehensive way of understanding the nature of human behavior and how it can potentially be affected by communicative messages. The second half of the semester will focus on applied persuasion research across the contexts of rhetorical, interpersonal, , mediated, health, and dark side communication. Specific learning outcomes for this course are as follows: (1) explain the key features of primary theoretical frameworks in the field of persuasion, (2) identify advantages and disadvantages to persuasive theories, (3) identify which theories of persuasion are being used when given examples of persuasive communication, (4) identify what persuasion theories are useful for different types of behavior change, (5) provide examples of research demonstrating support for persuasive effects, and (6) discuss appropriate methodological approaches for investigating persuasive effects across communication contexts. REQUIRED MATERIALS 1. O’Keefe, D. J. (2016). Persuasion: Theory and research (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 2. All other assigned readings available as a PDF on BlackBoard COURSE POLICIES Class Conduct First and foremost, I do not want to see your cell phone during class time ever. If your phone is out then you are not mentally here and it will count as absent. We will have fun this semester, and there will be a great deal of give and take in our discussions. But we will 1 only have fun if you conduct yourself with respect for yourself and others. This means you are to 1) come to class prepared (do all reading and come prepared to discuss it; do all homework) and take pride in the work you do, 2) offer support and encouragement to your classmates, 3) listen to others carefully before offering your opinion, and 4) talk to your instructor outside of class if anything that happens during class bothers you. Your active participation in the learning process is expected and valued. If a student consistently exhibits behavior that disrupts the class or contributes to a negative communication climate, action will be taken, including forced withdrawal from the course. In addition all university students are expected to abide by the Student Code of Conduct. Blackboard Policy Blackboard will be the main page for our course. I recommend that you check the announcements section before every class. I am responsive to student requests for changes in the schedule, which means that the daily schedule may change during the semester. You will be responsible for checking the online syllabus and schedule before beginning your homework for each of our class meetings for any changes or updates. I will post all assignments on Blackboard. Email Policy Please use email as a primary means of contact. Allow 24 hours response time. Use basic courtesy and proper grammar when emailing. Academic Integrity Plagiarism is one of the highest forms of academic offense. The University adheres to a strict policy regarding cheating and plagiarism. These activities will not be tolerated in this class (i.e. at the 400-level I do not give second chances regarding issues of plagiarism). Become familiar with the policy (http://www.sa.sdsu.edu/srr/conduct1.html). Any cheating or plagiarism will result in failing this class and a disciplinary review by Student Affairs. The Academic Dishonesty Policy of the School of Communication Plagiarism is theft of intellectual property. It is one of the highest forms of academic offense because in academe, it is a scholar’s words, ideas, and creative products that are the primary measures of identity and achievement. Whether by ignorance, accident, or intent, theft is still theft, and misrepresentation is still misrepresentation. Therefore, the offense is still serious, and is treated as such. Overview: In any case in which a Professor or Instructor identifies evidence for charging a student with violation of academic conduct standards or plagiarism, the presumption will be with that instructor’s determination. However, the faculty/instructor(s) will confer with the director to substantiate the evidence. Once confirmed, the evidence will be reviewed with the student. If, following the review with the student, the faculty member and director determine that academic dishonesty has occurred, the evidence will be submitted to the Office of Student Rights and Responsibilities. The report “identifies the student who was found responsible, the general nature of the offense, the action taken, and a recommendation as to whether or not additional action should be considered by the campus judicial affairs office.” (http://www.sa.sdsu.edu/srr/academics1.html). 2 Intellectual Property: The syllabus, lectures and lecture outlines are personal copyrighted intellectual property of the instructor, which means that any organized recording for anything other than personal use, duplication, distribution, or profit is a violation of copyright and fair use laws. Proper source attribution Proper attribution occurs by specifying the source of content or ideas. This is done by (a) providing quotation marks around text, when directly quoted, and (b) clearly designating the source of the text or information relied upon in an assignment. Specific exemplary infractions and consequences: a. Reproducing a whole paper, paragraph, or large portions of unattributed materials (whether represented by: (i) multiple sentences, images, or portions of images; or (ii) by percentage of assignment length) without proper attribution, will result in assignment of an “F” in the course, and a report to Student Rights and Responsibilities. b. Reproducing a sentence or sentence fragment with no quotation marks but source citation, or subsets of visual images without source attribution, will minimally result in an “F” on the assignment. Self-plagiarism Students often practice some form of ‘double-dipping,’ in which they write on a given topic across more than one course assignment. In general, there is nothing wrong with doubledipping topics or sources, but there is a problem with double-dipping exact and redundant text. It is common for scholars to write on the same topic across many publication outlets; this is part of developing expertise and the reputation of being a scholar on a topic. Scholars, however, are not permitted to repeat exact text across papers or publications except when noted and attributed, as this wastes precious intellectual space with repetition and does a disservice to the particular source of original presentation by ‘diluting’ the value of the original presentation. Any time that a writer simply ‘cuts-and-pastes’ exact text from former papers into a new paper without proper attribution, it is a form of self-plagiarism. Consequently, a given paper should never be turned in to multiple classes. Entire paragraphs, or even sentences, should not be repeated word-for-word across course assignments. Each new writing assignment is precisely that, a new writing assignment, requiring new composition on the student’s part. Secondary citations Secondary citation is not strictly a form of plagiarism, but in blatant forms, it can present similar ethical challenges. A secondary citation is citing source A, which in turn cites source B, but it is source B’s ideas or content that provide the basis for the claims the student intends to make in the assignment. For example, assume that there is an article by Jones (2006) in the student’s hands, in which there is a discussion or quotation of an article by Smith (1998). Assume further that what Smith seems to be saying is very important to the student’s analysis. In such a situation, the student should always try to locate the original Smith source. In general, if an idea is important enough to discuss in an assignment, it is important enough to locate and cite the original source for that idea. There are several reasons for these policies: (a) Authors sometimes commit citation errors, which might be replicated without knowing it; (b) Authors 3 sometimes make interpretation errors, which might be ignorantly reinforced (c) Therefore, reliability of scholarly activity is made more difficult to assure and enforce; (d) By relying on only a few sources of review, the learning process is short-circuited, and the student’s own research competencies are diminished, which are integral to any liberal education; (e) By masking the actual sources of ideas, readers must second guess which sources come from which citations, making the readers’ own research more difficult; (f) By masking the origin of the information, the actual source of ideas is misrepresented. Some suggestions that assist with this principle: When the ideas Jones discusses are clearly attributed to, or unique to, Smith, then find the Smith source and citation. When the ideas Jones is discussing are historically associated more with Smith than with Jones, then find the Smith source and citation. In contrast, Jones is sometimes merely using Smith to back up what Jones is saying and believes, and is independently qualified to claim, whether or not Smith would have also said it; in such a case, citing Jones is sufficient. Never simply copy a series of citations at the end of a statement by Jones, and reproduce the reference list without actually going to look up what those references report—the only guarantee that claims are valid is for a student to read the original sources of those claims. Solicitation for ghost writing: Any student who solicits any third party to write any portion of an assignment for this class (whether for pay or not) violates the standards of academic honesty in this course. The penalty for solicitation (regardless of whether it can be demonstrated the individual solicited wrote any sections of the assignment) is F in the course. Specific exemplary infractions and consequences Course failure: Reproducing a whole paper, paragraph, or large portions of unattributed materials without proper attribution, whether represented by: (a) multiple sentences, images, or portions of images; or (b) by percentage of assignment length, will result in assignment of an “F” in the course in which the infraction occurred, and a report to the Center for Student Rights and Responsibilities (CSRR2). Assignment failure: Reproducing a sentence or sentence fragment with no quotation marks, but with source citation, or subsets of visual images without source attribution, will minimally result in an “F” on the assignment, and may result in greater penalty, including a report to the CSRR, depending factors noted below. In this instance, an “F” may mean anything between a zero (0) and 50%, depending on the extent of infraction. Exacerbating conditions--Amount: Evidence of infraction, even if fragmentary, is increased with a greater: (a) number of infractions; (b) distribution of infractions across an assignment; or (c) proportion of the assignment consisting of infractions. Exacerbating conditions--Intent: Evidence of foreknowledge and intent to deceive magnifies the seriousness of the offense and the grounds for official response. Plagiarism, whether ‘by accident’ or ‘by ignorance,’ still qualifies as plagiarism—it is all students’ responsibility to make sure their assignments are not committing the offense. Exceptions: Any exceptions to these policies will be considered on a case-by-case basis, and only under exceptional circumstances. 4 However, there are no excuses allowed based on ignorance of what constitutes plagiarism, or of what this policy is. Attendance & Late Work This class only meets once per week (i.e., missing one day is equivalent to missing 3 classes). Thus, you are expected to be in class every day so you can benefit as much as possible from this course. Attendance will be taken but, attendance is not required. It is your responsibility to submit all assignments on or before the designated due dates. If you know in advance that you will be missing a class then it is your responsibility to communicate with me PRIOR to the class session you will be missing. In the event of an emergency, please contact me as soon as possible. LATE WORK IS NOT ACCEPTED AND WILL RECEIVE A GRADE OF ZERO. Moreover, there is NO EXCUSE for submitting assignments late when you know in advance that you will be missing class. In the event of an unanticipated EMERGENCY – and if it is feasible – students who provide proper written documentation for their absence may have up to one week after the absence to submit written assignments. Moreover, please be aware that some assignments (e.g., in-class exercises) cannot be made up. Accommodations Due to Disability If you have a documented disability that requires academic accommodations, please turn in your accommodation letter immediately. If you are a student with a disability and believe you will need accommodations for this class, it is your responsibility to contact Student Disability Services at (619) 594-6473. To avoid any delay in the receipt of your accommodations, you should contact Student Disability Services as soon as possible. Please note that accommodations are not retroactive, and that accommodations based upon disability cannot be provided until you have presented your instructor with an accommodation letter from Student Disability Services. Written Work Most all assignments will be submitted through Blackboard. If you just cannot get Blackboard to work for you on a particular assignment, email me a copy of the assignment before the deadline and then figure out what went wrong. This should not happen consistently!! Blackboard works the best through Mozilla Firefox. The final assignment will be submitted as a Turnitin assignment, meaning your assignments will be checked for textual similarity in order to detect plagiarism. You will be expected to use current APA 6th Edition style guidelines for all written work. Use of appropriate grammatical skills in your written work is very important. Your written work will be evaluated on both content and mechanics. Good writing should be reasonably free of mistakes and without composition errors, which are called gross errors (sentence fragments, run-on sentences, subject-verb disagreement, misspelled words, and typographical errors which result in such errors). PLEASE proofread your papers; do not leave it up to your computer software. All of your work MUST BE TYPED (using no more than 12 point type with 1 inch margins on all sides per APA) and double-spaced. Submit all written assignments before class on the designated due date (unless otherwise noted). Failure to meet written work guidelines will result in a grade penalty- I do not accept late work. 5 GRADED ASSIGNMENTS There is a total of 400 points that can be earned for this course. The final grade will be determined by adding the total points earned for each of the graded assignments. The final grade is based on number of points accumulated—NOT PERCENTAGES. Final grades are not rounded up and are NOT negotiable. The point breakdown for class assignments follows. Work is evaluated based on quality, not on effort. In-Class Participation and Activities – 100 points These points will be based on in-class work, participation, and activities. These activities are not always announced beforehand and cannot be made up. These activities will serve as opportunities for you to enhance your knowledge on class subject matter while working with peers in class. Weekly Reading Responses – 100 points You will complete 10 weekly reading responses (worth 10 points each); these will be due before the start of class. Reading the course materials is essential to learning, participation, and application. The prompt for a reading response assignment is available on BlackBoard. Paper/study Proposal – 50 points About half way through the semester you will complete a paper/study proposal. In this assignment, students will outline a project for the final assignment. This assignment is an opportunity to receive instructor feedback and ensure that projects are heading in the right direction. A detailed prompt and rubric will be made available on BlackBoard later in the semester. Final Project & Presentation – 150 points There will be two choices for the final project. Students can either (a) work individual to write a research proposal for a persuasion study or (b) work in pairs to conduct a persuasion study throughout the semester. The full project will include a final paper (worth 100 points) and a presentation to the class (worth 50 points). Specific rubrics and details for each assignment will be available on BlackBoard later in the semester. ACADEMIC GRADING SCALE *A—Consistently excellent work A 376-400 (94% +) A358-375 (90%-93%) B—Good or inconsistently excellent work B+ 347-357 (87%-89%) B 335-346 (84%-86%) B318-334 (80%-83%) C—Average/inconsistent work C+ 306-317 (77%-79%) C 296-305 (74%-76%) C280-295 (70%-73%) D—Below average/poor work D 239-279 F—Irrelevant, missing, and/or poor work F 0-238 *A does NOT stand for average; A work is high quality work that does more than address the bare minimum requested in a prompt. 6 Tentative Course Schedule It should be noted that the California Faculty Association is in the midst of a difficult contract dispute with management. It is possible that the faculty union will call a strike or other work stoppage this term. I will inform the class as soon as possible of any disruption to our class meeting schedule. Recommendation: Read in order of assignment DATE 1-25 2-1 TOPIC/READINGS/ACTIVITIES Welcome to Course! What is Persuasion? O’Keefe, Chapters 1 & 9 Theories of Attitude Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relations: A theoretical analysis and review of empirical research. Psychological Bulletin, 84(5), 888-918. DUE THAT DAY (Not reading response eligible) Proposal submission opens on BlackBoard O’Keefe, Chapter 2 Smith, S. W., Atkin, C. K., Martell, D., Allen, R., & Hembroff, L. (2006). A social judgment theory approach to conducting formative research in a social norms campaign. Communication Theory, 16(1), 141-152. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2885.2006.00009.x O’Keefe, Chapters 3 & 4 Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: an integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132(5), 692-731. 2-8 Online Class via BlackBoard Theories of Attitude, con. O’Keefe, Chapter 5 Description of course plan on BlackBoard. Ivanov, B., Parker, K. A., & Pfau, M. (2012). The interaction effect of attitude base and multiple attacks on the effectiveness of inoculation. Communication Research Reports, 29(1), 1-11. doi:10.1080/08824096.2011.616789 O’Keefe, Chapter 6 Glasman, L. R., & Albarracín, D. (2006). Forming attitudes that predict future behavior: A meta-analysis of the attitude-behavior relation. Psychological bulletin, 132(5), 778. Ajzen, I. (2011). The theory of planned behaviour: Reactions and reflections. Psychology and Health, 26(9), 1113-1127. 7 2-15 Theories of Change & Processing O’Keefe, Chapter 7 Patel, S., Shafer, A., Brown, J., Bulik, C., & Zucker, N. (2014). Parents of children with eating disorders: Developing theory-based health communication messages to promote caregiver wellbeing. Journal of Health Communication, 19(5), 593-608. doi:10.1080/10810730.2013.821559 O’Keefe, Chapter 8 Lane, R., Miller, A. N., Brown, C., & Vilar, N. (2013). An examination of the narrative persuasion with epilogue through the lens of the elaboration likelihood model. Communication Quarterly, 61(4), 431-445. doi:10.1080/01463373.2013.799510 Lazard, A., & Atkinson, L. (2015). Putting environmental infographics center stage: The role of visuals at the elaboration likelihood model’s critical point of persuasion. Science Communication, 37(1), 6-33. doi:10.1177/1075547014555997 2-22 Social Influence Boster, F. J., & Cruz, M. G. (2002). Persuading in the small group context. In J. P. Dillard & L. Shen (Eds.), The Sage handbook of persuasion: Developments in Theory and Practice (pp. 354-370). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. McGarty, C., Haslam, S. A., & Hutchinson, K. J. (1994). The effects of salient group memberships on persuasion. Small Group Research, 25(2), 267-293. Etzioni, A. (2000). Social norms: Internalization, persuasion, and history. Law & Society Review, 34(1), 157-178. doi:10.2307/3115119 Rimal, R.N. (2008). Modeling the relationship between descriptive norms and behaviors: A test and extension of the theory of normative social behavior (TNSB). Health Communication, 23(2), 103-116. Rimal, R. N., & Real, K. (2003). Understanding the influence of perceived norms on behaviors. Communication Theory, 13(2), 184203. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2885.2003.tb00288.x 8 2-29 Online Class via BlackBoard Influences of Sender & Receiver O’Keefe, Chapters 10 & 12 Description of course plan on BlackBoard. Faraji-Rad, A., Samuelsen, B. M., & Warlop, L. (2015). On the persuasiveness of similar others: The role of mentalizing and the feeling of certainty. Journal of Consumer Research, 42(3), 458-471. doi:10.1093/jcr/ucv032 Touré-Tillery, M., & McGill, A. L. (2015). Who or what to believe: Trust and the differential persuasiveness of human and anthropomorphized messengers. Journal of Marketing, 79(4), 94-110. Lim, T. S. (1990). The influences of receivers' resistance on persuaders' verbal aggressiveness. Communication Quarterly, 38(2), 170-188. Hosman, L. A., & Siltanen, S. A. (2011). Hedges, tag questions, message processing, and persuasion. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 30(3), 341-349. doi:10.1177/0261927X11407169 3-7 Influences of Message Content O’Keefe, Chapter 11 Allen, M., Bruflat, R., Fucilla, R., Kramer, M., McKellips, S., Ryan, D. J., & Spiegelhoff, M. (2000) Testing the persuasiveness of evidence: Combining narrative and statistical forms, Communication Research Reports, 17(4), 331-336, doi:10.1080/08824090009388781 Hosman, L. A. (2002). Language and persuasion. In J. P. Dillard & M. Pfau (Eds.), The Sage handbook of persuasion: Developments in Theory and Practice (pp. 371-390). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Salvoey, P., Schneider, T. R., & Apanovitch, A. M. (2002). Message framing in the prevention and early detection of illness. In J. P. Dillard & M. Pfau (Eds.), The Sage handbook of persuasion: Developments in Theory and Practice (pp. 391-406). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Eisend, M. (2009). A meta-analysis of humor in advertising. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 37(2), 191-203. 3-14 Persuasion & Rhetorical Communication Hogan, M. J. (2013). Persuasion in the rhetorical tradition. In J. P. Dillard & L. Shen (Eds.), The Sage handbook of persuasion: Developments in Theory and Practice (pp. 2-19). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 9 Bell, S. (2007). Narrative ethnography: The bridge between rhetoric and social science as revealed in a case study of persuasion in Alcoholics Anonymous. Conference Paper -- National Communication Association. Johnson, J. (2014). The Limits of Persuasion: Rhetoric and Resistance in the Last Battle of the Korean War. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 100(3), 323-347. doi:10.1080/00335630.2014.979861 Westwood, S. J. (2015). The role of persuasion in deliberative opinion change. Political Communication, 32(4), 509-528. doi:10.1080/10584609.2015.1017628 Pierce, D. L., & Kaufman, K. (2012). Visual persuasion tactics in narrative development: An analysis of The Matrix. Visual Communication Quarterly, 19(1), 33-47. doi:10.1080/15551393.2012.656063 Smith, J. H., & Just, S. N. (2009). Playful Persuasion. NORDICOM Review, 30(2), 53-68. 3-21 Persuasion & Interpersonal Communication Dillard, J. P., Anderson, J. W., & Knobloch, L. K. (2002). Interpersonal influence. In M. L. Knapp & J. A. Daly (Eds.), Handbook of interpersonal communication (3rd ed., pp. 425-474). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Last day to submit paper proposals to BlackBoard. Neiheisel, J. R., & Niebler, S. (2015). On the limits of persuasion: Campaign ads and the structure of voters’ interpersonal discussion networks. Political Communication, 32(3), 434-452. doi:10.1080/10584609.2014.958258 Wagner, T. R. (2011). Resistance to persuasion in committed romantic relationships: Reactance effects of forewarning and message explicitness. Ohio Communication Journal, 4915-42. Larrimore, L., Li, J., Larrimore, J., Markowitz, D., & Gorski, S. (2011). Peer to peer lending: The relationship between language features, trustworthiness, and persuasion success. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 39(1), 19-37. doi:10.1080/00909882.2010.536844 Goodboy, A. K., Bolkan, S., Beebe, S. A., & Shultz, K. (2010). Cultural differences between United States and Chinese students' use of behavioral alteration techniques and affinity-seeking strategies with instructors. Journal of Intercultural Communication Research, 39(1), 10 1-12. doi:10.1080/17475759.2010.520834 3-28 4-4 SPRING BREAK—NO CLASS Out of Class Work Day Class time should be spent incorporating instructor feedback on final projects, collecting literature, (for those who conducting a study) collecting data, and drafting the final paper. 4-11 Persuasion & Mediated Communication Holbert, R. L., & Tchernev, J. M. (2013). Media influence as persuasion. In J. P. Dillard & L. Shen (Eds.), The Sage handbook of persuasion: Developments in Theory and Practice (pp. 36-52). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. (Not reading response eligible) Oh, J., & Sundar, S. S. (2015). How does interactivity persuade? An experimental test of interactivity on cognitive absorption, elaboration, and attitudes. Journal of Communication, 65(2), 213-236. doi:10.1111/jcom.12147 Kim, K. J., & Sundar, S. S. (2016). Mobile persuasion: Can screen size and presentation mode make a difference to trust?. Human Communication Research, 42(1), 45-70. doi:10.1111/hcre.12064 Lwin, M. O., & Malik, S. (2014). Can exergames impart health messages? Game play, framing, and drivers of physical activity among children. Journal ointernf Health Communication, 19(2), 136151. doi:10.1080/10810730.2013.798372 Allen, M., Wicks, R. H., & Schulte, S. (2013). Online environmental engagement among youth: Influence of parents, attitudes and demographics. Mass Communication & Society, 16(5), 661-686. doi:10.1080/15205436.2013.770032 Waddell, T. F., & Ivory, J. D. (2015). It's not easy trying to be one of the guys: The effect of avatar attractiveness, avatar sex, and user sex on the success of help-seeking requests in an online game. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 59(1), 112-129. doi:10.1080/08838151.2014.998221 4-18 Persuasion & Health Communication O'Keefe, D. J., & Jensen, J. D. (2007). The relative persuasiveness of gain-framed loss-framed messages for encouraging disease prevention behaviors: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Health Communication, 12(7), 623-644. doi:10.1080/10810730701615198 11 Briňol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2006). Fundamental processes leading to attitude change: Implications for cancer prevention communications. Journal of Communication, 56, s81-s104. Pfau, M., Von Bockern, S., & Kang, J. G. (2008). Use of inoculation to promote resistance in smoking initiation among adolescents. In L. C. Lederman, Beyond these walls: Readings in health communication (pp. 304-319). New York: Oxford. Yeh, M. A., & Jewell, R. D. (2015). The myth/fact message frame and persuasion in advertising: Enhancing attitudes toward the mentally ill. Journal of Advertising, 44(2), 161-172. doi:10.1080/00913367.2015.1018466 Neubaum, G., & Krämer, N. C. (2015). Let’s blog about health! Exploring the persuasiveness of a personal HIV blog compared to an institutional HIV website. Health Communication, 30(9), 872-883. doi:10.1080/10410236.2013.856742 4-25 Persuasion & Health Campaigns Witte, K., & Allen, M. (2000). A meta-analysis of fear appeals: Implications for effective public health campaigns. Health Education & Behavior, 27(5), 591-615. Noar, S. M., Zimmerman, R. S., Palmgreen, P., Cupp, P. K., Floyd, B. R., & Mehrotra, P. (2014). Development and implementation of mass media campaigns to delay sexual initiation among African American and White youth. Journal of Health Communication, 19(2), 152-169. Andersen, P. A., Buller, D. B., Voeks, J. H., Walkosz, B. J., Scott, M. D., Cutter, G. R., & Dignan, M. B. (2008). Testing the long-term effects of the "Go Sun Smart" worksite health communication campaign: A group-randomized experimental study. Journal of Communication, 58(3), 447-471. doi:10.1111/j.14602466.2008.00394.x DeJong, W., & Smith, S. W. (2013). Truth in advertising: Social norms marketing campaigns to reduce college student drinking. In R. E. Rice & C. K. Atkin (Eds.), Public communication campaigns (4th ed., pp. 177-190). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Moran, M. B., Murphy, S. T., & Sussman, S. (2012). Campaigns and cliques: Variations in effectiveness of an antismoking campaign as a function of adolescent peer group identity. Journal of Health Communication,17(10), 1215-1231. 12 5-2 Persuasion & The Dark Side Powers, T. L., & Jack, E. P. (2013). The influence of cognitive dissonance on retail product returns. Psychology & Marketing, 30(8), 724-735. doi:10.1002/mar.20640 Nabi, R. L., & Hendriks, A. (2003). The persuasive effect of host and audience reaction shots in television talk shows. Journal of Communication, 53(3), 527-543. doi:10.1111/j.14602466.2003.tb02606.x Praxmarer, S. (2011). How a presenter's perceived attractiveness affects persuasion for attractiveness-unrelated products. International Journal of Advertising, 30(5), 839-865. doi:10.2501/IJA-30-5-839865 Paolucci, E. O., Genuis, M., & Violato, C. (1997). A meta-analysis of the published research on the effects of pornography. Medicine, Mind and Adolescence, 12, 1-8. Leone, M. (2015). Propaganda mala fide: Towards a comparative semiotics of violent religious persuasion. Semiotica, 2015(207), 631655. doi:10.1515/sem-2015-0057 Shane, S., & Hubbard, B. (August 2014). ISIS displaying a deft command of varied media. New York: New York Times. 5-6 Final projects submitted to BlackBoard by midnight. 5-9 Scheduled Final Exam Time, 6:00pm-8:00pm Paper Presentations (Not reading response eligible) *** THIS COURSE SCHEDULE IS SUBJECT TO CHANGE. ANY CHANGES WILL BE EMAILED, POSTED ON BB, AND/OR ANNOUNCED IN CLASS. YOU ARE RESPONSIBLE FOR KEEPING UP WITH CHANGES TO THE SCHEDULE BY CHECKING BLACKBOARD/EMAIL DAILY! *** 13