The benefits of archetypal prototyping when profiling market segments Chris Sherley, Charles Sturt University Mark Morrison, Charles Sturt University Roderick Duncan*, Charles Sturt University Abstract Prototyping is a market profiling technique used to develop more detailed descriptions of market segments than is possible with traditional segmentation analyses. This method describes real people that embody the salient features of a market segment. Morris and Schmolze (2006) demonstrated an approach that, unlike previous studies, used predominately quantitative procedures to identify prototypes. We extend Morris and Schmolze’s (2006) approach by utilising more sophisticated quantitative tools and archetypal interviewing techniques to build information rich segment profiles for Australian attitudes to climate change policy. Our improvements allow an improved selection of prototypical respondents, and the results highlight the importance of using both qualitative and quantitative analysis procedures. For practicing market and advertising researchers, this new technique offers a fast and cost effective way of researching those consumer traits such as archetypes that are often only found using extensive qualitative research. Keywords: Archetypes; prototypes; market profiling; segmentation; target marketing; climate change policy * Corresponding author: Dr. Roderick Duncan, Email: rduncan@csu.edu.au, Postal Address: School of Accounting and Finance, Faculty of Business, Panorama Avenue, Bathurst NSW 2795 Australia. 1 1. Introduction Traditional profiling procedures are often limited in their ability to explain the drivers behind consumer behavior, however understanding these motivations can be an important part of advertising and marketing research. For this reason, there is a need to develop better research procedures that not only identify what behaviors and attitudes define a market segment, but why these trends occur. Wansink (2000) suggested that traditional profiling techniques often result in over generalisations and, at times, may neglect important underlying attitudes or motivations of a market segment. Morris and Schmolze (2006, p. 291) also noted that: “The weakness of these [traditional profiling techniques] however is that they are, at best, approximations of tendencies within a segment, and at worst serious misrepresentations of the diversity within a particular segment.” Given these limitations, prototyping has been suggested by a number of authors as a more effective way of profiling targeted market segments (Hogg and Reid, 2006; Morris and Schmolze, 2006; Wansink, 2000). A prototype is a human representation of a market segment that embodies the typical behavioral, attitudinal and emotional responses of that segment to marketing stimulation (Hogg and Reid, 2006; Morris and Schmolze, 2006; Wansink, 2000). In recent literature, researchers have attempted to discover reliable methods of identifying and profiling representative prototypes from a data set (Hogg and Reid, 2006; Loken, Barsalou, and Joiner, 2008; Morris and Schmolze, 2006; Wansink, 2000). However, these methodologies are often subjective and require the researcher to make personal decisions regarding who may be considered prototypical of a segment. Wansink (2000, p. 34) argued that this issue can limit the effectiveness of prototyping as a research tool because “Our personal background heavily influences the degree to which we understand the psychology of people of different race, religions, ages or geographic backgrounds” 2 To increase objectivity, Morris and Schmolze (2006) suggested an innovative method for identifying prototypes in a market segment. Core members (prototypes) are found using a series of quantitative steps in which respondents with the highest probability of categorical membership to a market cluster are selected as representatives of the broader cluster. Once they are selected, these people are then profiled using in-depth interviews. However, while Morris and Schmolze (2006) refined the process of identifying and selecting prototypes, there were several limitations associated with their application. In terms of their quantitative analysis tools, they used k-means clustering to identify segments and single rather than multiple discriminant analysis to identify segment cores. The use of k-means clustering is problematic if there is missing data in the survey responses, as discussed below. We found a multiple discriminant analysis to be more effective than a single discriminant analysis at identifying probabilities of group membership. There was also an issue with Morris and Schmolze’s (2006) final discriminant analyses in which they narrowed down the remaining members using probability of group membership scores. We found that a substantial proportion of respondents scored 100%, so that the analysis did not effectively differentiate between members. Along with this, Morris and Schmolze (2006) used an expert panel when selecting the final prototypes – an essentially subjective step and not consistent with the objective approach that they recommend. Instead, we offer a more process without the use of experts in which members that are closest to the cluster centroid are selected. Along with these quantitative issues, Morris and Schmolze’s (2006) follow up qualitative research was limited and produced little new information about the prototype respondents. Given these limitations, we seek to make a number of methodological improvements to Morris and Schmolze’s (2006) original paper. This included replacing the k-means clustering procedure with a latent class cluster analysis, using multiple discriminant analysis procedures, as well as using a new objective procedure for identifying respondents at the centre of the archetypal pools. Finally, we offer a new interview structure for the qualitative research which was developed from archetypal literature in the marketing and advertising fields (Maso-Fleischman, 1997; Peattie and Peattie, 2009; Randazzo, 2006; Rapaille, 2007). 3 In the results, we find evidence that the new prototyping methodology is effective in providing a number of key insights that were not apparent in previously conducted segmentation analysis procedures. The archetypal profiles of the selected prototypes indicated that there were unique pools that differed according to a range of demographic variables, not discovered using more traditional segmentation techniques. These profiles indicated that there were likely to be a number of differing motivations for the attitudes identified at a segment level. While the quantitative analysis was found to be useful in identifying differing prototypes within a market segment, the analysis also demonstrated the critical importance of the qualitative interviews in producing comprehensive profiles of prototypes. Next, prototypes and archetypes are defined and the rationale for their usefulness in advertising is discussed. For practitioners, this new procedure has three key benefits. Firstly, having a better understanding of the motivations that drive consumer behaviour in target markets may allow more appropriate marketing strategies to be developed for those markets. Secondly, for research involving quantitative then qualitative procedures, archetypal prototyping may uncover important preliminary findings in the quantitative research that can be utilized to help direct more extensive follow-up qualitative research. As the number of interviews required when prototyping is relatively small, this technique can be a cost effective way of ensuring that follow-up qualitative research is focused on those areas that are most crucial to understanding consumer behaviour or attitudes. The final benefit for practitioners using archetypal prototyping is multi-faceted. This procedure can provide a more nuanced description of market segments than is possible using quantitative research. On the other hand, it is has more statistical basis than is perhaps the case with some qualitative research strategies, this can be important when presenting research to clients that are preferably disposed to quantitative research. 2. Prototypes and archetypes and their use in advertising 2.1 Prototypes A prototype is a human representation of a market segment that embodies the typical behavioral, attitudinal and emotional responses of that segment to marketing stimulation 4 (Hogg and Reid, 2006; Morris and Schmolze, 2006; Wansink, 2000). Wansink (2000) suggested that prototyping is a quick and effective way of identifying key consumers that closely resemble a broad market segment. This technique builds on segment profiles developed using traditional market research methods, such as cluster analysis. Hogg and Reid (2006) argued that members of a market segment share a series of visual, audio and olfactory schemas in their subconscious cognitive faculties. These schemas influence consumers by encouraging particular responses to certain stimuli. Ratneshwa and Shocker (1988) referred to this trend as ‘prototypicality’. Prototypicality is drawn from the psychological categorization field and refers to common cognitive, emotional and behavioral responses in groups of people. For example, if an Australian consumer walked into a McDonald’s in India they may be surprised to find that there is no beef available. This is because most Australian consumers have an underlying schema of what should be expected at a McDonald’s restaurant. When this schema is not met, the consumer may feel a sense of disbelief or dissonance towards the initial experience (Babin and Babin, 2001). Conflicts between consumer schemas and reality can be an issue when developing advertising communications. Certain imagery may trigger unexpected responses from targeted consumers. These may differ from what is considered rational by the researchers developing the communications. Identifying prototypes from a targeted population gives researchers the ability to study the prototypicality of that group. Wansink (2000, p. 30) argued that studying these members compared to more traditional market profiling techniques will allow companies to “develop specific characteristics [of a market segment] as opposed to the non-descriptive statistics provided by demographics”. In a study testing the effectiveness of one prototyping methodology, 23 brand managers were given a sample of 261 respondents. Twelve brand managers were asked to use traditional clustering procedures to predict which of these respondents were likely to be members of a public radio station. The remaining 11 brand managers were asked to use prototyping methods. The results showed that those managers using traditional segmentation analysis procedures predicted cluster membership correctly 45% 5 of the time, while those managers using prototyping predicted correctly 87%. While the potential advantages of prototyping are evident, there is one fundamental limitation with its use: deciding who is prototypical of a market segment. Only specific members of a social group can be treated as fully representative of that group. Hogg and Reid (2006) referred to these members as prototypical or core members of the group. These members are held to embody the prototypicality of a social grouping such as a market segment. Cantor and Mischel (1979, p. 188) argued that these people or prototypes can be used to identify a “large set of more or less associated qualities that together represent or exemplify the meaning of that personality category [the target social group].” For companies conducting marketing or advertising research, profiling these central or core members would allow a much more in-depth understanding of who the target customer really is. Identifying a core member of a market means it may be possible to observe important consumer traits that can be lost using traditional market profiling. However, a number of issues arise. Firstly, individuals must actually be representative of the broader segment. Wansink (2000) argued that if the wrong or too few prototypes are selected, the researcher may be given a narrow indication of the segment. Similarly, if too many consumers are selected, too much information would be produced to make the process viable. For this reason, Morris and Schmolze (2006) suggested using an innovative quantitative approach to address these problems. Qualitatively, prototyping not only requires the researcher to identify representative members of a market, but also conduct an in-depth analysis of various consumer traits to develop information rich consumer profiles. Morris and Schmolze (2006) suggested that Jungian archetypes may be effective at providing the basis for this analysis. That is, we may observe the prototypicality of a market segment by studying the archetypes of specific respondents. 2.2 Archetypes Archetype literature first became prominent with the psychologist Carl Jung in the 1950s. Recently, the application of archetypal research has become more accepted in the advertising field (Maso-Fleischman, 1997; Peattie and Peattie, 2009; Randazzo, 2006; Rapaille, 2007). 6 Archetypes can be broadly described as a set of subconscious associations or schemas learnt through previous sensory stimulations (Woodside, Stood, and Miller, 2008). Very similar to prototypicality, archetypal associations trigger particular emotional, behavioral and attitudinal responses when the same stimulus or very similar stimuli are re-encountered (Knox, 2004). There are a number of iconic archetypal figures that have been utilized by advertisers over the years. An example of a successful archetypal advertising campaign can be seen in the famous Marlboro cowboy mascot who spearheaded the company’s marketing communications in the 1950’s and 60’s. Randazzo (2006) suggested that in the USA, the image of a cowboy activates a distinct emotional response in certain consumers. To baby boomer consumers, the cowboy represents the archetypes of FREEDOM and the PIONEERING SPIRIT; this archetypal association was learnt by baby boomers as children when the cowboy was a prominent feature of US entertainment media. Randazzo (2006) accredited much of Marlboro’s commercial success to the company’s cowboy mascot. He suggested that the cowboy made American consumers feel a subconscious loyalty to the company. In essence, the company became the U.S. pioneering spirit to these consumers. In recent years, archetypal interviewing has become an accepted qualitative research technique by marketing research firms and corporations. Well known companies such as Chrysler, Shell, the Ritz-Carlton, Subaru and many more have utilized this interviewing style for new product development and market communications (Randazzo, 2006; Rapaille, 2007; Shalit, 1999). The technique aims to identify underlying archetypes and the way they may affect consumer buying habits. As can be seen, archetype and prototype theories share very similar backgrounds. Underlying schemas or archetypes are shared by members of a consumer group. These archetypes encourage particular responses to various marketing stimuli. 2.3 Archetypal prototyping We define an archetypal prototype as: a detailed description of a person who may be used to represent the archetypal, emotional and behavioral tendencies of a wider group of people. In 7 Morris and Schmolze’s (2006, p. 290) original study, they used an outdated description of archetypes that made the overall procedure somewhat confusing. They defined an archetype as: “A detailed description of a single person who represents a particular expression of a wider group of people along a prescribed dimension.” This definition is closer to that of prototyping. Therefore, we describe Morris and Schmolze’s (2006) procedure as archetypal prototyping. 2.4 Case study The data for our prototype analysis were collected from a broader case study into Australian’s attitudes towards climate change. This involved a replication of Maibach, Leiserowitz, RoserRenouf and Mertz’s (2011) Six America’s study. The intention was to observe the differences between Australian respondent’s attitudes towards climate change and those of American respondents. A sample of 1927 respondents was collected using an online database. The sample was selected to be representative of the Australian population based on 2007 Census data. Six Australian clusters were defined: Alarmed, Concerned, Cautious, Disengaged, Doubtful and Dismissive. The segments were located on a scale; Alarmed were the most inclined toward climate change action, while people in the Dismissive cluster were the least likely to believe in climate change. The segmentation analysis was done using a latent class analysis (LCA) clustering procedure. LCA is a classification technique used to group respondents using maximum likelihood methods (Maibach, et al., 2011). Madgison and Vermunt (2002) suggested that LCA is a much simpler form of clustering, not requiring standardization of variables. LCA is also able to handle nominal, ordinal, and continuous variables simultaneously meaning the technique is more robust than alternative clustering procedures such as hierarchical or K-means clustering (McCutcheon, 1987). LCA is also capable of handling missing data (Maibach, et al., 2011). This 8 is particularly useful for the prototype analysis and will be discussed further in the methodology section. For the prototype analysis, the Cautious (n=502) and Disengaged (n=385) clusters were selected. These two clusters were the most relevant to policy makers as they were most likely to capture the median voter, and hence were chosen for further analysis. The Cautious cluster was found to believe in climate change and support climate change action. However, they were very cautious about policies that may threaten them fiscally such as a carbon tax. They were also unlikely to believe that climate change policies in Australia would have any real effect. The Disengaged cluster was slightly less inclined towards supporting climate change action. The members of this segment were hesitant to believe that climate change was being caused by humans. They also felt that policies such as a carbon tax were a bad idea. Instead, they preferred direct action policies. They were also unlikely to believe any government communications on the issue. These segment profiles indicated distinct attitudinal characteristics. However, the profiles were limited in their ability to demonstrate other relevant trends within the data that may help explain why the segments have these particular attitudes. For example, in the Cautious cluster, approximately a third of people have full time work, while a fifth of people are retired or on a pension. Although these people may share similar attitudes towards climate change, these socio-demographic differences may be important. However, in the initial cluster analysis they were ignored. People in a segment who have similar attitudes are often treated as having the same motivations for those attitudes arising. However, while segments may appear to be homogenous, there can be multiple underlying factors that divide members into sub-categories (Woodside, et al., 2008). Archetypal literature suggests that researchers should seek to not only explain what people feel, but why they feel it (Hofstede, 1997; Peattie and Peattie, 2009; Randazzo, 2006; Rapaille, 2007). In this example, the 20% of Cautious respondents that are 9 aged pensioners are unlikely to have the same motivations for their attitudes as the younger 35% that have full time work. This issue is what the archetypal prototyping process aims to address. 3. Method Although our methodology is based on Morris and Schmolze’s (2006) prototyping methodology, we have refined a number of steps. Table 1 briefly outlines the steps used in this study. Stage 1: Initial segmentation analysis In the first stage of the process, we conduct a cluster analysis. While Morris and Schmolze (2006) used a K-means analysis in their study, we used LCA because it is able to model with missing data. When choosing a clustering procedure like K-means, any missing data will often be converted to mean scores. This poses a problem as the aim of the procedure is to identify the most appropriate cluster centroids for prototyping. If missing data is converted to mean scores, respondents that may have originally been segment outliers can be inadvertently moved to the cluster centre, which can lead to inappropriate prototypes being selected. Stage 2: Identify core members In the second stage we are looking to identify those members that have the highest probability of belonging to the segment. To complete this task we used SPSS to conduct a standard multiple discriminant analysis on all the segments, not just the two that would be prototyped. This step contrasts to Morris and Schmolze (2006) who undertook a single discriminant analysis. While the single analysis does show membership probabilities, we found that the multiple discriminant analysis had better discriminating power and the Wilks Lambda produced higher significance levels. We used a discriminant analysis because of the functions ability to predict respondents’ likelihood of categorical membership (Hair, William, Babin, and Anderson, 2010). Although certain regression approaches such as binary logits will also predict group membership, standard software packages do not have the ability to test multiple groups simultaneously without reference to a base category. 10 The items used in this discriminant analysis are the same as those used in the initial clustering procedure. For our analysis we used the 36 attitudinal items which were used by Maibach et al. (2011) to define their original six clusters. We use these variables because we are trying to identify the core members of the clusters. When the analysis was completed, members with the lowest probabilities of group membership were removed from the selected clusters. Morris and Schmolze (2006) suggested that eliminating approximately two-thirds of the segment population at this point leaves sufficient members to conduct the remaining steps of the analysis. Stage 3: Archetypal pool analysis In the archetypal pool analysis stage, we conduct another LCA using socio-demographic variables. This allows us to identify various sub-clusters or ‘archetypal pools’ that may exist within the broader cluster. These groups show differing socio-demographic profiles that may not be identified in the initial clustering solution which was based on attitudinal items (Morris and Schmolze, 2006). Each of the archetypal pools identified within a segment are likely to have differing behavioral patterns and archetypal profiles. Morris and Schmolze (2006) used a hierarchical cluster analysis. However, we again used a LCA as it is a more appropriate clustering procedure. Using Morris and Schmolze (2006) as a guide, we specified a three cluster solution for our archetypal pools as this is a feasible number of prototypes from a marketing perspective. Stage 4: Select the prototypes In the final quantitative stage, the individuals that will be used as prototypes are identified. A stepwise multiple discriminant analysis is run and the function scores are saved. The same socio-demographic items used in the previous analysis are treated as predictive variables while the archetypal pools are treated as categorical dependent variables. Unlike the first discriminant analysis, the goal is not to identify members with the highest probability of group membership. 11 Instead, using functional scores, we identify members that are closest to the cluster centroid. These respondents may not have the highest probability of group membership. However, their closeness to the centroid suggests that these people are most likely to embody their archetypal pool. This is because these members have scores that are closest to the mean points of the segment. Using the saved function scores, a scatterplot is developed. From this, we identify the mean function points on both the X and Y axes and the closest members to this centroid are selected as prototypes. This step differs from Morris and Schmolze’s (2006) final selection in which they undertook another two discriminant analyses, keeping the members with the highest probabilities of group membership. They were left with a small pool of approximately seven people. A panel of experts then selected the members that would be used as prototypes. The process used in our study is arguably easier and less subjective as it avoids the use of experts and provides an objective basis for the selection of respondents as prototypes. Stage 5: Archetypal profiles Once the final prototypes are selected, Morris and Schmolze (2006) recommended undertaking qualitative interviews. As they indicated, the process was drawn from Jungian literature. Hence, we chose to utilize a semi-structured archetypal interview format. A number of authors in the marketing field have suggested question types that should be included in archetypal interviews (Knox, 2004; Maso-Fleischman, 1997; Peattie and Peattie, 2009; Randazzo, 2006; Rapaille, 2007; Woodside, et al., 2008). We used a structure similar to the archetypal focus group suggested by Rapaille (2007). We begin by asking the respondent about themselves, their hobbies and their family. This step is very relaxed and occurs much like an unstructured conversation. The goal is to understand the behavioral and psychographic profile of the respondent, as well as build rapport. In the second step of the interview, a projective technique is applied (Donoghue, 2000) to begin discovering why the respondent feels and acts the way they do regarding the topic. Rapaille 12 (2007) suggested that this may help to provide access to respondent archetypes. From these archetypes we may begin to identify what marketing strategies may engage these prototypes. Using this projective technique, the moderator begins by telling the respondent he or she is an alien from another planet (Rapaille, 2007). They then ask the respondent to describe the research topic (climate change) in as much detail as possible. This exercise serves to get the respondent thinking about the topic without being directed toward specific issues by the researcher. The light hearted nature of the exercise also allows the researcher to continue building a positive relationship with the respondent. In the third section, another series of projective exercises involving free association are used. A number of relevant words such as “global warming” and “environment” are stated. The respondent is asked to indicate any images, thoughts or words that come to mind when each word is stated. The point of this exercise is to uncover underlying archetypal schemas that may exist. For example, when asked about climate change, all of the respondents spoke about clouds. This type of response indicates that marketers should be cautious of what images they use when discussing climate change. Using images that are not clouds or the sky may challenge Cautious and Disengaged consumers’ interpretation of the topic. Next the respondent is shown a number of pictures with a bland, but topical call to action on them, such as: “You can help stop climate change”. The pictures are all archetypal; for example, the archetypal image of a polluting power plant, the clean image of a wind farm, the farmer standing over his crop, the image of children walking into the future through nature, and so on. The respondent is then asked to describe the pictures and indicate the emotions they feel when they see the image. This exercise provides an indication of what marketing imagery is likely to stimulate consumer action. This section is completed by running the same exercise with six phrases that also make a call to action. The respondent is again asked to communicate their feelings for each and what their behavioral response may be. In the last part of the interview the respondent is asked their thoughts on a number of issues, such as the role of science in the climate change debate. This section is located last so as to not influence the respondent prior to the archetypal exercises occurring. These questions are 13 aimed at clarifying the respondent’s conscious thoughts on climate change issues. These questions have no archetypal implications. However, they are helpful when attempting to build contextually relevant consumer profiles. 4. Results 4.1 Quantitative Stages 1 and 2 As previously noted, six clusters were found by Maibach et al. (2011) and then in our replication study. From these six clusters, the two moderate segments Cautious and Disengaged were selected for prototyping. In order to identify the core members of each segment, the probability of group membership scores for the Cautious and Disengaged segments were identified using multiple discriminant analysis. For the Cautious segment, respondents that scored less than 98.7% of group probability were removed. This left a total of 167 core members from the original 502. For the Disengaged segment, members that scored less than 98.8% probability of group membership were removed. This left a total of 129 core members from the original 385. These people provided the basis for the archetypal pool analysis. Stages 3 and 4 Seven socio-demographic variables were used in the LCA to define the archetypal pools. These were: gender, education, age, household income, religion, political orientation, employment status. The statistics for the LCA suggested that the optimal number of clusters was larger than three for both the Cautious and Disengaged segments. However, using more than three prototypes per segment is not practical, and the archetypal pools for each of the segments did not present notably different profiles when a larger cluster solution was used. The Cautious archetypal pools were labeled: C1, C2 and C3; while the Disengaged segments were labeled D1, D2 and D3. Each pool had a unique set of socio-demographic variables that were not highlighted in the original segmentation. For example, the first Cautious pool C1 (n=78) was 83% women, had an average age of 39, most of the pool had completed a trade 14 certificate or TAFE course, approximately 90% of the pool did not work or worked part time, and most had at least one child living at home. Finally, the average household income of this segment was approximately $65,000 - $77,999 per year ($1,250 - $1,499 per week). This profile differed quite notably from the next two pools C2 (n=59) and C3 (n=30). Approximately 98% of respondents in C2 had full time work. The average age of the pool was 40 and the majority had completed a university degree. Gender was split with 56% of members being male. C3 differed again having approximately 93% retirees. The average age of the pool was 63 years old, 70% of the pool was male and the majority had not completed any education beyond high school. These socio-demographic differences may mean there are a number of alternative motivations for the same attitudes (Morris and Schmolze, 2006). For advertisers and marketers, this could mean that promotional campaigns targeted at specific segments may not be appropriately aligned. Identifying prototypes and utilising in-depth qualitative interviews may help identify more relevant promotional activities that would have a greater likelihood of appealing to the various archetypal pools within a broad segment. To isolate the prototypes, we conducted a stepwise multiple discriminant analysis. The analysis produced mixed results. For the Cautious cluster, three distinct and tightly bound clusters were identified. Figure 1 shows a Scatterplot of the three Cautious archetypal pools. As can be seen, there was a small number of outliers. These members affect the placement of the group centroids so they were removed. This step helps provide an accurate indication of the group centroid and, in turn, the identification of the most appropriate prototypes. In contrast to the Cautious segment, the Disengaged analysis (figure 2) produced pools that had less central tendency. As with the Cautious analysis, outliers were removed. A Scatterplot of the function scores for each archetypal pool was used to make the final prototype selection. Members that were closest to the centroid were identified as the possible prototypes. Figure 3 shows an example of this step using the C1 archetypal pool. 15 An issue with discriminant analysis at this step relates to missing data. Although the LCA was able to run without converting missing scores to means, the discriminant analysis cannot do this. In the Disengaged pool; D2, the most central member had a number of missing scores. Hence this person was not interviewed. 4.2 Qualitative results In this section, we report the results of the C1 prototype that was identified using the approach outlined in this paper. For each of the prototypes interviewed, including the one discussed below, the results indicated that the prototype was representative of their archetypal pool. Furthermore, the follow up interviews revealed many relevant characteristics that were not evident solely from the quantitative data. Table 2 shows the quantitative profile of the C1 archetypal pool. Although this gives us some indication of who is in the sub-segment, it is impersonal and gives no real indication of who these people may actually be. 4.3 Archetypal profile In order to present the final profile, we develop a narrative from the results of the quantitative questionnaire and the in-depth interview describing the prototype. This humanizes the subsegments resulting in a rich profile of the people in each segment. Although this format is similar to the more traditional Pen Portrait (Ratneshwar and Shocker, 1988), there are a number of improvements that need to be noted: 1. We are able to avoid approximations and over generalisations. For example, if we were describing the C1 archetypal pool we’d say that the majority of C1 think climate change is real and it is caused by man. However, this is not entirely accurate. When we interviewed the C1 prototype the results indicated that while she believed climate change is caused by mankind, she also believes that the climate will always change because of a natural cycle. 2. The prototype is given the opportunity to explain why they think and act as they do. The original C1 profile showed that the respondents were apathetic towards climate 16 change actions. However, when given a chance to elaborate on this, the prototype explained that she strongly supported any pro-environmental activities. The problem was that she felt many government policies directly threatened the well being of her family by targeting their weekly budget. Therefore, she was unable to support many policies such as a carbon tax. 3. The researcher is given the opportunity to describe what marketing communications are most likely to engage the sub-cluster. During the third section of the in-depth interview the prototype showed strong support for communications that focus on the impacts climate change may have on farmers. A simple pen portrait is unable to provide this level of detail. Therefore, the researcher will often have no real direction with which to begin developing further advertising research or activities. Table 3 shows the narrative developed by the researchers for the C1 prototype to whom we allocated the pseudonym, Angela. All of the information in the narrative was taken from either Angela’s questionnaire results, or from the Archetypal interview. The bolded text indicates information that was uncovered in the interview. This was also true for the other prototypes, and gives the reader some indication of the value of the follow up research. 5. Conclusion Effective and reliable market profiling techniques are an important part of market research. Successful marketing strategies can often rely on having an in depth understanding of who constitutes a target market (Wansink, 2000). The archetypal prototyping technique outlined in this paper offers a more nuanced consumer profiling process than traditional techniques that may overlook important underlying trends within a market. For practitioners, this new procedure may be beneficial for three reasons. Firstly, having a better understanding of the underlying motivations that drive target market behaviour may allow more appropriate marketing strategies to be developed. Secondly, for mixed methods research involving quantitative then qualitative research, archetypal prototyping may uncover important preliminary findings that can be utilized to help direct follow up focus groups or in-depth 17 interviews. As the number of archetypal interviews required is minimal, this can be a cost effective way of ensuring that extensive follow-up qualitative research is focused on those areas that are most crucial to understanding consumer behaviour or attitudes. The final benefit for practitioners using archetypal prototyping is that this procedure can provide a more human description of market segments than is possible using purely quantitative research. On the other hand, it is has more statistical reliability than is perhaps the case with some qualitative research strategies, this can be important when presenting research to clients that prefer research with a strong statistical basis. The motivation for this research was to perform a methodological improvement of Morris and Schmolze’s (2006) consumer understanding framework. The results of this paper present a number of interesting findings. 5.1 Conclusions for researchers Firstly, we found that the archetypal prototyping technique provided a richer description of the Cautious and Disengaged segments than what was identified in the replication of Maibach et al.’s (2011) study. While the original segmentation analysis identified a series of consistent attitudes, our results showed evidence that there may be a number of different motivations for these attitudes occurring. For example, the C1 archetypal pool and its prototype, Angela, showed that this group of people were cautious of climate change action because they felt it may threaten their family’s fiscal situation. However, this differed from the results of the C2 and C3 prototypes. The C2 sub-cluster were opinionated professionals that considered themselves to be well informed on the climate change debate. The results of the corresponding interview indicated that the C2 prototype felt he was being ignored by the government. This is why he is cautious of climate change action. Different again, the C3 archetypal prototype was a less informed retiree. His caution of climate change was driven by underlying apathy. Although he supports climate change action, he doesn’t think he is the man for the job. The second finding was that the prototypes were representative of the archetypal pools. As stated in the introduction, traditional market profiling techniques can often lead to issues of 18 over-generalising and subjective interpretations of data (Morris and Schmolze, 2006; Wansink, 2000). However, by identifying real individuals that embody the salient features of a market segment, it is possible to reduce these limitations and increase the level of information we have for a segment. Although Morris and Schmolze (2006) suggested in their original study that the archetypal prototypes would in fact be representative, they provided no evidence to support this claim. However, this study shows the technique is valid. The third finding is that follow-up archetypal interviews with the prototypes are a useful tool for uncovering important data. While a good preliminary profile of a prototype may be developed using their quantitative responses, we found that the qualitative interviews gave the researchers a much better understanding of who the prototype was and why they act and feel the way they do. Perhaps the most beneficial part of the qualitative interview was the ability to test preliminary marketing communications. By exposing the respondent to archetypal stimuli we were able to test what words, images and climate change messages were most likely to engage the respondent (Randazzo, 2006). As the prototype is representative of their archetypal pool we can then translate these results into what will be effective for the majority of the sub-segment. Our final finding was the benefit the new methodology gave to the validity of this technique. Morris and Schmolze’s (2006) original study was an innovative way of selecting prototypes from a data set. However, the steps used by Morris and Schmolze were, at times, limited. We found that LCA and multiple discriminant analyses used in this study had fewer of these limitations than with the previously used K-means and single discriminant procedures. We also found that the final two discriminant analyses used by Morris and Schmolze (2006) gave a misleading indication of who may be the most appropriate prototype. Instead, we found that identifying the most centrally located members using their functional scores gave a much better idea of who would ideally represent the archetypal pool. 5.2 Other issues for further research Nonetheless, there are a few limitations of this technique that should be noted. Wansink (2000) suggested that prototyping may be too in-depth. This can lead to communication 19 strategies that are too specific. However, it is possible to overcome this by running a thematic analysis over all three prototypes from a broad segment. Reoccurring themes may be treated as applicable to the whole cluster. For example, the Cautious prototypes all responded well to communications about Australian farmers and farmland. Another issue is the impact of missing data on the final solution. Although the LCA can model missing data, this is not the case with discriminant analysis. Missing scores need to be converted to mean scores, meaning respondents with a large amount of data missing will be moved to the centre of the cluster. For this reason, we suggest closely studying the results of any potential prototype to ensure that they do not have an unacceptable number of missing scores. Despite these issues, archetypal prototyping is an innovative technique that has the potential to produce a better understanding of target segments in a population. The results of this paper indicate that, with the improvements to Morris and Schmolze’s (2006) original methodology, this technique allows researchers to objectively and accurately identify respondents to be prototypes. By interviewing these people, one is able to build comprehensive archetypal profiles of broader market segments. From this, advertisers and marketers may develop effective communication strategies. 20 Tables and Figures Table 1: The five steps of the archetypal prototype analysis procedure Stages 1 Stage name Initial segmentation analysis Details Run the initial cluster analysis using attitudinal items Select segments for prototyping 2 Identify core members Run a multiple discriminant analysis on all clusters. Remove the members from the selected segments with the lowest probability of group membership (approximately two-thirds of the segment) 3 Archetypal pool analysis Run a cluster analysis on the remaining members using socio-demographic items Select the number of clusters or 'archetypal pools' 4 Select the prototypes Run a stepwise discriminant analysis on the archetypal pools Save functional scores Select members that are closest to the mean functional scores 5 Develop archetypal profiles Conduct qualitative interviews with the selected respondents 21 Figure 1: Scatterplot of the of the discriminant function scores for the Cautious archetypal pools. The results show three distinct clusters supporting the results of the second LCA. 22 Figure 2: Scatterplot of the of the discriminant function scores for the Disengaged Archetypal pools. 23 Figure 3: Scatterplot of the final scores for archetypal pool C1. - The cross hair shows the cluster centroid. The circled members are the possible prototypes for the pool. 24 Table 2: Mean scores of the C1 archetypal pool. C1 (means) Is climate change real? (1 = yes, 2 = not sure, 3 = no) 1.35 Is it man made or natural? (1 = yes, 2 = other, 3 = natural) 1.04 Age Income 39.5 $46,800.00 Education Trade or TAFE qualification Gender (1 = male) 0.17 Employment status 76% - Stay at home mothers/ part time work Household description 61% - Couple with at least one child living at home 25 Table 3: The archetypal prototype narrative Angela Angela (35) is one of many married women in Australia that are cautious of climate change. She lives with her husband and two children in the outskirts of a capital city. Angela has completed studies at TAFE for her work in administration. However, she now takes pride in being a stay at home mother. This puts extra strain on her family’s weekly budget meaning that they must be aware of any extra costs in their lives. Angela believes in climate change, she also believes it should be a high priority issue for the Australian government, however, only if it will have a moderate impact on the economy. She goes out of her way to reduce her personal impact on the environment with simple cost effective methods such as having her own vegetable garden and recycling. She believes that every person can make a difference by taking these small steps. Angela has noticed the area around her being affected by climate change and she is concerned about the impact this will have on her family’s health. Despite these beliefs, Angela is a Liberal voter. She believes that direct action policies are the best way for Australia to reduce climate change. Angela is not interested in Julia Gillard or Bob Brown’s policies on the issue. She feels that these politicians have lied to her and they will do it again. If she is to support climate change policies it can only be from people that are trustworthy. People that are trustworthy are those politicians that are looking out for the good of her family. Angela is threatened by new taxes as she comes from a single income household. Any new taxes may threaten her family and as a mother this is unacceptable. Instead, Angela thinks that the government should support companies that have a low impact on the environment, while high polluters should be punished. Angela is likely to use Newspapers and pop media to find her information on climate change. She is willing to listen to people like Ray Hadley and Alan Jones but doesn’t like to hear from celebrities such as Cate Blanchett. Angela does not often discuss climate change with her friends. However, she feels that a few of them follow her thoughts on the matter. Along with her tendency towards popular media, Angela wants to hear from people she feels are trustworthy scientists, such as the experts that are interviewed on popular current affair programs. Angela responds to marketing communications that show how we can help Australia and her children. For example, how can she help protect Australian farmers from drought and other extreme weather conditions? Alternatively, she responds to messages that encourage the protection of her children’s future. Angela also engages with positive messages that empower her such as: “You can help stop climate change” or “Help make a better life for everyone in the future”. These sorts of messages make her feel like she is a better person when she contributes. In summary, Angela feels that climate change is a major issue that needs to be addressed soon. She is not sure that Australia will have any real impact, but, it is worth it to try and secure a future for her kids. She does not support the current government’s policies. Direct action and punishing high polluters is the way to go, not taxing her average family. 26 References Babin, B., & Babin, L. (2001). Seeking something different? A model of schema typicality, consumer affect, purchase intentions and perceived shopping value. Journal of Business Research, 54, 8996. Cantor, N., & Mischel, W. (1979). Prototypicality and personality: effects on free recall and personality impressions. Journal of Research in Personality, 13(2), 187-205. Donoghue, S. (2000). Projective techniques in consumer research. Journal of Family Ecology and Consumer Sciences, 28(1), 47-53. Hair, J., William, C., Babin, B., & Anderson, R. (2010). Multivariate Data Analysis (7th ed.). New Jersey: Pearson. Hofstede, G. (1997). Cultural differences in teaching and learning. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 10(1), 301-320. Hogg, M., A., & Reid, S., A. (2006). Social identity, self categorization, and the commuincation of group norms. International Communication Association, 16(1), 7-30. Knox, J. (2004). From archetypes to reflective function. Journal of Analytical Psychology, 49, 1-19. Loken, B., Barsalou, L., & Joiner, C. (2008). Categorization theory and research in consumer psychology. In C. Haugtvedt, P. Herr & F. Kardes (Eds.), Handbook of Consumer Psychology (pp. 133-163). New York: Taylor and Francis group. Madgison, J., & Vermunt, J. (2002). Latent class models for clustering: A comparison with K-means. Canadian Journal of Marketing Research, 20(1), 37-44. Maibach, E., W., Leiserowitz, A., Roser-Renouf, C., & Mertz, C., K. (2011). Identifying like-minded audiences for global warming public engagement campaigns: An audience segmentation analysis and tool development. PLoS ONE, 6(3), e17571. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017571 Maso-Fleischman, R. (1997). Archetype research for advertising: A Spanish language example. Journal of Advertising Research, 37(5), 81-84. McCutcheon, A., L. (1987). Latent Class Analysis. California: Sage. Morris, L., & Schmolze, R. (2006). Consumer archetypes: A new approach to developing consumer understanding frameworks. Journal of Advertising Research, 46(3), 289-300. Peattie, K., & Peattie, S. (2009). Social marketing: A pathway to consumption reduction. Journal of Business Research, 62(2), 260-268. Randazzo, S. (2006). Subaru: the emotional myths behind the brands growth. Journal of Advertising Research, 46(1), 8-17. 27 Rapaille, G., C. (2007). The Culture Code: An Ingenious Way To understand Why People Around The World Live And Buy As They Do. New York: Random House Inc. Ratneshwar, S., & Shocker, A. (1988). The application of prototypes and categorization theory in marketing: Some problems and alternative perspectives. Advances in Consumer Research, 15(2), 280-285. Shalit, R. (1999). The Return of the Hidden Persuaders: Part 1. Salon Retrieved 1/12/2010, 2010 Wansink, B. (2000). New techniques to generate key marketing insights. Marketing Research, 12(2), 2836. Woodside, A., G., Stood, S., & Miller, K., E. (2008). When consumers and brands talk: Storytelling theory and research in psychology and marketing. Psychology and Marketing, 25(2), 97-145. 28