1 >> Jen Haas: I think we're ready to begin and we've worked out all of our technical difficulties. So my name is jen Haas, and I have the pleasure of introducing today's guest author, Leonard Mlodinow. Leonard received his Ph.D. in theoretical physics from the University of California Berkeley, and was an Alexander Vaughn Humboldt Fellow at the Max Plank Institute and currently teaches at the California Institute of Technology. He is also the author of "The Drunkard's Wife" and co author with Stephen hawking of "A Brief History of Time and Grand Design." I'm going to now turn it over to Leonard, and he can probably correct the pronounce nation of his name. Thanks. >> Leonard Mlodinow: Thank you. Thanks for waiting so patiently. Sorry about that. All right. Well, now I'm going to ask you so do some work. So first you're waiting and now you're working. It's all voluntary. So feel free not to do it. It's also anonymous, so don't worry about -- you will be graded, but no one's going to know who they're grading. Oh, we never, with all this, we never had that talk about what to do with -all right. We're going to have to figure that out, all right? So let's just start by dividing you experiments. We'll divide you into half. And I guess if you guys call say from here. So if you look down arm, you're group one. If you feel group two, all right? into two groups, okay. It be two two groups. It so it should be roughly yourself -- you guys be group one. Let's and you see that you're on this side of my like you're on this side of my arm, you're And also, if people are watching online, you can just pick a group one or two and if you want to participate in the experiment, you can. When I say look away, close your eyes, don't look at the screen. And then you can also participate and see how this works on you. So all right, let's go. So the first thing is group two, turn away from the screen or, if you're online, don't look at the screen if you want to be in group two. If you want to be in group two, right. Now, group one, please read this silently and write down your answer. I'm not going to give you as much time as I usually do, because we're so late starting. 2 Okay. Now, please read this question and write down your answer. Okay. Write down your answer. Okay. Now, group one, please turn away from the screen or, if you're online and you call yourself group one, don't look at the screen. Group two, you can turn forward. Group two online, you can look at the screen. Okay. Now, please read this silently and record your answer. Okay. Now, please read this silently and record your answer. Okay. Now, you can all look at the screen again. Thank you. And we'll do one more experiment. I'd like you to study these words, and I'm going to read them to you as well. I'll give you a second to study them. Verbally and then I'm going to read them to you. Candy, sour, sugar, bitter, good, taste, tooth, nice, honey, soda, chocolate, heart, cake, eat, pie. All right. So another test coming. Yes, there is. So ready, here we go. But this isn't the test so don't do anything yet. I just want you to think of the words that you can recall from the list. Don't write anything. Okay. Now, here are three words. Somewhere between zero and all three of these were on the list. Okay. I want you to write down, I want you to think back and I want only if you have a very concrete memory of me reading it and/or seeing it on the screen and you're confident of that memory, then write down the words if they were on the list. It might be that, as I said, maybe none of them, maybe two of them, maybe one of them, whichever of these three words were on the list and you have a concrete memory of it, please write it down now. Okay. Write down your last entries, and okay. Now, if you were in group one, please put a one on the top of your paper and circle it so we know it's not any data. And group two, put a 2 and circle it in case these get a little shuffled and I would ask group one to pass your papers to this aisle, to your left and group two, also pass to your left, to the center aisle here and we'll collect them. So normally, I give the instructions to the collectors in secret so you don't know what they're going to be doing with these things. But given the commotion that we had, I guess I'd better just tell them now. But actually they're not listening. Okay. Tell you collectors what to do. Okay. So I would like you to keep group one and group two separate for the first thing. I want you to give me 3 the mean, the average of what group one said in their guess and what group two said, but ignore the yes and the no from the first question. And also, then, for the tallies, put them together and just tell me how many people said each word. And I will call for that later, and it will be something that we'll talk about. All the 400 you you right. Now, you know how the experiment is done, sort of. Okay. Oh, by way, throw in any outliers, because I have people put a million or negative or, you know, all kinds of nice jokes. But since you're taking the mean, better don't count those, okay? I know, depends what kind of pranksters guys are. So I'm going to talk about subliminal, how your unconscious mind rules your behavior, and I'll start by defining what I mean by unconscious behavior or unconscious processes in your mind. And those are processes that occur automatically with no effort on your part, and no awareness, intention, and little or no control. Now, it's not always a very sharp line. Sometimes there's a little bit of awareness or a little bit of control. But generally, these are the characteristics of what we call the unconscious. So it's automatic. Your brain does it for you automatically. You don't have to work at it, at least consciously. You're not aware of it. You don't will it, and you can't control it. You can't turn it off. And I'll give you some examples of how that works. And as a result of this, with he don't understand often what is influencing our behavior, our social perceptions, our memories and so we can't avoid perceiving the world the way we do. And finally, I want to remind you or I want to tell you that this is not the Freudian unconscious. The Freudian unconscious, very emotional unconscious, very emotional picture of the unconscious, and it was hidden from people in Freud's theory due to motivational reasons and could be revealed through therapy, through introspection. And this new unconscious that we're talking about is not that way at all. It's outside your awareness because of the parts of the brain that it operates in and these are outside of your awareness for anatomical, structural reasons of your brain. So it's not the unconscious that Freud popularized but something really very, very different. And the study of it is called social neuroscience. Social neuroscience evolved 4 in the early 2000s. So it's a very new science and it's a menage-a-trois or household of three, represented by these pairs of feet here, and these feet on your left represent social psychology. Social psychology is the study of how we interact as human beings. And our social interactions are important to our species. And these feet on the right represent cognitive psychology, which is a psychology of how we think and how we deliberate. And they're tied together in social neuro science by neuroscience, and especially a technology that developed that was made, became widely available in the mid 1990s called functional magnetic resonance imaging or FMRI, and that is just like the MRI your doctor does, except has an added element. So when you go in and have an MRI of your brain, the doctor can get a 3D picture of your brain, rotate it, fly in and out, look at different parts. Because with FMRI, it always tells you what part of your brain is functioning at any given time, which shows the activation inside your brain. So we can now watch people as they're perceiving visual information, as they're feeling emotions, as they're thinking. And so what this has allowed scientists to do is to connect these behavioral studies that were done in psychology that maybe was kind of a soft science and make it more of a hard science by connecting it to the functional anatomy of your brain and seeing how the effects arise in different parts of your brain and learn more of the mechanics of what's going on. And my favorite illustrative study of the power of FMRI was this done at Berkeley by Jacques Gallant, and he showed his subjects a series of slides, like these, and he took -- while they were in an FMRI machine, so you can wear these goggles that project slides for you, project images and also they do the test with sounds. So you lay in an FMRI machine, and they can show you all kinds of things. And while they were, his subjects were looking at these slides, he was taking readings with their brain, these electromagnetic readings of FMRI. And he had a computer use those readings to try to reconstruct what the people were seeing. So a kind of mind reading. And he had a database of 6 million pictures that his computer could choose from, and the computer made guesses based -- with no data from what the students were actually -- subjects, sorry, at university too long. Subjects 5 were actually seeing and but busied purely on the FMRI. So let's see how close the computer guesses were. So I guess that's quite dramatic. I would be afraid to be in one of these machines. But fortunately, they can't do this to you as you're walking by. You have to be in the machine so you don't have to worry yet about people, scientists reading what you're thinking. So I'm going to talk about two main things in the rest of the lecture. The two illustrations of the unconscious. And I divided them this way, because this unconscious in vision and hearing is a great way to illustrate conscious processes. It's not as relevant to our social lives as the second, which is a social unconscious, but it's very, it's something that's very easy to illustrate in a talk like this. And the main point is that the same kind of processes that operate here, in the vision and hearing, also operate in the social unconscious. And the points are that our perceptions both our visual perceptions, our social perceptions, our perceptions as consumers, and our perceptions of memory are all constructed by our unconscious mind from limited data. So what you see is not really what's there. It's a construction that your mind creates by taking limited data and filling it in. And the same is true of your judgments of people, judgments of stocks, and all the other judgments we make in our everyday social life. And our unconscious does that using things like context, expectation, even your desires. Even what you want to see. And the other point is that this, the way we experience the world is largely driven by these unconscious processes, rather than by your conscious processes, or at least that there's no such thing as a purely conscious, objective perception of the world. It's all fed into by your unconscious. So let me show you, illustrate some of this in vision and hearing. Look at this picture, especially the guys in the room. This depends a lot on the projector, but I want you to see if you can find something very unusual, even bizarre in this picture. So in the fore ground is what psychologists call a high interest image. And somewhere else is something that's different. So how many, now I'm not talking about the ladder, okay. How many of you saw 6 something very unusual or bizarre in that picture, raise your hand. How many of you didn't? So it's about three quarters of those who vote the didn't, and a quarter of you do. That's pretty typical. Okay. So when you see something, when light hits your retina, that image does not get transmitted directly to your conscious mind. This is a good illustration of that. There's something called attention that has to be focused on different elements of the picture before you can consciously perceive it. And with this high interest image in the fore ground did, at least for the guys in the room, was it distracted your attention. Even when you consciously decide to overcome that, when I said look for something unusual, it's hard to do. Let's go back and let's look at it again. King Kong. The face of King Kong is in this picture. Look by the ladder. Okay. So right there is the face of King Kong. This projector's pretty good. You can see it. You can see it pretty clearly if you're in the center of the room. But it's hard to see because your mind -- your unconscious mind keeps you looking at this part instead. Actually, at this part. So the question is, what do you consciously experience versus what is coming into your eye. So let me start at the beginning. And show you what you actually see. This is what you perceive at a typical road side. You see some brush on the side of the road, the trees, the sign, the road, okay. You perceive that as being a very clear and real image. Let me show you now what data looks like as it actually hits your eye. If you did no unconscious processing, this is what you would see, okay. So all except for this yellow dot in the center, which is put there just to show the fixation point. So this is a person's right eye, fixating right here. As you can see, only for a small area around that is it clear. Otherwise, it's very fuzzy, and there's a black spot here, called the blind spot, where the optic nerve attaches to the retina, and, of course, it's two-dimensional data, it's not three-dimensional. And if you have to go through the world every time you saw a frame of the outer world and it looked like this, and you had to process it consciously to look like this, you'd get eaten by lions, Tigers, bears, whatever was out there to eat us back then, or you'd never catch the rabbit you're trying to get for dinner. So our unconscious mind developed to take data like this and turn it into data like that. We do it automatically. We don't have to stop and think. And happens very quickly. And we have no control over it. 7 So try and look at me or look at some part of the room or look at these pictures and try to see the fuzziness and not do the processing. Okay. You really can't do that. Let me just give you an example of one way your mind, one trick your mind does to fill in the data. And it also will illustrate again the lack of control you have over it. This is a picture of a checkerboard, and you'll see two squares, two rectangles outlined, one labeled A and one labeled B. You'll see that B is a white square and A is a black square. The square B looks to you to be lighter than the square A. Your mind is telling you that. But in reality, A and B are identical. So the rectangle A and rectangle B are identical from a physics point of view, but they're not identical from the perception point of view. So the light that hits your eyes from A and B is identical. The color is identical. But your mind, in its processing, without telling you, without asking you, makes them look different, because of the context. That's very useful to you, because if you have to figure out, if you -- if this looked the same as this and you had to reason, however, by counting the pattern out to say oh, I guess it's a white square, that would slow you down. Instead, you automatically see it as a white square. Let me help you not see it that way. Look at it. I'm going to take the context away, and look what happens to A and B when I take the context away. So now, A and B, you can see them realistically, out of context, that they are the same. But if you look at the A and B over here, they still look different. And you can't look at these and have them look different, nor can you look at these and have them look the same, because this is outside of your conscious control. So you probably recognize this fellow, though you probably rarely see him upside down. And if you look at these -- because we're such a social species, one of the modules in our unconscious is tuned particularly to the human face. So the human face is very important to us as we go through the world. We try to predict people's intentions, their emotions, what they're thinking. This is a natural thing that we do in order to have social interactions that work smoothly and it's very important to humans. And so with he do special unconscious processing of human faces. We have to be able to recognize our friends from our enemies and so on. 8 The thing is that this unconscious processing, this unconscious module of our brain does not often see people upside down. And so this is a very dramatic illustration of how specialized our brain is, our unconscious processing is to our social life. Let me show you this picture, I'm going to flip it over for you. It probably looks pretty normal, both of them probably look pretty normal to you up side down, because the unconscious part of your brain has not really kicked in yet in terms of identifying this face. But when I turn it right side up, one of them looks horribly disfigured. So if you met this guy in the street, you'd go whoa, what's wrong with him. This guy is normal. But when they're upside down, that hardly matters to you. So this big difference, this unconscious processing that's going on in your mind to identify the human face. Watch it one more time. I'll turn it over. Watch as it turns into a grotesque distortion as it turns over. This is simply made, you can do this to anybody. Just take the eyes and the mouth and flip them upside down. But if you ran into someone on the street, you'd go oh, whoa. Unless you're standing on your head and you go, hey, looks like a normal guy. Let me give you one example of hearing. I hope this works because we had our technical difficulties. We'll see if this works. Remember, I'm saying this unconscious processing has similar, has similar functions across different modalities. So I want to illustrate one in hearing. This is a song by Led Zeppelin called "stairway to heaven." Let me see if it plays. If there's a bustle in your hedgerow, don't be alarmed now. It's just a spring clean for the may queen. Yes, there are two paths you can go by, but in the long run, there's still time to change the road you're on. All right, very nice. Now the question is, was Led Zeppelin, were they clever enough to design a song that makes sense if you play it forwards and also if you play it backwards. I don't think this song makes much sense played forwards. The question is, if you play it backwards, does it make sense? I'm going to play it backwards for you. There are like eight or nine lines. I'll give a big prize to anyone who can read it back, the eight or nine lines of the backwards song so let's try that. 9 [Playing song backwards]. >> Leonard Mlodinow: Can you recite the words? Did you get the message? Did you hear the eight or nine lines or four lines or does it sound like gibberish? It sounds a lot like gibberish, right? Okay. So that's one perception of the world. Now I'm going to play it for you again, but provide a context, just like the checkerboard, provide a context for those squares and you saw them differently. I'm going to play this song for you again with the words so look at the words and listen to the song and see if your perception of the same audio file changes now. Oh. Here so my sweet Satan. The one whose little path would make me sad whose power is Satan. He'll give you, give you 666. There pass a tool shed where he made us suffer, sad Satan. So with a little help, it makes sense to you now, huh? All right. So now I've given you the context and the same physical phenomenon is perceived completely differently. So let me play it for you again, and I want you to look at the words. The question is which is realty, right? Gibberish or this. I'm going to play it for you again with the words. Don't close your eyes or look away. Watch the words, listen to it. I want you to go back, though. I want you to consciously override this, watch the words but hear it as gibberish. Watch the words but pay no attention to them. Or as the judges say, please disregard this testimony, okay? Let's see if that works. Oh here so my sweet Satan. The one whose little path would make me sad whose power is Satan. He'll give you, give you 666. There was a little toolshed where he made us suffer, sad Satan. >> Leonard Mlodinow: So were you successful, or did you hear the -because you can't really do it. You can't override it just like you override when the guy goes up and talks about -- or when you read in if you're a juror, about how this guy's guilty or an asshole. Can I asshole at micro -- I don't know. Cut that out, okay? Or bad guy. you go on trial. Oh, yeah, I heard that, but I can disregard that. can't. So that's part of the unconscious data that ->>: no. Oh, can't the paper, say And then But you Are you saying it's impossible or it's almost -- >> Leonard Mlodinow: It's between impossible and almost impossible. So in 10 this case, I don't know. They do study it, they have things called bistable images and like a vase or a face, and you're trying to generally have little control or usually no for instance, in different, when people try something that can look change it back and forth. You control. So anyway, but it depends on each phenomena has to be analyzed individually. Okay. So I had another, but I won't do that. I took that slide out because I was trying to go faster, but I was going to play it for you without the word at all and let me just tell you, like I'm a veteran of hearing this now. First of all, the backward words, as presented here, make more sense to me than the forward words. And it sounds just as real when I play it now, because I've heard it so many times. So in our social perception, the same things happen to build our picture of the world. I have a picture here of a few bus drivers. But just to illustrate the point when you see a bus driver, you don't stop and puzzle out what is his job using the data that you have. You automatically know it's a bus driver. If you see a woman racing or a bus, you know what she feels. You don't have to think about what is this animal using my logic and the data. What is she thinking, what is she feeling? It's all automatic to us and it helps us in our social interactions. If you see a guy in the street or a woman in a blue suit with a gun, you know what their job is. You know how to act toward them. And if you see captains of industry or politicians and you've heard something about them in the news or a defendant that you're hearing about in a trial, you get limited data on the person, but your mind does some filling in to make a much clearer picture than you actually have just as it does in physical perception and the audio perception. And the same thing with all sorts of people that you meet in your daily interactions, unless you know them intimately. So the point of this is that our social perception is also constructed by our minds employing the same tricks. Context, prior knowledge, belief, and even desire. Let me give you an example. Since it's an election year. There was a study done where subjects were brought in to a laboratory and shown a lot of women's faces, and they were asked to judge the competence of the women's faces, and the scientists studied what makes a woman look more competent. Then they 11 brought a few more hundred subjects into their experiment, and they gave them flyers for political candidates. They each got two flyers. One for a Democratic candidate and one for a Republican candidate. I know, I shouldn't put this slide up so soon, because you're all staring at it. I'll get to this part in a minute. One for a Democratic candidate, one for a Republican candidate. And they had, they listed the party, some of their views, their qualifications and a picture. And they were asked to judge who would you vote for. The Republican or the Democrat? The trick is that half of them saw -- the researchers had hired a Hollywood makeup artist and a photographer to create two versions of each candidate based on their earlier research of what makes a woman look competent. One version was a more competent looking one. And one was a less competent looking one, which I've illustrated here, but these aren't the actual pictures from the paper, because the research paper didn't include the pictures. But so in one case, half the people saw the Republican in her more competent image against a Democrat in her less competent looking one and half saw the Democrat in her more competent against the Republican in her less competent. And then they just had them vote. The question is what kind of vote swing do you get? And they got a vote swing of 14 percent. So she won, the Republican won 58 percent when she had her good picture. But with only 44 percent when she had her less good picture. So this shows that at least the swing voters, who aren't staunch Democrats or Republicans, as they're looking at the flyer, and taking in all this information about the views on the issues and their experience, they're also taking in the face and making a judgment based on how competent looking the person is. And this is, again, an automatic, effortless, a judgment that they're unaware of. They don't know that they're judging based on looks. They think they're looking at the data. Well, this is, of course, just in a laboratory. So if you're skeptical, you may say surely it doesn't affect real elections, right? Could our real elections be so fragile and decided by such a factor? Well, a psychologist named Alexander Todorov at Princeton decided to find out. In 2006, he gathered pairs of photos. These here, I don't know if you guys, anyone recognize these? This is the 2006 Wisconsin senatorial election. Russ Feingold against the loser, who we forget the loser's name. So just like in sports. He came in 12 second. So we remember this one. But they showed them pairs of -- he gathered pairs of photos like this on hundreds of gubernatorial, Congressional and senatorial elections. And showed them to subjects, asked them one question. Who looks more competent. Nothing to do about politics. Who looks more competent. Maybe you'd say this guy. Next, this guy. Next. They'd all get rated. He gathered statistics on who looked more competent, and he said let me predict the outcome of the 2006 elections without using any political science, no polling data, just look. I'm going to predict in every case that the more competent looking one won. Okay. So how successful do you think this guy could have been based purely on that? 70 percent. So he got 70 percent of the elections right by ignoring all politics and picking the person who looks more competent. So this does seem to be an effect that happens in the real world. One other kind of data that your unconscious mind takes in is tough. This slide illustrates four different kinds of primates engaged in grooming behavior. And these, all except for these primates in the center, these other three spend an hour or two a day doing it, much more than they actually need to do, because it's a way of creating bonds and sense of cooperation and trust among the individuals. Even in forming alliances. These primates also have that tendency. We have nerves built into our, especially into our face and our forearms that sense a certain kind of touch, transmit that to the pleasure centers of your brain that have nothing to do with deciding, feeling what you're touched, but just the emotional, pleasant feeling of being touched. That's because our evolutionary history, touch has always been a way we communicate. And this still affects us today in our everyday social life, in our financial life and all the decisions that we make. And it's very interesting study's been done to see how strong this effect is. My favorite study was done in France, where a three handsome Frenchmen, represented by this guy, were asked to stand on a sunny street corner in northern France and proposition single young women who walked by. And they were given the script, which is a translation of the script, and they all read exactly the same script. Hi, my name is Antoine. Can I have your phone number, basically. And I'll call you later. And the question was, the 13 difference was half of the women, these young Frenchmen, gave them a very light, half second or less touch on the shoulder or the forearm as they talked to them. And in half, they didn't. The question was, how would this affect their success rate. And the answer to the question is it doubled it. They went from ten percent to 20 percent. So this, of course, this is a situation of where touch is more directly related to the possible sexual experience they might have later. So you might ask, what about in very non-sexual situations. And this has been replicated in many other different contexts. For instance, waiters and waitresses, they did a similar experiment where they gave a light and subtle touch to their customers versus no touch, and they watch what happened to the tips. The tips went up from 14.5% to 17%. By the way, in all these studies more of the of these studies, people are debriefed afterwards, the subjects are debriefed, and they don't remember or didn't even register having been touched. So this is a very subtle effect. Also, when the waiters and waitresses were asked to give a touch as they were recommending the special, they got people's acceptance to go up from 40 percent to 60 percent, which this little subliminal persuasion. Finally, one more study here. People were asked in a mall if they would take five minutes out to do a survey. And they raised the number of people who agreed to cooperate from about half to about three quarters. Finally, before we get to the studies on you guy, I want to talk a little bit about self. This is Salvador Dali, who famously said, every morning upon awakening, I experience a supreme pleasure, that of being Salvador Dali. And I ask myself, wonderstruck, what prodigious thing will he do today, this Salvador Dali? And he kind of sounds like a pompous jerk, but I like this quote, but this is what our unconscious mind is helping us, telling us every day. Our unconscious mind, one of its roles is to give us the courage to face the world, to face in the old days, I suppose, extreme cold or heat with no shelter, to another day of finding food when it's scarce, of running from predators and whatever else people used to do. Or today, to start a business, spent hundred hour weeks for five years trying to get your business off the ground, to face six months of chemotherapy or go into medical school or whatever project you're working on now that probably, if you looked at realistically, you'd feel daunted. But it helps us to overcome these things 14 and it often is a self-fulfilling prophecy, that with this courage, we can do it. I'll tell you in a minute about how we do it, but let me give you some indications of exactly how strong the effect is. Let's start with high school kids. If you have a high school kid at home, you probably know if you say are you above average, the kid will say yes. Well, there was a survey of 3 million high school kids that asked them many, many questions. One of them was that. Guess what percent of them said that they're above average? 100 percent. All right. What about, well, okay, so I'm sure they rounded. It was probably 99.99 percent. But what about their professors? Here's a college professor. They should know better so they were asked, is your work above average and what percentage of them said that their work was above average? 94 percent. How about your doctors? So they did a study of doctors who had diagnosed pneumonia, and they asked them, what's your confidence level in the pneumonia diagnosis? And on average, they said 80 percent. They were 80 percent certain it was pneumonia. In reality, follow-up studies showed that only 20 percent of the patients had pneumonia. And this affects us in business as well. CEOs who acquire firms typically overpay and part of their reasoning is thinking that they are better than the CEO who is currently running the firm and that will make a difference that's worth over paying for. Again, I'm showing you, these are the cognitive illusions that are kind of like the optical illusions or the hearing illusion I showed you earlier. I don't mean for the message to be that your conscious mind is steering you wrong. In 95 percent of your interactions, it's steering you right. Or 99 percent. It's what you really need. But then there's these equivalents or analogies of optical illusions that happen where it leads you astray. One of the ways it does that is through memory. It tweaks your memory. You remember the good things more than the bad things. In this study, students who were four years out of college were asked about their grades, but they had signed over their transcripts so they knew the researchers had the transcripts so they had no reason to lie and the researchers could compare their answers to the actual grades. 15 The point is this he remembered their As very well, I think 86 percent or 89 percent. Their Bs less well, their Cs less well. They hardly remembered their Ds at all. Only 29 percent. So I always tell my students, if your grades aren't as good as you like them to be, just wait. They'll get better. So finally, final study I want to talk about is just in another way that your mind helps you get a rosy picture of the world to confirm that you can accomplish what you need to, also to confirm your political world view. So when we analyze political data, we tend to look at it this way. And tend to confirm that we want to believe by the way we look at the data, whether it's about ourselves or about the world around us. And the point that is we use this thing called motivated reasoning. Motivated reasoning is to look at the world like a lawyer. If you're a lawyer, you want to show that your client is either guilty or not guilty, or if it's your client, I guess you want to show that the client is not guilty. Or if you're the prosecutor, guilty or whatever you want to prove. You have your conclusion first and now you're looking for evidence for it. And you tend to pooh-pooh evidence that goes against your conclusion. You either think it doesn't carry much weight, it's not important evidence, or the methodology is bad, or you're trying to poke holes in it. And you tend to over value evidence that's supportive. That's the same thing we do with ourselves and our beliefs when we're looking at data about ourselves or about things that we believe in. Or that we want to believe in. But it's very important to realize, we do that unconsciously. So if we would have just told ourselves I'm going to ignore this very convincing evidence because I want to believe this, you're not really going to believe this. You're going to accept the evidence. It's your unconscious mind doing it. You're not aware of it. You don't have control over it. That's what this study shows. They had people, they took subjects into the lab and gave them data and transcripts from a real civil court case in Texas where a motorcyclists was suing a car driver. And they had all the facts of the case and the transcripts, and they told half of them that they would be representing the motorcyclist, trying to get money. And half were going to represent the car driver, trying to pay as little as possible and they were going pair up and have to negotiate their own version of the 16 settlement and that the real settlement was somewhere between zero and $100,000. So they all digested this information with this advocate's had on, wanting to go in and represent one side or the other, and then just before they went in to do that, the researchers said wait a minute, wait a minute. One other thing. Take your advocate's hat off now. Of course, doesn't really mean anything to you. You were just assigned to represent someone in this game. So it's just a game. Shouldn't be hard to take that hat off. Think back about all this stuff you digested. Think about it objectively, forget any conflict of interest. I'm going to give you a cash bonus if you can tell me what the real judge awarded, based on whatever the fairness, whatever the thought process of fairness was of the real judge. Try and be fair, analyze the data fairly and tell me what the real judge awarded. If you're within $5,000, you get a cash bonus. So the question is, could they do this? Could they, now that their unconscious has employed motivated reasoning to analyze the data, could they go back and look at the data and overcome it, like I asked you to listen to the song with the words or not pay attention or look at the checkerboard and not notice that the squares -- and not think that the squares are different. Well, when they were asked to do that, the motorcyclist's, people who were going to defend the motorcyclist said $40,000 and the people were going to defend the driver of the car said, on average, $20,000. So it was a factor of two in their, quote, objective reasoning abilities based on what they had been assigned to do earlier. Let's see if this affects you. So can I have the data? In the first question I gave you, okay. So no one said point, huh? So I asked you to analyze this room and I gave you some limited data. I gave you these three pictures of the room and the surroundings, and I gave you, I said it was in Tahiti, a one bedroom bungalow, and blah, blah, blah. And then I asked you what would you pay for this room? Now I'm asking you in this picture, a very limited picture of what the room is, but I want you now to, in your mind, you're forming some conception of what the room is and how much it's worth. I wanted you to guess how much it's worth. Group one guessed on average, $909. At this point, I get gasps usually from group two. No, nothing? Are you still awake. I know it was a long time 17 before we got started. The reason I get gasps is because group two guessed on average $247. So I got a factor of four through my subliminal mind control on you guys. So the question is how did I do that? I've had up to a factor of, I think, 20. I think I had $200 versus 2500 or something once when I tried this. The question is what's the difference between you guys? The difference was question number one, okay? Question number one, group one said is this hotel room more than $5500 a night. In group two, the question was, is it more than $55 a night. I think it's an absurd question, at least to me. I haven't traveled that much, but I know the room is obviously more than 55 a night and it's not going to be 5500 a night. So probably in your yeses and nos, it's pretty unanimous that the answer to the first question is. And the second question is yes. But even though it's a silly question, it influenced your thought greatly. So if you're going to a website and they managed to say would you expect to pay $5500 for this room, and you're saying, what kind of an idiot question is that, and you say how much do you want to pay, there they go, they just jacked up what you're offering by a factor of four. So that's how your unconscious feeds into your social perception in your consumer behavior in this case. >>: How much was the room? >> Leonard Mlodinow: The room was 550. So being mathematically inclined, I just took the geometric mean. I pumped it up by a factor of ten, pumped it down by a factor of ten and see what happens. Okay. So what about your memory? So I was telling you how everything is constructed, your visual perception, your hearing, your social perception. I want to see if your memory, I want to illustrate how your memory is also constructed from limited data, from context and the gist and played back to you. So just when you're looking at the side of the road, you're seeing something fuzzy and you think you're seeing something clear, your memory is based on something very fuzzy, just a few little pieces of data and the gist of what actually happened and when you call on your memory to replay it, you're hearing something clear and concrete. So because of that, when I asked you, when I showed you these words and I asked 18 you which of these three are on there, you're playing back in your memory. I hope that you listened to me and didn't write it down unless you could really hear me saying it, picture on the screen and you're really confident. So because of that nice instruction, nobody said point, which was on the list, and how many of you remembered taste? I have 21 people remembered taste. And that was on the list. But what about sweet? So sweet is something that you could typically reconstruct in your mind, because a lot of these words have to do with sweet. Cake, pie, chocolate. And so this was called false memory, because your brain has recorded some data, some kind of descriptor saying sweet word and then when you call them back, of course, sweet is the epitome of sweet words. So even though, for taste, I got 21 of you remembering it, for sweet, I also got 21 of you remembering it. So you remembered equally the word that wasn't on the list as the word that was on the list. So I'm just going to end with a quote from Carl Jung, who I don't subscribe to a lot of what he said, but I love this quote, I think encapsulates this very well. The subliminal aspects of everything that happens to us may seem to play very little part of in our daily lives, but they are the almost invisible roots of our conscious thoughts. Thank you. >> Jen Haas: So are there any questions? >>: With this song thing, does that depend on time, like if I've heard it once and go and don't hear it again for a year, will that still ->> Leonard Mlodinow: So the backwards song, yeah. If you forget the words, or they fade from your memory, then you start to hear it more at gibberish. I know from personal experience, having heard that over some time. If you keep reinforcing it, then it sounds more and more real. And by the way, that happens in courtrooms all the time with memories. People thinking they remember something, reconstructing it like you did here from the gist, and then getting asked about it repeatedly, recalling it repeatedly, and it becomes more and more real, even if it's not. >>: The second thing I was curious what surprised you the most when you wrote the book? >> Leonard Mlodinow: Almost every chapter. I mean, it was amazing. It was fun writing the book. It was fun doing the research for the book, because, you 19 know, in memory and perception, and in the way we categorize people and prejudice, our non-verbal communication, our feeling of self, our emotions. Everywhere, there were these great surprising studies. I went whoa, I learned so much about me, which I hope I digested for you. But it took me four years to learn it all. So it was a huge volume of literature out there. But I can't even say. I think maybe the most outrageous things I learned had to do with some of the memory stuff and how the courts don't recognize it at all and maybe some of the political stuff and how we are influenced in ways we don't realize. You know if you buy the wrong wine because German music is playing in the background, this is one study people bought German and French music, playing on alternate days, and they got people to buy two-thirds German wine on the German wine and two-third French wine on the French days. And if you get home and go why did I buy this wine? I don't like German wine. Big deal, all right. But if the wrong guy gets elected because he looks good, then that's a bigger deal. >>: So realizing how limited we are, what's more useful, hubris or humility? >> Leonard Mlodinow: I think humility is much more useful. And I guess hubris, if you take it in the charitable sense of confidence is good, but over confidence is bad, or that's why you end up paying more for the company that you're going to buy than you should pay. >>: Does the concept of micro expressions, it's been glorified with some Hollywood kind of things in the series called [indiscernible]. Just curious if there's any ->> Leonard Mlodinow: So that's Paul Eckman's work, and he did a lot of very good work on lying. Remember, I talked about how we read each other's emotions from our faces, and also our posture. And he did some, decided to apply that to whether you can tell if people are lying. And but then what's happened over the last 10 or 20 years with his work, he formed a company. It's very hard to say what is, you know, true and false in that, because they don't really publish their work. And there hasn't been a lot of definitive work published on whether you can really tell whether someone's lying by their facial expressions or by these micro expressions, which are very short facial expressions. 20 There has been some, but not lot of it. jury is still out on that. It's more proprietary. So I think the But I tend to be a little bit skeptical, because if there were something -- I'm sure people are looking into it. I would think if there was something dramatic there, researchers outside of the proprietary circles would have published more on it. >>: The question was does your subconscious then show up on your micro -- >> Leonard Mlodinow: That's the claim, but we don't know. It seems to some extent in these micro expressions when you're lying, but how accurate you can be, I don't know. >>: How do you use this, or do you use this to help yourself be more objective and to help yourself from being manipulated? >> Leonard Mlodinow: Well, I do. I mean, a very simple maybe banal example is you go into a grocery store and you want to buy whatever beer, some fancy beer, and it's $5.99. The bottle is $5.99. Because it sounds like less to you than 6 dollars, and it sound like less, and it automatically sounds like less, because of the 5. You hear the 5 and not the 6. And I don't -- I know intellectually that it's the same, but it's hard to overcome the feeling that it's less. So when I see the $5.99, I automatically look away and go six dollars. This is a six dollar bottle or whatever. I talk to a friend who has seminars and he charges 1995 for the week end. 1,995 dollars. And I remember telling one of his assistants, you mean, my God, he gets $2,000 for those? She says no, 1995. If I was thinking about buying it, I would say to myself, oh, is it worth $2,000, because it sounds like what it is. So in that sense, you can use -once you're aware of these effects, you can use your conscious mind to try to overrule them in various ways. Another thing is with credit cards, people spend a lot more on credit cards because it's abstract and the process of handing over the money inhibits you from handing over the money. So if you think you're overspending on a credit card, you have to catch 21 yourself and realize and try and think, let me pretend I'm paying cash for this. And would I really pay that, or is it just too easy with the credit card? >>: What interest distortions to our social perception of other people? there something that we can do to [indiscernible] that? Is >> Leonard Mlodinow: Again, get more data on people. Get to know them and try and keep an open mind. But the unconscious influences are still going to be there. So in each individual case, you can try and focus on learning more about them, but they've found that in terms of categorization and prejudice, there are things that you can do if you're really serious about it, which is mainly if you have a prejudice against some group that you don't have a lot of contact with, is to seek contact with people in that group, especially common goals that you can strive for together. And that's -- and in studies, that seems to help you override your unconscious prejudice. And by the way, if you guys are a little bit techie, go to the site called IAT, the implicit associations test. If you type in IAT and Harvard, you can get to the website. You can take a test that can test your unconscious prejudices against black people, gay people, I think obese people, and you'll find that even if you're very fair-minded or even if you are in an ethnic group, you often, even perhaps usually have unconscious prejudices against that group based on being inundated and these messages through television and film. If you have a minute, it takes five minutes to take the test. score, compares you to other people and it's kind of cool. It gives you a >>: Did you research any people with photographic memory and how that would affect? >> Leonard Mlodinow: I talk in the book about a guy named Sharashefsky who never forgot anything, and it was bad because your mind gets cluttered. He had trouble even following. He could remember what you said 30 years ago word for word, but he had trouble following the opposite, following the gist of what people were saying. Or if he saw your face. If I see your face now, I make a gist of your face. I might mistake your face. I don't know you very well. If I see someone else. If you commit a crime, I might identify the wrong person if he looks like you. 22 And the context is that the police grab them so I'm thinking must be one of these guys and it looks like you. But he was so accurate or so specific in his memory that he would remember your face like this, if you turn a little bit, he'd have another picture of your face like this. If it's a different lighting, he had another picture of your face. So he had trouble even identifying faces. Because can you imagine going through a catalog of all these faces when you have a picture from every angle? So it caused him a lot of problems. So it's kind of, he did make a living remembering things as a Newmanist, they called it, but he had social issues. >>: Any comment on social media, where you've got basically a lot of people self-publishing snippets of their personality. Is the gap between their actual personality and perceived widening or shrinking? >> Leonard Mlodinow: Sorry? I didn't -- >>: People are publishing things about themselves on Facebook, things like that. >> Leonard Mlodinow: Oh, I don't know of any study of that. Because based on the other studies of how people perceive themselves, yeah, there would be. Of course, you know, there's also conscious motivation there. You're trying to present yourself on Facebook or make a certain image for yourself, right. So there are also conscious things going on. How long do you guys have? >>: I could take a few more questions. How many more? Maybe two more questions. >> Leonard Mlodinow: Okay. You didn't ask a question before? Okay. I'm trying to get to this side of the room because I know I've been focusing. There's a right/left favoritism thing in your unconscious. So if you give people equal products and you say which one do you want, they'll like the one on the right more, so I'm trying to counteract that and go to the left. >>: So the experiment with FMRI, you saw that people saw different things than what was projected to them. Do we know why? 23 >> Leonard Mlodinow: >>: They saw different things than -- Than what was shown to them? >> Leonard Mlodinow: No, no, that wasn't what it showed at all. What it showed was what they're looking at and what the computer guessed they were looking at. So the other thing was not something that they were perceiving. It was what the computer guessed that they were looking at. >>: Is FMRI accurate enough to make any judgments based on those? >> Leonard Mlodinow: I thought those were extremely impressive, that you could take electromagnetic readings of the brain and say they're looking at a snake and you don't get the details of the snake right. But, yeah, I mean, what they use it for is to see what parts of your brain are functioning. Like when you're afraid, it's your amygdala is functioning. And they do behavioral studies to check that out. So they work hand in hand. So they do studies on people with amygdala damage. They'll let a scorpion crawl up their arm. They'll good to a haunted house and go this is what's going on here. They'll, I forgot, they put them all kinds of dangerous situations and they have no fear at all. So you correlate what's going on in the brain and then you do behavioral studies and then you really learn a lot more. Okay. Last question. Who's closest to where I'm pointing there? I'll take two more. You and then you. >>: I'm curious why the visual example where you had the rectangles, we can't change what we're seeing there. But then on the sound example, by having the words helped us learn. >> Leonard Mlodinow: You weren't learning but the words weren't really there. So think of the sound as -- so first imagine the visual example, I showed you the two squares outside of the checkerboard. And you said oh, they're the same. That's like, I'm playing you the music and it's all gibberish. Then I bring the context in, and now you can't help but seeing them as different, the checkerboard. And I bring the context in, which is the words, and now you can't help but using that information to interpret the words. >>: Right, but you said after a while, you could actually hear the words 24 without seeing them, because you'd heard it so many times. >> Leonard Mlodinow: >>: Right. So that almost seems like -- >> Leonard Mlodinow: It's not that I'm seeing the words that influences me. It's knowing that those are the words. So my unconscious mind hears the music, knows the words and goes this is the words to the music. And I can't not do that. I can't stop it. >>: So that seems like your unconscious was being trained. I'm looking for a word there. And you were remembering what the words were. But visually, you can't put the ->> Leonard Mlodinow: The context is still there, whether I'm looking at it -so with the checkerboard, I'm removing the visual context and it's not working quite the same way, because you're not remembering the context, right? But with the music, I am remembering the context. My audio memory or my memory of the words is better than my ability to -- when I see the squares outside of the checkerboard, I can see them outside of the checkerboard. Checkerboard doesn't stay there for me. But with the words, when you play the music, the words are still there for me. So, I mean, the analogy isn't perfect between the two, but ->>: Like in the brain, if there's a difference in the structure between the parts. >> Leonard Mlodinow: It's totally because one's the audio perception. So it's not a perfect analogy, and one is seeing something and you're actually, they're both to do with vision. The square and I'm taking away the context is visual. In the other one, the context is more abstract, it's language, and the effect is audio. And one more was? >>: So given what I've learned over the last hour here, I would suspect you overemphasizing some of those many studies that happen to be more controversial or fascinating in your book and in your work. How do you work to avoid that? >> Leonard Mlodinow: I probably talked -- I definitely talk in more details 25 about the more interesting studies, but I try to avoid any studies, you know, that were more controversial, that aren't that accepted. Like most of the priming studies. I don't know if you've heard of priming, but that are hard to replicate. So the idea was to find the headlines of what's going on and then to illustrate it with studies. If a study was boring, I might just mention it or I might say something based on it. But if the study itself was interesting, then I'll go into more detail. So like the Texas study of the lawsuit, study was kind of interesting to explain. I'll go into more detail. If it was a boring study, I would have studied it real quickly, like studies show that people taking one side or another in a criminal case can't ignore the data or something like that. So it's more of a question of how much space do I give it, but I weren't you to have fun reading the book and not just learn something. So thank you.