Correlational Problems and Fallacies James H. Steiger Introduction In this module, we discuss some common problems and fallacies regarding correlation coefficients and their interpretation Interpreting a correlation Correlation and causality Perfect correlation and equivalence No Correlation vs. No Relation Combining Populations, and Ignoring Explanatory Variables Restriction of Range Interpreting a Correlation If scores are on roughly similar scales, the shape of the scatterplot can reveal a substantial amount about the correlation. Interpreting a Correlation Scatterplot (Cigarettes v s. Cardiac Reserv e) 42 38 r = Cardiac Reserve 34 30 26 22 18 2 6 10 14 18 Cigarettes Smoked 22 26 30 Interpreting a Correlation Scatterplot (Shoe Size vs. IQ) 160 140 120 IQ 100 80 60 40 20 -2 r = .01 0 2 4 6 8 Shoe Size 10 12 14 16 Interpreting a Correlation Scatterplot (GPA vs. IQ) 160 140 120 IQ 100 80 r = .72 60 40 20 20 40 60 80 GPA 100 120 Anscombe’s Quartet X1 Y1 X2 Y2 X3 Y3 X4 Y4 10 8 13 9 11 14 6 4 12 7 5 8.04 6.95 7.58 8.81 8.33 9.96 7.24 4.26 10.84 4.82 5.68 10 8 13 9 11 14 6 4 12 7 5 9.14 8.14 8.74 8.77 9.26 8.10 6.13 3.10 9.13 7.26 4.74 10 8 13 9 11 14 6 4 12 7 5 7.46 6.77 12.74 7.11 7.81 8.84 6.08 5.39 8.15 6.42 5.73 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 19 8 8 8 6.58 5.76 7.71 8.84 8.47 7.04 5.25 12.50 5.56 7.91 6.89 Anscombe’s Quartet Each of the above 4 data sets has the following summary statistics: M Y 7.5 M X 9 s 11 2 X s 4.13 rXY .82 2 Y Each has a best fitting linear regression line of Yˆ .5 X 3 Anscombe’s Quartet Scatterplot (Anscombe.ST A 8v*11c) Scatterplot (Anscombe.ST A 8v*11c) y=3+0.5*x+eps y=3+0.5*x+eps 14 14 12 12 10 Y3 Y4 10 8 8 6 6 4 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 2 20 4 6 X4 Scatterplot (Anscombe.ST A 8v*11c) 8 10 12 14 16 X3 Scatterplot (Anscombe.ST A 8v*11c) y=3+0.5*x+eps y=3.+0.5*x+eps 12 10 11 9 10 8 9 7 Y2 Y1 8 7 6 5 6 5 4 4 3 3 2 4 6 8 10 X1 12 14 16 2 2 4 6 8 10 X2 12 14 16 Correlation and Causality Correlation is not causality. This is a standard adage in textbooks on statistics and experimental design, but it is still forgotten on occasion. Example: The correlation between number of fire trucks sent to a fire and the dollar damage done by the fire. Perfect Corrrelation and Equivalence Two variables may correlate highly (or even perfectly), without measuring the same construct. Example: Height and weight on the planet Zorg. Height and Weight on the Planet Zorg Zero Correlation vs. No Relation The Pearson correlation coefficient is a measure of linear relation. Many strong relationships are nonlinear. Always examine the scatterplot! Combining Populations If two groups with different means and/or covariances are combined, the resulting mixture can exhibit spurious correlations. Example. (C.P.) Suppose the correlation between strength and mathematics performance is zero for 6th grade boys, and zero for 8th grade boys. Does this mean it will be zero in a combined group of 6th and 8th graders? Restriction of Range Often, when linear regression is used to predict performance, the population is restricted. (For example, the GRE is used to predict performance in graduate school, but people with low GRE scores are often refused admission to graduate school. Consequently, the “available data” are a truncated version of the full data set. Restriction of Range Scatterplot (Restriction of Range.STA 10v*1000c) y=33.905+0.514*x+eps 100 N = 1000 r = .73 90 VAR2 80 70 60 50 40 20 30 40 50 60 VAR1 70 80 90 100 110 Restriction of Range Scatterplot (Restriction of Range.STA 10v*1000c) y=35.09+0.497*x+eps 96 r = .40 N = 153 90 VAR2 84 78 72 66 60 78 82 86 94 90 VAR1 98 102 106 The “Third Variable Fallacy” Often people assume, sometimes almost subconsciously, that when two variables correlate highly with a third variable, they correlate highly with each other. Actually, if rXW and rYW are both .7071, rXY can vary anywhere from 0 to 1. Only when rXW and/or rYW become very high does the correlation between X and Y become highly restricted.