THE WRITE THING: INTERACTIVE WRITING IN KINDERGARTEN Jeannette Suzanne Jentzen B.A., University of Minnesota, Duluth, 2001 THESIS Submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS in EDUCATION (Language and Literacy) at CALIFORNIA STATE UNIVERSITY, SACRAMENTO SPRING 2010 THE WRITE THING: INTERACTIVE WRITING IN KINDERGARTEN A Thesis by Jeannette Suzanne Jentzen Approved by: __________________________________, Committee Chair Marcy Merrill, Ed.D __________________________________, Second Reader Kay Moore, Ed.D ____________________________ Date ii Student: Jeannette Suzanne Jentzen I certify that this student has met the requirements for format contained in the University format manual, and that this thesis is suitable for shelving in the Library and credit is to be awarded for the thesis. __________________________, Graduate Coordinator Dr. Robert Pritchard Department of Teacher Education iii ___________________ Date Abstract of THE WRITE THING: INTERACTIVE WRITING IN KINDERGARTEN by Jeannette Suzanne Jentzen Statement of Problem Kindergarteners enter school with varying literacy skills based on prior experiences and education. Teachers are accountable for meeting state standards regardless of the kindergarten population’s cultural, linguistic, and developmental diversity. Kindergarten teachers struggle with the responsibility to implement a developmental approach to literacy within the guidelines of the state standards while concurrently addressing the unique ways their young readers and writers learn. Given the positive influence adult support has on young children’s learning, programs that incorporate teacher support of students’ composition would be sufficient at developing students’ writing literacy. The purpose of this study was to determine the effectiveness of an interactive writing treatment for improving young students’ writing and orthographic development. iv Sources of Data Data was gathered from pre- and post-treatment writing samples. The twenty-two students in the treatment group received traditional classroom instruction including alphabet knowledge, phonological awareness, and phonemic awareness instruction. Additionally, the day began with a fifteen to twenty minute interactive writing lesson four times per week. Included in each lesson was a five minute pre-writing routine. The duration of the treatment was nine weeks with a two week mid-winter break after the third week. The actual time for the treatment was seven weeks. Conclusions Reached Conclusions from examination of the data indicate that the participants developed more sophisticated orthographic writing characteristics as an outcome of this treatment. Most participants’ writing improved. Struggling students benefited most from the interactive writing treatment. _______________________, Committee Chair Marcy Merrill, Ed.D _______________________ Date v ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I owe a special thank you to the people who supported me throughout the process of writing this thesis. Both professional colleagues and personal friends have helped me realize that this work is not a lone enterprise, for which I am sincerely grateful. Dr. Merrill’s encouragement and expressions of confidence in my abilities were invaluable. Dr. Moore’s patience and persistence contributed significantly to the final product. All of the professors in the Language and Literacy department at California State University, Sacramento provided the foundation for answering the inquiry. My husband, James, has sacrificed both his time and resources to support my ability to take advantage of the opportunities provided as a result of this education. My daughters, Ginger and Helen, also sacrificed their free time to edit multiple drafts. The encouragement I received from friends, even when I struggled to focus, was consistent and steady through this entire project. Special thanks to Ann Sherman for her kind words and sharp editorial skills. My co-workers, Nan Muzinich and Kelly Churchill, committed their time to both this project and the classroom to ensure that I could achieve in both arenas. J.S.J. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS Page Acknowledgments....................................................................................................... vi List of Tables .............................................................................................................. ix List of Figures ............................................................................................................... x Chapter 1. INTRODUCTION ……………………………………………………………….. 1 Background ....................................................................................................... 1 Statement of the Problem ................................................................................. 2 Significance of the Study .................................................................................. 3 Limitations of the Study ……………………………………………………... 3 Definition of Terms ………………………………………………………….. 5 Organization of the Thesis ………………………………………………….... 8 2. REVIEW OF RELEVANT LITERATURE ........................................................... 9 Introduction ....................................................................................................... 9 Early Writing/Literacy Expectations .............................................................. 9 Research-Based Practices in Early Literacy …………………………………12 Children’s Writing Development ……………………………………………15 Stages of Spelling ……………………………………………………………19 Interactive Writing ………………………………………………………….. 23 Conclusion .......................................................................................................30 3. METHODOLOGY ……………………………………………………………... 32 Introduction …………………………………………………………………. 32 Participants …………………………………………………………………. 32 Consent Form ……………………………………………………………….. 37 Treatment …………………………………………………………………… 37 Data Collection ……………………………………………………………... 45 vii Page Data Analysis ……………………………………………………………….. 48 Conclusion …………………………………………………………………. .52 4. RESULTS .................................................................................................……….54 Introduction ………………………………………………………………… .54 Results …………………………………………………………………….... 54 Conclusion …………………………………………………………………. 63 5. SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS …………..... 65 Summary …………………………………………………………………… 65 Conclusions …………………………………………………………………. 65 Recommendations for Practice and Further Study …………………………..75 Appendix A. Consent to Participate in Research .......................................................79 Appendix B. Sample Interactive Writing Lesson ..................................................... 81 Appendix C. Characteristics of Emergent Spelling ………………………………. 83 Appendix D. Characteristics of Letter Name-Alphabetic Spelling ………………. 85 Appendix E. Characteristics of Student Writing ………………………………….. 87 Appendix F. Changes in Writing Characteristics ………………………………… 89 Appendix G. Incidence of Occurrence Based on Educator’s Journal ……………. 91 References ................................................................................................................... 95 viii LIST OF TABLES Page 1. Table 1 Topics of Instruction ……………………………………………….. 41 2. Table 2 Analysis of Orthographic Change Data …………………………... 57 ix LIST OF FIGURES Page 1. Figure 1 Gender and Ethnicity of Participants …….……………………….. 33 2. Figure 2 Percentage of Preschool Attendance …...………………………..... 34 3. Figure 3 Participants’ Parental Education Level ……...……………………. 35 4. Figure 4 Participants’ Parents Post High School Education ……………….. 36 5 Figure 5 Characteristics of Participants’ Spelling ………………………….. 55 x 1 Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION Background The California Standards for the English Language Arts and the National Council of Teachers of English and International Reading Association together outlined content standards for the English Language Arts based on the position that students must have the opportunities and resources to develop the language skills required to participate fully as productive members of society (Chin, 1996; California Department of Education, 1998). Despite national referendums, local districts hold ultimate responsibility for meeting standards which vary significantly from state to state. Therefore, it is difficult to measure students’ achievement across the country, yet teachers must prepare students for the necessities of literacy within a global environment. Students in the United States are expected to demonstrate competence as measured by the standards of international literacy. Kindergarteners enter school with varying literacy skills based on prior experiences and education. Teachers are accountable for meeting state standards regardless of this kindergarten population’s cultural, linguistic, or developmental diversity. To facilitate this need for a universal education, areas of questionable practices should be replaced by sound teaching which prepares the students to meet national and international demands (Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998). In short, some educators have the standards drive the curriculum while other teachers make curricular decisions based on the diverse needs of the students in the classroom. Interactive writing exemplifies a 2 teaching method for kindergarten classrooms which meets the writing standards while benefiting diverse learners. This chapter presents the background, the statement of the problem, significance of the study, limitations of the research, and the definition of terms. Statement of the Problem This study was intended to document the changes an interactive writing program would produce in a small Northern California elementary school’s kindergarten population’s writing. Kindergarten teachers struggle with the professional responsibility to implement a developmental approach to literacy within the guidelines of the standards set for education, while still addressing the best ways to educate their young readers and writers. Specifically, the question was: How does interactive writing promote young children’s orthographic development? To further probe this question and endorse the inquiry, the following three questions about orthography, instruction, and verification were considered: Question 1: On the orthographic writing developmental continuum, to what degree did the students’ writing change from their pre-treatment writing sample to their post-treatment writing sample? Question 2: In what ways did instruction in interactive writing make a difference in the students’ writing? Question 3: How closely were the observations of the observers aligned with the educator’s journal? 3 Significance of the Study Research has demonstrated that literacy begins before children receive formal education (Clay, 1975, 1998, 2001; Teale & Sulzby, 1986). Unlike the traditional kindergarten approach which stresses either learning letters before writing or emphasizes learning to read before learning to write, research recognized the value of an emergent literacy approach in today’s kindergarten (Teale & Sulzby, 1986; Whitehurst & Lonigan, 1998). Acknowledging the legitimacy of children’s early literacy-related behaviors, the emergent literacy approach suggests that literacy acquisition is best thought of as a developmental continuum (Clay, 2001; Teale & Sulzby, 1986). Armed with this information, California kindergarten teachers must still include the state-mandated components of a balanced, comprehensive approach to their reading and writing instruction (California Reading Task Force, 1995; California State Board of Education, 1999). Interactive writing is a research-based program that incorporates the instructional techniques of both emergent literacy and the California teaching standards. Therefore, to best respond to individual developmental needs within an educationally appropriate curriculum, writing instruction using interactive writing is suggested for kindergarten. Limitations of the Study Every attempt was made to keep this study legitimate; however, limitations could impact the outcome and the significance of the findings. Several limitations may affect the results. The study was limited to a sample size of 22 students. As I am the teacher in the classroom and the researcher of the study, bias may have affected the outcome. In early teacher training, instructors place great emphasis on eliminating bias in the 4 classroom. However, in terms of this study, I know my students well and may suffer from bias in that I saw their strengths and weaknesses in the classroom daily, and may have catered to them, compromising some aspects of this study. With this awareness, I worked to keep my judgments professional at all times. Another limitation was time, both the length of the study and the length of the school day. The treatment occurred for nine weeks with a two week school holiday scheduled within this period. Continuation of interactive writing for a longer duration should show greater growth of orthographic characteristics. The kindergarten academic day at this site was half-day. While the whole group interactive writing lesson fit within the schedule, separate time for individual writing with adult assistance was difficult to plan. A review of the research revealed that individual writing would best be accomplished with adult support. To address this and the time issue, volunteer parents were invited to assist students at the literacy center during learning centers. Often, though a schedule was cleared weeks before, the parent neglected to come. Another limitation on this study was prior educational experience of the participants. The majority of the sample population entered kindergarten without much experience using writing instruments. One way that children experience literacy development is through a developmental preschool program. Not only were many children in this study deprived of the advantage of a preschool program, but the majority of these students’ parents had minimal college experiences. 5 Parents can provide another way for children to acquire literacy before entering formal schooling. Parents who work full time often are stretched to meet even the basic needs of their children and may have limited time to construct experiences for children’s literacy development. The implication is that these working parents may not have in fact spent time working with their children prior to their kindergarten year. Definition of Terms For the purpose of this research, the following definitions will be used: Affix: a morpheme attached to a word stem to form a new word. The most common are prefix, which attaches to the front of a word, and suffix, which attaches to the end of a word. Alliteration: repeating the same consonant sound at the beginning of two or more words in close succession. Alphabetic knowledge: knowledge of the name and sound of the letters of the alphabet. Alphabetic principle: speech can be turned into print; print can be turned into speech; and letters correspond to speech sounds. Alphabet Linking Chart: a reference chart which illustrates alphabet letters written in both upper and lower case, a corresponding picture, and the word that accompanies the picture (McCarrier, Pinnell, & Fountas, 2000). Concept of word: spoken words match written words while reading. Concepts about print: knowledge about the conventions of print including letter, word, letters make up words, words comprise sentences, and directionality (left to right in English). 6 Decoding: the ability to translate the alphabet letters into sounds. Differentiating instruction: varying instructional techniques to ensure that people with differing abilities comprehend the lesson. Digraphs: two letters together make a single sound such as ow in brown, or sh in shin. Emergent literacy: The Literacy Dictionary defines emergent literacy as the “development of the association of print with meaning that begins early in a child’s life and continues until the child reaches the stage of conventional reading and writing” (Harris & Hodges, 1995, p.70). Grapheme: the letter(s) and letter combinations that represent a sound in writing. Interactive writing: a collaborative writing instruction in which the teacher and students actively dialogue, compose, and construct text. At times, based on instructional purpose, the teacher chooses individual students to “share the pen.” The objective of interactive writing is to teach children the writing skills, strategies, and conventions necessary to become competent independent writers (McCarrier, Pinnell, & Fountas, 2000; Roth, 2009). Inventive spelling; invented spelling: the non-conventional, temporary spelling children write as they make approximations of letters to their corresponding letter sounds. An example is skul iz Fon for school is fun. Letter knowledge: knowing that letters are different from each other and that they have different names and sounds. Morpheme: the smallest meaningful unit in a language which can not be divided. A morpheme may be a single word: cat; a word can be composed of two morphemes: 7 catwalk; a morpheme such as the s in cats both has its own meaning and adds meaning to the word to which it is attached. Onset-rime: the onset is the consonant(s) before the first vowel in a word. The rime is the first vowel in a word and what follows. If a word begins with a vowel, it does not have an onset. Orthography: The correct way to use a writing system of a language including spelling, spacing, capitalization, and punctuation. Spelling is recognized as only part of orthography. However, for the purpose of this paper, and based on usage within research sources cited, spelling and orthography will be used interchangeably. Phoneme: the smallest unit of language; individual sounds. An example: bear /b/ /ā/ /r/ Phonemic awareness: the awareness that spoken language consists of a sequence of phonemes (Yopp, & Yopp, 2000). Phonetics: the study of the speech sounds that occur in languages, including the way these sounds are articulated (Yopp, & Yopp, 2000). Phonics: a way of teaching reading and spelling that stresses symbol-sound relationships (in alphabetic orthographies) (Yopp, & Yopp, 2000). Phonological awareness: as defined in The Literacy Dictionary, “phonological awareness is awareness of the constituent sounds (phonemes) of words in learning to read and spell” (Harris & Hodges, 1995, p.187). Social constructivist theory: a theory proposed by Lev Vygotsky which proposes that learning is constructed within a social environment. 8 Zone of proximal development: skills which are too difficult for a child to complete on her own, but which can be accomplished with the guidance or encouragement of a more skilled person (either a peer or adult) (Vygotsky, 1978). Organization of the Thesis Chapter One introduces parameters, limitations, methodology for the study. Chapter Two presents a Review of the Literature regarding early writing/literacy expectations, research-based practices in early literacy, children’s writing development, stages of spelling, and interactive writing. Chapter Three will detail methodologies used in the execution of the interactive writing treatment. Chapter Four reports on the data derived from the study. Finally, Chapter Five shares implications from the study, discussion and outcomes of the study, as well as makes conclusions and recommendations for further study. 9 Chapter 2 REVIEW OF RELEVANT LITERATURE Introduction This review of the relevant literature presents the topics of early writing/literacy expectations, research-based practices, children’s writing development, stages of spelling, and interactive writing. There are two primary goals for creating this review of literature. The first is to outline children’s developmental processes as they become writers. The second goal is to give credence to the use of interactive writing in the kindergarten setting. The review of literature serves to illustrate the connection between children’s writing development and interactive writing. The anticipated outcome for this project will be a demonstration that the interactive writing strategy follows children’s writing development in a symbiotic fashion while simultaneously supporting expansion of students’ writing attempts. Early Writing/Literacy Expectations Literacy development begins long before children enter kindergarten (Clay, 1975, 1998, 2001; Teale & Sulzby, 1986). Settings for literacy formation vary based on the experiences and opportunities provided a child. One location where children acquire literacy skills is preschool. Currently in California, preschool learning foundations are in place to equip preschools with tools to build high-quality programs which strengthen students’ school readiness. These foundations describe what all young children usually learn with appropriate support (Child Development Division, 2008). 10 The support young children require in order to gain the skills essential for school readiness varies for each child. High-quality preschools offer programs to meet individual’s diverse needs through environments that foster active, playful exploration and experimentation. The California language and literacy foundations set by the state reflect current research and understanding of the knowledge that promotes children’s language and literacy learning during the preschool years (Child Development Division, 2008). Within the range of abilities that preschool children will need to support learning, the foundations focus on three segments, listening and speaking, reading, and writing. The preschool writing foundation describes age-appropriate knowledge and skills expected of older three-year-olds and older four-year-olds. Four-year-olds who are making progress toward being ready for kindergarten demonstrate increased emergent writing and writing-like behaviors including the following: hold a pencil or pen with finger grasp to write; draw recognizable figures, letters or shapes; move hand to hold paper in place while drawing or writing; draw a picture and write a label (label may not be readable); write strings of symbols that look like letters or actual letters; and write first name clearly with or without mistakes such as excluding letters and letter reversals (Child Development Division, 2008). The preschool language and literacy foundations are not intended to be the standard for assessment. The foundations describe research-based learning and development that would typically be expected with appropriate support. They are intended to provide preschools with a resource to guide successful learning and development in young children. 11 California preschools have identified foundations to support school readiness. A child may or may not have reached the foundation goals before entering kindergarten. In contrast, California public schools have standards by which children are measured. These standards clarify expectations of both teacher performance and student outcomes. Teachers should teach and students should know the developmentally appropriate systematic reading and language arts skills which indicate age appropriate competence in the language arts. Skills learned in kindergarten are foundational and requisite and deficits in skills and knowledge must be addressed “responsively and responsibly” (California Department of Education, 1999). If a foundation was not set in preschool, then it must be in place at the end of kindergarten. Currently in California, kindergarten standards are set higher than many parents might expect. According to the state writing standards, a kindergartener is expected to use letters and phonetically spelled words to write about experiences, write consonantvowel-consonant words, and move from left to right and from top to bottom when writing. All of this is accomplished while independently writing upper and lower case letters and attending to letter form and proper spacing (California Department of Education, 1999). These skills require explicit knowledge in topics such as phonological awareness, the alphabetic principle, and language structure. All learning in California public schools is standards-based. In addition to the writing standards, students must master other language arts goals. Subjects include reading, listening and speaking, and written and oral language conventions. Some specific concepts within these topics ensure that they will do the following: learn all 26 12 English letters and their corresponding sounds; understand concepts about print such as parts of a book and directionality; write a sentence using pre-phonetic knowledge when spelling; and read simple one-syllable and high frequency words (California Department of Education, 1999). Not only must children achieve mastery in these language arts subjects, but they are also simultaneously required to master skills in mathematics, science, and social studies. For kindergarten educators to implement an effective language arts program within the guidelines of the Reading/Language Arts Framework for California Public Schools (California State Board of Education, 1999), practices must include the four components of a balanced, comprehensive approach to reading and writing identified by the California Reading Task Force (1995). First, oral and written language must be balanced within the language arts program. Second, needs of the emergent reader/writer must be addressed through an explicit skills program including phonemic awareness, phonics and decoding. Third, ongoing assessment must guide instruction. Finally, an early intervention program must address needs of students at risk of failure. Research-Based Practices in Early Literacy Many research studies in the past several decades have focused on children’s literacy development (California Department of Education, 1995; National Early Literacy Panel, 2008; National Reading Panel, 2000; Neuman, Copple, & Bredekamp, 2000; Snow, Burns, & Griffin 1998). While the literacy umbrella covers both reading and writing, reading has traditionally received the most attention. However, as Snow et al. (1998) in the National Research Council (NRC) report took a broad view of literacy, in 13 which the Council regarded writing as an integral part of their discourse about reading, they recognized that nearly all literacy activities share “the use of the products and principles of the writing system to get at the meaning of a written text” (p.42). Simply put, writing activities which extend children’s knowledge of letter names and sounds, help them make phonemes concrete, and maintain their interest and active engagement provides foundational knowledge necessary for fully developed literacy (Dickinson & Neuman, 2006). Although two major components are reading and writing, literacy has its foundation in oral language (Sulzby, 1996). Research suggests that writing development is a process that is constructed through an interrelationship between social interaction, oral discourse, and written communication (Martinez & Teale, 1987; Neuman et al., 2000; Snow et al., 1998; Sulzby & Teale in Barr, 1991). Whitehurst and Lonigan (1998) noted that current research and theories suggest an emergent literacy approach to early literacy in which “reading, writing, and oral language develop concurrently and interdependently … from children’s exposure to interactions in the social contexts in which literacy is a component” (p. 849). Moreover, social interactions with adults and opportunities for independent explorations of written language are vital for early development (Teale & Sulsby, 1986). Contemporary literacy practices suggest that kindergarten instructors should create literate environments with an emergent literacy focus (Whitehurst & Lonigan, 1998). The research agenda behind the authors’ work in emergent literacy is that literacy acquisition is best thought of as a developmental continuum which extends throughout 14 early childhood. In contrast, “reading readiness,” refers to the child’s mastery of prerequisite skills in preparation for attaining the point at which a person is ready to learn to read (Teale & Sulsby, 1986). The emergent literacy perspective views children’s literacy-related behaviors throughout the preschool period as legitimate and important aspects of literacy (Whitehurst & Lonigan, 1998). Yaden, Rowe, and MacGillavray recommend an emergent literacy approach to educate kindergarten children. Principal characteristics of their emergent literacy design include the following: consider the children as socially competent partners; permit writing experimentation in an unthreatening environment; include adult- and peer-led dialogue about literacy; and provide opportunities for children to practice their unconventional, yet emerging skills (2000). Similarly, Clay acknowledges that children actively construct their learning throughout the preschool years, individuals become literate at different times, and there are many ways children get to the point of formal literacy instruction (1975, 1991, 1998). Given that emergent literacy acknowledges children’s variable literacy levels, recognizes the social aspect of learning, and encourages independent writing exploration, instruction using the emergent literacy approach appears to promote research-supported developmental writing practices. Whitehurst and Lonigan (1998) have hypothesized that the skills necessary to understand the components of emergent literacy can be grouped as two different processes: “outside-in” and “inside-out” processes. Outside-in processes represent children’s understanding of the context within which the text they are trying to write, or read, occurs. The three components of outside-in processes are: language, conventions of 15 print, and emergent reading. Inside-out processes represent children’s knowledge of the rules for translating the particular writing they are trying to read into sounds. Inside-out processes include the following skills: linguistic awareness, phoneme-grapheme correspondence, and emergent writing. A developmentally appropriate writing curriculum which complements these emergent literacy research findings would 1) integrate routine classroom practices with oral and written language, 2) incorporate communication with adults, 3) include a reading component, and 4) involve students in independent writing practice (Teale & Sulzby, 1998). Integrating developmentally appropriate, student-centered practices with the Reading/Language Arts Framework for California Public Schools (California State Board of Education, 1999) requires a synthesis of the research with the standards. The interactive writing model effectively brings research and the standards together. The teacher organizes instruction toward students’ needs, uses assessment to adapt lessons toward individual goals, encourages students to express themselves on paper, provides a wide exposure to print, and constructs lessons that develop student concepts about the forms and functions of print (Neuman, Copple, & Bredekamp, 2000). Children’s Writing Development Emergent writing is mostly pretend writing. An illustration of pretend writing is when a child scribbles some indecipherable marks on a paper and then asks an adult to read what it says. The child is indicating an understanding that print has meaning, but she doesn’t yet know how to write. Regardless of most children’s culture or where they live, when adults promote, model, and incorporate it into play, this pretend writing occurs 16 spontaneously (Ferreiro, & Teberosky, 1982). Optimal literacy development occurs in an environment that is physically, emotionally, socially, and cognitively suited to the child’s changing needs. Therefore, adult guidance is necessary to support children’s writing acquisition (Clay, 1998; McCarrier et al., 2000; Snow et al., 1998; Vygotsky, 1978). The adult supports the child to work at the outer limits of his own abilities thus moving the child forward to new knowledge (Vygotsky, 1978). Although people comprise a range of developmental variations, both biological and experiential, most children show interest in meaningful graphic representations such as their name and environmental print within the first three years (Bloodgood, 1999; Snow et al., 1998). From two to three years of age, children can create drawing-like scribbles and forms that resemble letters. Adults play an important role in literacy development at this age, often introducing and modeling letters and their related sounds to children. For example, this may occur within the social act of reading books to children. Three- to four-year-old children show a rapid growth in literacy, as demonstrated by their writing: scribbling, drawing letter-like forms, and formation of random strings of letters (Snow et al., 1998). The title of Clay’s (1975) seminal work, What did I write?, was uttered by a four year old. Examination of children’s early nonconventional writing led Clay (1975) to determine that, even when their writing is scribbling and non-phonetic letter strings, children appear to be exploring features of print, such as linearity and the use of recursive features. Through their play and exploration, many three- and four-yearold children develop an abstract understanding about print. 17 Evidence of writing progress between the ages of four and six years old is demonstrated by children’s beginning to “read” their unconventional writing consisting of scribbles, drawings as orthographic representations, and non-phonetic letter strings (Snow et al., 1998). The NRC report asserts that as soon as children know some letters of the alphabet, they should be encouraged to write words or parts of words and use them to write sentences. Writing supports identity and segmentation of speech sounds, letter knowledge, and phonological awareness. Phonological awareness and letter knowledge are essential skills for emergent readers and contribute to comprehension of prereading activities (Snow et al., 1998). Some five-year-olds begin to identify letter sounds and experiment with phonetically-based invented spellings (Clay, 1975, 1979; Read, 1986; Sulzby in Teale & Sulzby, 1986). Encouraging invented spelling can play an important role in promoting knowledge of the alphabetic principle. Teachers and parents foster a positive attitude about writing in general when they encourage children to experiment with spelling (Coker, 2007; Read 1986). When allowed to use their increasing knowledge of letters, sounds, and words, children invent creative text. As children continue to develop writing skills, their awareness of “the right way” influences their writing as they insist on conventional spellings (Clay, 1975). Classrooms that provide children with regular opportunities to express themselves on paper, without feeling too constrained for correct spelling and proper handwriting, also help children understand that writing has real purpose (Graves, 2003; Neuman, et al., 2000). In addition, teachers can use students’ writing to gauge their level of spelling knowledge and tailor instruction to fit their needs. 18 Clay (1998) recognizes that by the time they enter school, most children have explored some aspects of print, and that these first explorations may occur in writing rather than reading. As they mature, children gain control of two types of knowledge that support literacy: some understanding of the world; and control over oral language. School literacy policies regarding writing may vary the focus of the writing program. Some schools may encourage attempts to write messages; some schools begin with learning letters; while others stress learning to read before learning to write. Clay (1998, 1991, 2001) contends that the most practical approach to literacy development is to guide children to learn reading and writing at the same time. The interaction between composing and constructing a written message and reading it back provides opportunities for realizing some of the fundamentals of language. Forming letters calls awareness to forms and other important features of writing. Clay further postulates that while a child explores writing, the eye and brain are directed to characteristics such as constructing words, letter by letter, attending to spatial concepts, and breaking down the writing task into its smallest segments while simultaneously synthesizing them into words and sentences, and segmenting sounds to form words (2001). Ehri and Roberts agree that learning to spell is linked to learning to read. They found that early spelling requires that beginners know letters and phonemic segmentation so that they can pull apart the sounds in words and represent them with plausible letters (2006). Coker concludes that for young children to develop into proficient writers, they must learn to manage the many challenges of writing (2007). Moreover, teacher and adult 19 support can extend students’ understanding of the processes and purposes of writing. However, teaching children how to write has a limiting effect on children’s writing development. Children need to write every day; daily experimentation with writing helps children to formulate and mold their notions of how writing works (Coker, 2007). Working with word study and other language activities help to promote this experimentation. Stages of Spelling Several researchers have examined and defined the stages of spelling and orthographic knowledge (Bear, Invernizzi, & Templeton, 2000; Gentry, 1981; Henderson, 1985; Henderson & Beers, 1980). While the name for each stage varies by author, the stages track similar ages and developmental progress. Like oral language, spelling evolves from simple to more complex behaviors. While Gentry (1981) asserts that children’s writing moves through clearly defined linear stages, Clay (2001) would argue that these stages are recursive. Children in the earliest stage of orthographic knowledge range from one year to seven years of age, and spell in an emergent (Bear, et al., 2000), preliterate (Henderson, 1985; Henderson & Beers, 1980), and deviant (Gentry, 1981) way. Emergent writing is a pretend writing. Children experiment with print as they scribble or write in letter-like forms. “Spelling” may range from random marks to legitimate letters having no relationship to sound. This lack of correspondence to sound makes emergent spelling prephonetic (Bear et al., 2000). 20 Emergent spelling may be divided into three phases: early emergent, middle emergent, and late emergent writing (Bear et al., 2000). Characteristics of early emergent writing include large scribbles which are basically drawings with no designs that look like letters. Middle emergent writing has progressed to letter-like forms which can include numbers. At this step, the child demonstrates directionality, left to right in English. Also during this period, the child’s name becomes important as a source for letters (Bloodgood, 1999; Bear et al., 2000). Letters represent words, but there is no sound-syllable correspondence between what is written and the sounds of the word. Late emergent writing is signaled by some letter-sound matching and occasional initial consonants to represent words, with some memorized words written repeatedly. Throughout most of the emergent spelling stage, children do not know about letter-sound correspondence, but begin to know the basic visual characteristics of writing - that writing moves from left to right and top to bottom on the page (Bear et al., 2000). After the emergent stage of spelling, children advance to the letter namealphabetic (Bear et al., 2000), letter name (Henderson, 1985; Henderson & Beers, 1980), and pre-phonetic (Gentry, 1981) stage, which is usually between the ages of five and nine. Letter name-alphabetic writing is the beginning of conventional writing. Children at this stage have knowledge of the alphabetic principle: letters represent sounds, and words can be segmented into sounds. Writing during this period is slow as children match letter names to the sounds they are trying to write word by word and sound by sound (Bear et al., 2000). 21 Letter name-alphabetic spelling can also be divided into three steps: early letter name-alphabetic; middle letter name-alphabetic; and late letter name-alphabetic (Bear et al., 2000). Children who write in the early letter name-alphabetic phase apply the alphabetic principle mainly to beginning consonants, omitting vowels in syllables. Transition to the middle letter name-alphabetic phase is signaled by writing most beginning and ending consonants as well as attending to vowels and most stressed syllables. Children who have advanced to the late letter name-alphabetic step write with an awareness that syllables have vowels, which is the start of understanding the closed syllable, the consonant-vowel-consonant (CVC) unit. Writing at the end of this phase includes some conventionally spelled high-frequency words and most initial consonant digraphs and blends (Bear et al., 2000; Gentry, 1981). As children move away from the literal application of letter names, usually between the ages of six and twelve years of age, they begin to look at patterns or chunks of letter sequences within words. Known as within word pattern (Bear et al., 2000; Henderson, 1985), phonetic (Gentry, 1981), and vowel transition (Henderson and Beers, 1980), children in this stage pay closer attention to the vowel within syllables. At the within word pattern stage of their writing development, children begin to think more abstractly. This stage begins when children can correctly spell most single-syllable short vowel words and moves through the more abstract patterns of spelling single-syllable long vowel words including a consonant-vowel-vowel-consonant (CVVC) pattern as in peak or pail; a consonant-vowel-consonant-silent e (CVCe) pattern as in game, pine; and a consonant-vowel-vowel (CVV) pattern as in pay, pie (Bear et al., 2000). 22 Berninger et al. (1998) found that teaching multiple connections between spoken and written words at the whole-word level and within-word level may facilitate transfer of the alphabetic principle to other writing conditions. Teaching functional spelling units of varying sizes, including whole word and onset-rime, as well as codes for component letters (consonant and vowel blends), raised spelling achievement relative to age peers in standardized spelling tests. They contend that training in both explicit phonological and orthographic awareness and explicit phonological-orthographic connections should be taught to achieve transfer to new words. Once writers clearly and easily understand the differences between long and short vowel patterns, they are able to move to the syllables and affixes (Bear et al., 2000), syllable juncture (Henderson, 1985), transitional (Gentry, 1981) or vowel transition stage (Henderson & Beers, 1980). Typically occurring around the ages of eight to eighteen, this stage is typified by writers correctly applying the rules of spelling to include consonant doubles, plurals, and affixes. Spelling within this stage begins to look more conventional with most words spelled correctly (Bear et al., 2000). The final stage of spelling development, the derivational relations spelling stage (Bear et al., 2000), derivational constancies spelling stage (Henderson, 1985), or correct spelling stage (Gentry, 1981), corresponds with the advanced stage of writing. This is the stage in which writers attend to roots of words to determine their origin and proper spelling. Common prefixes and suffixes are spelled and used correctly. Word study, in tandem with reading and writing, fosters students’ vocabularies, which in turn enhances children’s spelling (Bear et al., 2000). 23 To summarize, the stages of spelling define characteristics present in students’ writing, which when used with assessment, can guide instruction. Planning lessons at the level a child uses, but confuses (Invernizzi, Abouzeid, & Gill, 1994), along with teacher support, encourages student competence as they gain inner control over constructing meaning from print (Clay, 1991). Permitting beginning readers to use invented spellings offers another form of assessment which assists teachers to understand the sound system of beginning readers prior to and during early reading instruction (Tangel & Blachman, 1995). An instructor uses the knowledge gained from the above assessments to construct interactive writing lessons. Interactive Writing Interactive Writing’s Foundation Interactive writing is born from the “organic teaching” (p.11) method developed by Ashton-Warner (1963) which evolved into the language experience approach (Dixon, & Nessel, 1983; Van Allen, 1969). Both Ashton-Warner’s “creative teaching” (p. 27) method and the language experience approach view children’s experiences as routes to developing their reading and writing. The premise of these two teaching methods is that words important to children are words children can and will read. Creative teaching like language experience uses a dictate and read style with the teacher writing for the children the words they wish to express. The language experience approach emphasizes individual rather than group compositions, while creative teaching begins with individual writing and evolves into a group teaching technique. Essential 24 elements of each method include both teacher-student discussion as central to oral language development, and student-generated material as key to reading comprehension. McKenzie (1985) developed a writing process called “shared writing” in which the teacher and students cooperatively composed text. Like language experience, the teacher acts as scribe as the students dictate the text. Unlike language experience, shared writing is a communal writing experience with all students participating. Shared writing matches the language experience and creative teaching philosophies of student involvement and investment. Therefore, students will learn to read and write more readily if they are the authors of the text and if the text is meaningful to them. Faculty from Ohio State University originated the term interactive writing in 1991 when they, along with teachers from Columbus, Ohio, expanded McKenzie’s shared writing model to include student engagement in direct construction of text through “sharing the pen” (McCarrier, Pinnell, & Fountas, 2000). Primarily intended for children in preschool, kindergarten, and first grade, interactive writing was initially designed to help children who had limited experiences in literacy understand how words work (McCarrier et al., 2000). McCarrier, Pinnell, and Fountas define interactive writing as “a dynamic, collaborative literacy event in which children actively compose together, considering appropriate words, phrases, organization of text, and layout. Individual children take over and share the pen with the teacher at points selected by the teacher for instructional value” (2000, p. xv). Interactive writing is based on what Clay (1991) calls a responsive model of teaching in which the teacher provides the clearest examples of instruction in 25 response to the learner’s needs at that particular moment. Also, interactive writing reflects Vygotsky’s (1978) theory of the zone of proximal development. This theory recommends that the teacher varies instruction to provide individual experiences that are beyond the level that a student could accomplish independently thus advancing that child’s learning. Instruction does not follow a specific sequence, but evolves from the teacher’s understanding of the child’s strengths and needs. Furthermore, children learn to write in a highly supported, social context that encourages risk taking (Button, Johnson, & Furgerson, 1996) which is an example of the social constructivist approach to learning (Vygotsky, 1978). Children have opportunities to solve, together in a group, problems they will face when writing independently (Brotherton & Williams, 2002). The teacher’s role is critical in helping children learn accurately (Clay, 1991). McCarrier, Pinnell, and Fountas (2000) maintain that, due to their varied experiences prior to entering school, children require individualized support to develop the necessary skills and knowledge for literacy. While Clay (1991) explained that children actively construct their own learning and literacy, and their ability grows as they gain inner control over creating meaning from print, this growth does not take place without support. Supportive instruction offers children an environment in which to develop in language and literacy competence (Vygotsky, 1962). During an interactive writing lesson, a group of children together compose text with the direct guidance and support of their teacher (Brotherton & Williams, 2002). 26 Interactive Writing Lesson Plan The basic interactive writing lesson follows the five part language experience session as outlined by Dixon and Nessel (1983) nearly exactly, except in the interactive writing lesson, the teacher gives the pen to the children at points that will advance their understanding of writing. The five part language experience model is as follows: 1) The teacher and students discuss the topic to be written about. 2) The student dictates the text to the teacher who is the scribe. 3) The student reads and rereads the text, with help as needed, until reading becomes fluent. 4) The teacher designs contextual lessons to develop student’s reading or writing skills (lessons based on the writing the teacher has created with the student). 5) The student’s reading confidence develops as reading skills learned from rereading self-created text are transferred to reading text created by others. Boroski (2004) has used the work of McCarrier, Pinnell and Fountas to develop simplified steps to interactive writing instruction including: 1) negotiate a sentence, 2) count the words on fingers, 3) recall the word to be written and stretch the word, 4) share the pen, 5) point and read, 6) recall the entire sentence to be written, then go back to step three to complete the writing activity. Interactive writing is not a mechanical process to be followed in order to produce a text. While the product is important, the process has the most value (McCarrier et al., 2000). As the class or small group collectively constructs text, their participation in the writing allows all students to practice phonological awareness, phonics skills, writing conventions, concepts of print, alphabetic knowledge and the writing process. 27 Impact of Interactive Writing on Kindergarten Instruction Most of the published material related to the topic of interactive writing is instructive in nature (McCarrier et al., 2000; Swartz, Klein, & Shook, 2001; Tompkins & Collom, 2003). However, a few significant studies support interactive writing as a useful component of an effective kindergarten language arts program (Button, et al., 1996; Craig, 2003; Roth, 2009; Sipe, 2001). Following the guidelines outlined by the California Reading Task Force (1995), studies noted below provide evidence for the effectiveness of addressing the four components determined to contribute to a balanced, comprehensive kindergarten reading and writing program. These features include the following: a balance between oral and written language; an explicit skills program including phonemic awareness, phonics, and decoding; use of ongoing assessment to direct instruction; and an early intervention program to support students at risk of failure. Roth (2009) implemented a ten-minute a day interactive writing program to determine the impact on first graders’ independent writing. Without increasing instruction time, the students who participated in interactive writing made greater gains than the students in the comparison group. Both groups were given the same writing prompts. Although the students in the interactive writing group began the study at a lower level than the students in the comparison group, they surpassed them by the end of the year. Also, students who participated in interactive writing made greater gains on nine out of ten of the components of the writing rubric, which included: ideas, organization, word choice, capitalization, sentence fluency, spelling of high frequency words, spelling 28 of other words, capitalization, punctuation, and handwriting. Both groups scored equally well in spacing skills. In Roth’s study, though lessons were short, they encompassed the components of balanced instruction. Discussions between the students and teacher were the foundation of the constructing phase prior to writing. Provisions for differentiating instruction for individual needs were based on continuous assessment. Direct instruction about conventions, concepts of print, phoneme-grapheme correspondence, as well as types and traits of writing was presented in an authentic context. The implications from this study, including the relatively short period of time required each day to achieve these positive results, makes this study important for anyone interested in implementing the interactive writing method into a balanced literacy program. It should be noted, however, that Roth was one of the initial instructors in the McCarrier et al. study. Her years of experience applying the interactive writing method may contribute to an efficiency of instruction not replicable by a novice teacher. Craig’s (2003) sixteen week study found that kindergarteners who participated in an adapted interactive writing treatment met or exceeded the performance of kindergarteners in a skill-sequenced metalinguistic games treatment on word identification, passage comprehension, and word reading development measures. Together, the three components of this interactive writing treatment, text experience, response to the text using interactive writing, and supplemental letter-sound instruction, mirrors the California Reading Task Force guidelines. 29 Composing text constructed through conversation demonstrated a balanced approach between oral and written language (Craig, 2003). Use of a responsive approach to instruction, one in which teachers both facilitated conversations related to children’s comments and questions and adapted the writing in response to children’s increased sophistication, further supported oral and written language development. The letter-sound patterns, selected from children’s spellings, and used for explicit instruction were chosen as a result of assessment. Explicit phonemic awareness instruction was embedded in the shared reading and writing activities employed in this treatment. Differentiating instruction and scaffolding to accommodate children of varied skill levels centered around four specific phonemic awareness skills: first sound identification, last sound identification, sound segmentation, and sound deletion (Craig, 2003). Having met all the objectives, Craig’s interactive writing study clearly follows the California Reading Task Force guidelines for a balanced kindergarten writing program. Sipe (2001) endorses interactive writing as one of four research-based writing models for promoting invented spelling instruction. The study recognized interactive writing as a successful model to advance spelling skills of students using explicit instruction and teacher support, as the “expert other” (Vygotsky, 1962), to link new spellings with words they already know. Within this model, the teacher’s active involvement and scaffolding promoted students creativity and independence. Button, Johnson, and Furgerson (1996) designed literacy activities for kindergarteners, including interactive writing lessons, to demonstrate the effect interactive writing had on children’s literacy learning. The interactive writing lessons 30 included direct teaching about language conventions, how words work, types of writing, and concepts about print while providing opportunities to hear sounds within words. Furgerson often focused the interactive writing lessons on a book read aloud in class. Repeated readings, along with the interactive writing lesson, helped scaffold the development and incorporation of the literacy processes of reading, writing, speaking, listening, and thinking. Building on the knowledge students had about the sounds represented by letters in their names, Furgerson began the school year using the individual child’s writing ability as a basis for instruction. Individual students’ needs were met and guided by ongoing assessment results. In May, student’s assessments showed growth in all areas, with nearly all the students reading. Thirteen of seventeen students were reading predictable text and all students had improved in their concept of print knowledge. The most growth was in the children’s ability to hear sounds in words where the mean increased from 9.8 in the fall to 29 in the spring. Increase in children’s literacy is clearly demonstrated by this study; however, three factors should be noted when considering these findings. First, the number of participants was only seventeen. Second, this study did not have a control group to compare findings. Third, instruction was not limited only to interactive writing. What this study showed was the procedures employed while applying interactive writing, which allowed teachers to understand more about interactive writing’s functional contribution to children’s literacy development. Conclusion These studies demonstrate interactive writing’s place within a balanced kindergarten curriculum. It is clear that instruction using interactive writing benefits 31 kindergarten students writing development. However, there is room to learn more. Additional research relating to interactive writing’s influence on students’ developmental processes as they become writers, within the range of abilities present in the kindergarten setting, would be of interest. The purpose of this study was to contribute to the body of knowledge by discussing the developmental processes children experience as they become writers as well as interactive writing’s role within a balanced literacy curriculum. 32 Chapter 3 METHODOLOGY Introduction In this study, the 22 students in my half-day traditional school year kindergarten classroom participated in an interactive writing treatment with the intention of monitoring the treatment effects toward improvement in the students’ orthographic development. Students’ concept of writing was developed through the use of interactive writing in the classroom. Children must have opportunities to write, but unfortunately the majority of these kindergartners had few writing experiences prior to this school year. This interactive writing approach allowed the students to have a more developed bridge between their limited home writing and their directed school writing experiences, thus building a sense of meaningful purpose for each child’s writing. This chapter describes the participants of the study, the sample population, followed by a record of the parental consent. The daily application of the treatment is then presented, along with a description of the procedures for data collection and analysis. Finally, the chapter concludes with an overview of data to be analyzed in Chapter 4. Participants This study included 22 five- and six-year-old kindergarteners from a small northern California town who were members of my kindergarten class. Two children from the sample were repeating kindergarten, and neither were in my class their first 33 year. All twenty-two students acquired family consent to participate in this study. An example of this form is in Appendix A. The class was composed of primarily English speaking White/Not Hispanic lowto middle-income students. Graph 1 illustrates that of the twenty-two students, twentyone spoke English, and one spoke primarily Vietnamese at home (Student Cumulative Files, 2009-2010). These students were unevenly divided by gender, with 13 male and 9 female students. Figure 1 Gender and Ethnicity of Participants 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 Male Female White Hispanic Vietnamese Chinese Figure 1 indicates the percentage of students who attended preschool. Just under two thirds of the students, 64 percent, attended at least one year of preschool before entering kindergarten, while slightly more than one third, 36 percent, of the participants 34 did not attend preschool (Student Cumulative Files, 2009-2010). The only local preschool offerings included State Preschool, Head Start, a preschool affiliated with the local Indian Casino, and a private preschool. Figure 2 Percentage of Preschool Attendance 40% 35% 30% 25% 20% 15% 10% 5% 0% State Preschool Head Start Indian Casino Preschool Private No Preschool Preschool Although approximately two thirds of the participants attended preschool, based on teacher observations, many of these children entered kindergarten with minimal beginner writing skills including proper pencil grip. There appears to be a tremendous variation in the amount of writing instruction prior to kindergarten. While some preschools may opt to include writing in their programs, this doesn’t seem to be the norm, as the majority of students showed few writing skills. Two students, or 9 percent, were considered socioeconomically disadvantaged based on qualification for the free or reduced price meal subsidy for students of families 35 who earn less than $35,798 each year (based on a family of four) (Student Cumulative Files, 2009-2010). As of March 2010, seventy-three percent of the participants’ parents attended some college or vocational school, as represented in Figure 3. Eighteen percent of students’ parents were high school graduates, while parents of nine percent of the participants had no high school degree (Student Cumulative Files, 2009-2010). Figure 3 Participants’ Parental Education Level 80% 70% 60% 50% 40% 30% 20% 10% 0% Post High School High School Graduate Not High School Graduate Of all parents with post high school education, just over half, 56 percent, had some college level education. The number of parents who earned college or graduate degrees was just under half, 44 percent, of all parents with training after high school. 36 Figure 4 Participants’ Parents Post High School Education 60% 50% 40% 30% 20% 10% 0% Some College College Graduate Post Graduate Training Through observations made from the beginning of the year, many of these kindergarten students appeared to be inexperienced with the demands of writing activities. One opportunity for monitoring student writing was to observe the students signing their names upon entering the classroom on a daily basis. I observed some children’s confusion as they tried to hold the pencil. For example, some children had awkward pencil grips, with fisted grips or fingers overlapping (thumb on top of index finger or index finger on top of thumb). Also, some children were reluctant to write, unwilling to write even the first letter of their name. The children seemed to lack exposure to both writing instruments and emergent writing. While they appeared to understand that the stories I read to them had words, they had not made the connection between the pictures and the words. So, when handed a blank piece of paper and asked to write about an interesting topic such as how to dress on 37 Halloween, my most struggling students left the page blank. They did not even attempt to draw. Consent Form Each child’s parent signed a participant consent form, shown in Appendix A, which was sent home in the children’s school-to-home folders before assessments or treatments took place. Because the researcher was also the teacher, parents were not punished or rewarded for allowing their child to be included in the study and were notified that their child would remain anonymous in the writing of the thesis. The consent form briefly outlined the timeline for the treatment and the types and approximate times pre- and post-assessments would be given to the children. Treatment The twenty-two students in the treatment group received traditional classroom instruction including alphabet knowledge, phonological awareness, and phonemic awareness instruction. Additionally, the day began with a fifteen to twenty minute interactive writing lesson four times per week. Included in each lesson was a five minute pre-writing routine. The duration of the treatment was nine weeks with a two week midwinter break after the third week. The actual time for the treatment was seven weeks. The basic interactive writing lesson plan used in this treatment combined several authors’ lesson ideas (Button, Johnson, & Furgeson, 1996; McCarrier, Pinnell, & Fountas, 2000; Setzer, 2005; Swartz, Klein, & Shook, 2001; Tompkins & Collom, 2004). An adaptation for whole group instruction, the addition of whiteboard paddles, was adopted after the first week (McCloskey, 2004). 38 Lesson Format The lesson format, as illustrated in Appendix B, is an adapted whole group version. To explain, some of the participants in this study were active children with social tendencies that interfered with their engagement in lessons when presented at the carpet. The carpet is in front of the teacher’s chair and easel. Children sit on the carpet when the teacher gives direct instruction. Their response and attention to lessons was documented by the teacher based on observations to be better suited to the one-on-one or small group setting, preferably while seated at a table. While interactive writing lesson routines and procedures were in place, reviewed before every lesson and followed by the majority of the students seated together on the carpet, these few children switched into social mode. To ensure engagement of all children, two modifications to the initially adopted interactive writing approach were applied. The first modification to the lesson plan was in response to further study into literacy’s foundation in oral language (Button, Johnson, & Furgeson, 1996; Clay, 1991; McCarrier, Fountas, & Pinnell, 2000; Sulzby & Teale, 1991). Think-pair-share was incorporated into lessons whenever possible (Setzer, 2005). Think-pair-share is a four part cooperative learning strategy credited to Professor Frank Lyman and his associates, University of Maryland Howard County Southern Teacher Education Center (1981). First, students listen as the teacher poses a question. Second, the group formulates a response during thinking time. Third, students are prompted to pair with a neighbor to discuss their answers. Finally, students are randomly called on to share their responses with the whole group. 39 The second modification to the lesson plan was applied to meet sound teaching objectives. To meet standards for active participation, evaluation of student learning, and classroom management, I added the routine distribution of dry-erase whiteboard paddles before the lesson began. A three step pre-lesson warm-up preceded each interactive writing lesson. The preparation routine, five minutes in length, began just after the morning bell rang. The bell became the routine signaling the students to sit on the carpet. In order to teach letter name recognition, the students sang the ABC Song while the teacher handed each child an Alphabet Linking Chart (McCarrier et al., 2000) along with a whiteboard paddle and cloth eraser. To model and assess, the teacher took her place beside the easel and pointed to each written rule while the students read in unison. The eleven step lesson began with the teacher introducing the topic for one minute. Students took two minutes to brainstorm ideas with partners. When their names were randomly chosen by the instructor, the students shared their ideas with the class. Since the class collectively chose the text, more than one child might be picked to offer text suggestions. After negotiating the words, one minute was spent repeating the text, enunciating each word, and counting the words in the sentence to build concept of word (Bear, Invernizzi, Templeton, & Johnston, 2000; McCarrier, Fountas, & Pinnell, 2000). The teacher then wrote this text on a note card as a reminder of the agreed upon text. Prior to calling on a student to write, if a word was phonetically regular (Swartz, et al., 2001), thirty seconds were spent as the teacher stretched the word with a Slinky while the 40 students pretended to stretch their own Slinky in an imitation of the teacher’s exaggerated elongation of the word. This promoted an attention to the individual sounds to develop phonemic awareness (Adams, 1990; Ehri, Roberts, & Neuman, 2006; McCarrier, et al., 2000; Tompkins & Collom, 2004; Yopp & Yopp, 2000). Using random assignment to choose the writer, the instructor adjusted the writer’s task based on ability to ensure success (McCarrier, et al., 2000; Swartz, et al., 2001; Tompkins, & Collom, 2004). Tasks might include writing the first letter of the word, writing the whole word, or writing the appropriate punctuation. For example, one student started the treatment as a finger space holder, but ended the program writing the first letter. The time for this writing varied based on the student’s confidence, aptitude, and accuracy, but was usually between one to three minutes. This was the important part of the project where success was measured not by quantity of words, but by quality of the learning. Each stroke attempted was a potential lesson and could direct the experience. If a student made a mistake, for example, used a capital letter in the middle of a word, “fix it” tape or white address labels were used to cover the error with a reassurance that the mistake was an opportunity for learning. While the chosen writer was writing at the easel, the students sitting on the carpet were actively participating in the lesson. They were instructed by the teacher to either use the whiteboard paddles to write as much of the word as they knew, for example, writing the first letter of the word. Alternatively, they might attend to a relevant topic such as the use of students’ names for clues to writing, for example the name “James” helps to spell the word “games.” Use of a magnetic writing board, a lightweight lap size writing 41 instrument, kept the teacher’s focus on the children (Swartz, et al., 2001). The magnetic writing board allowed the teacher to face the children while writing. Once the word was written, thirty seconds to one minute was spent reading or rereading the text depending on whether the day’s lesson started a new writing project or continued a project started on a previous day. The class repeated this process for as many words as time and opportunities for learning would allow. The interactive writing session always ended with one to three minutes of rereading the text. Instructional Topics Table 1 Topics of Instruction Phonological/ Concepts of Writing Phonemic Print Conventions Awareness Rhyme Directionality Spelling Parts of a Letter Heading Environmental Alphabetic Print Knowledge Sight Word Chart Onset rime Concepts of Letter Capitalization Greeting Name Chart Alliteration Concepts of Word Punctuation Body Syllables Punctuation Grammar Closing Phonemic Blending Reading Concepts Phonemic Segmentation Phonemic Substitution Partial Letter-sound Recognition Blends and Digraphs Posters Naming Letters Recognizing Letters in Print 42 Table 1 illustrates literacy applications of the interactive writing lessons used in this study. While each of these topics is critical for a kindergartener’s language acquisition, kindergarteners have more to learn under the subject phonemic awareness than the subject alphabetic knowledge, as exemplified in this table. Methodology used to implement and expand the interactive writing lessons was taken from best practices from research and made to fit my particular student body of kindergarteners. The following approach to writing development was chosen in consideration of schedule and student attentiveness constraints as well as ability to differentiate instruction. Phonological processing including phonemic awareness, phoneme blending, phoneme segmentation, and development of letter recognition was taught and practiced daily. One of the most common strategies for phonological processing was phoneme blending as demonstrated with Slinky-stretching words. Another regular phonemic awareness strategy was the use of students’ names to learn letter names and sounds. Students’ names lent themselves to first and last letter sound and name as well as onset rime activities (Bloodgood, 1999; Stewart, 2004). Occasionally phonemic substitution was taught as an extension of the onset rime activities. Phoneme segmentation was the least practiced phonological processing strategy of those mentioned in this paper. In addition to phonological processing, we also concentrated on parts of a letter as a subject of interactive writing. These letters were addressed to either members of the community or classmates. Letter writing was as much an individual assignment as a whole group activity. A classroom Post Office was one area for letter writing practice 43 during assigned kindergarten centers. Students were assisted by the parent helper at the Post Office. Each student received a Parts of a Letter Guide like the one used during the whole class interactive writing lesson. The teacher did not expect students to master letter format, but used letters as a means to entice them to write. Parent helpers were guided to gently support students in letter writing, and to concentrate on student writing over letter format. Interactive writing lessons always included writing conventions, concepts of print, environmental print, writing, and last, reading. Writing conventions include spelling, capitalization, punctuation, and grammar. Supporting children in recognizing conventional writing is one of the aims of interactive writing. Conventions were applied where appropriate to the text. Grammar at this level focused on spacing between words. Concepts of print include directionality, concept of letter and concept of word. Every lesson included directionality either in the left to right construction of a word or the left to right and wrap around of reading text that had been written. Teaching the concept of letter was less frequent than teaching the concept of word. Environmental print was included in most every lesson through the use of a student names’ pocket chart, a sight word pocket chart, blends and digraphs posters, and previous interactive writing text posted throughout the room. Students’ names were used in nearly every lesson either as the source of the writing such as writing letters to the Star Student or as a resource for letter names and sounds. Students enjoyed recalling aspects of previous writing. 44 Writing and reading occurred at every interactive writing session. After the second week of instruction, all students wrote using a whiteboard paddle. The students used the whiteboard paddles for various exercises. One example: the teacher led mini lessons based on the text while the assigned writer wrote at the easel. This may have included the teacher instructing the students to write the first or last letter sound of the word being written by the writer. The teacher used a magnetic writing board to instruct individuals on letter formation, directionality, or lead the mini lesson. Reading the day’s text completed every lesson. Writing Practice Classroom writing practice was designed to address the language and literacy needs of this population. Since a large portion of the population was not offered a preschool experience, I promoted a play-based literacy center to bridge the early literacy practices often presented in the preschool setting with the demands of kindergarten. Decisions for the addition of writing experiences were based on recommendations from the National Research Council (NRC) report (Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998, p. 184) which outlined guidelines for kindergarten play-based literacy development. These guidelines included the following: allocate enough time and space within the classroom for play provide necessary materials develop children’s background knowledge for the play setting scaffold the rehearsals of dramatic retellings, and guide the children’s attention and learning through modeling and interaction. Our classroom Post Office offered one example of play-based literacy development during both free exploration and structured centers. Another example of play-based 45 literacy development was the use of students’ names as a means to provide materials to distinguish letter names and sounds. Given that Bloodgood (1999) concluded that names were the most meaningful aspects of print for many 3-year-olds, and that 4- and 5-year olds were comparable in their literacy growth, the names’ pocket chart was an integral part of the introduction to interactive writing. Letter writing to the Star Student during our interactive writing session modeled both letter construct and the use of students’ names to create our print. An adult volunteer at the literacy center helped guide the children’s attention back to lessons introduced during the interactive writing session. Data Collection Qualitative information was gathered to examine the differences in writing and orthographic development. Student writing samples were collected at both the beginning and end of the treatment. The first writing sample was photocopied and set aside until another teacher and I scored them the end of the treatment. Lesson plans, lesson developments, and observations of student actions/reactions were recorded in an educator’s journal. At three times during the treatment, the beginning, middle and end, a third party observed and took notes on the interactive writing lesson to corroborate with the educator’s journal observations and insights. The following sub-sections outline each assessment used to ascertain student’s abilities. First Writing Sample The first writing sample was collected before the interactive writing treatment began, photocopied, and stored at the home of my professor. Since the season was Halloween, we read many books on the subject, and enthusiasm for costuming and trick- 46 or-treating flourished throughout the classroom. Students were directed to write about how they were going to dress on Halloween. The question was, “What will you be for Halloween?” Students were given a blank sheet of kindergarten lined paper, which included features we refer to as a “ground line,” “fence line,” and “sky line,” on which to write. Most standard school days, student work was completed at adult-led centers wherein a teacher or volunteer parent guided the students through the set activity. The exception was on Fridays when the lesson was conducted whole group with students sitting at tables and everyone completing the same task. Like a Friday lesson, the research writing sample was a whole group event with my partner teacher present. Prior to the lesson, we decided upon responses to any requests for support. Assistance would consist of one of the following three agreed upon responses: 1. “Try your best.” 2. “Write about what you want to be on Halloween.” 3. “What sounds do you hear?” Second Writing Sample The second writing sample was collected at the end of the treatment. This writing was a literature response. The Star Student was encouraged to bring a favorite book to school which was read daily throughout the Star Student’s week. The Star Student book selection was George Shrinks (Joyce, 1985). Student enthusiasm for this book was high with much discussion about life as a little person. A six inch Mickey Mouse character, the class mascot, was used as a visual reference for the children’s writing. Directions 47 were to write about what would happen to them if they were small like George or Mickey. The question was, “What adventures would happen to you if you shrank?” Students wrote on a similar blank sheet of kindergarten lined paper, with a “ground line,” “fence line,” and “sky line,” except, unlike the paper for the first sample, this paper had an illustration box. Like the first sample collection, the lesson was conducted whole group, with students sitting at tables and all completing the same task. My partner teacher was also present as in the first sample collection. Similar responses were offered when students requested assistance. 1. “Try your best.” 2. “Write about what would happen if you shrank.” 3. “What sounds do you hear?” Educator’s Journal An educator’s journal was employed as a means of documenting the daily developments related to the project including lesson plans, teacher observations, and adaptations based on these observations. Lesson modifications also occurred due to discoveries from the ongoing literature review. Activities and expectations including challenges and successes in the execution of the interactive writing lessons and anecdotes of students’ references or responses to the lessons were logged in the journal. Observer’s Notes On three occasions during the treatment, a third-party observed and took notes on my methods using the interactive writing strategy. These observations and notes were used to triangulate the journal annotations with classroom events. These third party 48 observations occurred at the beginning, middle and end of the treatment. During the nine weeks of the treatment, three observations were made at intervals of roughly three weeks each. A volunteer, a retired k-12 educator, completed the first and last observations, while another kindergarten teacher conducted the second observation. The change in observers was due to a death in the family of the retired teacher; therefore, another observer filled in for her. Observers were instructed to write details related to the lesson including classroom environment, teacher preparedness, teacher student interactions, student attentiveness, and student actions. No rubric was given as it would have made noting all behaviors difficult; however, consistent note taking was administered by both observers. Data Analysis The important question posed by this research: how does interactive writing promote orthographic development in young children’s writing? The following three sub-questions guided the answer to the main question: Question 1: On the orthographic writing developmental continuum, to what degree did the students’ writing change from their pre-treatment writing sample to their posttreatment writing sample? Using holistic assessment, changes in student writing from pre-treatment writing to post-treatment writing were identified. To score the writing samples, I chose definitions for the stages in spelling and orthographic knowledge that have been researched, described and refined by Henderson (1981). Appendices B and C contain Henderson’s noteworthy stages of orthographic writing characteristics. Invernizzi, 49 Abouzeid, & Gill (1994) defined each stage of students’ orthographic knowledge by three functional levels; first, what students do correctly, second, what students “use but confuse,” and last, what is absent in students’ spelling. At the conclusion of the treatment, the initial writing sample was obtained from the secure storage of the professor’s home and scored to align with the orthographic traits of either emergent spelling or letter name-alphabetic spelling. The researcher and another kindergarten teacher scored the writing samples using colored sticky notes. Both educators are trained in the implementation of writing education, one at the elementary level, the other at the college level. First, the researcher recorded on the sticky note the stage on the orthographic developmental continuum as described by Bear et al. (2000), folding in half and taping the note to ensure reliability. Second, the other teacher who was unfamiliar with the students reviewed the writing samples and the orthographic characteristics rubric, and scored the writing by recording the stage on the orthographic developmental continuum on the blank surface of the sticky note. To assure inter-rater reliability, the teacher was trained in scoring using characteristics of orthographic development. After both teachers completed the holistic writing scoring, their assessments of orthographic features were compared. Scoring discrepancies were presented to a third scorer who had also been trained in scoring using characteristics of orthographic development. The third scorer’s determination resolved the difference in four cases. 50 The day after scoring the initial writing sample, the two scorers evaluated the final writing. The same criteria and technique used to determine orthographic features in the initial writing was implemented in the final writing evaluation. Question 2: In what ways did instruction in interactive writing make a difference in the students’ writing? Prior to this treatment, the model for writing instruction was that of shared writing. The teacher and students together negotiated the text, but the teacher acted as scribe and model. Student writing consisted of letter practice and name writing. Students were observers of writing rather than contributors to the process. Interactive writing changed observers into participants. Constructing text with teacher support allowed each individual to acquire more difficult writing skills than could be attained alone. Consequently, the teacher considered zone of proximal development (Vygotsky, 1978) when selecting the writer’s task. Furthermore, the social aspect of text construction encouraged group problem solving which translated into a positive model to carry into independent writing (Brotherton & Williams, 2002; Vygotsky, 1978). Examination of changes in participants’ writing between the pre- and posttreatment writing samples as well as analysis of the incidence of direct teaching and student performance, as recorded in the educator’s journal, provided answers and implications to this question. A record of writing characteristics found in the participants’ initial writing samples was compared with a record of writing characteristics found in the participants’ final writing samples. A paper was divided into three columns. Each student’s randomly 51 selected number was placed in the first column. The second and third columns were titled pre-treatment writing and post-treatment writing respectively. Writing characteristics present in each sample were recorded in the rows next to the student’s number. Some characteristics, such as pretend writing and punctuation, appeared to occur more often. Appendix F registers the counts in student writing between the first writing sample and the second writing sample which shows differences in student writing. The educator’s journal contained three sections. First, the lesson plan part of the journal was divided into halves, the lesson plan was written on one side with checks on the other half to confirm that the lesson was followed. A record of student involvement was included with the checks. Below the lesson plan was a daily record of successes or ways to improve the lesson. Finally, documentation of teacher observations outside of the lesson including student actions and remarks completed the day’s journal entry. Data from the educator’s journal was obtained using a series of steps. First, a copy of Table 1 was color coded one color for each topic of instruction. Next, using the Table 1 color guide, each page of the educator’s journal was evaluated and color coded to indicate incidence of instruction. Then, a separate piece of paper was divided in two and used to record tallies of each instructional topic with teacher actions on one half and student actions in the other half. Last, a fresh copy of Table 1 was used to record the counts of each page. Analysis of the data revealed patterns of teacher and student actions. The following descriptions summarize those patterns which are used to classify information in 52 Appendix G: Incidence of direct instruction Student response to the interactive writing lessons –carryover from one lesson to the next; recall of topic outside of interactive writing lesson Student synthesis of learning into writing practice – integration as demonstrated by implementation Spontaneous student writing in reaction to the interactive writing sessions – increase in incidence of unsolicited student writing initiation Question 3: How closely were the observations of the observers aligned with the educator’s journal? I recorded frequent and detailed notes in the educator’s journal which included lesson plans, a review of each lesson, and student response to instruction. Observer’s notes were placed along side the educator’s journal and checked to ensure whether a correlation or disconnect existed between the two sources. Conclusion Initial observations of my kindergarten students’ writing suggested that many of them had little writing experience prior to the start of this study. Conversations I have had with local teachers and principals indicate that writing instruction in kindergarten is at a minimum. The students’ apparent lack of exposure and the trend to forego writing instruction until first grade was an opportunity to introduce interactive writing into our instruction design. Therefore, the aim of this project was twofold: first, to examine kindergarteners’ success at writing through the use of an interactive writing treatment, 53 and second, to put into practice an interactive writing treatment in order to obtain information which will contribute professional development to kindergarten teachers based on my findings. I examined three areas of interest in response to the project aims: first, student writing development in the area of orthographic features; second, interactive writing’s effect on the development of student writing; and finally, verification of the author’s notes and claims through inspection by a third party. After scoring pre- and post-writing samples from twenty-two kindergarten students, writing changes were analyzed based on orthographic features. Evolution of student’s writing development was described using the educator’s journal. Observer’s notes were compared with the educator’s journal. In addition, careful records were kept of the material students were being taught so that this approach could be replicated by others. Application of the outcomes will both direct my teaching and provide information to address other teaching professionals about the use of interactive writing. The results of this qualitative study will be presented in Chapter Four and discussed in Chapter Five. 54 Chapter 4 RESULTS Introduction Twenty-two kindergarten students underwent a seven week interactive writing treatment to determine the treatment’s effect on the student’s writing development. This chapter investigates and reports the outcomes of this treatment using qualitative measures. Spelling growth based on orthographic characteristics, as outlined in Appendices C and D, is measured and analyzed. Changes in orthographic characteristics shown in Figure 5 compare initial and final writing samples. Additional evaluation of pre- and post-treatment writing samples, as shown in Appendix E, measures writing change. Also in this chapter, I investigate the participants’ writing development as a result of instruction in interactive writing and correlate instructor’s/observer’s remarks as a reliable source on the teaching methods used. Results Data analysis will be presented in response to three secondary questions which arise from the main question which is: How does interactive writing promote orthographic writing in young children? Question 1: On the orthographic writing developmental continuum, to what degree did the students’ writing change from their pre-treatment writing sample to their posttreatment writing sample? Findings indicate that kindergarteners’ orthographic development increased from before the treatment to after the treatment as shown in their writing samples. Figure 5 55 identifies these results. Appendices C and D offer descriptions of Emergent Writing characteristics and Letter Name-Alphabetic Writing characteristics respectively. Figure 5 Characteristics of Participants’ Spelling 8 7 6 5 4 Initial Writing Sample 3 Final Writing Sample 2 1 0 A B C D E F Letters represent spelling characteristics due to space limitations. A key is listed below: A – Early Emergent Writing D – Early Letter Name-Alphabetic B – Middle Emergent Writing E – Middle Letter Name-Alphabetic C – Late Emergent Writing F – Late Letter Name-Alphabetic Figure 5 illustrates participants’ orthographic writing characteristics before and after an interactive writing treatment. Comparison of student writing characteristics from the pre-treatment writing sample to the post-treatment writing sample reveals that 90 percent of treatment participants began the treatment in the Emergent Writing stages. 56 The majority of the students, 73 percent, began the study writing in the Middle and Late Emergent Writing stages. Two students, 9 percent, entered the study in the Early Alphabetic Writing stage. The treatment ended with a greater number of students, 45 percent, in the Letter Name-Alphabetic stages than began the study. Prior to the interactive writing treatment the initial writing sample revealed that four students, or 18 percent of participants, wrote at an Early Emergent level. Eight students began the interactive writing treatment demonstrating both Middle Emergent Writing and Late Emergent Writing, each at 36 percent of the sample. Two students, 10 percent of the sample, wrote at an Early Letter Name-Alphabetic level at the start of the treatment. When the treatment ended, two students, 9 percent of participants, wrote at an Early Emergent level. The same number of writers, five each, displayed Middle Emergent Writing, Late Emergent Writing, and Early Letter Name-Alphabetic Writing which is 23 percent of participants each. Four students, 18 percent of participants, wrote at the Middle Letter Name-Alphabetic level, while one student, 4 percent of participants, exhibited Late Letter Name-Alphabetic characteristics in writing. 57 Table 2 Analysis of Orthographic Change Data: Change in Score Decreased by 1 Stayed the same Amount of Change Number of Students -1 2 0 7 Increased by 1 1 7 Increased by 2 2 2 Increased by 3 3 4 Based on comparison of orthographic characteristics from pre- and post-treatment writing samples as illustrated in Appendix E and further clarified in Table 2, fifty-nine percent of treatment participants increased at least one stage of orthographic knowledge while participating in an interactive writing treatment. Fifty-four percent of students who made gains in orthographic knowledge advanced one step. Fifteen percent of students who made gains in orthographic knowledge increased two places from where they began. Nearly one-third, or 31 percent, of students whose orthographic knowledge improved while undergoing the interactive writing treatment, advanced three steps. While these gains are impressive, it should be noted that almost one third, or 31 percent of all participants, demonstrated no change in orthographic characteristics, and two students who participated in the study, or 9 percent of participants, displayed a reduction in orthographic characteristics in their writing. Question 2: In what ways did instruction in interactive writing make a difference in the students’ writing? 58 Comparison between the pre-treatment and post-treatment writing samples is one way to provide a response to this question. This assessment showed an increase in the use of writing conventions and a decrease in pretend writing. Appendix F is a record of writing characters found in the students’ initial and final writing samples. This data shows that most students had already acquired directionality, left to right in English, prior to the treatment. Fewer students used pretend writing after the interactive writing treatment. The incidence of wrapping, returning to the left side of the paper when reaching the end of a line, stayed the same, but the intent had changed which will be discussed in chapter 5. Letter/sound match was another area of minimal accomplishment; only one more student demonstrated this in the final writing sample. The final writing samples revealed six characteristics not present in any of the initial writing samples. The students’ awareness of spacing and punctuation was most pronounced with fifteen students attempting spacing between words and eleven students using punctuation – a period at the end of their writing. Six students’ final writing samples began with a capital letter. Four students used digraphs and/or blends in their final writing. Writing words became more prevalent for some students. At the end of the treatment, six students wrote sight words while three students wrote complete English words. The patterns discovered in the coding of the educator’s journal are another way to address Question 2. The tables in Appendix G detail how the four patterns that emerged from the journal apply to the topics of instruction. These patterns are detailed in Chapter 59 3 and include direct instruction, student response to instruction, student synthesis of learning, and spontaneous student writing. Results from the tables in Appendix G show that instruction in interactive writing exposed kindergarten students to the vocabulary and use of terms in the following areas: phonemic awareness; concepts of print; writing conventions; parts of a letter; environmental print; and alphabetic knowledge. Some practices were more likely applied in most children’s writing than other practices. Alphabetic knowledge was applied by all students in this study. Naming letters and recognizing letters in print was most often associated with the students’ names. Elements of concepts of print were practiced often by the majority of students. Students included directionality, concept of letter, concept of word, and punctuation in their writing more often than reading concepts. Directionality and punctuation were most commonly included in students’ writing with punctuation occasionally over-generalized – placing a period after one’s name, for example. Writing conventions were practiced daily, but spelling and punctuation were generally including in students’ daily writing while capitalization and grammar were occasionally left out of students’ writing. “Parts of a letter” was used daily as an interactive writing lesson. Student letter writing included a greeting and body more often than a closing. A heading was most often missing from these letters. Environmental print in this classroom included a name chart, a sight word chart, and blends and digraphs posters. These resources were used during the interactive writing when appropriate. The name chart and sight word chart were incorporated into the interactive writing lessons daily, with the blends and digraphs posters used on occasion. 60 Consequently, students used the name chart daily in their independent writing and the blends and digraphs posters infrequently when writing independently. The phonemic awareness concepts of rhyme, onset-rime, phonemic blending, and partial letter-sound recognition were taught daily, but teaching did not transfer to student application of all of these concepts. Students recognized letter sounds in the first letters of classmates’ names, and mimicked phonemic blending with Slinkys, but never included rhyme, although they occasionally considered onset-rime in their independent writing. Syllables, phonemic segmentation, and phonemic substitution were often included in interactive writing lessons, but the students never referred to them while writing. Occasionally, interactive writing lessons included alliteration; however, the students in the treatment never referred to alliteration during their independent writing. All students in this study have made the transition from non-writer to writer, confident that what they put on the paper makes sense to them. The letters on the paper may not appear as words to the reader/observer, but when asked to read the writing, each child will convey with assurance the thoughts those letters express. Prior to this study, many students were willing to tell their stories, but unwilling to attempt to write them. Many students can now direct a writer to use the conventions of writing correctly. The students demonstrated the ability to identify norms in writing, such as beginning a sentence with a capital letter, using a space between words, and ending a sentence with, as they most often said, a period. Most students were concentrating on letter sounds during independent writing. Consequently, although they capably identify writing norms as a group, their individual writing lacks conventions. However, some students showed 61 confidence in their ability to formulate the language of their text and focus on conventions when writing independently. Several students were interested in writing details. They attempted to add colorful language as a flourish to their writing in response to a teacher-led lesson on observing details in a student’s photograph. In comparison with these few students, most students’ writing is straightforward with few details. Most participants in this study can listen for and use much of the vocabulary of writing and reading taught in this treatment, some of which encompasses rhyme, blending, sweep, punctuation, capitalization, and sight word. They can attend to sounds within words and focus on spelling patterns through the use of environmental print when they refer to their names for similar sounds or write a blend or digraph because they remember the poster on the wall. They explore their own ideas through writing when they spontaneously reach for their journal, or choose to write a letter to a friend. Interactive writing has made non-writers writers. Question 3: How closely were the observations of the observers aligned with the educator’s journal? The educator’s journal and the observers’ notes were checked for similarities or differences between both accounts. The educator’s journal was closely aligned with the observer’s comments, except that the observers recognized student data missing from the teacher’s journal including student enthusiasm for the project and classroom management issues. These comments were used to address issues of engagement and instruction differentiation. 62 The educator’s journal included comprehensive notes on lesson plans, an evaluation of each lesson, and student synthesis of interactive writing instruction into both the lessons presented during the session and assignments outside of the direct instruction. Observers were directed to note facts associated with the lesson including classroom environment, teacher preparedness, teacher student interactions, student attentiveness, and student actions. The first observation was rescheduled twice after a two day school closure due to snow. The classroom environment was conducive to student learning. Students sat on the carpet with space on the carpet for all to clearly see the writing on the easel. Charts and auxiliary materials were within reach and used when necessary. The teacher was prepared for the lesson of writing a letter to Santa. Teacher interactions with the students were typically positive, yet there were many interruptions associated to redirecting students to the writing task. Most students attended to the lesson and were eager to write their own letters. Some students became discouraged due to consequences for their behavior (they lost their sticker). Also, students were engaged at the beginning of the lesson, but the lesson was too long - the teacher was trying to make up for time lost. Students sitting on the rug became restless and fidgety. While the student at the easel was writing, the students sitting on the rug were at first attentive to the teacher’s lesson, but lost interest after ten minutes of a twenty minute lesson. The second observation was conducted by another teacher and took place on the scheduled day. Adjustments to the lesson’s routine, due to comments made from the first observation were implemented. Whiteboard paddles had been introduced for whole class 63 participation. The teacher differentiated instruction for each student while the writer was at the easel. She guided students in letter sound, direction, and formation. This lesson lasted ten minutes and ended with one student exclaiming, “Aw!” because the session was ending and she wanted to write. The third observation was scheduled, then rescheduled due to personal circumstances of the observer. The lesson as executed was aligned with the lesson plans except the teacher was surprised by an unscheduled fire drill. Until the fire drill, the students were attentive to the lesson: the whiteboard paddles offered great improvement on attentiveness to the lesson. Again, however, the lesson was too long in the eyes of the observer. Conclusion Information presented in this chapter illustrated differences in the participants’ writing from before introduction of interactive writing to the end of the interactive writing treatment. Data from Figure 5 suggests that an interactive writing treatment develops kindergarten students’ orthographic writing characteristics. A greater number of students ended the treatment in the Letter Name-Alphabetic stages than the number that had begun the treatment in the Letter Name-Alphabetic stages. Of the students who demonstrated improvement in orthographic knowledge, nearly one third increased by three steps. However, nearly one third of all students who participated in this interactive writing treatment displayed no change in orthographic writing characteristics. Orthographic characteristics of two of the participants were reduced by one step. 64 The educator’s journal identified areas of writing instruction that were not addressed prior to enactment of interactive writing. Writing was taught minimally prior to the interactive writing treatment. Student participation in interactive writing instruction led to greater comprehension of writing and reading terms as well as the value of understanding these terms in their own writing. These terms included punctuation, capitalization, spelling, spacing between words, read the room, writing, and reading. Expanded definitions of writing and reading, including picture drawing to tell a story and pretend reading of a favorite book, resulted in both greater enthusiasm to attempt writing and a willingness to read stories to classmates. An interactive writing treatment expanded the participants’ concepts of writing and reading. Data from the observer’s observations influenced lesson design and execution. The teacher appeared to follow the lesson plans, but reaction to students’ misbehavior posed an obstacle to lesson flow and led to longer sessions than was optimum. Addition of whiteboard paddles for total participation reduced teacher/student interchanges and resulted in engaged students even when not called upon to write at the easel. Implications and discussion of the impact of interactive writing on kindergarten writers will be presented in Chapter Five. 65 Chapter 5 SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS Summary Twenty-two kindergarten students participated in this study to determine the effectiveness of an interactive writing treatment for improving their writing and orthographic development. From this inquiry came the following question: How does interactive writing promote orthographic development in young children? Students’ writing before the interactive writing treatment was compared to their writing after the interactive writing treatment to note differences. Analysis of the data reinforces the findings about interactive writing found in chapter two, the review of the literature. This chapter, chapter five, contains the summary, conclusions, and recommendations for practice and further research. Conclusions This study followed one class of kindergarteners through an interactive writing treatment to determine what effect, if any, the interactive writing model would have on their writing. Initially, the goal of this study was to determine the effectiveness of an interactive writing program on a kindergarten population’s attempts at writing, vocabulary development, and expression. It became apparent, after beginning the treatment, that these aspirations were lofty and inappropriate for my population. Many of the students were inexperienced writers starting at a level far below the standards to facilitate this request. 66 Clay (1991) calls interactive writing a responsive model of teaching in which the instructor provides the clearest example of instruction in response to the learner’s needs at that particular stage in their literacy. Instructor flexibility is one reason why interactive writing is appropriate for teaching kindergarten writing - it allows a teacher to be attentive to the variable skill levels of a diverse population. If, for instance, members of my population had been more advanced than my initial design, I would have adjusted my teaching to address their particular writing needs while still facilitating the educational requirements of the inexperienced writers. Interpretation of the findings will be organized around the three questions that helped direct the study. Question 1: On the orthographic writing developmental continuum, to what degree did the students’ writing change from their pre-treatment writing sample to their posttreatment writing sample? As evidenced by research, learning to spell is intertwined with learning to read. Children’s ability to detect and write the sounds in words requires knowledge of letters and phonemic segmentation (Ehri, & Roberts, 2006). Additionally, the most practical approach to literacy development is to guide children to learn reading and writing at the same time (Clay 1991, 1998, 2001). Thus, it is not surprising to discover that the majority of participants’ writing as demonstrated by orthographic character development increased from before the treatment to after the treatment. This may be due to the structure of the interactive writing lesson. Daily teacher-supported writing instruction reinforced letter/sound acquisition. Using a Slinky to stretch letter sounds, supplied a visual 67 representation of letter sounds in words. The integration of whiteboards, used by the students, promoted active participation while assisting the teacher’s daily assessments on the lessons and student progress. Published text displayed on the walls filled the environment with student-created print. The children showed pride in their displayed work, which was doubly effective as a tool to consolidate reading and an enthusiasm for literacy. Reading is an integral part of the interactive writing method (McCarrier, Pinnell, & Fountas, 2000). The interactive writing lesson supports the understanding that what children say can be written down. Rereading text after recording each word reinforces the writing/reading connection. Therefore, it seems necessary for kindergarten teachers to employ instructional methods that include both reading and writing, such as the ones described in the interactive writing lesson, to promote orthographic character development in students’ writing. Orthographic characteristics increased measurably for a few students. The focus of the overall writing assignment was probably within their zone of proximal development (Vygotsky, 1978). Within the stages of orthographic knowledge, Invernizzi, Abouzeid, & Gill (1994) call the zone of proximal development the ideas students use, but confuse. This refers to the notion that students attempt to use a concept in word knowledge, but are not consistent with applying the concept correctly each time. Then the instruction that focuses on orthographic features and patterns used, but confused by a student, would most benefit that student. Consequently, interactive writing instruction should include a range of student involvement based on the teacher’s knowledge of each individual’s strengths and needs. 68 Many students increased one stage in orthographic knowledge. These students began the treatment at different stages of spelling. Mini-lessons, also known as teaching points (Swartz, Klein, & Shook, 2002), probably account for this development. Letter work is an example of a mini-lesson; students receive direct instruction to develop their connection of a letter sound to the letter symbol. While the writer was at the easel, the teacher assessed needs of the students seated at the carpet by viewing their responses on the whiteboard paddles. Student confusion or misunderstanding was addressed immediately and in context to the overall lesson. Examination of orthographic writing results from the initial to the final writing sample caused me to inquire about the seven students who showed no change in development of orthographic characteristics and the two students whose scores decreased in orthographic development. I was interested in knowing where on the orthographic writing developmental continuum students placed at the onset of this study. In addition, I wondered if the treatment failed these students or if the data would give insight into a more positive result. Further inspection of individual results showed that of the students whose scores did not change, one of these students began the treatment writing at the Early Letter Name-Alphabetic stage. Four of these students began the study writing at the Late Emergent stage. The last two students began the treatment writing at the Middle Emergent stage. Given that these students fall within the age range of emergent spelling, from one to seven years of age, and most of these non-advancing students began the treatment near the top of the emergent scale, it appears that they are developing appropriate writing skills. Perhaps a seven week writing program was not long enough to 69 bring about measurable writing improvement in these students. Since the interactive writing treatment did not end when the treatment ended, evaluation of student progress at the end of the year may yield greater advances in orthographic development. The two students whose orthographic writing characteristics decreased both began the treatment writing at the Middle Emergent level and wrote at the Early Emergent level when the treatment ended. Prior to implementing the interactive writing treatment, the teacher had been addressing the educational needs of these two students. Evaluation outside of the interactive writing program indicated areas of concern. Review of daily assessments showed that at the time of the last writing sample, neither student had mastered alphabetic knowledge as measured by singing the ABC song or accurately naming random letters. Furthermore, both had a limited knowledge of letter sounds. One student knew seven letter sounds and the other student knew zero letter sounds. Further assessment revealed each child’s specific areas of deficit which were being addressed with a direct instruction one-on-one intervention program. The implication is that these two students may have performed poorly on the assessment due to their needs in other areas. As writing is developmental, it also may be that these students need more time in learning this way of writing, and that perhaps they are neither ready nor are able at this point to master this process. Research confirms that adults play a major role in children’s literacy acquisition (Clay, 1998; McCarrier et al., 2000; Snow et al., 1998; Vygotsky, 1978). Inclusion of adult-supported play-based literacy centers may have played an additional role in furthering participants’ orthographic writing characteristics. Martinez and Teale state that 70 the presence of an adult in the writing center has the greatest impact on the children’s writing (1987). The adult encourages the children to think about what they want to say before they write and to work out writing problems. The adult facilitator serves as an audience, provides feedback for the children’s writing, and records the writing for the children whose writing can not be easily read (Martinez & Teale, 1987). For the sake of this program and in consideration of the population served, I permitted some preschool standards to guide an area that would have otherwise been free-exploration. Yaden et al. in Kamil (2000) wrote that the addition of themed literacy materials increases the amount of literacy-related activities children engage in during play (Morrow & Rand, 1991). Writing development requires practice, and many of the participants began kindergarten with minimal exposure to generating their own writing. Vygotsky (1978) asserted that children develop higher psychological processes within the social environment when an adult or “more literate other” plays an active role in the social construct. Adult modeling and interaction to guide the children’s attention and learning through daily writing practice, as outlined in the preschool standards, extended the lessons from the interactive writing session into the students’ own writing (Child Development Division, 2008; California Department of Education, 1999; McCarrier et al., 2000; Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998; Teale, & Martinez, 1987). Conclusions from examination of the data indicate that the participants developed more sophisticated orthographic writing characteristics as an outcome of this treatment. Not only did most participants’ writing improve, but many students’ writing improved significantly. Struggling students benefited most from the interactive writing treatment. 71 Each student who started the treatment writing at the Early Emergent writing level advanced at least one level with one student advancing three levels. As for the two students whose orthographic writing characteristics decreased, both experienced an opportunity to explore their own thoughts on paper and were exposed to the writing process. Question 2: In what ways did instruction in interactive writing make a difference in the student’s writing? McCarrier, et al., contends that writing is a complex process, and interactive writing is an effective way to support children as they become writers. Through the act of collaboratively composing text, children learn the fundamentals of writing. During the interactive writing experience, children learn to consider appropriate words, phrases, organization of text, and layout (2000). Swartz, et al. (2002) remind us that beginning writers need to learn the characteristics and functions of the printed word, and more importantly, that each concept associated with writing needs to be taught (explicitly). When the teacher chooses a child to “share the pen,” this decision is based on what the teacher knows to be that child’s zone of proximal development (Vygotsky, 1978). Measurable differences in student writing between the initial and final writing sample are illustrated in Appendix F. A notable comparison between the initial and final writing sample was that more than half of the participants used pretend writing in the initial sample, but only one child wrote with pretend features in the final sample. This seems logical since 91 percent of participants’ initial writing was located within the emergent writing spelling stages. One reason for this improvement could be daily 72 instruction connecting letters together to make words, words which the children devised themselves. Brotherton, & Williams accept that interactive writing provides children with a balance of explicit instruction and contextual participation in meaningful writing activities (2002). Another notable outcome revealed in the initial assessment for this treatment was that most participants apparently learned directionality, left to right in English, prior to entering school. Perhaps this relates to the notion that children are exposed to this at an early age through watching adults read stories. The children observe the adult’s eyes sweep across the page in the direction of the print. At the end of the treatment, all but two children demonstrated directionality in the final assessment. Repeated reading of the text during the interactive writing sessions may have helped to reinforce this important concept of print. Some other interesting information emerged from the final data analysis. As noted in the Appendix F results, the incidence of wrapping, returning to the left side of the paper when reaching the end of a line, called return sweep by Swartz, et al. (2002), stayed the same between the initial writing sample and the final writing sample. This may accord support to the students’ developing concepts about print. The examples from the initial writing sample which demonstrated wrapping were strings of letters which filled the page. No pages in the second sample were full of letters. Only five of the final writing samples were more than one line long. Therefore, these were the only examples of wrapping. This does not necessarily mean that the children who wrote on only one line do 73 not recognize and use the concept of wrapping. Mastery of this concept may be related to the reduction of students’ pretend writing. Data from Appendix F indicates that interactive writing appears to be a good instructional tool for instruction in writing conventions such as spacing between words and punctuation. More than half of the participants demonstrated some knowledge of these two concepts in their final writing sample. The frequency that spaces and punctuation occur in writing causes them to be part of the daily writing dialogue. Explicit teaching and guidance toward an attention to concrete examples reinforces what the teacher wants the students to comprehend (McCarrier et al., 2000). As illustrated in Appendix F, some of the students’ final writing samples included awareness of sight words and norms in conventional writing, for example, sentences begin with a capital letter. Six of the twenty-two children practiced these skills. These students were ready to transfer the daily explicit instruction of these concepts to their writing. While they were letter-name alphabetic spellers and regarded each sound letter by letter as they wrote, they had progressed in their attention to print to include in their writing sight words and the convention of capitalization (Bear, Invernizzi, Templeton, & Johnston, 2000) Research indicates that writing requires a level of phonological awareness, most notably blending and segmenting (Adams, 1990). Daily interactive writing instruction included the use of a Slinky to blend the sounds of the words as they were written. Students’ names were used as a source for first letter sounds, onset-rime, and rhyme 74 (Bloodgood, 1999; Snow et al., 1998). This instruction led to some students demonstrating more conventional spelling in their final writing sample. Question 3: How closely were the observations of the observers aligned with the educator’s journal? The educator’s journal and observer’s notes were closely aligned except in the area of student engagement during the lesson. The first observer noted teacher redirection of student misbehavior on occasions that were missing in the educator’s journal. The educator’s journal focused on academic goals with student engagement during and after the interactive writing session discussed in the notes. The teacher paid attention to enthusiasm for the process or understanding of the product rather than the number of times she interrupted the lesson to redirect a student. The first observer’s notes made clear that some students were not involved in either the process or the product. This revelation caused the teacher to offer her students whiteboard paddles before each interactive writing session from that time forward. Student engagement increased as noted by both the teacher and the second and third observers. Research suggests that writing development is a process that is constructed through an interrelationship between social interaction, oral discourse, and written communication (Martinez & Teale, 1987; Neuman et al., 2000; Snow et al., 1998; Sulzby & Teale in Barr, 1991). The observer’s notes showed the interplay between the participants and the process. Interactive writing provides scaffolding at the point along the developmental continuum when children begin to recognize the difference between their quasi-writing and the print they encounter daily. At this stage, when a child states, ‘I 75 can’t write,’ the message is ‘I am sophisticated enough to recognize that my writing doesn’t look like the writing I see in books or around me in my environment.’ What I realized through this project was that regardless of the education the children bring to kindergarten, my duty to meet the standards and spark each child’s interest in writing requires a program that deals with each student as an individual while simultaneously addressing the reality of growing class sizes. Interactive writing seems to be suitable for all populations of kindergarten writers. Recommendations for Practice and Further Study This study demonstrated only a portion of the potential effect of interactive writing on kindergarteners’ writing development. This small population responded positively to the interactive writing treatment. Therefore, it would be interesting to see the results of this treatment on a larger population. Also, since the instructor targets the interactive writing lesson to the needs of the population being served, the implication is that interactive writing lends itself to student understanding of wider writing subjects. As in Craig’s (2003) study, further research with small group sizes may have positive effects. Dividing the groups into small classes of four or five children with common strengths and learning goals could be an intervention for the two students whose orthographic development decreased during my interactive writing treatment. Similarly, small group sizes would help to accurately document the progress of students who started the program at a more advanced stage. Comparing the school’s mandated intervention program with small group interactive writing groups would be another fascinating expansion of this study. Research from Yaden, Rowe, and MacGillavray (2000) suggests 76 that kindergarten children designated at-risk respond very positively to emergent literacy programs. The features of emergent literacy are shared with interactive writing and include the following: consider the children as socially competent partners; permit experimentation in a non-threatening environment; include adult- and peer- lead dialogue about literacy; and provide opportunities for children to practice their unconventional, yet emerging skills (Yaden et al., 2000). Since ongoing assessment informs teachers’ decisions, use of Henderson’s (1981) stages of spelling and orthographic knowledge throughout the treatment could provide data to guide small group lessons throughout the program. The extent of the effect of the play-based literacy center on participants’ writing development is another area which could yield interesting results on effective instruction in further study. With the strict adherence to standards in the current kindergarten classroom, and considering the time constraints of a half-day program, play is never mentioned within the framework of the English Language Arts Content Standards for Kindergarten (California Department of Education, 1998). Based on the range of abilities displayed by students within this program, an assumption that students enter school ready for the demands of kindergarten is false. One can not assume that all children have received the benefits of prior experiences either in a developmentally appropriate preschool or other setting. Further study into whether play would benefit this population could help both teachers to coordinate lessons for children who display a use for playbased academic development, and policy makers to legislate standards that serve well the needs of the population. 77 While the results are encouraging for teacher practice and student writing development, further and more extensive research is needed to determine the impact of interactive writing instruction on development of orthographic characteristics. 78 APPENDICES 79 APPENDIX A Consent to Participate in Research 80 October 14, 2009 Consent to Participate in Research As your child’s teacher, I am asking that you allow me to work with your child on research supported writing instruction. I am interested in discovering if interactive writing will help your kindergarten child more than a different way to teach writing. I have every reason to believe it the best way to teach writing to kindergarteners, and am looking forward to finding this out this year. Since I am getting my Master’s Degree in Language and Literacy at California State University, Sacramento, I hoped you would allow your child to be included in the study. He or she will not do anything different in the classroom if you decide not to allow him/her to be part of the study, but if you sign this form, I will be able to use your child’s writings to see what kind of writing changes occur from the beginning of the year until the end. I would be delighted to share these results with you at the end of the year if you would like me to do so. Your child’s participation is quite simple. At the beginning of the school year a writing assessment will be administered to each child in the classroom. Writing will then be taught using an interactive method involving all students in the writing process. In February, another writing assessment will be given. Your child’s name will be kept anonymous in the reporting and analysis of this research. I request children’s initials for organizational purposes and will assign each child a number for confidentiality purposes. If you choose to consent to your child’s participation please return this form by October 26, 2009. Should you have any question or are interested in the results of this research study please email me at jjentzen@amadorcoe.k12.ca.us, or phone my advisor, Dr. Marcy Merrill at (916) 278-5524 at CSUS. ______ Yes, my child _____________________________ may participate in this research project. ______ No, my child _____________________________ may not participate in this research project. _____________________________________ Signature of Parent/ Guardian Date ____________________ 81 APPENDIX B Sample Interactive Writing Lesson 82 Interactive Writing Lesson Date________1/19/2010_____ Lesson preparation routine: 1. Sing ABC Song while handing out ABC Linking Charts. Signals start of lesson 2. Teacher distributes whiteboard paddles and cloth erasers. 3. Review Rules/Procedures/Expectations of interactive writing lesson Lesson: 1. Teacher introduces topic: Write a letter to this week’s Star Student 2. Students pair to formulate text. 3. Share. Teacher randomly chooses student as speaker for the pair. Repeat until text is agreed upon 4. Class agrees on text: Dear G., You will always be our friend. 5. Repeat the text; enunciating each word; counting the words in each sentence. Teacher and students in unison show number of words with fingers. 6. Teacher writes on a note card exact wording as agreed upon. 7. Slinky-stretch each word prior to student writing. Using a Slinky and voice, continuously stretch each letter sound in each word. 8. Student writes exact word as negotiated. Randomly chosen student (ensure that student will be successful - based on ability): student may be a space holder, write a letter, write part of a word, write a whole word, or write several words. 9. “Fix it” or “Boo boo” tape: White address labels are placed over mistakes with reassurance that mistakes are part of the process. 10. Students on the carpet are engaged in writing/Teacher instructs on relevant topic: first letter sound corresponding with student’s name; analogy; direct to previous writing/environmental print… 11. Read the text. Teacher and students re-read the text; ensure that students construct conventional text. 12. Repeat the process from number 7. 13. Close session: Reread the day’s text 83 APPENDIX C Characteristics of Emergent Spelling 84 Characteristics of Emergent Spelling What Students Do Correctly What They Use but Confuse What is Absent Early Emergent Write on the page Hold the writing implement Drawing and scribbling for writing Sound-symbol correspondence Directionality Middle Emergent Horizontal movement across page Clear distinction between writing and drawing Lines and dots for writing Letter-like forms Letters, numbers, Sound-symbol and letter-like correspondence forms Writing may wrap from right to left at the end of a line Late Emergent V for elevator D for down Consistent directionality Some letter sound match Substitutions of letter which sound, feel, and look alike: B/p, D/b Bear, Invernizzi, Templeton, & Johnston (2000) Complete soundsymbol correspondence Consistent spacing between words 85 APPENDIX D Characteristics of Letter Name-Alphabetic Spelling 86 Characteristics of Letter Name-Alphabetic Spelling What They Use but Confuse Letters based on point of articulation: J,JRF for drive Often long vowels by letter name What is Absent Early Letter NameAlphabetic B, BD for bed S, SHP for ship Y, for when L, LP for lump U for you R for are FT for float What Students Do Correctly Represent most salient sounds, usually beginning consonants Directionality Most letters of the alphabet Clear letter-sound correspondences Partial spelling of consonant blends and digraphs Middle Letter NameAlphabetic BAD for bed SEP or SHP for ship FOT for float LOP for lump Most beginning and ending consonants Clear letter-sound correspondences Frequently occurring short vowel words Substitutions of letter name closest in point of articulation for short vowels Some consonant blends and digraphs Preconsonantal nasals: LOP for lump Late Letter NameAlphabetic lump spelled correctly FLOT for float BAKR for baker PLAS for place BRIT for bright All of the above Substitutions of plus: common patterns for low-frequency Regular Short short vowels: vowel patterns COT for caught Most consonants blends and digraphs Preconsonantal nasals Some common long vowel words: time, name Bear, Invernizzi, Templeton, & Johnston (2000) Consistency on beginning and end of syllables Some spacing between words Vowels in syllables Most long vowel markers or silent vowels Vowels in unstressed syllables 87 APPENDIX E Characteristics of Student Writing 88 Characteristics of Student Writing Student Pretest 1 Middle Emergent Posttest Middle Letter NameAlphabetic 2 Late Emergent Late Letter Name-Alphabetic 3 3 Late Emergent Late Emergent 0 4 Middle Emergent Early Emergent -1 5 Middle Emergent Middle Emergent 0 6 Late Emergent Late Emergent Early Letter NameAlphabetic Early Letter NameAlphabetic Middle Letter NameAlphabetic 0 10 Late Emergent Late Emergent 0 11 Early Emergent Middle Emergent 1 12 Late Emergent 0 13 Late Emergent Early Letter Name14 Alphabetic Late Emergent Early Letter NameAlphabetic Early Letter NameAlphabetic 15 Early Emergent Middle Emergent 1 16 Middle Emergent 0 17 Middle Emergent Early Letter Name18 Alphabetic Middle Emergent Early Letter NameAlphabetic Middle Letter NameAlphabetic 19 Middle Emergent Early Emergent 20 Early Emergent Middle Emergent 1 21 Middle Emergent Late Emergent Middle Letter NameAlphabetic 1 7 Late Emergent 8 Early Emergent 9 Late Emergent 22 Middle Emergent Comparison 3 1 3 2 1 0 2 1 -1 3 89 APPENDIX F Changes in Writing Characteristics 90 Changes in Writing Characteristics 13 20 19 5 5 4 3 15 0 11 0 6 0 4 0 6 0 3 0 First Digraphs Sight English Spacing Punctuation Letter and Words Words Capital Blends Pre-treatment Writing 1 Writing Letter/ Characteristic Pretend Direction Wrapping Sound Writing Match Post- treatment Writing 91 APPENDIX G Incidence of Occurrence based on Educator’s Journal 92 Daily Occasionally Often Never Often Often Phonemic Substitution Daily Daily Daily Daily Partial LetterSound Recognition (Name as a source for letters and sounds) Phonemic Awareness Often Daily Never Never Phonemic Segmentation Occasionally Often Often Never Phonemic Blending Daily Occasionally Never Daily Syllables Daily Daily Never Never Alliteration Direct Instruction Often Occasionally Never Onset-rime Student Response Never Occasionally Rhyme Synthesis Never Focus Spontaneous 93 Concepts of Print Focus Directionality Concepts of Word Daily Punctuation Reading Concepts Daily Concepts of Letter Daily Direct Instruction Student Response Synthesis Spontaneous Daily Daily Daily Daily Daily Daily Often Daily Daily Daily Daily Daily Daily Daily Daily Often Often Writing Conventions Focus Direct Instruction Student Response Synthesis Spontaneous Spelling Capitalization Punctuation Grammar (spacing between words) Daily Daily Daily Daily Daily Daily Daily Daily Daily Daily Occasionally Occasionally Daily Daily Occasionally Occasionally Parts of a Letter Focus Direct Instruction Student Response Synthesis Spontaneous Heading Daily Greeting Daily Body Daily Closing Daily Often Daily Daily Often Occasionally Never Daily Daily Daily Daily Occasionally Occasionally 94 Environmental Print Focus Direct Instruction Student Response Synthesis Spontaneous Sight Word Name Chart Blends and Digraphs Chart Posters Daily Daily Occasionally Daily Daily Occasionally Often Often Daily Daily Infrequently Infrequently Alphabetic Knowledge Focus Direct Instruction Student Response Synthesis Spontaneous Naming Letters Recognizing Letters In Print Daily Daily Daily Daily Daily Daily Daily Daily Definition of terms: The terms Daily, Often, Occasionally, Infrequently, and Never, have the same meaning in all areas, but encompass different populations. Terms under the heading Direct Instruction, represent the teacher’s actions. Terms under the heading Student Response, represent the students’ actions during the interactive writing treatment. Terms under the headings Synthesis and Spontaneous, represent the majority of students’ actions outside of the interactive writing treatment. The term Spelling has a broad definition here. Emergent spelling including invented spelling is appropriately considered spelling for this population. 95 REFERENCES Adams, M. J. (1994). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Ashton-Warner, S. (1986). Teacher. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster. Barr, R. (1991). Handbook of reading research. New York, NY: Longman. Bear, D. R., Invernizzi, M., Templeton, S., & Johnston, F. (2000). Words their way: Word study for phonics, vocabulary, and spelling instruction. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill. Berninger, V. W., Vaughan, K., Abbott, R. D., Brooks, A., Abbott, S. P., Rogan, L., Graham, S. (1998). Early intervention for spelling problems: Teaching functional spelling units of varying size with a multiple-connections framework. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(4), 587-605. Bloodgood, J. W. (1999). What's in a name? Children's name writing and literacy acquisition. Reading Research Quarterly, 34(3), 342-367. Boroski, L. (2004). An introduction to interactive writing. In G. E. Tompkins & S. Collom (Authors), Sharing the pen: Interactive writing with young children (pp. 1-4). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson. Brotherton, S., & Williams, C. (2002). Interactive writing instruction in a first grade title 1 literacy program. Journal of Reading Education, 27(3), 8-19. Button, K., Johnson, M. J., & Furgerson, P. (1996). Interactive writing in a primary classroom. The Reading Teacher, 49(6), 446-454. 96 California Department of Education. (1995). Every child a reader: The report of the California Reading Task Force . Sacramento, CA: California Department of Education. California Department of Education. (1998). English-language arts content standards for California public schools: Kindergarten through grade twelve. Sacramento, CA: California Department of Education. California State Board of Education. (1999). Reading/language arts framework for California public schools: Kindergarten through grade twelve. Sacramento, CA: California Department of Education. Child Development Division. (2008). California preschool learning foundations (Vol. 1). Sacramento, CA: California Department of Education. Chin, B. A. (Ed.). (1996). Standards for the English language arts. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Clay, M. M. (1975). What did I write? beginning writing behaviour. London: Heinemann Educational. Clay, M. M. (1991). Becoming literate: The construction of inner control. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Clay, M. M. (1998). By different paths to common outcomes. York, ME: Stenhouse. Clay, M. M. (2001). Change over time in children's literacy development. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. 97 Coker, D. (2007). Writing instruction for young children: Methods targeting the multiple demands that writers face. In S. Graham, C. A. MacArthur, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Best practices in writing instruction (pp. 101-117). New York, NY: The Guilford Press. Cowen, J. E. (2003). A balanced approach to beginning reading instruction: A synthesis of six major U.S. research studies. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Craig, S. A. (2003). The effects of an adapted interactive writing intervention on kindergarten children's phonological awareness, spelling, and early reading development. Reading Research Quarterly, 38(4), 438-440. Cramer, R. L. (1978). Children's writing and language growth. Columbus, OH: Merrill. Cramer, R. L. (2001). Creative power: The nature and nurture of children's writing. New York, NY: Longman. Dickinson, D. K., & Neuman, S. B. (2006). Handbook of early literacy research. (Vol. 2). New York, NY: Guilford. Dixon, C. N., & Nessel, D. D. (1983). Language experience approach to reading (and writing): Language-experience reading for second language learners. Hayward, CA: Alemany Press. Ehri, L. C., Roberts, T., & Neuman, S. B. (2006). The roots of learning to read and write: Acquisition of letters and phonemic awareness. In D. K. Dickinson (Ed.), Handbook of early literacy research (Vol. 2, pp. 113-131). New York, NY: The Guilford Press. 98 Ferreiro, E., & Teberosky, A. (1982). Literacy before schooling. Exeter, NH: Heinemann Educational Books. Gentry, J. R. (1981). Learning to spell developmentally. The Reading Teacher, 34(4), 378-381. Graves, D. H. (2003). Writing: Teachers and children at work. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Harris, T. L., & Hodges, R. E. (1995). The literacy dictionary: The vocabulary of reading and writing. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Henderson, E. H. (1981). Learning to read and spell: The child's knowledge of words. DeKalb, IL: Northern Illinois University Press. Henderson, E. H. (1985). Teaching spelling. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin. Henderson, E. H., & Beers, J. W. (1980). Developmental and cognitive aspects of learning to spell: A reflection of word knowledge. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Invernizzi, M., Abouzeid, M., & Gill, J. T. (1994). Using student's invented spellings as a guide for spelling instruction that emphasizes word study. The Elementary School Journal, 95(2), 155-167. Joyce, W. (1985). George shrinks. New York, NY: Harper & Row. Kamil, M. L., Mosenthal, P. B., Pearson, P. D., & Barr, R. (Eds.). (2000). Handbook of reading research. (Vol. 3). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence. Erlbaum Associates. 99 Lyman, F. (1981). The responsive classroom discussion: Inclusion of all students. In A. S. Anderson (Ed.), Mainstreaming Digest (pp. 109-113). College Park, MD: University of Maryland College of Education. Martinez, M., & Teale, W. H. (1987). The ins and outs of a kindergarten writing program. The Reading Teacher, 40(4), 444-451. McCarrier, A., Fountas, I. C., & Pinnell, G. S. (2000). Interactive writing: How language and literacy come together, K-2. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. McCloskey, S. (2004). Grouping options to support student success. In G. E. Tompkins & S. Collom (Authors), Sharing the pen: Interactive writing with young children (pp. 9-13). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education. McKenzie, M. G. (1985). Shared writing: Apprenticeship in writing. Language Matters, 1(2), 1-5. Morrow, L. M., & Rand, M. K. (1991). Promoting literacy during play by designing early childhood classroom environments. The Reading Teacher, 44(6), 396-402. National Commission on Writing. (2003). The neglected "R": The need for a writing revolution (Rep.). New York, NY: College Entrance Examination Board. National Early Literacy Panel. (2008). Developing early literacy: Report of the national early literacy panel: A scientific synthesis of early literacy development and implications of intervention. Washington, DC: National Institute for Literacy. 100 National Reading Panel. Teaching children to read : An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. (2000). Washington, D.C.: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, National Institutes of Health. Neuman, S. B., Copple, C., & Bredekamp, S. (2000). Learning to read and write: Developmentally appropriate practices for young children. Washington, D.C.: National Association for the Education of Young Children. Read, C. (1986). Children's creative spelling. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Robinson, V. B., Ross, G., & Neal, H. C. (2000). Emergent literacy in kindergarten: A review of the research and related suggested activities and learning strategies. San Mateo, CA: California Kindergarten Association. Roth, K. (2009). Interactive writing: Investigating the effectiveness of a dynamic approach to writing instruction for first graders (Unpublished master's thesis). Harvard University. doi: UMI 3357300 Setzer, M. L. (2005). The role of interactive writing instruction in developing writing and phonological awareness in kindergarten (Unpublished master's thesis). California State University, Sacramento. Sipe, L. R. (2001). Invention, convention, and intervention: Invented spelling and the teacher's role. The Reading Teacher, 55(3), 264-273. Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. 101 Spiegel, D. L. (1992). Blending whole language and systematic direct instruction. The Reading Teacher, 46(1), 38-44. Standards for the English language arts. (1996). Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Stewart, C. (2004). Building on every child's favorite word. In G. E. Tompkins & S. Collom (Authors), Sharing the pen: Interactive writing with young children (pp. 14-17). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education. Sulzby E.(1996). Roles of oral and written language as children approach conventional literacy. In C. Pontecorvo, M. Orsolini, B. Burge, & L. Resnick (Eds.), Children's Early Text Construction (pp. 25-46). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Sulzby, E., & Teale, W. H. (1991). Handbook of reading research. (Vol. 2, pp. 727-757) (R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson, Eds.). New York, NY: Longman. Swartz, S. L., Klein, A. F., & Shook, R. E. (2002). Interactive writing & interactive editing: Making connections between writing and reading. Carlsbad, CA: Dominie Press. Tangel, D. M., & Blachman, B. A. (1995). Effect of phoneme awareness instruction on invented spelling of first-grade children: A one-year follow-up. Journal of Reading Behavior, 27(2), 153-185. 102 Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction: Reports of the subgroups. (2000). Washington, DC: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, National Institutes of Health. Teale, W. H., & Sulzby, E. (1986). Emergent literacy: Writing and reading. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Pub. Tompkins, G. E., & Collom, S. (2004). Sharing the pen: Interactive writing with young children. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson. Van Allen, R. (1969, September 29). Language experiences which promote reading. Speech presented at Early childhood lecture series in Eastern Michigan University, Ypsilanti, MI. Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press. Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development and emergent literacy. Child Development, 69(3), 848-872. Yaden, D. B., Rowe, D. W., & MacGillivray, L. (2000). Emergent literacy: A matter (polyphony) of perspectives. In M. L. Kamil (Ed.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 3, pp. 425-454). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Yopp, H. K., & Yopp, R. H. (2000). Supporting phonemic awareness development in the classroom.