THE EFFECTS OF LEADER SEX, LEADERSHIP STYLE, AND LOCUS OF CONTROL ON EMPLOYEE SATISFACTION Michelle Christina Garbato B.S., Rochester Institute of Technology, 2005 THESIS Submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS in PSYCHOLOGY (Industrial/Organizational Psychology) at CALIFORNIA STATE UNIVERSITY, SACRAMENTO SUMMER 2010 THE EFFECTS OF LEADER SEX, LEADERSHIP STYLE, AND LOCUS OF CONTROL ON EMPLOYEE SATISFACTION A Thesis by Michelle Christina Garbato Approved by: __________________________________, Committee Chair Lawrence Meyers, Ph.D. __________________________________, Second Reader Greg Hurtz, Ph.D. __________________________________, Third Reader Caio Miguel, Ph.D. ____________________________ Date ii Student: Michelle Christina Garbato I certify that this student has met the requirements for format contained in the University format manual, and that this thesis is suitable for shelving in the Library and credit is to be awarded for the thesis. __________________________, Graduate Coordinator Jianjian Qin, Ph.D. Department of Psychology iii ___________________ Date Abstract of THE EFFECTS OF LEADER SEX, LEADERSHIP STYLE, AND LOCUS OF CONTROL ON EMPLOYEE SATISFACTION by Michelle Christina Garbato Role congruity theory proposes that prejudice against women in leadership positions may be due to an incongruity between sex roles and leadership roles. Furthermore, leader sex, leadership style, and employee locus of control may also affect employee satisfaction. In the present study, one hundred and sixty undergraduates (132 females, 28 males, mean age 20 years) completed the Internal Control Index; using a vignette methodology, participants read a situation that described a theoretical job and then they completed the Job Descriptive Index, Job in General Scale, and Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire (short form) and the Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire (long form: supervision subscale). Results showed that higher internal locus of control and a democratic leadership style were associated with greater job satisfaction, while no effects for sex of protagonists were obtained. Results are interpreted in context of role congruity theory. _______________________, Committee Chair Lawrence Meyers, Ph.D. _______________________ Date iv ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Completing a Master’s degree is truly an accomplishment, and I would not have been able to complete this journey without the aid and support of many people over the past few years. I must first express my gratitude towards my committee chair and advisor, Dr. Lawrence Meyers. His leadership, patience, support, dedication, and scholarship have set an example I hope I can achieve one day. I would also like to thank my committee members, Dr. Gregory Hurtz and Dr. Caio Miguel for their assistance throughout the entire process, as well as the students and professors I have had the opportunity to work with at California State University, Sacramento. Each individual has positively contributed to my academic experience and has helped me grow more than I ever thought possible. I must also give thanks to my emotional support system, consisting mainly of family and friends, who have always been there to help lift me up when I felt as though I could not finish. I thank my parents and my extended family, for instilling in me a confidence and a drive for pursuing my Master’s degree, and my friends who have always been there for me and pushed me to go the extra mile. I am truly blessed to have such a strong support system and such beneficial academic experiences in my life. I certainly could not have finished without their support and for that, I dedicate this thesis to all of the people who have helped me pursue my dreams. v TABLE OF CONTENTS Page Acknowledgments..................................................................................................................... v List of Tables ........................................................................................................................ viii Chapter 1. INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................................. 1 2. LITERATURE REVIEW ................................................................................................... 3 Background ................................................................................................................... 3 History of Gender Roles .............................................................................................. 3 Biological Gender Differences .................................................................................... 7 Underlying Gender Role Theories ............................................................................... 9 Gender Inequality and Social Role Theory ................................................................ 13 Socialization of Gender Roles ................................................................................... 14 Gender Characteristics ............................................................................................... 19 Gender and Leadership Styles ................................................................................... 20 Leadership Styles and Employee Satisfaction ........................................................... 26 Social Role Theory and Role Congruity Theory ....................................................... 30 Women in the Workplace .......................................................................................... 36 Locus of Control ........................................................................................................ 40 Hypotheses ................................................................................................................. 46 3. METHOD ......................................................................................................................... 47 Participants................................................................................................................. 47 Materials .................................................................................................................... 48 Design ........................................................................................................................ 53 Procedure ................................................................................................................... 55 4. RESULTS ......................................................................................................................... 57 Dichotomizing Locus of Control ............................................................................... 58 Interaction Effects ...................................................................................................... 58 Effects for the Independent Variables........................................................................ 59 vi 5. DISCUSSION ................................................................................................................... 63 Limitations and Suggestions for Future Research ..................................................... 72 Research Implications ................................................................................................ 76 Appendix A. Human Subjects Application .......................................................................... 79 Appendix B. Instructions (Read to Participants) .................................................................. 84 Appendix C. Vignettes .......................................................................................................... 85 Appendix D. Demographic & Organizational Questions ...................................................... 93 References ............................................................................................................................... 97 vii LIST OF TABLES Page 1. Table 1 Multivariate and Univariate Analysis of Variance for Employee Satisfaction Measures ............................................................................................. 60 2. Table 2 Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for Employee Satisfaction Measures as a Function of Leadership Style ..................................................... 61 3. Table 3 Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for Employee Satisfaction Measures as a Function of Locus of Control ..................................................... 62 viii 1 Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION We have long understood that employees are an organization’s most valuable asset, yet their satisfaction is often an overlooked factor in organizational success. Management continues to deny how pertinent employee satisfaction is to financial performance. Many factors can affect employee satisfaction, such as leadership style, leader sex, and employee locus of control. By understanding employees and what satisfies them, we can then improve organizational loyalty, absenteeism, turnover, productivity, customer satisfaction, and organizational success. By creating a healthy work environment, employee satisfaction and job performance will blossom, allowing the organization to flourish as well. Studies tend to focus on factors, such as leadership style, leader sex, and their interaction, to investigate employee satisfaction. These studies tend to exclude personality characteristics that can affect employee satisfaction. More specifically, experts in the field of psychology have attempted to investigate how leadership style (e.g., autocratic, democratic, interpersonal, task oriented) and leader sex (i.e., male, female) affect employee satisfaction (Eagly, 2007). Although these factors are important to our understanding of employee satisfaction, one factor that has not received much attention involves the role of locus of control in conjunction with leadership style and leader sex on employee satisfaction. 2 This literature review is broken into three basic sections, including gender roles, leadership styles, and locus of control. First, I will review the history of gender roles and explain how these roles have influenced the gender roles in the United States today. Then I will continue with a discussion on the biological differences that exist between men and women and the implications that biology has on gender roles. Following this, a discussion will open regarding important sociological and psychological theories of the gender institution and gender role theories (e.g., social control theory, cognitive development theory, and social learning theory) and explain how gender roles continue. Then I will review the social role theory and discuss how people are socialized from birth and how gender role socialization later primes gender specific personality characteristics. In the next section, I will discuss how these gender roles spill over into the organizational world in terms of leadership styles. This will include a summary regarding leadership styles and an in depth discussion about gender differences and leadership styles and how the different leadership styles affect employee satisfaction. I will use social role theory and role congruity theory to help explain the differences in leadership styles between men and women. Then I will discuss the emergence of women in the workplace and the implications it has on leadership effectiveness. Finally, I will discuss employee locus of control and the potential effects that it may have on employee satisfaction. Collectively, this will provide an overview of gender roles, leadership styles, and locus of control; it will also help to explain how each of them may interact with one another to affect employee satisfaction. 3 Chapter 2 LITERATURE REVIEW Background In recent years, researchers have shown that people believe effective managers exhibit masculine qualities (e.g., Bass, 1990; Eagly & Johnson, 1990). Some researchers even assert that there are few women in managerial positions because upper level jobs are more characteristic of men (Bass, 1990). This leads to confusion about whether women should adhere to managerial or gender specific roles. Research further demonstrates that subordinates view female leaders negatively and question their abilities and competencies as managers (Riger & Galligan, 1980). In turn, this may affect the leadership styles each gender uses and may negatively impact employee satisfaction. History of Gender Roles Gender roles, or sex roles, are behaviors that society defines as appropriate behaviors for each sex (Whicker & Kronenfeld, 1986). In other words, they are beliefs regarding how men and women should act or behave based upon their identified sex and the attributes that are associated with that sex (Eagly, 1987; Eagly & JohannesenSchmidt, 2001). Based upon these gender roles, women are subordinates and inferior to men. However, evidence suggests that gender roles were not always like this throughout American history. During the beginning of human civilization, women and men were roughly equal in status (Nielsen, 1978). Hunting and gathering societies originated approximately 4 150,000 years ago; they were comprised of people that foraged for food. Since hunters and gatherers moved from one geographical area to the next, there was little opportunity to accumulate material goods. Both men and women worked and contributed resources to the family, which allowed little social stratification to develop. Men typically hunted for meat sources while women gathered plant based food sources. This separate and complimentary division of work ensured that both sexes contributed to the group’s survival (Nielsen, 1978). Despite these differentiated roles, the status remained relatively equal between the sexes until more societies developed (Nielsen, 1978; Whicker & Kronenfeld, 1986). Between 18,000 B.C. and 12,000 B.C, herding and horticulture societies developed in the Near East. These groups discovered that plants could be grown and animals domesticated, which allowed people to settle in a single area for an extended period. As civilizations advanced technologically, stratification between the sexes also increased, prompting gender roles to emerge. Although women were responsible for the majority of the food production in horticulture societies, men held a higher status within society (Nielsen, 1978). By 3,000 B.C., the invention of the plow improved agricultural methods by cultivating soil and producing crops more efficiently. Further agrarian societies then developed and with that, social stratification also increased. Men became primarily responsible for the agricultural work because of the large machinery involved. This increase in labor quantity and intensity forced women to abandon farming tasks, and instead raise children and become homemakers. This change shifted the responsibility of 5 subsistence onto the men and consequentially negatively affected the public lifestyles and status of women. Despite the shift in women’s roles and status throughout history, research shows that men have consistently been at least equal to women in status, regardless of how much labor they provided (Nielsen, 1978). Historians and anthropologists argue that this was due to men’s abilities to excel and specialize in physical activities, such as warfare and herding, which was associated with greater status and wealth (Harris, 1993). Industrialization replaced agricultural methods within the United States by the 1880s. Factories developed and people started working for wages; this prompted large cities to develop and flourish. Textiles (e.g., spinning, weaving), which were traditionally completed by women, had now become factory based. Women’s responsibilities shifted from food production and reproduction (during agrarian societies) to solely reproduction. Furthermore, men were economically important within the home, even though the nature of their jobs had changed (e.g., industrial work; Nielsen, 1978), which further increased social stratification. Historically speaking, the term “woman” was synonymous with the term “family” because women had distinct roles that confined them to the family within the home (Chetwynd & Hartnett, 1978). These attitudes toward women as homemakers prevailed from 1880 to 1940, when women started gaining equal rights, higher status, and employment in the workforce. However, gender roles continued despite the progressive changes that had occurred (Nielsen, 1978). 6 The workforce served as a viable alternative to marriage or a helpful option for women to contribute financially to the family and decrease economic dependence on their male relatives (Storkey, 2001). The invention of birth control in the 1960s allowed women to delay parenthood until later in life so that they could seek employment. Furthermore, advanced technologies deemphasized the need for physical strength, which made it easier for women to enter the workforce (Whicker & Kronenfeld, 1986). Despite these changes though, women still needed to adhere to the same traditional views; therefore, women continued to be subordinates to men in every domain of life, from family dynamics and occupational roles to social policy. There are many theories that explain why gender roles occur. For example, evolutionary psychologists believe that women and men have specific sex mechanisms that have evolved over time due to different pressures faced by each sex in primeval environments. Therefore, men and women are psychologically different and adopted different social roles because of these sex mechanisms (Eagly & Wood, 1999). On the other hand, social structuralists believe that men and women faced different situations across societies and throughout time. Deaux and LaFrance (1998) assert that these cultural and situational conditions affected the opportunities of men and women. Anthropologists believe that sex differences are culturally transmitted behaviors that reflect society’s current values (Rosenberg & Sutton-Smith, 1972). The ecological and social conditions then affect the type of work that each sex engages in because certain sexes can efficiently accomplish certain tasks (Deaux & LaFrance, 1998). Despite 7 this, it is common for women to have fewer resources and lower status and power in America and other societies throughout the world. Unlike traditional attitudes, researchers today believe that some personality attitudes developed because of social stratification. Men are typically associated with more dominant and independent behaviors while women are more submissive and subordinate (e.g., cooperative, nurturing). Yet, historically, many believed a woman’s role was within the home because women were intuitive, nurturing, emotional, and perceived to be easily distracted. In turn, women may have developed superior communication and nurturing abilities because they had to care for their children. On the other hand, a man’s role remained within the workplace and the public arena, as leaders and protectors, because they were believed to be more analytical, objective, and strong. In modern day society, people are socialized to take upon these gender roles (Eagly & Wood, 1999). Biological Gender Differences Historically, it was believed that men were biologically superior to women and naturally inherited these characteristics; to change it was to go against nature and the church’s teachings and beliefs. This distorted view continued until society’s needs and aspirations for women changed (Storkey, 2001). Certainly, biological differences between men and women do exist. For example, men and women have different chromosomes, anatomies, hormones, weight, height, and brain activity. Men are on average 7% taller and 20% heavier than women (Storkey, 2001); although differences exist, they are not a justifiable reason to discriminate against 8 either sex. After all, homosapians share 99.9% similar DNA with one another, meaning that only .1% of our genetic codes for proteins express the differences that exist between us, which account for differences in appearance, personality, and susceptibility to disease (Ptak, Roeder, Stephens, Gilad, & Paabo, 2004). To date, there is no agreement on whether brain differences between men and women are due to biological or environmental influences. Researchers have demonstrated that the right hemisphere of the brain is often associated with emotions, visual tasks, and facial recognition, while the left hemisphere of the brain specializes in language skills. Some research has shown that men and women differ in terms of their left and right hemispheres, where a larger portion in the right hemisphere in male brains is devoted to more spatial tasks (Whicker & Kronenfeld, 1986). Despite the biological differences between the sexes, one cannot dismiss sex roles as merely a reflection of biological differences that exist. The categories male and female, are categories that help to establish one’s identity. People cannot view sex and gender solely in terms of biology; they need to take into account the social, economic, and political factors that affect our perceptions (Storkey, 2001) and take responsibility for the differences in income, status, promotions, and opportunities between the sexes. The term gender, or the act of being masculine or feminine, reflects cultural concepts related to one’s sex. Therefore, being a man or a woman is not merely a reflection of biological makeup, but also behavioral characteristics (e.g., learning to behave in ways that are masculine or feminine). Since what is believed to be masculine and feminine varies from culture to culture, gender roles will vary too. One must fully 9 comprehend these gender roles to understand the implications that they may have in the real world. Underlying Gender Role Theories There are many overlapping theories that describe the formation and persistence of gender roles, but I will describe the most prominent only. Talcott Parson, a prominent American sociologist, devised the notion of the social system and provided a useful framework for understanding gender institutions. Parson believed that society is comprised of interlocking status roles and gender roles. Statuses are positions that individuals engage in, in relation to one another, such as the roles of a manager and a subordinate employee. On the other hand, roles are the behaviors expected of people in certain statuses. For example, a manager’s role is to lead subordinates while their status is one of higher leadership and rank. Collectively, these gender roles project onto different domains in life (e.g., family, education); although every individual may shift their roles and statuses within each domain, there exists a general consistency of roles within each institution (Lengermann & Wallace, 1985). Parson’s social control theory posits that society labels individuals as deviant if they question the social structure. People must adhere to certain beliefs and norms; when compliance does not occur then the individuals are negatively sanctioned both materially (e.g., salary, promotional opportunities) and psychologically (e.g., disapproval, negativity). When an individual challenges these beliefs and norms, inner turmoil, tension, guilt, anxiety and self-doubt may result because the individual and the societal customs are not aligned (Lengermann & Wallace, 1985). 10 Parson further argues that the family serves as the primary influencing agent in teaching children these roles and compliance with them. The family is also the source for the child’s emotional sustenance and social order. To maintain social order in the family, both male and females will each have to specialize in specific roles. Men connect the home and the wider social and natural environments, and must be ambitious and forceful, while women must maintain the functioning of the family internally and remain supportive, affectionate, gentle, and sympathetic. If too much similarity and equality between the gender exist, then the stability and social well-being of the family may be threatened because the genders will be competing with one another (Lengermann & Wallace, 1985). Peter Berger, a sociologist and Lutheran theologian, theorizes that individuals act in a certain way due to knowledge that they have acquired from real social processes. In other words, socialization, or the act of internalizing social meanings, occurs when the individual creates a knowledge set as they experience problems in life. These internalized ideas then become externalized through words, actions, and products where other people use them and integrate them with the social environment. As other people react to one’s words, actions, and products, that individual will further internalize these thoughts and reactions. The gender institution then becomes fragile because it largely depends on the definition of gender roles. Additionally, an individual’s life experience can affect these definitions, making the gender institution always susceptible to change (Lengermann & Wallace, 1985). 11 Cognitive development theory further explains how gender roles continue. These theorists believe that children change throughout their development, similar to those outlined by Piaget (Whicker & Kronenfeld, 1986). They argue that children learn to categorize themselves as boys or girls and learn to take upon roles that are associated with that specific gender. They also learn to categorize other people and are aware of the appropriate behaviors associated with these individuals. Eventually, children learn that one’s sex is constant and that one cannot alter one’s sex by, for example, changing one’s clothes (Whicker & Kronenfeld, 1986). Social learning theory emphasizes that children learn gender roles because of the environment in which they are immersed (Whicker & Kronenfeld, 1986). Dornbusch (1966; as cited in Rosenberg & Sutton-Smith, 1972) further theorized that sex roles developed to increase efficiency, so that each child did not have to be taught every type of appropriate behavior or activity. This implies that society dictates the behaviors and rules that are acceptable for each gender. Reinforcement and punishment conveys what behaviors are appropriate based upon the child’s sex and age (Rosenberg & Sutton-Smith, 1972; Whicker & Kronenfeld, 1986). Skinner (1953) as cited in (Rosenberg & Sutton-Smith, 1972) posits that parents are socialization agents for children and are essential to gender role development. More specifically, parents teach their same sex children appropriate gender roles (Whicker & Kronenfeld, 1986). Gender roles can then change based upon the gender roles that the society has implemented and the specific requirements associated with them (Rosenberg 12 & Sutton-Smith, 1972). These roles keep society orderly and functioning smoothly by helping to minimize conflict and protecting family life. Social learning theory developed from two major theories: stimulus-response theory and psychoanalysis. Social learning occurs very early in a child’s life; children become psychologically dependent on others for care and sustenance. The child depends on his or her mother for comfort and satisfaction, and will then become anxious when the mother is absent. By the time the child reaches one year of age, he or she has learned how to gain the mother’s approval and disapproval. In other words, the mother’s behaviors reinforce the child; the child will then generalize this reinforcement into other situations. Once a child learns the process of rewards and punishments, then he or she will develop a more complex learning process, which includes imitation and modeling (Rosenberg & Sutton-Smith, 1972). Both social learning and cognitive development theories focus on society’s role in shaping gender roles. Social learning theory posits that society uses reinforcement and punishment to develop and enforce rules. According to cognitive development theory, children have more responsibility in their development. Children will develop their gender identity by processing information and reasoning and then learn to mimic those behaviors and characteristics associated with their gender thereafter (Archer & Lloyd, 1982). Therefore, children learn gender appropriate characteristics at an early age and actively try to reflect those gender characteristics that are congruent with their gender and age. 13 Gender Inequality and Social Role Theory Gender inequality is due to the categorization, functional differentiation, segregation, and unequal power and status of men and women. Any member of society can categorize an individual as a male or female, using the person’s name, title, interaction, style of dress, and physiological parts. From birth, until death, the person will remain in this category (unless they intentionally seek otherwise) and will be subjected to the activities, gender roles, and personality traits that society dictates for them; people that do not adhere to this system are negatively sanctioned (Lengermann & Wallace, 1985). Society differentiates males and females in many ways. In America, male dominated occupations (e.g., construction worker, doctor, mechanic) and female dominated roles (e.g., homemaker) or occupations (e.g., nurse, receptionist) contribute to gender segregation. In tribal societies, religious rituals, doctrines, and social ridicule helped differentiate between females and males. Today, sex segregated schools, restrooms, and organizational clubs all separate males from females, but despite these sanctions, there is still considerable gender interaction that occurs (Lengermann & Wallace, 1985). Social role theory (Eagly, 1987) asserts that people internalize gender expectations or social roles and are pressured to act in a way that is consistent with these norms. These gender roles then affect other domains of life, such as organizational roles, because people act in ways that are congruent with their gender roles or face consequences for the incongruence (Eagly, Johannesen-Schmidt, & Van Engen, 2003). 14 Socialization of Gender Roles Gender role socialization begins early in life (Whicker & Kronenfeld, 1986); from birth, females and males are raised and treated differently according to their sex (Nielsen, 1978). This teaches children what social roles or positions are expected of them and what rights and responsibilities that they have. For example, a female is socialized throughout her life to take on responsibilities that are similar to social positions that are typical of females (Nielsen, 1978). Although these social positions may gradually change across time and across cultures, the expectations for women in the United States have remained fairly consistent throughout the years. Children continue to learn gender roles throughout their childhood and adolescence. Language serves as an important medium by which society communicates gender roles to children. The child observes and mimics behaviors observed through interactions with their parents and those with the rest of society. Although there is no consensus as to how children learn gender roles (e.g., cognitive development theory, social learning theory), it is apparent that children use different mediums to develop a sense of oneself (Lengermann & Wallace, 1985). During childhood, children learn the differences that exist between the different sexes. For example, parents provide different toys for the sexes based upon their gender (Archer & Lloyd, 1982). Children actively try to understand the world and their own identity and gender, and then incorporate all the information together. In school, gender specific rules and regulations teach children gender differentiation. The children then develop a sense of identity through their own appearance, actions, morals, and 15 relationships. These interactions will affect them in all aspects of life, thus allowing them to learn gender roles appropriately (Lengermann & Wallace, 1985). Society socializes men to be the sole earners of a family, while it teaches women that they must take upon the roles of the wife and homemaker. In other words, women were negatively viewed for establishing independence (although this has somewhat changed with time; Denmark, 1993). American culture also dictates what men should desire and value in a mate— an attractive, easygoing partner that supports him emotionally and serves as his subordinate (Lengermann & Wallace, 1985). However, people still expect women to be attractive to the opposite sex, not only physically, but emotionally as well (Denmark, 1993). With the demands of finding a partner, society pressures both females and males to take upon these roles. Parents serve as the primary source of socialization for children, making the relationship that occurs highly important and influential, especially in regards to gender role socialization. For example, mothers serve as the primary caretakers for their children, which affect the children’s development. By being able to interact with same sex caretakers (as females do with their mothers), they are able to develop empathy and intimacy that allows them to connect with other people. The relationship that boys engage in with their mothers helps them to develop distinct gender roles. Boys must differentiate between feminine gender models and develop their own gender role identity. This means that boys must learn how to distance themselves from intimacy, especially since their fathers are not intimate role models (Vianello & Siemienska, 1990). 16 As stated prior, schools serve as influential mediums to socialize children, especially since children are subjected to sex segregation in school. This reinforces the belief that girls and boys are different from one another and that they should behave differently, especially when they become adults (Whicker & Kronenfeld, 1986). For example, girls learn to negotiate relationships and express themselves through speech, while boys are more apt to tell stories, argue, and verbally posture. Furthermore, society teaches boys about dominance and hierarchies through competitive activities during adolescence, while girls are prompted to engage in athletics that require them to be supportive non-participants, such as cheerleaders. Cheerleaders are expected to be attractive and poised, and cheer for the leaders (the male athletes on the team). In essence, this is teaching women to “lead” the crowd of observers, while taking a lower level role within the game (Denmark, 1993). Socialization then leads girls to develop relationships and interpersonal skills. When girls play together in groups, they will often choose girls that are similar in age and play in pairs in relatively private settings. Girls tend not to be competitive, but instead develop cooperative and exclusive relationships, with best friendship prevailing. When girls argue, they often avoid confrontation and instead just distance the group; they also use subtle and acceptable vocal cues to criticize others, and learn quickly how to maintain relationships and who to confide in. Boys, on the other hand, will play in groups that adhere to hierarchies early in their development and will use speech to posture and counter posture others to assert status and gain approval. Through schooling, society 17 continuously reinforces gender roles during such a vital time in childhood development (Archer & Lloyd, 1982). The media is also a strong socializing agent in gender role development in children. The television is a dominant mass medium within the United States; it airs shows that reflect current stereotypes and gender roles. For example, television shows, like the Flintstones, primes children for gender roles by depicting the men as the sole monetary contributors and the wives as submissive homemakers. Additionally, the commercials aired during such shows often include advertisements, which further depict current stereotypes and gender roles (Whicker & Kronenfeld, 1986). Other socialization mediums include the clothing and other activities that children wear or engage in. Girls are taught to wear pink clothing and to like ruffles, while boys are prompted to like blue and sports oriented outfits. Even before birth, specific items are purchased because of the child’s sex, so that the color and other characteristics of the items are congruent with the child’s sex. Some studies even suggest that boys and girls are not only clothed differently, but are also treated differently by their parents (in terms of the amount affection received) simply because of the child’s sex (Whicker & Kronenfeld, 1986). Children are taught to engage in games and play with toys that further socialize gender roles. Girls play with dolls, play “house”, and engage in simple noncompetitive games like “jump rope” while boys play with trucks and blocks and play more aggressive and competitive games, like football and war games (Whicker & Kronenfeld, 1986). Furthermore, card games, like “Old Maid”, depict women as liabilities and as something 18 that can and should be discarded. The premise of the game is to have players collect cards in order to create a family, but they must also dispose of the Old Maid by pawning it off on another player. This may be because the Old Maid is viewed as a liability to the card player (and in reality family; Storkey, 2001). The cultural language also serves as a strong socializing force of gender roles due to the biasness of the language. For example, the words strength, power, vigor, force, and ability all define masculine items. This leads people to believe that strength is associated with masculinity, prompting people to believe that men are stronger than women are. However, if one were to factor in stamina, perseverance, and longevity, then a different paradigm may result. Collectively, language may help to denote a difference between sexes, but the language is tailored towards men, which potentially makes the language have a sexist connotation (Storkey, 2001). Behaviorally speaking, girls and boys are taught that they must adopt certain gender specific characteristics or risk consequences for not doing so. Gender theorists further assert that girls and boys are socialized to act in distinct ways; boys are conditioned to be ambitious and agentic, while girls are expected to seek relationships with others. Since birth, boys are socialized to be competitive and political, while girls are primed to take upon roles that are more cooperative. Feminists often attribute girls’ lack of interest in politics and public life because they have been primed to take upon these submissive gender roles and characteristics (Vianello & Siemienska, 1990). 19 Gender Characteristics Some developmental psychologists believe that children learn roles from sex segregated peer groups and then project this into their adult life (Tannen, 1990). This socializes children for gender role conformation in all aspects of life, including a lack of advancement in the workplace for women (Eagly & Johannesen-Schmidt 2001). If someone decides not to adhere to these gender roles, society may negatively sanction them for their rebellion. For example, traditionally speaking, if a woman established independence and remained unmarried, then she was negatively viewed by members of society for not being available or having her own family (Lengermann & Wallace, 1985). These pressures force people to adhere to gender roles in fear of being isolated from society. These conditioned gender roles then lead to behavioral and characteristic differences in their adult life. Research shows that men are believed to be “instrumental, competent, rational, and assertive” while women are believed to be viewed as “warm, sensitive, tactful and expressive” (Eagly & Johnson, 1990, p. 234). Men are also typically more dominant, competitive (Korabik, Baril, & Watson, 1993) controlling/forceful, independent, and confident than women (Eagly & Johannesen-Schmidt, 2001) and are more likely to interrupt, challenge their partners’ thoughts, ignore comments made by others, and respond less enthusiastically. Women are more apt to ask questions, maintain conversation flow, use more personal pronouns, and protest silently when they have been interrupted (Archer & Lloyd, 1982). These differences also manifest themselves in communication mediums, where men tend to be more direct and communicate downward 20 while females communicate upwards from subordinates (Emerick, 2006). These gender differences further add to the gender role stereotypes that spill over into the organizational world. Gender and Leadership Styles Gender egalitarianism is slowly becoming more prominent in today’s society, even in the workplace, but there is still need for improvement. Women are increasingly entering the workforce and are more educated and active in the political sphere than ever before. Traditional roles, where men dominate, may be fading. Instead, a more egalitarian view, which claims that differences in gender roles are not based upon the actual differences that exist between genders, but instead on the individual abilities of people, may be emerging (Vianello & Siemienska, 1990). Yet research suggests sex stereotypes are still present in today’s society and these gender roles spill over into leadership roles as well (Eagly & Johnson, 1990). Every person has in their long-term memory a belief system that details the characteristics of leaders and non-leaders (Lord, Foti, & Phillips, 1982). This leadership prototype is a multidimensional common knowledge structure that is formed early in life (Epitropaki & Martin, 2004). In this paper, when we refer to leadership, we are referring to a synonymous term, which encompasses both leadership and management, whose overall goal is to increase organizational performance. The term leadership originated in the 18th and 19th centuries when philosophers proposed the “great man theory”, a theory that postulated that great historical events occurred because of "great men", or heroes. These individuals 21 were highly influential men who were charismatic, intellectual, and/or who held great political impact. Unfortunately, the “great man theory” was based upon years of sexist beliefs that heroes were male and that the ability to lead was innate and inherited through the passing of superior genes. These beliefs were highly supported by others, who believed that rulers continue to rule through family lineage because they give birth to the most capable offspring (Denmark, 1993). These sexist beliefs contributed to an underlying conviction that women were not and could not be effective leaders or contribute substance to society as males could. These sexist beliefs laid the groundwork for the narrow-minded leadership research that would occur in the 1900s. Historically, society defined effective leadership in terms of masculine qualities and characteristics because of not only the “great men” and sexist beliefs of the day, but also because there were few, if any, female leaders. Therefore, research during the 1900s focused on identifying specific characteristics of successful and effective male leaders (Denmark, 1993). Lewin and Lippit defined leadership in terms of the leader’s involvement with people or work related issues. In 1938, Lewin and two other scientists contrived the term when they were studying children and their control capabilities. They found three basic control types (or leadership styles) that children used: autocratic, democratic, and laissezfaire. They also studied the childrens’ reactions (e.g., anger, hostility) in relation to the control types that certain children exhibited (Nicolaou-Smokoviti, 2004). These studies profoundly influenced future leadership research, which assessed the effects of leadership style on group member satisfaction. Lewin’s work influenced 22 research on different leadership styles, which demonstrated that there is a continuum of leadership styles, which range from extremely democratic to autocratic. Although these studies focused solely on the male population, they helped build the foundation for broader and more effective leadership research. Currently, leadership research investigates the use of leadership styles in various contexts and assesses employee reactions to specific conditions, using both male and female populations (Denmark, 1993). Leadership styles are, by definition, behaviors that fall within a range of typical behaviors (but are not always identical or exhibited; Eagly & Johnson, 1990). Leadership styles often differ in the amount that employees participate in the decision making process (Bass, 1990). For example, a democratic leader allows employees to engage in decision-making but ultimately the leader exudes final authority with decisions (Tannenbaum & Schmidt, 1973). In other words, democratic leaders encourage deliberation and suggestions, which help to empower their employees (Bass, 1990), distribute responsibility, and allow for self-determination, inclusiveness, equal participation, and deliberation (Pines et al., 2001). On the other hand, autocratic leaders allow minimal or no employee input because of strict standards and processes for accomplishments (Tannenbaum & Schmidt, 1973). Eagly and Johnson (1990) further contend that it is not possible for an individual to use both a democratic and autocratic leadership style simultaneously because they are polar opposites. Therefore, one can only use one leadership style at a time, but can alternate between or use different leadership styles at different times. 23 Several leadership styles (e.g., interpersonal orientation, task orientation) are appropriate for use depending on the specific circumstances of the situation. These other dimensions are implicit in the autocratic and democratic styles and thus will not be the focus of the current research. For example, Eagly and Johnson (1990) suggested that an interpersonal orientation is associated with a democratic leadership style, and those who have a task orientation tend to use an autocratic leadership style. Since these other leadership styles fall on a continuum between the autocratic and democratic leadership styles, it is unnecessary to focus on these other leadership styles in the current research. Furthermore, these other dimensions of leadership may not give rise to gender role associations because there are not consistent gender differences between them (Bass, 1990; Luthar, 1996). For example, the autocratic leadership style is similar to the agentic characteristics that are usually associated with males, because males tend to be more dominant and controlling than women (Eagly & Johannesen-Schmidt, 2001). On the other hand, women are more interpersonally oriented than males, which is more associated with the democratic leadership style. The democratic and autocratic leadership styles were the focus of the current research because they have dichotomized dimensions and are associated with gender roles (e.g., Eagly & Johannesen-Schmidt, 2001). One must know that leadership styles are not fixed behaviors. Instead, they are styles of leadership that encompass a range of behaviors. Depending on the situation, leaders will vary their behaviors based upon the range of behaviors within that leadership style. For example, democratic leaders may allow subordinates to make decisions fully on 24 his or her own, or they may ask subordinates for their thoughts on a topic, but make the final decision on his or her own. In times of crisis, the leader may abandon his or her typical leadership style altogether and engage in a behavior that does not fall within the typical range of his or her leadership style. For example, a democratic leader might make a decision and not involve others in the process at all (Eagly, 2007). As stated prior, certain leadership styles are believed to be related to sex stereotypes. For example, an autocratic leadership style emphasizes the maintenance of tasks while a democratic leadership style emphasizes the nurturing of relationships. These roles reflect dimensions of sex stereotypes within the masculinity and femininity domains respectively. In other words, women tend to be democratic leaders, or more “people oriented” than autocratic leaders, while males tend to be autocratic leaders (Eagly & Johnson, 1990). Women are believed to have communal qualities, like being selfless, concerned with the welfare of others, emotionally expressive (Eagly & Karau, 1991), helpful, kind, and sympathetic and are motivated by nurturance. They also tend to be less hierarchical, more cooperative and collaborative, and more focused on enhancing other people’s self worth (Eagly & Johnson, 1990). Women use more submissive and passive tactics than men to lead others (Guadagno & Cialdini, 2007). These sex traits are also similar to characteristics that are associated with certain leadership styles. In an organizational setting, this implies that the individual supports and listens to others, and speaks tentatively (Eagly & Johannesen-Schmidt, 2001). These traits are characteristic of democratic leadership since they involve group members in decision-making, focus on achieving the goals, empowering others, and encouraging 25 shared power and information. Democratic leaders are also more open with colleagues and are more concerned with employee welfare, which are similar to feminine characteristics (Pines et al., 2001). Furthermore, women tend to disagree with hierarchical downward decision-making processes, but like to adapt a more egalitarian view where employees within the organization are at an equal level (Pines et al., 2001). Traditionally speaking, these characteristics have not been associated with effective leadership, which has ultimately affected the perceptions people have towards female leaders. As stated prior, men are agentic and instrumental and have a more self-interested, task-focused orientation. They also tend to be more competitive, assertive, and dominant within their environment and consistently and forcefully argue a point, interrupt others, and avoid tentative speech patterns (Yoder, 2001). They try to intimidate others, promote themselves, and bask in reflected glory, which projects power (Guadagno & Cialdini, 2007) and are independent, ambitious, achievement oriented and use their power to ensure that each individual contributes accordingly. Therefore, leader characteristics are associated more with masculine gender roles than feminine gender roles (Eagly & Karau, 2002). Research demonstrates that women use a more democratic leadership style than men. Eagly and Blair Johnson (1990) conducted a meta-analytic study of organizational studies, leader assessments, and laboratory research. Results from leader assessments and laboratory studies showed women use a more interpersonal style of leadership than men, who were more task oriented. The results from the organizational studies showed 26 that there was no difference between the genders in terms of the task oriented and interpersonal style of leadership. However, all three types of studies showed a significant difference between leader sex and leadership style. More specifically, women tended to use a more democratic leadership style and men were more likely to use a more autocratic leadership style. Meta-analytic studies have shown that men were more likely to be more autocratic (or directive) than women, while women were more likely to be more democratic (participative) than men (Eagly & Johnson, 1990). The study assessed laboratory experiments, assessment studies, and organizational studies. Eagly and Johnson argued that this may reflect the attitudinal bias that exists against women; in other words, people expected women to act a certain way, and that is why they may have been bias in their ratings. Research completed within the last 30 years suggests that there is a gap between what people think is typical of female behavior and ideal leadership (Eagly & Karau, 2002). This may be due to the incongruity between the female gender role and the expected leadership role (Eagly & Karau, in press as cited in Eagly & Johannesen-Schmidt, 2001). Leadership Styles and Employee Satisfaction Job satisfaction is defined as the degree of positive or negative feelings that a person has towards different aspects of their job. Satisfaction is as an emotional state resulting from the job achieving one’s job values while dissatisfaction implies failure of the job in achieving one’s job values. The satisfaction one feels for different aspects of the job then form a general satisfaction level for the job (Salazar, Pfaffenberg, & Salazar, 27 2006). Leadership is an important variable in member satisfaction and has consistently been a topic of debate and research throughout the years. Unfortunately, there have been conflicting findings within the research; some research has shown that subordinates are more satisfied under democratic leadership (e.g., Kushell & Newton, 1986), whereas other researchers have demonstrated that people are more satisfied under autocratic leadership (e.g., Foels et al., 2000). Some research demonstrates members under autocratic leadership are more satisfied than those under democratic leadership, although these findings are rather sparse. Foels et al. (2000) studied satisfaction and leadership by having participants watch a video and then discuss it within a democratically or autocratically led group. Results showed that members in the autocratically led group were more satisfied than members in the democratically led group. Other research has found more dramatic differences in member satisfaction in regards to leadership style, where employees are more satisfied with democratic leaders than autocratic ones (Thomas, 1992). For example, in a laboratory study conducted by Kushell and Newton (1986, as cited in Foels et al., 2000), participants hypothetically rated the importance of several items they desired if they were on the moon. Results showed that those members in democratically led groups were more satisfied than those members in autocratically led groups. Some researchers suggest that members under democratic leadership are more satisfied than groups led by autocratic leaders. Some findings show that this difference is significant, whereas others have shown this tendency to be small. However, many 28 variables, including the group itself, the size of the groups, the gender composition of the groups, and the potency of the leadership style may affect satisfaction. For example, results of a gender composition based meta analysis conducted by Foels et al. (2000) suggested that as the difference between the autocratic leader and democratic leader became more pronounced, then the satisfaction differential increased. Other researchers suggest that democratic leadership enhances the quality of decisions and increases member satisfaction (Bass, 1990). For example, Looney (2004) found that male democratic leaders fared higher in leadership evaluations than their autocratic male counterparts. These employees also were absent less and found the work to be more enjoyable than those employees under autocratic leadership. Democratic leadership empowers employees by allowing people to engage in the decision-making process, which then leads to greater autonomy, satisfaction, and self-confidence (Salazar, Pfaffenberg, & Salazar, 2006). Employees, when empowered, take ownership of the job and feel more responsible for the work they do. In turn, this leads to organizational loyalty, decreased turnover and absenteeism, increased productivity, and greater customer satisfaction. Other research has demonstrated that the gender of the leader may not influence the employee’s satisfaction. Even though people are more satisfied in democratically led groups, the gender of the leader did not significantly affect their level of satisfaction (Kushell & Rae Newton, 1986). However, other studies show that although the gender of the leader itself does not affect employee satisfaction, the leadership style may interact with the leader’s gender and affect employee satisfaction (Fiedler, 1967). 29 Research further supports that female leaders are generally viewed more negatively than male leaders. Research has shown that when leadership styles were dichotomized in terms of democratic or autocratic leadership, male leaders were evaluated more positively than female leaders when using autocratic leadership styles (Eagly, Makhijani, & Klonksy, 1992). Other experiments have used written descriptions of managers where the sex of the leader varies between each participant. In review of 61 experiments, results showed that there was a small tendency for males to be favored more than females. However, this difference increased in male dominated leadership roles (Eagly et al., 1992). Experiments, where participants evaluated the effectiveness of theoretical leaders, have also shown bias against women. Davidson and Burke’s (2000) review of 49 experiments showed that men were preferred over women for masculine type jobs (e.g., auto salesperson, doctor) while women were preferred over men for feminine jobs. The leadership style and sex of the leader were shown to be just two factors that could affect people’s evaluations of a leader (Garcia-Retamero & Lopez-Zafra, 2006). Situational theorists argue that the type of leadership style that is appropriate depends on the context of the situation, such as the nature of the task, the characteristics of the followers, and the culture of the organization. When situations arise where an autocratic leadership style is appropriate, women are at a disadvantage because autocratic leadership is incongruent with their gender role (Schein, 1973). Collectively, research shows that group members are generally more satisfied with democratic leadership than autocratic leadership, although the extent of this is 30 unknown. It is important to note that no leadership style is appropriate to use in all circumstances or situations, and thus another leadership style may be more effective given that specific situation (Denmark, 1977). Situational leadership theorists argue that the context of the situation affects the type of leadership style appropriate for use (Eagly, 2007). For example, the nature of the task, the organizational culture, and the subordinates may all affect what leadership style will be most effective given that particular situation (Eagly, 2007). Nicolaou-Smokoviti (2004) argues that any leadership style will be effective if the desired outcome is productivity, but if group freedom is the desired outcome, then democratic leadership will be appropriate. Although the democratic and autocratic leadership styles do not fully explain the relationship between employee production and satisfaction, they may highly affect them, and thus, are the focus of this experiment. Social Role Theory and Role Congruity Theory Social role theory asserts that expected leadership roles influence how people will act under certain situations. People expect leaders to behave in certain ways due to preconceived expectancies based on gender roles (Eagly & Johannesen-Schmidt, 2001). In other words, men and women learn different skills and beliefs that later affect the different social behaviors they engage in. These social expectations can affect the type of jobs that each sex engages in and negatively affect the perceptions people have if these gender and organizational roles are incongruent. For example, a female doctor may be viewed as an assertive individual because doctors are typically thought to be men. This 31 negatively affects the way people perceive her because she is in a role that is incongruent with her gender role (Eagly, 1987). Role congruity theory, which is based off social role theory, explains that leaders are evaluated more positively and perceived to be leaders that are more effective when their characteristics are believed to be congruent with the current social or gender roles (Eagly & Diekman, 2005). Role incongruity occurs when a leader’s gender and leadership style are incongruent with one another. For example, a female manager who uses an autocratic leadership style is incongruent with the typical roles associated with females because, as women, they must exhibit communal characteristics, but as leaders, they are expected to exhibit agentic characteristics. This results in two incongruent and conflicting roles for women. On the other hand, men are believed to be effective leaders because their gender roles are more congruent with leadership roles (Eagly, 2007). Therefore, people view male leaders more favorably due to the consistency between male gender roles and leadership roles (Eagly & Johnson, 1990). According to role congruity theory, descriptive and prescriptive biases can result from the incongruence between a female leader’s role and gender role. Descriptive bias occurs when there is a gap between a female’s gender role and her leadership role. A female who exhibits characteristics in accordance with her gender role is not believed to have the necessary characteristics to be an effective leader. Similarly, prescriptive bias occurs when a female exhibits more masculine leadership characteristics, which violates her gender role. This results in a double standard; no matter what type of leadership role a 32 female adopts, they will be negatively sanctioned for either being an ineffective leader or an authoritative female (Eagly & Karau, 2002). Societal injunctive norms hold women to engage in certain behaviors because of gender roles (Eagly & Johnson, 1990; Eagly & Johannesen-Schmidt, 2001). Since men are leaders, they tend to be evaluated more positively and believed to be more successful in leadership roles than women. This biased view leads people to believe that feminine behaviors are less favorable than masculine behaviors, even if the sexes are displaying the same behavior. This leads to prejudice against women simply because their gender roles are incongruent with leadership roles (Eagly & Johannesen-Schmidt, 2001). Research shows that female managers are continually targets of prejudice and are evaluated more negatively than males, especially when they are in an industry that is incongruent with their gender role (Garcia-Retamero & Lopez-Zafra, 2006). Eagly and Karau’s (2002) role congruity theory explains that the amount of prejudice that female leaders face varies according to the amount of incongruence that exists between their gender role and leadership role. Since there is probably a greater incongruence between gender roles and leadership roles in male dominated industries, female leaders in male dominated industries may receive more prejudice than if they were in a female dominated industry (Eagly & Karau, 2002). For example, Eagly’s et al. (1992) meta-analytic study regarding gender and leader evaluations (using written vignettes and confederate studies) generally showed that females were evaluated slightly more negatively than male leaders, especially when exhibiting autocratic behavior. Although these are laboratory studies 33 using artificial situations, they still have merit because they reflect real world influences (Denmark, 1993). Another meta-analytic study completed by Davidson and Burke (2000) also showed that men were preferred over women for masculine jobs (e.g., sales manager, mechanic) and gender-neutral jobs (e.g., psychologist), but women were preferred over men for feminine jobs (e.g., secretary). This suggests that women may be viewed as more effective leaders when the position is congruent with their gender roles and incongruent with male gender roles. However, since most occupations are associated with male characteristics, women will be viewed as less effective leaders overall. Some researchers suggest that participant age and sex could have affected the results of these studies, where female and older participants may be more prejudiced against female leaders than male and younger participants were. Other research demonstrates that participants favored male leaders, regardless of the industry studied and that female leaders were perceived to be more masculine overall, but more feminine when they worked in an industry that was congruent with their gender role. However, men maintained a level of masculinity regardless of the industry in which they worked (Garcia-Retamero & Lopez-Zafra, 2006). A meta-analytic study by Wood and Eagly (2002) demonstrated that male and female leaders were equally effective, except when the leadership roles were incongruent with the female gender roles. In this case, men were perceived to be more effective than women when in more masculine roles, while women were more effective when the role was defined in less masculine terms (Wood & Eagly, 2002). Bass (1990) also revealed 34 that females were rated as less effective than male leaders, but this was reasoned to be because of stereotypes and biases from the research participants. Other researchers have also shown that male leaders are preferred over female leaders. For example, a 1979 study showed that undergraduate women believed that characteristics of a leader were masculine characteristics, and not feminine or androgynous traits. Furthermore, results showed that people perceived graduate women to have more masculine traits than undergraduate women, showing a bias towards those who achieved a higher academic level. Conclusively, the characteristics that are associated with effective managerial performance are not congruent with traditional female gender roles. Women often lose to their male counterparts when competing for a position; therefore, women in leadership roles face a disadvantage simply because of their gender. Some believe this is because men are believed to be more dominant and influential than women in leadership, whereas women are still believed to be submissive, complacent, and easily influenced (Merrick, 2002). Women inevitably face the dilemma of exhibiting these stereotypical feminine gender roles (e.g., communal qualities) or more agentic qualities (Eagly & JohannesenSchmidt, 2001). If women conform to their gender roles, they may face lack of respect and advancement in leadership positions. On the other hand, if they conform to leadership roles then they may be perceived negatively and be subjected to further prejudice, such as negative attitudes, pressure, and disapproval towards female leaders (Eagly, 2007). These preconceived gender roles diminish beliefs that women have the potential to be capable leaders and decreases people’s evaluations of their actual 35 leadership. Some suggest women can negate this by establishing a balance between exhibiting both communal and agentic qualities (Yoder, 2001). Perceptions of female desires and feminine characteristics may also affect management’s decisions during the hiring process. Since society has preconceived notions about women’s desires and potential, they may deem women incompetent, especially if that position is incongruent with stereotypic gender roles. Additionally, there are societal pressures that require females be attractive, making a women’s worth partially dependent on how attractive men perceive her to be. Even though they may be very competent and capable of managing others, they are not believed to be capable leaders and are therefore, not chosen to be. It is plausible to assume that as more women enter leadership roles, they will hire comparable women to themselves since people tend to accept and gravitate towards people that are similar to themselves. Therefore, over time the gap between the genders may minimize and perhaps diminish (Merrick, 2002). National polls are an available method used to gather information by polling representative samples of participants and asking them questions on a myriad of subjects, including leadership. Results have consistently shown that male leaders are perceived to be more effective and preferred over female leaders. From 1953 to 2006, those polled stated that they would prefer working for a male boss to a female one. However, this difference has steadily decreased over the years (Carroll, 2006). Other poll results showed that in 1937 only 33% of respondents believed that they could vote for a wellqualified woman nominated for president by their own party versus 92% in 2006 (Freeman, 2008). However, in 2006 only 55% agreed that the United States was ready for 36 a female president, compared to 40% just ten years prior. Therefore, once women increasingly gain access to higher leadership positions, it is likely that society will embrace them as leaders more so. Women in the Workplace The 1970s and 1980s marked a transition in gender equality because women started to gain employment in the paid workforce. Progressively more and more women were entering management positions; however, gender roles often hindered their opportunities for advancement. If a woman adopted male characteristics of leadership, then her colleagues might have alienated her, resulting in negative attitudes that stressed working relationships and decreased productivity (Eagly & Diekman, 2005). Women are treated unfairly sometimes, in terms of not only promotional opportunities, but also monetary rewards, promotional policies, employment utilization, and professional development (Merrick 2002). For example, research shows that men have earned higher wages than women in comparable jobs and that women earn a lower salary even though they tend to be older and employed by the firm for a longer time (Denmark, 1977). Furthermore, male managers were pessimistic regarding the promotional opportunities for women than women were themselves (Denmark, 1977). Research suggests that the pay scale differential between genders occurs because females are evaluated unfairly due to gender roles and stereotypes (Eagly et al., 1992). This suggests that women have a more difficult time obtaining promotions within organizations, regardless of level (Eagly, 2007) and suffer from a wage/salary 37 discrimination even when such variables (e.g., hours worked per year, type of occupation) are taken into consideration (Eagly & Carli, in press as cited in Eagly, 2007). Additionally, a majority of employees prefer to have a male leader to a female leader, making it harder for female leaders to gain opportunities and succeed in leadership positions. Despite these findings, women exhibit leadership skills that may put them at a slight disadvantage for leadership (Eagly, 2007). Research shows that even though there is nearly equal representation in the workplace, men still progress faster and higher than women (Guadagno & Cialdini, 2007). Since women face prejudice, they may self-select themselves and not apply for promotional opportunities simply because they anticipate that they cannot progress within the workplace. Although this may be due to many reasons, one reason may be that women do not desire to be employed in a hostile work environment (Stokes, Riger, & Sullivan, 1995). When women do apply for positions, they typically have to exceed the male applicants’ qualifications to obtain the position. Therefore, women are not equal to men’s success in most conditions (Eagly, 2007). People often believe that since men are typically larger and stronger than women, they are also more intelligent and effective leaders. Sociologists may argue that positions occupied by males and females are not directly related to one’s physical size or muscular mass. Instead, the difference in positions is related to social values and attitudes regarding the sexes (Storkey, 2001). Furthermore, the ability to promote within the workplace depends on how men and women are evaluated, which allows workplace politics and management to affect organizational business (Storkey, 2001). Since men 38 are often in charge of hiring other leaders (especially the higher-level positions), they will often choose people that are similar to themselves in status, background, beliefs, and sex (e.g., Denmark, 1977). Some people believe that there is a lack of women in management positions due to other reasons (e.g., a lack of skills). They also believe that female leaders need to receive more training and need more opportunities to develop their leadership skills. Others believe that this bias is due to sex role stereotypes that convey that women are not good leaders (Denmark, 1977). For example, a 1965 survey conducted by The Harvard Business Review indicated that of 2,000 executives, 820 (41%) stated that they were against females becoming executives because they felt that women were inadequate for the positions (Bass, 1990). Further research has demonstrated that men will naturally become leaders in mixed groups in both laboratory experiments and real world settings (Denmark 1977). This demonstrates the inequality of the sexes in management positions and implies that women are placed at a disadvantage for leadership positions simply because they are not typically in these roles. Although this trend has decreased over time, it has not been eliminated (Eagly & Karau, 2002) Studies indicate that people believe high performance from women is due to luck or chance, whereas high performance from men stems from their innate intelligence, abilities, or skills (Luthar, 1996). Swim and Sanna (1996; as cited in Luthar, 1996) showed that a woman’s success is often attributed to other circumstances, other than their actual ability (when compared to men’s success). These attributions, over time, lead 39 people to believe that such differentials in compensation, status, and power are justifiable (Luthar, 1996) and thus, reflect societies’ values at that given point in time. Researchers believe that for a woman to be successful she must liberate herself from pressures to conform to the traditional gender roles and constraints (Eagly & Diekman, 2005). They also argue that there are several ways in which a female’s authority is minimized including the way in which she is addressed (e.g., by her first name vs. Mrs.), not asserting oneself, allowing and accepting sexist language and lack of recognition and status, and minimizing one’s own credentials. Since women do not often speak up against this prejudice, then they are allowing it to continue. This means that they are becoming their own worst oppressors (Merrick, 2002). Although women are becoming more prominent in management, there is still a gender gap within management positions. For example, in 2002, women made up 5.2% of top earning corporate officers in the Fortune 500 (largest companies), which was an increase from 1.2% in 1996 (Catalyst, 2002). The U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics state that women only occupy 24% of the chief executive positions in organizations and only 37% of all managers within an organization on average (Eagly, 2007). This is compared with the 14% national estimate in 1947 and 22% thirty years later (although the positions were mostly in health administration, building supervision, and restaurant management; Merrick, 2002). Furthermore, only 39% of the entire workforce was occupied by women in the 1970’s, 1% of which were women who were upper level managers (Merrick, 2002). This increase of women in the workforce and managerial positions is partially due to a shift in sharing domestic work with husbands, increasing education (Eagly, 2007) 40 and seeking paid employment (U.S. Census Bureau, 2007, as cited in Eagly, 2007). Therefore, women are becoming more career oriented and seeking higher positions than they have historically, making the equality between the sexes continue to improve. To sum, women have been socialized to adhere to gender role stereotypes (Eagly & Johannesen-Schmidt, 2001). Since the effectiveness of leadership styles depends on other factors (e.g., features of the subordinates, groups, or organization) there is no clear consequence of employing a specific leadership style. Research on the effectiveness of democratic and autocratic leadership styles on group productivity and employee satisfaction show that features of the groups and organizations, among other conditions, are important moderating variables as well (Foels, et al., 2000). Gender roles spill over into leadership roles; women face more prejudice, especially when they engage in leadership styles that are incongruent with their gender roles (Eagly & Johannesen-Schmidt, 2001). Even if women exhibit leadership styles that are more effective, they may still face reluctance, especially from men who do not want to lose power and status within the workplace. If current trends persist and more women enter more management positions, leadership positions will become more gender balanced (Catalyst, 2002), which may weaken occupational roles and turn leadership into a more androgynous term (Wood & Eagly, 2002). Locus of Control Locus of control refers to a person's belief about what causes the positive or negative results in his or her life, either in general or in specific areas. In other words, locus of control describes a person’s attitudes regarding the relationship between their 41 behaviors and its outcomes. Internal locus of control is when people feel that reinforcement occurs because of their own actions (Meyers & Wong, 1988) and that their behavior does have an effect on everyday events that occur (Rotter, 1966; as cited in Meyers & Wong, 1988). Therefore, people with an internal locus of control believe that they are in control of things that happen within their life and are more confident, assertive, and proactive in seeking opportunities for achievement (Salazar, et al., 2006). They believe that they are in control of reaching their own goals, successes and even failures. Additionally, these individuals view challenges as an opportunity for professional growth, while those with external locus of control may ignore these opportunities and dismiss the possibility that they could be used as an opportunity for growth (Huang, 2006). People that have an external locus of control believe that reinforcement occurs because of things other than their own behavior (Meyers & Wong, 1988) and that the outcome did not cause his or her behavior. In other words, the person has minimal, if any, effect on any events that occur. Those with external locus of control believe that fate, luck, or chance controls what happens in life, whether in successes or failures (Salazar, et al., 2006). The belief of whether or not people can control events in their lives has profound effects on their attitudes and behaviors within work settings (Blau, 1987). Since those with internal locus of control attribute circumstances to factors that they can control, they are more likely to attribute things that happen in the workplace (e.g., promotions) to their own initiatives (Blau, 1987). They are also happier with their pay, perform better, and are 42 able to withstand more stress (Huang, 2006). Research also shows that people with internal locus of control exert more effort in changing their environment, by trying to influence operating procedures, job assignments, and relationships with others (Spector, 1982). Those with external locus of control are less likely to leave a job or considering leaving a job if they are not satisfied with current aspects of it and instead wait until environmental aspects force them to leave (Blau, 1987). Rotter (1966) as cited in Oliver, Jose, and Brough (2006) was the first to measure locus of control on a bipolar continuum. If we view locus of control in terms of a onedimensional construct, then we believe it is a general, global belief; if we view locus of control in terms of multidimensionality then we believe that there are multiple dimensions of locus of control in different areas of one’s life. Therefore, multiple measures should be used to assess one’s locus of control if it is multidimensional, whereas only one construct is needed if it is only unidimensional. There are many different measures that assess locus of control within various domains (Muhonen & Torkelson, 2004). For example, a work locus of control scale, developed by Spector (1988), measures an individual’s locus of control in terms of work related issues, such as promotions and salary increases. This 16-item scale measures a person’s general belief regarding work. Some researchers believe the Work Locus of Control Scale should be used with caution (Oliver et al., 2006) because the scale is not unidimensional. On the other hand, even though some researchers think that the domain specific locus of control measures, like the Work Locus of Control, may be more reliable (Lefcourt, 1992), the global measure was used in the current study because the workplace 43 locus of control index cannot be used in conjunction with experiments using simulations and vignettes. The current research also seeks to include some variables that are often omitted from the research: personality variables. Traditional research has left out personality variables, where subordinates react to the gender and leadership style that their leader encompasses. Past research, which has focused on personality variables, has evaluated how autocratic and democratic subordinates evaluate either autocratic or democratic leaders. In other words, research has shown that autocratic individuals tend to prefer autocratic leaders (e.g., Cellar et al., 2001). However, little research has been completed on the specific personality traits of subordinates. One such study has evaluated the role of agreeableness with employee satisfaction with leaders, where results showed that participants that had a high level of agreeableness rated democratic leaders higher than autocratic leaders, while the opposite was true for participants that had a low level of agreeableness. Furthermore, those participants that were low in agreeableness tended to rate female autocratic leaders and male democratic leaders lower, but generally leader sex did not affect leader effectiveness ratings (Cellar et al., 2001). Social learning theory (Rotter, 1966; as cited in Muhonen & Torkelson, 2004) asserts that people will learn through reinforcement; reinforcement will teach individuals the likelihood that a specific behavior or event will occur based upon the association of the behavior with that reinforcement. People will then generalize and project this into different situations and contexts, thus affecting the person’s behavioral choices. 44 Therefore, reinforcement will strengthen the relationship between a behavior and the reinforcement (Muhonen & Torkelson, 2004). This phenomenon can be applied to locus of control as well. Judge and Bono (2001) found that locus of control had an estimated true score correlation of .32 with job satisfaction. Other research has shown that external locus of control is negatively related to job satisfaction (Huang, 2006; Sidani & Gardner, 2000) but that there was no difference between men and women (Muhonen & Torkelson, 2004). Some believe that those with an external locus of control may be less satisfied because they feel that they have no control over organizational incidents, such as promotions, selection, appraisals, which then leads to lower job satisfaction (Sidani & Gardner, 2000). Greenberger, Strasser, and Cummings (1989) state that internal locus of control, or personal control was a predictor of work satisfaction for employees and that individuals with an internal locus of control were more motivated and participated more within their jobs (Salazar et al., 2006). Other research has shown that locus of control is an important variable in job satisfaction, job performance, and turnover, but there are theoretical and psychometric debates on how locus of control should be quantified (Oliver et al., 2006). Additionally, locus of control, although related to job satisfaction, is not a predictor of it (Salazar et al., 2006). Alarcon, Eschleman, and Bowling (1999) further showed that locus of control was significantly related to job burnout, a negative emotional reaction to one’s job that is caused from a stressful work environment. Internal locus of control was also negatively related to emotional exhaustion and depersonalization and positively related to personal 45 accomplishment. Since burnout is an indicator of employee wellbeing, and attitudes, health and behavior, it is important to address locus of control in the present study. Research has shown that locus of control may interact with employee satisfaction, such that external work locus of control is negatively related to job satisfaction for both men and women. In addition, work locus of control is a significant predictor of job satisfaction for women (Muhonen & Torkelson, 2004). Another study found that locus of control moderated work demands and strain; those with external locus of control responded more negatively when confronted with work demands than those with internal locus of control (Newton & Keenan, 1990). Other research showed that work locus of control had a negative correlation with job satisfaction (Muhonen & Torkelson, 2004). However, one must interpret these results cautiously because researchers used small sample sizes and potentially inadequate measures to assess job satisfaction. Another study, which researched the relationship between job insecurity and outcomes (e.g., mental health complaints, job dissatisfaction, job-induced tension) on a Swedish nurse acute care hospital found that those participants that had more external locus of control were more likely to be dissatisfied with their jobs than those that had less external locus of control. Locus of control was also found to moderate the relationship between job insecurity and mental health complaints, thus demonstrating that personality characteristics are important predictors in how people react to certain stressors (Naswall, Sverke & Hellgren, 2005). Furnham and Drakeley (1993; as cited in Muhonen & Torkelson, 2004) believe that people who have minimal power and opportunities are more likely to develop an 46 external locus of control. Based upon this logic, managers are more likely to develop an internal locus of control because of their higher rank and status within organizations. Other studies have shown that status within an organization can affect one’s external work locus of control (Muhonen & Torkelson, 2004). In the present study, I assessed locus of control and employee satisfaction (regardless of employee gender) with different leader sexes and leadership styles. Hypotheses Because employee satisfaction is a large part of organizational success, the factors that contribute to employee satisfaction were investigated. In other words, I assessed leader sex, leadership style, and locus of control to see if they affect employee satisfaction. Consistent with role congruity theory, it was hypothesized that participants would be more satisfied with male leaders than female leaders (Eagly & Johnson, 1990). Since people typically are not satisfied with autocratic leaders, respondents should be more satisfied with democratic leaders than autocratic leaders (Looney, 2004). The third hypothesis focused on the degree of employee satisfaction in regards to different leadership styles and sex of the leader. It was expected that participants would be less satisfied when they were led by autocratic women than when they were led by autocratic men (Eagly et al., 1992). Furthermore, I hypothesized that those participants who had a lower internal locus of control would exhibit lower job satisfaction than those with a higher internal locus of control (Muhonen & Torkelson, 2004). Finally, it was hypothesized that those participants who had more internal locus of control would be less satisfied with autocratic leaders than those people with less internal locus of control. 47 Chapter 3 METHOD Participants A convenience sample of 160 undergraduates enrolled in California State University, Sacramento’s psychology introductory classes (i.e., Psychology 1 and 5) participated in the study. There were 132 females and 28 males that participated. Participants were ethnically diverse and their ages ranged from 18 to 30 years old, with an average age of 20 years. The research took place in one of the research rooms on the third floor of Amador Hall at California State University, Sacramento and required 30 minutes of time from each participant. Participants completed informed consent forms; then the researcher informed participants that they could quit the study at any time without any penalties, except that they would be unable to receive course credit for their participation. They were debriefed at the end of the study to educate them about the study’s purpose. This research was conducted to fulfill graduate thesis requirements. A formal human subject’s review was completed and reviewed by the Human Subjects Committee. Individual responses were completed anonymously and kept confidential. A copy of the complete human subject’s application, including the potential risks to participants form, the consent form, a description of the procedure, and the debriefing sheet can be found in Appendix A. 48 Materials Internal Control Index. This inventory assesses the extent to which a person believes that the reinforcement they receive is due to internal or external factors (Duttweiler, 1984). In other words, those who believe that reinforcement is caused by external factors, such as luck or chance will have an external locus of control, whereas people who have an internal locus of control believe reinforcement stems from their own actions. This 28 item measure asks participants to rate the frequency of certain feelings, ranging from A (rarely) to E (usually). The specific anchors included are, A = rarely (less than 10 % of the time), B = occasionally (30% of the time), C = sometimes (50% of the time), D = frequently (70% of time), and E = usually (+90% of time). Example statements include, “When faced with a problem I try to forget it” and “If I want something I work hard to get it”. The Internal Control Index reportedly has an estimated reliability of .84, with the presence of a strong principal component, two replicable factors, and evidence for convergent validity (Meyers & Wong, 1988). A complete list of items and scoring instructions are included in Appendix B. Vignettes. These vignettes include a hypothetical scenario in which the participants imagined that they were employees at Smith Professional Services. The leadership styles in the vignettes were adapted from the manipulations of leadership style used in a previous study (Van Vugt, Jepson, Hart & De Cremer, 2004). The hypothetical scenarios included in the vignettes were modified for the leadership style and sex of the leader. A manipulation check and content validity study was completed for the vignettes. Several psychology graduate professors and graduate students reviewed the vignettes to 49 verify that the leadership style was manipulated in the intended manner. Descriptors, such as “she” and “he”, indicated the sex of the leader. The name Pat was chosen for the leader because it is believed to be one of the most gender-neutral American names; many people may think of Pat as being either a female or a male. This helped to minimize any bias that could result from the name of the leader. Other factors that could have potentially biased the ratings were considered when writing the vignettes. The vignettes were written to be as neutral as possible so that participants were not affected by the other information contained in the vignettes. The name Smith, for the organization Smith Professional Services contained in the vignette, was chosen because it was reported to be the most common surname in the 1990 census (U.S. Census Bureau, 1990). Similarly, the salary was chosen because it was reported as the two-year average median salary for the United States in 2006 according to the U.S. Census Bureau (U.S. Census Bureau, 2006). The work that was described in the vignettes (e.g., communicating with others, maintaining a professional disposition, facilitating team performance, and completing tasks that are relevant to your position) was based upon Campbell’s Theory of Performance (Schmitt& Chan, 1998). This theory includes general job performance dimensions, which are applicable to most jobs. Therefore, they were used to help describe the employee’s overall work dimensions. Finally, the vignettes were also constructed to include information regarding aspects of job satisfaction taken from the Job in General Scale, the Job Descriptive Index, and Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaires. This gave participants information to 50 respond to regarding these dimensions when filling out information from the job satisfaction measures. These aspects included information regarding their present job, pay, and opportunities for promotion, supervisor, and coworkers. Prior to experimentation, each vignette condition was reviewed and edited to ensure validity. Several psychology professors, psychology graduate students, and undergraduate nonpsychology graduate students reviewed and edited the vignettes and reacted to the leadership within the vignettes. There were four vignette conditions: female, autocratic leader; female, democratic leader; male, autocratic leader; male, democratic leader. A copy of each vignette condition is included in Appendix C. Job Descriptive Index. The Job Descriptive Index (JDI) was constructed to generate scores that indicate how satisfied a person is in different aspects of their job, including work, pay, promotions, supervision, and people (Balzer et al., 1997). This 72 item measure asks respondents to rate the work they do at present by describing how well each of the words or phrases describes their job; answers include either Y for yes (if it describes your work), N for no (if it does NOT describe it), or ? (if you cannot decide). The JDI consists of five facets including, work on present job, present pay, and opportunities for promotion, supervision, and coworkers, with each having 9 or 18 items per facet. Items consist of short words or phrases. For example, participants are asked to rate how “fascinating” and “boring” their work is using the work on present job facet. In the present study, these items are scored in their entirety to gain a global job satisfaction measure for the participant. 51 The JDI was validated using a sequential research strategy of discriminant and convergent validity within and across samples (Balzer et al., 1997). Cluster analysis and factor analysis was used to demonstrate evidence of discriminant and convergent validity. Results showed that the JDI was able to discern between different aspects of the job both reliably and validly. Subsequent studies of the JDI have shown that it correlates with other behavioral measures. Finally, the JDI has been proven to be reliable, having coefficient alpha values between .86 and .91, while the supervision facet has an alpha coefficient value of .91. Job in General. The Job in General (JIG) scale was constructed to measure the global satisfaction one feels for the job (Balzer, et. al., 1997). The JIG consists of 18 items and is given in conjunction with the JDI. It assesses a person’s long-term evaluation of their job by having people respond to “Think of your job in general. All in all, what is it like most of the time?” Similarly to the JDI, answers include either Y for yes (if it describes your work), N for no (if it does NOT describe it), or ? (if you cannot decide). Some examples of descriptors include, “pleasant, bad, ideal, and a waste of time”. These items are scored in the current study in their entirety to gain a global job satisfaction measure for the participant. Convergent validity evidence was calculated by correlating the measure with other satisfaction measures, including The Brayfield and Rothe, The Faces Scale, and a simple numerical rating scale (-100 to +100; Balzer et al., 1997). Correlations with the JIG ranged from .66 to .80. Construct validity evidence was demonstrated using patterns 52 of reliabilities of the test and 18 other measures using a sample of 680 employees. Coefficient alpha reliability estimates exceeded .90 (N = 3566). Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire. The Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire (MSQ) was made in response to the Work Adjustment Project, or the Minnesota Studies in Vocational Rehabilitation, a group of studies that assess ones potential and evaluate one’s work adjustment outcomes. Both short and long form of the MSQ measure employee satisfaction in regards to varying aspects of the work and the workplace (e.g., security, pay; Weiss, Dawis, England, & Lofquist, 1967). The short form consists of 20 questions, while the long form consists of 100 items. The MSQ items are scored in their entirety to gain a global satisfaction measure. Both questionnaire forms use a 5-point response scale, which include response options such as very satisfied, satisfied, neutral, dissatisfied, and very dissatisfied. Example items from the short form include “the chance to tell people what to do” and “the way my job provides for steady employment”. Questions from the long form consist of twentyone different facets from social service and recognition, to supervision; the supervision subscales include both an interpersonal/human relations and technical competence component (Weiss, Dawis, England, & Lofquist, 1967). In the present study, the MSQ short form was used because similar employee satisfaction measures were also used in conjunction with it. I did not want to inundate participants with a myriad of questions and wanted to ensure the length of the study was kept to a minimum. Additionally, the supervision subscales from the MSQ (long form) were used to measure participant’s satisfaction with their supervisor. 53 Demographic & Organizational Questions. The demographics page consisted of demographic and background information questions, such as participant’s sex, age, high school information, ethnicity, and socioeconomic status. In the California State University, Sacramento information section, participants reported their majors and minors, their grade point averages, and their year in school. The organizational questions consisted of questions that further probed the participants’ feelings regarding their job and the organization in the vignettes in which they worked. For example, one question asked participants the top three reasons that they would stay working at their present job. It listed several factors that may be important to employees, such as the supervisor, salary, coworkers, and benefits. Other questions addressed reasons for leaving their theoretical job and thoughts regarding their supervisor and the job they held. A copy of the demographic and organizational form in its entirety can be found in Appendix D. Design The study consisted of a 2 x 2 x 2 between subjects design. The independent variables were the leader’s sex, either male or female, and the leadership style, either democratic or autocratic. The blocking variable was the locus of control of the participant, either internal or external, while the dependent variable was the amount of satisfaction the person had with their supervisor and overall job satisfaction. By using a median split on the participant’s locus of control scores from the Internal Control Index, they were assigned to either a high or low internal locus of control level. The participants received one of four vignettes; the order in which the vignette conditions were administered was randomized, but occurred in a block fashion. Meanwhile, the order in 54 which the inventories were administered (e.g., Job in General, Job Descriptive Index) was also randomized. There were many other cautionary procedures taken to minimize the potential confounding and extraneous variables within this experiment. Control procedures included having the same researcher administer the packets and adhere to the same script for each participant (to minimize any bias); the vignettes were constructed to be similar on all aspects except the leadership style and sex of the leader. Other measures taken (to minimize any error effects) included having the research take place under the same general conditions. Studies were completed in the same relative location in the research rooms in Amador Hall at California State University, Sacramento. In addition, the inventories were administered in a randomized fashion to decrease any chances of fatigue or order effects. The thesis vignettes were randomly administered to ensure that the order in which they were administered to participants did not affect the study’s results. The vignettes also included information on various aspects of the job that was previously demonstrated to affect employee satisfaction. This included masking the aspects of the job that were actually being assessed, as well as including additional superfluous information so that participants could have enough information to answer questions about different aspects of the job. The language used in the vignettes was not leading and was neutral to ensure that participants were not biased by the extra information contained in the vignettes. The vignettes were also constructed to include actual numbers (e.g., for the person’s salary, commute time) and gender neutral names (e.g., leader’s name) to 55 describe aspects of the person’s job. Collectively, these factors helped to make the vignettes as realistic as possible, while controlling the experiment as much as possible. Procedure Participants were recruited from the respective introductory psychology courses; they received ½-hour credit for participating in research. One hundred and sixty undergraduates at California State University, Sacramento responded to the vignette study. Participants only participated in the study once. Students entered the research room and were seated; the researcher then distributed consent forms for the participants to sign. After the participant completed the consent form, the forms were placed in a separate folder to guarantee that they could not be traced back to specific participants. The participants were informed that they could withdraw from the study at any time without any penalties, except that they would be unable to receive course credit for participating in the study. They were also informed to complete the packet in order, and to not skip ahead or around within the packet. Following this, the packet of materials was provided to the participants. Participants were instructed not to place their name or any other identifying marks on the materials. The packets included the Internal Control Index, one vignette condition, a Job in General Inventory, a Job Descriptive Index, and the Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire (short form) and the Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire (long form: supervision subscale), as well as the demographic questions. The order of the Job Descriptive Index, Job In General Inventory, and Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaires were determined by a randomized block process to counterbalance them. 56 Participants first completed the Internal Control Index to assess the person’s locus of control. Following this, they read the vignette condition they were given. After reading the vignette, the participants completed the Job Descriptive Index, Job in General Inventory, Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire (short form), Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire (long form: supervision subscale), and the demographic and organizational questions in relation to the job described in the vignette they received. The vignette included a situation where the participant had to imagine that they were working at a hypothetical organization, Smith’s Professional Services. Two independent variables were contained in the vignette itself. In one version of the story, the employee had a female autocratic leader, whereas in other versions of the story the employee had a female democratic leader, male autocratic leader, or male democratic leader. The third variable in the study was the locus of control of the participants; they were coded as having an internal or external locus of control. In other words, a median split was completed on locus of control scores to create the levels for that independent variable. After participants completed the entire packet, the packets were collected and placed together in a different folder from the one containing the consent forms, guaranteeing that the packets could not be linked back to specific participants. The researcher orally debriefed the participants, making sure to answer any questions they had, and gave them the debriefing sheet. Everyone was thanked for their participation and was dismissed. Once all of the data was collected, participants were scored on their locus of control, job satisfaction, and supervision satisfaction. 57 Chapter 4 RESULTS The independent variables in the study were leader sex (i.e., male, female), leadership style (i.e., democratic, autocratic), and participant locus of control (i.e., high internal, low internal), while the dependent variables were employee job satisfaction (as measured with the Job Descriptive Index, Job in General Scale, and Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire (short form)) and employee supervision satisfaction (as measured by the Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire (long form: supervision subscale)). A 2 x 2 x 2 between-subjects multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was performed. No outliers were observed in the analysis; thus, all 160 participants were included in the analysis, with females consisting of 82.5% of the sample. Participants were ethnically diverse and their ages ranged from 18 to 30 years old, with an average age of 20 years. Evaluation of the properties of the data set (e.g., normality, equality of variance-covariance matrices) determined that the data met the necessary statistical assumptions to support the analysis. An alpha level of .05 was used for all multivariate statistical tests and a Bonferroni correction was used to evaluate the univariate effects using .0125 as the corrected alpha level. The results of this study are organized in the following way: dichotomizing locus of control is discussed, then the interaction effects are addressed, and finally, any main effects for the independent variables are then analyzed. 58 Dichotomizing Locus of Control A median split was performed on the participant scores from the Internal Control Index to assign participants to either a high or a low internal locus of control group. The mean scores ranged from 2.71 to 4.57 and the distribution had a median score of 3.65. Based on whether each participant’s score was above or below the median, respondents were assigned to either a high or a low internal locus of control group. Using this scoring method, participants in the high internal locus of control group had a mean score of 3.68 and higher and participants in the low internal locus of control group had a mean score of 3.65 and below. After the median split, the means and standard deviations were calculated for the high internal locus of control group (M = 3.94, SD = .20) and low internal locus of control group (M = 3.38, SD = .25). Interaction Effects Results showed all interaction effects were non-significant. It was expected that participants would be less satisfied when they were led by autocratic women than when they were led by autocratic men (Eagly et al., 1992), but this was not supported by the current study. Additionally, it was hypothesized that those participants who had higher internal locus of control would be less satisfied with autocratic leaders than those individuals with less internal locus of control, but this was also not supported by the current study (see Table 1). 59 Effects for the Independent Variables Using Wilks’s criterion (see Table 1), the dependent variables were significantly affected by leadership style (Wilks’s λ, F[4, 149] = 3.86, p < .005, η2 = .09). Univariate ANOVAs were conducted on each dependent measure separately to determine the locus of the statistically significant main effect. From Table 1, we can see that leadership style significantly affected Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire (long form: supervision subscale), The Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire (short form), and the Job In General satisfaction scores. Means and standard deviations of the dependent variables for the two groups are presented in Table 2. It appears that those individuals under democratic leadership styles have higher job satisfaction than those under autocratic leadership. Using Wilks’s criterion (see Table 1) the dependent variables were also significantly affected by locus of control (Wilks’s λ, F[4,149 ] = 2.46 , p < .048, η2 = .06). Univariate ANOVAs were conducted on each dependent measure separately to determine the locus of the statistically significant main effect. From Table 1, we can see that locus of control statistically affected the Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire (short form). Means and standard deviations of the dependent variables for the two groups are presented in Table 3. It appears that those individuals with higher internal locus of control had higher employee satisfaction than those with lower internal locus of control. No statistically significant leader sex effects were observed for employee satisfaction, thus showing no support for my hypothesis. 60 Table 1 Multivariate and Univariate Analysis of Variance for Employee Satisfaction Measures Multivariate Univariate Source df Fa JDI JIGb MSQb MSQ (Sup)b F ratios for Leadership Sex MSE 4 .647 .25 .07 1.47 .51 .52 .15 1.18 .74 F ratios for Leadership Style MSE 4 3.86 3.84 1.04 6.41* 2.21 9.51* 2.78 12.61* 7.90 F ratios for Locus of Control MSE 4 2.46 2.00 .54 5.44 1.88 6.88* 2.01 1.79 1.12 F ratios for Leader Sex* Leadership Style MSE 4 .48 .85 1.44 .13 .00 .23 .50 .04 .00 F ratios for Leader Sex* Locus of Control MSE 4 .11 .11 .44 .51 .73 .04 .13 .32 .11 1.69 .07 .00 .03 .58 .02 .00 2.34 .08 .28 .21 .63 .03 .08 .13 F ratios for Leadership Style* 4 Locus of Control MSE .95 .68 F ratios for Leader Sex* 4 .90 Leadership Style*Locus of Control MSE Note: Multivariate F ratios were generated from Wilks’s criterion. a. Multivariate df = 4, 149 b. Univariate df = 7, 152 *p< .0125. 61 Table 2 Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for Employee Satisfaction Measures as a Function of Leadership Style Leadership Style JDI JIG MSQ MSQ: Supervision Democratic Mean N Std. Dev 1.98 80 .54 2.26 80 .54 3.70 80 .55 3.69 80 .72 Autocratic Mean N Std. Dev 1.84 80 .51 2.01 80 .64 3.43 80 .54 3.24 80 .85 62 Table 3 Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for Employee Satisfaction Measures as a Function of Locus of Control Locus of Control JDI JIG MSQ MSQ: Supervision Lower Internal Mean N Std. Dev 1.85 78 .46 2.02 78 .58 3.44 78 .45 3.37 78 .72 Higher Internal Mean N Std. Dev 1.97 82 .58 2.24 82 .62 3.68 82 .63 3.56 82 .89 63 Chapter 5 DISCUSSION The literature on leadership style, leader sex, and employee locus of control suggests that leaders and employee locus of control might affect employee satisfaction. For example, someone who is under authoritative leadership may be less satisfied than someone under democratic leadership. The focus of this study was to examine the role of leader sex, leadership style, and employee locus of control and to investigate how these factors might influence employee satisfaction. This section will examine the findings in light of the theoretical framework and methods used in this study. We first examined whether leader sex played a role with employee satisfaction. We expected that participants would be more satisfied with male leaders than female leaders (Eagly & Johnson, 1990). Inconsistent with our hypothesis, results showed that leader sex did not significantly affect employee satisfaction. Some experiments, which had participants rate theoretical leaders, have shown bias against women. Davidson and Burke’s (2000) review of 49 experiments showed that men were preferred over women for masculine type jobs, like auto salesperson, while women were preferred over men for feminine type jobs. Other researchers have also shown that male leaders are preferred over female leaders (Merrick, 2002). Eagly et al. (1992) conducted a meta-analysis regarding gender and leader evaluations on written vignettes and confederate studies using mostly college students. Results generally showed that females were evaluated slightly more negatively than male leaders, especially when exhibiting autocratic behavior. 64 The results of the current study are interpreted in relation to the role congruity theory (Eagly & Karau, 2002). Role congruity theory examines sex differences regarding negative attitudes toward women and the impact that gender roles have on employee satisfaction. In particular, I consider that the lack of significant results between the male and female leaders may be attributed to leadership becoming a more androgynous term, meaning that there was minimal perceived incongruity between the gender roles and leadership roles. It has been suggested that the type of industry in which female leaders are employed could heighten the perceived incongruity between the gender roles and the leadership roles (Garcia-Retamero & Lopez-Zafra, 2006). In the current study there was no mention of the type of industry in which the individual worked. Since the vignettes were constructed to be as neutral as possible on all factors except the independent variables, it is possible that participants perceived the industry to be gender neutral. This would help explain why they were perceived similarly. It is also possible that the stereotype of men as leaders has decreased as well, and that may be why there was less prejudice found against women. After all, there has been a monumental change in gender equality over the years, especially since most women are employed in the paid workforce (Eagly & Diekman, 2005) and because there are more female managers than ever before. This suggests that gender egalitarianism is slowly becoming more prominent in today’s society, even in the workplace (Vianello & Siemienska, 1990). It is also possible that the results are reflecting generational differences that exist. Since most of the sample size were of a younger generation (ages ranged from 19 to 30 65 years old, with a mean age of 20 years), it is possible that this sample is reflecting new cultural differences. These participants have been raised in an era where women have had equal rights and have been employed in the workforce, possibly even as managers. If leadership has become a more androgynous term, it is possible that the participants have internalized that. Although the current research may have found no difference between employee satisfaction with female and male leaders, this does not imply that sex roles are still not present and that the sexes are equal. Instead, it may suggest that the equality between the genders has improved and continued to change, but has not necessarily disappeared overall. Although we must be cautious about drawing inferences based on null findings, it is possible that as more women have recently become career oriented and have sought higher leadership positions, the equality and balance between the sexes has improved. In turn, this may have weakened the impact of traditional occupational roles and shifted society’s perception of leadership into a more androgynous role (Wood & Eagly, 2002). Consistent with role congruity theory, if leadership has indeed become more androgynous, then this may help in explaining why female leaders were evaluated similarly to male leaders in the present study. This study also investigated the relationship between leadership style and employee satisfaction. Since people typically are not satisfied with autocratic leaders, it was hypothesized that respondents would be more satisfied with democratic leaders than autocratic leaders (Looney, 2004). This was significant in the present study, meaning that the overall mean employee satisfaction scores were statistically significant. In other 66 words, a democratic leadership style was associated with greater job satisfaction than an autocratic leadership style was. Some research has found that members under autocratic leadership are more satisfied than groups led by democratic leaders, although this research seems to be rather sparse (Foels et al., 2000). Other studies have found more dramatic differences in member satisfaction between the democratic and autocratic leadership styles, where employees are more satisfied with democratic leaders than autocratic ones (Thomas, 1992). Other researchers suggest that democratic leadership enhances the quality of decisions that are made and increases member satisfaction (Bass, 1990). The current study supports most other research and my own expectations, in that the results suggest that people under democratic leadership are generally more satisfied than people under autocratic leadership. The United States of America is a liberal and individualistic culture that values the right for personal choices (Kim, Triandis, Kagitcibasi, Choi, & Yoon, 1994). In regards to employees, it is logical that people would be more satisfied in a workplace that is conducive to a more collaborative relationship, where employees engage in the decision-making process, and their ideas are addressed. When under a democratic/individualistic style, the employees have more responsibility and personal achievement invested (Kagitcibasi, 1997). Furthermore, those people who are more individualistic are inclined to weigh the costs and benefits of situations and act according to what benefits them the most. People under autocratic leadership often do not have responsibility in the decision making process, since choices 67 are made solely by the leader. Therefore, it seems understandable that participants in the current study preferred a democratic leadership style to an autocratic one. These results can also be interpreted in terms of how people respond to aggressiveness and dominance in others. Since the sample was largely comprised of females, it is possible that the respondents responded differently than if the study were comprised of a mostly male sample. Since males are socialized to be more competitive and dominant than women, it is possible that women will respond more negatively to autocratic leadership because it is incongruent with their belief system. Kushell and Newtan (1986) further suggest that the gender of the subordinate and the leadership style of the leader are important moderating variables in employee satisfaction. Collectively, this suggests that the current research is congruent with past research. In the present study, participants were more satisfied with democratic leadership over autocratic leadership. This implies that democratic leadership may be another factor that contributes to employee satisfaction. Although the current study does not suggest that democratic leadership is useful to use in all situations and contexts, it supports the belief that democratic leadership may lead to more satisfied employees. The third hypothesis focused on the degree of employee satisfaction in regards to different leadership styles and sex of the leader. It was expected that participants would be less satisfied when they were led by autocratic women than when they were led by autocratic men (Eagly et al., 1992). Results showed that the interaction between leadership style, leader sex, and overall employee satisfaction was not significant, thus showing no support for my hypothesis. 68 Some research suggests sex stereotypes are still present in today’s society and these gender roles spill over into leadership roles as well (Eagly & Johnson, 1990). This results in women facing more prejudice, especially when they engage in leadership styles that are incongruent with their gender roles (Eagly & Johannesen-Schmidt, 2001). Women are forced to either adhere to gender roles and be viewed a poor leader or adhere to leadership roles and be subjected to negative consequences. This double standard has often left women in confusion about how they should act in leadership roles. Further research conducted by Eagly et al. (1995) suggests that there was no difference in leadership effectiveness between male and female leaders once all the studies in their meta-analysis were aggregated. This suggested that when women were managers, they were as equally as effective as men, even though they faced possible barriers and disadvantages as leaders. However, this generalization may not be applicable in all settings (e.g., gender congeniality of leadership roles, type of organization, level of leadership) but instead serves as a general tendency that is worthy of notation. Eagly et al. (1995) also suggest that rater sex may affect a study’s results and that leader sex does affect ratings, especially for gender congruent leadership positions. In other words, leader effectiveness increased when gender roles and leadership roles were congruent. Therefore, the leader gender and the specifics of the leadership role may influence leader effectiveness. Complicating this discussion are inconsistent and somewhat ambiguous and unclear patterns regarding leader evaluations and leader sex. Conclusively, these findings show evidence that leaders may receive similar evaluations in most settings, 69 even though they are differentially effective in many settings. Additionally, a myriad of other factors can contribute to leader effectiveness (e.g., leader’s skill, training). The current research suggests that although gender roles may spill over into organizational roles, employee satisfaction was not significantly affected by the interaction between leader sex, leadership style, and locus of control. The relationship between employee satisfaction and locus of control was also explored. It was hypothesized that those participants who had a lower internal locus of control would exhibit lower job satisfaction than those with a higher internal locus of control. This is because external locus of control is negatively related to job satisfaction (Muhonen & Torkelson, 2004). The main effect between internal locus of control and overall employee satisfaction was found to be significant, showing support for our hypothesis. Consistent with past research, the present study found that those with higher internal locus of control had higher employee satisfaction than those with lower internal locus of control. Judge and Bono (2001) found that locus of control had an estimated true score correlation of .32 with job satisfaction. Other research has shown that external locus of control is negatively related to job satisfaction but that there was no difference between men and women (Muhonen & Torkelson, 2004). Prior research has documented a correlation of negative affectivity on mental health complaints and job-induced tension (Naswall, Sverke, & Hellgreen, 2005). This indicates that negative people may be more likely to experience strain than those that have a positive mindset. Positive affectivity has been shown to significantly affect job 70 dissatisfaction and mental health complaints. Furthermore, those with lower internal locus of control have been shown to have higher mental health complaints, dissatisfaction, and job induced tension compared to those with less external locus of control. The results of the present study are interpreted in regards to locus of control and employee satisfaction. The significant findings support that personality variables (e.g., locus of control) may be an important moderator for satisfaction, but the extent of this is unknown. Those people with lower internal locus of control may believe that other people, the environment, fate, or chance determine events that happen to them. As people think that other sources have more power and control over them, they believe that their behaviors do not affect their current state, which then affects their satisfaction. If people feel that they are not in control of their life, then they may be unsatisfied with their current situation. Such implications could mean that one’s personality could potentially affect other aspects of their work life (e.g., job satisfaction, productivity, job stress, turnover). Therefore, there should be more theory development and empirical research conducted to explore the relationship between personality characteristics and their effect on employee satisfaction. Finally, the last hypothesis involved the issue of employee satisfaction, locus of control, leader sex, and leadership style. It was hypothesized that those participants who had more internal locus of control would be less satisfied with autocratic leaders than those people with external locus of control. This is because internal locus of control is when people feel that reinforcement occurs because of their own actions, whereas people 71 with external locus of control believe that reinforcement occurs because of things other than their own behavior (Meyers & Wong, 1988). Results showed the interaction between internal locus of control, leader sex, and leadership style and overall employee satisfaction was not significant. Past research, which focused on personality variables in this discipline of research, has evaluated how autocratic and democratic subordinates evaluate either autocratic or democratic leaders. Research has shown that autocratic individuals tend to prefer autocratic leaders over democratic ones (e.g., Cellar et al., 2001). No known research has evaluated the effects of subordinate internal locus of control on leader effectiveness ratings. The current research did not find a significant difference in employee satisfaction between varying leader sex, leadership style, and locus of control. Although leadership style and locus of control significantly affected employee satisfaction, there was not a significant interaction between leader sex, leadership style, and locus of control. Although we must be cautious about drawing inferences based on null findings, it is possible that leader sex, leadership style, and locus of control do not collectively affect employee satisfaction. In other words, although the locus of control and the leadership style may affect employee satisfaction, they do not directly interact and affect employee satisfaction together. Personality variables, such as locus of control, may not interact with external circumstances (e.g., leader sex, leadership style), but instead may be factors that affect employee satisfaction. This may help in explaining why there were no 72 significant interactions between leader sex, leadership style, and employee locus of control in the present study. Limitations and Suggestions for Future Research The present study has several issues and limitations that are worth our attention and consideration. A major limitation was the small number and demographics of the participants subjected to each condition, which prevented a more extensive data examination. More specifically, all of the participants were undergraduates at the California State University, Sacramento. Since the current study was completed with undergraduates, and mostly female undergraduates (which may or may not be currently employed) this could have biased the study’s results. This is because the undergraduate sample may not be a representative sample of the population and may have had more liberal views. Additionally, most of the participants used were psychology students/students enrolled in a psychology course. It is possible that those people enrolled in psychology courses may also be fundamentally different than people who take other courses (e.g., engineering, nursing). Had the study been conducted using engineering (a male dominated industry) and nursing students (a female dominated industry) different results could have been obtained. Similarly, if actual employees (that were not enrolled in school) were used in addition to the undergraduates, it is likely the results of the study could have changed. Other factors (e.g., geographic location, sex, ethnicity, age of participants) could have potentially affected the study as well. Therefore, the sample may be fundamentally different than the population and not a representative sample of 73 the population as a whole. Future research may consider using a more representative sample. Other limitations included the design methodology used. The current study used laboratory research methods to assess what may happen in real life situations. Although some researchers suggest that laboratory research can be applicable to real world settings, we must be cautious when interpreting these findings. Had we used real life situations in the present study, it would have been difficult to control the extraneous variables. Therefore, although there is real potential for the current study, we must be aware that these results may not generalize to all real life situations easily, but probably reflect culturally held beliefs. Furthermore, the current study used vignettes, brief descriptions of the job they held, along with other factors regarding their job. The vignettes were written to be realistic to the respondent, and mirror issues that were easily understood by the respondent (e.g., opportunities for promotion, leadership) and would occur in real life. Meanwhile, they were written so that all details within the story were internally consistent and provided enough contextual information for respondents to clearly understand the situation being portrayed, but were ambiguous enough to ensure that multiple solutions existed. The vignettes were consistent with the research question in mind and carefully worded to ensure that both the leader sex and leadership style (both the relevant variables being examined) were apparent, but did not directly influence the respondent’s answer. 74 Another limitation of the vignette could have been the operationalization of the leadership style variable in the vignette. Foels, et al. (2000) suggest that the potency of operationalization moderates group member satisfaction when using artificial groups and leadership. As the relative difference between the democratic and autocratic leadership styles increased, the difference in satisfaction increased as well. In the present study, the vignettes were written to be different enough so that those people who reviewed them would be able to distinguish between the conditions, but subtle enough so that participants would not be able to identify the variables that were being manipulated in the study. Likewise, the democratic and autocratic leader’s behaviors were written to be subtle behaviors of each leadership style to ensure that the potency between conditions were visible, but not obvious. However, the distinction between the conditions is subjective and the range in the democratic and autocratic leadership continuum is vague, so it is unclear if the efforts to appropriately differentiate between each leadership style were successful. Despite these efforts to ensure the vignettes were properly constructed, the fact that vignettes were used instead of a real world scenario might have biased the results. It is possible that the reader did not fully synthesize the sex of the leader as they may have done in real life. In other words, since they were merely reading the vignette, they did not fully process all the components of having a male or female supervisor. On the other hand, this can imply that leader sex does not, in and of itself, mean that employees will be satisfied with his or her leadership. Rather it may imply that other attributes that are associated with sex (e.g., interpersonal skills, gender roles) may affect the person’s 75 perceptions of the leader. Therefore, it is necessary to conduct more research to examine the relationship between leader sex, leadership style, and personality variables on employee satisfaction. Although this study used multiple satisfaction inventories (e.g., Job In General Index, Job Descriptive Index, Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire), all of which used multiple item measures to assess employee satisfaction, there are other ways for assessing how leaders and personality variables may affect organizational success and its employees. Such examples include, but are not limited to, turnover rates, personal leave, employee productivity, and performance reports. For example, employees are more likely to use sick leave if they are unhappy at work. Further research on these factors might help to understand the complex paradigm that is employee satisfaction. The current research has the strength of a between-subjects design and was an experiment that was carefully executed. However, the current study only explored employee satisfaction in regards to leader sex, leadership style, and locus of control. While these factors may affect employee satisfaction, they are not the sole factors that contribute to employee satisfaction. Future research is needed to further examine other factors that may affect employee satisfaction (e.g., personality variables). Other research shows the importance of investigating these other factors to gain a more comprehensive understanding about employee satisfaction. Employee satisfaction is a complex paradigm; no one factor solely influences someone’s satisfaction, but instead a myriad of factors may interact and affect employee satisfaction. A more thorough investigation is essential to understand this multifaceted model. 76 Research Implications The current study aimed to analyze categories of leader sex, leadership style, and locus of control in relation to employee satisfaction. Even though the interactions did not yield any significant results, this study still has contributed to a better understanding of one or more complex variables included in the study. As stated prior, many factors can affect employee satisfaction. Leaders and personality variables are two important variables worthy of our attention, especially since the interaction of personality variables, such as locus of control, and employee satisfaction has not been extensively studied. Since more women are entering management positions, traditional occupational roles may have shifted. The current study has attempted to review how and if leader sex affects employee satisfaction. Other implications of the current research suggest that training could be a viable source to help increase employee satisfaction. Since people generally prefer democratic leaders over autocratic ones, supervisors can be trained to exhibit characteristics that are more consistent with the democratic leadership style (e.g., allowing employees to engage in the decision making process). Some believe that women are ineffective leaders and believe that training is needed to minimize gender issues in the workplace (Guadagno & Cialdini, 2007). Organizations could also train women regarding social role theory and impression management so they are aware and can further address it. Further coaching would also encourage women to enter leadership roles. This will decrease the likelihood that women will self-select themselves out of leadership roles and help to increase female representation in leadership (Killeen, Lopez-Zafra, & Eagly, 2006). 77 Although women may not be adequately represented in management positions, there are still many successful women in the corporate world (Guadagno & Cialdini, 2007). These women may use a hybrid technique of masculine and feminine impression management tactics in their leadership style. It is possible that as more and more women enter into the male dominated industries (e.g., females in the automotive industry, women in politics) these gender roles will become more androgynous. The current study also suggests that this information may be helpful in developing new measures that assess or predict employee retention. Both the leadership style and the locus of control of the individual are factors that may affect employee satisfaction and in turn affect employee retention and organizational success. More research is necessary to see the correlation of these factors and to address how employee satisfaction can be used in predicting employee retention. Although research has shown that women are not equal to men in many aspects all over the world (e.g., Sczesny, Bosak, Neff, & Schyns, 2004) and may still not be equal within the United States of America, there has been an increase in women’s education, so much that young women have become more educated than young men (Eagly, 2007). When women do encounter barriers, organizational change can counteract or weaken these barriers. Likewise, as more women become successful, especially in male dominated societies, it is likely that the biases that exist will be reduced. Acknowledging problems associated with employee satisfaction is only the first step in the process. Collectively, this information can be used to develop new ways of counteracting employee dissatisfaction and instead create a positive working environment 78 for everyone. Since employee satisfaction is critical for organizational success, it is important to attend to those factors that will increase employee satisfaction. Then management, human resources, and employees alike can work to try to increase employee satisfaction. The implications of this study can be important also when considering personality variables and their effect on employee satisfaction. In particular, organizations may benefit from addressing the nature and the degree of employee personality to better assist with managing them. Managers should become more sensitive and familiarize themselves with different factors of employee satisfaction to ensure they are better able to understand employees and create an environment where employees are more satisfied and the organization is more successful. Not only must organizations be more conducive to change, but a broader cultural change is necessary to ensure that employees, managers, and organizations alike are empowered. It is important that they have the foundational knowledge, skills, desires, and opportunities to succeed so the organization can flourish as well. 79 APPENDIX A Human Subjects Application Consent Form I hereby agree to participate in research that will be conducted by Michelle Garbato. In this research, I will receive a packet of material containing some demographic questions, some inventories and a situation to read. Before I read the situation I will be asked to fill out an inventory; after reading the situation, I will also be asked to complete other inventories concerning the satisfaction I feel towards the situation. The research will take place in one of the research rooms on the third floor of Amador Hall and will require 30 minutes of my time. I understand that this research may have the following benefits: I understand that I will receive one half-hour of credit toward satisfying the Psychology Department’s research participation requirement by participating in this study. I understand that this research may have the benefit of helping to increase my knowledge of relations between people and of helping to increase my knowledge of research methods. I understand that there is a risk that I may have feelings of being uncomfortable with some of the material in this study; however, I also understand that I may discontinue my participation at any time without any penalty other than loss of research credit and that the investigator may discontinue my participation at any time. I also understand that my responses will not be examined individually, but only be used to generate group information, maintaining my confidentiality. This information was explained to me by Michelle Garbato. I understand that she will answer any questions I may have now or later about this research. Michelle Garbato can be reached at imgarbanzo@hotmail.com. Signature: Date: 80 Procedure The purpose of this study is to assess the effects of gender, leadership style, and locus of control on employee satisfaction. This was assessed using vignettes that were based on a combination of two independent variables. The experimental design is a 2 x 2 x 2 between subjects design. Before reading the vignettes, participants will fill out the Internal Control Index to assess the person’s locus of control. Following this, they will read the vignette. After reading the vignette, the participant will complete the Job Descriptive Index, Job in General Inventory, the Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire (short form) and the supervision subscales from the Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire (long form). The Job Descriptive Index measures the five facets of satisfaction, including work on present job, present pay, and opportunities for promotion, supervision, and coworkers. The Job in General Inventory and the Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire (short form) measures the person’s overall global job satisfaction. Meanwhile, the supervision facets (from the Minnesota Satisfaction (long form)) measure the person’s satisfaction in terms of supervision in both technical and human relations aspects. Once all of the data have been collected, participants will be scored on their locus of control, overall satisfaction level, as well as their supervision satisfaction. The vignette includes a situation where the participant has to imagine that they are working at a hypothetical organization, Smith’s Professional Services. Two independent variables are contained in the vignette itself. In one version of the story, the employee has a female, autocratic leader, whereas in other versions of the story the employee has a female, democratic leader, male, autocratic leader, or male, democratic leader. The third independent variable in the study is the level of locus of control of the participants: they are coded as having a high or low internal locus of control. The dependent variable in this study is the amount of overall job satisfaction and supervision satisfaction that the employee has. The 18 questions from the Job in General (JIG) Scale, 72 questions from the Job Descriptive Index (JDI), and 20 questions from the Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire (MSQ; short form) were used to assess the person’s overall job satisfaction. The JIG and JDI items are assessed on a 3-point response scale using responses such as Y for yes (if it describes their work), N for no (if it does not describe it) and ? (if they cannot decide). The 20 questions from the MSQ (short form) used a 5-point scale, using response options such as very satisfied, satisfied, neutral, dissatisfied, and very dissatisfied. Finally, the questions from the MSQ (long form) supervision facets (technical and human relations) were used to assess satisfaction with their supervisor. Responses to these items also used a 5-point response scale, which includes response options such as very satisfied, satisfied, neutral, dissatisfied, and very dissatisfied. When students enter the research room and are seated, the researcher will distribute consent forms to sign. After signing and returning them to the researcher, the consent forms will be placed together in a separate folder to ensure that they cannot be traced back to the experimental materials of particular participants. Then a script of initial 81 instructions will be read to the participants (Appendix A). Following that, the packet of materials will be provided to the participants who will be instructed to not place their name or any other identifying mark on the materials. This packet will include: an Internal Control Index, one of four vignettes, the Job in General Inventory and Job Descriptive Index, the Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire (short form), and the Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire supervision subscales from the long form. The Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire items from the long and short form will be combined and randomized. This is to further block the participant from knowing what is being manipulated. Then the order in which the Job Descriptive Index scales are given will also be randomized (e.g., supervision, coworkers) but will be administered together in a bunch so that it stays as one inventory. Finally, the order of the Job Descriptive Index, Job In General Inventory, and Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire inventories will be determined by a randomized block process. The demographic and organizational questions will attached last. After participants have completed all of their tasks, the packets will be collected and placed together in a different folder from the one containing the consent forms; this will ensure that the packets will not be able to be linked to specific participants. The researcher will then orally debrief the participants, answer any questions they may have at that time, and hand out the debriefing page for participants to keep. Everyone will then be thanked for their participation and will be dismissed. 82 Debriefing The Effects of Leader Sex, Leadership Style, and Locus of Control on Employee Satisfaction Purpose: The purpose of this study was to investigate the effects of three independent variables: the locus of control of the participant, the sex of the leader, and the leadership style of the leader. The dependent variable, was the amount of employee satisfaction as measured by the Job Descriptive Index, Job in General, and Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire items that you completed after reading the vignette. Hypotheses and Supporting Research: Research by Eagly, Makhijani & Klonsky (1992) has shown that people accept autocratic male leaders better than they do autocratic female leaders. Role congruity theory further elaborates that men are evaluated more positively than women (because they are perceived to be leaders) and are more likely to succeed in leadership positions than their female counterparts. Further research suggests that overall subjects were more satisfied with democratically led groups, but the sex of the leader did not affect the subordinate’s satisfaction level. In the present research, I wanted to see if participants would be more satisfied in situations with male leaders than female leaders overall and to see if participants would be less satisfied when they were led by autocratic women than when they were supervised by autocratic men. However, because people typically are not satisfied with autocratic leaders in general, I hypothesized that respondents would be more satisfied with democratic leaders than autocratic leaders. Furthermore, I hypothesized that those who had a lower internal locus of control would exhibit lower job satisfaction than those with a higher internal locus of control. Finally, I hypothesized that those participants that have higher internal locus of control will be less satisfied with autocratic leaders than those people with lower internal locus of control. To test my hypotheses, I used vignettes—short stories that were different only in the sex and leadership style of the leader. After reading the vignette, you completed the Job Descriptive Index, the Job in General Scale, the Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire (short form) and the Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire supervision subscales from the long form. The characteristics that these inventories collectively measure are overall satisfaction and employee satisfaction on five levels, including work on present job, present pay, and opportunities for promotion, supervision, and coworkers. Once all of the data is collected, I will score the locus of control and satisfaction inventories to assess the locus of control and satisfaction of employees whom had different sex and leadership style supervisors. I will then perform a multivariate analysis of variance to test my hypothesis. Psychological Services: If this research has made you feel uncomfortable and you feel that you may want to talk with someone about these feelings, you may contact the Sacramento State Student Health Center on campus, as there are psychological services 83 available to you on the second floor of the Health Center; the phone number there is 916278-6416. Contact Information: The results of this study will be available the last week of the next semester. If you would like further information about this study or have questions regarding this study, please contact imgarbanzo@hotmail.com.com at your convenience. 84 APPENDIX B Instructions (Read to Participants) My name is Michelle Garbato. Let me first of all thank you for taking part in this research study. In a moment we can begin the research, but first I would like to go over a few things with you: 1. Please read the packet thoroughly, making sure to pay attention to all aspects of the packet. Make sure that your answers are well thought out. However, remember that all the answers are subjective and that there is no right or wrong answers. 2. Please go through your packet and complete the scales and other materials in the exact order as they appear. 3. Please answer every question in the packet to the best of your ability. If you have any questions than please feel free to ask me for assistance. 4. I will administer a debriefing sheet when you are all through, and I will return your research card, which will be stamped with one half hour of credit. Do you have any questions before we begin? 85 APPENDIX C Vignettes Authoritarian (Autocratic leadership): Female Leader You have been working at an organization called Smith Professional Services for approximately two and a half years and have been promoted once and received two raises. The company expects its employees to follow the typical policies regarding punctuality, stealing, morality and ethics, as well as general guidelines surrounding dress code, internet and email use, ownership of copyright and design and the use of the company facilities. Smith Professional Services has been in business for approximately 50 years and has relatively stable job security. You make a salary of $47,584 per year, while having to work only minimal overtime. The current work you do consists of communicating with others, maintaining a professional disposition, facilitating team performance, and completing tasks that are relevant to your position. Occasionally you will receive special projects to complete, which will allow you to work on assignments of varying difficulty. You also train and guide new subordinates, although the bulk of your time is spent conducting projects. You are undoubtedly skilled at your job and show promise for a successful future. You receive a sufficient amount of praise for the work you do and are respected by the subordinates that you train. You are in a second level position that consists of a unique series of five promotional positions. You consider conditions of the workplace to be ok; in other words, you do not work in a state of the art facility, but it suits your needs. A typical day consists of working a shift from nine am to five-thirty pm, in which you have a ten-minute commute. You work in your own beige colored cubicle for some of the day; although you may travel a fair amount depending on the project you are working on. You always have work to do; but you have a minimal amount of independence and minimal input in the decision-making process, and you are unable to choose which projects you will participate in. All projects must be completed by the required deadline, but if this is unavoidable then sound justification must be provided. Your unit consists of about eleven people and you get along with most of them. There is one person in particular that seems to annoy you, but you are able to interact with them professionally and cordially. Your immediate supervisor’s name is Pat. She has been your manager for one year and three months and she is a reasonably competent upper level manager. Pat has just announced she assigned four people to work on a new project, one of which is you. She did not consult anyone prior to making this decision; instead she is requiring that each person work on mandatory parts of the project. She gave you, and other members of the 86 project staff, a document that stated her expectations for exactly what, when, and how the project must be completed. Each project staff member will be responsible for completing their part of the project accurately and on time, even if that falls out of their specialty area. Having known Pat for a while now, you have noticed that Pat always controls her department in this way even if the project falls out of her area of expertise. Pat tends to handle complaints, problems, training, praise, and punishment in this same structured and dictated way. She always seems to make decisions independently or with little or no input from the staff, even if it goes against the project staff or organization’s interest. One day you are sitting at your desk thinking about your current position. Reflecting back on the last two and a half years, you begin to think about your job. Knowing what you ideally want within an organization, you start comparing your current job to your ideal job. In order to assess how satisfied you are with your current job you decide to take some satisfaction inventories. Remembering to read all instructions carefully and answer the items thoughtfully, you begin to complete the inventories starting on the following page. 87 Participative Leadership (Democratic Leader); Female Leader You have been working at an organization called Smith Professional Services for approximately two and a half years and have been promoted once and received two raises. The company expects its employees to follow the typical policies regarding punctuality, stealing, morality and ethics, as well as general guidelines surrounding dress code, internet and email use, ownership of copyright and design and the use of the company facilities. Smith Professional Services has been in business for approximately 50 years and has reasonably stable job security. You make a salary of $47,584 per year, while having to work only minimal overtime. The current work you do consists of communicating with others, maintaining a professional disposition, facilitating team performance, and completing tasks that are relevant to your position. Occasionally you will receive special projects to do, which will allow you to complete assignments of varying difficulty. You also train and guide new subordinates, although the bulk of your time is spent conducting projects. You are undoubtedly skilled at your job and show promise for a successful future. You receive a sufficient amount of praise for the work you do and are respected by the subordinates that you train. You are in a second level position that consists of a unique series of five promotional positions. You consider conditions of the workplace to be ok; in other words, you do not work in a state of the art facility, but it suits your needs. A typical day consists of working a shift from nine am to five-thirty pm, in which you have a ten-minute commute. You work in your own beige colored cubicle for some of the day; although you may travel a fair amount depending on the project you are working on. You always have something to do and you are given a moderate amount of independence and input in the decision-making process, allowing you to suggest which projects you want to participate in. If you do not complete projects on time then you are granted extensions if you have an acceptable justification. Your unit consists of about eleven people and you get along with most of them. There is one person in particular that seems to annoy you, but you are able to interact with them professionally and cordially. Your immediate supervisor’s name is Pat. She has been your manager for approximately one year and three months and is a reasonably competent upper level manager. She has just announced a project in which she will need four people to participate. She is asking for knowledgeable volunteers to help complete the project so that she can then designate work for it. If she does not receive four volunteers, then she will request people to work on it. She will also give the project team guidance and will take suggestions as to exactly what, when, and how the project should be completed. Therefore, not all of the project responsibility will fall on one person; instead the people working on it can discuss which parts of the assignment are best suited for each project team member so that each person contributes in their specialty area. Then Pat will assign each project team member a specific assignment to complete. Having known Pat for a while now, you have noticed 88 that Pat always runs her department this way even if this falls out of her area of expertise. Pat tends to handle complaints, problems, training, praise, and punishment in the same flexible and creative way. She always tries to make decisions with team input, so that it is in line with the team or organization’s interest. One day you are sitting at your desk thinking about your current position. Reflecting back on the last two and a half years, you begin to think about your job. Knowing what you ideally want within an organization, you start comparing your current job to your ideal job. In order to assess how satisfied you are with your current job you decide to take some satisfaction inventories. Remembering to read all instructions carefully and answer the items thoughtfully, you begin to complete the inventories starting on the following page. 89 Authoritarian (Autocratic leadership): Male Leader You have been working at an organization called Smith Professional Services for approximately two and a half years and have been promoted once and received two raises. The company expects its employees to follow the typical policies regarding punctuality, stealing, morality and ethics, as well as general guidelines surrounding dress code, internet and email use, ownership of copyright and design and the use of the company facilities. Smith Professional Services has been in business for approximately 50 years and has relatively stable job security. You make a salary of $47,584 per year, while having to work only minimal overtime. The current work you do consists of communicating with others, maintaining a professional disposition, facilitating team performance, and completing tasks that are relevant to your position. Occasionally you will receive special projects to complete, which will allow you to work on assignments of varying difficulty. You also train and guide new subordinates, although the bulk of your time is spent conducting projects. You are undoubtedly skilled at your job and show promise for a successful future. You receive a sufficient amount of praise for the work you do and are respected by the subordinates that you train. You are in a second level position that consists of a unique series of five promotional positions. You consider conditions of the workplace to be ok; in other words, you do not work in a state of the art facility, but it suits your needs. A typical day consists of working a shift from nine am to five-thirty pm, in which you have a ten-minute commute. You work in your own beige colored cubicle for some of the day; although you may travel a fair amount depending on the project you are working on. You always have work to do; but you have a minimal amount of independence and minimal input in the decision-making process, and you are unable to choose which projects you will participate in. All projects must be completed by the required deadline, but if this is unavoidable then sound justification must be provided. Your unit consists of about eleven people and you get along with most of them. There is one person in particular that seems to annoy you, but you are able to interact with them professionally and cordially. Your immediate supervisor’s name is Pat. He has been your manager for one year and three months and he is a reasonably competent upper level manager. Pat has just announced he assigned four people to work on a new project, one of which is you. He did not consult anyone prior to making this decision; instead he is requiring that each person work on mandatory parts of the project. He gave you, and other members of the project staff, a document that stated his expectations for exactly what, when, and how the project must be completed. Each project staff member will be responsible for completing their part of the project accurately and on time, even if that falls out of their specialty area. Having known Pat for a while now, you have noticed that Pat always controls his department in this way even if the project falls out of his area of expertise. Pat tends to 90 handle complaints, problems, training, praise, and punishment in this same structured and dictated way. He always seems to make decisions independently or with little or no input from the staff, even if it goes against the project staff or organization’s interest. One day you are sitting at your desk thinking about your current position. Reflecting back on the last two and a half years, you begin to think about your job. Knowing what you ideally want within an organization, you start comparing your current job to your ideal job. In order to assess how satisfied you are with your current job you decide to take some satisfaction inventories. Remembering to read all instructions carefully and answer the items thoughtfully, you begin to complete the inventories starting on the following page. 91 Participative Leadership (Democratic Leader); Male Leader You have been working at an organization called Smith Professional Services for approximately two and a half years and have been promoted once and received two raises. The company expects its employees to follow the typical policies regarding punctuality, stealing, morality and ethics, as well as general guidelines surrounding dress code, internet and email use, ownership of copyright and design and the use of the company facilities. Smith Professional Services has been in business for approximately 50 years and has reasonably stable job security. You make a salary of $47,584 per year, while having to work only minimal overtime. The current work you do consists of communicating with others, maintaining a professional disposition, facilitating team performance, and completing tasks that are relevant to your position. Occasionally you will receive special projects to do, which will allow you to complete assignments of varying difficulty. You also train and guide new subordinates, although the bulk of your time is spent conducting projects. You are undoubtedly skilled at your job and show promise for a successful future. You receive a sufficient amount of praise for the work you do and are respected by the subordinates that you train. You are in a second level position that consists of a unique series of five promotional positions. You consider conditions of the workplace to be ok; in other words, you do not work in a state of the art facility, but it suits your needs. A typical day consists of working a shift from nine am to five-thirty pm, in which you have a ten-minute commute. You work in your own beige colored cubicle for some of the day; although you may travel a fair amount depending on the project you are working on. You always have something to do and you are given a moderate amount of independence and input in the decision-making process, allowing you to suggest which projects you want to participate in. If you do not complete projects on time then you are granted extensions if you have an acceptable justification. Your unit consists of about eleven people and you get along with most of them. There is one person in particular that seems to annoy you, but you are able to interact with them professionally and cordially. Your immediate supervisor’s name is Pat. He has been your manager for approximately one year and three months and is a reasonably competent upper level manager. He has just announced a project in which he will need four people to participate. He is asking for knowledgeable volunteers to help complete the project so that he can then designate work for it. If he does not receive four volunteers, then he will request people to work on it. He will also give the project team guidance and will take suggestions as to exactly what, when, and how the project should be completed. Therefore, not all of the project responsibility will fall on one person; instead the people working on it can discuss which parts of the assignment are best suited for each project team member so that each person contributes in their specialty area. Then Pat will assign each project team member a specific assignment to complete. Having known Pat for a while now, you have noticed 92 that Pat always runs his department this way even if this falls out of his area of expertise. Pat tends to handle complaints, problems, training, praise, and punishment in the same flexible and creative way. He always tries to make decisions with team input, so that it is in line with the team or organization’s interest. One day you are sitting at your desk thinking about your current position. Reflecting back on the last two and a half years, you begin to think about your job. Knowing what you ideally want within an organization, you start comparing your current job to your ideal job. In order to assess how satisfied you are with your current job you decide to take some satisfaction inventories. Remembering to read all instructions carefully and answer the items thoughtfully, you begin to complete the inventories starting on the following page. 93 APPENDIX D Demographic & Organizational Questions Demographic Questions Please fill out the following demographic information. It is strictly confidential and will not be used for purposes other than for research purposes. 1.) Age ____ 2.) Gender: ____ Male ____ Female 3.) Ethnic Background: _____ American Indian, Eskimo, or Aleut _____ Asian or Pacific Islander _____ Black or African American _____ Hispanic, Latino, or of Spanish Origin _____ Middle Eastern _____ White/European American _____ Multi-Ethnic, specify: _______________ _____ Other, specify: _____________________ 4.) Level of Education: _____ Freshman _____ Sophomore _____ Junior _____ Senior _____ Graduate 5.) _____ Cumulative GPA 6.) Major(s) ____________________________ 7.) Minor(s) ____________________________ 94 Organizational Questions Please fill out the next questions in response to the situation you read. 1.) What are the top THREE reasons you would stay at your present job? (Please circle the top THREE that apply) Salary Interesting work Benefits Enjoy my coworkers Sense of purpose/mission Location is convenient Challenging job assignments Flexibility in work hours Good boss Work assignments vary Feel appreciated for what I do Education and development No time to look for new job Career opportunities Autonomy Perks (car, club membership, etc.) 2.) What are the top THREE reasons you would leave your present employer for another? (Please circle the top THREE that apply) Salary Career advancement Challenging and interesting work Better managers/better managed place to work Work/life balance Higher calling/purpose/mission Better benefits Low morale Location/commute Education and development opportunities Unfair treatment Company in financial jeopardy More time off Job security 3.) How would you rate the supervisor you work for? (please circle ONE choice) a.) Exceptional (100% positive) b.) Very Good (75% positive) c.) Average (50% positive) d.) Poor (25% positive) e.) Very Poor (0% positive) 95 4.) How important is feeling appreciated for your work by your coworkers and supervisors? (please circle ONE choice) a.) Highly Important (100% important) b.) Important (75% important) c.) Both Important and Not Important (50% Important) d.) Not Highly Important (25% Important) e.) Not Important (0% Important) 5.) Are you presently considering leaving your job for another? (Please circle ONE choice) a.) Strongly Considering (100% that I am considering leaving) b.) Considering (75% that I am considering leaving) c.) Both considering and not considering (50% that I am considering leaving) d.) Not Strongly Considering (25% that I am considering leaving) e.) Not Considering (0% that I am considering leaving) 6.) How confident are you that you can find a better job elsewhere?(Please circle ONE choice) a.) Highly Confident (100% confident) b.) Very Confident (75% confident) c.) Confident (50% confident) d.) Somewhat Confident (25% confident) e.) Not Confident (0% confident) 7.) Please recall the sex of your supervisor without referring back to the situation you read. (Please circle ONE choice) a.) Female b.) Male c.) I don’t know 8.) Please recall your supervisor without referring back to the situation you read. Please pick the statement which best describes your supervisor’s leadership. (Please circle ONE choice) a.) The supervisor makes decisions independently. b.) The supervisor proposes decisions to the team, listens to their feedback and then decides on decisions. c.) The supervisor has the team propose decisions and then the supervisor makes the final decisions. d.) The supervisor makes joint decisions with the team. e.) The supervisor gives full delegation of decisions to the team. 96 9.) Please recall your supervisor without referring back to the situation you read. Pick the statement, which most accurately describes the supervision you received (Please circle ONE choice). a.) The supervisor always includes their employees in the decision making process. b.) The supervisor usually includes their employees in the decision making process c.) The supervisor sometimes includes their employees in the decision making process. d.) The supervisor rarely includes their employees in the decision making process. e.) The supervisor never includes their employees in the decision making process. Thank you for your participation. 97 REFERENCES Alarcon, G., Eschleman, K.J., Bowling, N.A. (2009). Relationships between personality variables and burnout: A meta-analysis. Work & Stress, 23(3), 244-263. Archer, J. & Lloyd, B. (1982). Sex and Gender. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Balzer, W. K., Kihm, J. A., Smith, P. C., Irwin, J. L., Bachiochi, P. D., Robie, C., Sinar, E. F., & Parra, L. F. (1997). Users' manual for the Job Descriptive Index (JDI; 1997 Revision) and the Job In General scales. Bowling Green, OH: Bowling Green State University. Bass, B.M. (1990). Bass & Stogdill’s handbook of leadership: Theory, research & managerial applications (rev. ed.). New York: Free Press. Blau, G.J. (1987). Locus of control as a potential moderator of the turnover process. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 60, 21-29. Catalyst (2002). 2002 Catalyst Census of women corporate officers and top earners in the Fortune 500. Retrieved April 26, 2010 from http://www.catalyst.org/publication/174/2002-catalyst-census-of-women corporate-officers-and-top-earners-of-the-fortune-500 Carroll, J. (2006). Americans prefer male boss to a female boss. Gallup News Service, Retrieved April 26, 2010 from http://www.gallup.com/poll/24346/americans-prefer-male-boss-female-boss.aspx 98 Cellar, D.F., Sidle, S., Goudy, K., O’Brien, D. (2001). Effects of leader style, leader sex, and subordinate personality on leader evaluations and future subordinate motivation. Journal of Business and Psychology, 16(1) 61-72. Chetwynd, J. & Hartnett, O. (1978). The sex role system: psychological and sociological perspectives. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul Ltd. Davidson, H. K., & Burke, M. J. (2000). A meta-analysis of sex discrimination in simulated selection contexts. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 56, 225-248. Deaux, K. & LaFrance, M. (1998). Gender. In D.Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds), Handbook of Social Psychology (4th ed.). New York: Random House. Denmark, F.L. (1977). Styles of leadership. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 2(2), 99113. Denmark, F.L. (1993). Women, leadership, and empowerment. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 17, 343-356. Duttweiler, P. (1984). The Internal Control Index: A newly developed measure of locus of control. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 44(2), 209-21. Eagly, A. (1987) Sex differences in social behavior: A social-role interpretation, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Eagly, A.H. (2007). Female leadership advantage and disadvantage: Resolving the contradictions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 31, 1-12. 99 Eagly, A. H. & Diekman, A. B. (2005). What is the problem? Prejudice as an attitude-incontext. In J. F. Dovidio, P. Glick, & L. A. Rudman (Eds.), On the nature of prejudice: Fifty years after Allport. (pp. 19-35). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing. Eagly, A. H., & Johnson, B. T. (1990). Gender and leadership style: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 233-256. Eagly, A. H. & Johannesen-Schmidt, M. C. (2001). The leadership styles of women and men. Journal of Social Issues, 57(4), 781-797. Eagly, A. H., Johannesen-Schmidt, M. C., & Van Engen, M. L. (2003). Transformational, transactional, and laissez-faire leadership styles: A meta-analysis comparing women and men. Psychological Bulletin, 129(4), 569-591. Eagly, A.H., & Karau, S.J. (1991). Gender and the emergence of leaders: A metaanalysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(5), 685-710. Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109, 573-598. Eagly, A.H., Karau, S.J., & Makhijani, M.G. (1995). Gender and the effectiveness of leaders; A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 117(1), 125-145. Eagly, A.H., Makhijani, M.G., & Klonsky, B.G. (1992). Gender and the evaluation of leaders: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 322. Eagly, A.H., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences in human behavior: Evolved disposition versus social roles. American Psychologist, 54(6), 408-423. 100 Emrick, A. (2006). The effects of single-sex student leadership positions on leadership style used, perceptions of effective leadership, self-efficacy, domain identification, intent to lead in the future, and perceptions and evaluations of leaders. Unpublished thesis, University of Richmond, Virginia. Epitropaki, O. & Martin, R. (2004). Implicit leadership theories in applied settings: Factor structure, generalizability and stability over time. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 293-310. Fiedler, F.E. (1967) A theory of leadership effectiveness, New York: McGraw-Hill. Freeman, J. (2008). We will be heard: Women’s struggles for political power in the United States. Lanham, Maryland: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers, Inc. Foels, R., Driskell, J.E., Mullen, B., & Salas, E. (2000). The effects of democratic leadership on group member satisfaction: An integration. Small Group Research, 3(6), 676-701. Garcia-Retamero, R. Lopez-Zafra, E. (2006). Prejudice against women in malecongenial environments: Perceptions of gender role congruity in leadership. Sex Roles, 55, 51-61. Greenberger, D.B., Strasser, S., & Cummings, L. (1989). The impact of personal control on performance and satisfaction. Organization Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 43, 29-51. Guadagno, R. E., & Cialdini, R. B. (2007). Gender differences in impression management in organizations: A qualitative review. Sex Roles, 56, 483-494. 101 Harris, M. (1993). The evolution of gender hierarchies: a trial formulation, in Miller, D. (Eds), Sex and Gender Hierarchies, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Huang, H. (2006). Understanding culinary arts workers: Locus of control, job satisfaction and turnover intention. Journal of Foodservice Business Research, 9(2/3), 151-168. Judge, T.A., & Bono, J.E. (2001). Relationship of core self-evaluations traits—selfesteem, generalized self-efficacy, locus of control, and emotional stability—with job satisfaction and job performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(1), 80-92. Kagitcibasi, C. (1997). Individualism and collectivism. In J. W. Berry, M. H. Segall, & C. Kagitcibasi (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology: Social and behavior applications (pp.1-49). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Kileen, L. A., Lopez-Zaria, E., & Eagly, A. (2006). Envisioning oneself as a leader: Comparisons of women and men in Spain and the United States. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 30(3), 312-322. Kim, U., Triandis, H. C., Kagitcibasi, C., Choi, S-C., & Yoon, G. (1994). Introduction. In U. Kim, H. C. Triandis, C. Kagitcibasi, S-C. Choi, & G. Yoon (Eds.), Individualism and collectivism: Theory, method, and applications (pp.1-16). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Korabik, K., Baril, G.L., & Watson, C. (1993). Mangers’ conflict management style and leadership effectiveness: The moderating effects of gender. Sex Roles, 29, 405420. 102 Kushell, E. & Newton, R. (1986). Gender, leadership style, and subordinate satisfaction: An experiment. Sex Roles, 14, 203-209. Lefcourt, H.M. (1992). Durability and impact of the locus of control construct. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 411–414. Lengermann, P.M. & Wallace, R.A. (1985). Gender in America: Social control and social change. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, Inc. Looney, J. D. (2004). Military leadership evaluations: Effects of sex, leadership style and gender-role attitudes. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Arizona State University, Arizona. Lord, R.G., Foti, R.J., & Phillips, J.S. (1982). A theory of leadership categorization. In J. G. Hunt, U. Sekaran, & C. Schriesheim (Eds.), Leadership: Beyond establishment views. Carbondale, Il.: Southern Illinois University Press. Luthar., H.K. (1996). Gender differences in evaluation of performance and leadership ability: Autocratic vs. democratic managers. Sex Roles, 35, 337-361. Merrick, B. G. (2002). The ethics of hiring in the new workplace: Men and women managers face changing stereotypes discover correlative patterns for success. Competitiveness Review, 12(1), 94 – 115. Meyers, L. S., & Wong, D.T. (1988). Validation of a new test of locus of control: The Internal Control Index. Education and Psychological Measurement, 48, 753-761. Muhonen, T., & Torkelson E. (2004). Work locus of control and its relationship to health and job satisfaction from a gender perspective. Stress and Health, 20(1), 21-28. 103 Naswall, K., Sverke, M., & Hellgren, J. (2005). The moderating role of personality characteristics on the relationship between job insecurity and strain. Work & Stress, 19(1), 37-49. Newton, T.J., & Keenan, A. (1990). The moderating effect of the Type A behavior pattern and locus of control upon the relationship between change in job demands and change in psychological strain. Human Relations, 43(12), 1229-1255. Nicolaou-Smokoviti, L. (2004). Business leaders’ work environment and leadership styles. Current Sociology, 52(3), 407-427. Nielson, J.M. (1978). Sex in society: Perspectives on stratification. Belmont: Wadsworth Publishing Company, Inc. Oliver, J. E., Jose, P.E., & Brough, P. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis of the work locus of control scale. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 66(5), 835851. Pines, A.M., Dahan-Kalev, H. & Ronen, S. (2001). The influence of feminist selfdefinition on the democratic attitudes of managers. Social Behavior and Personality, 29(6), 607-616. Ptak, S.E., Roeder, A.D., Stephens, M., Gilad, Y., & Paabo, S. (2004). A DNA recombination “hotspot” in humans is missing in chimps: Absence of the TAP2 human recombination hotspot in chimpanzees. Public Library of Science, Retrieved April 4, 2010 from http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.002 0192 104 Riger, S., & Galligan, P. (1980). Women in management: An exploration of competing paradigms. American Psychologist, 35, 902-910. Rosenberg, B.G., & Sutton-Smith, B. (1972). Sex and identity. New York, New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, Inc. Salazar, J., Pfaffenberg, C., & Salazar, L. (2006). Locus of control vs. employee empowerment and the relationship with hotel managers’ job satisfaction. Journal of Human Resources in Hospitality and Tourism 5(1), 1-15. Schein, V. E. (1973). The relationship between sex role stereotypes and requisite management characteristics. Journal of Applied Psychology, 57, 95-100. Schmitt, N., & Chan, D. (1998). Personnel selection: A theoretical approach; foundations for organizational science. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications. Sczesny, S, Bosak, J, Neff, D, Schyns, B. 2004. Gender stereotypes and leadership. Sex Roles, 51, 631-645. Sidani, Y.M. & Gardner, W.L. (2000). Work values among Lebanese workers. The Journal of Social Psychology, 140(5), 597-607. Spector, P. (1982). Behavior in organizations as a function of employee’s locus of control. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 482-497. Stokes, J. Riger, S., & Sullivan, M. (1995). Measuring perceptions of the working environment for women in corporate settings. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 19, 533-549. Storkey, E. (2001). Origins of difference: The gender debate revisited. Grand Rapids, Michigan: Baker Academic. 105 Tannen, D. (1990). Gender differences in topical coherence: Creating involvement in best friends’ talk. Discourse Processes, 13, 73-90. Tannenbaum, R. & Schmidt, W. (1973). How to choose a leadership pattern. Harvard Business Review, 6, 73. Thomas, J.D. (1992). A comparative analysis of the leadership styles of head football coaches from traditionally black and white universities and colleges. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Missouri, Columbia. U.S. Census Bureau: Housing and Household Economic Statistics Division (2006). Twoyear-average median household income by state: 2004-2006. Retrieved October 2, 2007 from http://www.census.gov/hhes/www/income/income06/statemhi2.html U.S. Census Bureau: Population Division, Population Analysis & Evaluation Staff (1990). Most common surnames in the U.S. Retrieved October 2, 2007 from http://names.mongabay.com/most_common_surnames.htm Van Vugt, M., Jepson, S.F., Hart, C.M., & De Cremer, D. (2004). Autocratic leadership in social dilemmas: A threat to group stability. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40 (1), 1-13. Vianello, M. & Siemienska, R. (1990). Gender equality: A comparative study of discrimination and participation. London: Sage Publications Ltd. Weiss, D. J., Dawis, R.V., England, G.W., & Lofquist, L.H. (1967). Manual for the Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire. Minnesota Studies in Vocational Rehabilitation (Whole No. XXII). 106 Whicker, M. L., & Kronenfeld, J.J. (1986). Sex role changes: Technology, politics, and policy. New York, New York: Praeger Publishers. Wood, W. & Eagly, A.H. (2002). A cross-cultural analysis of the behavior of women and men: Implications for the origins of sex differences. Psychological Bulletin, 128(5), 699-727. Yoder, J.D. (2001). Making leadership work more effectively for women. Journal of Social Issues, 57(4), 815-828.