THE LOCAL LADDER EFFECT: SOCIAL STATUS AND SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING Cameron Anderson Michael E. Kraus Adam D. Galinsky Dacher Keltner INCOME WEAKLY INFLUENCES SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING Within countries, income and subjective well-being (SWB) correlate weakly, r = .15 (e.g., Alston , Lowe, & Wrigley, 1974; Andrews & Withey, 1976; Bor tner & Hultsch, 1970; Clark & Oswald, 1994; Clemente & Sauer, 1976; Diener, Haring, Stock , & Okun, 1984; Diener, Sandvik , Seidlitz & Diener, 1993; Freudiger, 1980; Horowitz , & Emmons, 1985; Inglehar t, 1990; Kimmel, Price, & Walker, 1978; Mancini & Or thner, 1980; Myer s & Diener 1985; Nicker son et al., 2003; Riddick , 1980; Veenhoven , 1994) When national income increases over time, SWB does not (Easterlin Paradox) STATUS: AN EMPT Y PURSUIT? Valuing money => depression and anxiety 1 Power-oriented individuals lower in well -being 2 Implication: Achieving status does little for SWB 1 2 Kasser & Ryan, 1993; Nickerson et al., 2003 Emmons, 1991; Sheldon et al., 2007 SOCIOMETRIC STATUS Sociometric status: Respect and admiration in others’ eyes 1 It is “local,” or defined in one’s face-to-face groups It is peer-determined, involving others’ respect and admiration Sociometric status hierarchies emerge in all kinds of face -toface groups 2 Sociometric status (SMS) is empirically distinct from socioeconomic status (SES; income, education) People affiliate with others of similar SES 3 Differences in SMS emerge among individuals with same SES 4 1 2 3 4 Berger, Cohen, & Zelditch, 1972; Blau, 1964 Bernstein, 1981; Davis & Moore, 1945; Eibl-Eibesfelt, 1989; Hogan, 1983; Leavitt, 2005; Mazur, 1973; Schjelderup-Ebbe, 1935; Tannenbaum et al., 1974; Van Vugt et al., 2008 McPherson & Smith-Lovin, 1987 Blau, 1964; Roethlesberger & Dickson, 1939; Whyte, 1943 THE LOCAL LADDER EFFECT Relative, immediate comparisons matter more than absolute, distant comparisons 1 “Beggars do not envy millionaires, though of course they will envy other beggars who are more successful” – Bertrand Russell, 1930 Sociometric status leads to power 2 , and the sense of power increases happiness 3 Sociometric status leads to social acceptance 4 , which also increases happiness 5 Festinger, 1954 Berger, Cohen, & Zelditch, 1972 3 Keltner, Gruenfeld, & Anderson, 2003 4 Thibault & Kelley, 1959 5 Baumeister & Leary, 1995 1 2 HYPOTHESES Local Ladder Effect: Sociometric status will shape SWB The effect of sociometric status on SWB will be stronger than that of SES OVERVIEW OF STUDIES Study 1: College student groups, clubs, associations Study 2: National sample; mediation Study 3: Experimental manipulation Study 4: Longitudinal assessment of changes in status over time STUDY 1: METHODS 80 members of 12 student groups (fraternities, sororities, committees, clubs, ROTC, etc.) 53% men, 47% women Sociometric status Peer-ratings of respect, admiration, looked up to (α = .71) Self-ratings of status along same dimensions (r = .54, α = .93) Number of leadership positions (M = 1.71, SD = 1.56) ROUND-ROBIN DESIGN (KENNY & LA VOIE, 1984) Targets Raters Amy Bob Candace Dave Amy 7 2 5 7 Bob 4 3 4 6 Candace 5 3 5 7 Dave 6 2 4 6 .42 -2.25 -.25 2.08 Status: STUDY 1: METHODS 80 members of 12 student groups (fraternities, sororities, school committees, student clubs, ROTC) 53% men, 47% women Sociometric status Peer-ratings of respect, admiration, looked up to (α = .71) Self-ratings of status along same dimensions (r = .54, α = .93) Number of leadership positions (M = 1.71, SD = 1.56) Total household income (Adler et al., 2000) (a) under $15K, (b) $15–$25K, (c) $25–$35K, (d) $35–$50K, (e) $50–$75K, (f) $75–$100K, (g) $100-$150K, and (h) over $150K M = 6.17, SD = 1.44 Average household income between $75,000 and $100,000 STUDY 1: METHODS Subjective well-being: 3 components (Diener et al., 1999) Satisfaction with life scale (SWLS; Diener et al., 1985) M = 5.38, SD = .94, α = .77 Positive and Negative Affect (PANAS; Watson et al., 1988) Positive affect: M = 3.84, SD = .72, α = .89 Negative affect: M = 1.80, SD = .53, α = .83 Control for gender, ethnicity (White/non -White) SWB RESULTS 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 -0.1 -0.2 -0.3 -0.4 Sociometric status: β=.35**, B=.33, SE=.10 Socioeconomic status: β=.02, B=.01, SE=.06 Low High Socioeconomic Status Sociometric Status Sociometric status predicted SWB more strongly than SES (Cohen et al., 2003), F(1,78)=14.15, p<.001 GENDER DIFFERENCES? Several theorists have argued that men and women dif fer in the way they think about and are motivated by status (Buss, 1999; Hoyenga, 1993; Sidanius, Pratto, & Bobo, 1994) Men care about status more than women (Buss, 1999, p. 43) Does sociometric status matter more for men’s SWB than for women’s? No: For men, r = .40**, and for women, r = .38** STUDY 1 FINDINGS Sociometric status predicted SWB (A Local Ladder Ef fect) The relationship between sociometric status and SWB was stronger than that between SES and SWB The ef fect of sociometric status held up for men and women STUDY 2 Mediation: Why does sociometric status matter? Sense of power, social acceptance Focused on groups that participants chose as most important to them Better gauge of the importance of sociometric status More representative sample Is personality a third variable? Control for extraversion Measure other major component of SES: education STUDY 2: METHODS 315 participants (36% men, 64% women) from national sample Amazon.com’s Mechanical Turk: Reliable and more diverse than college samples (Buhrmeister, Kwang, & Gosling, 2010) Average age = 32.8, SD = 11.0 Sociometric status “List your three most important groups (e.g., friends, family, athletic team, work group),” rate status in each (same items as in Study 1) M = 5.16, SD = .93, α = .94 Socioeconomic status Household income: M = 4.12 ($35,001 - $50,000), SD = 1.94 Education: M = 2.66 (between high school and some college), SD = . 75 STUDY 2: METHODS Subjective well-being Satisfaction with life scale: M = 4.29, SD = 1.47, α = .92 Positive affect: M = 3.38, SD = .78, α = .90 Negative affect: M = 2.08, SD = .81, α = .91 Control for gender, ethnicity (White/non - White) Extraversion (BFI; John & Srivastava, 1999) M = 3.01, SD = .82, α = .96 Sense of power in each group (Anderson et al. , in press) e.g., “If I want to, I get to make the decisions” M = 4.82, SD = .75, α = .90 Social acceptance in each group (Lear y et al., 1995) accepted, included, liked, welcomed M = 5.80, SD = .79, α = .96 RESULTS 0.6 Sociometric status: β=.50**, B=.43, SE=.04 SWB 0.4 0.2 0 Socioeconomic status: β=.08, B=.07, SE=.05 -0.2 -0.4 -0.6 Low High Socioeconomic Status Sociometric Status Sociometric status predicted SWB more strongly than SES (Cohen et al., 2003), F(1,313)=14.13, p<.001. SMS: MEDIATION Sense of Power .55** (.33**) .57** Sociometric Status .65** Sobel z = 4.90, p < .01 .50** (.10) Social Acceptance Subjective Well-Being .59** (.39**) Sobel z = 5.89, p < .01 SES: NO EFFECTS ON SENSE OF POWER, SOCIAL ACCEPTANCE Sense of Power .55** (.56**) -.09 Socioeconomic Status -.06 .10+ (.14) Social Acceptance Subjective Well-Being .59** (.60**) STUDY 2 FINDINGS Sociometric status predicted SWB (more strongly than SES) Sense of power, social acceptance mediated the link between sociometric status and SWB More representative sample with wider range in SES The results held up even after controlling for extraversion The results held up for men (r=.54) and women (r=.48) STUDY 3: CAUSATION 228 participants (38% men, 62% women) via MTurk 2X2 between-subjects design: Status type (socioeconomic vs. sociometric) Status level (low vs. high) Manipulated subjective sense of SES and SMS Imagine-an-interaction procedure (Kraus et al., 2010) e.g., Low SES: “Imagine interacting with someone high in SES” Builds from dominance complementarity principle (Horowitz et al., 2006; Tiedens, Unzueta, & Young, 2007) SOCIOECONOMIC STATUS MANIPULATION “Think of the ladder above as representing where people stand in the United States . Now please compare your self to the people at the ver y bottom (top) rung of the ladder. At the bottom (top) of the ladder are the people with the least (most) money, education, and wor st (best) jobs. In par ticular, we'd like you to COMPARE YOURSELF TO THESE PEOPLE. Now imagine yourself in a getting acquainted interaction with one of these people. Think about how the SIMILARITIES AND DIFFERENCES BET WEEN YOU might impact what you would talk about, how the interaction is likely to go, and what you and the other per son might say to each other. Please write a brief description about how you think this interaction would go.” Blue = high socioeconomic condition Red = low socioeconomic condition SOCIOMETRIC STATUS MANIPULATION “Think of the ladder above as representing where people stand in the impor tant groups to which they belong. For example, these can include their groups of friends, family, work group, etc. Now please compare your self to the people at the ver y bottom (top) rung of the ladder. These are people who have NO (A GREAT DEAL OF ) RESPECT and ADMIRATION in their impor tant social groups. In par ticular, we'd like you to COMPARE YOURSELF TO THESE PEOPLE in terms of your own respect and admiration in your impor tant groups. Now imagine your self in a getting acquainted interaction with one of these people. Think about how the SIMILARITIES AND DIFFERENCES BET WEEN YOU might impact what you would talk about, how the interaction is likely to go, and what you and the other per son might say to each other. Please write a brief description about how you think this interaction would go.” Blue = high sociometric condition Red = low sociometri c condition STUDY 3: METHODS Subjective well-being Satisfaction with life scale: M = 4.28, SD = 1.45, α = .91 Positive affect: M = 2.92, SD = .83, α = .91 Negative affect: M = 1.56, SD = .73, α = .91 MANIPULATION CHECK: “WHERE WOULD YOU PLACE YOURSELF ON THIS LADDER?” Sociometric status Socioeconomic status 10 10 9 9 8 8 7 6 5 6.45 7 6 5.12 6 5.27 5 4 4 3 3 2 2 1 1 t (115) = 3.65, p < .01 t (109) = 2.06, p < .05 No interaction: F(1,224) = 1.38, p = .24 SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING Sociometric status Socioeconomic status 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 -0.1 -0.04 -0.03 -0.2 -0.3 t (115) = 3.05, p < .01 -0.4 t (109) = .06, n.s. Interaction: F(1,224) = 4.73, p < .05 STUDY 3 FINDINGS Sociometric status more strongly af fected SWB than SES Experimental methods helped establish causality The ef fect again held up across both men and women (Interaction F[1 ,111] = 2.98 n.s.) STUDY 4 When status rises or falls after a significant life transition, does SWB rise or fall accordingly? In a longitudinal design MBA students were assessed: One month before they graduated Nine months after graduation when they had entered the workforce Graduating involves moving from one sociometric status hierarchy to another. Such a move could involve an increase or decrease in sociometric status. STUDY 4: LONGITUDINAL CHANGES IN SMS Time 1: April 2010 Time 2: February 2011 156 2 nd -year MBA students 116 (74% return rate), 71% men, 29% women Sociometric status in MBA cohort (α = .94) Sociometric status in workplace (α = .94) Income (M = 4.89, SD = 2.82), $35,001-$50,000 Income (M = 6.89, SD = 1.46), $75,001-$100,000 SWB: SWLS, PANAS (all α’s > .85) SWB: SWLS, PANAS (all α’s > .86) RESULTS Time 1: April 2010 Time 2: February 2011 Sociometric Status in MBA cohort Sociometric Status in workplace .46** Subjective Well-Being .00 .44** .63** (.40**) Subjective Well-Being Difference in sociometric status predicted difference in SWB (β=.22, p<.05). RESULTS Time 1: April 2010 Time 2: February 2011 SES SES -.12 Subjective Well-Being -.19* .68** .01 (.11) Subjective Well-Being Changes in sociometric status more strongly affected SWB, F(1,114)=20.17, p<.01. STUDY 4 FINDINGS Changes in sociometric status predicted changes in SWB As sociometric status rose or fell, so did SWB Changes in sociometric status more strongly predicted changes in SWB than did changes in SES SUMMARY Local Ladder Ef fect: Sociometric status predicted SWB (average β across correlational studies = .49) Effect emerged in correlational, longitudinal, experimental designs Consistent across men and women Held up after controlling for demographic variables, personality (extraversion) Ef fect of sociometric status consistently stronger than SES Two underlying mechanisms: Sense of power Social acceptance IMPLICATIONS Status does matter to subjective well -being But not all forms of status matter equally Possessing higher social standing might have dif ferent psychological consequences that striving for higher standing (Nickerson et al., 2003) Organizations that can raise average levels of sociometric status might promote job performance, satisfaction (Cohn, 1979; Near, Rice, & Hunt, 1978; Weaver, 1978 ) FUTURE DIRECTIONS Why does SMS af fect happiness more than SES? Do people not adapt to high/low SMS, as they adapt to money? SES may not lead to happiness because striving for it involves behaviors that detract from happiness. Striving for SMS involves behaviors that contribute to happiness (e.g., generosity). Does the impact of SMS on happiness depend on its source? What determines one’s “local ladder” ?