CAUSES, REASONS AND INTENT: INTEGRATING ATTRIBUTION AND SOCIAL INFERENCE A Thesis Presented to the faculty of the Department of Psychology California State University, Sacramento Submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS in Psychology by Elizabeth Redford SPRING 2013 © 2013 Elizabeth Redford ALL RIGHTS RESERVED ii CAUSES, REASONS AND INTENT: INTEGRATING ATTRIBUTION AND SOCIAL INFERENCE A Thesis by Elizabeth Redford Approved by: __________________________________, Committee Chair Lisa Harrison, Ph.D. __________________________________, Second Reader Larry Meyers, Ph.D. __________________________________, Third Reader Rebecca Cameron, Ph.D. ____________________________ Date iii Student: Elizabeth Redford I certify that this student has met the requirements for format contained in the University format manual, and that this thesis is suitable for shelving in the Library and credit is to be awarded for the thesis. _____________________________, Graduate Coordinator ___________________ JianJian Qin, Ph.D. Date Department of Psychology iv Abstract of CAUSES, REASONS AND INTENT: INTEGRATING ATTRIBUTION AND SOCIAL INFERENCE by Elizabeth Redford The current study investigated whether and which conditions of causation contribute to perceptions of intent. The study used a 2 (locus: internal or external) x 2 (stability: stable or unstable) x 2 (controllability: controllable or noncontrollable) x 2 (performance: success or failure) between-groups design. The independent variables were manipulated by vignettes, which described the causes of a woman’s performance at a job. The dependent variables were participants’ (N = 308) inferences of the woman’s intent to succeed, intent to succeed in the future, probability of future success, Conscientiousness, and Need for Achievement. Performance of a 4-way MANOVA indicated support for 3 of 18 hypotheses. The results suggest which conditions lead to perceptions of behavior as guided by intent, and has implications for perceived causal powers. ____________________________, Committee Chair Lisa Harrison , Ph.D. _______________________ Date v ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Thanks to my thesis committee: Dr. Harrison, Dr. Meyers, and Dr. Cameron; to Gali Lavee and Sarah Leavitt for their work on data collection for this study; and to the limitless support of my family and friends. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS Page Acknowledgements ..................................................................................................... vi List of Tables ............................................................................................................... ix List of Figures ............................................................................................................... x Chapter 1. INTRODUCTION ................................................................................………… 1 Attribution Theory ...........................................................................………… 1 Motivation and Intent ....................................................................................... 4 Explanatory Modes and Stances ...................................................................... 5 Psychological Models of Action ...................................................................... 7 The Current Study .......................................................................................... 11 2. METHOD ............................................................................................................. 16 Participants ...................................................................................................... 16 Materials ......................................................................................................... 16 Procedure ........................................................................................................ 20 3. RESULTS ............................................................................................................. 21 Multivariate Tests .......................................................................................... 23 4. DISCUSSION ....................................................................................................... 34 Explanation of Current Findings ..................................................................... 36 Limitations ...................................................................................................... 37 Future Directions ............................................................................................ 38 vii Appendix A. Vignettes ............................................................................................. 41 Appendix B. Intent and Outcome Measures ............................................................. 50 Appendix C. Need for Achievement Scale .............................................................. 51 Appendix D. Big Five Inventory for Conscientiousness Measure ………………... 53 Appendix E. Post-Research Questionnaire .............................................................. 54 References ................................................................................................................... 55 viii LIST OF TABLES Tables Page 1. Pearson Correlation Matrix for Dependent Variables ...………………………. 22 2. Inferred Initial Intent as a Function of Performance, Locus, and Controllability (means and standard deviations)………..……………………. 25 3. Inferred Need for Achievement as a Function of Performance, Locus, and Controllability (means and standard deviations)……………………….... 27 4. Inferred Conscientiousness as a Function of Performance, Locus, and Controllability (means and standard deviations)..……………………….. 29 ix LIST OF FIGURES Figures 1. Page Interaction effect of Performance x Locus on Means of Inferred Future Intent…………………………………………………...……. 31 2. Interaction Effect of Performance x Controllability on Means of Inferred Future Intent……………………………………………………..…. 31 3. Interaction Effect of Performance x Locus on Means of Participant Judgments of Probability of Future Success ………………………….…….. 33 x 1 Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION How do people explain others’ behavior? Researchers interested in this question investigate topics such as when intent and reasons are relevant to explaining others’ actions. Such investigations cover a broad area, appealing to research and theory in attribution, causal learning, social inference, moral evaluation, theory of mind and related fields. For example, studies in causal learning illuminate processes of causal reasoning that people implicitly and explicitly subscribe to. Researchers in morality or law often focus on the influence of normative concerns on ascriptions of intent. In regard to theory of mind, it is often asked how and at what developmental stage we begin to infer another person’s mental states. However, the field could benefit from more defined relationships between aspects of causal attribution and social inference. The current study was designed to address this by asking which causal conditions contribute to perceptions of intent, probability of future outcomes, and traits. Attribution Theory Attribution theory (Weiner, 1992) is one of the more prominent social psychological theories regarding how laypeople explain others’ behavior. It is generally concerned with two broad areas: ascription of traits/dispositions, and causal explanation of behavior. As this discussion will illustrate, these two areas may not necessarily be entirely distinct. 2 Heider’s (1958) work was foundational for much thinking in folk psychology, causal attribution, and social perception (Malle, 2008). Heider’s work began with perception of objects, finding that participants spontaneously attributed motives to moving shapes (Heider & Simmel, 1944). This paved the way for the conceptualization of other people as perceptual objects, leading him to develop a theoretical framework around causal attributions. Heider emphasized investigation of the layperson’s “naïve psychology” of concepts of causal attribution and social inference. In analyzing everyday concepts in terms of their underlying properties and psychological processes, Heider noted the concept of “can” to be related to power and ability. Heider also introduced the concept of “trying” as a fundamental concept to causal understanding, linking intent to personal causality, with the assumption that intent guides action. Similarly, “wanting” was linked to motivational factors underlying intent. Although Heider did not develop a thorough dimensional structure of causal attribution, his work invited such analysis. Heider noted the distinction between causes that lie within the actor or within the environment, a property of causes now commonly referred to as the “locus” dimension of attribution. Since then, other research has investigated this concept. Rotter conducted studies investigating “locus of control”, reasoning that dispositional tendencies toward internal or external control represented a personality variable (Rotter, 1966). Later, Wimer and Kelley (1982) used factor analysis to examine people’s spontaneous attributions. Allowing participants to rate causes on broad scales, factor analyses yielded “the Person” as one of five factors. Kelley (1973) 3 also identified “person” as a dimension of causality that referred to whether “some property, characteristic, or predisposition” of a person caused some action or outcome. Thus, one of the first well-established dimensions of causal attribution was locus, which refers to whether a cause is internal or external to the actor. In a reanalysis of locus of control developed by Rotter, research done by Weiner (1972) revealed that locus and control were confounded. For example, chance and experimenter intervention are both external causes of outcomes, but experimenter intervention is perceived to be under the control of someone (namely, the experimenter) more so than chance is. Weiner thus empirically disentangled control from locus, forming a second dimension of attribution: controllability. Lastly, the stability dimension of attribution concerns whether a cause varies over time. In Kelley’s (1973) original three-dimensional model, “time” is referred to as a particular set of circumstances perceived to be causal. Kelley did not explicitly distinguish between circumstances that may be expected to change and those that may not. However, later work by Wimer and Kelley (1982) identified “enduring vs transient” as one of five factors of attribution, and this dimension has since crystallized into the term “stability” (Weiner, 1985). Some time ago, Wimer and Kelley (1982) noted a lack of consensus among attribution researchers on categories or dimensions of attribution, but now the field has generally accepted those three dimensions outlined in Weiner’s (1985) work: locus, stability, and control. 4 Motivation and Intent Although Heider’s discussion of “disposition” may be interpreted as referring to personality traits, Heider in fact referred to motivational states, and was concerned with observers’ perceptions of intentions and “personal causality”. Heider distinguished between personal (intentional) and impersonal (nonintentional) causality (Heider, 1958). However, investigation of intent ascription in attribution theory has often been limited in scope to topics such as one’s own motivation to make attributions and to defend or promote one’s self-image (Kelley and Michela, 1980). In 1980, in Kelley and Michela’s Annual Review on attribution theory and research, the “Motivation” section notes the limited research available. Weiner did not resolve the question of how and whether to incorporated intent into attribution theory, and people’s everyday concepts of intent tended to be neglected in research on explanation (Malle, 2011). After calls to attention for the logical distinction between causes and reasons (e.g., Buss, 1978), more social psychologists began to apply this distinction to understanding attributional processes (Locke & Pennington, 1982). Since then, research has begun to highlight “teleological obsession” or “teleological promiscuity”--a general bias toward intentional accounts of behavior (Csibra & Gergely, 2007; Rosset, 2008). This evidence fits well with the theory of correspondent inference, which describes a general tendency to attribute behaviors to dispositions (Gilbert & Malone, 1995). Intentionality bias is also reflected in the automaticity of goal inference, in which goals and intent are the fastest and most likely inferences to be made about others (Aarts, Dijksterhuis, & Dik, 2008; Hassin, Bargh, & Uleman, 2002). 5 Explanatory Modes and Stances Psychologists define forms of explanation with terminology that parallels that used in philosophy. A paper by Lombozo (2010) outlines two “modes of explanation”. She identifies mechanistic explanations with “Aristotle’s efficient cause, Dennett’s physical stance, and Keil’s physical mode of construal” (p. 304); these are causes that physically and proximally effect a change. For example, a causal explanation of why a dropped ball falls to the ground is that gravity caused it to fall. On the other hand, Lombrozo (2010) identifies teleological explanations with “Aristotle’s final cause, Dennett’s design stance, and Keil’s teleological mode of construal” (p. 304). Reason explanations appeal to teleology: that purpose which an action is for. For example, a teleological explanation of why I dropped a ball is that I wanted to watch it bounce. Philosopher Daniel Dennett (1987) refers to a third mode that he calls the intentional stance. For the purposes of this paper, the terms “mechanistic” will be associated with “cause”, and “intent” or “intentional” with “goal-based”. In keeping with psychological literature (e.g., Keil, 1995, Lombrozo, 2010), the two relevant forms of explanation will be referred to as “modes”. Each mode of explanation focuses on certain aspects of the thing to be explained. For example, Hinkle and Schmidt (1984) found reason explanations to be used more when explaining actions than when explaining occurrences. In the same vein, Malle (1999) asserts that cause explanations are applicable to unintentional behavior and reasons to intentional behavior, successfully accounting for phenomena such as actorobserver asymmetries (Malle, Knobe, & Nelson, 2007). 6 Necessity and Sufficiency Enabling mechanistic causes and motivating teleological causes may be understood as necessary and sufficient for action causation. Necessity holds that some condition must be true in order for an outcome to obtain; the action would not occur if the cause were absent (McClure & Hilton, 1997; Wells & Gavanski, 1989). For example, to purchase something that costs $10, the actor must have $10. Otherwise, the actor will not purchase the item. Certain preconditions are perceived to be necessary to enable an action to be accomplished, and difficult actions are seen as more heavily reliant on such preconditions (McClure, Densley, Liu, & Allen, 2001). On the other hand, sufficiency holds that some condition necessitates that something obtain; the action will occur if the cause is present. For example, if the actor (having $10, satisfying the necessary condition) wants to purchase the item, the actor will necessarily purchase the item. Empirically, perceivers have been shown to prefer reason explanations when outcomes are not difficult (that is, necessary preconditions are satisfied), demonstrating an understanding about how means realize ends (Mazzone, 2011). Thus, at least where lay explainers are concerned, enabling factors (having enough money) link goals/desires (wanting something) and accomplishment of goals (e.g., buying something; e.g., see McClure at al., 2001). Thus, “causes” may be understood as enabling/necessary, and “reasons” or “intent” as sufficient for action. Preconditions are necessary to enable action, while intent is sufficient to motivate action (Boonzaier, McClure, & Sutton, 2005). 7 Psychological Models of Action Rational Agency These conceptual analyses and empirical findings indicate that perceivers view actions within a framework of rational agency, assuming that actions realize goals (Gergely & Csibra, 2003; Kashima, McKintyre, & Clifford, 1998) and holding agents to conditions of rationality, coherence and consistency. Perceivers hold impressions of themselves and others to such conditions, preferring coherent accounts of beliefs, desires and intentions (Kashima, Yoshihisa, McKintyre, & Clifford, 1998). Under the theory of rational action, reasons are composed of coherent beliefs and desires. Belief refers to the agent’s belief that accomplishment of the action will contribute to relevant desire fulfillment, corresponding to Heider’s (1958) concept of “wanting”. These beliefs and desires rationally correspond to a relevant intent and subsequent action; this corresponds to Heider’s concept of “trying”, which links intent to action. This coherent, intelligible network of goals, intentions, and actions is ascribed to the agent and explains observed behavior. Inferred or observed pieces of this network are used to sketch out other relevant components: desires, beliefs, intentions, character, skill, awareness and probable actions. For example, knowledge of an agent’s desire may serve as a premise upon which traits are inferred, and vice versa. For example, Gergely and Csibra (2003) found infants to use an implicit theory of rational action to infer goals from actions. Psychological models of this network vary in terms of the components investigated. For example, Fishbein and Ajzen’s (1975) theory of reasoned action is 8 based on the relationship between intent and action, under conditions of volitional control. Ajzen later modified this idea into the theory of planned behavior to account for the perception of volitional control, a change that did indeed boost the theory’s predictive power (Ajzen, 1985). More recent theories are more expansive and emphasize different methodological approaches, including folk psychology and automaticity. Folk Psychology Malle and Knobe (1997) have advocated for the folk psychological approach to this “network of concepts that people use to make sense of human behavior” (Malle, 2011). Based on studies regarding the folk psychology of intentionality, the authors put forth a “folk-conceptual” model in which intent takes on a central role. In an exploratory study, Malle and Knobe (1997) allowed participants to define intentionality, and coded responses into categories: awareness, desire, intention, belief, and skill. A response qualified for the “intention” category if it mentioned deciding, choosing, planning, intending, or meaning to perform an act. The results suggested that belief and desire contribute to intention, which in turn with skill and awareness, produce relevant action. This evidence offers powerful support of the theory of rational action. Sloman, Fernbach, and Ewing (2012) applied a causal modeling approach to the model specified by Malle and Knobe (1997), extending it to include character/disposition and outcome/consequence. In this model, the components are organized such that “character” contributes to “belief” that an action will result in a consequence, and a “desire” for that consequence. Consistent with folk-conceptual theory (Malle & Knobe, 1997), belief and desire form “intention” toward the relevant action. Where this theory 9 differs and extends beyond the folk-conceptual approach is in its conditions for translation of intent into action. Given awareness of doing, and the skill to execute the action, the consequence occurs. In addition, this model describes inference as operating along the connections in two ways: top-down and bottom up. A bottom-up inferential process allows one to infer, for example, intent from action or consequence, and character from intent. A top-down inferential process is presumed to be incurred to use character to infer a probable intent or action. Other researchers have discussed this bidirectional goalto-action and action-to-goal inferential process (Csibra & Gergely, 2007). Multiple inference Model Reeder’s Multiple Inference Model (MIM; 2002) brings intent back into attribution to offer a more comprehensive and integrated view of social inferences. The MIM accounts for multiple social inferences, an advantage over models that account only for single inferences (Gawronski, 2009; Molden, 2009). The general idea is that perceivers make multiple inferences and then integrate them (Reeder, Hesson-McInnis, Krohse, & Scialabba, 2001). In an investigation of what Sloman, Fernbach and Ewing (2012) would term “bottom-up” processing, Reeder (2004) studied inferences from motive to disposition. Originally interested mostly in inference of disposition, Reeder later considers both inferential paths: from trait to motive, and motive to trait. He also incorporates evidence for the occurrence of much processing in parallel rather than in a rigid sequence, and occurring either analytically or intuitively (Reeder, 2009). Reeder’s MIM has prompted the emergence of a debate as to whether these multiple inferences are made in parallel, or in some serial or hierarchical pattern that 10 could be ordered by priority or likelihood of occurrence. For example, a recent study based on response times suggested spontaneous goal inferences to be inferred faster, encoded more strongly, and reactivated more quickly than spontaneous trait inferences (Van Overwalle, Van Duynslaeger, Coomans, & Timmermans, 2012). However, results of other studies support a view that multiple social inferences occur spontaneously and in parallel (Todd, Molden, Ham, & Vonk, 2011), and that spontaneous goal inferences contribute to trait attributions (Reeder & Trafimow, 2005). These findings offer support for the idea that action is determined by intention, which is determined by coherent beliefs and desires (Kashima et al., 1998). In summary, the general structure that emerges from empirical research conforms to the rational action model. Lay explanations tend to see actors as rational agents acting with reasons (composed of mental states such as beliefs and desires), which lead to intent, culminating in the relevant intentional action. Methodological Themes Several methodologies have been successfully used to investigate people’s explanations of behavior. A major methodological paradigm involves participants’ explanatory preferences. Participants are instructed to indicate their preference for explanations that conform to one of the modes. They may choose mechanistic, enabling preconditions or reason explanations. Alternatively, participants may be allowed explanatory pluralism: they may cite both causal and reason explanations in explaining a single event or action, since both are often applicable in explaining a single event or action (Keil, 2006). Because either or both explanatory modes may be applicable to the 11 situation described in the current study, participants were not forced to subscribe to either explanatory mode. The current study instead assessed the extent to which participants deemed intent to be applicable. There are several ways that these explanatory preferences may be measured, one aspect of which concerns whether responses are structured or unstructured. While acknowledging that open-ended response formats may be useful for some purposes, Elig and Frieze (1979) offer evidence of higher convergent validity for structured response formats. Among structured response options, there is not yet consensus regarding the relative benefits of scales or dichotomous options. Some studies ask whether a behavior was intentional, offering one response in the positive and one in the negative. However, those options may not necessarily suggest total certainty. For example, Wright and Bengson (2009) offer “probably, yes” and “probably, no” as responses to the question of whether a behavior was intentional. Recent research in the area most commonly uses seven- or eight-point scales to assess level of intent (e.g., Hughes & Trafimow, 2010; Ohtsubo, 2007). In addition, studies vary with regard to whether they include more than one item to assess intentionality. There is not a clear consensus thus far on the reliability or validity provided by single or multiple item formats. Thus, the current study used a single item to measure intent, using a seven-point response scale. The Current Study Malle and Knobe’s (1997) folk-conceptual theory and the work of Sloman, Fernbach and Ewing (2012) describe relationships between intent, traits, and actions. However, these models of actor and action do not indicate how their components interact 12 with aspects of causes. The current study sought to address this by examining what aspects of causes contribute to perceptions of intent and traits. The study used a 2 x 2 x 2 x 2 between-groups design. The independent variables were locus (internal or external), stability (stable or unstable) controllability (controllable or noncontrollable, and performance (success or failure), and were manipulated by vignettes which described a woman’s performance at a job and the causes of her performance. The dependent variables of interest were participants’ assessments of the target’s initial intent to succeed (initial intent), that of the target’s intent to succeed in the future (future intent), and that of the target’s probability of future success (future success). In addition, measures of trait inference consisted of participant’s assessment of a target’s Need for Achievement (NAch) and Conscientiousness. There were 18 hypotheses for this research. Initial Intent A three-way interaction of Performance x Locus x Control on inferred initial intent is expected. The general bias to infer intent for observed behaviors should lead to an effect of performance on intent (Rosset, 2008). Indeed, data show that goals predict performance (Hulleman, Durik, Schweigert, & Harackiewicz, 2008). Previous research on intention in one’s own volitional behavior (Ajzen, 1985), and the reasoning that this pattern may extend to inferences about others, also suggests that this effect should be qualified by locus and control. In addition, perceivers have been shown to prefer reason explanations when outcomes are controllable by the actor (McClure, Densley, Liu, & Allen, 2001): a state of affairs that may be termed “personal control” or “free will”. Thus 13 participants should perceive outcomes as most intentional when they are results of internal, controllable causes, but only for successful performance. Hypothesis 1a. It is predicted that in the success condition, perceived initial intent will be greater when locus is internal and causes are controllable. This would be supported by a three-way interaction of Performance x Locus x Controllability. Hypothesis 1b. It is predicted that in the success condition, internal locus will produce higher perceived initial intent in the controllable than the noncontrollable condition. Hypothesis 1c. It is predicted that in the unsuccessful condition, for unstable causes, external locus will produce higher perceived initial intent than internal locus. Hypothesis 1d. It is predicted that in the unsuccessful condition, it is predicted that internal locus will produce higher perceived initial intent in the noncontrollable than in the controllable condition. Need for Achievement Results for NAch are expected to be similar to those for Conscientiousness. However, differences in stability may not be as likely to produce differences in the interactions of the other independent variables on NAch. Hypothesis 2a. It is predicted that in the success condition, for controllable causes, internal locus will produce higher NACh scores than external locus. Hypothesis 2b. It is predicted that in the success condition, for internal causes, NAch scores will be higher for stable causes than for unstable causes. 14 Hypothesis 2c. It is predicted that in the success condition, for internal causes, NAch scores will be higher for controllable than for noncontrollable causes. Hypothesis 2d. It is predicted that in the unsuccessful condition, for stable causes, scores will be higher for noncontrollable than for controllable causes. Conscientiousness A four-way interaction of Performance x Locus x Control x Stability on inferred Conscientiousness is expected. The correspondence bias (Gilbert & Malone, 1995) should lead participants to use information about performance to infer the woman’s traits and dispositions, especially those relevant to accomplishment such as Conscientiousness. However, when she fails due to external causes, it may be assumed that she still would have wanted to succeed, so external locus may be expected to result in higher Conscientiousness scores than internal locus. Hypothesis 3a. It is predicted that in the success condition, internal locus will produce higher scores on the Conscientiousness measure than external locus. Hypothesis 3b. It is predicted that in the success condition, Conscientiousness scores for internal locus will be higher for stable causes than for unstable causes. Hypothesis 3c. It is predicted that for the unsuccessful condition, Conscientiousness scores for external locus will be higher than for internal locus, but only for stable causes. Hypothesis 3d. It is predicted that in the unsuccessful condition, Conscientiousness scores would be higher for noncontrollable than for controllable causes, but only for stable, internal causes. 15 Future Intent Stability contributes to assessment of probability of future action (Carroll, 1978), and intention and “streakiness” are related (Caruso, Waytz & Epley, 2010). Thus, stability is expected to qualify the effects of other independent variables. Hypothesis 4a. It is predicted that in the success condition, for controllable causes, levels of inferred future intent will be higher for internal locus than for external locus. Hypothesis 4b. It is predicted that in the success condition, for external causes, levels of inferred future intent will be higher for stable causes than for unstable causes. Hypothesis 4c. It is predicted that in the unsuccessful condition, for controllable causes, external locus will produce higher levels of inferred future intent than internal locus. Future Success Stability influences expectations and perceptions of probable future outcomes (e.g., Anderson & Jennings, 1980; Weiner, 1985), contributing to assessment of probability of future action (Carroll, 1978). Hypothesis 5a. It is predicted that in the success condition, levels of inferred future success will be higher for stable than for unstable causes. Hypothesis 5b. It is predicted that in the success condition, levels of inferred future success will be higher for internal locus than for external locus. Hypothesis 5c. It is predicted that in the unsuccessful condition, levels of inferred future success will be higher for unstable causes than for stable causes. 16 Chapter 2 METHOD Participants Participants were recruited from the Psychology Department research pool of a large public university in Northern California. Participants completed the study for partial course credit for Psychology courses. A total sample of 330 was collected. This number was reduced to 308 with deletion of 20 cases in which the participants failed the manipulation check and six in which packets with condition 15 were misprinted. Of the 308 cases analyzed, 229 were female and 75 male, and ages ranged from 17 to 58 (M = 21.45, SD = 4.98). There were four cases in which participants did not specify their gender, and six cases failed to specify age. Participants were ethnically diverse; 90 (29.2%) identified their ethnicity as White/European American, twenty (6.5%) as Black/African American, 61 (20.2%) as Asian/Asian American, 67 (22.2%) as Hispanic/Latino/Latina, one as Arab/Arab American, 35 (11.4%) as other, and 28 (9.1%) identified themselves as belonging to more than one ethnic group. Six participants did not specify ethnicity. Fifteen participants identified themselves as lower class, 111 as working class, 164 as middle class, and 10 as upper class. Materials To manipulate the independent variables, fictional vignettes were constructed regarding a woman’s performance at a job and the causes of her performance. Some of the vignettes were pilot tested to ensure correspondence to the intended levels of each 17 attributional dimension (locus, stability, and controllability), as well as to provide guidance in the development of the rest of the vignettes. During the pilot testing, participants read sample versions of the vignettes, completed the Causal Dimension Scale, Revised (CDSII; McAuley, Duncan, & Russell, 1992) and answered open-ended questions concerning their impressions of the vignettes regarding what factors caused the woman’s success. The vignettes were used to manipulate performance (successful or unsuccessful) as well as each of the three attributional dimensions: locus (internal or external), stability (stable or unstable), and controllability (controllable or noncontrollable), resulting in sixteen between-group conditions in total (see Appendix A). Each of the sixteen vignettes content varied only with regard to the manipulation of the independent variables. Each opened with the same introductory information about a woman and a job she had, and then went on to describe her performance at the job, how the causes of her performance were external or internal, and whether they were controllable. Lastly, the causes of her performance are described as stable or unstable. The vignette ends with a sentence that summarizes the causes of her career outcome as external or external, controllable or noncontrollable, and stable or unstable.” Measure of intent inference and Performance Prediction Most recently published studies use a single item to explicitly measure perceived intentionality. These items often use seven- or eight-point response scales (Hughes, 2010; Ohtsubo, 2007), but alternatively may utilize as few as two response options (Wright, 2009; Knobe, 2003). Hughes (2010), for example, asked participants about intent and 18 traits with items such as “to what extent is Tom honest?” The current study adopted similar wording in developing the intent inference and performance prediction survey. Specifically, participants were asked the following questions: “To what extent did the woman intend to succeed?”, “If given another opportunity at the same job, to what extent do you believe that the woman would have the intention to succeed?” and “If given another opportunity at the same job, to what extent do you believe that the woman would actually succeed?” A seven-point response scale was used (1 = very little and 7 = very much; see Appendix B). Measure of Need for Achievement The Need for Achievement construct regards the extent to which a person strives to achieve difficult tasks, and may be described as “striving, accomplishing, [and] capable,” among other achievement-related trait adjectives. Low scores on the Need for Achievement measure correspond to a person who tends to be less ambitious and may be described as “unmotivated, indolent, [and] non-capable” (Jackson, 1984). The measure of Need for Achievement (NAch) used in the current study consisted of sixteen items, with eight reverse-scored. The researcher adapted the scale by rewording items from the original first-person format in the Personality Research Form (e.g., “I enjoy difficult work”; Jackson, 1984) to third-person format in order to reference the target (e.g., “She enjoys difficult work”; see Appendix C). Participants were instructed to respond with whether they believed the item to be true about the woman in the story, and response options were “True” and “False”. Responses of “false” were coded as 0, and “true” were coded as 1. Scores were totalled, and the total unstandardized 19 scores used for analysis in the current study. A Cronbach’s alpha of .823 was attained, indicating good reliability. Measure of Conscientiousness Conscientiousness has been identified with six facets: achievement, order, dependability, forward-planning, competitiveness, and traditionalism (Saville et al., 1984). The current study used the Big Five Inventory (BFI; Costa & McCrae, 1992b) to measure Conscientiousness, adapting the opening sentence that uses the first person into the third person to address the woman in the story (see Appendix D). The BFI consists of 44 items, with five response options (1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree). The current study used the nine items on the BFI that correspond to Conscientiousness (items three, eight, thirteen, eighteen, 23, 28, 33, 38, and 43). These items were combined to form a Conscientiousness index for use in the current study. A Cronbach’s alpha of .923 was attained, indicating good reliability. Post-research Questionnaire The post-research questionnaire (PRQ, see Appendix E) included a manipulation check and demographic questionnaire. The manipulation check was included to verify that participants had understood the information included in the vignette. It asked about the woman’s performance at the job and offered two response options: “She was generally successful” or “She was generally unsuccessful”. The PRQ also asked whether anyone other than the researcher had told the participant anything about the study before her or his participation. These parts of the PRQ was used to screen participants for having read the vignette, and for excessive knowledge of the experiment before participating. 20 The demographic questionnaire instructed participants to indicate their sex, ethnicity/race, age, and family socioeconomic status. Procedure Two undergraduate research assistants collected all data for the experiment. Both research assistants were present for the first few sessions of data collection, and each research assistant ran sessions independently thenceforth. Research assistants collected data from up to eight participants per session. Participants first read and signed consent forms that provided an orientation to the study and indicated voluntary participation. Following that, the packet of materials was provided in an envelope to the participants, who were instructed to not place their name or any other identifying mark on the materials. This packet included in the following order: one of the sixteen vignettes (the order of which was determined by a randomized block process), the intent inference and performance prediction survey, the Need for Achievement scale and the BFI (to measure Conscientiousness). The order of delivery of the NAch and BFI was counterbalanced. Finally, participants completed the PRQ. After completion, participants replaced the materials in the envelope, returned the envelope to the researcher, received a full debriefing and were given course credit for their participation. 21 Chapter 3 RESULTS No more than 5% of values were missing from any variable, but sample sizes were unequal. Hair et al. (2010) recommend cell size of 20 cases or more to ensure adequate statistical power. Thus, the current analyses conducted may be expected to be relatively conservative. In dealing with unequal cell sizes, the SPSS default method is most commonly recommended for experimental research with random dropout and equal importance of cells (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2013). Skewness and kurtosis tests indicated nonsignificant skewness and kurtosis for initial intent, future intent, future success, Conscientiousness, and NAch (all skewness and kurtosis scores ranged between .291 and -1.09). Box’s test of equality of covariance matrices was significant, indicating unequal covariance matrices, M = 421.63, F = 1.66, p = .000. However, Box’s M is notoriously powerful, and often too strict in multivariate analysis of variance situations (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2013). Levene’s statistic indicated that the assumption homogeneity of variance was violated by initial intent, F(15, 282) = 3.08, p < .001, future intent, F(15, 282) = 2.69, p = .001, and future success, F(15, 282) = 3.18, p < .001, while the Conscientiousness and NAch measures met homogeneity of variance standards (Levene’s statistic = ns). A Bonferroni adjustment would ordinarily be adopted in evaluating the univariate effects. However, given that Box’s M was statistically significant and that three of the dependent variables violated the assumption of homogeneity of variance, the following strategy was adopted. For initial intent, future 22 intent, and future success, where heterogeneity of variance was observed, an alpha level of .005 was adopted; for Conscientiousness and NAch an alpha level of .01 was used (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). Any effect not meeting the corrected alpha level but demonstrating a probability of less than .05 was treated as a trend. Some of the DV intercorrelations were higher than recommended for MANOVA (Meyers, Gamst, & Guarino, 2013), ranging from .27 to .69 (see Table 1). However, most of the intercorrelations were in the acceptable range, indicating a MANOVA was appropriate. Conscientiousness and NAch were the only two DVs measured by scales; the other DVs were single item measures. Table 1 Pearson Correlation Matrix for Dependent Variables Measure Initial intent Future intent Future success Conscientiousness Initial intent -- Future intent .68** -- Future success .56** .69** -- Conscientiousness .66** .49** .56** -- Need for Achievement .33** .27** .22** .38** ** Correlation is significant at the .01 level (2 tailed). Need for achievement -- 23 Multivariate Tests A 2 (performance: success, failure) x 2 (locus: internal, external) x 2 (stability: stable, unstable) x 2 (controllability: controllable, noncontrollable) multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was performed on the dependent variables: initial intent, future intent, future success, Conscientiousness, and Need for Achievement. The MANOVA indicated several significant effects, which are discussed below. Examination of profile plots and Bonferroni-corrected tests of simple effects revealed the nature of the effect for each dependent variable. Follow-up tests were designed to use simple effects to test specific hypotheses; the a priori hypotheses dictated the analyses. Initial Intent There were four hypotheses regarding the effects of the independent variables on initial intent. Hypothesis 1a. Hypothesis 1a predicted a three-way interaction of Performance x Locus x Controllability such that in the success condition, participants’ assessments of the woman’s level of initial intent to succeed would be significantly greater for internal than for external locus for controllable causes. The three-way multivariate effect of Performance x Locus x Controllability was significant, F(5, 278) = 3.88, p = .002. Tests of between-subjects effects were performed, which indicated that the Performance x Locus x Controllability effect was significant for initial intent, F(1, 282) = 11.17, p = .001, η2 = .02 (see Table 2). A test of simple effects revealed that scores were higher for internal than external locus for both controllable causes, F(1, 282) = 20.89, p < .001, and 24 noncontrollable causes, F(1, 282) = 4.29, p = .039, indicating that controllability did not differentiate the Locus x Performance interaction as expected. Hypothesis 1b. Hypothesis 1b predicted a three-way interaction effect of Performance x Locus x Controllability such that in the success condition, internal locus would produce higher scores in the controllable than the noncontrollable condition. The three-way multivariate effect of Performance x Locus x Controllability was significant, F(5, 278) = 3.88, p = .002, so it was followed up with a test of between-subjects effect on initial intent, which was significant, F(1, 282) = 11.17, p = .001, η2 = .02. However, the simple effect contrast of interest was nonsignificant, F (1, 282) = 2.89, p = .09. Hypothesis 1c. Hypothesis 1c predicted a three-way interaction effect of Performance x Stability x Locus such that in the unsuccessful condition, for unstable causes, external locus would produce higher initial intent scores than internal locus. However, the multivariate effect of Performance x Stability x Locus was nonsignificant, F(5, 278) = .72, p = .61. Hypothesis 1d. Hypothesis 1d predicted a three-way effect of Performance x Locus x Controllability such that in the unsuccessful condition, internal locus would produce higher initial intent scores in the noncontrollable than in the controllable condition. The three-way multivariate effect of Performance x Locus x Controllability was significant, F(5, 278) = 3.88, p = .002, so it was followed up with a test of betweensubjects effects. This indicated that the Performance x Locus x Controllability effect was significant for initial intent, F(1, 282) = 11.17, p = .001, η2 = .02. Simple effects tests 25 indicated the contrast of interest to be significant, F(1, 282) = 28.04, p < .001. Overall, for initial intent, one of four hypotheses was supported. Table 2 Inferred Initial Intent as a Function of Performance, Locus, and Controllability (means and standard deviations) Performance Successful Unsuccessful Locus Controllable Noncontrollable Controllable Noncontrollable Internal 6.26 (1.04) 5.68 (1.54) 2.02 (.96) 3.81 (1.99) External 4.70 (1.38) 4.95 (1.56) 3.80 (1.63) 4.17 (1.84) Note. Scale ranged from 1 (“very little”) – 7 (“very much”). Need for Achievement There were four hypotheses regarding the effects of the independent variables on Need for Achievement. Hypothesis 2a. Hypothesis 2a predicted a three-way interaction effect of Performance x Locus x Controllability such that in the success condition, for controllable causes, internal locus would produce higher NAch scores than external locus. The multivariate interaction effect of Performance x Locus x Controllability was significant, F(5, 278) = 3.88, p = .002, so it was followed up with between-subjects tests for NAch, for which the univariate effect of Performance x Locus x Controllability indicated a trend 26 toward significance, F(1, 282) = 6.45, p = .012, η2 = .02 (see Table 3). Tests of simple effects indicated that the hypothesized contrast was supported, F(1, 282) = 8.89, p = .003. Hypothesis 2b. Hypothesis 2b predicted a three-way interaction effect of Performance x Locus x Stability such that in the success condition, for internal causes, NAch scores will be higher for stable causes than for unstable causes. However, the multivariate effect of Performance x Locus x Stability was nonsignificant, F(5, 278) = .72, p = .60. Hypothesis 2c. Hypothesis 2c predicted a three-way interaction effect of Performance x Locus x Controllability such that in the success condition, for internal causes, NAch scores will be higher for controllable than for noncontrollable causes. The multivariate interaction effect of Performance x Locus x Controllability was significant, F(5, 278) = 3.88, p = .002, so it was followed up with between-subjects tests for NAch, for which the univariate effect of Performance x Locus x Controllability was also significant, F(1, 282) = 6.45, p = .012, η2 = .02. Tests of simple effects indicated that the hypothesized contrast was marginally significant, F(1, 282) = 3.71, p = .055. Hypothesis 2d. Hypothesis 2d predicted a two-way interaction of Performance x Controllability x Stability such that in the unsuccessful condition, for stable causes, NAch scores would be higher for noncontrollable than for controllable causes. However, the multivariate interaction effect of Performance x Controllability x Stability was nonsignificant, F(5, 278) = 1.93, p = .09. Overall, although complete statistical significance was not achieved, two of four hypotheses demonstrated trends toward significance. 27 Table 3 Inferred Need for Achievement as a Function of Performance, Locus, and Controllability (means and standard deviations) Performance Successful Unsuccessful Locus Controllable Noncontrollable Controllable Noncontrollable Internal 9.33 (3.96) 7.51 (3.59) 4.38 (4.14) 6.74 (4.20) External 6.53 (4.73) 6.76 (3.60) 6.31 (3.46) 6.11 (3.63) Note. Need for Achievement scores were indexed and ranged from 0 to 16. Conscientiousness There were four hypotheses regarding the effects of the independent variables on Conscientiousness. Hypothesis 3a. Hypothesis 3a predicted a two-way interaction effect of Performance x Locus such that in the success condition, internal locus would produce higher scores than external locus. The multivariate interaction effect of Performance x Locus was significant, F(5, 278) = 18.32, p < .001, so it was followed up with betweensubjects tests for Conscientiousness, for which the univariate effect of Performance x Locus was also significant, F(1, 282) = 62.55, p < .001, η2 = .10 (see Table 4). Tests of simple effects indicated that the hypothesized contrast was supported for both controllable causes, F(1, 282) = 27.17, p < .001 and noncontrollable causes, F(1, 282) = 10.73, p = .001. 28 Hypothesis 3b. Hypothesis 3b predicted a three-way interaction effect of Performance x Locus x Stability such that in the success condition, Conscientiousness scores for internal locus would be higher for stable causes than for unstable causes. However, the multivariate effect of Performance x Locus x Stability was nonsignificant, F(5, 278) = .721, p = .61. Hypothesis 3c. Hypothesis 3c predicted a three-way interaction effect of Performance x Locus x Stability such that for the unsuccessful condition, Conscientiousness scores for external locus would be higher than for internal locus, but only for stable causes. However, the multivariate effect of Performance x Locus x Stability was nonsignificant, F(5, 278) = .721, p = .61. Hypothesis 3d. Hypothesis 3d predicted a four-way interaction of Performance x Locus x Controllability x Stability such that in the unsuccessful condition, Conscientiousness scores would be higher for noncontrollable than for controllable causes, but only for stable, internal causes. However, the test of the four-way effect of Locus x Performance x Controllability x Stability was nonsignificant, F(5, 278) = 1.78, p = .12. Overall, one of four hypotheses was supported. 29 Table 4 Inferred Conscientiousness as a Function of Performance, Locus, and Controllability (means and standard deviations) Performance Locus Successful Unsuccessful Controllable Noncontrollable Controllable Noncontrollable Internal 1.27 (.75) 1.03 (.79) -1.04 (.58) -.51 (.75) External .40 (.78) .50 (.87) -.08 (.67) -.35 (.61) Note. Conscientiousness analyzed as z-scores. Future intent There were three hypotheses regarding the effects of the independent variables on future intent. Hypothesis 4a. Hypothesis 4a predicted a three-way interaction of Performance x Locus x Controllability such that in the success condition, for controllable causes, levels of inferred future intent would be higher for internal locus than external locus. The multivariate interaction effect of Performance x Locus x Controllability was significant, F(5, 278) = 3.88, p = .002, so it was followed up with between-subjects tests for future intent, for which the univariate effect of Performance x Locus x Controllability demonstrated a trend toward significance, F(1, 282) = 5.47, p = .02, η2 = .01. This hypothesis was supported by simple effects tests; scores were significantly greater for 30 internal causes than for external causes across levels of controllability, F(1, 282) = 15.74, p < .001. Hypothesis 4b. Hypothesis 4b predicted a three-way interaction effect of Performance x Locus x Stability such that in the success condition, for external causes, levels of inferred future intent would be higher for stable causes than for unstable causes. However, the multivariate effect of Locus x Stability was nonsignificant, F(5, 278) = .72, p = .60. Hypothesis 4c. Hypothesis 4c predicted a three-way interaction effect of Performance x Locus x Controllability such that in the unsuccessful condition, for controllable causes, external locus would produce higher levels of inferred future intent than internal locus. The multivariate interaction effect of Performance x Locus x Controllability was significant, F(5, 278) = 3.88, p = .002, so it was followed up with between-subjects tests for future intent, for which the univariate effect of Performance x Locus x Controllability demonstrated a trend toward significance, F(1, 282) = 5.47, p = .02, η2 = .01. Tests of simple effects indicated that the hypothesized contrast was marginally significant, F(1, 282) = 3.71, p = .055. Overall, although complete statistical significance was not achieved, two of three hypotheses demonstrated trends toward significance. 31 7 6 5 Internal 4 External 3 2 1 Success Failure Figure 1. Interaction effect of Performance x Locus on means of inferred future intent. Note. Standard deviations are indicated by the error bars attached to each column. 6.5 6 5.5 5 Controllable 4.5 Noncontrollable 4 3.5 3 2.5 Success Failure Figure 2. Interaction effect of Performance x Controllability on inferred future intent. Note. Standard deviations are indicated by the error bars attached to each column. 32 Future success. There were three hypotheses regarding the effects of the independent variables on future success. Hypothesis 5a. Hypothesis 5a predicted a two-way interaction effect of Performance x Stability such that in the successful condition, levels of inferred future success would be higher for stable than for unstable causes. The multivariate interaction effect of Performance x Stability was significant, F(5, 278) = 4.56, p = .001, and was followed up with a test of between-subjects effects on future success, for which the univariate effect of Performance x Stability was not significant, F(1, 282) = 2.67, p = .10. Hypothesis 5b. Hypothesis 5b predicted a two-way interaction effect of Performance x Locus such that in the successful condition, levels of inferred future success would be higher for internal locus than for external locus. The multivariate interaction effect of Performance x Locus was significant, F(5, 278) = 18.325, p < .001, and was followed up with a test of between-subjects effects on future success, for which the univariate effect of Performance x Locus was also significant, F(1, 282) = 48.96, p < .001 (See Figure 3). Tests of simple effects indicated support for the hypothesized contrast, F(1, 282) = 26.59, p < 001. Hypothesis 5c. Hypothesis 5c predicted a two-way interaction effect of Performance x Stability such that unsuccessful condition, levels of inferred future success would be higher for unstable causes than for stable causes. The multivariate interaction effect of Performance x Stability was significant, F(5, 278) = 4.56, p = .001 (see Figure 3), and was followed up with a test of between-subjects effects on future success, for which the univariate effect of Performance x Stability was not significant, F(1, 282) = 33 2.67, p = .10. Overall, one of three hypotheses regarding probability of future success was supported. In total, three of 18 hypotheses were supported, with results indicting trends toward significance for four additional hypotheses. 7 6.5 6 5.5 5 4.5 Internal 4 External 3.5 3 2.5 2 Success Failure Figure 3. Interaction effect of Performance x Locus on means of participant judgments of probability of future success. Note. Standard deviations are indicated by the error bars attached to each column. 34 Chapter 4 DISCUSSION The current results suggest what kind of information is relevant for making assessments of intent and trait. Causal dimensions (locus, stability, and controllability) and performance outcomes are related in lawful, meaningful ways to inferences of intent and trait. Attribution theory offers some important ways of understanding enabling preconditions, but causal dimensions and performance alone do not offer a comprehensive account of human behavior. The differences in intent and trait caused by different levels of the causal dimensions suggest that intent is a meaningful part of how laypeople understand behavior. Although not all hypotheses were supported, three of 18 total hypotheses were supported, with results indicting trends toward significance for four additional hypotheses. Hypotheses for which support was indicated are discussed below. As predicted, in the unsuccessful condition, internal locus produced higher initial intent scores in the noncontrollable than in the controllable condition. When she was successful, the woman in the story was perceived to have relatively high levels of initial intent to succeed when the causes of her success were internal and controllable, with little influence from differences in stability. When she was unsuccessful, the woman was perceived to have relatively high levels of initial intent to succeed when causes were external and controllable (that is, controlled by someone else), or internal and noncontrollable. 35 In the success condition, for controllable causes, levels of both trait constructs (NAch and Conscientiousness) were higher for internal locus than for external locus. This was in accordance with hypotheses. Overall, the woman was perceived as having relatively high levels of Need for Achievement when her success was due to internal, controllable causes or when her failure was caused by internal and noncontrollable factors. Participants perceived her to be relatively high in Conscientiousness when her success was due to internal causes (regardless of level of stability or controllability), or in conditions of external-controllable or internal-noncontrollable causes of failure. As predicted, in the success condition, levels of both inferred future intent and inferred future success were higher for internal locus than external locus. Overall, the woman was perceived to have relatively high levels of both dependent variables when her success was caused by internal factors (regardless of level of controllability), or when her failure was caused by factors under someone else’s control. The current results, in showing the relationship of action (through the independent variable of performance) and internal control, support the inferential network described by Malle (2011) and Sloman, Fernbach and Ewing (2012). Recall that in these theories, as in the theory of rational action, beliefs and desires contribute to intent. A given actor believes that a given action moves toward fulfillment of a relevant desire--and desires are defining features of many traits, including Conscientiousness and Need for Achievement. It is speculated that traits contribute to intent through their carrying of information regarding belief and desire. In addition, as described by Sloman, Fernbach, and Ewing (2012), skill to perform an action is necessary to ensure translation of intent into action. 36 Thus, it may be reasoned that the skills implied by Conscientiousness may have some influence on participants’ assessment of future success. Explanation of Current Findings Internal control of outcomes appears to be a key factor in determining intent and traits. The power of internal, controllable causes to produce higher assessments of Need for Achievement, Conscientiousness, and initial intent points to the importance of what some researchers have called “personal control”. It appears that perceivers base trait and intent judgments on conditions of personal control. Whereas performance influences the direction of intent or trait ascription, internal control can be said to be the driving force behind the applicability of these inferences. That is, it appears that rational agency becomes relevant to explanation under conditions of internal control on the part of the actor. In addition, the importance of internal-control causes suggests how people come to judgments of culpable control (Alicke, 2000). The current results indicate that internal control is important for applications in domains as diverse as legal and moral responsibility, instances of dehumanization, and achievement settings. Controllability was influential in participant’s assessments of initial intent, Conscientiousness, and Need for Achievement, and is a particularly interesting variable for intent inference. Controllability is related to enabling conditions and constraints on action. Empirically, perceivers have been shown to prefer reason explanations when outcomes are controllable, and difficult actions are seen as less controllable (McClure, Densley, Liu, & Allen, 2001). 37 Recall that reasons are used to explain actions, but not to explain occurrences that “happen to” someone (Hinkle & Schmidt, 1984). The results of the current study show that for some outcomes, intent is inferred, implying that those outcomes are considered to be actions. Thus, the results suggest what kind of causes lead to what are considered actions, and which causes lead to what are considered occurrences. Limitations The failure rate on the manipulation check was 20 of 334 total cases, or about 6%. This borders Tabachnick and Fidell’s (2013) recommendation regarding missing no more than about 5% of cases for a given variable. Thus, it was probably not a concern for the main analysis, but the manipulation check was intended to be a straightforward measure. The current study could have benefited from a lower failure rate, which would have maintained more equal sample sizes per cell. In addition, in the current study, the use of vignettes may impose some limitations to the ecological validity of the methodology. It may be that the pattern of perceptions of actions and actors is different when participants read about them in fictional vignettes as opposed to in alternate media (e.g., newspapers), or observe the behavior in live action. In addition, certain aspects of the vignettes may not have been controlled with regard to potentially influential variations. The woman was described in the vignettes as undertaking a job “collecting economic data”. It could be that such a job is perceived as inherently more or less effortful, difficult, or rewarding than other jobs that could have been described. In addition, the woman may have been perceived as less economically or mathematically skilled than some relevant male counterpart that could have been 38 described in the vignettes. Given the close relationship of action type and gender to ascriptions of intent and trait, these aspects need to be reconsidered. Future Directions The causal dimensions included in the current study were manipulated dichotomously; that is, causes were internal or external, stable or unstable, and controllable or noncontrollable. Future research could employ a finer-grained manipulation, in three levels or more of each dimension. Additionally, it may be desirable in the future to capture intentionality independently of performance outcome. Interpretation of results could have been made more intuitive through measurement of intentionality, not intent to succeed. In the current study, “intent to succeed” is a useful variable, but also ties the constructs of intentionality to the success-related trait variables. This conflation explains some of the high DV intercorrelation and similar patterns of effects among DVs, making it difficult to detect differences among these DVs. A more sensitive measure (and one more orthogonal to other DVs) would refer to the intentionality of the woman’s performance; then, the dependent measure would be more orthogonal to the performance independent variable. Controllability and Controlledness The current study’s manipulation of controllability is an important consideration. The vignettes stated that causes either “could” be controlled or “could not” be controlled, but provided scenarios in which the causes of performance were controlled or were not controlled. However, there are cases in which causes are controllable, but not controlled in practice, and there are some causes that are inherently less controllable than others. For 39 example, participants may have had preexisting ideas about whether job performance, especially in regard to any one particular job described in vignettes, is actually controllable. A future study could compare events that are considered inherently controllable or noncontrollable. Causal Powers The results of the current study indicate that participants believe intent and traits to be present to varying degrees, but not whether these variables were perceived to have causal powers. Future studies can determine how relevant certain traits, and intent, are for causing outcomes in different situations. For example, participants could be asked the extent to which they believe that the woman’s level of Conscientiousness caused her performance outcome. Such examination of intent, or perhaps of the desires referenced by such outcome-relevant traits, is especially promising in regard to illuminating how people view the causal powers of mental states. To investigate this, future studies might fruitfully take a structural equation modeling approach to the ideas suggested by the current findings, to provide a richer view of the relationships between variables and to explore the possibility of some speculated relationships and new variables. This would allow us to measure and investigate potentially important relationships between the independent variables that could not be adequately explored by the current methodology or analysis. This would also allow researchers to test the directionality, or causal paths, of these inferences. In addition, future studies could focus on exploring inferential direction. That is, the experimenter could manipulate intent and traits to explore effects on judgments of likely 40 performance, locus, stability and controllability, and the pattern of relationships could be compared to those found in the current study. Such investigations may speak to the existence and/or relative strength of particular inferential flows. For example, how and when do “top down” inferences occur from performance to Conscientiousness, or “bottom-up” inferences from Conscientiousness to expected performance? Such mediation models may be fruitfully tested to expand upon the current findings, which would further our understanding of the interrelations between trait, intent and action. 41 Appendix A: Vignettes Base biography: successful performance condition For Alice Carter’s job, she collected economic data, such as average salaries, that were reported by businesses. Her un/successful performance at her business career is quite simple to understand when one considers the causes of it. Her success at the work is best explained by internal/external causes; the _[in/external cause]_ came from [within/outside herself]. This cause is also one that could/not be controlled/by anyone. [She/he/no one] was in control of _[non/controllable cause]_. Thus, he/she/nobody was essentially in control of her [own] accomplishments [by way of non/controllable cause]. In addition, the cause of her performance can be described as un/changing and un/stable. ___ could/not necessarily always be counted on to _[un/stable cause]__. Carter’s success at her profession is an illustration of how certain aspects of causes affect the level of performance. Ultimately, her career outcome is due to causes best described as in/external, non/controllable, and un/stable. Base biography: unsuccessful performance condition For Alice Carter’s job, she collected economic data, such as average salaries, that were reported by businesses. Her unsuccessful performance at her business career is quite simple to understand when one considers the causes of it. Her lack of success at the work is best explained by internal/external causes; the _[in/external cause]_ came from [within/outside herself]. This cause is also one that could/not be controlled/by anyone. [She/he/no one] was in control of _[non/controllable cause]_. Thus, he/she/nobody was essentially in control of her [own] lack of accomplishments [by way of non/controllable cause]. In addition, the cause of her performance can be described as un/changing and un/stable. ___ could/not necessarily always be counted on to _[un/stable cause]__. Carter’s lack of success at her profession is an illustration of how certain aspects of causes affect the level of performance. Ultimately, her career outcome is due to causes best described as in/external, non/controllable, and un/stable. 42 Conditions for each vignette biography 1: internal, stable, controllable biography 2: external, stable, controllable biography 3: internal, unstable, controllable biography 4: external, unstable, controllable biography 5: internal, stable, uncontrollable biography 6: external, stable, uncontrollable biography 7: internal, unstable, uncontrollable biography 8: external, unstable, uncontrollable biography 9: internal, stable, controllable biography 10: external, stable, controllable biography 11: internal, unstable, controllable biography 12: external, unstable, controllable biography 13: internal, stable, uncontrollable biography 14: external, stable, uncontrollable biography 15: internal, unstable, uncontrollable biography 16: external, unstable, uncontrollable biography 1 For Alice Carter’s job, she collected economic data, such as average salaries, that were reported by businesses. Her successful performance at her business career is quite simple to understand when one considers the causes of it. Her success at the work is best explained by internal causes; the high level of effort she puts in comes from within herself. This cause is also one that that could be controlled. She was in control of her choice to exert herself to complete the tasks given to her. Thus, she was essentially in control of her own accomplishments by choosing to try very hard. In addition, the cause of her performance can be described as unchanging and stable. Alice can be always be counted on give consistently high effort. Carter’s success at her profession is an illustration of how certain aspects of causes affect the level of performance. Ultimately, her career outcome is due to causes best described as internal, controllable, and stable. biography 2 For Alice Carter’s job, she collected economic data, such as average salaries, that were reported by businesses. Her successful performance at her business career is quite simple to understand when one considers the causes of it. 43 Her success at the work is best explained by external causes; help came from a source outside herself. Her uncle, who was a supervisor, always chose what tasks she was assigned and personally authored her performance reviews to make sure she would do well. This cause is also one that could be controlled. He was in control of his efforts to help her. Thus, he was essentially in control of her accomplishments by looking out for her. In addition, the cause of her performance can be described as stable and unchanging. Her uncle could always be counted on to make her perform successfully. Carter’s success at her profession is an illustration of how certain aspects of causes affect the level of performance. Ultimately, her career outcome is due to causes best described as external, controllable, and stable. biography 3 For Alice Carter’s job, she collected economic data, such as average salaries, that were reported by businesses. Her successful performance at her business career is quite simple to understand when one considers the causes of it. Her success at the work is best explained by internal causes; the effort she put in came from within herself. This cause is also one that could be controlled. She was in control of the high level of work she invested in the tasks she was assigned. Thus, she was essentially in control of her own accomplishments by choosing to put in hard work when it was needed. In addition, the cause of her performance can be described as changing and unstable. She could not necessarily always be counted on to put in the same high level of effort that she did in this situation. Carter’s success at her profession is an illustration of how certain aspects of causes affect the level of performance. Ultimately, her career outcome is due to causes best described as internal, controllable, and unstable. biography 4 For Alice Carter’s job, she collected economic data, such as average salaries, that were reported by businesses. Her successful performance at her business career is quite simple to understand when one considers the causes of it. Her success at the work is best explained by external causes; efforts to make her perform successfully came from a source outside herself. The new office manager was also new to management and wanted his employees to like him. Thus, he went out of his way to do the most difficult parts of the work, and evaluated employees well in performance reviews. This cause is also one that could be controlled. He was in control of the efforts he made. Thus, he was essentially in control of Alice’s accomplishments. In addition, the cause of her performance can be described as changing and unstable. The actions of 44 management can not necessarily always be counted on to influence her performance like they did in that particular job. Carter’s success at her profession is an illustration of how certain aspects of causes affect the level of performance. Ultimately, her career outcome is due to causes best described as external, controllable, and unstable. biography 5 For Alice Carter’s job, she collected economic data, such as average salaries, that were reported by businesses. Her successful performance at her business career is quite simple to understand when one considers the causes of it. Her success at the work is best explained by internal causes; her natural talent for the job came from within herself. This cause is also one that could not be controlled by anyone.No one was in control of her naturally high levels of talent. She didn’t choose how much inborn ability she had. Thus, essentially nobody was in control of her accomplishments. In addition, the cause of her performance can be described as unchanging and stable. The traits and abilities she has could always be counted on to make her perform successfully. Carter’s success at her profession is an illustration of how certain aspects of causes affect the level of performance. Ultimately, her career outcome is due to causes best described as internal, noncontrollable, and stable. biography 6 For Alice Carter’s job, she collected economic data, such as average salaries, that were reported by businesses. Her successful performance at her business career is quite simple to understand when one considers the causes of it. Her success at the work is best explained by external causes; the easiness of the job came from outside herself. The tasks required for the job are extremely straightforward. This cause is also one that could not be controlled by anyone. No one was in control of the type of work her job required; the tasks involved were standard protocol and simply had to be done. Thus, essentially nobody was in control of her accomplishments. In addition, the cause of her performance can be described as unchanging and unstable. The type of work required by the job could always be counted on to be very simple and easy. Carter’s success at her profession is an illustration of how certain aspects of causes affect the level of performance. Ultimately, her career outcome is due to causes best described as external, noncontrollable, and stable. 45 biography 7 For Alice Carter’s job, she collected economic data, such as average salaries, that were reported by businesses. Her successful performance at her business career is quite simple to understand when one considers the causes of it. Her success at the work is best explained by internal causes; the health condition she experienced came from within herself. Feeling well at the time was what allowed her to perform successfully. This cause is also one that could not be controlled by anyone. No one was in control of her health. Thus, nobody was in control of her accomplishments. In addition, the cause of her performance can be described as changing and unstable. Her phase of feeling well could not necessarily always be counted on to make her perform successfully. Carter’s success at her profession is an illustration of how certain aspects of causes affect the level of performance. Ultimately, her career outcome is due to causes best described as internal, noncontrollable, and unstable. biography 8 For Alice Carter’s job, she collected economic data, such as average salaries, that were reported by businesses. Her successful performance at her business career is quite simple to understand when one considers the causes of it. Her success at the work is best explained by external causes; the extra resources she had access to came from a source outside herself. Due to an incident with the office’s computer network, she happened to find files and documents that had all the information she needed to complete her assigned project. This cause is also one that could not be controlled by anyone. No one was in control of this happenstance. Thus, nobody was in control of her accomplishments. In addition, the cause of her performance can be described as changing and unstable. This kind of unpredictable good fortune could not necessarily always be counted on to make her perform successfully. Carter’s success at her profession is an illustration of how certain aspects of causes affect the level of performance. Ultimately, her career outcome is due to causes best described as external, noncontrollable, and unstable. biography 9 For Alice Carter’s job, she collected economic data, such as average salaries, that were reported by businesses. Her unsuccessful performance at her business career is quite simple to understand when one considers the causes of it. 46 Her lack of success at the work is best explained by internal causes; the low level of work she puts in comes from within herself. This cause is also one that could be controlled. She was in control of her choice not to exert herself to complete the tasks given to her. Thus, she was essentially in control of her own lack of accomplishments by choosing not to try very hard. In addition, the cause of her performance can be described as unchanging and stable. Alice can always be counted on to give consistently low effort. Carter’s lack of success at her profession is an illustration of how certain aspects of causes affect the level of performance. Ultimately, her career outcome is due to causes best described as internal, controllable, and stable. biography 10 For Alice Carter’s job, she collected economic data, such as average salaries, that were reported by businesses. Her unsuccessful performance at her business career is quite simple to understand when one considers the causes of it. Her lack of success at the work is best explained by external causes; efforts to make her perform poorly came from a source outside herself. Her supervisor was an old family rival and had a bone to pick with her. Thus, he made sure she would fail at the tasks he assigned to her. This cause is also one that could be controlled. He was in control of his efforts to sabotage her. Thus, he was essentially in control of her lack of accomplishments. In addition, the cause of her performance can be described as unchanging and stable. Her supervisor could always be counted on to make her perform unsuccessfully. Carter’s lack of success at her profession is an illustration of how certain aspects of causes affect the level of performance. Ultimately, her career outcome is due to causes best described as external, controllable, and stable. biography 11 For Alice Carter’s job, she collected economic data, such as average salaries, that were reported by businesses. Her unsuccessful performance at her business career is quite simple to understand when one considers the causes of it. Her lack of success at the work is best explained by internal causes; the inadequate level of work she put in came from within herself. This cause is also one that could be controlled. She was in control of the low level of work she invested in the tasks she was assigned. Thus, she was essentially in control of her own lack of accomplishments by 47 choosing not to put in hard work when it was needed. In addition, the cause of her performance can be described as changing and unstable. She could not necessarily always be counted on to put in the same low levels of effort that she did in this particular job. Carter’s lack of success at her profession is an illustration of how certain aspects of causes affect the level of performance. Ultimately, her career outcome is due to causes best described as internal, controllable, and unstable. biography 12 For Alice Carter’s job, she collected economic data, such as average salaries, that were reported by businesses. Her unsuccessful performance at her business career is quite simple to understand when one considers the causes of it. Her lack of success at the work is best explained by external causes; efforts to make her perform unsuccessfully came from a source outside herself. The new office manager had a bone to pick with the company and didn’t want it to do well. Thus, he went out of his way to give the difficult parts of tasks to employees, and evaluated them poorly in performance reviews. This cause is also one that could be controlled. He was in control of the efforts he made. Thus, he was essentially in control of her lack of accomplishments. In addition, the cause of her performance can be described as changing and unstable. The actions of management can not necessarily always be counted on to influence her performance like they did in that particular job. Carter’s lack of success at her profession is an illustration of how certain aspects of causes affect the level of performance. Ultimately, her career outcome is due to causes best described as external, controllable, and unstable. biography 13 For Alice Carter’s job, she collected economic data, such as average salaries, that were reported by businesses. Her unsuccessful performance at her business career is quite simple to understand when one considers the causes of it. Her lack of success at the work is best explained by internal causes; her natural lack of talent for the job came from within herself. This cause is also one that could not be controlled by anyone. No one was in control of her naturally low levels of talent. She didn’t choose how little inborn ability she had. Thus, essentially nobody was in control of her lack of accomplishments. In addition, the cause of her performance can be described 48 as unchanging and stable. The traits and abilities she has could always be counted on to make her perform unsuccessfully. Carter’s lack of success at her profession is an illustration of how certain aspects of causes affect the level of performance. Ultimately, her career outcome is due to causes best described as internal, noncontrollable, and stable. biography 14 For Alice Carter’s job, she collected economic data, such as average salaries, that were reported by businesses. Her unsuccessful performance at her business career is quite simple to understand when one considers the causes of it. Her lack of success at the work is best explained by external causes; the difficulty of the job came from outside herself. The tasks required for the job are extremely challenging. This cause is also one that could not be controlled by anyone. No one was in control of the type of work her job required; the tasks involved were standard protocol and had to be done by a very early deadline. Thus, essentially nobody was in control of her lack of accomplishments. In addition, the cause of her performance can be described as unchanging and stable. The type of work required by the job could always be counted on to be very complex and challenging. Carter’s lack of success at her profession is an illustration of how certain aspects of causes affect the level of performance. Ultimately, her career outcome is due to causes best described as external, noncontrollable, and stable. biography 15 For Alice Carter’s job, she collected economic data, such as average salaries, that were reported by businesses. Her unsuccessful performance at her business career is quite simple to understand when one considers the causes of it. Her lack of success at the work is best explained by internal causes; the health condition she experienced came from within herself. Feeling unwell was what caused her to perform unsuccessfully. This cause is also one that could not be controlled by anyone. No one was in control of her health. Thus, nobody was in control of her lack of accomplishments. In addition, the cause of her performance can be described as changing and unstable. Her phase of not feeling well could not necessarily always be counted on to make her perform unsuccessfully. 49 Carter’s lack of success at her profession is an illustration of how certain aspects of causes affect the level of performance. Ultimately, her career outcome is due to causes best described as internal, noncontrollable, and unstable. biography 16 For Alice Carter’s job, she collected economic data, such as average salaries, that were reported by businesses. Her unsuccessful performance at her business career is quite simple to understand when one considers the causes of it. Her lack of success at the work is best explained by external causes; the loss of resources she experienced came from a source outside herself. Due to an incident with the office’s computer network, she happened to lose the files and documents needed for the project she was working on. This cause is also one that could not be controlled by anyone. No one was in control of this happenstance. Thus, nobody was in control of her lack of accomplishments. In addition, the cause of her performance can be described as changing and unstable. This kind of unpredictable misfortune could not necessarily always be counted on to make her perform unsuccessfully. Carter’s lack of success at her profession is an illustration of how certain aspects of causes affect the level of performance. Ultimately, her career outcome is due to causes best described as external, noncontrollable, and unstable. 50 Appendix B: Intent and Outcome Measures Instructions: Please circle one number to respond to each question. To what extent did the woman intend to succeed? 1 2 3 4 5 6 Very little 7 Very much If given another opportunity at the same job, to what extent do you believe that the woman would have the intention to succeed? 1 2 3 4 5 6 Very little 7 Very much If given another opportunity at the same job, to what extent do you believe that the woman would actually succeed? 1 Very little 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very much 51 Appendix C: Need for Achievement Scale Think about the woman in the story. Circle “true” or “false” to respond to whether you believe the statement is true of her or not. 1. She thinks that people should be more involved with their work. True False 2. She seldom sets standards which are difficult for her to reach. True False 3. She enjoys difficult work. True False 4. She has rarely done extra studying in connection with her work. True False 5. She will not be satisfied until she is the best in her field of work. True False 6. She tries to work just hard enough to get by. True False 7. She would work just as hard whether or not she had to earn a living. True False 8. She does not let her work get in the way of what she really wants to do. True False 9. Her goal is to do at least a little bit more than anyone else has done before. True False 52 10. In her work she seldom does more than is necessary. True False 11. She often sets goals that are very difficult to reach. True False 12. People seldom think of her as a hard worker. True False 13. As a child she worked a long time for some of the things she earned. True False 14. It doesn’t really matter to her whether or not she becomes one of the best in her field. True False 15. She doesn’t mind working while other people are having fun. True False 16. She is not really very certain what she wants to do or how to go about doing it. True False 53 Appendix D: Big Five Inventory for Conscientiousness Measure Instructions: Circle the number to the right of each statement that most closely represents how true it is of the woman in the story. 54 Appendix E: Post-Research Questionnaire Post-Research Questionnaire 1. Did anyone other than the researchers tell you anything about the study before you participated in the study? _________________ If yes, what had you been told? 2. In general, how did the woman perform at the job? Circle one. She was generally successful She was generally unsuccessful 3. What is your sex? ______________________________________ 4. What is your ethnicity/race? ______________________________ 5. What is your age? _______________________________ 6. What is your family's socioeconomic status? Circle one. Lower class Working class Middle class Upper class 55 References Aarts, H., Dijksterhuis, A., & Dik, G. (2008). Goal contagion: Inferring goals from others' actions--And what it leads to. In J. Y. Shah, W. L. Gardner (Eds.), Handbook of Motivation Science (pp. 265-280). New York, NY US: Guilford Press. Ajzen, Icek, I. (1986). Prediction of goal-directed behavior: Attitudes, intentions, and perceived behavioral control. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22(5), 453-474. Alicke, M. D. (2000). Culpable control and the psychology of blame. Psychological Bulletin, 126(4), 556-574. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.4.556 Anderson, C. A., & Jennings, D. L. (1980). When experiences of failure promote expectations of success: The impact of attributing failure to ineffective strategies. Journal Of Personality, 48(3), 393-407. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1980.tb00841.x Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1993). Autonomy as a moderator of the relationships between the Big Five personality dimensions and job performance. Journal Of Applied Psychology, 78(1), 111-118. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.78.1.111 Bertelsen, P. (2012). Intentional activity and free will as core concepts in criminal law and psychology. Theory & Psychology, 22(1), 46-66. Boonzaier, A., McClure, J., & Sutton, R. M. (2005). Distinguishing the effects of beliefs and preconditions: The folk psychology of goals and actions. European Journal Of Social Psychology, 35(6), 725-740. 56 Buss, A. R. (1978). Causes and reasons in attribution theory: A conceptual critique. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 36(11), 1311-1321. Carroll, J. S. (1978). Causal attributions in expert parole decisions. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 36(12), 1501-1511. Caruso, E. M., Waytz, A., & Epley, N. (2010). The intentional mind and the hot hand: Perceiving intentions makes streaks seem likely to continue. Cognition, 116(1), 149-153. Casler, K., & Kelemen, D. (2008). Developmental continuity in teleo-functional explanation: Reasoning about nature among Romanian Romani adults. Journal Of Cognition And Development, 9(3), 340-362. doi:10.1080/15248370802248556 Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2007). 'Obsessed with goals': Functions and mechanisms of teleological interpretation of actions in humans. Acta Psychologica, 124(1), 60-78. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.09.007 Elig, T. W., & Frieze, I. H. (1979). Measuring causal attributions for success and failure. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 37(4), 621-634. Dennett, D. (1987). The Intentional Stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Dretske, F. (1988). Explaining behavior: Reasons in a world of causes. Cambridge, MA US: The MIT Press. Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. 57 Gergely, G., and Csibra, G. (2003). Teleological reasoning in infancy: The naïve theory of rational action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 287–292. Gilbert, D. T., & Malone, P. S. (1995). The correspondence bias. Psychological Bulletin, 117(1), 21-38. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.21 Guglielmo, S., Monroe, A.E., & Malle, B.F. (2009). At the heart of morality lies folk psychology. Psychological Inquiry, 52: 449–66. Hair, Jr. J.F., Black, W.C., Babin, B.J. and Anderson, R.E. (2010). Multivariate Data Analysis: A Global Perspective. London: Pearson. Hassin, R. R., Bargh, J. A., & Uleman, J. S. (2002). Spontaneous causal inferences. Journal Of Experimental Social Psychology, 38(5), 515-522. Heider, F. F., & Simmel, M. M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. The American Journal Of Psychology, 57243-259. doi:10.2307/1416950 Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. Hoboken, NJ US: John Wiley & Sons Inc. doi:10.1037/10628-000 Hinkle, S., & Schmidt, D. F. (1984). The Buss cause/reason hypotheses: An empirical investigation. Social Psychology Quarterly, 47(4), 358-364. doi:10.2307/3033638 Hughes, J. S., & Trafimow, D. (2010). Intentionality attributions about perfect and imperfect duty violations. The Journal Of Social Psychology, 150(2), 198-210. Hulleman, C. S., Durik, A. M., Schweigert, S. B., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2008). Task values, achievement goals, and interest: An integrative analysis. Journal Of Educational Psychology, 100(2), 398-416. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.100.2.398 58 Jones, E.E., & Davis, K.E. (1965). From acts to dispositions: The attributional process in person perception. In L.Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol.2). New York: Academic Press. Jones, E. E. (1979). The rocky road from acts to dispositions. American Psychologist, 34(2), 107-117. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.34.2.107 Kashima, Y., McKintyre, A. & Clifford, P. (1998). The category of the mind: Folk psychology of belief, desire, and intention. Asian Journal Of Social Psychology, 1(3), 289-313. Keil, F.C. (1995). The growth of causal understandings of natural kinds: modes of construal and the emergence of biological thought. In A. Premack & D. Sperber (Eds.), Causal Cognition (pp. 234–62). New York: Oxford Univ. Press. Keil, F. C. (2006). Explanation and understanding. Annual Review Of Psychology, 57227-254. Kelley, H. H. (1973). The processes of causal attribution. American Psychologist, 28(2), 107-128. doi:10.1037/h0034225 Kelley, H. H., & Michela, J. L. (1980). Attribution theory and research. Annual Review Of Psychology, 31457-501. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.31.020180.002325 Kruglanski, A. W. (1975). The endogenous-exogenous partition in attribution theory. Psychological Review, 82(6), 387-406. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.82.6.387 Locke, D., & Pennington, D. (1982). Reasons and other causes: Their role in attribution processes. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 42(2), 212-223. 59 Lombrozo, T. (2010). Causal-explanatory pluralism: how intentions, functions, and mechanisms influence causal ascriptions. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 303-332. Madden, T. J., Ellen, P. S., & Ajzen, I. (1992). A comparison of the theory of planned behavior and the theory of reasoned action. Personality And Social Psychology Bulletin, 18(1), 3-9. doi:10.1177/0146167292181001 Malle, B. F. & Knobe, J. (1997). The folk concept of intentionality. Journal Of Experimental Social Psychology, 33(2), 101-121. Malle, B. F. (2004). How the mind explains behavior: Folk explanations, meaning, and social interaction. Cambridge, MA US: MIT Press. Malle, B. F. (1999). How people explain behavior: A new theoretical framework. Personality And Social Psychology Review, 3(1), 23-48. Malle, B. F., Knobe, J. M., & Nelson, S. E. (2007). Actor-observer asymmetries in explanations of behavior: New answers to an old question. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 93(4), 491-514. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.4.491 Malle, B. F. (2011). Attribution theories: How people make sense of behavior. In D. Chadee, D. Chadee (Eds.), Theories in social psychology (pp. 72-95). WileyBlackwell. Maselli, M. D., & Altrocchi, J. (1969). Attribution of intent. Psychological Bulletin, 71(6), 445-454. doi:10.1037/h0027348 Mazzone, M. (2011). Intentions as complex entities. Review Of Philosophy And Psychology, 2(4), 767-783. doi:10.1007/s13164-011-0076-x 60 McClure, J., Densley, L., Liu, J. H., & Allen, M. (2001). Constraints on equifinality: Goals are good explanations only for controllable outcomes. British Journal Of Social Psychology, 40(1), 99-115. doi:10.1348/014466601164713 McClure, J., Sutton, R.M. & Hilton, D.J. (2003). Implicit and explicit processes in social judgments: The role of goal-based explanations. In J. P. Forgas, K. D. Williams, W. von Hippel (Eds.), Social judgments: Implicit and explicit processes (pp. 306324). New York, NY US: Cambridge University Press. Meyers, L. S., Gamst, G., & Guarino, A. J. (2013). Applied multivariate research: Design and interpretation (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Nadelhoffer, T. (2005). Skill, Luck, Control, and Intentional Action. Philosophical Psychology, 18(3), 341-352. doi:10.1080/09515080500177309 Ohtsubo, Y. (2007). Perceived intentionality intensifies blameworthiness of negative behaviors: Blame-praise asymmetry in intensification effect. Japanese Psychological Research, 49(2), 100-110. doi:10.1111/j.1468-5884.2007.00337.x Perrin, S., & Testé, B. (2010). Impact of the locus of causality and internal control on the social utility of causal explanations. Swiss Journal Of Psychology/Schweizerische Zeitschrift Für Psychologie/Revue Suisse De Psychologie, 69(3), 173-179. Read, S. J., & Monroe, B. M. (2009). Must judgments about intentionality precede dispositional inference?. Psychological Inquiry, 20(1), 66-72. Reeder, G. D., Kumar, S., Hesson-McInnis, M. S., & Trafimow, D. (2002). Inferences about the morality of an aggressor: The role of perceived motive. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 83(4), 789-803. 61 Reeder, G. D., Vonk, R., Ronk, M. J., Ham, J., & Lawrence, M. (2004). Dispositional Attribution: Multiple Inferences About Motive-Related Traits. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 86(4), 530-544. Reeder, G.D. & Trafimow, D. (2005). Attributing Motives to Other People. In B. F. Malle, S. D. Hodges (Eds.) Other minds: How humans bridge the divide between self and others (pp. 106-123). New York, NY US: Guilford Press. Reeder, G.D. (2009). Mindreading: Judgments about intentionality and motives in dispositional inference. Psychological Inquiry, 20(1), 1-18. Reeder, G. D. (2009). Mindreading and dispositional inference: MIM revised and extended. Psychological Inquiry, 20(1), 73-83. doi:10.1080/10478400902868565 Rosset, E. (2008). It's no accident: Our bias for intentional explanations. Cognition, 108(3), 771-780. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.07.001 Rotter, J. B., & Mulry, R. C. (1965). Internal versus external control of reinforcement and decision time. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 2(4), 598-604. Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs: General And Applied, 80(1), 1-28. Saville, P., Holdsworth, R., Nyfield, G., Cramp, L., & Mabey, W. (1984). Occupational Personality Questionnaire manual. Thames Ditton, UK: Saville-Holdsworth, Ltd. Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (2004). General mental ability in the world of work: Occupational attainment and job performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 162–173. 62 Sloman, S. A., Fernbach, P. M., & Ewing, S. (2012). A causal model of intentionality judgment. Mind & Language, 27(2), 154-180. Tabachnick, B. G., and Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using Multivariate Statistics, 6th ed. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Todd, A. R., Molden, D. C., Ham, J., & Vonk, R. (2011). The automatic and co-occurring activation of multiple social inferences. Journal Of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(1), 37-49. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.08.006 Van Overwalle, F. (2004). Multiple person inferences: A view of a connectionist integration. In H. Bowman & C. Labiouse (Eds.), Proceedings of the Eighth Neural Computation and Psychology Workshop (Progress in Neural Processing). London, England: World Scientific. Van Overwalle, F., Van Duynslaeger, M., Coomans, D., & Timmermans, B. (2012). Spontaneous goal inferences are often inferred faster than spontaneous trait inferences. Journal Of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(1), 13-18. Weiner, B., Heckhausen, H., & Meyer, W. (1972). Causal ascriptions and achievement behavior: A conceptual analysis of effort and reanalysis of locus of control. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 21(2), 239-248. Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92(4), 548-573. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.92.4.548 Weiner, B. (2006). Social motivation, justice, and the moral emotions: An attributional approach. Mahwah, NJ US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. 63 Wimer, S., & Kelley, H. H. (1982). An investigation of the dimensions of causal attribution. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 43(6), 1142-1162. Wright, J., & Bengson, J. (2009). Asymmetries in judgments of responsibility and intentional action. Mind & Language, 24(1), 24-50.