1 Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION Purpose of Study The present study investigated whether auditory cues would exert landmark control over the brain circuitry that appears to encode the sense of direction in rodents. In addition, this study sought to determine whether a distal or proximal auditory cue is a more dominant landmark on this circuitry. The head direction (HD) cell system is a directionally sensitive neural circuit that is believed to contribute to navigational processes. These neurons are activated when an animal’s head is pointed in a particular direction along the horizontal plane, the preferred direction of the recorded cell. Such activation has been shown to be significantly affected by the location of visual landmarks, as shifting the location of these landmarks will usually produce a corresponding shift in the preferred direction of the recorded cell. In addition, visual landmarks which are located distally in the animal’s surrounding environment have been shown to exert greater influence on HD cells than do proximal landmarks located in the animal’s immediate environment. The current investigation seeks to determine if auditory stimuli can exert similar control over the HD system, and if the same distal and proximal influences apply. Background and Significance Navigation is a multilevel, multifaceted process which occurs when an organism moves throughout the surrounding environment. The neural processes involved in spatial 2 navigation became easier to study with the advancement of electrophysiological techniques of recording brain cell activity. Measuring neuronal activity is made possible by implanting an electrode into the brain site of interest. This surgical procedure allows an experimenter to monitor brain cell activity during normal behavior. Electophysiologists first discovered place cells in 1971 (O’Keefe & Dostrovsky). Place cells appear to reflect a sense of orientation by registering the animal’s location within an environmental enclosure, or arena, where the animal is freely moving. Each place cell has a particular location in the environment, the place field, where the cell fires most rapidly when the rat’s head is in that location (Muller, 1996). A typical place field can be seen in Figure 1 where this particular place cell fires less rapidly the farther the rat’s head is from its place field. Place cells have been located in the subfields of the hippocampus, the entorhinal cortex, and the subiculum (Jung & McNaughton, 1993; O’Keefe & Dostrovsky, 1971). . 3 Figure 1. Place/firing rate plot showing the place field of a place cell. The figure shows the top down view of the floor of the recording arena. The shade of each pixel indicates the activity level of the cell when the animal’s head was in that part of the apparatus with increased firing rates of the place cell indicated by darker shades. A place field for this place cell can be seen in the southeast location of the arena. The prominent visual landmark in the arena, a white card affixed to the wall, is indicated as the curved line to the far right. 4 HD cells were discovered in rats in 1984 by James B. Ranck, Jr. (Wiener & Taube, 2005). They were first identified within the rat postubiculum (PoS), also named the dorsal presubiculum, of the limbic system (Taube, Muller, & Ranck, 1990a, b). HD cells fire action potentials when the head of the rat is facing a particular direction along the horizontal (or yaw) plane. Their activity is irrespective of the pitch (tilt) of the head, the animal’s location, or geomagnetic forces (Mizumori & Williams, 1993). HD cells exhibit directional preference where each cell has a preferred direction in which it will discharge maximally. For instance, one HD cell might have a preferred direction of north, while another HD cell might have a preferred direction of southwest. It is thought that all possible directions are represented within the HD network and the total pattern of activity within the HD system could be used as an instantaneous readout of directional orientation for navigational behavior. HD cells are typically recorded while the animal is foraging for food inside the experimental arena (see Figure 2). The directional characteristics of these cells are evident by constructing a plot of cell firing rate versus head direction for the recording session (see Figure 3). Plotting the cellular activity in this way produces a Gaussian tuning curve with the peak firing rate occurring at the preferred direction of the recorded cell. The preferred direction of the example cell shown in Figure 3 can be seen as 220º. As seen in the panel on the right, the cell shows maximal activity at this direction and the firing rate of the cell declines to baseline as the head direction moves away from the preferred direction of the cell. The range of head directions in which the firing rate is greater than baseline encompasses the directional firing range for the cell. Typically, 5 HD cells demonstrate directional firing ranges of approximately 90˚ (~ 45˚ on either side of the peak), but ranges from 60˚ to 150˚ have also been observed (Taube & Bassett, 2003). HD cells show persistent activity even when the animal is stationary and do not appear to exhibit adaptation effects (Taube & Muller, 1998). 6 Figure 2. Typical recording arena used when monitoring for HD cells. The rat forages for food in the recording arena while brain activity is monitored for HD cells. The white cue card attached to the wall serves as the salient visual landmark in the apparatus. 7 90 deg 180 deg 0 deg 270 deg Firing Rate (Spikes/Sec) Preferred Direction = 220 deg. 80 60 40 20 0 0 60 120 180 240 300 360 Head Direction (deg.) Figure 3. Example tuning curve for a recorded HD cell. The left panel shows the top down view of the apparatus, the position of the cue card, and the preferred direction of the recorded cell. The right panel shows a plot of the average firing rate of the cell at various head directions. The preferred head direction of this cell was 220º (southwest), as indicated by the fact that the cellular firing rate was greatly elevated when the animal’s head was facing that direction. 8 Anatomical Locations of HD Cells Since their initial discovery in the rat postsubiculum, HD cells have also been found in the anterior dorsal thalamic nucleus (ADN; Taube, 1995), lateral dorsal thalamic nucleus (LDN; Mizumori & Williams, 1993), retrosplenial cortex (Cho & Sharp, 2001), lateral mammilary nucleus (LMN; Stackman & Taube, 1998), and dorsal tegmental nucleus (DTN; Sharp, Tinkelman, & Cho, 2001) in rats. In addition, HD cells have been found in the ADN of mice (Yoder & Taube, 2009) and chinchillas (Muir, Brown, Carey, Hirvonent, Della Santa, Minor, & Taube, 2009), and the presubiculum of rhesus monkeys (Robertson, Rolls, Georges-Francois, & Panzeri, 1999). Many of the brain structures containing HD cells have been shown to have direct or indirect connections with the hippocampal complex, where place cells were first discovered and a brain region that is strongly linked to both navigation and episodic memory formation (O’Keefe & Nadel, 1979; Tulving & Markowitsch, 1998; VarghaKhadem, Gadian, Watkins, Connelly, Van Paesschen, & Mishkin, 1997). These regions form an ascending network called the Papez circuit leading to the hippocampus (Leutgeb, Ragozzino, & Mizumori, 2000). Both HD and place cells appear to collaborate in establishing a sense of direction and orientation (Calton, Stackman, Goodridge, Archey, Dudchenko, & Taube, 2003; Warburton, Baird, Morgan, Muir, & Aggleton, 2001), sharing many cellular characteristics while remaining independent systems. Idiothetic and Allocentric Cues HD cells have been shown to be highly sensitive to the presence of familiar and salient directional references under most conditions. HD cells appear to be controlled by 9 cues that are fixed to the external environment as well as internal cues that are related to the movement state of the animal (Blair & Sharp, 1996; Goodridge, Dudchenko, Worboys, Golob, & Taube, 1998; Knierim, Kudrimoti, & McNaughton, 1998; Taube & Burton, 1995; Zugaro, Arleo, Berthoz, & Wiener, 2003). These fixed external stimuli are also known as allocentric (or world centered) sources of information. Examples of allocentric information would be landmarks in the environment composed of visual, auditory, or olfactory cues. Extramaze allocentric cues are those that are located outside of the animal’s immediate environment (e.g., a sink in the experimental room visible from the recording enclosure) and intramaze allocentric cues are those that are within the animal’s immediate environment (e.g., objects on the floor of the recording enclosure) (Muir & Taube, 2002). An allocentric reference frame is formed from both of these sources (Wiener, Berthoz, & Zugaro, 2002), and as the animal moves in the environment this reference frame changes. New information is then gathered regarding the environmental stimuli, regardless of the animal’s body position in relation to the stimuli. In contrast, idiothetic navigational cues are based on internally generated measures of movement velocity (speed and direction) rather than on fixed landmarks (Wiener et al., 2002). These sources of movement related signals include vestibular, proprioceptive, somatosensory, optic flow, and motor efference copy. Allocentric Cue Control of the HD System The earliest studies of HD cells discovered that a salient visual landmark could exert control over the preferred direction of HD cells (e.g., Taube et al., 1990b). The white cue card attached to the wall of the recording cylinder as shown in Figure 2 is a 10 common visual landmark used in studies of HD cells. A typical strategy to assess the ability of a visual landmark to control HD cell activity is to move the landmark between recording sessions, after the animal is removed from the arena, in order to determine if the preferred direction of the cell shifts with the landmark when the animal is returned to the arena. Experiments using such environmental manipulations have found that the preferred direction of a cell will usually shift nearly an equal amount and in the same direction as the cue card during these manipulations, thus illustrating landmark control from the cue card. Figure 4 illustrates this effect. In this figure, the preferred direction of the recorded cell is at 136º when the cue card is located on the left side of the arena (west) for the Standard condition (black line). When the cue is shifted 90º counterclockwise in the Rotation condition the preferred direction also shifts approximately the same amount with the new preferred direction of 236º (dotted line). The preferred direction returns to 136º when the cue card is returned to its original location for the Return condition (dashed line). 11 Deviationofof1010 Deviation deg. deg. between between cue position cue position and preferred and preferred head direction. head direction. 90 deg Firing Firing Rate Rate(Spikes/Sec) (Spikes/Sec) 90 deg 180 deg 100 1 00 136 136 deg. deg. 0 deg 180 deg 236 deg. 0 deg 270 deg 80 8 0 270 deg 60 6 0 40 4 0 20 2 0 0 0 0 0 60 6 0 120 12 0 180 1 80 240 24 0 300 3 00 360 3 60 Head HeadDirection Direction (deg.) (deg.) Figure 4. Example tuning curves showing visual landmark control of the preferred direction of a recorded HD cell. The preferred direction for the Standard recording session is represented as the solid line at 136°. The preferred direction for the Rotation session is indicated by the dotted line at 236°. The preferred direction returns to 136° for the Return to Standard session as shown by the dashed line. 12 Further studies of cue control have shown that visual cues which are most reliable or familiar tend to exert greatest stimulus control over preferred direction (Goodridge et al., 1998; Goodridge & Taube, 1995; Knierim et al., 1998; Taube & Burton, 1995; Taube, Goodridge, Golob, Dudchenko, & Stackman, 1996; Taube et al., 1990b). For example, cue control is strengthened when the animal is not disoriented before being placed in the arena for all screening sessions (Knierim et al.). In this case it is thought that the idiothetic cues that are maintained when the animal is placed in an unfamiliar environment become associated with the new allocentric cues in the environment, thereby allowing the allocentric cues to control the HD system in future exposures to the environment. While nearly all studies of landmark control of HD cells have utilized visual cues, Goodridge et al. (1998) tested for auditory cue influences over HD cells. In this study, they presented an auditory cue from one of four speakers mounted to the wall of the recording cylinder. Similar to the procedures described above to illustrate the influence of visual landmarks, the experimenters changed the position of the auditory cue by activating different speakers positioned around the arena to determine if the preferred direction of the recorded cell would likewise rotate. The room was darkened during experimental sessions in order to eliminate visual cues while testing. In spite of rotating the auditory cues by 90º, the HD cell preferred firing directions typically shifted a much smaller amount (37.8 + 12.9º on average), suggesting only weak control by the auditory cue compared to what is normally found with visual cue manipulations. 13 Despite the apparent lack of control of an auditory cue of the HD network described above, Rossier, Haeberli, and Schenk (2000) found that auditory cues were able to exert some behavioral control during a place navigation task when presented simultaneously with a visual cue. Specifically, rats were able to best discriminate the location of a platform within a water maze when two auditory cues, which served as beacons, and one visual cue were present. This was true when comparing sessions where they either removed the visual cue (leaving only the auditory cues) or removed the auditory cue (leaving only the visual cue). They concluded that perhaps rats will utilize different sensory modalities depending on the need for spatial discrimination during a particular navigational task. It has been known for many decades that rodents can exhibit accurate localization of sounds (Barber, 1915; Kelly & Masterton, 1977; Heffner & Heffner, 1985), suggesting that sounds could in theory serve as directional cues within the appropriate environmental set-up. There are at least two possible reasons for the lack of influence of auditory cues over HD cells in the experiments of Goodridge et al. (1998). First, it may be that this study did not eliminate all available visual cues during screening or testing sessions, which allowed these visual cues to compete for control over the HD system (Goodridge et al.; Mizumori & Williams, 1993). Second, the recording arena that was used may have made sound discrimination difficult (Goodridge, et al.; Rossier, et al., 2000). In their discussion, Goodridge et al. bring up this latter possibility that the geometric layout of the circular walled arena that was utilized may not have been conducive for accurate sound localization. Therefore, the one previous study examining the control of auditory cues on 14 the HD system may have been conducted in environmental conditions in which the auditory cues were perceived as an unreliable indication of directional heading. Proximal and Distal Cues Research using multiple visual cues has investigated the effects of landmarks that are near to the animal (proximal landmarks) and cues that are distant from the animal (distal landmarks). Proximal landmarks that have been used in such experiments have been visual, 3-dimensional, and/or tactile objects placed inside the arena, serving as intramaze allocentric cues, whereas distal landmarks have typically been visual cues that lie outside the arena, serving as extramaze cues (Cressant, Muller, & Poucet, 1997; Knierim, 2002; Renaudineau, Poucet, & Save, 2007; Save & Poucet, 2000; Yoganarasimha, Yu, & Knierim, 2006; Zugaro, Berthoz, & Wiener, 2001). Support for the role of the hippocampus in the utilization of both proximal and distal visual cues during navigational tasks was shown in Save and Poucet’s study where rats with hippocampal lesions were unable to properly utilize either type of cue. Differences in the characteristics of place cells and HD cells have been discovered with respect to proximal and distal cue control. When both proximal and distal visual cues are present, place cells can behave differently either between sessions or between animals. Unlike typical HD cell recordings, multiple place cells are often recorded simultaneously bringing up the possibility that not all cells will respond the same to a manipulation. When proximal and distal cues are rotated in opposite directions, causing a cue conflict situation, the place fields from all recorded place cells in the same animal may shift in concert with either the proximal or distal cues, or alternatively some place 15 cells may follow the proximal cues while others follow the distal cues. In addition, either all or some recorded place cells may cease firing, while others may establish entirely new place fields (Knierim, 2002; Renaudineau et al., 2007; Yoganarasimha et al., 2006). To summarize, control of place cells by proximal and distal visual cues seems to be unpredictable from session to session and from animal to animal. There have been a few studies which have examined proximal and distal visual cue control over the activity of HD cells. When examined, HD cells behave in a much different manner than place cells during many of the same environmental manipulations. When a cue conflict is presented between a set of proximal and a set of distal visual cues, almost all preferred directions will shift according to the distal cues (Yoganarasimha & Knierim, 2005; Yoganarasimha et al., 2006; Zugaro et al., 2001). It is unknown, however, if this same finding can be observed in the case of auditory cues. As in the case of a distal visual cue, there is reason to believe that a distal auditory cue may be more effective than a proximal cue at controlling the HD cell network. In theory, the animal may perceive a proximal auditory cue as having varying intensity levels depending on the position of the animal within the arena relative to the stable cue. This may cause the animal to perceive the proximal auditory cue as originating from slightly different locations as the animal locomotes within the arena. If the auditory cue appears to resonate from different locations as the animal explores the arena the cue may then be perceived as unstable and unreliable. In contrast, a distal auditory cue may be heard at a relatively stable intensity regardless of the animal’s location within the arena. If the sound is perceived as originating from the same location within the room regardless 16 of the position of the animal the cue may be perceived as more stable and reliable. Therefore, it may be more likely that when a conflict between proximal and distal auditory cues is presented, the distal auditory cue will exert greater stimulus control over the preferred direction of the recorded HD cell. The purpose of the current study was twofold: (1) To determine whether a complex auditory cue, consisting of simultaneously presented proximal and distal auditory sounds, would exert control over HD cell behavior and (2) To decipher whether a proximal or distal auditory cue would exert more salient control over the opposing auditory cue when the animal is presented with a cue conflict situation. 17 Chapter 2 METHOD Experimental Subjects Three female Long-Evans rats obtained from Simonsen Labs (Gilroy, CA) served as subjects for this study. They weighed between 208 and 303 grams at the start of the study. A food-restricted diet was used to establish the motivation for foraging within the experimental arena. This entailed maintaining the rats at approximately 85% of free feeding weight. Water was available ad libitum in the home cage. The rats were housed individually in clear plastic cages containing wood chips in a room that was maintained on a 16/8 hour light/dark cycle. The vivarium room temperature remained approximately 70º F. Individual cages were located next to each other in a row to maintain social contact. Materials Electrode Assembly Recording electrodes were constructed by the experimenter according to the methods described by Kubie (1984). They consisted of a bundle of ten 25-μm in diameter nichrome wires (California Fine Wire Co., Grover City, CA). The wires were insulated except at the tip and were threaded through a stainless steel cannula that was moveable in the dorsal/ventral direction after being fixed to the skull using dental acrylic. 18 Electrophysiological Recordings To detect cellular activity, electrical signals from the brain were recorded using a field-effect transistor in a source-follower configuration. Extracellular activity was amplified by a factor of 10,000-50,000 and bandpass filtered from 500 Hz to 30 KHz. These single-unit signals were passed through a dual window discriminator (BAK Electronics, Mount Airy, MD) and oscilloscope for electrical spike discrimination. Spike information was stored on a desktop computer for offline processing using custom analysis software (LabView; National Instruments, Austin, TX). Apparatus The same arena was used for all cell screening and experimental sessions. The recording arena was uniquely constructed by the experimenter in order to eliminate all obvious visual and tactile cues (see Figure 5). The geometric shape of a triangle was chosen due to potential difficulties of localizing sound through the walls of the traditional cylindrical arena (Goodridge et al., 1998). Neither a square nor rectangle was appropriate as rats have not been reported to consistently discriminate between diagonally opposite corners (Cheng, 1986; Golob, Stackman, Wong, & Taube, 2001; Margules & Gallistel, 1988). The floor of the triangular arena made a solid equilateral triangle with sides measuring at 94 cm in length. A short wall, measuring at 7 cm in height, bordered this platform. Both the floor and walls were sanded smooth so as to eliminate any obvious tactile cues. Black screen was attached to a thin wooden frame over the platform creating a pyramid-like enclosure in order to prevent the rat from trying to escape. This screen met 51 cm above the platform at a circular opening (9 cm in diameter) at which the 19 recording cable entered the arena. Velcro allowed the experimenter to fold down the top half of each of the three screen walls of the arena to enable placement of the rat inside. All wood was spray painted black in order to eliminate any obvious tactile (intramaze) cues that could be created by paintbrush strokes. The black paint also eliminated any obvious visual cues while the rat was inside the curtain area because this area was darkened at all times. 20 A B C P Arena P P D Curtain Figure 5. Representations of the triangular recording arena. A, Photograph showing the triangular recording arena, surrounded by three proximal speakers. It can be seen where the top of the three screen walls were folded down for placing the rat inside the arena. B, 21 Photograph showing the triangular recording arena from above. At the center is the circular opening where the headstage wire and food pellets entered the arena. C, Schematic showing the recording arena, surrounded by three proximal speakers (P), a mobile distal speaker (D), and a black floor-to-ceiling curtain. 22 The entire arena was elevated 61 cm above the room floor and rested on a wooden base, where three removable proximal speakers were fastened to wooden speaker stands using Velcro. The speakers were black and measured 16.5 cm tall and 9 cm wide. They stood 9 cm away from the arena and the bases of the speakers were level with the top of the triangular arena walls. Speakers were positioned facing inward at each point of the triangular arena so that the sound was directed into the center of the triangular arena. A fourth speaker (with identical physical measurements as the three proximal speakers) served as the distal cue. The distal speaker was positioned 91 cm directly behind one silent proximal speaker, adjacent to the proximal speaker that was active on a given trial (see Figure 5). A floor-to-ceiling black curtain surrounded the arena, eliminating all exterior visual cues. This curtain hung approximately 15 cm outside of the proximal speakers, with the distal speaker approximately 76 cm outside the curtain. The room that housed the enclosure had a food pellet dispenser mounted to the ceiling directly above the arena. The food dispenser released sugar pellets on a variable interval 30 seconds schedule in order to evoke foraging behaviors. Pellets fell through the circular opening at the top of the apparatus and bounced to random locations within the arena. Auditory Cues All speakers were elevated at equal heights and produced an auditory cue generated by a computer located within the recording room. The auditory cue was created by the experimenter using the LabView (National Instruments) computer program, and consisted of a 2 kHz (Heffner & Heffner, 1985; Kelly & Masterton, 1977; Polley, 23 Steinberg & Merzenich, 2006; Rossier et al., 2000) tone emitted from one consistent proximal and one consistent distal speaker position in an alternating pattern. Specifically, each tone sounded for one second and was followed by one and one half seconds between each tone, so that a persistent and alternating beeping was heard throughout all screening and testing sessions (see Figure 6). Intensity (dB) 24 100 80 60 40 20 Distal Proximal 1 2 3 Distal 4 5 Proximal 6 7 Time (Sec) Figure 6. Pattern of alternating tones emitted by distal and proximal speakers surrounding the triangular recording arena. 8 9 25 While the volume of the proximal and distal speakers was set to be equal at the speakers, the greater physical distance of the distal speaker from the center of the arena allowed for a difference of 10 decibels (dB) between the proximal and distal speakers (approximately 66 ½ dB for the proximal auditory cue, with a range of 65-68 dB throughout screenings, and approximately 56 dB for the distal auditory cue, with range of 54-58 dB throughout screenings), as detected by a sound level meter (Realistic, CAT. No. 33-2050) which was temporarily positioned in the center of the arena facing the sounding speaker before the screening session began (Bushnell, 1995; Heffner & Heffner, 1985; Kelly & Masterton, 1977; Polley et al., 2006). Sound level measurements were assessed six times throughout the 47 week duration of the study. The efforts made to enhance discriminability and salience of the auditory cues included: 1) alternating beeps between the speakers (as discussed above) so that there was a break between beeps (rather than one continuous sound emanating within the room), 2) allowing for a 10 dB difference to be heard between the proximal and distal auditory cues from the center of the arena, 3) searching for HD cells and conducting experimental sessions in a darkened room so that the opportunity to utilize visual cues was minimized, and 4) using a tone that has been previously found to be within the hearing range of rats (Bushnell, 1995; Heffner & Heffner, 1985; Kelly & Masterton, 1977; Polley et al., 2006; Rossier et al., 2000). 26 Procedure Surgical Procedure Surgical procedures for implantation of the recording electrode were conducted using an approved protocol from the CSUS Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Rats were anesthetized with an intra-muscular injection of a drug cocktail containing ketamine (30 mg/kg), xylazene (6 mg/kg), and acepromazine (1 mg/kg). The head was shaved and disinfected with betadine. The animal was then placed in a stereotaxic device for support of the head during electrode implantation. The top of the skull was exposed by a single incision. Six holes were drilled around the edges of the incision and stainless steel screws were placed in these holes to serve as anchors for the electrode assembly. Another hole was drilled in the right parietal plate based on the stereotaxic coordinates of 1.5 mm posterior to bregma and 1.3 mm lateral to bregma, provided by Paxinos and Watson (1998), to allow for the electrode to be lowered into the brain tissue. The electrode was implanted slightly above the ADN of the right hemisphere 3.7 mm below the dura mater. The electrode was secured to the screws using orthopedic cement. Once the electrode was secured, a topical antibiotic (Neosporin) was packed around the incision site and the incision was sutured closed. Analgesics were provided for 1-2 days following the surgery as needed. Animals were given seven days to recover from the surgery before the next phase of the experiment. 27 Screening Sessions for HD Cells When searching for HD cells (screening) the activated proximal speaker was positioned at 120º clockwise (CW) of the activated distal speaker (see Figure 7). The three identical proximal speakers were positioned around the arena in order to maintain symmetry and eliminate any potentially obvious visual cues. At the start of a session, the auditory cues were activated and the rat was brought into the experimental room in its home cage. The black curtain enclosed the arena so as to hide it from the rat’s view while the animal was plugged in to the recording equipment. The external area outside of the curtain was temporarily lighted, whereas the area within the curtain area remained dark. The experimenter randomly predetermined the rat’s side of entry into the arena by the roll of a die. After the rat was connected to the recording cable it was then brought through the curtain and placed into the darkened arena facing the center of the triangle through one of the three sides of the triangular enclosure. The experimenter secured the screen wall and exited the curtain from the location of entrance. The exterior lights were turned off and the door was closed quietly. The screening session began when the food pellet dispenser was activated from the adjoining room. It is notable that the animal was not given the disorientation treatment prior to screening sessions that was utilized for recording sessions (see below). This difference is based on evidence by Knierim et al., (1995) that when an animal is consistently disoriented prior to placement into an environment the HD network fails to be controlled by the relevant cues in the environment. 28 Figure 7. Representation of the triangular recording arena during screening sessions. All screening sessions were conducted with the proximal speaker at 120º CW, or to the right, of the distal speaker. 29 Each rat underwent screening sessions once per day on an average of three days per week. Screening for HD cells involved examining the electrical activity on each of the ten wires in the electrode as the rat foraged in the arena. This process lasted anywhere from 10-30 minutes per rat. Video tracking hardware (Ebtronics Corp.; Elmont, NY) monitored the behavior, head position, and directional orientation of the rat’s head. This tracking system detected the x and y coordinates (256 x 256) of red and green light emitting diodes (LEDs) secured to the recording headstage six cm apart, above the head and back of the animal, respectively. Tracking software used the coordinates of these LEDs to register both head position and head direction. The presence of directionally specific activity on an electrode wire is an indication of an isolated HD cell. If an HD cell was not identified during a given screening session, the electrode was advanced approximately 100 m and the animal was returned to the vivarium in its home cage. If an HD cell was identified, an experimental session was conducted either that same day or the following day. Experimental Sessions for HD Cells There were four experimental conditions for each HD cell tested. Figure 8 presents the procedure for the study. During testing, the experimenter turned off all exterior room lights and wore a headlamp when necessary to maneuver within the darkened room. A session began by placing the rat in a small cardboard box with a removable lid. There was an opening in the lid that allowed a small space for the recording cable to remain attached to the rat. The auditory cues were turned off and the experimenter cleaned the arena floor with household cleaning wipes in order to eliminate 30 any obvious olfactory cues. The speakers were positioned accordingly. The rat was given disorientation treatment by the experimenter slowly turning the box in full circles in one direction for approximately six seconds and then in the opposite direction for about the same duration. This was done in order to disrupt path integration processes and strengthen the rat’s dependence on experimenter-controlled cues (Biegler and Morris, 1996). After disorientation treatment the auditory cues were turned on. The rat was taken out of the box and placed into the arena. The experimenter’s headlamp was turned off, the experimenter left the curtain from a pseudorandom location, and the pellet dispenser was activated. Cellular activity was recorded for ten minutes while the animal foraged for food pellets. At the conclusion of each session the rat was removed from the arena, placed back in the box and the previously mentioned procedures were repeated for the next session. 31 Figure 8. Representations of the triangular recording arena during experimental sessions. The four conditions were: Standard, Rotate Both, Return to Standard, and Flip, with the position of the auditory cues manipulated between conditions. For the Standard condition, the proximal auditory cue was set at 120º CW to the distal auditory cue. For the Rotate Both condition, both auditory cues were rotated by 120º CW relative to the standard positions. For the Return to Standard condition, both auditory cues were returned to the same position as in the Standard condition with a CCW rotation of 120º. 32 For the Flip condition, the proximal auditory cue was rotated CCW by 120º and the distal auditory cue was rotated CW by 120º, so that they ended up flipping their relative positions. 33 Within the Standard condition the proximal and distal auditory cues maintained a similar relationship as within the screening sessions in that if the rat were to face the two sounds (hence, the side of the triangle between the two speakers) the distal sound could be heard on the left while the proximal sound could be heard on the right. The Rotate Both condition maintained this same sound relation while rotating both cues 120º in the CW direction. Therefore, if the rat was using the auditory cues together to establish head direction, the preferred head direction would be expected to rotate roughly CW 120º along with the auditory cues. In the Return to Standard condition, both sound cues were rotated CCW 120º, returning the cues to the same positions as in the Standard condition. Here again, if the rat was using the two auditory cues as a single landmark, the preferred head direction of the cell would be expected to rotate roughly CCW 120º relative to the previous session. The purpose of the Flip condition was to set up a cue conflict between the proximal and distal cue to determine if one cue exerts more control over the HD system than the other. This was accomplished by “flipping” the position of the proximal and distal cues, so that the proximal auditory cue was now located to the left of the distal auditory cue, when facing both cues from the center of the arena. If one auditory cue exerts stronger control than the other then it would be expected that the preferred direction would shift with the cue that exerts stronger control (Yoganarasimha & Knierim, 2005; Yoganarasimha et al., 2006). Data Analysis Circular statistics (Batschelet, 1981) were used to determine the stability of the directional signal for all HD cells. The preferred direction shifts between consecutive 34 sessions (i.e., the Standard and Rotate Both conditions, Rotate Both and Return to Standard conditions, and Return to Standard and Flip conditions) were examined to determine whether the recorded cells changed their directional specificity with the position of the auditory cues. These comparisons involved determining directional deviation scores across the compared sessions using a cross-correlation method (Taube & Burton, 1995). This analysis involves shifting the firing rate/HD function of the first condition in 6° increments while correlating this shifted function with the unshifted function from the comparison condition. The amount of shift required to produce the maximal Pearson r correlation between the two tuning curves is defined as the directional deviation score between the conditions. The directional deviation scores from the entire sample of cells were then subjected to Rayleigh tests (Batschelet, 1981) to determine if the scores were clustered randomly, as would expected if the preferred directions were not stable between the sessions. The critical statistic of the Rayleigh test is the mean vector length, r, which varies between 0 and 1, with higher values indicating that the distribution of directions is clustered nonrandomly. The directional deviation scores were also used to calculate the mean vector angle, m, which estimates the mean angle of the sample. In the case where the HD cells are controlled by the position of one or both auditory cues, it would be expected that the mean vector angle would shift 120°along with the position of the controlling cue(s). 35 Histological Verification of Recording Sites At the conclusion of recordings, the brains were processed for verification of recording locations. This involved deeply anesthetizing the animals and a small anodal current (20 μA, 10 seconds) was passed through the electrode wire(s), in order to conduct a Prussian blue reaction. The animals were then perfused transcardially with saline, followed by 10% formalin in saline. The brains were removed and placed in 10% formalin for at least 48 hours. The brains were then placed in a 10% formalin solution containing 2% potassium ferrocyanide for 24 hours and then rinsed in 10% formalin for several days. They were sectioned at 80 to 100 μm in the coronal plane, stained with cresyl violet, and examined microscopically for localization of the recording sites. 36 Chapter 3 RESULTS A total of eleven cells were recorded in three animals (range = 1- 6 cells per animal). Rats JD6 and JD7 had been exposed to the arena and auditory cues for seven screening sessions before conducting the first cue rotation session. Rat JD5 was exposed to the arena for 18 screening sessions before this testing occurred. With a typical screening session lasting ~ 20 minutes, all animals had been exposed to the auditory cues for at least 140 minutes prior to testing. Qualitative Analysis of Cellular Activity Cellular activity was examined subjectively throughout testing conditions to determine how shifting the position of the auditory cues influenced the preferred directions of the recorded cells. Representative tuning curves were produced by averaging firing rate over head directions for each condition. Resulting tuning curves were subjectively examined to determine directionality and preferred HD cell behavior. It was observed that all of the cells (11 of 11) exhibited directional tuning, with variations in strength and isolation for each cell and for each condition. Figure 9 presents example firing rate versus HD tuning curves for one representative HD cell from rat JD6 during experimental conditions. For this rat, the preferred direction of approx. 190º remained consistent throughout all four conditions. Thus, no shift of preferred direction was observed for this cell across the different conditions despite rotations of the auditory cues. Other cells showed a similar insensitivity to the position of the auditory cues, some 37 showing preferred directions remaining stationary relative to the cues, while other cells showed preferred directions that shifted randomly relative to the cues. Figure 10 shows tuning curves from one representative cell from JD5 where its preferred direction appeared to shift with the auditory cues during the Rotate Both session, but then the preferred direction remained the same for the Return to Standard and Flip sessions despite the moving auditory cues. In another cell (cell JD7-3), the preferred direction shifted in the opposite direction of the auditory cues during both the Standard to Rotate Both and Rotate Both to Return to Standard transitions (where the preferred direction returned back to the original preferred direction for the Return to Standard session), and appeared to move with the distal cue for the Flip session (Figure 11). Clearly there did not seem to be consistent findings of auditory cue control from one cell to another across the sample, or even from one session to another for individual HD cells. 38 Firing Rate (Spikes/Sec) . cell JD6-1 50 Standard 40 Rotate Both 30 Return to Standard Flip 20 10 0 0 60 120 180 240 300 360 Head Direction (Degrees) Figure 9. Representative firing rate/HD tuning curves for four conditions for cell JD6-1. The dashed line indicates the tuning curve during the Standard condition. The solid and bolded line indicates the tuning curve during the Rotate Both condition. The dotted line indicates the tuning curve during the Return to Standard condition. The shaded line indicates the tuning curve during the Flip condition. 39 Firing Rate (Spikes/Sec) . cell JD5-1 60 Standard 40 Rotate Both Return to Standard Flip 20 0 0 60 120 180 240 300 360 Head Direction (Degrees) Figure 10. Representative firing rate/HD tuning curves for four conditions for cell JD51. The dashed line indicates the tuning curve during the Standard condition. The solid and bolded line indicates the tuning curve during the Rotate Both condition. The dotted line indicates the tuning curve during the Return to Standard condition. The shaded line indicates the tuning curve during the Flip condition. 40 Firing Rate (Spikes/Sec) . cell JD7-3 30 25 20 15 10 5 0 Standard Rotate Both Return to Standard Flip 0 60 120 180 240 300 360 Head Direction (Degrees) Figure 11. Representative firing rate/HD tuning curves for four conditions for cell JD73. The bold and solid line indicates the tuning curve during the Standard condition. The dashed line indicates the tuning curve during the Rotate Both condition, representing a shift ~ 120º in the opposite direction from the auditory cue. The dotted line indicates the tuning curve during the Return to Standard condition, also showing an opposite of cue shift. The shaded line indicates the tuning curve during the Flip condition, representing a shift in the same direction as the distal auditory cue. 41 Quantitative Analysis of Cellular Data Figure 12 presents the shifts in preferred directions of all recorded HD cells for the three transitions between conditions that were examined. Rayleigh tests of these preferred direction shifts between conditions indicated that the shifts were clustered (i.e., distributed nonrandomly) for all three transitions [mean vector lengths (r) = 0.500, 0.889, and 0.840; mean vectors (m) = - 19.0º, - 6.6º, and 1.6º for Standard Vs Rotate Both, Rotate Both Vs Return to Standard, and Return to Standard Vs Flip conditions, respectively; ps < 0.05]. Generally a clustering of preferred directions after a landmark shift could indicate one of two results: either the preferred directions of the population tended to shift predictably with the shifting landmark (e.g., the shifting auditory cues), or the preferred directions of the population tended to remain the same as in the previous session despite the shift of the landmark. An examination of the observed preferred direction shifts provides the strongest support for the latter outcome. Specifically, for the Standard to Rotate Both transitions only three cells (27.3%) rotated in the same direction as the auditory cue (approximately 120º) and one cell rotated approximately 120º in the opposite direction from the cue. The remaining seven cells (63.6%) rotated approximately 0º (i.e., showed no change in preferred direction) for this transition. In the Rotate Both to Return to Standard transitions, one cell rotated approximately 120º in the opposite direction from the cue while the remaining cells failed to rotate at all. Finally, in the Return to Standard to Flip transition, once again the majority of the cells failed to rotate at all, while one cell rotated with the proximal auditory cue and another cell appeared to rotate with the distal auditory cue. 42 If P im rox If D ist al al Figure 12. Scatter diagrams illustrating the amount of angular shift between experimental conditions. The left column shows the amount of preferred direct angular shift in degrees observed between the Standard and Rotate Both conditions. The middle column shows the amount of shift observed between the Rotate Both and Return to Standard conditions. The right column shows the amount of shift observed between the Return Standard and Flip conditions. The position of each filled circle represents the amount of angular shift of the preferred direction of a single cell between the first and second conditions. The curved arrows in each panel indicate the amount and direction of shift of the auditory cues between the two sessions, with the distal cues shifting clockwise and the proximal cues shifting counterclockwise in the third transition. The solid arrow denotes the expected mean vector angle if the angular shift is perfectly predicted by the auditory cues and the dotted arrow denotes the observed mean vector angle. The length of the dotted arrow denotes the mean vector length, with a length of 1.0 (no variability in shift scores) represented by a vector spanning the radius of the circle. Each plot uses Cartesian coordinates with 0º at the 3 o’clock position and increasing degree values proceeding in a CCW direction. 43 Taken together, poor auditory cue control was shown over the recorded HD cells, and this is especially apparent when the results of each transition are considered across individual cells and individual animals. Table 1 shows the results of each condition transition for each cell from each of the three animals (JD5, JD6, and JD7). For the purposes of this table, the outcome of each transition was coded as: 1) “Cued Shift” if the preferred direction shifted within 24º of the new position of the auditory cue(s), 2) “Opposite Shift” if the preferred direction was within 24º of where the auditory cue(s) would have been if they had been shifted in the opposite direction, 3) “No Shift” if the preferred direction shift remained within 18° of its preferred direction for the previous session, and 4) “Ambiguous Shift” if the preferred direction shift did not meet any of the above criteria. As described above, the most common outcome of a transition was a preferred direction that was maintained despite shifting auditory cues (i.e., the “No Shift” outcome). It is notable that while the preferred direction shifted in the same direction as the auditory cues in at least five transitions (see Table 1), in each of these instances this apparent cue control occurred for only one of the three transitions for that particular cell. Hence, cue control did not seem to be cell specific; rather it seemed to occur randomly on some sessions but not others, even within the same cell. These results support the conclusion that anterior thalamic HD cells do not respond to auditory cues, regardless of whether the cue is located proximal or distal to the rat. 44 Table 1 Shift Outcomes for Session Transitions Cell Standard Vs. Rotate Both Vs. Return Return Both Rotate Both Standard Transition Vs. Flip Transition Transition JD5-1 Cued Shift No Shift No Shift JD5-2 Cued Shift No Shift No Shift JD6-1 No Shift No Shift No Shift JD6-2 No Shift No Shift No Shift JD6-3 No Shift No Shift No Shift JD6-4 No Shift Ambiguous Shift No Shift JD6-5 No Shift No Shift No Shift JD6-6 No Shift No Shift No Shift JD7-1 Cued Shift No Shift No Shift JD7-2 No Shift No Shift Cued Shift JD7-3 Opposite Shift Opposite Shift Cued Shift Note: See text for definitions of transition outcomes. 45 Point of Entry Analysis Due to the fact that some cells showed occasional shifts of approximately 120º, but these shifts appeared largely unrelated to the position of the auditory cues, the experimenter examined the possibility that the rat’s point of entry (POE) into the arena may have been controlling the preferred directions of recorded cells. This possibility is supported by a place cell study that found that the addition of a second visual cue leading to a visually symmetrical environment (i.e., a cylindrical shaped environment with two white cards located 180º from each other) resulted in place field locations controlled by the direction of entry into the environment (Sharp, Kubie, & Muller, 1990). The fact that POE was randomly altered in 120º increments in this study, and that the cells occasionally seemed to shift in approximately the same amounts made it important to test the possibility that POE exerted control over the preferred directions of recorded cells. This analysis involved determining what the predicted preferred direction shifts between two consecutive conditions would have been if the POE was the controlling variable. Based on the geometry of the apparatus, at the beginning of each condition animals entered the apparatus facing one of three randomly determined directions: 30º, 150º, and 270º. If the POE was truly the variable that determined the preferred direction of a given condition, then the preferred direction shift between conditions should equal the POE shift between those conditions. As an example, if an animal entered the Standard condition at a POE of 30º and entered the Rotate Both condition with the same POE of 30º, and POE was truly the variable controlling preferred direction, then the predicted preferred direction shift during the transition between those conditions would 46 be 0º. In contrast, if the POE for the Standard condition was 30º and the Rotate Both condition POE was 150º, then the shift in preferred direction between the conditions (again assuming POE is the controlling factor) would be predicted to be +120º. Figure 13 presents these shifts in preferred directions of HD cells for the three transitions relative to the POE between the sessions. Rayleigh tests of these preferred direction shifts between conditions indicated that the shifts were not clustered (i.e., they were distributed randomly) for all three transitions [mean vector lengths (r) = 0.174, 0.152, and 0.170; mean vectors (m) = - 29.5º, 29.5º, and 129.8º for Standard Vs Rotate Both, Rotate Both Vs Return to Standard, and Return to Standard Vs Flip conditions, respectively; ps > 0.05]. Because shifts were distributed randomly relative to the POE shift between sessions, this is strong evidence that the POE did not appear to have control over HD cell behavior. In other words, if the rat had used the POE into the arena as the salient cue, then the shift in preferred directions relative to POE shifts would have been clustered around zero degrees. This was clearly not the case. At first glance, it may seem odd that the preferred direction shifts relative to the POE as shown in the figure usually varied in increments of 120º, however that is explained by the fact that most cells did not shift between conditions (see Figure 12), while POE did shift randomly between conditions in multiples of 120º. 47 Figure 13. Scatter diagrams illustrating the amount of angular shift between the observed preferred directions of recorded cells and the predicted shift of preferred direction if the cells were using the point of entry (POE) into the recording enclosure. In each transition, the expected shift would be zero degrees (solid arrow) if the cells were using POE to maintain a consistent preferred direction. The solid arrow denotes the expected mean vector angle if the angular shift is perfectly predicted by the POE and the dotted arrow denotes the observed mean vector angle. The length of the dotted arrow denotes the mean vector length, with a length of 1.0 (no variability in shift scores) represented by a vector spanning the radius of the circle. Each plot uses Cartesian coordinates with 0º at the 3 o’clock position and increasing values proceeding in a CCW direction. 48 Histology At the completion of the study, histological processing was performed on two of the three animals (JD6 and JD7). This analysis could not be performed on the third animal (JD5) due to premature electrode failure. Figure 14 presents a photomicrograph from the rat in which the majority of cells were recorded (n = 6). For this rat, the Prussian Blue reaction was performed on the two wires on which the HD cells were recorded. The dark spots within the ADN and anterior ventral nucleus (AVN) represent the position of the wires at the time of animal sacrifice. While it is unclear as to the precise thalamic nucleus containing each recorded HD cell in this animal (ADN or AVN), it is apparent that the cells were localized in one of those two nuclei of the anterior thalamus. The second animal showed an electrode localized to the ADN. 49 M2 RSA M1 S1HL S1FL S1DZ RSGb cg S1BF IG cc df DHC dhc LV fi sm st CA3 DG D3V S2 PC PT iml CM IAM mt ic ns mfb VA Rt AMV DI Re VRe SPa PaV cst LGP VM IPAC LaDL PaDC PaLM SI LH VEn MeAD Pir BMA Pe RCh BLA IM SO sox AIP DEn CeM CeL CeC AHP AHC 3V Cl ZI PaMP opt GI CPu IAD AM Sub Xi f AVVL AVDM Rh rf al LDVL MD PVA ec AD MHb S1 BAOT ACo CxA LDN AVN ADN Figure 14. Illustration of histology for rat JD6. A diagrammatic representation of a coronal section of the rat brain at the level of the electrode placement (reprinted from 50 Paxinos and Watson, 1998) is shown at the top of the figure. The bottom of the figure shows an example brain slice from rat JD6. Two of the ten wires were isolated within the anterior thalamus as can be seen from the darkened stains from the Prussian blue. 51 Chapter 4 DISCUSSION The purpose of the current study was twofold: (1) To determine whether a complex auditory cue, consisting of proximal and distal auditory sounds presented in synchrony, would exert control over HD cell behavior when significant steps were taken to minimize the saliency of other landmarks in the environment and (2) To determine whether either a proximal or distal auditory cue would exert more salient control over the HD network than the opposing auditory cue when the animal is presented with a cue conflict situation. The results of the current study showed that under these experimental conditions, auditory cues do not exert control over anterior thalamic HD cells. It is not clear why inconsistent patterns for each rat and each cell were found. The mixed patterns seem to be somewhat animal specific in that none of the HD cells recorded from JD6 showed a cued shift in their preferred directions for any of the three rotation transitions but all cells from each of the other two animals showed one (and only one) transition that was accurately predicted from the cue (see Table 1). It is unusual for HD cells recorded from the ADN to respond so inconsistently, as they characteristically tend to either follow landmarks or not, rather than exhibiting landmark control on some sessions but not others within a single experiment (e.g., Taube, 1995). The least predictable outcome was seen in cell JD7-3, where the preferred direction shifted for all transitions, but only one of the shifts was in the same direction as the auditory cue (see Figure 11 and Table 1). 52 The fact that the HD cells were able to establish a stable preferred direction that usually did not shift between sessions shows that there was a cue controlling HD cell directional specificity. However, it is unlikely that this stable salient cue was auditory, visual, or olfactory because specific steps were taken to eliminate the influence of these extraneous landmarks. In addition, the observation that preferred directions seemed to shift randomly in some sessions points to the possibility that if a landmark was being used, it was not always stable between sessions. There are various possible explanations for why the current results were obtained. First, it is possible that even though the rats could hear the proximal and distal sounds, they could not discriminate between them. The lack of direct evidence that the sounds used were discriminable is the largest limitation of the current study. The sounds used were set at a frequency (2 kHz) and intensities (~ 66.5 dB for the proximal auditory cue and ~ 56 dB for the distal auditory cue) that have been shown to be within rodents’ hearing range: 1-Hz - 80 kHz at 35 - 70 db sound pressure level (SPL) (Bushnell, 1995; Goodridge et al., 1998; Heffner & Heffner, 1985; Kelly & Masterton, 1977; Polley et al., 2006; Rossier et al., 2000). Also, rats have exhibited the ability to localize sounds (Barber, 1915; Heffner & Heffner; Kelly & Masterton) however; perhaps the rats in the current study were not able to determine the location of the two sounds given the specific recording environment utilized. This may be true despite efforts to enhance discriminability, or maximize the likelihood that the HD cells would utilize the information from the auditory cues, such as alternating the beeps between the speakers so 53 that there was a break in between beeps (rather than one continuous sound emanating within the room). Second, even if the locations of the auditory cues were discriminable, it is possible that the animal (or HD network) failed to properly “attend” to the sounds consistently. Additional research might examine these two issues by utilizing behavioral measures to ensure that the animals can discriminate between the positions of the auditory cues used in the current study. For example, an experiment might attempt to train an unoperated rat to perform a particular behavior, such as pressing a lever in the presence of one sound, but not the other. Once it has been determined that the animal can discriminate the location of a simple sound cue, then further experiments may be performed to verify the ability to make the more complicated discriminations between proximal and distal auditory cues. Another approach would be to examine whether behavioral training to discriminate sound location leads to HD cells that respond to auditory cues. This would further address the issue of whether the influence on the HD network by an auditory cue requires the animal to attend to the cue. In other words, will a rat that has undergone behavioral training to discriminate the location of an auditory cue possess HD cells that are more controlled by the auditory cue than an animal that has not had the same behavioral training? Lastly, an attempt may be made to not only train animals to discriminate the location of an auditory cue, but to use this cue for directional orientation in a navigation task. 54 Studies have shown that behavior and head direction can be controlled either by independent cues or by the same cue depending on the type of task and procedures used (Dudchenko & Taube, 1997; Golob et al., 2001; Martin, Harley, Smith, Hoyles, & Hynes, 1997; Muir & Taube, 2002; Muir & Taube, 2004). These studies have shown evidence leading to the possible conclusion that the correlation between HD cell behavior and the animal’s overt behavior will vary under different settings and for HD cells located in different brain regions. Dudchenko and Taube (1997) used an eight-arm radial maze to examine the correlation between spatial behavior and a visual cue for HD cells located within PoS and ADN. They found that HD cell behavior was not directly related to the location of a reward for a reference memory task. Therefore, the HD system was shown to be independent of behavior under those conditions because the directional signal carried by the HD network was not directly influenced by the reinforcement contingencies. Muir and Taube (2002) reviewed all studies which had examined HD cell behavior during various behavioral tasks utilizing reward contingencies and they concluded that rats may not consistently rely on their sense of direction to guide behavior on all spatial tasks. However, the HD network activity may correspond with behavior if the location of the reward and cues remain consistent over repeated trials (or the animal learns over the course of training) and if the rat’s point of entry to the environment is consistent with these cues. In other words, there must be consistency with both the allocentric cues and the rat’s point of entry into the arena. A future study may mimic the present experiment by adding a similar behavioral component where a rat performs a 55 reference memory task in which the proximal and distal auditory cues remain in the same relation to each other and they consistently predict a reward location. In addition, the rat would not have random point of entries into the environment during screening sessions, as in the current study, but would instead have the same entrance location into the arena. The current study varied the point of entry for all testing sessions in order to lessen the possibility that the HD cell would use the consistent point of entry as the salient cue. Conversely, given the complex relationship between the HD network and the animal’s behavior, varying the point of entry during testing may have lessened the HD cell’s dependency on the auditory cues within the current study. A third possible explanation for the present findings may be that HD cells, similar to place cells, may behave differently either between sessions, animals, or brain regions with respect to proximal and distal auditory cue rotations. Future investigations might record more than one HD cell within the same brain region during the same session in order to see if they respond similarly after auditory cue rotations. Although this phenomenon has not been observed for HD cells, perhaps one HD cell located within the ADN would respond differently to auditory cue rotations than would a different HD cell also within the ADN. Likewise, it is possible that HD cells in other brain regions (i.e., LDN) may respond differently from HD cells located in the ADN. Goodridge, et al. (1998) examined auditory landmark control for only one PoS HD cell. This HD cell was said to have responded similarly to the ADN cells, but perhaps a larger sample size would have shown different results. 56 A fourth explanation for the current findings is that the HD cells were utilizing the geometric shape of the environment in order to establish a preferred direction, as was observed in the study conducted by Golob et al. (2001) where the HD cells could not distinguish diagonally opposite corners in a square or rectangular arena. This might explain why shifts of ~ 120º occurred in the wrong direction and for only some transitions for rats JD5 and JD7. If these HD cells were using a corner as their cue, it would be reasonable for them to utilize different corners throughout testing. One notable interpretation of the current findings of poor auditory cue control over HD cell behavior is that the typical procedure for turning on white noise to mask auditory cues during experimental sessions is apparently unnecessary. This has been standard procedure for decades as it was assumed that HD cell behavior might be controlled by an auditory cue perceptible from outside the recording arena. Eliminating this aspect to the typical procedures may be acceptable since the present findings suggest that it is unlikely that latent/ambient auditory cues, such as the sound of the door closing after the experimenter, would exert HD cell control. If a latent auditory cue were strong enough to establish cue control it would be expected that the prominent and consistent beeping in the current experiment would have exerted cue control for all transitions for at least one HD cell. In summary, the current study has shown that unlike salient visual cues, salient auditory cues do not exert stimulus control over HD cells located within the ADN when rats are given a complex cue consisting of both proximal and distal sounds. Despite many efforts made throughout experimentation to enhance both the saliency and 57 discriminability of the auditory cue(s), the persistent lack of auditory cue control might suggest that the brain circuitry directly connected to the ADN is not tight coupled with auditory centers. Perhaps these HD cells are more strongly linked to the neural pathways that process visual, tactile, olfactory, and vestibular electrical signals. The results from this experiment may aide future researchers in determining the neighboring neural pathways in order to better understand the behavioral characteristics and purpose of HD cells located within the anterior thalamus. Uncovering these additional fragments will contribute to the body of knowledge that seeks to understand how HD cells aid an animal during navigation. 58 REFERENCES Barber, A. G. (1915). Localization of sound in the white rat. Journal of Animal Behavior, 5(4), 292-311. Batschelet, E. (1981). Circular statistics in biology. London: Academic Press. Biegler, R., & Morris, R. G. M. (1996). Landmark stability: Further studies pointing to a role in spatial learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49B(4), 307-345. Blair, H. T., & Sharp, P. E. (1996). Visual and vestibular influences on head-direction cells in the anterior thalamus of the rat. Behavioral Neuroscience, 110(4), 643660. Bushnell, P. J. (1995). Over orienting in the rat: Parametric studies of cued detection of visual targets. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109(6), 1095-1105. Calton, J. L., Stackman, R. W., Goodridge, J. P., Archey, W. B., Dudchenko, P. A., & Taube, T. S. (2003). Hippocampal place cell instability after lesions of the head direction cell network. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 9719-9731. Cho, J., & Sharp, P. E. (2001). Head direction, place, and movement correlates for cells in the rat retrosplenial cortex. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115(1), 3-25. Cheng, K. (1986). A purely geometric module in the rat’s spatial representation. Cognition, 23, 149-178. Cressant, A., Muller, R. U., & Poucet B. (1997). Failure of centrally placed objects to control the firing fields of hippocampal place cells. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17(7), 2531-2542. 59 Dudchenko, P. A., & Taube, J. S. (1997). Correlation between head direction cell activity and spatial behavior on a radial arm maze. Behavioral Neuroscience, 111(1), 3-19. Golob, E. J., Stackman, R. W., Wong, A.C., & Taube, J. S. (2001). On the behavioral significance of head direction cells: Neural and behavioral dynamics during spatial memory tasks. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115(2), 285-304. Goodridge, J. P., Dudchenko, P. A, Worboys, K. A., Golob, E. J., & Taube, J. S. (1998). Cue control and head direction cells. Behavioral Neuroscience, 112, 749-761. Goodridge, J. P., & Taube, J. S. (1995). Preferential use of landmark navigational system by head direction cells in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109(1), 49-61. Heffner, H. E., & R. S. Heffner (1985). Hearing in two cricetid rodents: Wood rat (Neotoma floridana) and Grasshopper mouse (Onychomys leucogaster). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 99(3), 275-288. Jung, M. W., & McNaughton, B. L. (1993). Spatial selectivity of unit activity in the hippocampal granule layer. Hippocampus, 3(1), 165-182. Kelly, J. B., & Masterton, B. (1977). Auditory sensitivity of the albino rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 91(4), 930-936. Knierim, J. J. (2002). Dynamic interactions between local surface cues, distal landmarks, and intrinsic circuitry in hippocampal place cells. The Journal of Neuroscience, 22(14), 6254-6264. Knierim, J. J., Kudrimoti, H. S., & McNaughton, B. L. (1998). Place cells, head direction cells, and the learning of landmark stability. The Journal of Neuroscience, 15(3), 1648-1659. 60 Kubie, J. L. (1984). A driveable bundle microwires for collecting single-unit data from freely-moving rats. Physiology & Behavior, 32(1), 115-118. Leutgeb, S. Ragozzino, K. E., & Mizumori, S. J. Y. (2000). Convergence of head direction and place information in the CA1 region of hippocampus. Neuroscience, 100, 11-19. Martin, G. M., Harley, C. W., Smith, A. R., Hoyles, E. S., & Hynes, C. A. (1997). Spatial disorientation blocks reliable goal location on a plus maze but does not prevent goal location in the Morris maze. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 23(2), 183-193. Margules, J., & Gallistel, C. R. (1988). Heading in the rat: Determination by environmental shape. Animal Learning and Behavior, 16(4), 404-410. Mizumori, S. J., & Williams, J. D. (1993). Directionally selective mnemonic properties of neurons in the lateral dorsal nucleus of the thalamus of rats. The Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 4015-4028. Muir, G. M., Brown, J.E., Carey, J.P., Hirvonent, T.P., Della Santa, C.C., Minor, L.B., & Taube, J.S. (2009). Disruption of the head direction cell signal after occlusion of the semicircular canals in the freely moving chinchilla. Journal of Neuroscience, 46, 14521-14533. Muller R. U. (1996). A quarter of a century of place cells. Neuron, 17(5), 813-822. Muir, G. M., & Taube, J. S. (2002). The neural correlates of navigation: Do head direction and place cells guide spatial behavior? Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 1(4), 297-317. 61 Muir, G. M., & Taube, J. S. (2004). Head direction cell activity and behavior in a navigation task requiring a cognitive mapping strategy. Behavioural Brain Research, 153, 249-253. O’Keefe, J., & Dostrovsky, J. (1971). The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Research, 34, 171-175. O’Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. (1979). Précis of O’Keefe and Nadel’s: The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2(4), 487-533. Paxinos, G., & Watson, C. (1998). The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Polley, D. B., Steinberg, E. E., & Merzenich, M. M. (2006). Perceptual learning directs auditory cortical map reorganization through top-down influences. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(18), 4970-4982. Renaudineau, S., Poucet, B., & Save, E. (2007). Flexible use of proximal objects and distal cues by hippocampal place cells. Hippocampus, 00, 000-000. Robertson, R. G., Rolls, E. T., Georges-Francois, P., & Panzeri, S. (1999). Head direction cells in the primate pre-subiculum. Hippocampus, 9(3), 206-219. Rossier, J. Haeberli, C., & Schenk, F. (2000). Auditory cues support place navigation in rats when associated with a visual cue. Behavioural Brain Research, 117, 209-214. Save, E., & Poucet, B. (2000). Involvement of the hippocampus and associative parietal cortex in the use of proximal and distal landmarks for navigation. Behavioural Brain Research, 109, 195-206. 62 Sharp, P. E., Kubie, J. L., & Muller, R. U. (1990). Firing properties of hippocampal neurons in a visually symmetrical environment: Contributions of multiple sensory cues and mnemonic processes. Journal of Neuroscience, 10, 3093-3105. Sharp, P. E., Tinkelman, A., & Cho, J. (2001). Angular velocity and head direction signals recorded from the dorsal tegmental nucleus of Gudden in the rat: Implications for path integration in the head direction cell circuit. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115(3), 571-588. Stackman, R. W., & Taube, J. S. (1998). ‘Firing properties of rat lateral mammillary single units: Head direction, head pitch, and angular head velocity:’ Erratum. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23(4), 1555-1556. Taube, J. S. (1995). Head direction cells recorded in the anterior thalamic nuclei of freely moving rats. The Journal of Neuroscience, 15(1), 70-86. Taube, J. S., & Bassett, J. P. (2003). Persistent neural activity in head direction cells. Cerebral Cortex, 13, 1162-1172. Taube, J. S., & Burton, H. L. (1995). Head direction cell activity monitored in a novel environment and during a cue conflict situation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 74(5), 1953-1971. Taube, J. S., Goodridge, J. P., Golob, E. J., Dudchenko, P. A., & Stackman R. W. (1996). Processing the head direction cell signal: A review and commentary. Brain Research Bulletin, 40(5-6), 477-486. 63 Taube, J. S., & Muller, R. U. (1998). Comparisons of head direction cell activity in the postsubiculum and anterior thalamus of freely moving rats. Hippocampus, 8(2), 87-108. Taube, J. S., Muller, R. U., & Ranck, Jr., J. B. (1990). Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. I. Description and quantitative analysis. The Journal of Neuroscience, 10, 420-435. Taube, J. S., Muller, R. U., & Ranck, Jr., J. B. (1990b). Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. II. Effects of environmental manipulations. The Journal of Neuroscience, 10, 436-447. Tulving, E., & Markowitsch, H. J. (1998). Episodic and declarative memory: Role of the hippocampus. Hippocampus, 8, 198-204. Vargha-Khadem, F., Gadian, D. G., Watkins, K. E., Connelly, A., Van Paesschen, W., & Mishkin, M. (1997). Differential effects of early hippocampal pathology on episodic and semantic memory. Science, 277, 376-380. Warburton, E. C., Baird, A. L., Morgan, A., Muir, J. L., & Aggleton, J. P. (2001). The conjoint importance of the hippocampus and anterior thalamic nuclei for allocentric spatial learning: Evidence from a disconnection study in the rat. The Journal of Neuroscience, 21(18), 7323-7330. Wiener, S. I., Berthoz, A., & Zugaro, M. B. (2002). Multisensory processing in the elaboration of place and head direction responses by limbic system neurons. Cognitive Brain Research, 14, 75-90. 64 Wiener, S. I., & Taube, J. S. (Eds.). (2005). Head direction cells and the neural mechanisms of spatial orientation. Cambridge: The MIT Press, 5 Cambridge Center. Yoder, R. M., & Taube, J. S. (2009). Head direction cell activity in mice: Robust directional signal depends on intact otolith organs. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 1061-1076. Yoganarasimha, D., & Knierim, J. J. (2005). Coupling between place cells and head direction cells during relative translations and rotations of distal landmarks. Experimental Brain Research, 160, 344-359. Yoganarasimha, D., Yu, X., & Knierim, J. J. (2006). Head direction cell representation maintains internal coherence during conflicting proximal and distal cue rotations: Comparison with hippocampal place cells. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26(2), 622-631. Zugaro, M. B., Berthoz, A., & Wiener, S. I. (2001). Background, but not foreground, spatial cues are taken as references for head direction responses by rat anterodorsal thalamus neurons. The Journal of Neuroscience, 21(14), 1-5. Zugaro, M. B., Arleo, A. Berthoz, A., & Wiener, S. I. (2003). Rapid spatial reorientation and head direction cells. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 3478-3482.