ATTITUDES AND BELIEFS TOWARDS LGBTQ COMMUNITY IN A COLLEGE CAMPUS A Thesis Presented to the faculty of the Division of Social Work California State University, Sacramento Submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SOCIAL WORK by Nicolas Caballero SPRING 2013 © 2013 Nicolas Caballero ALL RIGHTS RESERVED ii ATTITUDES AND BELIEFS TOWARDS LGBTQ COMMUNITY IN A COLLEGE CAMPUS A Thesis by Nicolas Caballero Approved by: __________________________________, Committee Chair Jude Antonyappan, Ph.D. __________________________________, Second Reader David Nylund, Ph.D. ____________________________ Date iii Student: Nicolas Caballero I certify that this student has met the requirements for format contained in the University format manual, and that this thesis is suitable for shelving in the Library and credit is to be awarded for the thesis. __________________________, Graduate Coordinator___________________ Dale Russell, Ed.D., LCSW Date Division of Social Work iv Abstract of ATTITUDES AND BELIEFS TOWARDS LGBTQ COMMUNITY IN A COLLEGE CAMPUS by Nicolas Caballero This study examined possible predictors such as hypermasculinity and ethnicity in explaining negative attitudes towards the LGBTQ student community in a college campus in the context of queer theory within the paradigm of social constructionism. A non-probability sample of 158 undergraduate students was studied using the Hypermasculinity Inventory (HMI – Modified) consisting of 26 scale questions and Herek’s (1988) Attitudes Toward Lesbians and Gay Men (ATLG) scale. Demographic information such as age, ethnicity, college major and gender were collected to contextualize the study responses. The hypothesis that hypermasculinity scores are significantly correlated with negative attitudes towards LGBTQ population was tested as was the hypothesis that ethnic background is correlated with negative attitudes towards individuals with the LGBTQ identity. Study findings indicate a positive weak correlation of (r =. 227, N=158, p<. 05) between hypermasculinity and negative attitudes towards LGBTQ and a positive weak correlation between ethnicity and attitudes toward LGBTQ population (r=. 154, N=158, p<. 05). In light of the small value of correlation v coefficients the following predictions must be considered with caution. The regression equations are: Negative attitudes toward LGBT community members (Y) = .309 + .332 (hypermasculinity) and Negative attitudes toward LGBT community (Y) = - .199 (ethnicity) + 2.193. Although the possible levels of variations that can be explained in the dependent variable, the negative attitudes toward LGBTQ population, due to the variation in the independent variables of hyper masculinity and ethnicity are relatively small the fact that as scores on hypermasculinity increases, the scores on negative attitudes also increases with an ascending slope is a significant finding in this study. This relationship is worthy of further investigation. These findings need further examination with random samples and more rigorous standardized scales to measure the relationship between negative attitudes toward LGBTQ population in particular and the hypermasculinity scores. The findings hold relevant implications for the field of social work in broadening the narrowly defined gender conceptualizations thus warranting advocacy at the macro level, and in providing direct services to clients/students. _______________________, Committee Chair Jude Antonyappan, Ph.D. _______________________ Date vi ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to dedicate this project to the LGBTQ community in Sacramento and CSU, Sacramento. Since the first day that I arrived to Sacramento in 2011 I made it a personal goal to be part of the change and advancement of justice and equal rights for LGBTQ in this community -- this project represents a small step towards that change. “ A strong community is an organized community” #levantatucara I would like to thank The Sacramento Sisters of Perpetual Indulgence, Holders of Hopes & Dreams for giving me the orotundity to be part of such an amazing movement in modern queer history. Thank you to the sister with long beautiful legs who save my life, Sister Frida Oppressed – ME. Thank you to all the amazing people that I have met in this past years. Thank you to my family for supporting me in this journey. Finally, special thanks to Professor Jude Antonyappan, Ph.D. without her compassionate wisdom and unconditional support I would not have been able to complete this project, thank you professor. vii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page Acknowledgments...................................................................................................... vii List of Tables .............................................................................................................. xi Chapter 1. THE PROBLEM …………………………………………..………………...….. 1 Introduction ……………….. .......................................................................... 1 Background of the Problem ............................................................................ 3 Purpose of the Study ....................................................................................... 4 Definition of Terms ........................................................................................ 5 Theoretical Framework .................................................................................. 6 Justifications ................................................................................................... 8 Delimitations ................................................................................................ 10 Assumptions ................................................................................................. 10 2. REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE ...................................................................... 12 Introduction ......................................................................................................... 12 The Effects of Homophobic School Environment .............................................. 14 LGB Identity Formation .................................................................................... 16 LGBTQ Attitude Development .......................................................................... 19 Prejudice ............................................................................................................. 21 Heteronormativity and Homophobia ................................................................. 22 viii Masculinity and Femininity ............................................................................... 25 Elements of the Construct of Hypermasculinity ................................................. 28 Risk Correlated To Hypermasculinity ................................................................ 30 Conclusion .......................................................................................................... 32 3. METHODOLOGY ............................................................................................... 33 Research Question ..........................................................................................33 Study Design ...................................................................................................33 Sampling Procedures .......................................................................................34 Data Collection Procedures..............................................................................35 Instruments .......................................................................................................36 Proposed Statistical Analyses ..........................................................................37 Protection of Human Subjects .........................................................................37 4. RESULTS ............................................................................................................. 39 Study Findings And Discussions .................................................................... 39 Demographics ................................................................................................. 40 Summary ......................................................................................................... 49 5. DISCUSSION ....................................................................................................... 50 Conclusion, Summary, Recommendations ..................................................... 50 Summary ....................................................................................................... 50 Conclusion ...................................................................................................... 51 Limitations ...................................................................................................... 53 Recommendations .......................................................................................... 54 Social Work Implications ............................................................................... 56 Appendix A. Human Subjects Approval Letter ....................................................... 58 ix Appendix B. Consent Form ...................................................................................... 60 Appendix B. Study Questionnaire ............................................................................ 62 References……………………….…………………………………………………...74 x LIST OF TABLES Tables Page 1. Demographics - Gender and Age of Study Participants (N=158) ………….....40 2. Demographics - Ethnicity of Study Participants (N=158)..……………………41 3. Demographics – College Major of Study Participants (N=158)…….…………42 4. Hypermasculinity Mean Model Summary (N=158)..…...………………..……43 5. Hypermasculinity Mean ANOVA (N=158) ………………………….………. 44 6. Hypermasculinity Mean Coefficients (N=158) .…………………………. …..45 7. Ethnicity Model Summary (N=158) …..………………………………………46 8. Ethnicity ANOVA (N=158)..…………………..………………………………47 9. Ethnicity Coefficients (N=158)..……………....……………………………….47 10. Correlation Analysis (N=158)...………………………………………………. 48 xi 1 Chapter 1 THE PROBLEM Introduction College years can have a great impact on the development of an identity for students who devote their efforts to scholarship and education both inside and outside the classroom. Students are exposed to new ideas, cultures, people, and ideologies that challenge their initial beliefs creating an environment conducive to growth as an individual as well as the possibility of self-actualization. Though academic demands coupled with exposure to new experiences could potentially create intellectual and emotional experiences that could be perceived as terrifying, demanding, and even exhausting – these experiences are often memorable at the same time. While these experiences are often positive, some students find that they have unique challenges because of how they are perceived and treated as a result of their sexual orientation or gender identity. Such challenges can prevent lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender and queer (LGBTQ) students from achieving their full academic potential or participating fully in the campus community. LGBTQ students have limited access to culturally competent resources, inclusion in college campus activities, visibility on campus life, and limited in housing safety on campus. Like LGBTQ students, other campus community members, including LGBTQ faculty, staff and administrators, may suffer as a result of the same prejudices, limiting their ability to achieve their career goals and to mentor or support students. 2 Hostile environments that LGBTQ students, faculty, staff and administrators often experience have been documented in the research. Compared to heterosexual students, LGBTQ students face higher rates of mental health problems, sexual health risks, substance abuse, and family issues leading to negative health outcomes (Evans, 2001; Wright and McKinley, 2011). LGBTQ students are at risk of harassment and violence as a result of homophobic attitudes, intolerance, discrimination, and marginalization on college campuses (Brown, Clarke, Gortmaker, & Robinson-Keilig, 2004). Many LGBTQ students find that they must hide significant parts of who they are as a person from peers, leading to increased emotional and social isolation. Those who do not hide their sexual orientation or gender identity have a range of experiences including discrimination, verbal or physical harassment, and subtle or outright silencing of their sexual identities. While higher education provides a variety of opportunities for students, these are greatly limited for those who fear for their safety when they walk on campus. The college experience is different for those students who feel they must censor themselves in the classroom. Their academic performance and self-actualization as a student are jeopardized because they are so distracted by harassing remarks that they are unable to concentrate on their studies. The purpose of the present study is to explore factors that predict negative attitudes towards lesbian, gay, bi-sexual, transgender, and queer students in order to understand the possible role played by the hyper-masculinity concepts in the prejudice against the LGBTQ campus community. 3 Background of the Problem Grossman et al, (2009) conducted a qualitative study focusing on the experience of school-based violence against a group of five LGBTQ youths in a public school. The study suggested that as a result of the violence that LGBTQ students experienced at school, LGBTQ youth escaped and avoided stressors by distancing themselves from school. The findings explained that being harassed because of their sexual orientation was one of the most common reasons why students drop out of school. One of the major predictors of violence against LGBTQ youth is the lack of social capital and community resources available to them, making them vulnerable towards violence and exposing them to a lack of human agency in school (Grossman, et al, 2009). In regards to the empowerment of the LGBTQ student community, Craig, Tucker and Wagner (2008) conducted a qualitative research project examining responses of participants in a Safe Schools Summit. The study examined the participants’ experiences with school-based violence, harassment, and discrimination, and the effects of their attendance at the Summit. Results showed that all respondents have been witnesses or victims of school-based harassment. The data also showed that participants did nothing when witnessing or experiencing harassment due to a general sense of powerlessness stemming from a variety of sources including fear of retribution, lack of administrative support, and the overwhelming nature of antigay bullying in school contexts. Quantitative analyses of the study found that 92% of respondents felt more empowered following participation in the Safe School Summit. The qualitative analyses found that themes related to experiences of victimization lead to a sense of powerlessness. In contrast, their 4 experiences at the summit itself was found to lead to feelings of catharsis, strengthened skills, and a commitment to confront bullies and to make schools safer (Craig, Tucker & Wagner, 2008). The body of research suggests that in order to empower the LGBTQ community, peer education, planned educational activities, changes in policies and rules, and in-service training for school personnel about LGBTQ youth are all necessary to create inclusive school communities. Purpose of the Study Schools, being social institutions in a democratic society, have a duty to provide a supportive and safe environment for all their members through a teaching and learning framework that promotes respect and recognition of diversity and difference. Schools need to be much more aware and proactive in addressing issues pertaining to diversity in order to ensure a safe and equitable learning and teaching environment for all members of the school community. Negative feelings or attitudes towards non-heterosexual behavior, identity, relationships and community, can lead to homophobic behavior which is the root of discrimination experienced by many lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender and questioning people. For many members of the LGBTQ community, acceptance and embracement of their sexuality does not come until they find other people like them in a more supportive environment. Colleges and universities are supposed to embrace acceptance of diversity and inclusion of all members of the community, but, as the research shows, this is not always the case. The present study will examine the relationship between hypermasculinity attitudes/beliefs and negative attitudes towards members of the LGBTQ 5 community in a college campus. It is hypothesized that hyper-masculinity attitudes will be significantly correlated with negative attitudes towards LGBTQ people in a college campus. The purpose of the present study is to explore factors that predict negative attitudes towards LGBTQ students in order to understand the possible role played by the hyper-masculinity concepts in the prejudice against LGBTQ campus community. Definition of Terms Heterosexism Defined as the individual person, group, or institutional norms and behaviors that result from the assumption that all people are heterosexual. This system of oppression, which assumes that that heterosexuality is inherently normal and superior, negates LGBTQ people’s lives and relationships. Homonegativity Similar to homophobia, but instead of being a fear of homosexuality, is the negative belief and attitude towards homosexuality. LGBTQ While specifically being an acronym for Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, and Transgender, it is often expanded to include Queer, Intersex, Transsexual, Questioning and other appropriate terms. Groups and individuals use some or all of the terms, in varying orders. For the purposes of this study, LGBT is used as an inclusive term of for anyone that selfidentifies as such. 6 Hypermasculinity The hypermasculinity man is thought of as being prepared to challenge any real or imagined provocation from other men with violence. Men who hold calloused sexual attitudes, consider violence manly, and consider danger exciting are thought to be hypermasculine men. Theoretical Framework What follows is an assessment of theories underlining the terms homophobia, and heterosexism which systematically review terms of anti-gay or anti-lesbian practice and ideologies. The structural section includes theories that look towards the organization of society, in particular those of male dominance, such as feminist theory. The gender panic section explores deconstructionist queer theory. Both of these theories look at the source of the problem in psychological contradictions and tension of contemporary male masculinity and dominance. Feminist Theory From a feminist viewpoint, Gayle Rubin (1975) notes in her landmark paper “The Traffic in Women”, that social relations of a kinship system specify that men have certain rights to their female kin and that women do not have the same rights either to themselves or to their male kin. The system of exchange of woman by male clans reproduces a gender system that oppresses both same-sex and cross-sex relations. Heteronormativity is recreated each generation as a result of a system of fraternal interest, in groups/societies that exercise control over women’s reproductive power. 7 Homosexuality among men and among women encountered a different but related source of oppression, similar to the one that women experience. Homosexuality among men violates the fundamental social game plan of the fraternal interest group to acquire, control, and trade in the reproductive power of women. Lesbianism, as Monique Wittig (1992) argues violates the same social order by asserting will and subjectivity among the female gender, intended by patriarchal groups, to be objects of change. Wittig calls female homosexuality a revolt of the trade goods in the traffic in women. Anti-lesbianism in this view becomes virtually indistinguishable from misogyny, as it is one among many aspects of the larger force of oppression applied by patriarchy to maintain its domination over the lives of women. Its alleviation would entail much more than education but rather a dismantling of patriarchy and empowerment of women as controllers of their own bodies and destinies. Queer Theory In general, queer theorists propose to destabilize hegemonic cultural ideals of heteronormativity (Britzman, 1997; Butler, 1991; Tierney & Dilley, 1998; Seidman, 1996). Some of the core tenets of queer theory include the concept that all categories are falsifications, especially if they are binary and descriptive of sexuality. Queer theory suspends the classifications of “lesbian,” “gay,” “masculine,” and “feminine.” (Tierney & Dilley, 1998) Instead of categorizing and grouping individual experiences queer theory recognizes sexual and gender identities as social, multiple and fluid (Britzman, 1997). All assertions about reality are socially constructed, identify categories are suspended based on the idea that identity is performed and unstable (Butler, 1991) texts form discourses 8 that are exercises in power/knowledge and which, properly analyzed, reveal relations of dominance within historically-situated systems of regulation (Seidman, 1996 pg. 199). Most of the psychological research associated with the study of homophobia agrees on some version of a gender panic theory. Gregory Herek (1986, 1990) argues that homophobia stems from personal insecurities about personal capability in meeting gender-role demands and is inherent in the cultural construction of heterosexual male role and identity. Defensiveness against losing male status and privilege generates homophobia. Homosexuality signifies the discredited male whose body has been violated and presumably feminized. Herek (1986, 1990) observed that homophobia serves as a display of achieved masculinity by many males toward other males. Anti-gay insults, which circulate persistently and pervasively among male youth, serve to police gender boundaries and enhance male heterosexual bonding. Stephen, Morin and Ellen (1978) report that among groups of male students confederates who actually performed the labeling were all perceived as significantly more masculine and more sociable when they labeled someone homosexual than when they do not. Justification The results of the present study may assist social workers and college campus professionals to be aware of the culture climate and the access of resources by LGBTQ students. When exposing a correlation between hyper-masculinity and negative attitudes towards gays and lesbians, social workers can advocate for more resources and programing that would benefit LGBTQ community. As noted in the National Association of Social Workers Code of Ethics (1999) in Section 4, Social Workers’ Ethical 9 Responsibilities as Professionals, Dignity and Worth of the Person, “it is part of the ethical practice to treat each person in a caring and respectful fashion, mindful of individual differences, cultural, and ethnic diversity” by being sensitive to the people’s needs and desires and considering how this may affect wellbeing. This study addresses the perceived predictor of homophobic attitudes in a college campus. Social workers have an ethical responsibility to understand barriers of their clients when accessing resources. Therefore, it is important to understand the challenges and strengths that LGBTQ youth might face in a college campus. The NASW Code of Ethics (2006) states that social workers have the responsibility to be culturally competent and socially diverse. Furthermore, “social workers should understand culture and function in human behavior and society, recognizing the strengths that exist in all cultures” (p. 9). Therefore, when social workers work with LGBTQ youth, or LGBTQ youth in a college campus, they should be aware of challenges and strengths this population has, so that they can more appropriately approach and work with this population. Social workers have ethical responsibilities to advocate for vulnerable populations such as LGBTQ youths; according to NASW’s Code of Ethics, “a social worker’s primary responsibility is to promote the wellbeing of clients/people” (p. 7). In order to promote the client’s wellbeing, social workers must know the importance that structural systems such as homophobia and heterosexism play in the lack of resources available for LGBTQ students. It is important for social workers to know and understand the perceived challenges and strengths that LGBTQ youth face in a college campus. In addition social 10 workers must be informed of the resources, or the lack of, for LGBTQ students as this could potentially influence their wellbeing and their capacity to advocate for equality. Delimitations Although this research study has been carefully prepared, the researcher is aware of the limitations and shortcomings of the study. First, the questionnaire measures attitudes and beliefs about a particular college campus population; therefore, the researcher depends upon self-report and not direct observation of phenomenon of interest. Consequently, the respondents' honesty, seriousness, accurate memory, and interest in the research determine the accuracy of the findings. Second, the researcher considers that certain terms used in the questionnaire might be a limitation to the study. The participants in the study are undergraduate students who might not have much exposure to terms pertaining to the LGBTQ community. Lastly, correlational research does indicate the existence of a relationship, but gives no clue to the causal relationship. If there was to be a significant correlation between hypermasculinity and negative attitudes towards LGBTQ students, this does not mean that one is the cause of the other one. Assumptions The present study is based on the assumption that there is a hypermasculine culture embraced in college campuses, as may be the case in CSU Sacramento. This unilateral presentation of hypermasculinity is assumed to create conflict and limit the potential for students who do not conform to heteronormative attitudes. It is also assumed that hypermasculine attitudes are not only embraced by male students, but by female 11 students as well. It is assumed that the LGBTQ student community on campus is limited in resources, inclusion in college campus culture, limited visibility on campus life and limited in housing safety on campus. These presumed limitations of access to resources for the wellbeing of LGBTQ students are thought to be correlated to the hypermasculine culture present at CSU Sacramento. Because the measurement use for the present study is a self- report questionnaire, it is assumed that the participant will answer truthfully. The study is limited to measuring attitudes and beliefs of all students, not just male students. There is an assumption that the selected sample population of students is representative of the entire college campus community. 12 Chapter 2 REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE Introduction This chapter reviews the literature concerning attitudes and beliefs towards LGBTQ students in the context of hypermasculinity and attitudes toward this population on college campuses. This review of the literature will explore LGB identity formation and the impact that universities have in the identity formation. Secondly, attitude development towards LGB community will be discussed, followed by a review of the literature in prejudicial attitudes; and the potential effects that ideologies such as homophobia and heterosexism could have on LGBTQ students. Thirdly, this review of the literature will discuss gender roles, masculinity and femininity as predictors of negative attitudes towards LGBTQ students. Lastly, the construct hypermasculinity and its effects as potential predictors of negative attitudes towards LGBTQ students will be reviewed. Colleges and universities remain largely hostile environments for members of the LGBTQ community. Compared to heterosexual students, LGBTQ students face higher rates of mental health problems, sexual health risks, substance abuse, and family issues leading to negative health outcomes (Evans, 2001; Wright and McKinley, 2011). LGBTQ students are more prone to harassment and violence as a result of homophobic attitudes, intolerance, discrimination, and marginalization on college campuses (Rankin, 1998; Brown, Clarke, Gortmaker, & Robinson-Keilig, 2004). Because colleges and universities are very hostile towards LGBTQ issues, students who had been “out” in high school have 13 gone back into the closet due to the lack of support networks at colleges and universities (Beemyn & Rankin, 2011). Rankin (2003) found that undergraduate students encountered various issues. For example, 36% of LGBTQ undergraduates had encountered harassment within the last year; 38% had been pressured to hide their gender identity or sexual orientation; 20% were concerned for their physical safety; and 51% decided to suppress their sexual orientation or gender identity to avoid persecution. In a study focusing on the availability of mental health resources for lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender college students, Wright and McKinley (2011) found that LGBTQ targeted communications were the exception, rather than the norm. Even though LGBTQ students are at a higher risk of violence, harassment and oppression, which could affect their mental wellbeing, only a handful of college mental health services are targeted towards the LGBTQ community. Only 30% of campus counseling centers stated that they offered individual counseling for LGBTQ students, and only 11.3% stated that they offered group counseling for LGBTQ students (Wright & McKinley, 2011). Research has shown that the perception of a non-discriminatory environment is significantly correlated to greater educational attainment for students (Rankin, 2005). Pascarella and Terenzini (1991) found that African American students who attended a Historically Black College had greater educational attainment and a greater academic self-image. By comparison, in a study looking at both LGBTQ community and heterosexual students, Brown et al (2004) found that LGBTQ students generally perceive the climate on campus as less welcoming than heterosexual students. A 2008 report 14 issued by the California Postsecondary Education Commission showed that 93% of heterosexual students said they feel respected on campus, while only 73% of LGBTQ students reported feeling respected on campus. Significantly more LGBTQ students than heterosexual students said they are aware of expressions of negative or stereotypical views about sexual orientation. The purpose of the present study is to explore factors that predict negative attitudes towards LGBTQ students in order to understand the possible role played by the hyper-masculinity concepts in the prejudice against LGBTQ campus community. The Effects of Homophobic School Environment The research suggests that because non-heterosexual feelings and behaviors are stigmatized, students who experience same-sex attraction may become distressed and withdrawn. The academic performance and future educational opportunities of these students are likely to be negatively affected, in part because they may become depressed or begin using harmful substances, and also because they may detach and disengage from school. There is a lack of research in the area of academic success and sexual orientation; for the purpose of this study, the present review of the literature will draw information from research on hostile work environment and secondary education campus climate. In a study examining workplace experiences of 900 gay men, lesbians, and transgendered people, Irwin (1999) found that harassment and/or prejudicial treatment on the basis of homosexuality or gender identity was widespread with 59% of the participants experiencing this in their current or previous workplace. Acts of bigotry included sexual and physical assault, verbal harassment and abuse, destruction of 15 property, ridicule and homophobic jokes. Prejudicial treatment in the workplace included unfair rosters, unreasonable work expectations, undermining of work and restrictions to career (Irwin, 1999). The individuals who were affected by these events showed an increase in stress, depression, illness, loss of self-confidence, increased substance abuse and attempted suicide. Work performance was negatively impacted evident by an increase in stress related leave, not wanting to be at work and the need to be constantly on guard (Irwin, 1999). The homophobic and heterosexist workplace culture affected the work environment for LGBTQ people who were “out” in their work. In their work on social identity in workplace encounters, Creed and Scully (2000), point out that the unspoken disclosure of heterosexuality occurs constantly in the workplace, such as the common instance of heterosexual individuals sharing their partner's name and family photos on their desk. For LGBTQ people, these simple acts become difficult choices and place them at risk of stigmatization. Gay lives and careers become characterized by a preoccupation with self- disclosure and skill in the management of sexual identity, with decisions such as naming a partner becoming a source of intense frequent concern. Invisibility and isolation in the workplace becomes common results of these difficulties. Universities enforce heterosexuality as the norm; the review of the literature suggests that this attitudes and beliefs have devastating effects on LGBTQ students. In the secondary education level, high school and middle school, work has been done on campus climate. For instance, the Gay, Lesbian and Straight Education Network (GLSEN), a Boston community-based, teacher-led organization is working to end homophobia in schools. An initial report Making Schools Safe for Gay and Lesbian 16 Youth (The Governor's Commission on Gay and Lesbian Youth, 1993) resulted in statewide teacher training on homophobia. It aimed to create inclusion of LGBTQ students whose narratives have not been reflected in the classroom experience or the curriculum. This initiative has led to an increase in acts of inclusion of LGBTQ students; and has drawn attention to the importance of classroom climate and its effects on educational outcomes (Fraser, 1991). Woodward (2000) explores the issues for gays and lesbians in higher education in the UK. The research suggests that even when universities have a strong equity focus and record of achievement, sexual orientation is invisible in the equal opportunity agenda. With so few students and staff feeling brave enough to be out, they remain a group not readily identifiable making it difficult to get on the agenda. Raising the issue, challenging homophobia, making the invisible visible and making the stigmatized acceptable become necessary steps in making universities a place where LGBTQ staff and students can thrive. LGB Identity Formation According to Erik Erikson, the process of identity development consists of identity formation in which the internal reality of the individual begins to assert and demand its expression as earlier identifications are discarded or reconfigured (Erikson, 1968, 1980). Identity development also consists of identity integration, in which a commitment to, and integration of, the evolving identity with the totality of the self are expected, although not guaranteed (Marcia, 1966). Identity integration involves an acceptance of the unfolding identity, its continuity over time and settings, and a desire to be known by others as such, none of which is surprising given that identity integration 17 concerns an inner commitment and solidarity with oneself (Erikson, 1968,1980). The opposite of identity integration is diffusion or confusion, a sense of self as other or inauthentic either because an invalid identity has been assumed or imposed upon an individual, or because one is searching for a meaningful identity (Erikson, 1968, 1946,1980). Although most theories of LGB identity development do not explicitly reference Erikson’s more general theory of identity development, the general notions of identity formation and integration are implicit in the models (Cass, 1979; Chapman & Brannock, 1987; Fassinger & Miller, 1996; Troiden, 1989). In keeping with Erikson, sexual identity development is conceived as having two related developmental processes (Morris, 1997). The first, identity formation, is the initiation of a process of self-discovery and exploration of one’s LGB identity. This consists of the individual becoming aware of sexual orientation, questioning whether the individual may be LGB, and having sex with members of the same sex (Chapman & Brannock, 1987; Fassinger & Miller, 1996; Troiden, 1989). The second, identity integration, is a continuation of sexual identity development as individuals integrate and incorporate the identity into their sense of self, which then allows an increase in their commitment to the new LGB identity (Morris, 1997). Specifically, identity integration is composed of engaging in LGB related social activities, working through negative attitudes toward homosexuality, feeling more comfortable with other individuals knowing about their LGB identity, and disclosing that identity to others (Morris, 1997). On the other hand, a growing body of research and theory has offered alternative understandings of sexual identity development based on the claim that this development 18 has no universal endpoint and interacts with contextual factors (Horowitz & Newcomb, 2001; Sophie, 1986). Lesbian, gay or bisexual (LGB) identity stage models or developmental models of identity have been created through a combination of theory and empirical research in western countries, such as the USA (Cass, 1984; Coleman, 1982). These models of identity formation are based on the assumption that sexual identities are formed through roughly sequential stages beginning in adolescence or early adulthood and resulting in a healthy and integrated LGB identity. It is important to clarify that no developmental model applies to every individual (Lee, 1977; Martin, 1991). The majority of the stage models of LGB identity development explain the sexual identity development of all or most LGB persons. For example, most stage models include at least three broad stages: first awareness of same-sex attractions; exploration and testing of LGB feelings; and integration of LGB identity or coming out. According to these models, gay men and lesbian women should be able to form committed romantic relationships with members of the same sex, maintain a positive sense of self and integrate their sexuality with other aspects of their identities (Hanley-Hackenbruck, 1989; Hiestand and Levitt, 2005; SavinWilliams, 1998). These outcomes are considered markers of successful LGB identity development and, once they are reached, the resulting identity is largely resistant to change. On the other hand these stages of identity development rely on a linear sequential account of a developmental process that can be fluid and do not attend to the development of intersecting identities, such as race, profession or gender, that may be 19 inseparable from sexual identity development (Diamond, 1998; Eliason, 1996; Bowleg, 2008; Crenshaw, 1991; Shields, 2008). Additionally, other approaches emphasize the fluidity and flexibility of sexual identity (Diamond, 2007; Hansen and Evans, 1985). For example, Diamond’s (2007) dynamical systems theory offers a contextualized and flexible account of lesbian identity development, which characterized by non-linear changes and spontaneous reformations that occur throughout women’s lives. This theory is based on a homogeneous convenience sample of young, urban white American women and may not apply to women from other racial, class or national backgrounds. Furthermore, Diamond addresses only changes in how women label their sexual orientation, for instance lesbian, heterosexual, and bisexual which may or may not correspond to broader ways in which they conceptualize their sexual identities. These recent additions to conceptualizing sexual identity development have complicated and improved upon traditional stage models. However, they often ignore how sexual identity development intersects with other aspects of social and developmental experience such as cultural, racial, gender, national, and professional identities and their development. In addition, few researchers incorporate the narratives of lesbian, gay and bisexual individuals into the construction and validation of theoretical models, Hiestand and Levitt (2005) and Sophie (1986) are exceptions. LGBTQ Attitude Development The research suggests that direct contact with the LGBT community is correlated with the acceptance of homosexual relationships (Herek, 1984, 1988; D’Augelli and Rose 20 1990). Furthermore, individuals categorize social reality primarily on the basis of ones previous interactions with homosexual persons (Herek, 1984). Research on the topic of attitude development suggests that positive attitudinal changes are associated with direct exposure to LGBT students. Herek and Capitanio (1995) indicate a difference in acceptance between genders. A woman’s acceptance of sexual diversity is often a result of direct exposure, and Gilligan (1981) posits that a woman’s capacity for empathy and perception disassembles prejudicial barriers towards LGBT students. Research on the prediction of factors related to attitudes toward LGBT students has often found that men are less accepting of sexual diversity and LGBT persons than women. Not surprisingly, men account for the majority of anti-gay violence on college campuses (D’Augelli & Rose, 1990; Forstein, 1988; Kurdek, 1988; Morin & Garfinkle, 1978; Whitley & Kite, 1995). The research suggests that men have more negative attitudes towards the LGBTQ persons than women, particularly when the person being rated was a gay man, and gender role attitudes mediated gender differences. On the other hand, in a meta-analysis that included an investigation of gender differences in attitudes toward homosexuality, Oliver and Hyde (1993) found no substantial difference between the sexes on this aspect. Additionally, Oliver and Hyde (1995) suggested that the gender difference found by Whitley and Kite (1995) might be a result of the construction of the ‘‘attitudes toward homosexuality’’ paradigm. Kite and Whitley (1995) addressed the various components that researchers have measured related to anti-gay and lesbian prejudice. Given the contradictory findings of the meta-analyses, Oliver and Hyde (1995) posited that whether such a gender difference exists is to date unresolved, suggesting that there is a possibility 21 of having no gender differences in regards to negative attitudes towards gays and lesbians. Prejudice The predominant form of prejudicial expression towards LGBTQ seems to have changed over the years; the research suggests that society considers blatant prejudice unacceptable (Tougas, Brown, Beaton, & Joly, 1995). However, as the outward expression of prejudice distinguishes, a hidden or subtler prejudice remains ingrained. Modern prejudice differs from its old-fashioned, or traditional, equivalent in that it has not emphasized biological or character logical inferiority. Rather, modern prejudice appears to be cultural in origin, reflecting the belief that minority groups are pursuing undeserved gains in society (Tougas et al., 2004). When talking about prejudice, both modern and traditional, it is evident that in the orientation towards minority groups it’s the same, meaning that modern and traditional forms of prejudice are interrelated. However, modern prejudice is more subtle in nature and therefore better at predicting indirect forms of discrimination such as social distancing from resources (Morrison & Morrison, 2002). Modern prejudice has been studied in relation to African Americans (McConahay, 1986), women (Swim, Aiken, Hall, & Hunter, 1995) and, most recently, sexual minorities (Cowan, Heiple, Marquez, Khatchadourian, & McNevin, 2005; Morrison & Morrison, 2002). This latter one is the primary focus of the present study. Aberson and Dora (2003) argue that homonegativity is similar in origin and form, to prejudice directed against other groups. The research has documented that prejudice 22 appears to be a generalized phenomenon where negative attitudes towards one social group, such as women or African Americans, often correlate with negative attitudes toward other groups, such as gay men and lesbian women (Davies, 2004; Ficarotto, 1990; Morrison & Morrison, 2002; Parrott, Adams, & Zeichner, 2002; Robinson & Schwartz, 2004). Using the models of modern racism and modern sexism, Morrison and Morrison (2002) assert that homonegativity now exists in two interrelated, but theoretically distinct forms. Old-fashioned homonegativity is grounded in objections to, or misconceptions about, gay men and lesbian women based on religious or moral condemnation. According to Morrison and Morrison (2002), modern homonegativity is based on the endorsement of one or more of the following beliefs: (a) Gay men and lesbian women are making unnecessary demands for social change for instance, the right to marry; (b) prejudice and discrimination against gay men and lesbian women has become a thing of the past; and (c) gay men and lesbian women place too much emphasis on their sexuality and, in so doing, are culpable for their own marginalization. With modern homonegativity, the objections are not that gay men and lesbian women are immoral, sinful, or evil but, rather, that these groups have all the rights they need, and should stop asking for more. Heteronormativity and Homophobia There is no single definition for the term homophobia, as it covers a wide range of different viewpoints and attitudes. Fyfe (1983) has defined homophobia as any negative reaction or attitude towards homosexuals. Fyfe (1983) suggests that homophobia can be seen on many different levels, including culture which is manifested by the preservation 23 of traditional sex role distinctions; attitudinal homophobia, which is seen as a hostility towards, or fear of, gay people, but can also refer to social ideologies, which stigmatizes homosexuality; and homophobia as a personality dimension, correlated with rigidity, authoritarianism, conservatism, and intolerance for ambiguity and nonconformity. Hudson and Ricketts (1980) provided groundbreaking work in measuring affective responses among non-gay people. They focused on the personal affective response (disgust, anxiety, aversion, discomfort, fear, and anger) to the proximal or distant contact with “homosexual” individuals. Hudson and Ricketts’s (1980) work, and others in this area, has tended to focus on the non-gay person’s affective responses to gay men. Research has demonstrated that the experiences of gay men and lesbians are strikingly different (Harwood, 1998) and non-gay responses differ based on the gender of the respondent. In order to understand homophobia among males, Lock and Kleis (1998) conducted an analysis of the data and developed a clinical illustration of homophobia. Their study suggested that there is a range of homophobic attitudes among males that initiate in childhood. The study suggested that homophobia is related to anxieties about gender, gender roles, and gender-role conformity. Although much research has investigated predictors of homophobia in males, little attention has been given to the predictors of homophobia in females. In a study investigating how self-esteem, self-discrepancy and gender-attribute importance related to homophobia college women, Basow and Johnson (2000) found that homophobia in women is not significantly predicted by feelings of inadequacy about living up to cultural expectations of appropriate gender traits. Whereas Herek (1986) proposed that the 24 pressure on men to conform to heterosexual masculinity causes male heterosexuals to develop anxiety over not fulfilling these expectations. In addition, college women who feel stereotypic feminine traits are important to their femininity are more homophobic than their female peers who believe such traits are less important. This suggests that men and women who are insecure about their stereotypic gender construction are more likely to hold negative attitudes toward homosexuals. The research shows that there are consistent patterns across different samples in regard to negative affective responses and attitudes towards persons who are gay. There seems be an increase in homophobic attitudes among persons with an authoritarian personality style who are sexually restricted or feel guilt about sexual expression. Herek (1984) suggests that heterosexuals who positively interact with gay, lesbian and bisexual persons are more likely to hold more accepting attitudes toward them. Lastly, research shows that persons with conservative values (religion and gender roles) who are unsupportive of diversity (racism and sexism) tend to be homophobic (Hunter, Shannon, Knox, & Martin, 1998; Reinhardt, 1994). These findings support Herek’s notion of symbolic homophobia. It was found that in males homophobia is closely related to anxieties about power, authority, and dependency issues regarding femininity and passivity. There is a direct link between heterosexism and homophobia, the irrational fear or hatred of homosexuals. Heterosexism could be thought of as the assumption that all people are heterosexual and that heterosexuality is seen as more desirable than non-heterosexuality (Neisen, 1990). Heterosexist attitudes are used to justify the mistreatment, discrimination 25 and harassment of gay, lesbian, bisexual, transgender and questioning individuals. Ferfolja (2007) conducted a data analysis of 17 in-depth interviews with highly experienced teachers who self identified as lesbian. The discussion focused on the data related to participants’ perspectives of their school’s institutional practices, including LGBTQ invisibility in policies, programs, education and practices. This research suggests that school culture foments heteronormativity and heterosexism. Ferfolja (2007) suggests that in order for schools to become truly impartial institutions that serve the needs of all in their communities, it is crucial that they recognize how such marginalization negatively impacts the lives and educational experiences of many students and teachers. Masculinity and Femininity A significant body of research supports the claim that masculinity in modern western societies consists in part to the devaluation of femininity, homophobia, violence, and sexual success with women. Research in disciplines such as social work and psychology suggest that anti-femininity, including negative attitudes towards sexual minorities, is an integral concept of masculinity (Davis, Yarber, Bauserman, Schreer, & Davis, 1996; Smiler, 2004; Thompson & Pleck, 1995). Anti-femininity can be defined as the devaluation of feminine attributes in men and women. Such a belief system leads to the devaluation of women and the derogation of men who display such attributes, often leading to the avoidance of feminine behaviors by men. Anti-femininity is evident in scales measuring extreme gender beliefs, such as the Macho Scale (Villemez & Touhey, 1977), the Hypermasculinity Inventory (HMI: Mosher & Sirkin, 1984) and the Hypergender Ideology Scale (HGIS: Hamburger, Hogben, McGowan, & Dawson, 1996). 26 These scales define masculinity as involving a devaluation and denigration of femininity, and thus as being superior to femininity. The implication from such subscales is that masculinity is very much heterosexual. Specifically it is attitudes towards male homosexuality, not necessarily homosexuality, that are important to masculinity. Evidence supporting a link between anti-femininity and gay-male homophobia suggests that violence is a binding element in their relation. Research has shown that gender roles play an important role in shaping attitudes towards the LGBTQ community. In order to develop an understanding of the reasoning behind this phenomenon, McCreary (1994) tested two theories: the social status model and the sexual orientation hypothesis. The social status model predicts that males are punished because of feminine behavior, as femininity is seen as lower in status than masculine behavior. The sexual orientation hypothesis sees males as stronger; the research speculates that there is a link between gender roles and sexuality and finds that a male acting in a feminine way is more likely to be considered a homosexual than a female acting in a masculine way (McCreary, 1994). The research suggests that these two models may not be mutually exclusive, especially from within a developmental framework. For instance, in a study focusing on gender differences in heterosexual reactions to gay people, Herek (1988) found that religiosity, conformity to traditional ideologies of family, and past interactions with lesbians and gay men shapes the way people think about the LGBTQ community. Herek (1988) suggests that heterosexual males have more negative attitudes towards gay men than do heterosexual females. Males who hold direct aggressive impulses may hold more 27 hostile attitudes towards gay people. These results are in line with findings by McCreary (1994) suggesting that males are expected to portray a masculine persona so that they are not punished. Davies (2004) investigated the relationship between hostile sexism, male toughness, and attitudes toward male sexuality by analyzing a questionnaire related to attitudes towards gay men completed by 517 undergraduate university students. Results revealed that attitudes toward homosexual behavior and homosexual persons were primarily influenced by one factor -- affective reactions toward gay men, suggesting that men respond more negatively to affective reactions than women do. This finding supports previous results of studies that found negative attitudes toward gay men to be part of a larger construct concerning traditional gender roles. Dunkle & Francis (1990) conducted a study that examined whether participants would differentially perceive male and female faces as homosexual based upon facial masculinity/femininity, and the extent to which their attitudes toward homosexuals would influence their perceptions. The participants also completed the Index of Homophobia, the Bem Sex Role Inventory, the Attitudes Toward Women Scale, a conservatism scale, and a demographic questionnaire. The subjects assigned higher homosexuality ratings to feminine male and masculine female faces compared to their masculine and feminine counterparts, respectively. It was found that subjects with negative attitudes toward homosexuals would be more likely to perceive feminine men as gay and masculine women as lesbians. Theodore and Basow (2001) tested how much college men conformed to heteronormative gender roles and how important it was for college men to fulfill 28 this stereotype. It is speculated that men who feel more insecure about their masculinity are more likely to hold negative attitudes towards LGBTQ students. Findings showed that college men who believe it important to have masculine traits and not to have feminine traits, and felt that they did not meet this stereotype, were the most homophobic. In a study focusing on predictors of female attitudes toward lesbians, Basow & Johnson (2000) found that homophobia functions differently for college women than for college men. Homophobia in women is not predicted by feelings of inadequacy of living up to cultural expectations of gender, instead these attitudes were attributed to feminineness. College women who find that their femininity, or hyper-feminine traits, is important to them are more homophobic than other college students who do not hold those beliefs. Theodore and Basow (2001) found similar patterns among college men; greater homophobia was related to the importance of stereotypical masculine traits. These findings suggest that homophobia originates from the conformity of individuals to heteronormative gender roles. Elements of the Construct of Hypermasculinity There are two primary developmental theories used to understand the construct of hypermasculinity -- social learning theory and attachment theory. Social learning theory suggests that social learning is how behavior is acquired. Theoretically hypermasculinity results from gender role socializations (Cowell & Burgers, 1996) in which cultural expectations of maleness produce norm for men to follow (Bergman, 1991) and also an adversarial relationship with femininity which is seen as deficient. Attachment theory provides the motivation to maintain appropriate behavior. The majority of the studies in 29 the construct of hypermasculinity grew out of the research of Mosher and Sirkin (1981). The hypermasculinity man is thought of as being prepared to challenge any real or imagined provocation from other men with violence. Mosher and Sirkin (1981) referred to “calloused sexual attitudes” as men who have hostile competitive feelings toward women and desire to control and dominate them. Groth (1979) stated that the most common classification of rapists included men who raped to assert their power and validate their masculinity. The hypermasculine personality construct, especially the component of calloused sexual attitudes toward women, has been associated with a history of sexual aggression. According to Mosher and Sirkin (1981), scores on the Hypermasculinity Inventory (HMI) were positively correlated with a self-reported likelihood of raping. The subscale of “Violence as manly” defines aggression of any kind as an acceptable expression of dominance over other men (Mosher & Sirkin 1981). The hypermasculine personality style has many characteristics predictive of aggressive action. Hypermasculine males endorsed the expression of anger, action, and domination of others and repressed emotions that they considered “weak” such as caring and empathy. Unfortunately this hypermasculine personality is believed to be developed both by socialization and enculturation. “Danger as exciting” reflected the attitude that survival in dangerous situations was a display of a man’s dominance over the environment. The construct was created after it was noticed that while some men display many of these qualities, others display all of them (Mosher & Sirkin, 1981). The research suggests that during early and middle childhood, parents tend to use contempt and humiliation as strategies to discourage the 30 emotions of fear and distress in boys. It has been theorized that when a boy inhibited the emotions of fear and distress the exaggerated masculine style was formed (Izard, 1977). Furthermore, boys are shamed whenever they display normal emotions of fear or distress. This exaggerated masculine style was supported throughout adolescence through the participation in activities, including sports and less acceptable dangerous, delinquent, or aggressive behaviors. Risk Correlated To Hypermasculinity Hypermasculinity in men has been linked to several significant risk factors for child maltreatment including physically and sexually coercive behavior, verbally aggressive behavior, alcohol abuse, and lower frustration tolerance and levels of empathy. The research suggests that hypermasculine men’s perpetration of sexual coercion, is correlated to a lack of rape-related empathy and deficiency in general empathy and warmth (Norris, George, Davis, Martell, & Leonesio, 1999). Hypermasculine narratives may facilitate expressions of anger and excitement but suppress empathy when they feel distressed or threatened in a way that challenges their perceived masculinity (Mosher & Tomkins, 1988; Norris et al., 1999). A woman refusing sexual activity may threaten a hypermasculine man’s sense of masculinity in which sexual prowess is valued. Similarly, a child’s misbehavior may threaten a hypermasculine man’s sense of masculinity if familial dominance is valued. Either situation may activate hypermasculine scripts, suppressing men’s empathy raising his level of anger or excitement, and facilitating his use of sexual, physical, or verbal aggression to lower perceived threat levels. Hypermasculine men have been found to report cultural values consistent with viewing 31 women as sexual conquests (Bogaert & Fisher, 1995) and engaging in misogynistic fantasy (Johnson & Knight, 2000), both of which may increase likelihood of engaging in sexually coercive acts. Researchers have also found significant positive relationships between men’s levels of hypermasculinity and their willingness to act sexually coercively according to their self- reports and greater attitudinal acceptance of rape (Sullivan & Mosher, 1990; Vass & Gold, 1995). In a study involving using electrical shocking behavior as retaliation against female competitors, Parrot and Zeichner (2003) found that men high in hypermasculinity had a higher frequency of shock administrations to their female opponents, higher shock durations, and a significantly larger proportion of instances in which they administered the highest possible levels of electrical shock toward their female opponents (Parrot & Zeichner, 2003). Norris et al. (1999) also found that the higher a man’s level of hypermasculinity, the less likely he was to show empathy toward a woman in response to violent pornography if the woman ex- pressed distress. Verbal aggression has been linked to both intimate partner violence (Ryan, 1995) as well as child physical abuse (Teicher, Samson, Polcari, & McGreenery, 2006). Research on the relationship between hypermasculinity and verbal aggression has suggested that hypermasculinity was correlated to a man’s reported levels of experiencing psychological maltreatment (Ray & Gold, 1996). Further, Ray and Gold found that hypermasculine men were more likely to feel verbally abused by their partners than men scoring low in hypermasculinity. These are important findings because psychological aggression has been found to precede physical aggression (Ryan, 1995), especially when the perceived threat was not reduced by verbal aggression (Ray & Gold, 1996). Thus, hypermasculine 32 men may be more likely to perceive more psychological or verbal aggression from their female partners than men low in hypermasculinity, and, they may respond with intimate partner violence. Conclusion As discussed in the themes of this literature review, there is ample evidence for the constructs of hypermasculinity and negative attitudes toward the LGBTQ population. This study specifically adds to the existing knowledge by examining this relationship in the context of a state institution such as California State University. Institutions of higher education seek to create an environment characterized by equal access for all students, faculty and staff regardless of cultural, political or philosophical differences; where individuals are not just tolerated but also valued. Creating and maintaining a community environment that respects individual needs, abilities and potential is one of the most important functions of universities and colleges. A welcoming and inclusive climate is grounded in respect, nurtured by dialogue and evidenced by a pattern of civil interaction. 33 Chapter 3 METHODOLOGY This chapter presents the methodology and research design utilized for this research study. The following areas are addressed: Research Question, Study Design, Sampling Procedures, Data Collection Procedures, Instrumentation, Proposed Statistical Analyses and a Summary. In addition, there is a description of the steps taken to protect human subjects. Research Question This study examined the relationship between hyper-masculinity attitudes/beliefs and negative attitudes against members of the LGBTQ community in a college campus. It is hypothesized that hyper-masculinity attitudes will be significantly correlated with negative attitudes towards gays and lesbians in a college campus. The purpose of the present study is to explore factors that predict negative attitudes towards LGBTQ students in order to understand the possible role played by the hyper-masculinity concepts in the prejudice against LGBTQ campus community. Study Design This research study examines the relationship between hypermasculinity and attitudes and beliefs towards LGBT community in a college campus. The research approach used for this study is quantitative and correlational. A quantitative rather than a qualitative or mixed method approach was selected to examine the relationship between hypermasculinity and attitudes towards LGBT community. A quantitative survey approach was selected because it accomplished the study’s purpose and attempted to 34 answer the research questions. A regression was selected to guide this study’s analysis by answering the following questions: 1. What is the relationship between two things? 2. How precise is the estimate of the relationship? The regression answered the first question by the estimated “coefficient” and the second question in the “t” statistic. In essence, the analysis attempted to estimate the extent to which hypermasculinity is a predictor of negative attitudes toward LGBTQ population. These relationships as well as the results of the analysis are discussed in Chapter 4. Data was collected via Likert type questionnaire. Participant’s consent was obtained prior to filling out the questionnaire. The researcher distributed the questionnaire in undergraduate level classes with professor permission. Descriptive statistics were obtained for each variable and T-tests and correlations were used for variables of interest. The purpose of administering a questionnaire in an anonymous and confidential way was to allow the participants to report their attitudes and beliefs without any impacts on the validity of the responses. This process will assist the researcher in gaining a greater understanding of the effects of heteronormative attitudes such as hypermasculinity on LGBT students. The researcher hopes that the conclusions of this study will further expand the knowledge of predictors of homophobia and heterosexism, which create negative effects on academic performance. Sampling Procedures The researcher used non-random, purposive sampling of current California State University, Sacramento students, at the undergraduate level. The researcher recruited 35 students to survey from a sociology class, which is a requirement for graduation, and from an honors class program; this allowed the researcher to survey students from different college majors. An initial contact to professors was established and consent from the professors was obtained to survey their students. The participants were selected based on who was willing to participate in the study after reading the consent form. Any student at least 18 years of age and currently enrolled in class was able to participate. The total sample size consisted of 158 participants. Data Collection Procedures All subjects participated in the study voluntarily, were over the age of 18 and enrolled in an undergraduate course at CSUS. Researcher presented a 5-minute oral presentation introducing the study and reading the rights of the participants. Researcher informed the potential participants about consenting to participate in the study by signing with an “X” at the bottom of the consent form and completing the questionnaire. Following the presentation, participants were asked to complete a 46-item questionnaire that would take 20 minutes to complete. In order to collect the completed questionnaires and consents forms, participants were asked to drop the questionnaire off in a covered cardboard box with a slit. The consent forms were dropped off in two separate envelopes labeled “consent forms.” Researcher stepped out of the room while participants completed the questionnaire to eliminate any risk variables arising out of potential researcher influence that could result from the presence of the researcher. This ensured anonymity and confidentiality of the 36 participants. Participants were asked not to write any information that could identify them in any way (i.e., address, phone number, or email address). Participants were informed prior to completing the questionnaire that the researcher and the advisor will be the only people with access to the completed forms, that individual answers would not be reported on their own but they will be examined as a whole to ensure that participants are aware that the information is confidential. Instruments Participants were asked to complete a 46 questions questionnaire consisting of two different scales that measure Hypermasculinity and Attitudes Towards LGBT people. Participants were also asked to provide demographic information such as gender, race, age and college major. A modified version of the Hypermasculinity Inventory (HMI) will be used. The Hypermasculinity Inventory is a valid forced-choice scale that is used to measure a macho personality constellation pattern. The macho personality is defined as a cognitive structure, which has developed over time because of repeated interactions of emotions, cognitions and beliefs about events which led to a formation of a macho self concept. This scale consists of 30 items. The questions assess the view of danger as exciting, idea that violence is manly, and calloused sexual attitudes toward women. To measure attitudes towards LGBTQ community the researcher used a modification to Herek’s (1988) Attitudes Toward Lesbians and Gay Men (ATLG) scale. This scale measured attitudes towards Transgender people, bisexual people, and queer people. The participants rated the extent to which they agreed with statements 37 concerning LGBTQ community. Higher scores indicate attitudes that are more homophobic. Proposed Statistical Analyses All study hypotheses are correlational in nature. The operational null hypothesis suggests that there is no significant correlation between hypermasculinity and negative attitudes towards LGBT people. It is speculated that hypermasculinity attitudes are significantly correlated with negative attitudes towards LGBT people. It is also speculated that ethnic background is significantly correlated with negative attitudes towards LGBT people. Finally it is speculated that people whose college major is in the social sciences have more positive attitudes towards LGB people. A simple regression using ethnicity as the independent variable and negative attitudes against LGBT as the criterion was conducted. In addition, a second simple regression using college major as the independent variable and negative attitudes against LGBT as the criterion was conducted. Protection of Human Subjects As required by California State University, Sacramento, a human subject application was submitted to the Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects from the Division of Social Work. This committee approved the proposed study and determined the research as "minimal risk" to the study participants. The approval number of the study is12-13-037. The approval was received prior to participant contact and the collection of any study data. 38 Participation in this research study by subjects over the age of 18 and enrolled in an undergraduate course at CSUS was on a strictly voluntary basis. Participants were informed in writing and verbally of their right to decline to answer any questions or stop the interview at any time and for any reason. Participants were not given any type of compensation for their participation in the study. Participants were informed that their participation in the study will not affect their grade in the class. To protect the identity of the participants, participants were told to not use any personal information such as name, email address or telephone number. Questionnaires were given a number after being completed in order to protect their confidentiality. Privacy was maintained through separately locking away all interview sheets researcher noted on and the informed consents. All the digital recordings and transcribed materials were stored in a locked drawer and were immediately destroyed after being transcribed, analyzed and coded. All interview materials were destroyed by June 15, 2013. This information was described in the participant's consent form in English (see Appendix ###) which was signed prior to the interview taking place. 39 Chapter 4 RESULTS Study Findings And Discussions This chapter presents the study results stemming from the quantitative and qualitative analysis of the data gathered from the 158 respondents who responded to a survey on the impact of hypermasculinity on attitude toward members of the LGBT campus community. The purpose of the present study was to identify factors that predict negative attitudes towards LGBTQ students in order to understand the possible role played by the hyper-masculinity and ethnicity concepts in the attitudes toward LGBTQ campus community. This chapter begins with the demographics of the study participants and continues to present the key study findings. All study hypotheses are correlational in nature. The operational null hypothesis suggesting that there is no significant correlation between hypermasculinity and negative attitudes towards LGBTQ people was tested in conjunction with the alternative hypothesis that hypermasculinity attitudes are significantly correlated with negative attitudes towards LGBTQ people. Another alternative hypothesis tested was that ethnic background is significantly correlated with negative attitudes towards individuals with the LGBTQ orientation and or identity. A simple regression using ethnicity as the independent variable and negative attitudes against LGBTQ as the dependent variable was conducted. The constructs used in the hypotheses formulation was constructed after extensive literature review on the primary 40 factors driving the conceptualization of gender and attitude construction in the context of attitudes toward LGBQT population. Demographics The participants consisted of 158 undergraduate students, 107 females (67.7%) and 58 males (32.3%). Majority of the participants (77.2%) reported to be in the age group 18-24, with 19 % in the age group 25-30, and 1.9% reported to be in the 31-35 age group with very few participants in the 31-45 age group and an outlier indicating as above 50 years of age. Table 1 Demographics - Gender and Age of Study Participants (N=158) Frequency Valid Gender Female Male Total Valid Age Group 18-24 25-30 31-55 41-45 50+ Total 107 51 158 Percent Valid Percent Cumulative Percent 67.7 32.3 100.0 67.7 32.3 100.0 67.7 100.0 122 77.2 77.2 77.2 30 3 2 1 158 19.0 1.9 1.3 .6 100.0 19.0 1.9 1.3 .6 100.0 96.2 98.1 99.4 100.0 The ethnic composition of the sample included 24.1% Latino-Americans, 5.1% African Americans, 41.1% European-Americans, 17.1% Asian Americans, 44.4% 41 multiethnic, 3.8% West Asia Descend/ Indians, and 1.3% Native Americans and 3.2% did not indicated their ethnic background. Table 2: Demographics - Ethnicity of Study Participants (N=158) Frequency Percent Valid Percent Hispanic/Latino/Mexican Valid African American/African Asian American South West Asians/Indians European American/ White Interracial/Mix Native American Total Missing System Total Cumulative Percent 38 24.1 24.8 24.8 8 5.1 5.2 30.1 27 17.1 17.6 47.7 6 3.8 3.9 51.6 65 41.1 42.5 94.1 7 4.4 4.6 98.7 2 153 5 158 1.3 96.8 3.2 100.0 1.3 100.0 100.0 The college major composition of the respondents included the distribution of 8.9% in Business Administration, 1.3 % English and Education, 5.7% Engineering & Computer Sciences 10.2% Health and Human Services 12.7% Natural Sciences & Mathematics, 55.7% Social Sciences and Interdisciplinary Studies, 2.5 Arts, and 3.2% undeclared. As is evident from this summary majority of the respondents of the study identified themselves as from the social sciences and interdisciplinary studies lending the 42 possibility that study results might be influenced by the predominant representation of students from social sciences and interdisciplinary studies. Table 3 Demographics – College Major of Study Participants (N=158) Frequency Percent Business Administration 14 8.9 Valid Missing Total Valid Percent 8.9 Education/English 2 1.3 1.3 Engineering & Computer Science 9 5.7 5.7 Health & Human Services 16 10.1 10.2 Natural Sciences & Mathematics 20 12.7 12.7 Social Sciences & Interdisciplinary Studies 87 55.1 55.4 Arts 4 2.5 2.5 Undeclared 5 3.2 3.2 157 1 158 99.4 .6 100.0 100.0 Total System Key Study Findings A simple regression analysis was conducted to examine the relationship between negative attitudes towards the LGBTQ students and hypermasculinity. Assumptions concerning the linearity of the response variable, attitudes toward LGBTQ population and hyper masculinity scores and ethnicity scores were met along with the assumption that the variables are normally distributed . However, there is no certainty to the meeting 43 of assumption that each observation is not determined or influenced by any other observation in the distribution. Pearson’s Product Moment Correlation between Negative Attitudes – LGBTQ and the Hypermasculinity Mean value is 0.227, which represents a weak degree of correlation between Negative Attitudes – LGBTQ and the Hypermasculinity Mean. Percentage of variation in the negative attitudes toward the members of the LGBTQ community as explained by the variation in the independent variable, the hyper masculinity score in the population, R squared is 5.1%, which is minimal. Table 4 summarizes the analysis results. Table 4 Hypermasculinity Mean Model Summary (N=158) Model R R Square Adjusted R Square Std. Error of the Estimate 1 .227a .051 .041 .55670 a. Predictors: (Constant), Hypermasculinity Mean There is a positive weak correlation, of .227 between negative attitudes toward LGBQT population and the hypermasculinity scores of the respondents, .which despite its statistical significance needs to be studied further with random samples and more rigorous standardized scales to measure the negative attitudes and the hypermasculinity scores. The fact that as scores on hypermasculinity increases, the scores on negative 44 attitudes also increases is a significant finding in this study. This finding is worthy of further in depth investigation. The following table indicates the statistical significance of the regression model that was applied p < 0.26, which is less than 0.05. However, in the light of the low R value the results of the ANOVA, despite indicating the regression equation model to be statistically significant, the regression equation’s capacity to predict the relationship between the hypermasculinity scores and the negative attitude toward LGBTQ members, must be treated as a tentative relationship that needs further investigation. The model applied can statistically significantly predict the outcome variable regression model with Negative Attitudes – LGBTQ as the dependent variable and Hypermasculinity Mean as the independent variable (R=. 227, N=158, p<. 05). Table 5 Hypermasculinity Mean ANOVA (N=158) Model Sum of df Mean Square F Sig. 5.086 .026b Squares 1 Regression 1.576 1 1.576 Residual 29.132 94 .310 Total 30.709 95 a. Dependent Variable: Negative Attitudes – LGBTQ b. Predictors: (Constant), Hypermasculinity Mean 45 Table 6 Hypermasculinity Mean Coefficients (N=158) Model (Constant) 1 Unstandardized Standardized Coefficients Coefficients B Std. Error .332 .681 .309 .137 t Sig. .486 .628 2.255 .026 Beta Hypermascul .227 inity Mean a. Dependent Variable: Negative Attitudes – LGBTQ In addition a simple regression analysis was conducted to examine the relationship between negative attitudes towards the LGBTQ students and various potential predictors such as hypermasculinity. The regression equation is as follows: Negative attitudes toward LGBT community members= .332+ .309. Thus the attitude toward members of the LGBT community can be predicted by the following equation using the hypermasculinity score. The regression equation to make a point prediction is: Negative attitudes toward LGBT community = .309 + .332 (hypermasculinity). Additionally the slope is an ascending line with the increase in the independent variable resulting in the increase in the dependent variable. This relationship is worthy of further investigation. For the simple regression analysis using ethnicity as the independent variable to predict the attitude toward members of the LGBT members, the R value is 0.154, which 46 represents the simple correlation and it indicates a weak positive correlation between Negative Attitudes – LGBTQ and Ethnicity. The R Square value indicates how much of the proportion or percentage of variation in the dependent variable, Negative Attitudes – LGBTQ, can be explained by the independent variable, Ethnicity. In this case, 2.4% thus indicating that ethnicity is a weak predictor of negative attitudes toward members of the LGBT community, within the limits of the observations. However this relationship needs to be studied further using randomized trials or random samples. Table 7 Ethnicity Model Summary (N=158) Model R R Square Adjusted R Square Std. Error of the Estimate 1 .154a .024 .016 .63469 a. Predictors: (Constant), Ethnicity The correlation between negative attitudes Negative Attitudes – LGBTQ as the dependent variable and Ethnicity as the independent variable (r=. 154, N=158, p<. 05), as it can be seen in Table 7. The regression equation with ethnicity as the predictor of attitudes toward LGBTQ community is not statistically significant (p>.05) as indicated by the significance of the F ratio. The slope of this regression was a negatively sloping regression line (-.199) which indicates that these two variable are actually moving in the opposite direction. 47 Table 8 Ethnicity ANOVA (N=158) Model 1 Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Regression 1.206 1 1.206 2.993 .086 Residual 49.549 123 .403 Total 50.754 124 t Sig. 12.702 .000 -1.730 .086 a. Dependent Variable: Negative Attitudes – LGBTQ b. Predictors: (Constant), Ethnicity Table 9 Ethnicity Coefficients (N=158) Model Unstandardized Coefficients Standardized Coefficients B Std. Error (Constant) 2.193 .173 Ethnicity2 .115 Beta 1 -.199 -.154 a. Dependent Variable: Negative Attitudes – LGBTQ As indicated in table 9, the regression equation with ethnicity as the predictor variable is as follows: Negative attitudes toward LGBT community (Y) = 2.193-(.199) (ethnicity). 48 Table 10 Correlation Analysis (N=158) Negative Hypermascul Ethnicity Attitudes – inity Mean LGBTQ Correlation 1.000 .197 -.169 Sig. (2-tailed) . .055 .059 N 128 96 125 .197 1.000 -.186 Sig. (2-tailed) .055 . .058 N 96 108 104 -.169 -.186 1.000 Sig. (2-tailed) .059 .058 . N 125 104 153 Negative Coefficient Attitudes – LGBTQ Correlation Hypermasculi Coefficient Spearman's rho nity Mean Correlation Coefficient Ethnicity In addition to the correlations explained as part of the regression analysis this correlation matrix indicates that there is a negative correlation between ethnicity and hypermasculinity score (r=-.186, N=158, p=. 05), which is statistically significant. Although this correlation is relatively weak it is important to consider the role of 49 traditional sex role stereotypes and conceptions of gender in general among various ethnic groups to understand the variations in attitudes and beliefs toward LGBQT populations. Summary This chapter presented the demographics of the participants of this study. It also explored the role that hypermasculinity and ethnicity play in predicting negative attitudes towards LGBTQ campus community and evidenced the fact that while hypermasculinity score is a relatively significant predictor of negative attitudes toward LGBQT population than ethnicity. The correlations of both the predictor variables with the dependent variable of attitudes toward LGBQT community are weak although hypermasculinity has a higher correlation with negative attitudes toward the LGBQT population than ethnicity. In the following chapter, the study conclusions, recommendations, implications for social work practice and limitations of the study are presented in the context of social work values, principles and the various domains of social work practice. 50 Chapter 5 DISCUSSION Conclusion, Summary, Recommendations This chapter provides a summary of the conclusions from this study. It includes results and interpretations of the relationship between hypermasculinity, ethnicity and negative attitudes towards LGBTQ community. In addition, the chapter discusses limitations to the study and provides implications for social work practice and policy. The chapter ends with suggestions for future research and recommendations on improving attitudes toward LGBTQ population. Summary The purpose of this study was to explore possible predictors of negative attitudes towards the LGBTQ student community in a college campus such as hypermasculinity and ethnicity. In order to measure hypermasculinity the researcher used a modified version of The Hypermasculinity Inventory (HMI – Modified) composed of 26 forced-choice scale questions used to measure a macho personality constellation pattern in the participants. To measure attitudes towards LGBTQ community the researcher used a modification to Herek’s (1988) Attitudes Toward Lesbians and Gay Men (ATLG) scale. Demographic information such as age, ethnicity, college major and gender were collected as well. In order to analyze the data a simple regression using ethnicity as the independent variable and negative attitudes against LGBTQ as the criterion was conducted. In addition, a second simple regression using hypermasculinity as the 51 independent variable and negative attitudes against LGBT as the criterion was conducted. Participants were 158 undergraduate students, 107 females (67.7%) and 58 males (32.3%). The majority of participants reported to be between the ages of 18 and 24 (77.2%). The major ethnic composition of the sample included 24.1% Latino-Americans, 5.1% African Americans and 41.1% EuropeanAmericans. Pearson’s Product Moment Correlations were conducted to examine the relationship between the study variables. Conclusion The present study examined the relationship between the hypermasculinity construct and negative attitudes towards LGBTQ campus community. The primary purpose of the study was to explore factors such as hypermasculinity and ethnicity, which can potentially predict negative attitudes towards LGBTQ campus community. The null hypothesis suggesting no significant correlation between hypermasculinity and negative attitudes towards LGBTQ people exists was tested in conjunction with the alternative hypothesis stating that hypermasculinity attitudes are significantly correlated with negative attitudes towards LGBTQ people. Furthermore, a simple regression using ethnicity as the independent variable and negative attitudes against LGBTQ people as the dependent variable was conducted to test the hypothesis that ethnic background is significantly correlated with negative attitudes towards LGBTQ campus community. The study evidenced the fact that hypermasculinity score is a relatively 52 significant predictor of negative attitudes toward LGBQT population than ethnicity. The correlations of both the predictor variables with the dependent variable of attitudes toward LGBQT community are weak although hypermasculinity has a higher correlation with negative attitudes toward the LGBQT population than ethnicity. The regression analysis results suggested that the capacity to predict the relationship between hypermasculinity and negative attitudes towards LGBTQ campus community was statistically significant but in need of further examination. The fact that the scores in the hypermasculinity scale increase as the negative attitudes towards LGBTQ campus community increase is important to note. In the research, hypermasculinity attitudes have been associated to other significant risk factors such as child maltreatment (Teicher et all., 2006), physically and sexually coercive behavior (Bogaert & Fisher, 1995), verbally aggressive behavior, alcohol abuse (Norris et all., 1999) and intimate partner violence (Ryan, 1995). In addition, theoretically, hypermasculinity results from gender role socializations (Cowell & Burgers, 1996) in which cultural expectations of maleness produce a norm for men to follow (Bergman, 1991). Feminist theory suggests that this norm creates a confrontational relationship with femininity, or anything other than, which is seen as deficient thus producing negative attitudes and prejudice. This rationale proposes hypermasculinity attitudes as potential predictor of homophobia in college campuses that needs to be further assessed in the research. 53 Secondly, the results of the simple regression model examining the relationship between ethnicity and negative attitudes towards LGBTQ community was not statistically significant. Moreover the slope of the regression was negative, indicating that people who identify as Caucasian move further away in the slope from negative attitudes towards LGBTQ people, than people who identify as non-Caucasian. This specific finding needs to be studied further using randomized trials or random samples. Research on the topic of attitude development suggests that positive attitudinal changes are associated with direct exposure to LGBT students (Herek, 1984). This can potentially explain why this results came about, it can be speculated that people who identify as Caucasian in this study had more exposure to LGBTQ people on campus or in the community. Not only that, but as indicated in the literature review, their social realty about LGBTQ people was affected possibly by previous positive interactions with homosexual persons (Herek, 1984). The research on this topic is somewhat dated, further information needs to be obtained to study the differences in attitude development in current college students. Limitations More information is needed to meet all the assumptions of the regression analysis. Despite the reasonable study sample size, not all of the assumptions of the regression analysis have been met due to the nature of the sample. be collected in order to predict the influence of college major and age to negative attitudes towards LGBTQ students. The participants in the study are undergraduate 54 students who possibly, did not have much exposure to terms pertaining to the present study- terms such as transgender, hypermasculinity, LGBTQ, and heterosexism. In addition, the wording of the questionnaire possibly could have been confusing for some participants and could have influenced the way they answer. Stronger instrumentation is needed to measure the study variables in specific terms. Lastly, the correlations between the study variables are weak although they are statistically significant. A significant correlation indicates the existence of a relationship, but does not provide enough to the causal nature of the relationship. Further research needs to be explored n the area for predictors of negative attitudes in college campus. Recommendations The following recommendations are suggested to improve attitudes towards LGBTQ population on campus. Detailed specific data pertaining to hyper masculinity and ethnicity identifications must are needed to understand the extent to which preconceptions and socialization patterns of gender role identifications and ethnic group identifications influence prejudice, attitudes and negative assumptions about the other than the self. Policy makers and program planners need to actively maximize LGBTQ equity on campus. In order to prevent discrimination towards LGBTQ students change needs to happen at the micro level to provide Educational Programming on GLBTQ issues and concerns and include sexual orientation and gender identity issues in student orientation programs. 55 As universities operate as systems of constituents, all stakeholders must be educated about the underlying assumptions about beliefs and attitudes. Sexual orientation and gender identity issues must be included in new faculty/staff orientations. Additionally workshops/programs need to be developed to address GLBTQ issues within residence life, especially geared toward resident assistants. These workshops/programs can be structured to address homophobia/heterosexism within fraternities, sororities and intercollegiate athletics. Continuous education of the university community needs to occur through lectures, concerts, symposia, and other activities to increase GLBTQ awareness on campus. Provide training for campus health care professionals to increase their sensitivity to issues of sexual orientation and gender identity and the special health needs of GLBTQ individuals. Training sessions for public safety officers on GLBTQ issues and concerns and anti-GLBTQ violence must include discussion of sexual orientation and gender identity or expression in the institution’s nondiscrimination clause. Such measures can be advocated through single stall gender-neutral restroom facilities and developing visible scholarships targeting GLBT students. Preventive measures need to include integrating GLBTQ concerns into university documents/publications (grievance procedures, housing guidelines, application materials) and creating a GLBT alumni group within the existing alumni organization. 56 Social Work Implications While the purpose of this study was to identify the role of hypermasculinity as possible predictor of negative attitudes towards the LGBTQ community in a college campus, the intent is to use this broader understanding of LGBTQ students to increase the body of knowledge around this population. This can be beneficial within the field of social work, and will hopefully be used to create positive change in the form of improvements made to the systems that serve LGBTQ students and members of the community. The findings proposed in the present study, speculate a number of implications for the field of social work, both in terms of advocacy on the macro level, and in providing direct services to clients/students in clinical social work. In terms of social justice advocacy, this profession would do well to work toward a broader conception of social justice and translate this into actions such as create a documentation form in police services for reporting hate crimes committed against GLBT population. Social workers can professionally create avenues for providing a clear, safe, visible means of reporting acts of intolerance with special attention to including openly GLBTQ people on university committees. Social workers can also act to respond visibly and expeditiously to acts of intolerance directed at GLBT members of the community. Most importantly, social workers need to actively explore the needs of this vulnerable population through research, in order to best advocate for this group. Specifically research on gender roles and its effect of people who do not conform 57 to normative gender roles. One implication of this research is the need for additional programs and services that meet the needs of this population. Social workers should advocate for the creation of LGBTQ-specific programs and services in schools, and should promote anti-discrimination practices around sexual orientation and gender expression in college campuses, improve mental health treatment and engagement of the LGBTQ campus community and provide safe/friendly housing programs for all students. Additionally, as a profession there is a need to address the fragmentation of information about resources, thereby improving access to services. The need for cultural competency training around LGBTQ population is suggested as a way of decrease negative attitudes and prejudice against LGBTQ students. Cultural incompetence on behalf of service providers serves as a barrier for this population in accessing needed services. While this research serves as a guiding point to shed light on problems within the higher education system, the impact of these problems should be further researched and fully addressed in order to truly understand predictors of homophobia and discrimination in our communities. 58 Appendix A Human Subjects Approval Letter 59 CALIFORNIA STATE UNIVERSITY, SACRAMENTO DIVISION OF SOCIAL WORK To: Nicolas Caballero Date: February 21, 2013 From: Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects RE: YOUR RECENT HUMAN SUBJECTS APPLICATION We are writing on behalf of the Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects from the Division of Social Work. Your proposed study, “Attitudes and Beliefs Towards LGBTQ Community in a College Campus.” __X_ approved as _ _ _EXEMPT __X__ MINIMAL RISK Your human subjects approval number is: 12-13-037 . Please use this number in all official correspondence and written materials relative to your study. Your approval expires one year from this date. Approval carries with it that you will inform the Committee promptly should an adverse reaction occur, and that you will make no modification in the protocol without prior approval of the Committee. The committee wishes you the best in your research. Professors: Maria Dinis, Jude Antonyappan, Teiahsha Bankhead, Serge Lee, Kisun Nam, Maura O’Keefe, Dale Russell, Francis Yuen Cc: Antonyappan 60 Appendix B Consent Form 61 CONSENT FORM The research study you are being asked to participate in is entitled “ Attitudes and Beliefs Towards LGBTQ Community in a College Campus” This is a correlational study measuring attitudes and beliefs about LGBTQ community in college campus. The questionnaire consists of 46 likert scale questions and it should take you about 20 minutes to complete. I understand that my name will not be linked to any data acquired. I understand that this study will feature a number of surveys, some of which may request information that may be of a personal nature. I am 18 years old or older. The data collected from this study will be kept confidential and no other person other than the ones mentioned in this consent form will be able to review that data. These rights include the right to confidentiality and to withdraw from this study at any time, for whatever reason and at no penalty. To ensure my confidentiality, all surveys will only be accessible to the primary researcher and will be subsequently destroyed one year after the analysis of data is conducted. The researcher has informed me that the study will look at attitudes and beliefs and my participation in this research will add to the scientific literature. This is a study considered to be exempt, there will be no discomfort or harm while completing the questionnaire. If any discomfort may arise while completing the questionnaire please contact the Counseling and Psychological Services (CAPS) on campus at tel: 95819-6045 If you wish to know more information or the results of the study please contact the primary researcher after May 31, 2013. The primary researcher of this study is Nicolas Caballero. He can be reached via e-mail at nc2389@saclink.csus.edu . Nicolas Caballero is under the supervision of Professor Jude Antonyappan and she may be reached at 916-278-7171, e-mail judea@saclink.csus.edu By putting an “X” over the line I acknowledge that I have read the content of this page and that I consent the use of this information for the study. ____________________________________________ Please sigh with an “X” over the line to provide consent. 62 Appendix C Study Questionnaire 63 Demographics Age o 18-24 o 25-30 o 31-35 o 36-40 o 41-45 o 46-50 o 50 + Sex o Female o Male o Transgender M/F F/M o Other College Major o _______________________________________ Ethnic Background o ________________________________________ 64 Instructions You are asked to select one of the alternatives as true or more true for you. Sometimes it may be difficult to agree with either alternative, as neither may seem very desirable to you -- it may be the case that you agree with both alternatives, but you are still asked to select the one item that most represents you and your opinion. The forced-choice format solves some technical problems of measurement, but it is not always subjectively comfortable to complete. Although you are, of course, free to refuse to answer any item, the scientific purposes of the research are best fulfilled through you cooperation. In addition the language use in some of the questions might be considered offensive by some people, others may find the language representative of comments overheard in society. It is not the author intent to offend anyone’s sensibility or to endorse any of the alternative attitudes and beliefs as scientifically or socially valid. Rather, the intent of the inventory is to measure your personal preferences in order to investigate beliefs, attitudes, and behavior in a domain specific to the LGBTQ community. 1. transgender people just can’t fit into our society Strongly Agree 2 3 4 Strongly Disagree 2. After I’ve gone through a really dangerous experience my knees feel weak and I shake all over 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 I feel high. 3. A woman’s homosexuality should not be cause for job discrimination in any situation Strongly Agree 2 3 4 Strongly Disagree 65 4. I’d rather gamble than play it safe. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 play it safe than gamble. 5. Female homosexuality is detrimental to society it breaks down the natural division between the sexes Strongly Agree 2 3 4 Strongly Disagree 6. Call me a name and I’ll pretend not to hear you. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 I’ll call you another. 7. State laws regulating private, consenting lesbian behavior should be loosened Strongly Agree 2 3 4 Strongly Disagree 8. In love and war you should still play by the rules. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 anything goes. 9. Female homosexuality is a sin Strongly Agree 2 3 4 Strongly Disagree 66 10. When I go to parties I like wild, uninhibited parties. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 I like quiet parties with good conversations. 11. The growing number of queer people indicates a decline in American morals Strongly Agree 2 3 4 Strongly Disagree 12. Some people have told me I take foolish risks. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 I ought to take more chances. 13. Female homosexuality in itself is not problem, but what society makes of it can be a problem Strongly Agree 2 3 4 Strongly Disagree 14. So-called effeminate men are more artistic and sensitive. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 deserve to be ridiculed. 15. Female homosexuality is a threat to many of our basic social institutions 67 Strongly Agree 2 3 4 Strongly Disagree 16. Using drugs or alcohol to “encourage” a woman to have sex with you is gross and unfair. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 OK if you can get away with it 17. Female/Male homosexuality is an inferior form of sexuality Strongly Agree 2 3 4 Strongly Disagree 18. I like fast cars and fast lovers. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 dependable cars and faithful lovers. 19. Lesbians are sick Strongly Agree 2 3 4 Strongly Disagree 20. So-called prick teasers should be forgiven. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 should be raped. 21. Male homosexual couples should be allowed to adopt children the same as heterosexual couples 68 Strongly Agree 2 3 4 Strongly Disagree 22. When I have had a few drinks I mellow out. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 I look for trouble. 23. I think male homosexuality are disgusting Strongly Agree 2 3 4 Strongly Disagree 24. Any man who is a man needs to have sex regularly. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 can do without sex. 25. Male homosexuals should not be allowed to teach in schools Strongly Agree 2 3 4 Strongly Disagree 26. When I have a drink or two I feel ready for whatever happens. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 I like to relax and enjoy myself. 69 27. Male homosexuality is a perversion Strongly Agree 2 3 4 Strongly Disagree 28. When it comes to taking risks I like to play it safe. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 I’m a high roller. 29. Just as in other species, male homosexuality is a natural expression of sexuality in human men Strongly Agree 2 3 4 Strongly Disagree 30. In conflicts with others I win by not fighting. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 I fight to win. 31. If a man has homosexual feelings, he should do everything he can to overcome them Strongly Agree 2 3 32. Getting into fights 4 Strongly Disagree 70 is natural for me. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 never solves an issue. 33. I would not be too upset if I learned that my son were homosexual Strongly Agree 2 3 4 Strongly Disagree 34. When I feel like fighting I try to think of alternatives. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 go for it. 35. Homosexual behavior between men is just plain wrong Strongly Agree 2 3 4 Strongly Disagree 36. Given what I know about fighting, it’s just stupid. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 he who can, fights; he who can’t runs away. 37. The idea of male homosexual marriages seems ridiculous to me Strongly Agree 2 3 4 Strongly Disagree 71 38. When I’m bored I watch TV or read a book. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 I look for excitement. 39. Male homosexuality is merely a different kind of lifestyle that should not be condemned Strongly Agree 2 3 4 Strongly Disagree 40. I like to drive safely, avoiding all unnecessary risks. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 drive fast, right on the edge of danger. 41. So-called pick-ups should expect to put out. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 42. Lesbians have a particular lifestyle 9 choose their men carefully. 72 and should be respected for it. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 but really just need a good, stiff cock. 43. In my opinion I only want to have sex with someone who is in total agreement. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 I never feel bad about my tactics when I have sex 44. If someone challenges you to a fight, there’s no choice but to fight. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 it’s time to talk your way out of it. 45. I would prefer to be a famous scientist. 2 3 4 5 6 7 46. If you insult me, 8 9 famous WWF wrestler. 73 be prepared to back it up. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 I’ll try to turn the other cheek. 74 References Aberson, C. L., & Dora, J. (2003). Reactions to gay men: Contact and overcorrection in an employee selection simulation. Current Psychology: Developmental, Learning, Personality, and Social, 22, 164–174. Basow, S., & Johnson, K. (2000). Predictors of Homophobia in female College Students . Sex Roles, 42(5/6), 391-404. Bogaert, A. F., & Fisher, W. A. (1995). Predictors of university men’s number of sexual partners. The Journal of Sex Research, 32(2), 119–130. Britzman, D. (1997). What is this thing called love?: New discourses for understanding gay and lesbian youth. In S. de Castell & M. Bryson (Eds.), Radical interventions: identity, politics, and differences on educational praxis (pg. 183207). Albany: State University of New York Press. Brown, R. D., Clarke, B., Gortmaker, V., & Robinson-Keilig, R. (2004). Assessing the campus climate for gay, lesbian, bisexual, and transgender (GLBT) students using a multiple perspectives approach. Journal of College Student Development, 45, 8–26. Brown, R. D., Clarke, B., Gortmaker, V., & Robinson-Keilig, R. (2004). Assessing the campus climate for gay, lesbian, bisexual, and transgender (GLBT) students using a multiple perspectives approach. Journal of College Student Butler, J. (1991). Imitaton and gender subordination. In D. Fuss (ed.), Inside/out: Lesbian theories, gay theories (pg.13-31). 75 California Postsecondary Education Commission -- Program Review. (n.d.). California Postsecondary Education Commission - Home Page. Retrieved March 28, 2012, from http://www.cpec.ca.gov/Reviews/ProgramReview.asp Cowan, G., Heiple, B., Marquez, C., Khatchadourian, D., & McNevin, M. (2005). Heterosexuals’ attitudes toward hate crimes and hate speech against gays and lesbians: Craig, S., Tucker, E., & Wagner, E. (2008). Empowering Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, and Transgender Youth: Lessons Learned From a Safe Schools Summit. Journal of Gay & Lesbian Social Services, 20(3), 237-252. Davies, M. (2004). Correlates of Negative Attitudes Toward Gay Men: Sexism, Male Role Norms, and Male Sexuality . The Journal of Sex Research, 41(3), 259266.Development, 45, 8–26.DOI: 10.1080/00918369.2011.605728 Dunkle, J., & Francis, P. (1990). The Role of Facial Masculinity/Femininity in the Attribution of Homosexuality . Sex Roles, 23(3/4), 157-167. Evans, N. J. (2001). The experiences of lesbian, gay, and bisexual youths in university communities. In A. R. D’Augelli & C. Patterson (Eds.), Lesbian, gay, and bisexual identities and youth: Psychological perspectives (pp. 181–198). New York: Oxford University Press. Ferfolja, T. (2007). Schooling Cultures: Institutionalizing Heteronormativity and Heterosexism . International Journal of Inclusive Education, 11(2), 147-162. Fyfe, B. (1983). "Homophobia" or homosexual bias reconsidered. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 12, 549-554. 76 Grossman, A., Haney, A., Edwards, P., Alessi, E., Ardon, M., & Howel, T. (2009). Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual and Transgender Youth Talk about Experiencing and Coping with School Violence: A Qualitative Study. Journal of LGBT Youth, 6, 24–46. Groth, A.N. (1979). Men who rape. New York: Plenum. Herek, G. (1988). Heterosexuals' attitudes Toward Lesbian and Gay Men: Correlates and Gender Differences . The Journal of Sex Research, 25(4), 451-477. Journal of Homosexuality, 58, 1159-1164. Herek, G. (1998). Attitudes Toward Lesbians and Gay Men Scale. In C. Davis, W. Yarber, R. Bauserman, G. Schreer, and S. Davis (Eds.), Handbook of Sexuality Related Measures (pp. 392-394). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Izard, C.E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum Press. Johnson, G. M., & Knight, R. A. (2000). Developmental antecedents of sexual coercion in juvenile sexual offenders. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 12(3), 165–178. Lock, J., & Kleis, B. (1998). Origins of Homophobia in Males Psychosexual Vulnerabilities and Defense Development . American Journal of Psychotherapy, 52(4), 425-436. McConahay, J. B. (1986). Modern racism, ambivalence, and the Modern Racism Scale. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination, and racism (pp. 99–125). Orlando, FL: Academic Press. 77 McCreary, D. (1994). The Male Role and Avoiding Femininity . Sex Roles, 31(9/10), 517-531. Morrison, M. A., & Morrison, T. G. (2002). Development and validation of a scale measuring modern prejudice toward gay men and lesbian women. Journal of Homosexuality, 43(2), 15–37. Mosher, D.L., & Tomkins, S.S. (1988). Scripting the macho man: Hypermasculine socialization and enculturation. Journal of Sex Research, 25, 60-84. Mosher, D.L., Sirkin, M. (1992). Vicarious emotional responses of macho college males. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 7, 165-174. Mosher, D.L., Sirkin, M. (1992). Vicarious emotional responses of macho college males. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 7, 165-174. Neisen, J. (1990). Heterosexism: Redefining Homophobia for the 1990s. Journal of Gay & Lesbian Psychotherap, 1(3), 21-35. Norris, J., George, W. H., Davis, L. C., Martell, J., & Leonesio, R. J. (1999). Alcohol and hyper- masculinity as determinants of men’s empathic responses to violent pornography. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 14, 683–701. Old-fashioned and modern heterosexism. Journal of Homosexuality, 49(2), 67–82. Parrot, D.J., & Zeichner, A. (2003). Effects of Hypermasculinity on Physical Aggression Against Women. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 4, 70-78. Parrott, D. J., Adams, H. E., & Zeichner, A. (2002). Homophobia: Personality and attitudinal correlates. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 1269–1278. 78 Pascarella, E. T., & Terenzini, P. T. (2005). How college affects students. Vol. 2. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Rankin, S. (1998). The campus climate report: Assessment and intervention strategies. In R. Sanlo (Ed.), Working with lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender college students: A handbook for faculty and administrators (pp. 277–284). Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. Rankin, S. (2003). Campus climate for LGBT people: A national perspective. New York: National Gay and Lesbian Task Force Policy Institute. Rankin, S. (2005). Campus climates for sexual minorities. New Directions for Student Services, 111, 17–23. Ray, A. L., & Gold, S. R. (1996). Gender roles, aggression, and alcohol use in dating relation- ships. The Journal of Sex Research, 33(1), 47–55. reaction to the self? Journal of Homosexuality. Robinson, D. T., & Schwartz, J. P. (2004). Relationship between gender role conflict and attitudes toward women and African-Americans. Psychology of Men and Masculinity, 5, 65–71. Ryan, K. M. (1995). Do courtship-violent men have characteristics associated with a “battering personality?” Journal of Family Violence, 10, 99–120. Seidman, S. (1996). The Politics of Inside/out: Queer Theory, Posttructuralism, and a Sociological Approach to sexuality . Queer theory/sociology (pp. 194-212). Cambridge, Mass: Blackwell. 79 Sullivan, J. P., & Mosher, D. L. (1990). Acceptance of guided imagery of marital rape as a function of macho personality. Violence and Victims, 5, 275–286. Swim, J. K., Aiken, K. J., Hall, W. S., & Hunter, B. A. (1995). Sexism and racism: Oldfashioned and modern prejudices. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 199–214. Teicher, M. H., Samson, J. A., Polcari, A., & McGreenery, C. E. (2006). Sticks, stones, and hurtful words: Relative effects of various forms of childhood maltreatment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(6), 993–1000. Theodore, P. S., & Basow, S. A. (in press). Heterosexual masculinity and homophobia: A Tierney,W.,& Dilley, P, (1998). Constructing Knowledge: Educational research and gay and lesbian studies. InW.Pinar (Ed.), Queer Theory in education (pg.49-71). Mahwah, NJ:Lawrence Erlbaum. Tougas, F., Brown, R., Beaton, A. M., & Joly, S. (1995). Neosexism: Plus ça change, plus c’est pareil. [The more things change, the more they are the same]. Personality and So- cial Psychology Bulletin, 21, 842–849. Tougas, F., Desruisseaux, J. C., Desrochers, A., St-Pierre, L., Perrino, A., & de la Sablonnière, R. (2004). Two forms of racism and their related outcomes: The bad and the ugly. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 36, 177–189. Vass, J. S., & Gold, S. R. (1995). Effects of feedback on emotion in hypermasculine males. Violence and Victims, 10(3), 217–226. 80 Watson, D., Clark, L., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063-1070 Wright, P. J., & McKinley, C. J. (2010). Services and information for sexually compulsive students on college counseling center websites: Results from a national sample. Journal of Health Communication