THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN COPING STYLE, OPEN-MINDEDNESS, AND ATTITUDES TOWARD LESBIANS, GAYS, AND BISEXUALS A Thesis Presented to the faculty of the Department of Psychology California State University, Sacramento Submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS in Psychology (Counseling Psychology) by Alexandra P. Haas SUMMER 2012 THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN COPING STYLE, OPEN-MINDEDNESS, AND ATTITUDES TOWARD LESBIANS, GAYS, AND BISEXUALS A Thesis by Alexandra P. Haas Approved by: __________________________________, Committee Chair Rebecca P. Cameron, Ph. D. __________________________________, Second Reader Lawrence S. Meyers, Ph. D. __________________________________, Third Reader Lisa A. Harrison, Ph. D. ____________________________ Date ii Student: Alexandra Patricia Haas I certify that this student has met the requirements for format contained in the University format manual, and that this thesis is suitable for shelving in the Library and credit is to be awarded for the thesis. __________________________, Graduate Coordinator Jianjian Qin, Ph.D. Department of Psychology iii ___________________ Date Abstract of THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN COPING STYLE, OPEN-MINDEDNESS, AND ATTITUDES TOWARD LESBIANS, GAYS, AND BISEXUALS by Alexandra Patricia Haas The present study explored the relationship between open-minded thinking coping styles, and knowledge of and attitudes toward LGB individuals. Three hundred thirty-seven heterosexual college students (79.9% female and 41.4% European-American/White) at a large Western public university volunteered to participate in exchange for course credit. Questionnaires were used to measure study variables. Results of a canonical correlation analysis were consistent with previous research, suggesting that the less open-minded a person is, the less knowledge they have about and more negative attitudes they have toward LGB individuals, including their civil rights. In addition, an individualistic coping style was directly related to endorsement of LGB civil rights, suggesting that valuing autonomy may be connected to valuing others’ rights to autonomy. Future research should continue to evaluate this finding in addition to exploring ways of fostering openminded thinking (Killen, 1997). _______________________, Committee Chair Rebecca P. Cameron, Ph.D. ___________________________________ Date iv ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank my thesis chair, Dr. Rebecca P. Cameron, and my committee members, Dr. Larry S. Meyers and Dr. Lisa A. Harrison, for their guidance and support over the course of my graduate program and for helping me get through the thesis process. In addition, I would like to thank Pegah Naemi for her assistance and statistical knowledge in helping me analyze my data and Brian Hall for editing the many drafts of my thesis. Most importantly, I would like to thank my parents and my aunt, Dr. Patricia Takeda, for allowing me this opportunity to pursue my academic and career goals. Lastly, I would like to thank my friends for their support and encouragement as I complete my journey of graduate school. v TABLE OF CONTENTS Page Acknowledgements ..................................................................................................... vi List of Tables ............................................................................................................ viii List of Figures ..................................................................................... .........................ix Chapter 1. INTRODUCTION …… ..……………………………………………………….. 1 2. LITERATURE REVIEW ...................................................................................... 4 Overview of Attitudes ....................................................................................... 4 Affirmativeness and Positive Correlates ......................................................... 11 Overview of Open-Mindedness ...................................................................... 14 Correlates of Open-Mindedness ..................................................................... 20 Attitudes and Open-Mindedness ..................................................................... 23 Overview of Coping .........................................................................................28 Hypotheses .......................................................................................................34 3. METHOD..... ..........................................................................................................36 Participants .......................................................................................................36 4. RESULTS…….. .................................................................................................... 42 Effective Sample Size ..................................................................................... 42 5. DISCUSSION ........................................................................................................ 52 vi Appendix A. Consent to Participate as a Research Subject ....................................... 57 Appendix B. Debriefing ............................................................................................. 59 Appendix C. Demographics ........................................................................................60 References……………………………………………………………………………63 vii LIST OF TABLES Tables 1. Page Means and Standard Deviations for Predictor and Dependent Variables…………………………….….……………………………………. 43 2. Total Variance Explained………………………………………………….….43 3. Structure Matrix of SACS Subscales with Promax Rotation…………..……. 45 4. Correlations and Standardized Canonical Coefficients between Attitudes toward LGB Individuals, Coping Styles, and Open-mindedness……………..47 5. Bivariate Correlations of Dependent Variables of LGB Attitudes……………48 6. Bivariate Correlations of Predictor Variables………………………………....48 7. Bivarite Correlations Between Predictors and Dependent Variables..….…….49 8. Eigenvalues, Cumulative % of Explained Variance, & Squared Canonical Correlates for Each Canonical Function…………….….……………………..49 viii LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1. Page Interaction between Political affiliation and LG Contact……......…...…….51 ix 1 Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION People who identify themselves as lesbian, gay, or bisexual (LGB) are becoming more visible in today’s society. Increased visibility has been accompanied by increased acceptance among the general population (Yang, 2000), although LGB issues remain highly controversial (Herek, Gillis, & Cogan 2009; Wright, 1999). Specifically, ongoing conflicts related to same-sex marriage, resistance to the recent repeal of the military’s institutionalized policy of discrimination Don’t Ask Don’t Tell (DADT), and the use of gay rights issues by some conservative politicians to polarize and rally the electorate highlight the anti-gay sentiments that are still prevalent in American society. Stotzer (2008) found evidence that compared to older adults, younger cohorts of Americans are increasingly accepting of LGB individuals, which might allow for a gradual shift to new norms of acceptance of LGB people. During this period of societal focus on LGB people and LGB civil rights, it is important to understand factors associated with both positive and negative attitudes toward LGB people. Similar to racism and other oppressive ideologies, heterosexism, defined as “an ideological system that denies, denigrates, and stigmatizes any form of non-heterosexual behavior, identity, relationship, and community” (Herek, 1990, p. 321), is manifest in both social customs and institutions. For example, twenty-nine of the fifty United States currently ban same-sex marriage (New York Times, 2012). Policies that fail to ensure 2 equal rights for all citizens arise from and contribute to beliefs that LGB individuals are abnormal, unnatural, and deserving of hostility (Herek et al., 2009). The term “heterosexism” highlights the parallels between prejudice toward LGB individuals and other forms of prejudice and discrimination and at the same time emphasizes the societallevel ideologies of institutionalized oppression of non-heterosexual people. Anti-gay attitudes function to the preserve the privileged status of heterosexuals; therefore, resistance to change must be overcome on multiple levels for transformation to occur at a societal level. Cultural construction of attitudes toward lesbians, gays, and bisexuals is multifaceted and complex. Past research has focused mainly on negative attitudes such as homophobia and heterosexism. Research has linked negative attitudes toward LGB individuals to personality and demographic characteristics including low openness to experience and values, right-wing authoritarianism, avoidant and defensive coping styles (Johnson, Brems, & Alford-Keating, 1997), little to no contact with LGB individuals, being male, high religiosity, preference for traditional gender roles, and living in Southern states (Keiller, 2010; Stotzer, 2008). Less is known about what predicts positive attitudes toward these populations. However, the research that has been conducted has shown positive attitudes to be linked to high levels of openness to experience, increased contact with LGB people, and high abstract reasoning skills (Cullen, Wright, & Alessandri, 2002; Keiller, 2010; LaMar & Kite, 1998; Stotzer, 2008). Previous research has focused more extensively on attitudes toward gay men; the present study includes 3 attitudes toward lesbians and bisexuals in addition. In an effort to expand our understanding of attitudes toward LGB individuals, open-minded thinking, coping styles, contact with lesbians and gay men, and political orientation are examined in relation to positive and negative attitudes toward LGB people. 4 Chapter 2 LITERATURE REVIEW Overview of Attitudes Historically, both lay people and mental health professionals have generally viewed homosexuality as either a moral failing or a sign of mental illness. In fact, the American Psychological Association (APA) officially considered homosexuality a form of psychopathology until its removal from the Diagnostic Statistical Manual (DSM) in 1973 (Bullough & Bullough, 1977; APA, 2000). Although a great deal of progress has been made in subsequent decades, it is not uncommon for mental health providers to hold negative or ambivalent attitudes about lesbian, gay, bisexual individuals (LGB), and LGB acceptance and civil rights remain a controversial issue in American society and politics. One of the primary reasons that negative attitudes toward LGB individuals persist is that homosexuality is viewed as a deviation from a socially constructed norm that is wideranging and multidimensional (Worthington, Dillon, & Becker-Schutte, 2005). As Cardoso (2010) notes, one’s perception of homosexual and heterosexual categories is related to culture and to social position within that culture. Presumably, the more value an individual places on his or her culture and the more there is to lose by contradicting that culture’s values, the more the individual may cling to socially accepted beliefs, including attitudes towards LGB individuals. Many people still view homosexuality as an illness, sexual deviance, or as a chosen 5 sexual identity that can be reversed. Homosexuality, however, is only considered deviant because society has labeled it as such. This is thought to have at least two primary causes. One is that the United States is a majority Judeo-Christian society (Hartmann, Zhang, & Wischstadt, 2005) and this set of faith traditions has consistently included teachings that homosexuality is immoral. The second has to do with heteronormativity that arises from the fact that heterosexuality is a majority experience, at least in the way it is socially constructed. One result of this assumption is that the concept of “homosexual” as a distinct identity did not exist until recently. When taken to an extreme, negative attitudes toward homosexuality become homophobia, which has been defined as “an unreasoning fear or antipathy toward homosexuality or homosexuals” and as “the dread of being in close proximity of a homosexual” (Pardie & Luschetta, 1999, p. 4). Pardie and Luschetta (1999) note however, that the term homophobia is problematic because homophobic people do not always react to homosexuals in a truly phobic way. For example, some homophobic people actively seek out homosexuals for the purpose of committing violence or other hate crimes against them. In contrast, people, with true phobias often do everything in their power to avoid that which terrifies them. Herek (1984) argues that most research has portrayed homophobia as unidimensional construct of attitudes expressed through irrational fears. He proposes that it would be more useful to view homophobia through a model that distinguishes three types of attitudes: 1) experiential, categorizing past interactions with homosexuals, 2) defensive i.e., coping with inner conflicts by projecting 6 them onto homosexual persons, and 3) symbolic, expressing abstract concepts linked to one’s notion of self and social network. Additionally, LaMar and Kite (1998) identify two components of negative attitudes toward homosexuality. The first of these is the perception that homosexuality is immoral. The second component is a general condemnation of homosexual individuals based on the notion that their gender role violation threatens the social order (LaMar & Kite, 1998, p.190). Kite and Whitley (1996) divided attitudes toward homosexuality into three subcomponents: 1) attitudes towards homosexual persons by means of social restrictions, 2) attitudes towards homosexual behavior as moral reprehensibility of homosexuality, and 3) attitudes towards homosexual persons’ civil rights such as free speech, parental rights, and same sex marriage An attitude is “a psychological tendency that is expressed by evaluating a particular entity with some degree of favor or disfavor” (Herek et al., 2009, p. 34). A negative attitude reflects low self-esteem and psychological distress (Herek et al., 2009). Those who study positive psychology have made it clear that the absence of a negative is not the same as the presence of a positive and have demonstrated the independent nature of positive and negative constructs (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). Within the research literature, studies have mainly examined negative attitudes, without examining predictors of positive attitudes. Attitudes are important in part because they predict behavior. For example, prejudicial attitudes may be linked to actively discriminatory or hateful behavior (Herek et al., 2009). Herek and colleagues (2009) found that attitudes were 7 “associated to beliefs positive or negative regarding sexual orientation, affective stance toward belonging to a sexual orientation group, and past actions relevant to sexual orientation” (p. 36). One important finding reported by Worthington et al. (2005) states that negative feelings and thoughts about LGB orientations or individuals are associated with self-reports of violence and hate crimes. They emphasize “anti-gay attitudes function toward the preservation of the privileged status of heterosexuals which is thought to be developed through gendered socialization” (p. 105). According to Worthington and colleagues (2005), antigay violence reflects attitudes encompassing hatred and violence that go beyond the level of “condemnation.” A notable omission is that attitudes toward lesbians and bisexuals have not been examined to the extent that attitudes toward gay men have. With the increased visibility of bisexual individuals it is important to explore heterosexuals’ attitudes toward them as well. Research that has looked at attitudes toward lesbians has found significant gender differences; for instance, men tend to express less negativity toward lesbians than toward gay men (LaMar & Kite, 1998). Stigma Society as a whole devalues homosexuality in comparison to heterosexuality and hostility and malevolent stereotypes associated with these populations are prevalent (Herek et al., 2009). Herek and colleagues describe heterosexism as a system of attitudes, bias, and discrimination favoring heterosexuality (Herek et al., 2009, p. 33). Heterosexism occurs through two general processes: it exists because society presumes 8 that everyone is heterosexual and because LGB individuals are largely unrecognized by social institutions (Herek et al., 2009). Heterosexism is also independent from the prejudice of individual members of society (Herek et al., 2009). This gives reason for the general public to make LGB individuals feel of a lesser status than heterosexuals because heterosexuals are seen as ideal members of society (Herek, 2009). Haider-Markel and Joslyn (2008) found that the belief that one’s sexual orientation is involuntary is associated with lower levels of heterosexual prejudice. Society’s reaction to LGB individuals often mirrors the reaction to racial minorities. For example, both groups are often accused of or blamed for drug abuse, sexually transmitted diseases, child abuse, and drug addiction. In addition, both groups regularly experience discrimination in housing, employment, insurance, and health care (Whitley, 2009). Whitley (2009) found that people who score high on measures of negative attitudes toward racial ethnicities also score high on negative attitudes toward lesbians and gay men. Past social psychology research has found that the greater the extent to which in-group members believe the out-group poses a threat, the more likely they are to have negative attitudes toward the out-group, in this case lesbians and gay men, because they are often perceived as violating American values (Whitley, 2009). A literature review conducted by Cullen and colleagues (2002) identified the following partial list of variables correlated with and known to influence homophobia: an individual’s support for traditional gender roles, gender, religious affiliation, degree of religiosity, amount of personal contact with a gay man or lesbian, coping style, and 9 degree of empathy. According to Cullen et al. (2002), the amount of contact with LGB individuals was found to be a critical predictor of homophobia. Specifically, those who had never had any contact with an LGB individual indicated higher rates of homophobia. Pardie and Luschetta (1999) state that stereotypes and stigma are the single greatest impediment to gay and lesbian equality. For example, they report one study linking AIDS/HIV related stigma to negative attitudes toward lesbians and gay men (Pardie & Luschetta, 1999). According to Herek and colleagues (2009), sexual stigma is defined as “the negative regard, inferior status, and relative powerlessness that society collectively accords anyone associated with non-heterosexual behaviors” (p. 33). Researchers use a variety of related terms to refer to the stigma surrounding LGB individuals, such as heterosexism, sexual prejudice, and sexual stigma (Herek, et al., 2009). Herek and colleagues (2009) discussed the importance of exploring sexual stigma as it relates to heterosexuals’ attitudes toward lesbian, gay, and bisexual people. Herek’s social psychological model for understanding sexual stigma describes its structural and individual manifestations while highlighting parallels between the stigma-related experiences of sexual minorities and heterosexuals. Herek et al. (2009) states that most of us are socialized about sexual stigma as children through childhood experiences because it is expected that we will be heterosexual. The internalized sexual stigma is defined as “a heterosexual or sexual minority person’s acceptance of sexual stigma as part of their value system” (p. 33) and involves adapting one’s self-concept to be congruent with society’s expectations. Additionally, sexual orientation can often be hidden and people 10 may be perceived as a different sexual orientation than they are. One consequence of such socialization reported by Whitley (2009) is that those who believe LGB persons have a choice in their sexual orientation were more likely to hold negative attitudes toward them. In other words, the belief that a behavior is under the control of the stigmatized person is linked to more negative attitudes toward the stigmatized person (Whitley, 2009). Herek and colleagues (2009) identify three ways sexual stigma, or sexual prejudice, is manifest: “through acts of shunning, antigay epithets, overt discrimination, and violence”; altering behavior in ways to conceal being recognized as part of this stigmatized group”; and lastly, “adopting one’s self concept to be congruent with the stigmatizing responses of society” (p. 33). This occurs because of the shared knowledge of society’s collective reaction to homosexuality and the anticipation of suffering enactments of stigma in a given situation (Herek et al., 2009). An example of this is that both heterosexuals and non-heterosexuals engage in modifying their behaviors due to felt stigma, such as gender non-conformity and avoidance of physical contact with same-sex friends. For LGB individuals, the perception that one’s culture is homophobic is associated with higher levels of depression and perceived stress, and with lower selfesteem. This provides support for the association between negative attitudes and the effects on well-being (Mireshghi & Matsumoto, 2008). 11 Affirmativeness and Positive Correlates Civil Rights Endorsement Little research has focused on the development of positive attitudes and the factors that contribute to heterosexuals affirming LGB individuals. A change in attitudes toward these populations is mostly credited to the gay and lesbian civil rights movement, which first entered the national consciousness during protests such as the Stonewall riots (Wright, 1999) and continues into the present with the recent struggle to end the military’s “Don’t Ask, Don’t Tell” policy as well as through the ongoing debate over gay marriage. In recent years, the movement has included an increasing number of heterosexual advocates, demonstrating that this minority group is not fighting civil rights battles and struggles alone (Stotzer, 2009). In order for equal rights to occur for LGB individuals, changes in legal protections are a crucial factor. In a meta-analysis by Kite and Whitley (1996), heterosexuals were found to be more accepting of gay civil rights than of homosexual behavior. They note that gender does not seem to affect heterosexual endorsement of gay civil rights because “evaluations of civil rights issues are probably based on global beliefs about Americans’ civil liberties” (Kite & Whitley, 1996, p. 339). The laws can either reinforce beliefs people may have on a particular issue or encourage change in those beliefs. Those who know an LGB individual and endorse civil rights are more inclined to be LGB affirmative and hold positive attitudes toward LGB (Barber, 2003). 12 LGB Knowledge and Contact As LGB individuals become more visible in mainstream culture, it is likely that there will be a corresponding increase in heterosexuals’ knowledge of LGB history, symbols, and community (Worthington et al., 2005). Additionally, the highest levels of LGB-affirmativeness require heterosexuals to gain knowledge of LGB history, symbols, and community, and to recognize heteronormativity in a way that goes beyond mere tolerance (Worthington et al., 2005). Stotzer (2008) discusses the role pop culture television shows and the media have played in providing knowledge and contributing toward the development of positive attitudes toward LGB individuals. Media portrayals of LGB individuals are one way the general population can gain familiarity with these populations, albeit indirectly. Stotzer (2008) found that parents’ attitudes toward LGB individuals are positively correlated with their children’s attitudes; however, there is a much stronger positive correlation with peer’s attitudes about LGB individuals, suggesting that knowledge is gained from various sources. Fingerhut (2011) argued that “high-quality contact experiences allow majority group members (heterosexuals) to discuss and reflect on the role of sexual orientation in their lives and in society, which, in turn, inspires action” (Fingerhut, 2011, p.2234). In other words, normalizing homosexuality by challenging myths and stereotypes about LGB people increases reflection and thought about one’s own sexuality. Additionally, Cullen et al. (2002) found that previous experience with a homosexual individual might lower one’s prejudice because as a heterosexual individual gains further exposure to and 13 understanding of a different group, feelings of discomfort, disgust, and anxiety are significantly lowered. Research has identified early normalizing childhood experiences, meeting LGB peers in high school and college, and experiences of empathy based on an LGB peer’s struggles and successes, or resistance to hatred expressed by those who hold negative attitudes as important in the development of positive attitudes toward LGB population (Stotzer, 2008). Worthington and colleagues (2005) describe being “fully LGB affirmative” as reevaluating one’s identity as heterosexual through sexual identity exploration, past or current, and synthesis of sexual identity. They go on to state “heterosexual individuals who have engaged in meaningful sexual self-exploration are more likely to be affirming of LGB individuals because their personal sexual experiences are likely to be more varied, making them less susceptible to homophobic confusion” (p. 105). Aside from the correlates just discussed, other correlates of positive attitudes are high levels of empathy (Fingerhut, 2011; Stotzer, 2008), female gender (Stotzer, 2008;), higher levels of education (Fingerhut, 2011; Lambert, Ventura, Hall & Cluse-Tular, 2006), openness to experience (Cullen et al., 2002; Proctor & McCord, 2009; Baron, Struckman-Johnson, Quevillon, & Banka, 2008; Stotzer, 2008) and high abstract reasoning (Keiller, 2010). Certain cognitive traits, such as actively open-minded thinking, may also be linked to positive attitudes. This cognitive style is similar to abstract reasoning, in the sense that they both involve perspective taking. Open-minded thinking may be teachable and may benefit individuals and society by promoting 14 listening to and considering multiple points of view as a part of evaluating one’s own beliefs about an issue (Baron, 1993). Overview of Open-Mindedness Openness has been looked at as a personality trait and as a cognitive capacity. It can be seen as the converse of several constructs that have been linked to homophobia, such as authoritarianism, fundamentalism, and LGB contact. Initial research on the personality construct of openness to experience suggests that greater openness to experience is linked to more positive attitudes toward LGB persons. As a cognitive construct, abstract reasoning has been linked to lower homophobic attitudes. A newer approach to openness focuses on open-mindedness, borrowing from openness to experience, but emphasizing the cognitive style of seeking disconfirming information. Open-mindedness has not yet been examined as a personality characteristic within the context of attitudes toward LGB individuals. This is an interesting omission, considering that many people, both among the general public and among researchers exploring positive psychology, view open-mindedness as a virtue. Baerh (2011) describes openmindedness as a virtue that allows one to “think outside the box, to conceive of an explanation” that lies within unfamiliar territory (p. 207). Essentially, open-mindedness is the ability and willingness to suspend temporarily one’s prior belief in order to give a fair and impartial hearing to an opposing belief, argument, or body of evidence (Baehr, 2011). Additionally, a crucial element of open-mindedness is the ability to adopt a new mode of thinking when new evidence suggests that previously held beliefs are obsolete or 15 incorrect. Previous research on character strengths, such as open-mindedness, has focused primarily on the relationship of these strengths to psychological health and wellbeing. For example, Proctor, Maltby, and Linley (2011) found that from a list of 24 character strengths, open-mindedness was among the top five virtues linked to people with a positive self-identity and strong concept of self. This may be because people who feel safe and secure in their own concept of self are less likely to feel threatened by ideas and beliefs that contradict their own. When presented with an idea, belief, or argument that stands in opposition to an individual’s own beliefs, an open-minded person possesses both the emotional and intellectual strength to assess the new information from an unbiased point of view (Baehr, 2011). In addition to rational assessment and evaluation, open-mindedness requires other cognitive activities such as the ability to comprehend unfamiliar or challenging subject matter (Baehr, 2011). As issues surrounding the LGB community are often unfamiliar to a heterosexual audience, the need to be able to comprehend this information makes openmindedness a relevant topic of study in regard to the formation of attitudes toward the LGB community. Thus, for the purposes of this study “open-mindedness” will be defined as being receptive to new ideas, beliefs, or arguments and possessing the ability to both comprehend that information and then assess it from an impartial perspective. While there appears to be a consensus among both the general public and researchers that open-mindedness is a positive trait, the concept itself is multifaceted and rather ambiguous. There is no simple formula for identifying open-mindedness but an 16 open-minded person would presumably exhibit many, if not all, of the following characteristics: a preference for deep thought and reflection over impulsivity; a desire to seek information that contradicts their currently held beliefs; a willingness to adapt beliefs as new information becomes available; the disposition to spend a great deal of time on a problem before giving up; and the skill of unbiased reasoning or giving equal weight to opinions and evidence with which they disagree. These characteristics, which have been identified by various researchers (Stanovich & West, 1997; Fingerhut, 2011; Bernstein, 2000) are interrelated and overlap in many crucial ways. As Milton Rokeach proposes, knowing how an individual forms her or his belief system may allow us to predict how they will respond to threats to their belief system. Bernstein (2000) and Fingerhut (2011) both stress that in order to challenge any homophobic or heterosexist bias, a heterosexual individual must be able to acknowledge that differences in sexual orientation exist and possess the willingness to reflect on and examine her or his own privileged heterosexual status. This is also similar to Stanovich and West’s characteristic of reflection rather than impulsivity. According to Stanovich and West (1997), “reasoning objectively about data and arguments that contradict prior beliefs is often seen as the quintessence of thought” and suggest the importance of examining individual differences (p. 342). It is essential to identify cognitive components and relate these differences to other cognitive traits and personality variables (Stanovich & West, 1997). Further, “the perseverance of mistaken beliefs taint our perception of new data, whereas pre-existing accurate beliefs serve to provide a more 17 accurate picture of the world and inform our understanding more than distort it” (Stanovich & West, 1997, p. 343). They argue that people tend to only engage in logical reasoning about a premise when the belief seems questionable or inaccurate but are quick to accept other opinions and beliefs when they seem believable. This relates to one’s disposition to spend time on a problem before giving up and varies based on the individual. This also relates to the ability to give equal thought to differing opinions in relation to ones’ own. For example, allowing same-sex marriage may contradict or pose a threat to one’s beliefs about marriage, however, being open to hearing different perspectives on the issue demonstrates one’s cognitive ability to think critically about it in the process of forming one’s own position. Milton Rokeach, one of the leading researchers in the area of dogmatism and open-mindedness, began his research by examining what behaviors and thoughts indicate that someone is open or closed in their way of thinking. In addition, he explored what characteristics are involved in all belief systems. If we know how an individual believes, also referred to as the individual’s “cognitive style,” we may be able to predict his or her problem-solving behavior. According to Rokeach (1960), a closed way of thinking typically involves conflict about who is right and who is wrong, who is rational and who is rationalizing, and conflict over whose convictions are dogmatic and whose are intellectual. Within this construction of closed-mindedness, the dialogue between the individual and cultural authority figures is responsible for either rejection or acceptance of (1) ideas, which are categorized as a cognitive phenomenon, (2) prejudice or 18 intolerance toward a particular group of people, and (3) authority as authoritarianism (Rokeach, 1960). Theoretically, those with relatively closed belief systems will experience greater isolation because their belief system makes it more difficult to coexist with those who hold contradictory beliefs. This is partially the result in having faith in an authority and a refusal to believe that there could be multiple solutions to a given problem, and thus remaining dogmatically loyal to a closed belief system. On the contrary, less dependence on authority allows for greater communication among differing belief systems creating a more open system (Rokeach, 1960). “When authority is seen as absolute, it leads to extreme cognitive distinctions between faithful and unfaithful… American and un-American” (Rokeach, 1960, p. 45). Those who agree will be accepted and those who disagree will be rejected. According to Rokeach (1960), a closed way of thinking is associated with an authoritarian outlook on life, intolerance toward those with opposing beliefs and acceptance of those with similar beliefs. Previous research has identified authoritarianism as a potential factor that affects attitudes toward lesbian, gay, and bisexual individuals. Whether someone is open or closed-minded can be “observed in the ‘practical’ world of politics and religion in addition to the more academic world of science, philosophy, and humanistic thought” (Rokeach, 1960, p.5). Researchers of belief systems view open-mindedness and closed-mindedness as being on a continuum with both ends considered extreme (Rokeach, 1960; Stanovich & West, 1997). 19 Jonathon Baron (1997) has also been an important contributor to openmindedness research specifically in regard to the benefits of teaching actively openminded thinking. Stanovich and West, who have been heavily influenced by Baron’s work, use his concept and definition of open-minded thinking in the construction of their actively open-minded thinking scale (Stanovich & West, 1997). Baron (1997) argues that learning what to believe is a formative experience within a society. He argues that a main determinant of how people think is how they believe they ought to think. Furthermore, he provides evidence regarding errors in thinking, such as “myside bias” meaning people tend to respond more to evidence on their own side of an issue. Baron states that the two errors in thinking are myside bias and the use of inferential heuristics without understanding their purpose. Both of these errors are counteracted when the individual is taught actively open-minded thinking. Open-minded thinking challenges the individual to consider alternative possibilities, neglected goals, and counter evidence (Baron, 1990). Baron (1990) concludes that the absence of open-minded thinking is harmful because it leads to the use of harmful heuristics in judgments and decisions. He goes on to say that thinking is made up of search and inference. “We search for possibilities, answers, and goals…possibilities are potential answers to our questions… evidence is whatever bears on the strength of possibility, and goals are the standards we use to evaluate possibilities in light of the evidence” (p. 394). Baron goes on to describe the errors that are made when we use “closed-minded” thinking such as missing something 20 during the search for possibilities or evidence and neglecting goals. Secondly, we make incorrect inferences from the evidence we have due to faulty heuristics (Baron, 1990). Studying open-minded thinking may provide support for its teachable characteristics and in turn be useful in efforts to influence positive attitudes toward LGB individuals. Correlates of Open-Mindedness One negative correlate to open-mindedness is resistance to the idea of changing a behavior, belief, or personality. Rokeach (1960) suggests this is because we explain resistance to change in terms of conformity, rigidity, and intolerance, traits that are all diametrically opposed to the characteristics of open-mindedness. Those whose resistance to change is at the extreme end of the spectrum are labeled dogmatic. Cullen and colleagues (2002) define dogmatism as “a closed cognitive structure blocking an individual from learning [or accepting] anything different from what has already been established as societal norms” (p.123). To the dogmatic mind, the belief structure is an inflexible whole and cannot be separated thus when a particular belief is threatened, “the preservation of his total system will be at stake rather than the preservation of a particular belief in his system” (p. 183). Right Wing Authoritarianism A tendency towards right-wing authoritarianism is another negative correlate of open-mindedness. The tendency toward specifically right wing authoritarianism involves a conservative orientation to preserving traditional values and the status quo as opposed to extending new rights and liberties. Rokeach (1960) defines authoritarianism as being 21 “intolerant of those who disagree and closed in mode of thought and belief” (p. 15). According to Cullen et al. (2002), the authoritarian personality type is well documented as one that is prejudicial, seeks to impose his or her own will and beliefs on others, and then demands conformity to those beliefs. Thus, authoritarian personality is incompatible with open-minded thinking. The cognitive rigidity exhibited by the dogmatic and authoritarian belief systems serves the purpose of controlling and making sense of ambiguous stimuli (Stanovich & West, 1997). New ideas that conform to the existing belief system are quickly deemed acceptable while ideas that do not conform can be quickly dismissed. Separate from the issue of the relative merits or disadvantages of this mode of thinking, it is easy to understand the appeal of a thought process that quickly eliminates ambiguity. The result of this rigidity, however, is that the individual is more likely to exhibit prejudice towards others (Cullen et al., 2002). In fact, Cullen et al. (2002) report that cognitive rigidity may also be part of a general set of attitudes that make up the prejudicial personality. In contrast, an open-minded person possesses the willingness to set aside temporarily his or her own beliefs in order to assess a new belief or argument from a neutral position (Baehr, 2011). The result is that the open-minded person’s belief about any particular idea will be based on a fair and rational analysis rather than an emotional response. Unfortunately, this comes at the cost of living with a level of ambiguity that may be uncomfortable for many people. 22 Knowledge Rokeach (1960) discusses the relative amount of knowledge a person possesses, (facts, ideas, and interpretations consistent with his or her belief system) as a positive correlate to open-mindedness, generally resulting in more differentiation, or richness of detail, within his or her belief system. Ways of increasing knowledge that have been researched include exposure to the mass media (Pardie & Luscetta, 1999), contact with those who differ in beliefs and opinions (Worthington et al., 2005), and receiving higher education (Stotzer, 2009). In addition, Cosme, Pepino, and Brown (2010) identify a liberal political orientation and empathy as being positively correlated with open-minded thinking using Rokeach’s (1960) dogmatism scale to measure the inverse of openmindedness. In their study, they argue that little research has looked at the relationship of empathy to open-mindedness and discuss the similarities stating, “Both involve understanding another person's ideas or state of mind, and perhaps as a consequence, gaining some form of acceptance from this understanding” (para. 3). Cosme and colleagues (2010) comment on the finding that those with a liberal political orientation are generally more open to different experiences and are also understanding of the inclusion of diverse populations in society. They further provide support that a moderate or liberal political orientation is positively correlated with flexibility, receptiveness, and tolerance, in addition to open-minded thinking. 23 Attitudes and Open-Mindedness Little research has been conducted in the area of cognitive functioning (thinking, memory, and perception) and social attitudes. Of the research that has been conducted on attitudes, the focus has largely been on ethnic prejudice and discrimination. With the increased awareness of homophobia and its destructive consequences, with similarities to prejudice exhibited toward ethnic minorities, it is important to examine attitudes toward these populations. In regards to in-group versus out-group behavior, Rokeach states that sharing similar or compatible beliefs is often more important than having a shared ethnic or religious background. In other words, when two individuals share the same beliefs, they are more likely to overlook or minimize their ethnic, religious, and perhaps sexual differences. Rokeach (1960) provides an important finding that people high in ethnic prejudice and authoritarianism have more cognitive rigidity, are narrow in their grasp of particular topics, have an increase in memory distortions, and decreased intolerance of ambiguity. High levels of openness were associated with tolerance for ambiguity and increased mental flexibility while those with low levels of openness were more often male, less likely to have had meaningful contact with homosexuals, and more homophobic (Stotzer, 2008). Baron and colleagues (2008) further add valuing others’ perspectives and an increased tolerance of ambiguity to the characteristic of openness. Rokeach (1960) elaborates on this idea of ambiguity by discussing a psychoanalytic finding that suggests that strict and authoritarian parenting during child development may make children more likely to adopt their parents’ rigid worldview. If, however, the 24 parents are more permissive, children may initially adopt the parents’ stereotypical views about ethnic minorities but they will not be as rigidly dedicated to that belief. Keiller (2010) reports further data on the relationship between cognitive resources and prejudice stating, “Automatically elicited stereotypes may be suppressed or tempered by controlled processing (conscious and deliberate mental efforts)” (p. 915). Cognitive resources, like open-mindedness, are defined as enabling people to minimize bias in the process of perceiving and thinking about other groups of people. An important study by Keiller (2010), found that low abstract reasoning, defined as “being able to take the perspectives of others and consider multiple aspects of other people that contradict one’s beliefs (p.915),” was a predictor of anti-gay attitudes and right wing authoritarianism. Other predictors of anti-gay attitudes included the participant being male and having little to no contact with LGB individuals. Potential limitations of this study are that it focused on attitudes toward gay men only and measured abstract reasoning using an inventory of Piaget’s Developmental tasks. Along these same lines, Johnson, Brems, and AlfordKeating (1997) found that the inability to understand others’ perspectives was linked to increased levels of homophobic attitudes. Therefore, in the current study, it is predicted that a closely related cognitive trait, such as open mindedness, would produce similar attitudes toward lesbians and bisexuals in addition to LGB people. Openness to Experience The construct of openness to experience, included in the Big Five Personality Model, is defined as “the propensity to pursue aesthetic interests, and to being open to a 25 variety of novel ideas, values, and experiences” (Hong, Paunonen, & Slade, 2008, p. 161). Stanovich and West adopted and expanded on the cognitive aspects of openness in the development of their construct of actively open-minded thinking. To date, more research has been conducted on the link between openness to experience and attitudes toward LGB individuals than has been conducted on the links between open-mindedness and these attitudes. Openness is similar to open-mindedness in the sense that it affects social perceptions and the formation of social attitudes including choice of friends and political affiliation (McCrae & Sutin, 2009). Openness signifies individuals who are intellectually curious, hold unconventional beliefs and attitudes, are more creative and aware of their feelings, and open to a variety of experiences (Proctor & McCord, 2009; Baron et al., 2008). McCrae and Sutin (2009) acknowledge that there are substantial correlations with the openness facet of the NEO-PI-R with other openness measures and measures of cognitive processes. The openness-values facet measures receptivity to non-traditional values; traditional values were found to be correlated with negative attitudes toward LGB individuals (Steffens, 2005) and low levels of openness (Stotzer, 2009; Keiller, 2010). McCrae and Sutin (2009) found openness to values to have a strong inverse correlation with dogmatic, or closed thinking, and authoritarianism. Stanovich and West’s (1997) scale includes eight items from the values facet of the openness to experience scale from the well-known NEO-PI-R measure (Stanovich & West, 1997). Stotzer (2008) reports that the relative level of openness is an important 26 predictor of homophobia because it reflects dogmatism and cognitive rigidity. Stotzer’s study differs from the existing literature by focusing on positive attitudes toward lesbians, gays, and bisexuals rather than negative attitudes. Stotzer (2008) studied a sample of 68 participants utilizing a half hour semi-structured interview method asking participants about their thoughts, beliefs, and attitudes regarding homosexuality and bisexuality. A potential limitation of Stotzer’s study (2008) was the use of Herek’s Attitudes Toward Lesbians and Gay Men short form scale, which does not assess attitudes about bisexuality. However, one strength was gathering data by interviewing participants. To gain a better understanding of predictors related to attitudes toward lesbians, gays, and bisexuals, a larger sample size would be required as well as taking into consideration whether questionnaires and interviews yield similar findings or whether social desirability plays a differential role across these two methodologies. Cullen et al. (2002) also found openness to experience to be correlated with a decrease in homophobia suggesting that openness to having contact with lesbian, gay, and bisexual individuals may allow for a decrease of unrealistic fears and expectations. Two studies conducted by Proctor and McCord (2009) similarly compared openness to experience with religious fundamentalism and open-mindedness using a different measure than the one developed by Stanovich and West, and found openness to experience to be negatively correlated with religious fundamentalism and positively correlated with open-mindedness. Although this study examined participant’s openness to different religions and tolerance toward contact with an ethnic minority, it is expected 27 that similar correlations will be found when examining the relationship between openmindedness and attitudes toward lesbian, gay, and bisexual individuals. A study by Baron and colleagues (2008) examined heterosexual men’s attitudes toward gay men and correlated personality and demographic variables of anti-gay attitudes including openness, sexism, defensiveness, contact, religiosity, and political affiliation. Using a relatively large sample size (N=243), they found the previously mentioned variables to be correlated with anti-gay attitudes. The more closed-minded a person’s belief system is, the more difficulty would be encountered in problem solving within a new belief system. If new beliefs, or information, are not really accepted, then they will not be remembered, and would be less likely to be synthesized or integrated into a person’s belief system (Rokeach, 1960). Rokeach (1960) further says that not just thought and memory processes are involved in the openness or closedness of a mind, but also the emotional and motivational attitude of the thinker. An individual who resists viewing problems from different perspectives or dislikes new ideas would typically respond to new situations, such as individuals identifying themselves as LGB, with rejection and defensiveness, both considered ways of coping. Rokeach (1960) acknowledges the role of personality variables in problemsolving behavior and basic emotional attitudes, which may help or hinder problemsolution. Emotions during change have an effect on our attitudes also, by being positive or negative depending on the person and how he or she anticipates or experiences the 28 outcomes and processes (Smollen, Sayers, & Matheny, 2010). Smollen, Sayers, and Matheny (2010) acknowledge that emotions have cognitive antecedents and behavioral outcomes such as acting out positive/negative attitudes, which either help or hinder the implementation of change. For example, anxiety or anger can result from change that it is too quick, too slow, or poorly timed. Ongoing change could maintain a feeling of anxiety or anger as participants struggle with constant adaptation. The slow process of acceptance of LGB people may create ongoing stress for some, requiring the use of coping resources to adapt. “Dogmatism and closed-minded thinking serves the purpose of ensuring continued existence of the institution and belief system for which it stands. On the individual level, threat may arise out of adverse experiences. To varying degrees, individuals may become disposed to accept or form closed systems of thinking to the degree to which they are made to feel alone, isolated, and helpless in society in which they live causing anxiety for what the future holds. Attempts made to overcome such feelings may result in compensatory and discriminating attitudes” (Rokeach, 1960, p. 360). In other words, acceptance of LGB individuals within our heteronormative society may be threatening to a heterosexual individual and his or her approach to coping with threat and stress may be relevant to his or her attitudes toward LGB individuals. Overview of Coping One potential correlate of attitudes toward LGB individuals that has not received much focus in the research literature is coping style. Coping style can be defined as the 29 characteristic way in which an individual confronts and attempts to resolve problems and challenges and the typical efforts made to manage stress. This means that anything people do or think about to deal with a situation, regardless of how well the strategy may work, is part of their coping style (Alexander, 2008; Hobfoll et al., 1996). Aldwin (2000) noted that past research has predominately identified coping strategies associated with poor outcomes rather than focusing on strategies that are associated with positive outcomes. Underlying much of the coping literature is the assumption that the ways in which people manage their stress, rather than simply the existence of stress, is what leads to distress and dysfunction. Some coping styles are maladaptive, in that they distort reality, whereas others are considered adaptive and healthy. Coping researchers have attempted to simplify the broad array of coping strategies by dichotomizing coping into problem-focused vs. emotion-focused, situational vs. dispositional coping, and active vs. passive/avoidant coping. Hobfoll and his colleagues proposed that the emphasis on the distinction between problem-focused and emotion-focused strategies was limited in that it did not account for the social valence of coping, and therefore tended to privilege characteristically masculine forms of coping as effective and condemn characteristically feminine forms of coping as ineffective (Dunahoo, Hobfoll, Monnier, Hulsizer, & Johnson, 2002; Hobfoll, Dunahoo, Ben-Porath, & Monnier, 1994). However, coping that is understood as problem-focused or active may vary from individualistic, or even exploitive, to highly prosocial, involving team efforts and social network based strategies. Monnier et al. 30 (1998) acknowledged and demonstrated empirically that the majority of coping measures did not tap instrumental means of using supportive coping strategies. The dual-axis model not only addressed the activity dimension of coping, but also highlighted the existence of both pro-social and antisocial means of problem-solving in order to increase our understanding of social consequences of coping (Monnier et al., 1998). Building on the original dual-axis model, they proposed a multi-axial model of coping that subdivided active coping into strategies along a prosocial to individualistic continuum; furthermore, in an effort to capture cultural variation in coping, they included indirect approaches in addition to direct strategies. The active-passive dimension reflects how active or passive-avoidant a person is in achieving his or her goals and the pro-social-anti-social dimension “depicts the degree to which individuals act in terms of their social interactions while seeking their goals” (Monnier et al., 1998, p. 249). The direct-indirect dimension aims to “describe cultural and gender differences in coping style and circumstantial constraints versus allowances for direct action” (Monnier et al., 1998, p. 250). The Strategic Approach to Coping Scale (SACS), developed by Hobfoll and colleagues (1994), was developed to facilitate research on the Multiaxial Model of Coping. Compared to other coping models, this model of coping depicts shared reliance rather than self-reliance as a healthier way to cope (Monnier et al., 1998). Monnier et al. (1998) further state that individualistically oriented measures of coping disfavor women by ignoring pro-social and anti-social aspects. Monnier et al. (1998) found the Multi- 31 Axial Model of Coping able to balance individualistic and collectivist notions that lead to successful predictions of coping outcomes under stressful conditions without disfavoring women. Dunahoo and colleagues (1998) cite Riger’s (1993) critique of the individualistic bias of problem-focused coping, asserting that “individualism pits man against the elements in a fight for survival, a viewpoint that esteems control and action and ignores social and communal aspects of coping” (Dunahoo et al., 1998, p. 138). The individualistic coping model de-emphasizes the influence of the social environment and misses important gender, ethnic, and cultural differences, therefore Dunahoo, et al. (1998) argues the multi axial model is necessary. The individualistic model operates under the idea that “the healthy individual is one who is self-contained, independent and self-reliant, capable of asserting himself an influencing his environment” (as cited in Dunahoo et al., 1998). Folkman and Lazarus (1984) propose that the major alternatives to action are typically individualistic, specifically avoidance or attempts to reduce discomforting emotions. “Pro-social coping is thought to provide additional resources to successfully cope with stress over time, whereas, coping in a manner that strains one’s support network such as individual or anti-social coping may deplete social resources needed for future stress events” (Monnier et al., 1998, p. 4). Monnier, et al. (1998) discussed that pro-social behavior benefited an individual by building relationships and enhancing a person’s support network allowing for future support if needed. 32 Within Western cultures, individualistic coping styles are idealized because they promote the Westernized values of autonomy and independence compared to nonWestern cultures that utilize a more communal approach to coping (Bardi & Guerra, 2011). Prior research has emphasized coping as individualistic, drawing from a sense of personal agency or control over one’s personal problems rather than the social and communal aspects of coping (Dunahoo et al., 1998). Monnier and colleagues (1998) identify one limitation of the coping literature by acknowledging too much focus being placed on individual outcomes, such as anxiety level, rather than the support system surrounding the individual. For instance, one’s coping behavior may reduce the distress he or she is experiencing yet damage interpersonal relationships, or help an individual to progress while hindering another’s progress or goals (Monnier et al., 1998). Hobfoll, et al. (1998) stated that past coping research has promoted problem focused coping and personal agency, ignoring the potential for this approach to be anti-social having a negative effect on others, whereas support seeking has been devalued as a passive coping style. Prior to the multi axial model of coping, Folkman and Lazarus (1991) examined coping as emotion-focused versus problem-focused and introduced a psychoanalytic ego model involving realistic thoughts and behaviors that solve problems and reduce stress. They defined coping as “consisting of cognitive and behavioral efforts to manage specific external and internal demands that are exceeding the resources of a person” (Folkman & Lazarus, 1991, p. 190). Hobfoll and colleagues attempted to have a more balanced model 33 that addressed both individualistic and communal orientations. In addition, they confronted and addressed that coping has relational consequences that can be either prosocial, for example seeking out social support to tackle a problem, or antisocial perhaps by attacking others to meet a goal. Coping styles that become habitual may be seen as dispositional coping, described as the general tendency to approach problems with a characteristic set of behaviors. Situational coping as defined by Monnier et al. (1998) is using a specific set of coping behaviors to address any given situation. Monnier, et al. (1998), acknowledge that people will utilize different coping styles based on their environment and this flexibility is considered a strength. Aldwin (2000) described dispositional coping styles as being learned early on as effective ways of reducing stress leading to viewing dispositional coping as a personality characteristic. Gender Differences in Coping Coping research has also largely focused on gender differences for instance, Monnier et al. (1998) found that women have a more pro-social coping style, whereas men report using a more anti-social coping style. In addition, problem-focused coping and personal agency have been viewed more favorably than emotional or communal coping, ignoring the potential negative affect individualistic approaches can have on others and the value of seeking out potential support when needed (Dunahoo, et al., 1998). Men and women do not differ on the active-passive dimension of coping when social aspects are considered. 34 Dunahoo and colleages (1998) and Hobfoll; (1991) found that men tend to cope in a more individualistic manner while women tend to seek more social support. Increased rates of depression were found in both men and women with regard to avoidant coping styles. Howerton and Van Gundy (2009) found that men engage in “externalizing” behavior, such as aggression and substance abuse at higher rates than do women. These findings suggest that stereotypically male coping may be more discriminatory or aggressive and Hobfoll’s model of coping proves to be fairer to women by highlighting prosocial coping as an active and effective approach, rather than placing emphasis on individualistic action as the “good” way to cope. In other words, active prosocial coping is psychologically healthier and more effective for both men and women. Hypotheses There were five major predictions for the present research concerning the relationship that coping style and open-mindedness have to heterosexuals’ positive and negative attitudes toward LGB individuals. First it was hypothesized that individuals who express high levels of open-minded thinking would have more positive attitudes toward LGB individuals. Second, it was also predicted that individuals who utilize a more active coping style, such as communal coping or individualistic coping, would hold more positive attitudes toward LGB individuals. Third, it was predicted that avoidant coping would be related to lower LGB-affirmativeness. The fourth hypothesis predicted that individuals who have had prior contact with a lesbian or gay individual would be more accepting of and hold more positive attitudes toward LGB individuals. Fifth, it was 35 predicted that individuals who consider themselves to be more liberal in regard to political affiliation would hold more positive a attitudes toward LGB individuals. 36 Chapter 3 METHOD Participants Participants consisted of 337 (268 female and 69 male) undergraduate college students at a large public university in the Western United States who were recruited from introductory psychology courses. The participants ranged in age from 18 to 50 years, with a mean age of 20.6 (SD = 3.2) years. All participants received partial course credit for their participation in the study. The sample consisted of the following racial/ethnic groups: 41.4% European-American/White, 18.4% Asian-American/Pacific Islander, 17.1% Latino/Hispanic, 14.5% Multi-Ethnic/Other, 8.4% African American/Black. The participants were 41.6% Liberal (n = 122), 36.7% Conservative (n = 110) and 18.2% Moderate (n = 58) and majority of the participants (77.7%) identified as Christian. With regard to contact with lesbian and gay individuals, 91 participants (27%) had non-meaningful contact implying the participant has little to no contact with lesbian or gay individuals and 199 (59%) had meaningful contact referring to having a close friend or family member who is lesbian or gay. Measures Subscale scores were derived by averaging across the responses for each of the scales to account for any item participants may have skipped or left blank and to make total scores interpretable with reference to the response options. 37 Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Knowledge and Attitudes Scale for Heterosexuals (LGB-KASH). The LGB-KASH scale, developed by Worthington, Dillon, and BeckerSchutte (2005), assesses heterosexuals’ attitudes toward and knowledge about lesbians, gays, and bisexuals. This measure was selected because of its multi-dimensionality and ability to capture heterosexual attitudes toward those with a different sexual orientation. The LGB-KASH consists of 28 items and five subscales assessing internalized affirmativeness (e.g., “I would display a symbol of gay pride (pink triangle, rainbow, etc.) to show my support of the LGB community.”), civil rights (e.g., “Hospitals should acknowledge same-sex partners equally to any other next of kin”), knowledge (e.g., “I am knowledgeable about the history and mission of the PFGLAG organization.”), religious conflict (e.g., “I keep my religious beliefs to myself in order to accept LGB people.”), and hate (e.g., “LGB people deserve the hatred they receive.”). Participants responded to the items of the LGB-KASH on a 7-point response scale ranging from 1 (very uncharacteristic of me) to 7 (very characteristic of me). Higher scores indicate the corresponding subscale was more characteristic of the participant and lower scores indicate that the corresponding subscale is less characteristic of the participant. Internal consistency coefficients (Cronbach’s alpha) for the five subscales at the time of its development were .81 (Hate), .81 (Knowledge), .87 (Civil Rights), .76 (Religious Conflict), and .83 (Internalized Affirmativeness). Attitudes Regarding Bisexuality Scale. The Attitudes Regarding Bisexuality Scale (ARBS) was developed by Mohr and Rochlen (1999) to assess two dimensions of 38 attitudes of both women and men. The first dimension tolerance, relates to the degree to which bisexuality is accepted as a morally tolerable sexual orientation (e.g., “I would not be upset if my sister were bisexual.”). The second dimension, stability, involves viewing bisexuality as a legitimate stable sexual orientation (e.g., “Most women who call themselves bisexual are temporarily experimenting with their sexuality.”). Participants responded to the items of the ARBS measure on a 5-point response scale ranging from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 5 (Strongly Agree). Upon the author’s suggestion, the two subscales were combined into a single scale. Higher scores indicated higher agreement with regard to each of the subscales. At the time of development, internal consistency coefficients (Cronbach’s alpha) were .89 for the Stability subscale and .77 for the Tolerance subscale. Strategic Approach to Coping Scale. The Strategic Approach to Coping Scale (SACS-D) was developed by Hobfoll and colleagues (1994) to assess dispositional coping styles using the Multiaxial Model of Coping. The measure consists of 51 items and nine subscales: assertive action (“Don’t give up when things look their worst, because you can often turn things around”), social joining (“I would join together with others to deal with the situation together”), seeking social support (“Check with friends about what they would do”), cautious action (“Move very cautiously, there may be a hidden agenda”), indirect action (“Others need to feel they are the boss, so I would work around them to get things done”), avoidance (“Back off and just let the smoke clear”), antisocial action (“ Counter attack and catch others off guard”), and aggressive action (“I 39 would act fast, it’s better to throw myself into the problem”). Participants responded to the SACS items on a 5-point response scale ranging from 1 (I would not do this at all) to 5 (I would do this a lot) based on how they generally cope in stressful situations. Higher scores on each of the subscales indicated a greater tendency to utilize the corresponding coping strategy. According to Dunahoo et al. (1998) Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for the nine subscales ranged from .61 to .86. Actively Open-Minded Thinking Scale. The Actively Open-Minded Thinking Scale (AOTS) was developed to assess one’s ability to reason independently of prior belief as well as think critically about those who differ in opinion or belief without letting that disagreement affect one’s process. Stanovich and West (2007) developed this 41item scale consisting of 10 items from the flexible thinking scale (Stanovich & West, 1997), 8 items from the Openness-Values facet of the NEO-PI-R (Costa & McCrae, 1992), 3 items from Epstein and Meier’s (1989) categorical thinking subscale, 9 items from the belief identification scale (Sa et al. 1999), 2 items from the counterfactual thinking scale (Stanovich & West, 1997), and 9 items measuring dogmatism (Paulhus & Reid, 1991; Robinson, Shaver, & Wrightsman, 1991; Troldahl & Powell, 1965). The authors reported internal consistency coefficients (Cronbach’s alpha) for the scale ranging from .83-.84. Participants responded to the AOTS items on a 6-point response scale ranging from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 6 (Strongly agree) with higher scores indicating a greater tendency toward open-minded thinking. 40 Demographic Questionnaire. A demographics questionnaire was used in order to obtain information regarding the participant’s age, gender, race/ethnicity, and political orientation. Participants identified their political orientation by circling a number on a seven-point scale from 1 (liberal) to 7 (conservative). Additionally, participants were asked to report their sexual orientation and the amount of meaningful contact with an LGB individual by checking the closest relationship the participant had out of a list of responses. Procedure The participants for this study were recruited from introductory psychology courses at California State University, Sacramento. In exchange for their participation, the participants received one hour of credit toward satisfying the Psychology Department research participation requirement. The scales were collected as part of a larger project that examined religiosity and sexual identity development. Undergraduate students signed up to participate in the study at a designated time and location through the psychology department’s research participation website. Participants were verbally read the consent form and explained the general purpose of the study by a research assistant. Research assistants included both men and women and undergraduate as well as graduate students. They were then asked to sign and return the consent forms and to return completed questionnaires by placing them face down in a box. Questionnaires were completed in small groups of no more than eight students supervised by at least one researcher each session. The researcher informed all participants that their participation 41 was completely voluntary and anonymous. In order to reduce the potential for order effects, each of the questionnaire packets was assembled in counterbalanced order. Data were entered twice to ensure there were no errors made during data entry. 42 Chapter 4 RESULTS Effective Sample Size The initial sample size consisted of 373 participants; however, participants were excluded from the analyses if they identified their sexual orientation as lesbian, gay, or bisexual, or if a participant left a substantial number of majority items unanswered. The final sample size was 337 participants. Descriptive Statistics The means and standard deviations of both the predictor and dependent variables in this sample (See Table 1) were generally consistent with the means and standard deviations reported in other studies (Mohr & Rochlen, 1999; Stanovich & West, 1997; Worthington, Dillon, & Becker-Schutte, 2005). Cronbach’s alpha for the five subscales of the LGB-KASH and ARBS measure are shown in Table 4 and for the predictor variables in Table 5. Principal Component Analysis In keeping with the theoretical model on which it is based and in order to simplify data analyses, the nine subscales of the Strategic Approach to Coping Scale (SACS) were analyzed using a principal component analysis with a promax rotation. Three factors were obtained with eigenvalues greater than 1.0, cumulatively accounting for 66.7% of the variance (See Table 2). 43 Table 1 Means and Standard Deviations for Predictor and Dependent Variables Dependent Variables LGB Hate M 1.52 SD .72 LGB Knowledge 2.39 1.19 LGB Civil Rights 5.66 1.52 LGB Religious Conflict 3.40 1.17 LGB Affirmative 3.51 1.63 Bi-Affirmative 3.56 .77 Open-minded 4.10 .51 Individualistic Coping 2.89 .57 Communal Coping 2.36 .56 Avoidant Coping 3.62 .53 Predictor Variables Table 2 Total Variance Explained Component 1 2 3 Eigenvalues 2.94 1.64 1.43 % of Variance 32.68 18.17 15.85 Cumulative Variance 32.68 50.85 66.70 44 Table 3 presents the promax rotated structure matrix of the nine SACS subscales and factor loadings. The first component was indicated by higher levels of anti-social action, aggressive action, indirect action and instinctive action and was interpreted as individualistic coping because the subscales that comprise it are more self-focused than communal coping. The second component was indicated by higher levels of social joining, social support, and cautious action and interpreted as communal coping. The third component was indicated by higher levels of avoidance and lower levels of assertive action, and was interpreted as avoidant coping. Similar results were found by Dunahoo et al. (1998) when they performed a second order factor analysis of the SACS revealing that the nine subscales could be grouped into three larger subscales and still be statistically and theoretically sound. Canonical Correlation A canonical correlation analysis was used to explore the relationship between a set of attitude variables towards LGB and sets of coping styles and open-mindedness variables. The dependent variables were hate, knowledge, civil rights, religious conflict, internalized affirmativeness, and bi-affirmativeness. The predictor variables were openmindedness, individualistic coping, communal coping, and avoidant coping. With 323 cases in the analysis the relationship between the sets of variables was statistically significant, Wilk’s Lambda = .63, R c ² = .37, Approximate F(24, 1093.14) = 6.42, p < .001. Four canonical roots were extracted, two of which were statistically significant accounting for 84.07% and 9.52% of the explained variance, respectively. 45 Eigenvalues, percentages of variance explained, and the squared canonical correlations for each function are shown in Table 8. The first function accounted for 84.07% of the explained variance and the second function accounted for 9.52% of the explained respectively. Based on the Cramer-Nicewater Index (1979), it appears that approximately 10% of the variance of the dependent variates was explained by the four canonical roots. Table 3 Structure Matrix of the SACS Subscales with Promax Rotation (N = 373) Factor Loading Subscale 1 2 3 Communality Antisocial Action .32 -.002 .69 .83 Aggressive Action .82 .21 -.19 .74 Indirect Action .74 .19 .29 .61 Instinctive Action .64 .18 .16 .42 Social Joining .13 .85 .13 .75 Cautious Action .42 .68 -.001 .52 Social Support .22 .77 -.02 .60 Assertive Action .14 .10 -.90 .85 Avoidance .35 .26 .85 .83 Note. Boldface indicates highest factor loadings. Factor 1 = Individualistic Coping; Factor 2 = Communal Coping; Factor 3 = Avoidant Coping. 46 The structure coefficients for the two functions for the predictor and dependent variates are shown in Table 4. The first predictor function was associated with lower levels of open-mindedness; the first dependent function was associated with higher levels of hateful attitudes toward LGB individuals, somewhat less LGB knowledge, less endorsement of civil rights, more religious conflict, less internalized affirmativeness, and more negative attitudes toward bisexuality. The first function appeared to indicate that heterosexual individuals who are not as open-minded report more negative attitudes toward LGB individuals. The second predictor function was associated with lower levels of individualistic coping; the second dependent function was associated with lower levels of endorsement of civil rights. This second function appeared to indicate that those who did not utilize an individualistic coping style also tended to not endorse LGB civil rights. Bivariate Correlations Higher levels of positive attitudes toward bisexuality were associated with having more knowledge, higher levels of endorsement of LGB civil rights, lower levels of LGB religious conflict, and had higher levels of LGB affirmativeness than those who expressed negative attitudes (Table 5). Higher levels of individualistic coping style were associated with lower levels of open-minded thinking (Table 6). Lastly, higher levels of open-mindedness were associated with less hatefulness toward LGB individuals, greater knowledge regarding LGB individuals, greater endorsement of LGB civil rights, lower 47 levels of religious conflict regarding LGB issues, higher LGB affirmativeness, and more positive attitudes toward bisexuality (Table 7). Table 4 Correlations and Standardized Canonical Coefficients between Attitudes toward LGB Individuals, Coping Styles, and Open-mindedness First Variate Second Variate Dependent Variable Correlation Correlation Attitudes Toward LGB Hate .73 -.06 Knowledge -.27 -.37 Civil Rights -.72 -.67 Religious Conflict .46 -.13 Internalized Affirm -.61 -.22 Positive Bisexual Attitudes -.90 .08 Individual Coping .26 -.94 Avoidant Coping .07 -.36 Prosocial Coping .27 -.35 Open-mindedness -.99 -.01 Predictor Variable Coping Style & OpenMindedness Note. Boldface indicates highest variate loadings. 48 Table 5 Bivariate Correlations of Dependent Variables of LGB Attitudes 1 2 3 4 5 1. LGB Hate - 2. LGB Knowledge -.18** - 3. LGB Civil Rights -.49** .36** - 4. LGB Religious Conflict .16** -.26** -.15** - 5. LGB Affirmative .44** .49** .64** -.29** - 6. Bisexual Attitudes -.49** .36** .61** -.35** .58** 6 Cronbach’s Alpha .68 .80 .88 .67 .81 - .92 Note. **p < .01. Table 6 Bivariate Correlations of Predictor Variables 1 2 3 1. Openmindedness - 2. Individual -.25** - 3. Avoidant -.03 .31** - .20** .12* 4. Communal -.25** Note. ** p < .01, * p < .05. 4 Cronbach’s Alpha .86 .85 .68 - .86 49 Table 7 Bivariate Correlations Between Predictors and Dependent Variables Dependent Variables Predictors 1 2 3 4 5 Individual Coping .13* .04 .03 .08 -.04 -.15** Communal Coping .15** -.09 -.04 .13* -.10 -.13* Avoidant Coping -.03 .02 .02 .05 -.03 -.07 .15** .41** -.26** .35** .50** Openminded -.41** Note. ** p < .01. * p <.05 6 Table 8 Eigenvalues, Cumulative % of Explained Variance, & Squared Canonical Correlates for Each Canonical Function Overall Variance Squared Canonical Function Eigenvalue Explained Correlation 1*** .46 84.07 .31 2* .05 9.52 .05 3 .03 4.86 .03 4 .01 1.55 .01 *** p < .001, * p < .05 Group Differences A two-way between subjects multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted to determine whether heterosexual individuals’ political affiliations (liberal, moderate, and conservative) and having contact with LGB persons (meaningful or nonmeaningful) were related to their attitudes toward bisexuals, LGB hate, LGB knowledge, 50 LGB civil rights, religious conflict, and LGB internalized affirmativeness. The multivariate interaction of political affiliation and contact was significant, Wilks’ Lambda = .937, F(12, 674) = 1.86, p = .036, using Wilks’ criterion to determine significance. Only the interaction for LGB knowledge was statistically significant, F(2, 342) = 3.43, p = .033, η2 = .149. The interaction for LGB knowledge is shown in Figure 1. Tests of simple effects, with a Bonferroni alpha level correction, revealed that liberals who had meaningful contact with LGB individuals had more knowledge than those with non-meaningful LGB contact. No differences were found between the groups for non-meaningful LGB contact. For meaningful contact, liberals were more knowledgeable than either conservatives or moderates; conservatives and moderates did not differ. 51 Figure 1. Interaction between Political affiliation and LGB Contact 52 Chapter 5 DISCUSSION The present study examined the relationship of open-mindedness and coping styles, as well as degree of social contact with lesbian and gay individuals and political orientation, to a range of variables representing heterosexuals’ knowledge of and attitudes toward lesbians, gays, and bisexuals. In summary, open-mindedness, individualistic coping, and, among those endorsing a liberal political orientation, greater contact with lesbian and gay individuals were linked to LGB-affirmative responses on study measures. Hypothesis one, that heterosexual individuals higher in open-mindedness would hold more positive attitudes toward LGB individuals, was supported. This finding was consistent with Keiller’s (2010) findings that those low in abstract reasoning, a cognitive structure similar to open-mindedness, were higher in anti-gay prejudice. Keiller (2010) hypothesized that low abstract reasoning skills would account for variance not accounted for by previously found predictors, specifically gender, right-wing authoritarianism, and prior LGB contact. This finding is also consistent with research on the personality variable openness to experience and specifically, openness to values. The second hypothesis contended that utilizing higher levels of active coping, including both individualistic and communal coping, would be linked to holding more positive attitudes toward LGB individuals. The present study did not find a link between active, communal coping and attitudes toward LGB individuals; however, higher levels 53 of individualistic coping were related to stronger endorsement of LGB civil rights. Research has demonstrated that those seeking advice or support from their social network in times of stress may be more easily influenced by their attitudes about whatever the issue is (Gino, Brooks, & Schweitzer, 2012). Gino, Brooks, and Schweitzer (2012) found that individuals experiencing anxiety or uncertainty about an issue would seek out social support and information when trying to figure out their own feelings about it. However, communal coping was unrelated to attitudes toward LGB individuals in the present study. Results did not support hypothesis three, that higher avoidant coping would be linked to lower LGB-affirmativeness. This is inconsistent with previous research in which avoidant coping styles (Cullen et al., 2002) have been found to be correlated with negative attitudes toward LGB people. Other research that has explored the relationship between coping style and homophobic attitudes has found that avoidant defenses are used when the individual feels threatened or uncertain about their own sexuality, thus creating anxiety (Johnson, Brems, & Alford-Keating, 1997). Hypothesis four stated that heterosexual individuals who have had prior contact with an LGB person will be more open-minded and hold more positive attitudes toward LGB individuals. Research has repeatedly demonstrated that those who have had contact with LGB individuals are more likely to hold positive attitudes toward these populations (Satcher & Schumacker, 2009). Having one friend or acquaintance that is gay or lesbian is associated with lower homonegativity and more internal affirmativeness (Satcher & Schumacker, 2009). Further, it is suggested that those who have had this contact with the 54 LGB community would generally be more open-minded, perhaps because whatever reservations, beliefs, or fears one has would be disconfirmed by interacting with LGB individuals. The fifth, and final, hypothesis predicted that heterosexual individuals who consider themselves to be more liberal in political orientation would be more likely to hold positive attitudes toward LGB individuals. Research has consistently shown that those who considered themselves liberal hold more positive attitudes toward LGB individuals and have more knowledge about LGB communities (Lambert, Ventura, Hall, & Cluse-Tolar, 2006; Mohr & Rochlen, 1999). The findings of the present study are generally consistent with previous research, but suggest that contact with lesbian and gay individuals may be linked to greater LGB knowledge only among individuals who consider themselves to be politically liberal, and not among those who consider themselves moderate or conservative. This study adds to the current literature on attitudes toward sexual minorities by including attitudes toward lesbians and bisexuals in addition to gay men, and by including measures of positive as well as negative attitudes toward these groups. Openmindedness was found to be linked to LGB-affirmative attitudes and knowledge, suggesting that there may be a benefit to teaching open-minded thinking skills in efforts aimed at improving acceptance of LGB people. Baron (1993) stresses and discusses how open-minded thinking is a teachable phenomenon and encourages people to practice this way of thinking as it is seen as virtuous within positive psychology. Coping was not found to be linked to LGB attitudes, with the exception that active, 55 individualistic coping was related to endorsement of LGB civil rights. Although past theoretical and empirical work on individualistic coping suggests that it may be linked to lower levels of relational well-being (Monnier et al., 1998), it may signal respect for autonomy and self-determination that manifests in support for LGB civil rights protections. Future research should continue to explore personality variables and beliefs about personal freedoms and privacy in an effort to understand more fully the links among heterosexuals’ cognitive, personality, and ideological characteristics in relation to attitudes toward LGB individuals. One of the variables most consistently related to attitudes toward LGB individuals is contact with lesbian and gay people, suggesting that greater visibility of LGB people and greater integration between heterosexual and LGB communities will result in greater acceptance. Interestingly, in this study, contact with lesbian and gay individuals was only related to greater knowledge about LGB cultural history, and then only for those who identify as liberal. This finding should be replicated with a larger and more diverse population, extending beyond college students, in order to better understand how to promote LGB affirmativeness through opportunities to interact across sexual orientation lines. This study was limited to heterosexual college students, who are mostly Christian. The sample was racially and ethnically diverse, with under half of the participants identifying as European American/White. However, the sample was primarily female, and racial/ethnic subsamples were not large enough to allow for analysis by cultural 56 group; questions of gender and cultural variability in attitudes might be addressed in a larger study. Research has shown that the younger generations are becoming increasingly more accepting of LGB individuals; results might differ among an older adult population, or a more religiously diverse group. In addition, because this research was voluntary, it may not be as representative of the population. Overall, this research demonstrates that for the heterosexual college student population, open-mindedness is an important predictor of positive attitudes toward LGB individuals. Efforts to develop open-mindedness and to promote respect for individual autonomy may influence future generations to be able to achieve equal rights for all. Future research could explore other cognitive and personality characteristics that play a role in the development of and our attitudes toward the LGB population. Herek et al. (2009) have drawn parallels between negative attitudes and sexual prejudice that affects both heterosexuals and lesbian and gay individuals, specifically in the instance of hate crimes or concealing one’s sexual orientation from members of society. These findings are especially relevant for mental health professionals working with LGB individuals who will become increasingly visible and integrated within society. The present study focused on attitudes toward LGB populations because of the ongoing political issues related to LGB civil rights; however, future research should also address attitudes toward transgendered individuals as their visibility is also increasing. 57 APPENDIX A Consent to Participate as a Research Subject I hereby agree to participate in a research project, entitled A Study of Sex and Sexuality, which will be conducted by Rebecca Cameron, Ph.D., Associate Professor in the Psychology Department of California State University, Sacramento, and which will involve the following procedures: Completing a questionnaire assessing demographics, sexual orientation, attitudes, and identity, attitudes toward sexual minority individuals, religious attitudes, mood, coping, and personality variables. The research will take place in Amador Hall on the CSUS campus and will require approximately 1 hour of my time. The purpose of this research project is to better understand the connections among personality variables and attitudes about sex, sexuality, and religion. I understand that the research procedures and the nature of the questionnaire topics could involve a risk of psychological discomfort for some individuals. Although the researcher may need to avoid a complete description of the procedures at this time, I am entitled to a full explanation after the research. If I experience any psychological discomfort during the research, I may stop my participation. If I want help at that time or after completing the research, I may contact the Student Health Center’s Counseling and Psychological Services at (916) 278-6416 for assistance. Other than tracking my participation in the study to provide me with research participation credit and to avoid duplicate participation, my participation in the study is confidential. In addition, my questionnaire is anonymous and will not be linked to my name. Reports of results will be in the form of group data, and will not include identifying information. I understand that this research may have the following benefits: benefit to the community/society in the form of an increased understanding of attitudes about sex and sexuality. This information was explained to me by _________________________________. I understand that he/she will answer any questions I may have now or later about this research (contact Dr. Cameron and/or her research assistants at (916) 278-6892). I understand that my participation in this research is entirely voluntary. I may decline to participate now, or I may discontinue my participation at any time in the future without penalty other than loss of research credit. I understand that the investigator may terminate 58 my participation at any time. I understand that I will not receive any compensation other than research credit for participating in this study. Signature: _______________________________Date: __________________________ Print name_______________________________________ 59 APPENDIX B Debriefing Please do not discuss the purpose of this study with your peers who may participate in the project in the future. Numerous studies have linked personality factors (e.g., open-mindedness), religious variables, and sexual attitudes to attitudes toward sexual minority individuals. However, societal attitudes have been changing rapidly and sexual minority issues have been a focus of a great deal of political and media attention. The goal of this study is to establish current attitudes about sexual minority individuals among Sacramento State students, and to examine whether relationships of other variables to these attitudes previously found in the literature apply to Sac State students. This study expands on previous literature by including attitudes toward bisexuality and attitudes toward transgender individuals. I understand that Dr. Cameron will answer any questions I may have now or later about this research (contact Dr. Cameron and/or her research assistants at (916) 278-6892). If I want help at that time or after completing the research, I may contact the Student Health Center’s Counseling and Psychological Services at (916) 278-6416 for assistance. 60 APPENDIX C Demographics Please respond to the following questions as accurately as possible. If it would be helpful to clarify a particular answer, feel free to make a note in the margin. Please write legibly. 1. Age_______ 2. Gender: (1) Male_____ (2) Female_____ 3. Year in school (check one) (1) ___ freshman (2) ___ sophomore (3) ___ junior (4) ___ senior (5) ___ unclassified (3) Other gender ________ 4. Major________________________________ 5. Ethnicity (please check one) (1) _____ Asian American/Pacific Islander (2) _____African American/Black (3) _____American Indian/Native American (4) _____European American/Caucasian/White (5) _____Latino/Hispanic American (6) _____Foreign national (please list country of origin)_______________________ (7) _____Multiethnic (please list ethnic groups) _____________________________ (8) _____Other ethnicity (please describe)__________________________________ 6. Country of Birth ______________________ 7. Country of Citizenship________________________ 8. The following generation best applies to me. Check only one. (1) _____1st generation = I was born in a country other than the USA. (2) _____2nd generation = I was born in USA; either parent born in a country other than USA. (3) _____3rd generation = I was born in USA; both parents born in USA and all grand parents born in a country other than USA. (4) _____4th generation = I and my parents born in USA and a t least one grandparent born in a country other than USA with remainder born in USA. (5) _____5th generation = I and my parents born in the USA and all grandparents born in the USA. (6) _____I do not know what generation I am. 61 7. Parental education a. Mother: highest schooling completed _________________ b. Father: highest schooling completed _________________ c. Other parental figure: highest schooling completed _____________ (indicate relationship________________________) 8. Sexual orientation a. ____ Heterosexual b. ____ Gay or lesbian c. ____ Bisexual d. ____Other sexual orientation 9. (1) (2) (3) (4) Current employment status (please check one) _____Not employed _____Employed 20 hours per week or fewer _____Employed 21-39 hours per week _____Employed 40 or more hours per week 10. Country of Citizenship Housing (please check one) (1) _____Dorm (2) _____Off-campus 11. Relationship status (please check one) a. _____Single b. _____Single, in a long-term committed relationship c. _____Married, living together d. _____Separated e. _____Divorced f. _____Other (please describe) __________________ 12. a Number of religious services you attend: 0 _ none, 1 _ one a year, 2 _ a few times year, 3 _ one a month, 4 _ one a week, and 5 _ more than one a week. 13. How much meaningful contact have you had with gay men or lesbians: 1 _ “I have never had any contact with gay men or lesbians,” 2 _ “I have been in the same room with someone I knew was openly gay or lesbian, but otherwise have had no contact with gay people,” 62 3 _ “I have met a gay or lesbian person, but we did not really have a meaningful conversation,” 4 _ “I have an acquaintance who disclosed to me that he or she is gay or lesbian, but we’re not really close,” 5 _ “I have a relative who is gay or lesbian, but I hardly ever see him/her,” 6 _ “I have a friend who is gay or lesbian; we talk or see each other every once in a while,” 7 _ “I have a close friend or immediate family member who is gay or lesbian; we spend a good deal of time together.” 14. Please rate your political orientation on the following scale: Liberal 1 Moderate 2 3 Conservative 4 5 6 7 63 References Aldwin, C. M. (2000). Stress, coping, and development: An integrative perspective. New York, NY: The Guilford Press. Alexander, A. L. (2008). Relationship resources for coping with unfulfilled standards in dating relationships: Commitment, satisfaction, and closeness. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 25, 725-747. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Baehr, J. (2011). The structure of open-mindedness. Canadian Journal of Philosophy, 41, 191-213. Barber, M. (2003). Lesbian and gay issues are mental health issues. Behavioral Health Management, 23(6), 8-9. Bardi, A., & Guerra, V. (2011). Cultural values predict coping using culture as an individual difference variable in multicultural samples. Journal of CrossCultural Psychology, 42, 908-927. Baron, J. M. (1993). Why teach thinking? An essay. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 42(3), 191-237. 64 Baron, J. M., Struckman-Johnson, C., Quevillon, R., & Banka, S. R. (2008). Heterosexual men’s attitudes toward gay men: A hierarchical model including masculinity, openness, and theoretical explanations. Psychology of Men and Masculinity, 9, 154-166. Bullough, V. L., & Bullough, B. (1977). Sin, sickness, & sanity: A history of sexual attitudes. New York: Garland Publishing, Inc. Cardoso, F. (2010). Political and sexual attitudes concerning same-sex sexual behavior. Sexuality and Culture, 14, 306-326. Cosme, D., Pepino, C., & Brown, B. (2010). Empathy, open-mindedness, and political ideology. A Journal of Undergraduate Work, 1(3), 167-175. Cullen, J. M., Wright, L.W., & Alessandri, M. (2002). The personality variable openness to experience as it relates to homophobia. Journal of Homosexuality, 42(4), 119-134. Dunahoo, C. L., Hobfoll, S. E., Monnier, J., Hulsizer, M. R., & Johnson, R., (1998). There’s more than rugged individualism in coping. Part 1: Even the Lone Ranger had Tonto. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping: An International Journal, 11(2), 137-165. Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Coping and emotion. New York, NY, US: Columbia University Press. Fingerhut, A. W. (2011). Straight allies: What predicts heterosexuals’ alliance with the LGBT community. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 41, 2230-2248. 65 Haider-Markel, D., & Joslyn, M. (2008). Beliefs about the origins of homosexuality and support for gay rights: An empirical test of attribution theory. Public Opinion Quarterly, 72, 291-310. Hartmann, D., Zhang, X, & Wischstadt, W. (2005). One (multicultural) nation under God? Changing uses and meanings of the term ‘Judeo-Christian’ in American Media. Journal of Media and Religion, 4, 207-234. Herek, G. M. (1984). Beyond ‘homophobia’: A social psychological perspective of attitudes towards lesbians and gay men. Journal of Homosexuality, 10(1-2), 1-21. Herek, G. M. (1990). The context of anti-gay violence: Notes on cultural and psychological heterosexism. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 5, 316-333. Herek, G.M. (2009). Sexual prejudice. In T. D. Nelson (Ed.), Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination (pp. 441-467). New York, NY: Psychology Press. Herek, G. M., Gillis, J. R., & Cogan, J. C. (2009). Internalized stigma among sexual minority adults: Insights from a social psychological perspective. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 56, 32-43. Hobfoll, S. E., Dunahoo, C. L., Ben-Porath, Y., & Monnier, J. (1994). Gender and coping: The dual-axis model of coping. American Journal of Community Psychology, 22, 49-82. 66 Hobfoll, S. E., Cameron, R.P., Chapman, H. A., & Gallagher, R. W. (1996). Social support and social coping in couples. In G. R. Pierce, B. R. Sarason, & I. G. Sarason (Eds.), Handbook of social support and the family (pp. 413-433). New York: Plenum Press. Hong, R. Y., Paunonen, S. V., & Slade, H. P. (2008). Big five personality factors and the prediction of behavior: A multitrait-multimethod approach. Personality and Individual Differences, 45(2), 160-166. Howerton, A., & Van Gundy, K. (2009). Sex differences in coping style and implications for depressed mood. International Journal of Stress Management, 16, 333-350. Johnson, M. E., Brems, C., & Alford-Keating, P. (1997). Personality correlates of homophobia. Journal of Homosexuality, 34(1), 57-69. Keiller, S. W. (2010). Abstract reasoning as a predictor of attitudes toward gay men. Journal of Homosexuality, 57, 914-927. Killen, M. (1997). Culture, self, and development: Are cultural stereotypes useful or stereotypic? Developmental Review, 17, 239-249. Kite, M. E., & Whitley, B. E. (1996). Sex differences in attitudes toward homosexual persons, behaviors, and civil rights: A meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(4), 336-353. LaMar, L. A., & Kite, M. E. (1998). Sex differences in attitudes toward gay men and lesbians: A multi-dimensional perspective. The Journal of Sex Research, 35, 189-196. 67 Lambert, E. G., Ventura, L. A., Hall, D. E., & Cluse-Tolar, T. (2006). College students’ view on gay and lesbian issues: Does education make a difference? Journal of Homosexuality, 50(4), 1-30. McCrae, R. R., & Sutin, A. (2009). Openness to experience. In M. Leary, & R. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior (pp. 257-273). New York: Guilford Press. Mireshghi, S., & Matsumoto, D. (2008). Perceived cultural attitudes toward homosexuality and their effects on Iranian and American sexual minorities. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 14, 372-376. Monnier, J., Hobfoll, S. E., Dunahoo, C. L., Hulsizer, M. R., & Johnson, R., (1998). There’s more than rugged individualism in coping. Part 2: Construct validity and further model testing. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping: An International Journal, 11, 247-272. Monnier, J., Stone, B., Hobfoll, S., & Johnson, R. (1998). How anti-social and pro-social coping influence the support process among men and women in the U.S. Postal service. Sex Roles, 39(1/2), 1-20. New York Times. (2012). Same sex marriage, civil unions, & domestic partnerships. Retrieved on: May 31, 2012, from http://www.nytimes.com Pardie, L., & Luchetta, T. (1999). The construction of attitudes toward lesbians and gay men. New York: Haworth Press. 68 Proctor, S. L., & McCord, D. M. (2009). Correlates of the openness to experience domain. Individual Differences Research, 7, 222-227. Rokeach, M. (1960). The open and closed mind. New York: Basic Books, Inc. Sa, W., Kelley, C., Ho, C., & Stanovich, K. E. (2005). Thinking about personal theories: individual differences in the coordination of theory and evidence. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1149-1161. Satcher, J., & Schumacker, R. (2009). Predictors of modern homonegativity among professional counselors. Journal of LGBT Issues in Counseling, 3(1), 21-36. Smollan, R. K., Sayers, J., & Matheny, J. (2010). Emotional responses to speed, frequency, and timing of organization change. Time and Society, 19(1), 28-53. Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1997). Reasoning independently of prior belief and individual differences in actively open-minded thinking. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 342-357. Stotzer, R. L. (2008). Straight allies: Supportive attitudes toward lesbians, gay men, and bisexuals in a college sample. Sex Roles, 60, 67-80. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063-1070. Whitley, B. E. (2009). Religiosity and attitudes toward lesbians and gay men: A metaanalysis. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 19, 21-38. 69 Worthington, R. L, Dillon, F. R., & Becker-Schutte, A. M. (2005). Development, reliability, and validity of the lesbian, gay, and bisexual knowledge and attitudes scale for heterosexuals (LGB-KASH). Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52(1), 104-118. Wright, L. (1999, July 1). Stonewall Riots- A Turning Point for Gay and Lesbian Liberation. Socialist Alternative. Retrieved: December 28, 2011 from http://socialistalternative.org/literature/stonewall.html Yang, A. (2000). From wrong to rights: Public opinions on gay and lesbian Americans’ move toward equality. Washington, DC: National Gay and Lesbian Task Force Institute.