CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS IN THE U.S. A Thesis Presented to the faculty of the Department of Psychology California State University, Sacramento Submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS in Psychology (Counseling) by Amandeep Gill FALL 2012 CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS IN THE U.S. A Thesis by Amandeep Gill Approved by: __________________________________, Committee Chair Phillip D. Akutsu, Ph.D. __________________________________, Second Reader Marya Endriga, Ph.D. __________________________________, Third Reader Kelly Cotter, Ph.D. ____________________________ Date ii Student: Amandeep Gill I certify that this student has met the requirements for format contained in the University format manual, and that this thesis is suitable for shelving in the Library and credit is to be awarded for the thesis. __________________________, Graduate Coordinator Lisa Harrison, Ph.D. Department of Psychology iii ___________________ Date Abstract of CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS IN THE U.S. by Amandeep Gill The current study examined the ethnic differences in acculturative stress between nonEuropean and European international students. In addition, the relationships between perceived discrimination (race/ethnicity-based and foreigner-based), social support from five sources (family in home country, co-nationals, other international students, American friends, and online ethnic support groups), and acculturative stress were examined. Data were collected from international students at California State University, Sacramento. Results revealed that race/ethnicity-based discrimination, foreigner-based discrimination, social support from family in home country, and social support from American friends were significant predictors of acculturative stress in international students. _______________________, Committee Chair Phillip D. Akutsu, Ph.D. _______________________ Date iv ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Foremost, I would like to express my deepest appreciation towards my thesis committee chair and advisor, Dr. Phillip Akutsu, for his continued support and advice during this project. His valuable support and guidance have greatly assisted the completion of this thesis. I would also like to extend my sincere appreciation towards Dr. Marya Endriga and Dr. Kelly Cotter for joining my thesis committee and providing helpful feedback during this project. Also, I would like to thank Eric Merchant, Associate Director, Office of Global Education, CSU Sacramento, for his valuable assistance not only during this project, but throughout my academic career at CSU Sacramento. In addition, I would like to thank my husband, parents, and parents-in-law for their tremendous love, support, and encouragement throughout this educational process. Finally, I would like to thank all the international students who took part in this study. v TABLE OF CONTENTS Page Acknowledgements ............................................................................................................. v List of Tables ................................................................................................................... viii Chapter 1. INTRODUCTION ........................................................................................................ 1 Adjustment Difficulties of International Students in the U.S. ................................. 2 Literature Review ..................................................................................................... 6 Negative Outcomes of Acculturative Stress ........................................................... 32 Purpose of the Study............................................................................................... 37 2. METHOD ................................................................................................................... 42 Participants ............................................................................................................. 42 Measures ................................................................................................................. 43 Procedure ................................................................................................................ 50 3. RESULTS ................................................................................................................... 53 Preliminary Findings .............................................................................................. 53 4. DISCUSSION ........................................................................................................... 110 Summary and Interpretations of Findings ............................................................ 110 Limitations of the Present Study .......................................................................... 119 vi Implications for Researchers ................................................................................ 121 Future Directions in Research .............................................................................. 125 References ...................................................................................................................... 127 vii LIST OF TABLES Tables 1. Page Summary of Means and Standard Deviations for Demographic Variables…………………………………………………………. 55 2. Means and Standard Deviations of Perceived Discrimination, Social Support, and Acculturative Stress………………………... 3. 56 Estimated Marginal Means of Non-European and European International Students for Perceived Discrimination, Social Support, and Acculturative Stress……………………………… 4. 58 Multiple Hierarchical Regression Results with Ethnicity and Length of U.S. Residency Predicting Race/Ethnicity-Based and Foreigner-Based Discrimination………………………………… 5. 60 Multiple Hierarchical Regression Results with Race/EthnicityBased and Foreigner-Based Discrimination Predicting Acculturative Stress……………………………………………… 67 6. Multiple Hierarchical Regression Results with Social Support from Family in Home Country and American Friends Predicting Acculturative Stress……………………………………………… 69 viii 7. Multiple Hierarchical Regression Results with Interaction between Social Support from Family in Home Country and Perceived Discrimination (Race/Ethnicity-Based and ForeignerBased) Predicting Acculturative Stress………………………… 8. 90 Multiple Hierarchical Regression Results with Interaction between Social Support from Co-nationals and Perceived Discrimination (Race/Ethnicity-Based and Foreigner-Based) Predicting Acculturative Stress………………………………….. 9. 94 Multiple Hierarchical Regression Results with Interaction between Social Support from Other International Students and Perceived Discrimination (Race/Ethnicity-Based and ForeignerBased) Predicting Acculturative Stress………………………….. 10. 98 Multiple Hierarchical Regression Results with Interaction between Social Support from American Friends and Perceived Discrimination (Race/Ethnicity-Based and Foreigner-Based) Predicting Acculturative Stress………………………………… 11. 102 Multiple Hierarchical Regression Results with Interaction between Social Support from Online Ethnic Support Groups and Perceived Discrimination (Race/Ethnicity-Based and ForeignerBased) Predicting Acculturative Stress………………………….. ix 106 1 Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION The United States has been the number one destination of choice for higher education for large numbers of students from foreign countries across the world. According to the Institute of International Education (IIE, 2011), the number of international students enrolled in U.S. higher education institutions was estimated to be 540,000 in the 2000-2001 academic year. This number has increased significantly by 32% over the past decade and there were about 723,277 international students enrolled at U.S. higher education institutions in the 2010-2011 academic year (IIE, 2011). Notably, international students from Asia made up nearly 60% of the total number of foreign student enrollment in the U.S. with Mainland China supplying the largest number of international students (157,558), followed by India (103,895), South Korea (73,351), Taiwan (24,818), and Japan (21,290). The highest numbers of international students from non-Asian countries in the U.S. were from Canada, Mexico, Turkey, Germany, and the United Kingdom (IIE, 2011). The majority of international students at U.S. colleges and universities are majoring in a few select fields of study: Business and Management (21.5%), Engineering (18.7%), Physical and Life Sciences (8.8%), and Mathematics and Computer Sciences (8.9%) (IIE, 2011). Considering these statistics from the IIE (2011), international students not only bring diversity and internationalization to U.S. college campuses and surrounding communities, but they also make contributions to the advancement of research in the U.S. universities and colleges. 2 Adjustment Difficulties of International Students in the U.S. Beyond the many rewards of pursuing a higher education in the U.S., there are some major challenges that international students must face to achieve their academic goals of completing a U.S. college education. Even though international and American students are confronted with similar challenges in having to adapt to a new college campus and lifestyle, international students must deal with a unique set of challenges in adjusting to a new culture. For example, Rajapaksa and Dundes (2002) found that international students were less content, lonelier, and more homesick than their American classmates, suggesting there may be qualitative differences in the demands of being an international student compared to a national student. Unlike their American counterparts, international students must adjust to cultural issues and additional cultural stressors such as learning to communicate in a foreign language, becoming accustomed to a new education system, being away from their families and social support networks in their native homelands, and facing experiences of racism and discrimination in the U.S. (Poyrazli, Kavanaugh, Baker, & Al-Tamimi, 2004; Poyrazli & Lopez, 2007; Yeh & Inose, 2003). Due to these cultural adjustment issues, Leong and Chou (1996) noted that an estimated 15% to 20% of international college students may be at risk for developing psychological problems. While previous studies report mastering the English language as the most significant stressor for international students in the U.S. (Mori, 2000; Yeh & Inose, 2003), other factors have also been studied to understand this often difficult acculturation process of international students. These cultural adjustment factors include the loss of 3 social support from friends and family in their native homelands and the difficulty of developing new social support networks in the U.S. (Duru & Poyrazli, 2007; Hayes & Lin, 1994; Lee, Koeske, & Sales, 2004; Misra, Crist, & Burant, 2003; Poyrazli et al., 2004; Rajajapaksa & Dundes, 2002; Sandhu, 1994; Sumer, Poyrazli, & Grahame, 2007; Yeh & Inose, 2003). In addition, international students are often confronted with experiences of prejudice and discrimination based on their race, ethnicity, and foreignstatus in the U.S. (Constantine, Anderson, Berkel, Caldwell, & Utsey, 2005; Lee & Rice, 2007; Poyrazli & Grahame, 2007; Poyrazli & Lopez, 2007; Yeh & Inose, 2003; Ying, Lee, & Tsai, 2003). Other factors that have been linked to acculturation difficulties of international students in the U.S. were length of residency in the U.S. and the ethnicity of the international students (Constantine, Okazaki, & Utsey, 2004; Poyrazli et al., 2004; Yeh & Inose, 2003). The difficulties and challenges that arise out of the immediate experience of encountering a range of cultural differences between native homelands and the United States can lead to acculturative stress for international students. Acculturative stress refers to a particular type of stress that is associated with difficulties experienced from cross-cultural encounters, which can manifest in physical, psychological, and social aspects (Berry, Kim, Minde, & Mok, 1987). The key point of this definition is that moving to and living in a new cultural environment can produce physical, social, and psychological discomfort in the acculturating individuals. Berry et al. (1987) stated that a particular set of stress behaviors and consequences such as a decrease in mental health status (specifically confusion, depression, and anxiety), increased feelings of marginality 4 and alienation, and heightened levels of psychosomatic symptoms may occur for individuals in response to this acculturation process. Although these symptoms of distress are similar to other stress responses, acculturative stress has been identified as specifically resulting from cross-cultural transitions (Berry et al., 1987). For example, in Rajapaksa and Dundes’s (2002) study of international and American students who were living away from home at U.S. college campuses, the authors found that international students consistently reported greater adjustment problems than American students. Specifically, more international students reported feeling lonely (29%) and homesick (30%) than American students (14%, 9% respectively). In another study, Cemalicilar and Falbo (2008) examined the psychological well-being of international students before and after their move to the U.S. to pursue a college education. These authors reported a significant decline in the psychological well-being of the international students within the first three months of their move to the U.S. Summary Research shows that international students encounter unique stressors related to their transition from their home countries to the U.S. These unique stressors, termed as acculturative stress, can have a detrimental effect on the psychological well-being of these international students. With the projected increases in the international student population in the U.S. for the future (IIE, 2011), it is important to develop a better understanding of contemporary stressors and buffers that may be significantly associated with the acculturation experiences of international students. 5 In the following sections, I will summarize and review the psychology literature on the major concepts related to acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. The literature review is organized as follows: a) An overview of acculturation and acculturative stress as it is related to the experiences of international students in the U.S. Specifically, I will focus on studies on acculturative stress and provide a critical analysis of acculturative stress measures that were used in past research. b) I will critically analyze the role of perceived discrimination in acculturative stress for international students in the U.S. Specifically, I will examine different forms of discrimination (e.g. race/ethnic-based, foreigner-based) as perceived by international students in the U.S. In addition, I will explore the contribution of demographic factors such as ethnicity and length of residency in the U.S. to reported levels of perceived discrimination for international students. c) I will critically examine the role of social support in the acculturation process of international students. Specifically, I will focus on different sources of social support networks and their relationship to acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. Moreover, I will focus on the direct and moderating effects of social support on acculturative stress. Besides, I will examine the role of demographic factors such as ethnicity and length of residency in the U.S. in relation to perceived social support. d) Finally, I will explore the negative impact of acculturative stress on the psychological well-being of international students in the U.S. Specifically, I will 6 focus on whether acculturative stress has serious consequences on mental well being of international students. Literature Review International students come to the United States in hopes of receiving a better education and expanding their knowledge, which will allow them more opportunities in their future careers (Wadsworth, Hecht, & Jung, 2008). However, with these opportunities come the challenges of adapting to a new culture and educational system. Empirical studies on international graduate students in the United States have indicated that adjusting to another culture can have a negative impact on the psychological and emotional well-being of these students (Fritz, Chin, & DeMarinis, 2008; Hyun, Quinn, Madon, & Lustig, 2007; Lee et al., 2004; Mori, 2000; Sumer et al., 2008; Ying, 2005). The following discussion will provide a comprehensive summary of various empirical studies related to acculturative stress reported by international students in the U.S. Although the majority of this literature review will focus on the experiences of international students in the U.S., some of the studies of international student groups in colleges abroad will be examined to provide additional information that is relevant to this discussion. Acculturation and Acculturative Stress The process of acculturation takes place when a person from one society or culture travels to another country for a prolonged period of time for work, adventure, study, immigration, or refuge (Ward, Bochner, & Furnham, 2001). Redfield, Linton, and Herskovits (1936) proposed the term “acculturation” to explain the process of how 7 individuals from different cultures experience their lives in their new cultural environment. These authors described that “acculturation comprehends those phenomena which result when groups of individuals having different cultures come into continuous first-hand contact, with subsequent changes in the original cultural patterns of either or both groups” (p. 149). Such changes may be observed in a number of different domains such as attitudes, behaviors, values, and a sense of cultural identity (Ryder, Alden, & Paulhus, 2000). This definition of acculturation emphasizes the changes that occur in both groups that come into first-hand contact with each other. However, cross-cultural literature on international students shows that it is the international students, not the members of the host country, that undergo major changes related to cross-cultural transitions to the U.S. For instance, in a review of over 30 years of research, Church (1982) reported that international students confronted “language difficulties, financial problems, adjusting to a new educational system, homesickness, adjusting to social customs and norms, and for some students, racial discrimination” (p. 544). To better understand the acculturation issues of international students, individual aspects of this dynamic process should be emphasized. Graves in1967 was the first to consider the individual in the acculturation process which he referred to as “psychological acculturation” (as cited in Miller, 2007, p. 118). Psychological acculturation was described as the process by which an individual undergoes cultural change as a result of continuous exposure to a second culture (Miller, 2007). A strongly associated aspect of the process of adjusting to a new culture is a consideration of the stressors that accompany these changes. These stressors, termed as “acculturative 8 stressors,” may be viewed as stressful life events that arise due to significant physical, cultural, social, and political differences between the sending and receiving nations (Ying, 2005). Ying (2005) defined acculturative stressors as “stimuli or sources of acculturative stress the migrant encounters during the acculturation process” (p. 60). The research on international students’ acculturation process implies that they encounter a number of acculturative stressors which induce acculturative stress in these students. Berry (1997) stated the level of acculturative stress that may be experienced in a new culture and environment is assumed to be influenced by a number of factors that function on both personal and societal levels. For example, Sandhu and Asrabadi (1994) identified six stressors that could significantly contribute to acculturative stress among international students. These stressors were: 1) perceived discrimination, 2) homesickness, 3) perceived hate, 4) fear, 5) culture shock, and 6) guilt. Mori (2000) also identified language barriers, financial difficulties, cultural gap, and racial/ethnic prejudice and discrimination as significant stressors related to acculturative stress for international students. Similarly, Chen (1999) identified common stressors such as language anxiety, cultural shock, social alienation, and racial discrimination as being linked to the acculturation process of international students. In sum, there are quite a number of stressors that are suggested to be related to acculturation difficulties in international students. Before a discussion on different acculturative stressors can be initiated, it is important to understand whether the association between acculturative stressors and acculturative stress has been validated in the past studies. Empirically, many studies 9 have tried to identify possible acculturative stressors that might be related to adjustment difficulties of international students in the U.S. (Cemalcilar & Falbo, 2008; Chavajay & Skowronek, 2008; Chen, 1999; Constantine et al., 2004; Duru & Poyrazli, 2007; Hartshorne & Baucom, 2007; Lee et al., 2004; Lin & Yi, 1997; Misra et al., 2003; Mori, 2000; Poyrazli et al., 2004; 2010; Rajapaksa & Dundes, 2002; Sandhu & Asrabadi, 1994; Wei et al., 2007; Wilton & Constantine, 2003; Ye, 2005; Ye, 2006a; Yeh & Inose, 2003; Ying, 2005; Zhang & Goodson, 2011). However, only few of these studies have specifically examined the relationship between acculturative stressors and acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. (Chavajay & Skowronek, 2008; Constantine et al., 2004; Duru & Poyrazli, 2007; Lee et al., 2004; Poyrazli et al., 2004; Poyrazli et al., 2010; Sandhu & Asrabadi, 1994; Wilton & Constantine, 2003; Ye, 2006a; Yeh & Inose, 2003). In addition, only few studies have clearly validated the relationship between certain acculturative stressors (e.g., academic pressures, language difficulties, and length of residence in the U.S.) and acculturative stress. Another limitation of previous research on acculturative stress is the inconsistencies in the conceptualization and operationalization of acculturative stress. For instance, Berry and his associates (1987) have used generic measures of stress to measure acculturative stress and it is possible such measures may not accurately evaluate acculturative stress. Other researchers, primarily those who have used international students as their unit of analysis, have conceptualized acculturative stress based on constructs that reflect the acculturation process such as the Index of Life Stress (ILS) (Yang & Chum, 1995), the Cultural Adjustment Difficulties Checklist (CADC: 10 Acculturative Distress Subscale) (Sodowsky & Lai, 1997), and the Acculturative Stress Scale for International Students (ASSIS) (Sandhu & Asrabadi, 1994). Among these measures, the most widely used is the ASSIS (Chavajay & Skowronek, 2008; Constantine et al., 2004; Duru & Poyrazli, 2007; Poyrazli et al., 2004, 2010; Ye, 2005; Ye , 2006a; Yeh & Inose, 2003; Wei et al., 2007). The ASSIS was first developed by Sandhu and Asrabadi (1994) to measure the overall adjustment problems of international students to the U.S. The ASSIS is a 36-item survey, which utilizes a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree) that assesses adjustment problems of international students. The ASSIS is used to assess five areas of stress, including perceived discrimination, homesickness, perceived hate, fear, and stress due to change/culture shock, guilt, and non-specific concerns. Besides ASSIS, Index of Life Scale ILS is also used in few studies with international students to measure acculturative stress. The ILS is a 30-item measure that is used to assess five areas of stress, including concern about finances and desire to stay in the United States, language difficulties, interpersonal stress, stress from new culture and desire to return to one’s own country, and academic pressures (Yang & Clum, 1994). A major limitation of these scales is that even though they were designed to measure overall adjustment problems of the international students in the U.S., they did not incorporate concerns that reflect overall adjustment difficulties of the international students. For example, the ASSIS primarily focuses on the interpersonal stressors like perceived discrimination, fear, and perceived hate. The functional and cultural stressors such as academic pressures, language difficulties, financial concerns, and stress from 11 living in a new culture are not broadly accentuated in this 33-item scale. Even though, the ILS emphasized on these stressors, it overlooked the emotionally taxing stressor for international students in the U.S., which is homesickness for the country and family left behind. Considering these measures, it is apparent that international students experience acculturative stress from a number of stressors in the U.S. and that no single measure fully captures these stressors. In the development of the ASSIS, Sandhu and Asrabadi (1994) reported that perceived discrimination was the strongest predictor of acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. Perceived discrimination as a possible predictor of acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. has been suggested by other researchers as well (Chen, 1999; Hayes & Lin, 1994; Mori, 2000). Unfortunately, the relationship between perceived discrimination and acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. has not been clearly examined by researchers. The following paragraphs will focus on perceived discrimination as it is related to reported acculturative stress in international students. Perceived Discrimination Jackson, Brown, and Kirby (1998) defined discrimination as “intentional acts that draw unfair or injurious distinctions, that are based on ethnic or racial biases, and that have effects favorable to in-groups and negative to out-groups” (p.110). Jackson et al. (1998) pointed out that discrimination practices originate from ethnocentrism and racial prejudice. The authors explained that ethnocentrism refers to both generally held positive attitudes about one’s own group and a negative view of other groups that differ ethnically 12 and racially from one’s own group. In addition, racial prejudice refers to negative attitudes towards racially categorized members of a group. Perceived discrimination is therefore a subjective perception of being a target of prejudice and discrimination (Dion, 2002). Sandhu and Asrabadi (1994) defined perceived discrimination as “experiences of discrimination based on race or color, receiving mistreatment, and feeling socially isolated” (p. 440). Existing literature has supported the possibility that perceived discrimination might be related to acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. (Chen, 1999; Hayes & Lin, 1994; Mori, 2000; Sandhu & Asrabadi, 1994). However, only limited studies have investigated the role of perceived discrimination in international students’ acculturation process and related stress. Also, previous research has not clearly identified possible sources of perceived discrimination (e.g., race/ethnicity and foreign-status). International students are a diverse group in terms of belonging to different ethnicities and different countries. Thus, it is important to understand whether these students face prejudice and discrimination in the U.S. because of their race or ethnicity or because of their foreign-status. Even though perceived discrimination based on foreign-status has been debated in some of the studies with international student samples in the U.S., this construct has not been clearly identified and explored in research with international student populations. Thus, it may be useful to further explore the types of perceived discrimination (e.g., racial/ethnic-based, foreigner-based) and their relationship to acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. to bridge this gap in literature. 13 Foreign-status and perceived discrimination. In some cases, international students in the U.S. have reported experiencing discrimination from both U.S.-born ethnic majority and minority students (Constantine et al., 2005; Sodowsky & Plake, 1992). For example, Constantine et al. (2005) stated some international African students in the U.S. reported they experienced discriminatory treatment from African Americans. Lee and Rice (2007) described that such experiences of discrimination in international students can be attributed to their foreign-status, limited English proficiency, different English accents, and lack of familiarity with the host culture’s rules and norms. In comparison to previous definitions of racism which were directly linked to race or ethnicity, neo-racism is viewed as discrimination that is based on reported differences in culture and national order (Lee & Rice, 2007). Spears described that “neo-racism rationalizes the subordination of people of color on the basis of culture, which is of course acquired through acculturation within an ethnic group, while traditional racism rationalizes it fundamentally in terms of biology” (as cited in Lee & Rice, 2007, p. 389). Lee and Rice (2007) explained that in neo-racism, discrimination is justified by cultural differences due to nationality and cultural beliefs, norms, values, and practices rather than physical characteristics or biological differences alone. These authors have suggested that international students’ experiences of neo-racism may include negative campus social interactions, interactions with faculty and administration, and off-campus interactions. For example, several international students in Lee and Rice’s (2007) study reported direct insults about their home country and culture by other students and their professors. The study elaborated further on the concept of perceived discrimination by 14 international students in the U.S., but the authors found that it was difficult to determine if such instances of perceived discrimination were due to race/ethnicity or foreign-status, or both. So far, one study has investigated perceived discrimination that is based on foreign-status in international students in the U.S. (Schmitt, Spears, & Branscombe, 2003). Schmitt et al. (2003) measured perceived discrimination based on foreign status or nationality on responses to the following two items: “I feel like students at the University of Kansas look down on me because of my nationality” and “Students at the University of Kansas have discriminated against me because I am not from the United States”. The authors reported that foreign status is a significant source of perceived discrimination for international students in the U.S. This study further revealed that perceived discrimination based on foreign-status also increased international students’ identification with other international students. The authors believed this increased identification with other international students might be a symbol of empathy because of shared common experiences of being treated as an “outsider” or “foreigner”. The importance of foreign-status as a contributor to perceived discrimination for foreigners has also been studied by other researchers with international students in the U.K. and Germany (Krahe, Abraham, Felber, & Helbig, 2005). These researchers have operationalized perceived discrimination that is based on foreign status as “discrimination of increasing severity, comprising antilocution (verbal derogation), avoidance, behavioral discrimination, and physical assault” because the perpetrator clearly assumed the target of the discrimination was a foreigner (Krahe et al., 2005, p. 263). The findings of this study 15 revealed that participants who were identifiable as foreigners by their appearance reported more discrimination than others who were not as easily identifiable as foreigners. This study helped to facilitate a broader scope of discrimination for foreigners in a new culture. However, this scope of perceived discrimination and its direct relation to acculturative stress has not been explored in international student samples in the U.S. One reason for the lack of more research on perceived discrimination based on foreign-status may be attributed to the lack of available measures that have separately assessed this construct. For example, some measures that have identified discrimination based on foreign-status have identified it under a single category and have not distinguished it from other forms of discrimination, such as discrimination based on race/ethnicity (e.g., ILS). Krahe et al. (2005) have taken the initiative in developing a measure to assess foreigner-based discrimination. The measure known as the Experience of Discrimination Scale (EDS) is a 22-item scale with four sub-scales, including physical assault (5 items), (active) behavioral discrimination (6 items), (passive) avoidance (7 items), and antilocution or verbal discrimination, (4 items). The scale utilizes 5- point Likert-type scale ranging from 1= never to 5= very often. The authors reported high reliability of the EDS with alphas of .91 in a German students’ sample, .86 in a U.K. students’ sample. The reported reliability coefficients for the four sub-scales were moderate to high ranging from .68 to .87. Ethnicity and perceived discrimination. The studies on perceived racial and ethnic discrimination for international students reported that students from different 16 ethnicities and geographic regions experience discrimination to varying degrees (Lee & Rice, 2007; Poyrazli & Grahame, 2007; Poyrazli & Lopez, 2007; Yeh & Inose, 2003). Poyrazli and Lopez (2007) noted that a student’s race or ethnicity helped to predict the level of perceived discrimination that was experienced by international students. In a comparative study, these authors examined possible group differences in perceived racial and ethnic discrimination in a sample of international college students and American college students in the U.S. The international college student sample was composed of students from European, Asian and Pacific Island, Middle Eastern, Latin American, and African countries and the American college student sample was composed of African, Asian, Latino, and White American students. The results first indicated that international students experienced higher levels of racial and ethnic discrimination than U.S. students. In addition, international students from Europe reported less racial discrimination than international student from Asia, Africa, and Latin America. The authors posited that nonEuropean international students tend to be a part of a visible racial or ethnic group and thus may be subjected to more discrimination that is due to race or ethnicity. Yeh and Inose (2003) reported that greater levels of racial discrimination and prejudice are likely to occur among international students who are from countries that are racially the most different from host countries such as Africa, Asia, etc. These authors explained that European international students face less discrimination because they are racially White, which helped them ‘fit in’ more easily to American society than international students from Asia, Africa, and Latin/Central America. Studies on ethnic group differences in perceived discrimination among international student population in 17 the U.S. are limited. Nonetheless, the few studies that are available signify that ethnic group differences do exist in the experiences of racial/ethnic perceived discrimination in international students in the U.S. Thus it may be useful to explore ethnic group differences in perceived discrimination for international students not only for reported discrimination based on race/ethnicity, but also based on foreign-status. Length of residence in the U.S. and perceived discrimination. In addition to the impact of race/ethnicity and foreign-status on perceived discrimination reported by international students, length of residence in the U.S. may work with immigrant or foreign-status to contribute to the experiences of perceived discrimination. In the majority of studies on the length of residence for international students, this construct was used to measure the amount of time (often in months or years) after the arrival of international students to the U.S. for their college studies (Klomegah, 2006; Poyrazli & Lopez, 2007; Sumer et al., 2008; Wei et al., 2007; Wilton & Constantine, 2003; Ying, 2005). One documented study on international students reported on the relationship between perceived racial/ethnic discrimination and the length of residency in the U.S. (Poyrazli & Lopez, 2007). The authors reported that length of residence in the U.S. (in years) predicted 19% of the variance of perceived discrimination reported by international students. That is, international students who had lived in the United States for a longer duration reported experiencing more discrimination than the students who lived for a shorter duration. This is not a surprising finding because the longer time period in the U.S. would merely expose international students to more situations where 18 discrimination could take place. Based on this possibility, it may be important to control for years of U.S. residency when studying reported experiences of discrimination by international students because higher reported discrimination may actually be better explained as an artifact of this extended time factor. Perceived discrimination and acculturative stress. Empirically, preliminary studies have examined the association between perceived discrimination and acculturative stress. However, the relationship between these two variables has not been clearly established. Zhang and Goodson (2011), in a systematic review of the predictors of psychosocial adjustment for international students in the U.S., reported only a limited number of studies have examined perceived discrimination or prejudice as a distinct predictor or part of acculturative stress. These authors stated that much research is needed to address macro-level factors that may be associated with acculturative stress for international students such as perceived discrimination. So far, few studies have examined the direct relationship between perceived discrimination and acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. (Chavajay & Skowronek, 2008; Sandhu & Asrabadi, 1994; Sullivan, 2011). These studies revealed somewhat conflicting results on the role of perceived discrimination in acculturative stress for international students. For example, Sandhu and Asrabadi (1994) reported that perceived discrimination accounted for the highest variance (38.3%) in acculturative stress in international students in comparison to homesickness (9%) perceived hate/rejection (7.2%), fear (6.10%), cultural shock (3.2%), and guilt (3.1%). 19 Similarly, Sullivan (2011) found that perceived discrimination was significantly related to acculturative stress in international students. In contrast, Chavajay and Skowronek (2008) reported that perceived discrimination was not correlated with acculturative stress in 130 international students when discrimination and acculturative stress were both being measured quantitatively. However, responses to four open-ended questions in this study revealed that students were experiencing acculturative stress in response to perceived discrimination. These contrasting findings might be attributed to the differences in the approaches used to assess perceived discrimination. Nevertheless, these studies suggest that perceived discrimination may be related to acculturative stress and should be examined in future studies. Previous literature offers limited documentation about the specific source (e.g., foreign-status, race/ethnicity) of perceived discrimination in international students in the U.S. Given that international students are racially and ethnically diverse and from different geographic world regions, they may face discrimination based on their foreignstatus alone, race/ethnicity alone, or both. Some researchers have questioned whether this perceived discrimination is based on race/ethnicity or foreign-status. However, this difference between perceived discrimination based on foreign-status or race/ethnicity has not been clearly investigated. Hence, it may be important to measure perceived discrimination based on multiple levels including discrimination that may be linked to race/ethnicity and foreign-status to bridge this gap in the literature. Furthermore, even with the theoretical importance of perceived discrimination as an acculturative stressor, only limited studies have actually investigated the relationship 20 between perceived discrimination and acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. Consequently, more studies are needed to explore the relationship between perceived discrimination and acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. Also, comparative investigation of perceived discrimination may help in clarifying how it may vary across ethnically different international student groups. Ethnicity Research on the effects of ethnicity and the adjustment to a new culture indicated that the adjustment process becomes more stressful as the differences between the two cultures increase (Poyrazli et al., 2004; Yeh & Inose, 2003). Existing research has found strong correlation between ethnicity and acculturative stress in international students (Constantine et al., 2004; Poyrazli et al., 2004; Poyrazli et al., 2010; Wilton & Constantine, 2003; Yeh & Inose, 2003). For example, Asian international students were found to experience higher levels of acculturative stress than European international students (Poyrazli et al., 2004; Yeh & Inose, 2003). In a comparative study, Yeh and Inose (2003) examined possible ethnic differences in acculturative stress in a sample of European, Asian, Latin American, and African international students. The results indicated that non-European (Asian, Latin American, and African) international students experienced higher levels acculturative stress than European international students. In contrast, another study with a sample of Asian and Latin American internationals students, reported that Latin American international students experienced higher levels of acculturative stress than Asian international students (Wilton & Constantine, 2003). Yet, another study involving Asian, African, and Latin American 21 internationals students found that African international students experienced higher levels of acculturative stress than Asian and Latin American international students. Furthermore, the study revealed that Latin American students experienced higher levels of acculturative stress than Asian international students (Constantine et al., 2004). Overall, past literature seems to support possible ethnic differences in the levels of acculturative stress in international students, however only limited studies have examined ethnic differences in the levels of acculturative stress between European and nonEuropean international students. Further research is needed to examine ethnic group differences in acculturative stress experienced by international students. Social Support Another important construct of interest in the acculturation process of international students is social support. The American Psychological Association (2007) defined social support as “the provision of assistance or comfort to others, typically in order to help them cope with a variety of biological, psychological, and social stressors” (p. 869). Elaborating further on the definition of social support, American Psychological Association (2007) stated, “social support may arise from any interpersonal relationship in the individual’s social network, involving family members, friends, neighbors, religious institutions, colleagues, caregivers, or support groups” (p. 869). This is a broad definition of social support which highlights two key points: the importance of social support in dealing with various stressors and the different types of social support that may come from family, friends, support groups, etc. Mallinckrodt and Leong (1992) stated, ‘‘not only is social support crucial in itself for positive well-being, but social support also 22 provides a powerful coping resource for persons experiencing stressful life changes, including the stress of adjusting to an unfamiliar culture’’ (p. 71). Empirical research on the acculturation process of international students suggest that social support is absolutely critical for these students during the adjustment process as they not only deal with the loss of close familial support who will remain back in their home countries, but also must extend the energy and effort to create new social support networks in the U.S. (Hayes & Lin, 1994; Mallinkrodt & Leong, 1992). For example, a number of studies have indicated that social support is an influential factor in helping international students to overcome a stressful situation (Duru & Poyrazli, 2007; Lee et al., 2004; Misra et al., 2003; Poyrazli et al., 2004; Rajajapaksa & Dundes, 2002; Sumer et al., 2008; Yeh & Inose, 2003 ). However, a significant limitation of the empirical studies is they do not provide a consistently used operational definition of the construct, social support. For example, Misra et al. (2003) operationalized social support as the quantity and quality of contact with one’s own culture, one’s immediate family (e.g., parents and siblings) and new friends in the U.S., local community and student organizations, and religious organizations (e.g., churches and temples). Sumer et al. (2008) on the other hand, operationalized social support as a degree to which respondent’s social relationships provide various dimensions of social support, such as attachment, social integration, reassurance of worth, reliable alliance, guidance, and opportunity for nurturance. Inconsistencies in the use of operational definitions of social support in international students’ acculturation process were seen in other studies that specifically 23 conceptualized acculturative stress (Lee et al., 2004; Poyrazli et al., 2004; Ye, 2006a; Yeh & Inose, 2003). For example, Ye (2006a) operationalized social support as the number of people in the respondent’s interpersonal support network in the U.S. and their level of satisfaction with the support that was offered by these networks. Yeh & Inose (2003) operationalized social support as the number of people (e.g., family and nonfamily members) on whom a person could satisfactorily rely when he or she needed support. In contrast, Lee et al. (2004) operationalized social support as practical and emotional support from different sources such as parents, spouses/partners, friends, American friends, and academic advisors. Furthermore, Poyrazli et al. (2004) operationalized social support as levels of respondents’ perceived social support. Another limitation of the previous studies on the role of social support in international students’ acculturation process is these studies have not clearly identified different types of social support. In the acculturation process of international students, several types of social support can be of different importance. For example, emotional support can be helpful for international students in dealing with a personal crisis and informational support can be helpful for international students in providing information or guidance in understanding how things work in U.S. such as finding a residence, finding a job, etc. However, only limited studies have examined the different types of social support in international students’ acculturation related stress (Lee et al., 2004; Sumer et al., 2008). Besides these different types of social support, the different sources that could provide social support must be taken into account when examining the role of social 24 support in international students’ acculturation process in the U.S. The significance of social support networks to the acculturation process of international students has been examined by a few researchers (Bochner, McLeod, & Lin, 1977; Lee et al., 2004; Poyrazli et al., 2004; Sumer et al., 2008; Swagler & Ellis, 2003; Ye, 2006a; Yeh & Inose, 2003). Among the initial efforts to investigate the role of different sources of social support, the Functional Model of Friendship (Bochner et al., 1977) proposed there were three different social support networks that international students could access during their studies in the U.S: (a) a group of co-national students from the same country who assist with home culture identity maintenance, (b) a network of host nationals for academic and professional purposes, and (c) a multinational group of students from other nations who play an important role in recreational activities. Correspondingly, Misra et al. (2003) concluded that international students perceive social support from three sources: Family members (parents and siblings), members of one’s own culture, and new friends in the U.S. When considering different sources of social support for international students in the U.S, it should also be noted that international students often leave their familiar social support network (e.g., families and friends) in their home countries. As such, these students must meet the challenge of establishing comparable social support networks in the U.S. (Mallinkrodt & Leong, 1992). These new social support networks in the U.S. can be made up of co-nationals, other international students, and U.S. nationals. Research findings, however, have been inconsistent in reporting which of these social support networks play a more significant role in the acculturation process of international 25 students. For example, Church (1982) suggested that international students may benefit from close ties with their co-nationals, who may provide social and emotional support during the adjustment process. In contrast, Swagler and Ellis (2003) reported that social support networks with both co-nationals and American nationals are important for international students. Few studies that have specifically conceptualized acculturative stress also studied different sources of social support for international students in the U.S. For example, Ye (2006a) reported interpersonal support networks in the U.S. and Yeh and Inose (2003) reported family and non-family members (where it was not clearly identified whether family and non-family members were from the international student’s home country or the U.S.) as important sources of social support for international students. Furthermore, Poyrazli et al. (2004) reported that social support from both international and American students is beneficial to international students in the U.S. In addition, in the buffering effects of social support in Korean international students in the U.S., Lee et al. (2004) reported that social support from both Korean (e.g., parents, spouse, friends) and American sources (e.g., friends, academic advisor) buffered the negative effects of acculturative stress on mental health symptoms. These research findings demonstrated that perceived social support from co-nationals, other international students, and American nationals is important for the adjustment and well being of international students. Besides the above mentioned sources of social support for international students, there is a new source of social support that needs more research attention: Online social 26 groups. With increasing access to the internet, many online social groups have been established by international students to communicate with one another about their common concerns of living in a new culture (Ye, 2006a). According to King (1994), online social groups are ‘‘groups of people that exchange ideas and information through the posting of message[s] on electronic bulletin boards (BBS), by sending email to a common list of recipients and by exchanging private emails” (p. 47). Salem, Bogat, and Reid (1997), in their investigation of online mutual-help groups reported that participants in these online groups corresponded in certain ways that were comparable to traditional support groups such as offering high levels of support, acceptance, and positive feelings. One form of these online social groups is online “ethnic” social groups that were developed to provide emotional and informational support to ethnic group members (Ye, 2006a). Ye (2006b) defined online ethnic social groups as “online groups developed for people who have the same national origin and are currently living in a foreign country”. (p. 866). In her study on Chinese international students, Ye (2006b) described online ethnic social groups as “online social groups developed specifically for Chinese students or immigrants in the United States” (p.869). The author reported that perceived social support from both U.S. social networks and online ethnic social groups were negatively related to reported social difficulties for Chinese international students in the U.S. This study emphasized the importance of online social support in the cross-cultural adaptation of Chinese international students. Despite the information that online ethnic social groups can play a vital role in the cross-cultural adaptation, only a few studies have examined the relationship between 27 acculturation related issues and the use of online social support groups for international students in the U.S. (Ye, 2006a, b). For instance, Ye (2006a) found that Chinese international students who received higher amounts of informational support from online ethnic social groups reported lower levels of acculturative stress. The author suggested that online ethnic social groups may play a protective role against acculturative stress that is similar to face-to-face social support in terms of stress reduction. However, this study focused on Chinese international students and this reduced the generalizability of the findings to international students from other countries. Future studies which include ethnically diverse international student samples may provide a better understanding of the importance of social support from online social groups and its relation to acculturative stress for international students. In addition, future investigations with ethnically diverse international student samples may help to increase our understanding of the possible differences in the use of social support resources and preferred sources of social support for international students. Ethnicity and social support. International students are comprised of students from different countries (e.g., European, Asian, African, and Latin American) from all over the world. In the few studies on international student samples from Asia (Lee et al., 2004; Swagler & Ellis, 2003; Ye, 2006b), researchers have reported fairly similar results concerning the relationship between social support and acculturative stress for Chinese, Korean, and Taiwanese international students. For example, a study with Korean international students reported the importance of social support from both host and conational students in moderating the effects of acculturative stress (Lee et al., 2004). Ye 28 (2006a) also found that support from interpersonal (friends and relatives in the U.S.) and online social support groups was related to better adjustment for Chinese international students. While both of these samples were from Asia, the results were consistent in participants’ reporting that different sources of social support helped to reduce or buffer against acculturative stress for both these international students samples. However, these samples were based only on international students from Asian countries and this limits their application to other international students. Future studies should consider the inclusion of a more diverse international student sample to help augment the application of these preliminary conclusions. Length of residency in the U.S. and social support. The length of residence in a host country can influence an international student’s use of social support in the acculturation process. Only a few studies on acculturative stress in international students have focused on the length of residence in the U.S. (Klomegah, 2006; Sumer et al., 2008; Wei et al., 2007; Wilton & Constantine, 2003; Ying, 2005). Within this limited number of studies, only one examined the relationship between length of residence in the U.S. and social support for international students (Ye, 2006b). In a study on the impact of length of U.S. residence (ranging from 1 month to 9 years post-immigration), Ye (2006b) found that newly arrived Chinese international students (i.e., those with an average U.S. residency of 2.9 years) reported higher perceived social support from online ethnic social groups than Chinese international students who had lived in the U.S. for a longer period of time. 29 The length of residency in the U.S. can influence international students’ use of different sources of social support including social support from family members, U.S. nationals (e.g., friends, faculty, and advisors), other international students, and online social support groups. For instance, in the beginning phases of their transition to a new country, international students may come to rely more on international student advisors and co-nationals for informational support. In addition, to deal with acculturative stressors, these students may rely on U.S. friends for emotional support. Whether these sources of social support remain consistent or change during international students’ length of residence in the U.S. is important to consider and should be explored in future studies on the acculturation process of international students. Social support and acculturative stress. Social support is found to be crucial for the adjustment and well being of international students as it can serve as a coping resource. According to Mallinckrodt and Leong (1992), social support serves as a coping resource that lessens or blocks the potentially pathogenic impact of stressors. Despite the theoretical significance of social support to the acculturation process, relatively little research has been done on social support in relation to acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. A review of the literature on the direct and moderating effects of social support on acculturative stress for international students will be discussed in the following paragraphs. Few of the available studies have focused on the direct (Poyrazli et al., 2004; Yeh & Inose, 2003) and moderating effects of social support on acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. (Chen, Mallinckrodt, & Mobley, 2002; Lee et al., 2004; 30 Ye, 2006a). For example, Yeh and Inose (2003) reported that social support significantly predicted the levels of acculturative stress in European and non- European international students. These authors found that international students with higher levels of satisfaction with family and non-family social support networks experienced lower levels of acculturative stress than students who had lower levels of satisfaction with these social support networks. A major limitation of this study is that the authors did not clearly define the term, social support networks; that is, whether these support networks were specifically focusing on family and friends in their home country, in the U.S., or both. In addition, sources of non-family members were not clearly identified by these authors; that is, whether non-family members were friends, academic advisors, teachers, etc. Similar to Yeh and Inose’s (2003) findings, Lee et al. (2004) found that lower levels of social support from Korean sources (e.g., parents, spouse/partner) and American sources (friends, academic advisors) significantly correlated with higher levels of acculturative stress in Korean international students. In another study, Poyrazli et al. (2004) found that international students from European and non-European countries (e.g., Asia, Middle East, Latin America, and Africa) with lower levels of perceived social support experienced higher levels of acculturative stress in comparison to students who reported higher levels of perceived social support. In addition, international students who only received social support from other international students reported higher levels of acculturative stress than those who received social support from both international and American students. 31 In addition to these direct effects, few documented studies have supported the moderating role of social support on stress and mental health issues in which the impact of stress on negative outcomes is lessened in the presence of social support (Chen et al., 2002; Jou & Foukada, 1997; Lee et al., 2004; Mallinckrodt & Leong, 1992). For example, Chen et al. (2002) in their study on East Asian international students found that social support from the International Student Office (ISO) helped to moderate the negative effects of racism on psychological symptoms of distress. In another study, Lee and colleagues (2004) reported a moderating effect of social support on acculturative stress symptoms on mental health symptoms in Korean international students in the U.S. One of the limitations of these studies is that these studies were conducted on Asian international students which can limit their application to international students from non-Asian countries. In addition, these studies have included limited sources of social support, such as social support from family in the home country, new contact support in the U.S., and support from the ISO. The inclusion of a more diverse international student sample along with more sources of social support (e.g., co-nationals, other international students in the U.S., and online ethnic social support groups) and the clear identification of these different sources of support in future studies may provide a better understanding of the effects of social support on acculturative stress in international students population in the U.S. After reviewing the literature on direct and moderating effects of perceived social support on acculturative stress, it could be argued that perceived social support from different sources (i.e., family, co-nationals, other international students, and online ethnic 32 support groups) would have an influence (direct or moderating) on international students’ acculturative stress. In addition, few studies have clearly supported the role of conationals, other international students, and American nationals in influencing the levels of acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. Considering these results and based on the possibility that international students access a number of social support sources during their stay in the U.S., future studies should examine the role of social support sources available to international students in the U.S. in influencing the levels of acculturative stress. Future research should also examine how different types of social support affect acculturative stress and whether perceived social support from different sources (e.g., conationals, other international students, and American nationals) may differentially impact acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. For example, it is possible that the type of social support (e.g., emotional vs. practical/guidance) from co-nationals, host nationals, other international students, and online ethnic support groups may have distinct influences on acculturative stress in international students. Negative Outcomes of Acculturative Stress Adjustment to a new educational and social environment for most college students can be a stressful process. However, many international students may experience more serious stress because of the addition of adjusting to a new culture (Hechanova- Alampay, Beehr, Christiansen, & Van Horn, 2002; Poyrazli & Grahame, 2007). Research has indicated that moving to a new culture can have potentially detrimental effects on people’s mental health (Mallinckrodt & Leong, 1992; Sandhu, 33 1994). The stress due to acculturation can cause a variety of symptoms related to psychological complaints among international students (Berry et al., 1987; Mori, 2000). These symptoms can include anxiety, homesickness, depression, loneliness, the feeling of marginality and alienation, decreased self-esteem, and identity confusion (Lin & Yi, 1997). Mori (2000) reported that acculturative stress can cause international students to feel a sense of hopelessness and, in turn, an intense sense of hopelessness may manifest into depression. Findings suggest that acculturative stress increases the risk for mental health problems such as major depression, anxiety, etc. (Constantine et al., 2004; Lee et al., 2004; Wei et al., 2007). For example, Constantine et al. (2004) examined the relationship between acculturative stress and depressive symptoms in newly arrived African, Asian, and Latin American international college students in the U.S. The results reported that higher acculturative stress scores predicted greater depressive symptoms among international students. In another study by Lee et al. (2004), the authors examined the relationship between acculturative stress and mental health symptoms among 74 Korean international students. Acculturative stress was measured by using the Index of Life Stress (ILS) scale that focused on specific aspects of acculturative stress that were unique to Asian international students. This study demonstrated that acculturative stress was positively correlated with mental health symptoms. Similarly, Wei et al. (2007) in their study on Chinese and Taiwanese international students found that acculturative stress was positively associated with depression. 34 Considering the negative outcomes of acculturative stress on the psychological well-being of the international students in the U.S., it is important to better understand what contributes to acculturative stress for international students. An increased understanding of the possible sources of acculturative stress for international students in the U.S. can have important implications for creating and implementing programs that can provide academic and personal support for this population. For example, the recognition of specific sources of acculturative stress will enable counselors, international student advisors, and professionals working with such international students to actively engage in the development and evaluation of appropriate programs that may help in promoting positive international adjustment and cross-cultural education. Summary This literature review identified specific factors that may be associated with acculturative stress for international students and certain themes emerged from studies in the current literature: 1. Researchers studying international students in the U.S. have identified that international students have to face many difficulties and challenges during their acculturation to the U.S. These difficulties and challenges can cause a particular type of stress described as “acculturative stress” in these international students which can have a detrimental effect on the psychological well-being of these students. Researchers have also identified various sources (i.e., stressors) of acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. However, only a few sources of acculturative stress were clearly 35 identified in the past studies. Future studies should examine less investigated sources of acculturative stress such as perceived discrimination and social support to clearly understand the scope and severity of acculturation-related stress of international students in the U.S. 2. Researchers have suggested perceived discrimination as a possible source of acculturative stress for international students. However, only limited studies have investigated the relationship between perceived discrimination and acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. In addition, the literature is inconclusive and offers limited explanation of the different contributors of perceived discrimination (e.g., race/ethnicity, foreign-status). Factors such as race/ethnicity and foreign status may contribute to perceived discrimination in international students in the U.S. However, past studies on perceived discrimination of international students in the U.S. have not clearly identified whether these acts of discrimination are due to race/ethnicity, national origin/foreign status, or both. Few studies have also reported differences in the perceived discrimination reported by individuals across different ethnicities. That is, international students from non-European countries reported experiencing more perceived discrimination then international students from European countries. Thus, it is important for future studies to take these differences into consideration when examining different types of perceived discrimination and its relationship to acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. Examining such differences may 36 help investigators to better understand whether European and non-European international students differ in their experiences of perceived discrimination based on race/ethnicity and foreign-status. 3. Researchers have suggested that social support is an important factor in the acculturation process of international students and it has both direct and moderating effects on international students’ adjustment to the U.S. However, literature offers limited documentation of the direct and moderating role of social support in acculturative stress for these students. In addition, previous studies do not provide a consistently used operational definition of social support. Although the majority of studies have reported different sources of social support (e.g., family, co-nationals, other international students, American sources) for international students, only a few studies have included specific types of social support (i.e., emotional and practical) in their operational definitions. In addition, certain sources of social support, specifically online ethnic social groups, need to be further investigated with other sources of social support in future studies. It would be beneficial if future studies examine how different types of social support affect acculturative stress and whether perceived social support from different sources (e.g., co-nationals, other international students, American nationals, and online ethnic support groups) may differentially impact acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. The majority of studies that examined social support and acculturative stress were based on Asian 37 international student samples. Given that international students are ethnically diverse, ethnic differences may exist in the preferred sources of social support for these students. Future studies should examine ethnic differences in the preferred sources of social support and how these differences relate to acculturative stress in these students. Purpose of the Study In the present study, I examined if there were ethnic group differences in the reported levels of acculturative stress among non-European and European international students in the U.S. In the present study, I further examined if there were ethnic group differences in the levels of perceived discrimination (i.e., based on foreign-status and race/ethnicity) among non-European and European international students in the U.S. In addition, I examined if there were ethnic group differences in the social support networks such as co-nationals, other international students, American friends, and online ethnic support groups among non-European and European international students in the U.S. The significant relationship between the levels of perceived discrimination (e.g., foreigner-based and race/ethnicity-based) and the levels acculturative stress was also investigated. In addition, the significant relationship between five sources of social support (family in home country, co-nationals, other international students, American friends, and online ethnic support groups) and the levels acculturative stress was investigated. Finally, in this study, I examined whether social support from five sources moderated the relationship between perceived discrimination and acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. 38 In the first hypothesis, I determined whether the ethnicity of international students (non-European and European) was related to reported levels of acculturative stress. 1A: Non-European international students will report higher levels of acculturative stress than European international students. In the second set of hypotheses, I determined whether any ethnic differences existed in the levels of perceived discrimination (race/ethnicity and foreign-status.) after controlling for length of residence in the U.S. 2A: Non-European international students will report more foreigner-based discrimination than European international students. 2B: Non-European international students will report more race/ethnicity-based discrimination than European international students. 2C: International students who have lived in the U.S. longer will report higher rates of foreigner-based and race/ethnicity-based discrimination than those who have lived in the U.S. for a shorter period. In the third set of hypotheses, I determined whether any ethnic differences existed in the five sources of social support (family in home country, co-nationals, other international students, American friends, and online ethnic support groups) after controlling for length of residence in the U.S. 3A: Non-European students will report higher social support from family in home country than European international students 3B: Non-European students will report higher social support from co-nationals than European international students. 39 3C: Non-European students will report lower social support from other international students than European international students. 3D: Non-European students will report lower social support from American friends than European international students. 3E: Non-European students will report higher social support from online ethnic support groups than European international students. In the fourth set of hypotheses, I determined whether levels of perceived discrimination based on foreigner-status and race/ethnicity were related to levels of acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. 4A: Foreigner-based discrimination will be positively related to acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. 4B: Race/ethnicity-based discrimination will be positively related to acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. In the fifth set of hypotheses, I determined whether five sources of social support were related to the levels of acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. 5A: Social support from family in home country will be negatively related to acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. 5B: Social support from co-nationals will be negatively related to acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. 5C: Social support from other international students will be negatively related to acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. 40 5D: Social support from American friends will be negatively related to acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. 5E: Social support from online ethnic support groups will be negatively related to acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. In the sixth set of hypotheses, I determined whether five sources of social support moderated the negative effects of perceived discrimination (e.g., foreigner-based and race/ethnicity-based) on acculturative stress in international students. 6A: For international students who perceive higher levels of foreigner-based discrimination, higher levels of social support from family in home will lead to lower acculturative stress. 6B: For international students who perceive higher levels of foreigner-based discrimination, higher levels of social support from co-nationals will lead to lower acculturative stress. 6C: For international students who perceive higher levels of foreigner-based discrimination, higher levels of social support from other international students will lead to lower acculturative stress. 6D: For international students who perceive higher levels of foreigner-based discrimination, higher levels of social support from American friends will lead to lower acculturative stress. 6E: For international students who perceive higher levels of foreigner-based discrimination, higher levels of social support from online ethnic support groups will lead to lower acculturative stress. 41 6F: For international students who perceive higher levels of race/ethnicity-based discrimination, higher levels of social support from family in home country will lead to lower acculturative stress. 6G: For international students who perceive higher levels of race/ethnicity-based discrimination, higher levels of social support from co-nationals will lead to lower acculturative stress. 6H: For international students who perceive higher levels of race/ethnicity-based discrimination, higher levels of social support from other international students will lead to lower acculturative stress. 6I: For international students who perceive higher levels of race/ethnicity-based discrimination, higher levels of social support from American friends will lead to lower acculturative stress. 6J: For international students who perceive higher levels of race/ethnicity-based discrimination, higher levels of social support from online ethnic support groups will lead to lower acculturative stress. 42 Chapter 2 METHOD Participants The final sample consisted of 87 non-European (43 female, 44 male) and 13 European (9 female, 4 male) undergraduate and graduate international students at California State University, Sacramento. Initially, about 500 international students at California State University, Sacramento were recruited to participate in this study via an email sent by the Office of Global Education. Of this number, 154 international students provided written consent to participate in the survey, which was a 30.8% rate of response. However, only 81 international students completed the survey and provided valid responses, reflecting a 16.2% rate of completion. This rate of completion was comparatively lower to the completion rate reported by previous studies that have used online survey methods (Duru & Poyrazli, 2007; Poyrazli et al., 2004; Poyrazli, Thukral, & Duru, 2010; Wei et al., 2007). In addition, 19 other international students were recruited to participate in this study from the English Language Institute and the Department of Psychology at California State University, Sacramento. All 19 of these students provided written consent and completed the survey with valid responses. Participants in this study were provided the opportunity to receive financial compensation (i.e., random drawing for $30 gift certificates for students recruited from the office of global education and the English language institute) or class credit (i.e., introductory psychology participant pool) for volunteering for the study. With regard to power analysis, Cohen (1982) reported that a minimum 107 participants were required for the 43 multiple regression analysis with more than 8 variables and with a power level of .80 and alpha level of .05 for a medium effect size. Given that the final sample for this study was below the cut-off number, the results of the statistical analysis on this sample must be interpreted with some caution. For this study, only participants who provided valid information for the covariates, independent variables, and dependent variable were included in the final sample. The final sample consisted of 100 international students and the ethnic distribution was 75 Asian, 10 Middle Eastern, 2 Hispanic, and 13 White international students. The Asian sample was represented by participants from India (41%), Taiwan (17%), China (15%), Japan (8%), Vietnam (8%), Hong Kong (4%), South Korea (3%), Singapore (2%), Sri Lanka (1%), and Bangladesh (1%). Regarding visa status, 90 participants had F-1 (student) visas (90%) and 10 had J-1 (temporary educational exchange-visitor) visas (10%). Concerning marital status, 94 reported being single (94%) and 6 reported being married (6%). The sample consisted of both undergraduate (46%) and graduate international students (54%). Finally, in this study, the age of the final sample ranged from 18 to 38 years (M = 25.45, SD = 3.26) and the participants’ length of U.S. residency ranged from .33 to 10.7 years (M = 3.12, SD = 1.96). Measures Demographic Variables Participants provided demographic information including age (in years and months), gender, level of education, marital status, socioeconomic status (SES), ethnicity, visa status, and length of U.S. residency (in years and months). For this study, ethnicity 44 was coded into two broad categories, European (White participants) = 1 and nonEuropean (Asian, Middle Eastern, and Hispanic participants) = 0. Gender was coded as male = 1 and female = 2. To measure SES of participants’ families, the Hollingshead’s Four Factor Index of Social Status (1975) was used to calculate level of socioeconomic status. The Hollingshead’s measure is one of the most frequently used measures of SES (EdwardsHewitt & Gray, 1985). The four factors measured in the scale were marital status and gender of the parents, education level of each parent, and occupation of each parent. However, due to a large number of missing, invalid, or vague responses on these SES questions, this SES variable could not be used in the statistical analysis. English Proficiency To assess for level of English proficiency, a 5-point Likert-type scale was used on three items. Specifically, participants were asked to rate their English language skills in the areas of reading, writing, and speaking. The responses ranged from 1 (poor) to 5 (excellent). The scale was comparable to other English proficiency scales used in previous studies (Duru & Poyrazli, 2007; Sumer et al., 2008; Yeh & Inose, 2003). In the present study, this scale was shown to have a high internal consistency with a Cronbach’s alpha of .93. Perceived Discrimination The Everyday Discrimination Scale (EDS) was used in the present study to measure discrimination based on race/ethnicity and foreigner-status (Williams, Yu, Jackson, & Anderson, 1997). The original 9-item scale has been commonly used to 45 assess discrimination in several studies (Clark, Coleman, & Novak, 2004; Gee, Spencer, Chen, & Takeuchi, 2007; Mays & Cochran, 2001; Panter, Daye, Allen, Wightman, & Deo, 2008; Roberts, Vines, Kaufman, & James, 2008). The item content and stem of the EDS do not specifically mention everyday discrimination due to race or ethnicity, but rather refer to the general experience of unfair treatment, which could be due to race, gender, national origin, religion, or other personal characteristics. The EDS requires respondents to rate the frequency with which they experience unfair treatment in day-to-day life on a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (four or more times) to 4 (never), with higher scores representing lower amount of perceived discrimination. The EDS includes items such as, “You are treated with less courtesy than other people,” “You receive poorer service than other people at restaurants or stores,” and “People act as if they think you are not smart.” After the responses to the nine questions are provided, respondents are then asked to provide the main reason for these experiences from eleven options (e.g., ancestry or national origin, gender, race, sexual orientation, or physical disability). This scale has shown high internal consistency with Cronbach’s alphas ranging from .87 to .91 in racially and ethnically diverse samples (Clark et al., 2004; Gee et al., 2007; Mays & Cochran, 2001; Panter et al., 2008; Williams et al., 1997). The criterionrelated validity of the EDS was assessed by exploring the relationship between perceived racism and youth-reported symptoms (externalizing and internalizing) (Clark et al., 2004). 46 In the present study, this scale was modified to measure discrimination due to race/ethnicity and foreign-status separately. The modifications included changing the scale instructions from “In your day-to-day life, how often have any of the following things happened to you?” to “In your day-to-day life, how often have any of the following things happened to you because of your race/ethnicity” and “In your day-today life, how often have any of the following things happened to you because of your foreign-status.” In addition, to adapt the scale to the present study, the response format was changed from a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (four or more times) to 4 (never) to a 4point scale ranging from 1 (never) to 4 (often). The response format of the EDS has also been changed in a number of previous studies (e.g., Clark et al., 2004; Mays & Cochran, 2001) and found to be appropriate in reflecting a cohesive scale. Based on these modifications, these two perceived discrimination (race/ethnicity and foreigner-based) scales would now reflect that higher scores would represent higher amounts of perceived discrimination. For the current study, Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for race/ethnicitybased discrimination and foreign-based discrimination were calculated as .91 and .92 respectively. Social Support The multi-dimensional support scale (MDSS) was used to measure participants’ social support (Winefield, Winefield, & Tiggemann, 1992). The original 19-item scale is used to measure participant’s availability and adequacy of social support from three sources: family/close friends, peers, and authority figures. Each source of social support requires a participant to respond to the same six items. However, the family/close friends 47 group consisted of an additional item, “How often did they really make you feel loved?” Participants are required to rate the “availability” of social support from these three sources within the past month on a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (never) to 4 (usually/always). Participants are required to rate the “adequacy” of social support from these three sources on a 3-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (would have liked more) to 3 (it was just right). The items on the MDSS measure emotional, practical, and informational support. An example of the emotional support item includes, “How often did they really listen to you when you talked about your concerns or problems?” An example of practical and informational support includes, “How often did they help you in practical ways, like doing things for you or lending you money?” and “How often did they answer your questions or give you advice about how to solve your problems?” The scale has been widely used with a range of groups, with reports of high internal consistency ranging from .81 to .90 as measured by Cronbach's alphas (Albar Marin & Garcia-Ramirez, 2005; Cotton, Dollard, & Jonge, 2002; Jackson, Tucker, Keith, & Herman, 2007; Muller, Gragtmans, & Baker, 2008; Utsey, Lamier, Williams, Bolden, & Lee, 2006; Winefield et al., 1992). The Cronbach’s alpha coefficients reported for the subscales of family/close friends, peers, and authority figures were .81, .85, and .86 respectively (Muller et al., 2008). The scale has also shown predictive validity for adjustment and measures of psychological well-being, including self-esteem, depression, anxiety, and health (Winefield et al., 1992). 48 The MDSS is described as a flexible instrument as it can be adapted for use with various populations. Thus, the potential sources of social support can be selected for the MDSS on the basis of their significance to the recipient group being studied (Winefield et al., 1992). The present study intended to measure social support of international students from five sources: family in home country, co-nationals, other international students, American friends, and online ethnic support groups. Consequently, five sources were listed in contrast to the original three sources of social support and the labels “family/close friends,” “ peers,” and “authority” were replaced by “family in home country,” “ co-nationals,” “ other international students,” “ American friends,” and “online ethnic support groups.” It was recommended that one item, “How often did they try to take your mind off your problems by telling jokes or chattering about other things?” should be omitted from the scale because the item occurs relatively at a low frequency and is not considered supportive (Winefield et al., 1992). Hence, this item was not included in the present study. In sum, 30 items were used to measure social support from the above five sources. Finally, the adequacy of social support was not measured in this study to control for survey length. For the present study, Cronbach’s alpha coefficients reported for the subscales of family in home country, co-nationals, other international students, American friends, and online ethnic support groups were .85, .93, .91, .91, and .90 respectively. 49 Acculturative Stress The Index of Life Stress (ILS) was used to measure participants’ acculturative stress (Yang & Clum, 1995). This 30-item scale was developed to measure culturallybased stressors for international students in the United States and measured five areas of stress, including concern about finances and desire to stay in the United States (6 items), language difficulties (5 items), interpersonal stress (6 items), stress from new culture and desire to return to one’s own country (8 items), and academic pressures (5 items). Specifically, the concern about finances and desire to stay in the United States subscale includes items such as, “I worry about my financial situation.” The language difficulties subscale includes items such as, “my English embarrasses me when I talk to people.” The interpersonal stress subscale comprises items such as “I can feel racial discrimination toward me in restaurants.” The stress from new culture and desire to return home to one’s own country subscale includes items such as, “I don’t like American food.” The academic pressure subscale comprises of items such as, “I worry about my academic performance.” This scale utilizes a 4-point Likert-type scale (0 = never to 3 = often) and higher scores indicates higher life stress. Test retest reliability of the ILS was .87 in the initial study (Yang & Clum, 1995), while other studies have found Cronbach’s alpha reliability to range from .83 to .94 (Chen et al., 2002; Misra et al., 2003). The KuderRichardson 20 internal consistency in the initial study was .86. In regards to concurrent validity, the scale is significantly correlated with the UCLA Depression Scale, the Zung Depression Scale, and the Beck Hopelessness Scale (Yang & Clum, 1995). For the Yang and Clum study, the internal consistency estimates for the five factors were: concern 50 about finances and desire to stay in the U.S. (.80), language difficulties (.79), interpersonal stress (.82), stress from new culture and desire to return to one’s own country (.70), and academic pressures (.75). A study by Misra et al. (2003) also found coefficient alphas of .71 to .88 on the ILS subscales. The ILS has been used in studies in the United States with East Asian international students (Chen et al., 2002) and a general international student population (Misra et al., 2003). In the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient reported for the scale was .88. In addition, the Cronbach’s alphas reported for the five sub-scales in the present study were: concern about finances and desire to stay in the U.S. (.83), language difficulties (.76), interpersonal stress (.85), stress from new culture and desire to return to one’s own country (.68), and academic pressures (.56). Procedure In collaboration with the Office of Global Education at California State University, Sacramento, an invitation to participate in the study using electronic mailing lists was sent to approximately 500 currently enrolled international students. The invitation included a link to the online survey website (www.surveymonkey.com). Approximately two weeks later, a follow-up reminder email invitation with a link to the online survey was sent to the Office of Global Education to be forwarded to enrolled international students. Subjects’ anonymity was assured by not including any identifiable information in the survey. Typically, the online survey took about 30 minutes to complete. 51 In addition, subjects were recruited from the English Language Institute at California State University, Sacramento. Prospective subjects from the English Language Institute arrived in one of the classrooms in Del Norte Hall. Upon arrival, subjects were informed of the study’s purposes and risks and then asked to provide written consent to participate in the study. If the student agreed to participate in the study, s/he received a packet of survey questionnaires. The subjects took approximately 30 minutes to complete the survey. After completing the survey, subjects received a debriefing form which explained the purpose of the study in greater detail and contact information about the primary investigator was provided should they have any further questions about the study. The participants from the English Language Institute and online survey who wished to be entered into a random drawing for a gift certificate were directed to send their names to the researcher after the completion of the surveys. As an incentive for participation, 11 $30 visa cards were randomly distributed to participants who expressed an interest in financial compensation. Finally, subjects were recruited from the introductory psychology pool in the Department of Psychology at California State University, Sacramento. After signing up for the study, prospective subjects from the Department of Psychology arrived in one of the shared research laboratories in Amador Hall. Upon arrival, subjects were informed of the study’s purposes and risks and then asked to provide written consent to participate in the study. If the subject agreed to participate in the study, s/he received a packet of survey questionnaires. The subjects took approximately 30 minutes to complete the 52 survey. After completing the survey, subjects received a debriefing form, which explained the purpose of the study in greater detail and contact information about the primary investigator was provided should they have any further questions about the study. The subjects from the Department of Psychology were assigned a course credit of half an hour for their participation. 53 Chapter 3 RESULTS Preliminary Findings Preliminary analyses on the covariates and independent variables were conducted to determine if some of the variables may be highly correlated to each other. A Pearson product-moment correlation analysis showed that the covariates of age and length of U.S. residency were not significantly correlated, (r = .09). A chi-square analysis reported that European and non-European participants did not differ in terms of gender, ² (1, N = 100) = 1.78, p = .18. However, a t-test analysis revealed significant ethnic differences in the levels of English proficiency, t(99) = 3.64, p < .01. Specifically, European international students (M = 4.23, SD = .61) reported higher English proficiency than non-European international students (M = 3.48, SD = 1.10). Based on this information, the covariate of English proficiency was not included in the main analyses. The means, standard deviations, and percentages of the covariates, independent variables, and dependent variables are reported in Tables 1 and 2. Hypothesis 1 An analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was conducted to investigate whether nonEuropean international students reported higher acculturative stress than European international students after controlling for the contribution of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency. The results of the ANCOVA showed a marginally significant main effect for ethnicity on acculturative stress, F(1, 98) = 3.32, p = .07, η² = .11. Specifically, nonEuropean international students (M = 1.28, SE = .05) reported slightly higher levels of 54 acculturative stress than European international students (M = 1.03, SE = .13). The results of the ANCOVA showed no significant effect for the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency. The estimated marginal means of acculturative stress for non-European and European international students are reported in Table 3. Hypothesis 2A and 2B A hierarchical multiple regression analysis was conducted to determine whether length of U.S. residency and ethnicity were significantly related to perceived race/ethnicity-based discrimination and foreigner-based discrimination after controlling for the contribution of age and gender. In these two regression models, the covariates of age and gender were entered in the first block and length of U.S. residency and ethnicity were added in the second block. Tests for multicollinearity of the covariates and independent variables in these two regression models were not found to be significant. The results of these two regression models are reported in Table 4. For the regression analysis on race/ethnicity based discrimination, the first block of the covariates of age and gender was not found to be significant. The second block with length of U.S. residency and ethnicity was also not found to be significant. For the regression analysis on foreigner-based discrimination, the first block of the covariates of age and gender was not found to be significant. However, when length of U.S. residency and ethnicity were added in the second block, the overall regression model was found to be marginally significant F(2, 97) = 2.53, p = .05, and accounted for 10% of the explained variance. Specifically, international students who lived in the U.S. longer reported higher levels of foreigner-based discrimination, t(98) = 2.17, p = .04. Table 1 Summary of Means and Standard Deviations for Demographic Variables Total sample Non-European European (N = 100) (n = 87) (n = 13) Variables M SD M SD M SD Participant age 25.21 3.32 24.46 3.39 25.21 2.79 Participant length of U.S. residency 3.05 2.00 2.97 1.89 3.05 2.77 Participant gender: Male 48.00% 49.43% 30.77% Female 52.00% 50.57% 69.23% Note: Ethnicity was coded as non-European = 0, European = 1. Age ranged from 18-38 years and length of U.S. residency ranged from 0.33-10.67 years. Participant gender indicates the percentages of males and females. 55 Table 2 Means and Standard Deviations of Perceived Discrimination, Social Support, and Acculturative Stress Total sample Non-European European (N = 100) (n = 87) (n = 13) Variables M SD M SD M SD Race/ethnicity-based discrimination 1.82 0.65 1.85 0.63 1.59 0.76 Foreigner-based discrimination 1.69 0.62 1.74 0.62 1.41 0.58 Social support family in home country 3.56 0.56 3.59 0.56 3.38 0.58 Social support co-nationals 2.63 0.86 2.68 0.84 2.27 0.91 Social support other international students 2.19 0.79 2.23 0.79 1.92 0.77 Social support American friends 2.34 0.74 2.32 0.77 2.47 0.49 Social support online ethnic support groups 1.58 0.67 1.61 0.68 1.37 0.53 Acculturative stress 1.24 0.45 1.27 0.45 1.05 0.49 56 Note: Perceived discrimination scores range from 1-4, social support scores range from 1-4, and acculturative stress scores range from 0-3. Higher scores for acculturative stress, perceived discrimination, and social support represent a higher amount of acculturative stress, perceived discrimination, and social support. 57 Table 3 Estimated Marginal Means of Non-European and European International Students for Perceived Discrimination, Social Support and Acculturative Stress Non-European European (N = 87) (N = 13) Variables M SE M SE Race/ethnicity-based discrimination 1.85 0.07 1.61 0.18 Foreigner-based discrimination 1.73 0.07 1.44 0.17 Social support family in home country 3.58 0.06 3.41 0.16 Social support co-nationals 2.68 0.09 2.29 0.24 Social support other international students 2.22 0.09 1.94 0.22 Social support American friends 2.32 0.08 2.44 0.21 Social support online ethnic support groups 1.61 0.07 1.40 0.18 58 Acculturative stress 1.28 0.05 1.03 0.13 Note: Perceived discrimination scores range from 1-4, social support scores range from 1-4, and acculturative stress scores range from 0-3. Higher scores for acculturative stress, perceived discrimination, and social support represent a higher amount of acculturative stress, perceived discrimination, and social support. 59 Table 4 Multiple Hierarchical Regression Results with Ethnicity and Length of U.S. Residency Predicting Race/Ethnicity-Based and Foreigner-Based Discrimination Variables Race/ethnicity-based Foreigner-based discrimination discrimination B SE β Step 1 Variables B SE β Step 1 Gender 0.09 0.02 0.07 Gender 0.13 0.13 0.10 Age -0.02 0.02 -0.09 Age -0.02 0.02 -0.13 R² 0.01 R² Step 2 0.03 Step 2 Gender 0.08 0.13 0.06 Gender 0.11 0.12 0.09 Age -0.01 0.02 -0.04 Age -0.01 0.02 -0.07 Length of U.S. residency -0.06 0.03 -0.17 Length of U.S. residency* -0.07 0.03 -0.22 60 Ethnicity R² 0.24 0.06 0.20 0.12 Ethnicity R² 0.29 0.18 0.16 0.10 Note: Gender was coded as male = 1, female = 2. Ethnicity was coded as non-European = 0, European = 1. *p < .05. 61 62 Hypothesis 3A-3E A multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) was conducted to determine if non-European international students reported higher social support from family in home country, co-nationals, and online ethnic support groups and lower social support from other international students and American friends than European international students after controlling for age, gender, and length of U.S. residency. The overall results of the MANCOVA on social support revealed no significant main effect for ethnicity. In addition, the overall results reported no significant effects for the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency. The ANCOVAs for social support from family in home country, co-nationals, other international students, online ethnic support groups, and American friends showed no significant main effect for ethnicity. Also, the covariate of age was not found to be significant for all five measures of social support. However, the covariate of length of U.S. residency was found to be marginally significant for social support from online ethnic support groups, F(1, 98) = 3.23, p = .08. Specifically, international students who lived in the U.S. longer reported higher social support from online ethnic support groups than international students who lived in the U.S. for a shorter period of time. In addition, the covariate of gender was reported to be marginally significant for social support from online ethnic support groups, F(1, 98) = 3.16, p = .08. Specifically, female international students reported higher social support from online ethnic support groups than male international students. 63 Hypothesis 4A and 4B A hierarchical multiple regression analysis was performed to determine whether there was a significant relationship between perceived discrimination (foreigner-based and race/ethnicity-based) and acculturative stress after controlling for the contribution of ethnicity, age, gender, and length of U.S. residency. To control for issues of multicollinearity, two separate hierarchical multiple regression analysis were completed, one with race/ethnicity-based discrimination and the other with foreigner-based discrimination, as independent variables. Tests for multicollinearity on the covariates and independent variables were not found to be significant. In these two hierarchical multiple regression models predicting acculturative stress, the covariates of ethnicity, age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were entered in the first block and race/ethnicity-based discrimination or foreigner-based discrimination was entered in the second block. The results of these two regression models are reported in Table 5. In the first regression model with race/ethnicity-based discrimination as an independent variable, the covariates of age, gender, length of U.S. residency, and ethnicity were not found to be significant predictors of acculturative stress. However, when race/ethnicity-based discrimination was added in the second block, the overall regression model was found to be significant, F(5, 94) = 10.51, p < .001, and this model accounted for 35.9% of the variance of acculturative stress. Specifically, participants who reported higher levels of race/ethnicity discrimination reported higher levels of acculturative stress, t(98) = 6.60, p < .001. In addition, the covariate of gender became 64 marginally significant t(98) = 2.01, p = .05, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. However, the covariates of age and length of U.S. residency continued to be not significant in the second model. In the second regression model with foreigner-based discrimination as an independent variable, the covariates of gender, length of U.S. residency, age, and ethnicity were not found to be significant. However, when foreigner-based discrimination was added in the second block, the overall model was found to be significant, F(5,94) = 13.51, p < .001, and this accounted for 41.8% of the variance of acculturative stress. Specifically, participants who reported higher levels of foreignerbased discrimination reported higher levels of acculturative stress, t(98) = 7.60 , p < .001. In addition, the covariate of gender was found to be significant, t(98) = 2.40, p = .02, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. However, the covariates of length of U.S. residency and age continued to be not significant in the second model. Hypothesis 5A-5E A hierarchical multiple regression analysis was performed to determine whether the five sources of social support (family in home country, co-nationals, other international students, American friends, and online ethnic support groups ) were significantly related to acculturative stress in international students while controlling for the contribution of ethnicity ,age, gender, and length of U.S. residency. Five separate hierarchical multiple regression analysis were completed to avoid issues of 65 multicollinearity associated with the different types of social support. Tests for multicollinearity on the covariates and independent variables were not found to be significant. In each hierarchical regression analysis, the covariates of ethnicity, age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were entered in the first block of the regression model and the source of social support was entered in the second block of the regression model. In the first regression model which included social support from family in their home country as an independent variable, the results indicated that the covariates of ethnicity, age, gender, and length of U.S. residency in the first block were not found to be significant. When social support from family in home country was entered in the second block, the overall model was found to be marginally significant, F(5, 94) = 2.31, p = .05 , and this model accounted for 10.9% of the variance of acculturative stress. Specifically, international students with higher social support from family in their home country reported lower acculturative stress, t(98) = 2.26, p = .03. In addition, the covariate of ethnicity in the second model was found to be marginally significant, t(98) = 2.09, p = .04. Specifically, non-European international students reported higher acculturative stress than European international students. However, the covariates of length of U.S. residency, age, and gender continued to be not significant in the second block. The results of this regression model are reported in Table 6. In the second regression model which included social support from co-nationals as an independent variable, the results indicated that the covariates of ethnicity, age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be significant in the first block. 66 Also, the addition of social support from co-nationals in the second block was not found to be significant. In the third regression model which included social support from other international students as an independent variable, the results showed that the covariates of ethnicity, age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be significant in the first block. Also, the addition of social support from other international students in the second block was not found to be significant. In the fourth regression model which included social support from American friends as an independent variable, the results indicated that the covariates of ethnicity, age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be significant in the first block. When social support from American friends was added to the second block, the overall model was found to be significant, F(5, 94) = 3.31, p = .01 , and this model accounted for 14.9% of the variance of acculturative stress. Specifically, international students who reported higher social support from American friends reported lower acculturative stress, t(98) = 3.12, p = .001. In addition, the covariate of ethnicity in the second model was found to be marginally significant, t(98) = 1.74, p = .09, providing support that non-European international students reported higher acculturative stress than European international students. However, the covariates of length of U.S. residency, age, and gender continued to be not significant in the second block. The results of this regression model are reported in Table 6. Table 5 Multiple Hierarchical Regression Results with Race/Ethnicity-Based and Foreigner-Based Discrimination Predicting Acculturative Stress Acculturative stress Variables B SE Acculturative stress β Step 1 Variables B SE β Step 1 Gender 0.12 0.09 0.14 Gender 0.12 0.09 0.14 Age -0.00 0.01 -0.00 Age -0.00 0.01 -0.00 Length of U.S. residency 0.03 0.02 0.12 Length of U.S. residency 0.03 0.02 0.12 Ethnicity -0.25 0.14 -0.18 Ethnicity -0.25 0.14 -0.18 R² 0.06 R² Step 2 0.06 Step 2 Gender† 0.15 0.08 0.17 Gender* 0.17 0.07 0.19 Age -0.00 0.01 -0.03 Age -0.01 0.01 -0.05 67 Length of U.S residency 0.01 0.02 0.03 Length of U.S. residency -0.00 0.02 -0.02 Ethnicity -0.15 0.11 -0.12 Ethnicity -0.12 0.11 -0.09 Race/ethnicity-based -0.40 0.06 -0.56 Foreigner-based -0.46 0.06 -0.63 discrimination*** R² discrimination*** 0.36 R² 0.42 Note: Gender was coded as male = 1, female = 2. Ethnicity was coded as non-European = 0, European = 1. † p < .10. *p < .05. ***p < .001. 68 Table 6 Multiple Hierarchical Regression Results with Social Support from Family in Home Country and American Friends Predicting Acculturative Stress Acculturative stress Variables B SE Acculturative stress β Step 1 Variables B SE β Step 1 Gender 0.12 0.09 0.14 Gender 0.12 0.09 0.14 Age -0.00 0.01 -0.00 Age -0.00 0.01 -0.00 Length of U.S. residency 0.03 0.02 0.12 Length of U.S. residency 0.03 0.02 0.12 Ethnicity -0.25 0.14 -0.18 Ethnicity -0.25 0.14 -0.18 0.09 0.15 R² 0.06 R² Step 2 Gender 0.06 Step 2 0.09 0.09 0.10 Gender 0.13 69 Age -0.00 0.01 -0.01 Age -0.01 0.01 -0.05 Length of U.S. residency 0.02 0.02 0.09 Length of U.S. residency 0.03 0.02 0.15 Ethnicity* -0.28 0.13 -0.21 Ethnicity† -0.23 0.13 -0.17 Social support family in -0.18 0.08 -0.23 Social support American -0.19 0.06 -0.30 home country* R² friends** 0.11 R² 0.15 Note: Gender was coded as male = 1, female = 2. Ethnicity was coded as non-European = 0, European = 1. † p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01. 70 71 In the fifth regression model which included social support from online ethnic support groups as an independent variable, the results showed that the covariates of ethnicity, age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be significant in the first block. Also, the addition of social support from online ethnic support groups in the second block was not found to be significant. Hypothesis 6A-6J A hierarchical multiple regression analysis was performed to determine whether the five sources of social support (family in home country, co-nationals, other international students, American friends, and online ethnic support groups ) moderated the effects of perceived discrimination (foreigner-based and race/ethnicity–based) on acculturative stress. As suggested by Frazier, Tix, and Barron (2004), standardized scores were calculated and subsequently used for analyzing the interaction between the social support and discrimination variables in the regression model. Standardization of the predictor and moderator variables is generally recommended when using a regression model with interactions to limit the problems associated with multicollinearity. In addition, the standardization of predictor and moderator variables is recommended to control for possible errors that may arise from using different types of scaling for the independent variables. For standardization, Z scores for age, length of U.S. residency, perceived discrimination (foreigner-based and race/ethnicity-based), and sources of social support (family in home country, co-nationals, other international students, American friends, and online ethnic support groups) were created. After standardization of these variables, ten 72 product-terms were formulated to represent the interaction between perceived discrimination (foreigner-based and race/ethnicity-based) and sources of social support (family in home country, co-nationals, other international students, American friends, and online ethnic support groups). For gender and ethnicity variables, counter-balancing steps had to be taken to correspond with the z-scoring of the other variables in the regression model as recommended by Frazier et al. (2004). Specifically, for gender variable, males were re-coded as -1 and females were re-coded as 1. Similarly, for the ethnicity variable, non-European internationals students were re-coded as -1 and European international students were re-coded as 1. To control for issues of multicollinearity, ten separate hierarchical multiple regression analysis were completed, one each for the interaction of race/ethnicity-based discrimination and social support from family in home country, co-nationals, other international students, American friends, and online ethnic support groups and the interaction of foreigner-based discrimination and social support from family in home country, co-nationals, other international students, American friends, and online ethnic support groups. Tests for multicollinearity on the covariates and independent variables for these 10 different models were not found to be significant. For each hierarchical regression analysis, the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were entered in the first block. Ethnicity was entered in the second block, perceived discrimination (race/ethnicity-based and foreigner-based) was entered in the third block, source of social support (family in home country, co-nationals, other international students, American friends, and online ethnic support groups) was entered 73 in the fourth block, and the interaction term (foreigner-based and race/ethnicity-based discrimination and social support from family in home country, co-nationals, other international students, American friends, and online ethnic support groups) was entered in the fifth block. The results of these regression models using race/ethnicity-based and foreigner-based discrimination as independent variables are reported in Tables 7, 8, 9, 10, and 11. Results for the first hierarchical multiple regression analysis indicated that the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be significant in the first block. Similarly, ethnicity and the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be significant predictors of acculturative stress in the second block. However, when race/ethnicity-based discrimination was entered in the third block, the regression model was found to be significant, F(5, 94) = 10.51, p < .001, and this model accounted for 36% of the variance of acculturative stress. Specifically, international students with higher race/ethnicity-based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress, t(98) = 6.60, p < .001. The covariate of gender in the third block was also found to be marginally significant, t(98) = 2.01, p = .05, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. When social support from family in home country was added in the fourth block, the regression model continued to be significant, F(6, 93) = 10.24, p < .001, and the overall variance explained by the model increased to 40% . Specifically, international students with higher support from family in home country reported lower acculturative 74 stress t(98) = 2.46, p = .007. In addition, race/ethnicity-based discrimination in the fourth block continued to be significant, t(98) = 6.68, p < .001, providing support that international students with higher race/ethnicity-based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress. The covariate of gender in the fourth block was also found to be marginally significant, t(98) = 2.02, p = .05, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. The covariates of age and length of U.S. residency and ethnicity in the fourth block were not found to be significant. With the interaction between race/ethnicity-based discrimination and social support from family in home country being entered in the fifth block, the overall model continued to be significant, F(7,92) = 8.70, p < .001, but the overall explained variance (40%) did not increase significantly. The covariates of gender, age, and length of U.S. residency, ethnicity, race/ethnicity-based discrimination, and social support from family in home country, and the interaction between race/ethnicity-based discrimination and social support from family in home country in the fifth block were not found to be significant. The results of this regression model are reported in Table 7. Results for the second hierarchical multiple regression analysis indicated that the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be significant in the first block. Similarly, ethnicity and the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be predictors of acculturative stress in the second block. However, when race/ethnicity-based discrimination was entered in the third block, the regression model was found to be significant, F(5, 94) = 10.51, p < .001, and this model 75 accounted for 36% of the variance of acculturative stress. Specifically, international students with higher race/ethnicity-based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress, t(98) = 6.60, p < .001. The covariate of gender in the third block was also found to be marginally significant, t(98) = 2.01, p = .05, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. When social support from co-nationals was added in the fourth block, the regression model continued to be significant, F(6, 93) = 8.68, p < .001, but the overall variance explained by the model (36%) did not increase significantly. Race/ethnicity based discrimination in the fourth block continued to be significant, t(98) = 6.43, p < .001, providing support that international students with higher race/ethnicity-based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress Also, the covariate of gender in the fourth block continued to be marginally significant, t(98) = 2.00, p = .05, with female international students reporting higher acculturative stress than male international students. The covariates of age and length of U.S. residency, ethnicity, and social support from co-nationals in the fourth block were not found to be significant. With the interaction between race/ethnicity-based discrimination and social support from co-nationals being entered in the fifth block, the overall model continued to be significant, F(7,92) = 7.37, p < .001, but the overall explained variance (36%) did not increase significantly. The covariate of gender in the fifth block continued to be significant, t(98) = 1.98, p = .05, with female international students reporting higher acculturative stress than male international students. However, the covariates of age and 76 length of U.S. residency, ethnicity, race/ethnicity-based discrimination, social support from co-nationals, and the interaction between race/ethnicity-based discrimination and social support from co-nationals in the fifth block were not found to be significant. The results of this regression model are reported in Table 8. Results for the third hierarchical multiple regression analysis indicated that the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be significant in the first block. Similarly, ethnicity and the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be predictors of acculturative stress in the second block. However, when race/ethnicity-based discrimination was entered in the third block, the regression model was found to be significant, F(5, 94) = 10.51, p < .001, and this model accounted for 36% of the variance of acculturative stress. Specifically, international students with higher race/ethnicity-based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress, t(98) = 6.60, p < .001. The covariate of gender in the third block was also found to be marginally significant, t(98) = 2.01, p = .05, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. When social support from other international students was added in the fourth block, the regression model continued to be significant, F(6, 93) = 9.21, p < .001, and the overall variance explained by the model increased to 37%. Race/ethnicity based discrimination in the fourth block continued to be significant, t(98) = 6.50, p < .001, providing support that international students with higher race/ethnicity-based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress. Also, the covariate of gender in the 77 fourth block was also found to be marginally significant, t(98) = 1.84, p = .07, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. However, the covariates of age and length of U.S. residency, ethnicity, and social support from other international students in the fourth block were not found to be significant. With the interaction between race/ethnicity-based discrimination and social support from other international students being entered in the fifth block, the overall model continued to be significant, F(7,92) = 7.98, p < .001, and the overall explained variance explained by the model was reported to be 38%. Race/ethnicity based discrimination in the fifth block continued to be significant, t(98) = 2.94, p = .004, providing support that international students with higher race/ethnicity-based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress. Also, the covariate of gender in the fifth block was also found to be marginally significant, t(98) = 1.80, p = .08, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. However, the covariates of age and length of U.S. residency, ethnicity, and social support from other international students, and the interaction between race/ethnicity-based discrimination and social support from other international students in the fifth block were not found to be significant. The results of this regression model are reported in Table 9. Results for the fourth hierarchical multiple regression analysis indicated that the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be significant in the first block. Similarly, ethnicity and the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. 78 residency were not found to be predictors of acculturative stress in the second block. However, when race/ethnicity-based discrimination was entered in the third block, the regression model was found to be significant, F(5, 94) = 10.51, p < .001, and this model accounted for 36% of the variance of acculturative stress. Specifically, international students with higher race/ethnicity-based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress, t(98) = 6.60, p < .001. The covariate of gender in the third block was also found to be marginally significant, t(98) = 2.01, p = .05, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. When social support from American friends was added in the fourth block, the regression model continued to be significant, F(6, 93) = 9.65, p < .001, and the overall variance explained by the model increased to 38%. Specifically, international students with higher support from American friends reported lower acculturative stress t(98) = 1.94, p = .06. In addition, race/ethnicity based discrimination in the fourth block continued to be significant, t(98) = 5.95, p < .001, providing support that international students with higher race/ethnicity-based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress Also, the covariate of gender in the fourth block continued to be significant, t(98) = 2.06, p = .04, with female international students reporting higher acculturative stress than male international students. However, the covariates of age and length of U.S. residency, and ethnicity in the fourth block were not found to be significant. With the interaction between race/ethnicity-based discrimination and social support from American friends being entered in the fifth block, the overall model 79 continued to be significant, F(7,92) = 8.58, p < .001, and the overall explained variance increased to 40%. Race/ethnicity based discrimination in the fifth block continued to be significant, t(98) = 3.04, p = .003, providing support that international students with higher race/ethnicity-based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress. Also, the covariate of gender in the fifth block continued to be significant, t(98) = 2.06, p = .04, with female international students reporting higher acculturative stress than male international students. However, the covariates of age and length of U.S. residency, ethnicity, social support from American friends, and the interaction between race/ethnicity-based discrimination and social support from American friends in the fifth block were not found to be significant. The results of this regression model are reported in Table 10. Results for the fifth hierarchical multiple regression analysis indicated that the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be significant in the first block. Similarly, ethnicity and the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be predictors of acculturative stress in the second block. However, when race/ethnicity-based discrimination was entered in the third block, the regression model was found to be significant, F(5, 94) = 10.50, p < .001, and this model accounted for 36% of the variance of acculturative stress. Specifically, international students with higher race/ethnicity-based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress, t(98) = 6.60, p < .001. The covariate of gender in the third block was also found to be marginally significant, t(98) = 2.01, p = .05, providing support that female 80 international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. When social support from online ethnic support groups was added in the fourth block, the regression model continued to be significant, F(6, 93) = 9.28, p < .001, and the overall variance explained by the model increased to 37%. Race/ethnicity-based discrimination in the fourth block continued to be significant, t(98) = 6.68, p < .001, providing support that international students with higher race/ethnicity-based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress. However, the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency, ethnicity, and social support from online ethnic support groups in the fourth block were not found to be significant. With the interaction between race/ethnicity-based discrimination and social support from online ethnic support groups being entered in the fifth block, the overall model continued to be significant, F(7,92) = 9.34, p < .001, and the overall explained variance increased to 42%. The interaction between race/ethnicity-based discrimination and social support from online ethnic support groups in the fifth block was found to be significant, t(98) = 2.54, p = .03, providing support that social support from online ethnic support groups moderate the effects of race/ethnicity-based discrimination on acculturative stress. Specifically, international students who reported higher levels of race/ethnicity-based perceived discrimination and who reported higher levels of social support from online ethnic groups, experienced lower levels acculturative stress. Race/ethnicity-based discrimination in the fifth block continued to be significant, t(98) = 5.12, p < .001, providing support that international students with higher race/ethnicity- 81 based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress. Also, the covariate of gender was found to be marginally significant, t(98) = .1.92, p = .06, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. However, the covariates of age and length of U.S. residency, ethnicity, and social support from online ethnic support groups in the fifth block were not found to be significant. The results of this regression model are reported in Table 11. Results for the sixth hierarchical multiple regression analysis indicated that the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be significant in the first block. Similarly, ethnicity and the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be predictors of acculturative stress in the second block. However, when foreigner-based discrimination was entered in the third block, the regression model was found to be significant, F(5, 94) = 13.51, p < .001, and this model accounted for 42% of the variance of acculturative stress. Specifically, international students with higher foreigner-based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress, t(98) = 7.59, p < .001. The covariate of gender in the third block was also found to be significant, t(98) = 2.40, p = .02, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. When social support from family in home country was added in the fourth block, the regression model continued to be significant, F(6, 93) = 13.33, p < .001, and the overall variance explained by the model increased to 46% . Specifically, international students with higher support from family in home country reported lower acculturative stress t(98) = 2.76, p = .01. In addition, foreigner based discrimination in the fourth 82 block continued to be significant, t(98) = 7.81, p < .001, providing support that international students with higher foreigner-based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress. The covariate of gender in the fourth block was also found to be marginally significant, t(98) = 2.02, p = .05, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. However, the covariates of age and length of U.S. residency, and ethnicity in the fourth block were not found to be significant. With the interaction between foreigner-based discrimination and social support from family in home country being entered in the fifth block, the overall model continued to be significant, F(7,92) = 11.30, p < .001, but the overall explained variance (46%) did not increase significantly. The covariate of gender in the fifth block was also found to be marginally significant, t(98) = 2.00, p = .05, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. However, the covariates of age and length of U.S. residency, ethnicity, foreigner-based discrimination, social support from family in home country, and the interaction between foreigner-based discrimination and social support from family in home country in the fifth block were not found to be significant. The results of this regression model are reported in Table 7. Results for the seventh hierarchical multiple regression analysis indicated that the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be significant in the first block. Similarly, ethnicity and the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be predictors of acculturative stress in the second block. 83 However, when foreigner-based discrimination was entered in the third block, the regression model was found to be significant, F(5, 94) = 13.51, p < .001, and this model accounted for 42% of the variance of acculturative stress. Specifically, international students with higher foreigner-based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress, t(98) = 7.59, p < .001. The covariate of gender in the third block was also found to be marginally significant, t(98) = 2.40, p = .02, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. When social support from co-nationals was added in the fourth block, the regression model continued to be significant, F(6, 93) = 11.12, p < .001, but the overall explained variance (42%) did not increase significantly. Foreigner based discrimination in the fourth block continued to be significant, t(98) = 7.48, p < .001, providing support that international students with higher foreigner-based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress. Also, the covariate of gender in the fourth block continued to be significant, t(98) = 2.38, p = .02, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. However, the covariates of age and length of U.S. residency, ethnicity, and social support from conationals in the fourth block were not found to be significant. With the interaction between foreigner-based discrimination and social support from co-nationals being entered in the fifth block, the overall model continued to be significant, F(7,92) = 9.63, p < .001, but the overall explained variance (42%) did not increase significantly. The covariate of gender in the fifth block continued to be significant, t(98) = 2.38, p = .02, providing support that female international students 84 reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. However, the covariates of age and length of U.S. residency, ethnicity, foreigner-based discrimination, social support from co-nationals, and the interaction between foreigner-based discrimination and social support from co-nationals in the fifth block were not found to be significant. The results of this regression model are reported in Table 8. Results for the eighth hierarchical multiple regression analysis indicated that the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be significant in the first block. Similarly, ethnicity and the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be predictors of acculturative stress in the second block. However, when foreigner-based discrimination was entered in the third block, the regression model was found to be significant, F(5, 94) = 13.51, p < .001, and this model accounted for 42% of the variance of acculturative stress. Specifically, international students with higher foreigner-based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress, t(98) = 7.58, p < .001. The covariate of gender in the third block was also found to be marginally significant, t(98) = 2.40, p = .02, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. When social support from other international students was added in the fourth block, the regression model continued to be significant, F(6, 93) = 11.58, p < .001, and the overall variance explained by the model increased slightly to 43%. Foreigner-based discrimination in the fourth block continued to be significant, t(98) = 7.43, p < .001, providing support that international students with higher foreigner-based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress. The covariate of gender in the fourth block was also 85 found to be significant, t(98) = 2.24, p = .03, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. However, the covariates of age and length of U.S. residency, and ethnicity and social support from other international students in the fourth block were not found to be significant. With the interaction between foreigner-based discrimination and social support from other international students being entered in the fifth block, the overall model continued to be significant, F(7,92) = 10.36, p < .001, and the overall explained variance increased to 44%. Specifically, international students with higher social support from other international students reported lower acculturative stress, t(98) = 1.81, p = .07. The covariate of gender in the fifth block was also found to be significant, t(98) = 2.34, p = .02, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. However, the covariates of age and length of U.S. residency, ethnicity, foreigner-based discrimination, and the interaction between foreigner-based discrimination and social support from other international students in the fifth block were not found to be significant. The results of this regression model are reported in Table 9. Results for the ninth hierarchical multiple regression analysis indicated that the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be significant in the first block. Similarly, ethnicity and the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be predictors of acculturative stress in the second block. However, when foreigner-based discrimination was entered in the third block, the regression model was found to be significant, F(5, 94) = 13.51, p < .001, and this model 86 accounted for 42% of the variance of acculturative stress. Specifically, international students with higher foreigner-based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress, t(98) = 7.59, p < .001. The covariate of gender in the third block was also found to be significant, t(98) = 2.40, p = .02, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. When social support from other American friends was added in the fourth block, the regression model continued to be significant, F(6, 93) = 11.67, p < .001, and the overall variance explained by the model increased to 43%. Foreigner-based discrimination in the fourth block continued to be significant, t(98) = 6.76, p < .001, providing support that international students with higher foreigner-based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress. Also, the covariate of gender in the fourth block continued to be significant, t(98) = 2.41, p = .02, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. However, the covariates of age and length of U.S. residency, ethnicity, and social support from American friends in the fourth block were not found to be significant. With the interaction between foreigner-based discrimination and social support from American friends being entered in the fifth block, the overall model continued to be significant, F(7,92) = 10.48, p < .001, and the overall explained variance increased to 44.4%. Specifically, international students with social support from American friends reported lower acculturative stress, t(98) = 1.90, p = .06. The covariate of gender in the fifth block continued to be significant, t(98) = 2.43, p = .02, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international 87 students. However, the covariates of age and length of U.S. residency, ethnicity, foreigner-based discrimination, and the interaction between foreigner-based discrimination and social support from American friends in the fifth block were not found to be significant. The results of this regression model are reported in Table 10. Results for the tenth hierarchical multiple regression analysis indicated that the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be significant in the first block. Similarly, ethnicity and the covariates of age, gender, and length of U.S. residency were not found to be predictors of acculturative stress in the second block. However, when foreigner-based discrimination was entered in the third block, the regression model was found to be significant, F(5, 94) = 13.51, p < .001, and this model accounted for 42% of the variance of acculturative stress. Specifically, international students with higher foreigner-based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress, t(98) = 7.59, p < .001. The covariate of gender in the third block was also found to be marginally significant, t(98) = 2.40, p = .02, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. When social support from online ethnic support groups was added in the fourth block, the regression model continued to be significant, F(6, 93) = 11.32, p < .001, and the overall variance explained by the model (42%) did not increase significantly. Foreigner-based discrimination in the fourth block continued to be significant, t(98) = 7.48, p < .001, providing support that international students with higher foreigner-based discrimination reported higher acculturative stress. The covariate of gender in the fourth block was also found to significant, t(98) = 2.21, p = .03, providing support that female 88 international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. However, the covariates of age and length of U.S. residency, ethnicity, and social support from online ethnic support groups in the fourth block were not found to be significant. With the interaction between foreigner-based discrimination and social support from online ethnic support groups being entered in the fifth block, the overall model continued to be significant, F(7,92) = 10.45, p < .001, and the overall explained variance increased to 44%. The interaction between foreigner-based discrimination and social support from online ethnic support groups in the fifth block was found to be marginally significant, t(98) = 1.86, p = .07, providing support that social support from online ethnic support groups moderate the effects of foreigner-based discrimination on acculturative stress. Specifically, international students who reported higher levels of foreigner-based perceived discrimination and who reported higher levels of social support from online ethnic groups, experienced lower levels acculturative stress. Also, social support from online ethnic support groups in the fifth block was found to be marginally significant, t(98) = 1.70, p = .05, providing support that international students with higher social support from online ethnic support groups reported lower acculturative stress. In addition, the covariate of gender in the fifth block was also found to be marginally significant, t(98) = 2.08, p = .04, providing support that female international students reported higher acculturative stress than male international students. However, the covariates of age and length of U.S. residency, ethnicity, and foreigner-based 89 discrimination in the fifth block were not found to be significant. The results of this regression model are reported in Table 11. Table 7 Multiple Hierarchical Regression Results with Interaction between Social Support from Family in Home Country and Perceived Discrimination (Race/Ethnicity-Based and Foreigner-Based) Predicting Acculturative Stress Acculturative stress Variables B SE Acculturative stress β Step 1 Variables B SE β Step 1 Gender 0.05 0.05 0.11 Gender 0.05 0.05 0.11 Age 0.01 0.05 0.01 Age 0.01 0.05 0.01 Length of U.S. residency 0.06 0.05 0.12 Length of U.S. residency 0.06 0.05 0.12 R² 0.03 R² Step 2 0.03 Step 2 Gender 0.06 0.05 0.14 Gender 0.06 0.05 0.14 Age -0.00 0.05 -0.00 Age -0.00 0.05 -0.00 Length of U.S residency 0.06 0.05 0.12 Length of U.S. residency 0.06 0.05 0.12 90 Ethnicity R² -0.12 0.07 -0.18 0.06 Ethnicity R² Step 3 -0.12 0.07 -0.18 0.06 Step 3 Gender† 0.08 0.04 0.17 Gender* 0.09 0.04 0.19 Age -0.01 0.04 -0.03 Age -0.02 0.04 -0.05 Length of U.S residency 0.01 0.04 0.03 Length of U.S. residency -0.01 0.04 -0.02 Ethnicity -0.08 0.06 -0.12 Ethnicity -0.06 0.05 -0.09 Race/ethnicity-based -0.25 0.04 -0.56 Foreigner-based -0.28 0.04 -0.63 discrimination*** R² discrimination*** 0.36 R² Step 4 0.42 Step 4 Gender† 0.06 0.04 0.14 Gender† 0.07 0.04 0.16 Age -0.01 0.04 -0.03 Age -0.02 0.04 -0.05 Length of U.S residency -0.00 0.04 -0.00 Length of U.S. residency -0.02 0.04 -0.05 91 Ethnicity -0.09 0.06 -0.14 Ethnicity -0.07 0.05 -0.11 Race/ethnicity-based -0.24 0.04 -0.55 Foreigner-based -0.28 0.04 -0.63 -0.10 0.04 -0.22 discrimination*** Social support family in discrimination*** -0.09 0.04 -0.21 Social support family in home country* R² home country** 0.40 R² Step 5 0.46 Step 5 Gender 0.06 0.04 0.13 Gender † 0.07 0.04 0.16 Age -0.02 0.04 -0.03 Age -0.02 0.04 -0.05 Length of U.S residency 0.00 0.04 0.00 Length of U.S. residency -0.02 0.04 -0.05 Ethnicity -0.10 0.06 -0.14 Ethnicity -0.07 0.06 -0.11 Race/ethnicity-based -0.18 0.25 -0.41 Foreigner-based -0.29 0.23 -0.65 discrimination discrimination 92 Social support family in -0.04 0.20 -0.09 Social support family in home country Race/ethnicity-based R² -0.11 0.20 -0.24 0.02 0.31 0.35 home country -0.09 0.33 -0.19 Foreigner-based discrimination × Social discrimination × Social support family in home support family in home country country 0.40 R² 0.46 Note: Gender was re-coded as male = -1, female = 1. Ethnicity was re-coded as non-European = -1, European = 1. † p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. 93 Table 8 Multiple Hierarchical Regression Results with Interaction between Social Support from Co-nationals and Perceived Discrimination (Race/Ethnicity-Based and Foreigner-Based) Predicting Acculturative Stress Acculturative stress Variables B SE Acculturative stress β Step 1 Variables B SE β Step 1 Gender 0.05 0.05 0.11 Gender 0.05 0.05 0.11 Age 0.01 0.05 0.01 Age 0.01 0.05 0.01 Length of U.S. residency 0.06 0.05 0.12 Length of U.S. residency 0.06 0.05 0.12 R² 0.03 R² Step 2 0.03 Step 2 Gender 0.06 0.05 0.14 Gender 0.06 0.05 0.14 Age -0.00 0.05 -0.00 Age -0.00 0.05 -0.00 Length of U.S residency 0.06 0.05 0.12 Length of U.S. residency 0.06 0.05 0.12 94 Ethnicity R² -0.12 0.07 -0.18 0.06 Ethnicity R² Step 3 -0.12 0.07 -0.18 0.06 Step 3 Gender† 0.08 0.04 0.17 Gender* 0.09 0.04 0.19 Age -0.01 0.04 -0.03 Age -0.02 0.04 -0.05 Length of U.S residency 0.01 0.04 0.03 Length of U.S. residency -0.01 0.04 -0.02 Ethnicity -0.08 0.06 -0.12 Ethnicity -0.06 0.05 -0.09 Race/ethnicity-based -0.25 0.04 -0.56 Foreigner-based -0.28 0.04 -0.63 discrimination*** R² discrimination*** 0.36 R² Step 4 0.42 Step 4 Gender† 0.08 0.04 0.17 Gender* 0.09 0.04 0.19 Age -0.01 0.04 -0.03 Age -0.02 0.04 -0.05 Length of U.S residency 0.01 0.04 0.03 Length of U.S. residency -0.01 0.04 -0.02 95 Ethnicity -0.08 0.06 -0.11 Ethnicity -0.06 0.06 -0.09 Race/ethnicity-based -0.25 0.04 -0.56 Foreigner-based -0.28 0.04 -0.63 0.00 0.04 0.00 discrimination*** Social support co- discrimination*** 0.01 0.04 0.02 Social support co- nationals R² nationals 0.36 R² Step 5 0.42 Step 5 Gender† 0.08 0.04 0.17 Gender* 0.09 0.04 0.19 Age -0.01 0.04 -0.03 Age -0.02 0.04 -0.04 Length of U.S residency 0.01 0.04 0.03 Length of U.S. residency -0.01 0.04 -0.02 Ethnicity -0.08 0.06 -0.12 Ethnicity -0.07 0.06 -0.10 Race/ethnicity-based -0.22 0.14 -0.50 Foreigner-based -0.17 0.12 -0.40 discrimination discrimination 96 Social support co- 0.05 0.22 0.12 Social support co- nationals Race/ethnicity-based 0.20 0.39 -0.22 0.25 -0.48 nationals -0.06 0.28 -0.13 Foreigner-based discrimination × Social discrimination × Social support co-nationals support co-nationals R² 0.17 0.36 R² 0.42 Note: Gender was re-coded as male = -1, female = 1. Ethnicity was re-coded as non-European = -1, European = 1. †p < .10. *p < .05. ***p < .001. 97 Table 9 Multiple Hierarchical Regression Results with Interaction between Social Support from Other International Students and Perceived Discrimination (Race/Ethnicity-Based and Foreigner-Based) Predicting Acculturative Stress Acculturative stress Variables B SE Acculturative stress β Step 1 Variables B SE β Step 1 Gender 0.05 0.05 0.11 Gender 0.05 0.05 0.11 Age 0.01 0.05 0.01 Age 0.01 0.05 0.01 Length of U.S. residency 0.06 0.05 0.12 Length of U.S. residency 0.06 0.05 0.12 R² 0.03 R² Step 2 0.03 Step 2 Gender 0.06 0.05 0.14 Gender 0.06 0.05 0.14 Age -0.00 0.05 -0.00 Age -0.00 0.05 -0.00 Length of U.S residency 0.06 0.05 0.12 Length of U.S. residency 0.06 0.05 0.12 98 Ethnicity R² -0.12 0.07 -0.18 0.06 Ethnicity R² Step 3 -0.12 0.07 -0.18 0.06 Step 3 Gender† 0.08 0.04 0.17 Gender* 0.09 0.04 0.19 Age -0.01 0.04 -0.03 Age -0.02 0.04 -0.05 Length of U.S residency 0.01 0.04 0.03 Length of U.S. residency -0.01 0.04 -0.02 Ethnicity -0.08 0.06 -0.12 Ethnicity -0.06 0.05 -0.09 Race/ethnicity-based -0.25 0.04 -0.56 Foreigner-based -0.28 0.04 -0.63 discrimination*** R² discrimination*** 0.36 R² Step 4 0.42 Step 4 Gender† 0.07 0.04 0.15 Gender* 0.08 0.04 0.18 Age -0.02 0.04 -0.05 Age -0.03 0.04 -0.06 Length of U.S residency 0.02 0.04 0.05 Length of U.S. residency 0.00 0.04 0.00 99 Ethnicity -0.09 0.06 -0.13 Ethnicity -0.07 0.06 -0.10 Race/ethnicity-based -0.24 0.04 -0.55 Foreigner-based -0.27 0.04 -0.62 -0.05 0.04 -0.10 discrimination*** Social support other discrimination*** -0.05 0.04 -0.12 Social support other international students R² international students 0.37 R² Step 5 0.43 Step 5 Gender† 0.07 0.04 0.15 Gender* 0.09 0.04 0.19 Age -0.03 0.04 0.05 Age -0.02 0.04 -0.03 Length of U.S residency 0.02 0.04 0.05 Length of U.S. residency -0.01 0.04 -0.02 Ethnicity -0.08 0.06 -0.12 Ethnicity -0.08 0.06 -0.13 Race/ethnicity-based -0.34 0.12 -0.77 Foreigner-based -0.11 0.12 -0.24 discrimination** discrimination 100 Social support other -0.22 0.19 -0.50 Social support other R² 0.22 0.61 -0.37 0.25 -0.83 international students† international students Race/ethnicity-based 0.27 0.20 0.23 0.45 Foreigner-based discrimination × Social discrimination × Social support other international support other international students students 0.38 R² 0.44 Note: Gender was re-coded as male = -1, female = 1. Ethnicity was re-coded as non-European = -1, European = 1. †p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. 101 Table 10 Multiple Hierarchical Regression Results with Interaction between Social Support from American Friends and Perceived Discrimination (Race/Ethnicity-Based and Foreigner-Based) Predicting Acculturative Stress Acculturative stress Variables B SE Acculturative stress β Step 1 Variables B SE β Step 1 Gender 0.05 0.05 0.11 Gender 0.05 0.05 0.11 Age 0.01 0.05 0.01 Age 0.01 0.05 0.01 Length of U.S. residency 0.06 0.05 0.12 Length of U.S. residency 0.06 0.05 0.12 R² 0.03 R² Step 2 0.03 Step 2 Gender 0.06 0.05 0.14 Gender 0.06 0.05 0.14 Age -0.00 0.05 -0.00 Age -0.00 0.05 -0.00 Length of U.S residency 0.06 0.05 0.12 Length of U.S. residency 0.06 0.05 0.12 102 Ethnicity R² -0.12 0.07 -0.18 0.06 Ethnicity R² Step 3 -0.12 0.07 -0.18 0.06 Step 3 Gender† 0.08 0.04 0.17 Gender* 0.09 0.04 0.19 Age -0.01 0.04 -0.03 Age -0.02 0.04 -0.05 Length of U.S residency 0.01 0.04 0.03 Length of U.S. residency -0.01 0.04 -0.02 Ethnicity -0.08 0.06 -0.12 Ethnicity -0.06 0.05 -0.09 Race/ethnicity-based -0.25 0.04 -0.56 Foreigner-based -0.28 0.04 -0.63 discrimination*** R² discrimination*** 0.36 R² Step 4 0.42 Step 4 Gender* 0.08 0.04 0.17 Gender* 0.09 0.04 0.19 Age -0.03 0.04 -0.05 Age -0.03 0.04 -0.06 Length of U.S residency 0.02 0.04 0.05 Length of U.S. residency 0.00 0.04 0.00 103 Ethnicity -0.08 0.06 -0.11 Ethnicity -0.06 0.05 -0.09 Race/ethnicity-based -0.23 0.04 -0.52 Foreigner-based -0.26 0.04 -0.59 -0.05 0.04 -0.12 discrimination*** Social support American discrimination*** -0.08 0.04 -0.17 Social support American friends† R² friends .038 R² Step 5 0.43 Step 5 Gender* 0.08 0.04 0.17 Gender* 0.09 0.04 0.19 Age -0.02 0.04 -0.05 Age -0.02 0.04 -0.05 Length of U.S residency 0.01 0.04 0.02 Length of U.S. residency -0.03 0.04 -0.08 Ethnicity -0.07 0.06 -0.11 Ethnicity -0.06 0.05 -0.09 Race/ethnicity-based -0.39 0.13 -0.88 Foreigner-based -0.09 0.12 -0.21 discrimination** discrimination 104 Social support American -0.32 0.19 -0.70 Social support American R² 0.21 0.58 -0.41 0.27 -0.90 friends† friends Race/ethnicity-based 0.26 0.33 0.25 0.72 Foreigner-based discrimination × Social discrimination × Social support American friends support American friends 0.40 R² 0.44 Note: Gender was re-coded as male = -1, female = 1. Ethnicity was re-coded as non-European = -1, European = 1. † p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. 105 Table 11 Multiple Hierarchical Regression Results with Interaction between Social Support from Online Ethnic Support Groups and Perceived Discrimination (Race/Ethnicity-Based and Foreigner-Based) Predicting Acculturative Stress Acculturative stress Variables B SE Acculturative stress β Step 1 Variables B SE β Step 1 Gender 0.05 0.05 0.11 Gender 0.05 0.05 0.11 Age 0.01 0.05 0.01 Age 0.01 0.05 0.01 Length of U.S. residency 0.06 0.05 0.12 Length of U.S. residency 0.06 0.05 0.12 R² 0.03 R² Step 2 0.03 Step 2 Gender 0.06 0.05 0.14 Gender 0.06 0.05 0.14 Age -0.00 0.05 -0.00 Age -0.00 0.05 -0.00 Length of U.S residency 0.06 0.05 0.12 Length of U.S. residency 0.06 0.05 0.12 106 Ethnicity R² -0.12 0.07 -0.18 0.06 Ethnicity R² Step 3 -0.12 0.07 -0.18 0.06 Step 3 Gender† 0.08 0.04 0.17 Gender* 0.09 0.04 0.19 Age -0.01 0.04 -0.03 Age -0.02 0.04 -0.05 Length of U.S residency 0.01 0.04 0.03 Length of U.S. residency -0.01 0.04 -0.02 Ethnicity -0.08 0.06 -0.12 Ethnicity -0.06 0.05 -0.09 Race/ethnicity-based -0.25 0.04 -0.56 Foreigner-based -0.28 0.04 -0.63 discrimination*** R² discrimination*** 0.36 R² Step 4 0.42 Step 4 Gender 0.07 0.04 0.15 Gender* 0.08 0.04 0.18 Age -0.02 0.04 -0.05 Age -0.03 0.04 -0.05 Length of U.S residency 0.02 0.04 0.05 Length of U.S. residency -0.00 0.04 -0.00 107 Ethnicity -0.09 0.06 -0.13 Ethnicity -0.06 0.06 -0.09 Race/ethnicity-based -0.25 0.04 -0.56 Foreigner-based -0.28 0.04 -0.62 -0.03 0.08 -0.07 discrimination*** Social support online discrimination*** 0.06 0.04 0.13 Social support online ethnic support groups R² ethnic support groups 0.37 R² Step 5 0.42 Step 5 Gender† 0.07 0.04 0.16 Gender* 0.08 0.04 0.17 Age -0.01 0.04 -0.03 Age -0.03 0.04 -0.07 Length of U.S residency 0.02 0.04 0.03 Length of U.S. residency 0.00 0.04 0.00 Ethnicity -0.05 0.06 -0.07 Ethnicity -0.09 0.06 -0.14 Race/ethnicity-based -0.45 0.09 -1.03 Foreigner-based -0.10 0.10 -0.22 discrimination*** discrimination 108 Social support online -0.43 0.15 -1.00 Social support online R² 0.22 0.82 -0.45 0.24 -1.00 ethnic support groups† ethnic support groups** Race/ethnicity-based 0.37 0.42 0.16 1.00 Foreigner-based discrimination × Social discrimination × Social support online ethnic support online ethnic support groups** support groups† 0.42 R² 0.44 Note: Gender was re-coded as male = -1, female = 1. Ethnicity was re-coded as non-European = -1, European = 1. † p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. 109 110 Chapter 4 DISCUSSION Summary and Interpretations of Findings The results of the present study partially supported the proposed hypotheses. In the first hypothesis, marginally significant ethnic differences were found in the levels of acculturative stress. Specifically, non-European international students reported higher levels of acculturative stress than European international students. However, the mean scores for acculturative stress for European and non-European international students’ levels of acculturative stress were found to be very low, suggesting both ethnic groups experienced very little acculturative stress. Nonetheless, the mean scores for acculturative stress reported in the present study were found to be higher than previous findings (Lee et al., 2004; Misra et al., 2003; Yeh & Inose, 2003). For example, the mean scores reported in the present study were found to be 0.49 points higher than what the mean scores were reported in Misra et al.’s (2003) study, suggesting international students in the present study reported higher levels of acculturative stress as compared to international students in these studies. It is important to note that some of these studies used modified rating scales on the ILS to increase the variability in acculturative stress scores (Lee et al., 2004; Misra et al., 2003). For example, Lee at al. (2004) modified the rating scale from 0 (never) to 3 (often) to 0 (never) to 4 (very often). Hence, the mean scores of the present study were adjusted accordingly in attempting to determine if the current mean scores were similar or different to the mean scores reported in these previous studies. The findings of the 111 present study provided marginal support of the results in previous research that showed non-European international students experienced higher levels of acculturative stress than European international students (Constantine et al., 2004; Poyrazli et al., 2004; Yeh & Inose, 2003). The current study also found that international students in the present study reported higher levels of acculturative stress than international students in previous studies (Lee et al., 2004; Misra et al., 2003; Yeh & Inose, 2003). When exploring the second hypothesis, no significant differences in the levels of race/ethnicity-based and foreigner-based discrimination were found between nonEuropean and European international students. The mean scores for race/ethnicity-based and foreigner-based discrimination for both European and non-European international students were found to be very low, suggesting both non-European and European international students experienced very low levels of perceived discrimination (race/ethnicity-based and foreigner-based). Corresponding to previous findings, the expectation that non-European international students would report higher levels of race/ethnicity-based discrimination was not supported. In addition, the findings did not support that non-European international students experienced higher levels of foreignerbased discrimination when compared to European international students. Ethnic differences in foreigner-based discrimination were not explored in previous studies and the assumption that non-European international students will report higher levels of foreigner-based discrimination than European international students was based on previous research which focused specifically on race/ethnicity-based discrimination in international students (Poyrazli & Lopez, 2007; Sodowsky & Plake, 112 1992). Also, the mean scores for race/ethnicity-based discrimination in the current study were lower than the reported mean scores for race/ethnicity-based discrimination in previous research (Porazli & Lopez, 2007). While exploring the relationship between length of U.S. residency and discrimination (race/ethnicity and foreigner-based), it was found that length of U.S. residency was not significantly related to race/ethnicity-based discrimination. In contrast, length of U.S. residency was positively related to foreigner-based discrimination. Specifically, international students who lived in the U.S. for a longer period of time reported higher levels of foreigner-based discrimination than international students who lived in the U.S. for a shorter duration. These findings were slightly different from what was proposed in the hypothesis and reported in previous research, that international students who lived longer in the U.S. reported higher levels of race/ethnicity-based discrimination (Poyrazli & Lopez, 2007). It is important to note that prior research used a single dimension of perceived discrimination (race/ethnicity-based) in investigating acculturation-related difficulties in international students. The current study attempted to expand on and investigate broader aspects of perceived discrimination (race/ethnicity and foreigner-based). Thus, it is possible the addition of foreigner-based discrimination in this study may have contributed to different findings than what was reported in past research. It may be possible that the longer non-European international students live in the U.S., the more salient their different English accents and limited English proficiency become in identifying their foreigner-status in the U.S. For example, Lee and Rice (2007) described that experiences 113 of discrimination in international students can be attributed to their foreign-status, limited English proficiency, different English accents, and lack of familiarity with the host culture’s rules and norms. These results show some support that foreigner-based discrimination should be explored separately in future studies. The analysis conducted to explore the third hypothesis revealed no significant ethnic differences in the levels of five sources of social support (family in home country, co-nationals, other international students, American friends, and online ethnic support groups). That is, non-European and European international students did not differ significantly in their reported levels of social support from these five sources (family in home country, co-nationals, other international students, American friends, and online ethnic support groups). This finding was directly contradictory to the proposed hypothesis as it was expected that non-European international students would report higher levels of social support from family in home country, co-nationals, and online ethnic support groups and lower levels of social support from other international students and American friends. However, the mean scores for both European and non-European international students on social support from family in home country were found to be very high, suggesting both these ethnic groups received social support from family in home country. In addition, the mean scores for both European and non-European international students on the other sources of social support (other international students, American friends, online ethnic support groups, and co-nationals) were found to be low, suggesting both these ethnic groups were received lower levels of social support from these sources. This finding is consistent with previous research that suggested 114 international students received low or inadequate amounts of social support from American friends (Gareis, 2012). The study by Gareis (2012) revealed that East Asian international students received lower amounts of social support from American friends than international students from English-speaking countries. Although the findings of the present study did not find any significant ethnic differences in the levels of the five sources of social support (family in home country, co-nationals, other international students, American friends, and online ethnic support groups) between non-European and European international students, these findings lend some support to previous findings that indicated international students received higher social support from family in home country (Misra et al., 2004; Lee et al., 2004; Ye, 2006a). While exploring the fourth hypothesis, a significant relationship between discrimination (foreigner-based and race/ethnicity-based) and acculturative stress was found after controlling for the covariates of age, gender, length of U.S. residency, and ethnicity. Specifically, international students who reported higher levels of both foreigner-based and race/ethnicity-based discrimination reported higher levels of acculturative stress. These results are consistent with previous research that indicated that race/ethnicity discrimination was positively related to acculturative stress in international students (Chavajay & Skowronek, 2008; Sandhu & Asrabadi, 1994; Sullivan, 2011). However, these studies did not explore foreigner-based discrimination. Nonetheless, as expected and described in the proposed hypothesis, the findings of this study supported that both race/ethnicity-based and foreigner-based discrimination were positively related to acculturative stress. 115 Furthermore, this finding reported that foreigner-based discrimination accounted for a higher amount of explained variance (42%) in acculturative stress in international students than race/ethnicity-based discrimination (36%). As shown in Table 5, the pattern between perceived discrimination (foreigner-based and race/ethnicity-based) and acculturative stress clearly indicates that while both types of discrimination are positively related to acculturative stress in international students, foreigner-based discrimination is the stronger predictor of acculturative stress in international students than race/ethnicitybased discrimination. However, the mean scores for both race/ethnicity-based and foreigner-based perceived discrimination were found to be very low, suggesting international students experienced very low levels of race/ethnicity-based and foreignerbased discrimination. Despite relatively low rates of reported discrimination, the variability for these scores was related to significant changes in the reporting of acculturative stress, suggesting international students experienced significant levels of acculturative stress due to race/ethnicity-based and foreigner-based discrimination. This finding was supportive of previous research that reported that race/ethnicity-based discrimination was related to acculturative stress in international students (Sandhu & Asrabadi, 1994). These findings show some support for examining both race/ethnicity-based and foreigner-based discrimination and their relationship to acculturative stress in future studies. In the next set of hypothesis, the relationship between the sources of social support (family in home country, co-nationals, other international students, American friends, and online ethnic support groups) and acculturative stress was explored after 116 controlling for the covariates of gender, age, ethnicity, and length of U.S. residency. In spite of the correlational relationship that was hypothesized between the five sources of social support (family in home country, co-nationals, other international students, American friends, and other ethnic support groups) and acculturative stress, it was interesting to note that only social support from family in home country and American friends were associated with statistically significant lower levels of acculturative stress in international students. When the mean scores of the five sources of social support (family in home country, co-nationals, other international students, American friends, and online ethnic support groups) were compared, social support from family in home country had the highest mean scores, suggesting international students received the highest amount of social support from family in home country. However, the significant findings suggested that while both social support from family in home country and American friends were negatively related to acculturative stress, social support from American friends was a stronger predictor of acculturative stress in international students. These findings clearly indicated that despite a relatively low rate of reported social support from American friends, it was significantly related to acculturative stress in international students. These findings are consistent with prior research that reported that higher levels of social support from American friends and family in home country were related to lower levels of acculturative stress in international students (Chen et al., 2002; Misra et al., 2003; Lee at al., 2004; Poyrazli et al., 2004). However, contrary to what was hypothesized, no significant relationship was found between social support 117 with co-nationals, other international students, and online ethnic support groups and acculturative stress in international students. These findings clearly show social support from American friends and family in home country provide a significant role in dealing with the difficulties involved in adjusting to life and culture in the U.S. These findings show some support for examining these two sources of social support in future studies. While investigating the sixth hypothesis that social support (family in home country, co-nationals, other international students, American friends, and online ethnic support groups) moderates the relationship between perceived discrimination (race/ethnicity and foreigner-based) and acculturative stress. The results suggest that higher social support from online ethnic support groups moderated the negative effects of both race/ethnicity-based and foreigner-based discrimination on acculturative stress in international students, suggesting that international students who reported higher levels of perceived discrimination (race/ethnicity-based and foreigner-based) and who reported higher levels of social support from online ethnic groups, experienced lower levels acculturative stress. This finding is consistent with the proposition by prior studies that social support serves as a moderator between a stressor and acculturative stress in international students (Chen et al., 2002; Jou & Foukada, 1997; Lee et al., 2004; Mallinckrodt & Leong, 1992). Furthermore, the results of this analysis revealed that race/ethnicity-based and foreigner-based discrimination were the strongest predictors of acculturative stress. In addition, both social support from family in home country and American friends were 118 found to be significant, but lesser predictors of acculturative stress in international students. Another important finding in this hypothesis indicated that when interactions between race/ethnicity-based discrimination and social support from family in home country and co-nationals were added, the effect of race/ethnicity-based discrimination on acculturative stress was eliminated. Similarly, when interactions between foreignerbased discrimination and social support from family in home country, co-nationals, American friends, other international students, and online ethnic support groups were added, the effect of foreigner-based discrimination on acculturative stress was eliminated. Likewise, interactions between perceived discrimination (race/ethnicity and foreignerbased) and social support (family in home country and American friends) eliminated the significant effects of both social support from family in home country and American friends on acculturative stress in international students. In conclusion, when the interactions between perceived discrimination (race/ethnicity and foreigner-based) and social support from family in home country, conationals, other international students, American friends, and online ethnic support groups were added to the regression models, only social support from online ethnic support groups was found to moderate the relationship between perceived discrimination (race/ethnicity-based and foreigner-based) and acculturative stress. This would suggest there was minimal support of moderating effect of social support on the negative impact of race/ethnicity-based and foreigner-based discrimination on acculturative stress for 119 international students in the current study. However, further research is needed to further examine this relationship. Limitations of the Present Study The current study had some limitations that should be considered when interpreting the results. One limitation consists of the generalizability of current sample of international students to the larger international student population in the U.S. The current sample of international students may vary from other international students in universities in the U.S. in a number of ways such as level of education, marital status, and length of U.S. residency. For example, previous studies included a larger number of doctoral and married international students (Duru & Poyrazli, 2007; Poyrazli et al., 2004, Wei et al., 2007). However, the participants in this study were more evenly balanced between undergraduate and graduate international students. In addition, the sample of the present study consisted largely of single international students (96%), and this phenomenon may limit the generalizability of these findings to doctoral or married international students. Another limitation is that even within this institution, the current sample size was relatively small due to a low response rate. Of the approximately 500 international students who received the email invitation to participate in the study, only 154 (30.2%) initially responded and with only 81 (16.2%) provided complete information to be included in the final data analysis. Although the response rate was lower than other studies with international students (Lee et al., 2004; Duru & Poyrazli, 2007; Poyrazli et al., 2004), the reason for such a low rate may be attributed to collecting data from a single 120 campus. Some prior studies had collected data from more than one university or college in the U.S. (Duru & Poyrazli, 2007; Poyrazli et al., 2004). For example, in their study with Turkish international students, Duru and Poyrazli (2007) collected data from 17 campuses in the U.S. Yet, in another study on international students, the data was collected from two different universities and the reported response rate was 15% (Sumer et al., 2008). Nonetheless, it may be beneficial for future studies on international students to collect data from different colleges and universities. Recruiting international students from different universities may help future studies in getting an adequate sample size. Yet another limitation of the current study was the disproportionate low number of European international students (13%). Furthermore, the non-European sample in the present study consisted largely of Asian international students (75%). However, the ethnic distribution in the present study was comparable to the ethnic distribution of international students in previous studies (Misra et al., 2003; Poyrazli et al., 2004, Poyrazli & Lopez, 2007; Poyrazli et al., 2010; Yeh & Inose, 2003). For example, the sample in Poyrazli et al.’s (2010) study consisted of 86% non-European and 14% European international students. In addition, 63% of the non-European sample consisted of Asian international students (Poyrazli et al., 2010). This ethnic distribution is particularly apparent at California State University, Sacramento, where 60% of international students were Asian and 9.8% were European international students (E. D. Merchant, personal communication, October 22, 2012). In addition, the ethnic distribution in the present study is representative of the ethnic distribution of international 121 students in the U.S., where 60% of international students were reported to be Asian and 5.9% were European international students (IIE, 2011). Another limitation may lie in the use of the interpersonal stress subscale of the acculturative stress scale (ILS) in the present study. This acculturative subscale was included five items that were related to race/ethnicity-based and foreigner-based discrimination. These items were not removed from the scale even though two of the predictor variables were race/ethnicity-based discrimination and foreigner-based discrimination. It was assumed that the removal of these discrimination items from the ILS may possibly affect the psychometric properties and the integrity of the original scale. Hence, a decision was made to use the ILS with all five subscales intact in the present study. It may be possible the significant relationship between perceived discrimination (race/ethnicity-based and foreigner-based) and acculturative stress in the study may have resulted from a high correlation between both the ILS and discrimination scales. However, a post hoc analysis conducted without the discrimination items in the ILS resulted in similar findings. These findings showed that race/ethnicity-based discrimination was still significantly related to acculturative stress, F(5, 94) = 6.89, p < .001, and accounted for 27% of the variance in acculturative stress. Similarly, foreignerbased discrimination was still significantly related to acculturative stress, F(5, 94) = 7.51, p < .001, and accounted for 29% of the variance in acculturative stress. Implications for Researchers The current study presents a number of implications for researchers and counselors. First, the study established that perceived discrimination is a consistent 122 predictor of acculturative stress in international students in the U.S. This finding supported previous studies that indicated that international students with higher levels of perceived discrimination experienced higher acculturative stress (Chavajay & Skowronek, 2008; Sandhu & Asrabadi, 1994). While previous studies have focused on race/ethnicity-based discrimination, this study attempted to investigate both race/ethnicity-based and foreigner-based discrimination and its relationship to acculturative stress. Although the findings suggested that both types of discrimination were positively related to acculturative stress, foreigner-based discrimination was found to be a stronger predictor of acculturative stress than race/ethnicity based discrimination. Hence, it may be beneficial to continue exploring distinct types of this variable and their relationship to acculturative stress in international students. Second, only marginally significant ethnic differences in the levels of acculturative stress between non-European and European international students in the present study were found. However, the results were comparable to prior studies have reported that non-European international students experience higher acculturative stress than European international students (Constantine et al., 2004; Poyrazli et al., 2004; Yeh & Inose, 2003). It appears the majority of these studies, including the present study, have attempted to understand the possible differences between European and non-European international students’ levels of acculturative stress. However, considering the ethnic distribution in the present study and some other studies (Misra et al., 2003; Poyrazli et al., 2004; Yeh & Inose, 2003), the majority of international student samples tend to consist Asian students. Furthermore, the sub-grouping of the Asian sample in the present study 123 found that the majority of students were Indian and East Asian (e.g., Chinese, Taiwanese). It is possible that international students from one Asian subgroup may differ from international students from another Asian subgroup on their levels of acculturative stress. However, within-group differences between Indian and East Asian international students in relationship to acculturative stress were not tested in the present study as the small sample size prevented such analyses. Future studies may benefit from exploring differences in the levels of acculturative stress between various subgroups of Asian international students. In addition, social support, specifically from family in home country and American friends was found to be negatively related to acculturative stress. Also, social support from online ethnic support groups was found to moderate the effects of perceived discrimination on acculturative stress. However, these findings suggest that social support, particularly from family in home country and American friends, was found to have a significant role to play in the adjustment of international students in the U.S. It may be beneficial to continue examining this variable in future studies. Also, future studies may benefit from including other sources of social support such as online social networking sites as a source of social support to better understand changing patterns of this variable and its relationship to acculturative stress in international students. In addition, future studies may include different types of social support such as informational, practical, and emotional and its relationship to different sources of social support. It may be possible that international students utilize different sources for different types of social support. For example, it may be possible that American friends 124 provide informational support to international and family in home country provides emotional support to international students. The results of this study have some implications for counselors and staff and administrators in the offices of international education in U.S. universities and colleges. One, while prior research has examined the role race/ethnicity-based discrimination in international students’ adjustment in the U.S., the present study suggested that the role of foreigner-based discrimination in international students’ adjustment process should be considered as well. This study found that international students who lived longer in the U.S. reported higher levels of foreigner-based discrimination than international students who lived for a shorter period of time. This information may be used by offices of international offices to increase international students’ awareness of how particular types of discrimination may affect their adjustment at different time points of their stay in the U.S. It may be beneficial to include and communicate this information to newly arrived international students during international students’ orientation programs. In addition, the results of this study showed that social support from family in home country and American friends helped international students in dealing with their acculturative stress. Similar to this finding, a recent study showed that social support from American friends not only helped international students in reducing stress levels, but this also helped with their academic performance and language difficulties (Gareis, 2012). Since family members may not be readily available to international students during their stay in the U.S., it may be beneficial to help international students in building social support networks that are comprised of American students. Specifically, peer 125 programs are needed in which an international student is matched with an American student so that international students’ interactions with American students are promoted. Offices of international education in U.S. universities and colleges can work with other campus offices and departments to develop such peer programs for international students. Future Directions in Research The present study utilized international students from one university and this resulted in a relatively small sample size. Future studies with international students may expand their sample size by utilizing international students from more than one university or institution. Hence, it may be beneficial to contact international student offices of different universities for recruiting international students from their campuses. When collecting data, it may also be beneficial to use alternative methods of data collection such as online surveys, mailing survey packets to international students, and collecting data from international students in different departments at universities and colleges. The examination of race/ethnicity-based discrimination has been shown to be important in studies of acculturative stress and in the present study. In addition, the present study showed the importance of foreigner-based discrimination to acculturative stress in international students. It may be beneficial to continue examining this type of discrimination in future studies. Also, future studies may attempt to explore relevant predictors, such as English accent, lack of English proficiency, and appearance, to better understand the causes of foreigner-based discrimination. In addition, the present study found that length of U.S. residency was related to foreigner-based discrimination. It may be useful to continue exploring this relationship 126 in future studies. Future studies may attempt to explore how foreigner-based discrimination changes over a period of time by using longitudinal designs. Finally, while the present study and previous studies have focused on ethnic differences in acculturative stress, these differences have been limited to Asian and European international students. Since, the majority of sample in these studies and the present study included Asian international students; it may be beneficial to explore ethnic differences between various subgroups of Asian international students such as Indian, East Asian, and South Asian. 127 References Albar Marin, M. J., & Garcia-Ramirez, M. (2005). Social support and emotional exhaustion among hospital nursing staff. The European Journal of Psychiatry, 19, 96-106. American Psychological Association. (2007). APA dictionary of psychology. Washington DC: American Psychological Association. Berry, J. W. (1997). Immigration, acculturation, and adaptation. Applied Psychology, 46(1), 5-34. doi:10.111/j.1464- 0597.1997.tb01087. Berry, J. W., Kim, U., Minde, T., & Mok, D. (1987). Comparative studies of acculturative stress. International Migration Review, 21, 491-511. Bochner, S., McLeod, M. B., & Lin, A. (1977). Friendship patterns of overseas students: Functional model. International Journal of Psychology, 12, 277-294. Cemalcilar, Z., & Falbo, T. (2008). A longitudinal study of the adaptation of international students in the United States. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39(6), 799804. doi:10.1177/0022022108323787 Chavajay, P., & Skowronek, J. (2008). Aspects of acculturation stress among international students attending a university in the USA. Psychological Reports, 103, 827-835. Chen, C. P. (1999). Common stressors among international students: Research and counseling implications. Journal of College Counseling, 2, 49-64. 128 Chen, H-J., Mallinckrodt, B., & Mobley, M. (2002). Attachment patterns of East Asian international students and sources of perceived social support as moderators of the impact of U.S. racism and cultural differences. Asian Journal of Counselling, 9, 27-48. Church, A. (1982). Sojourner adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 540-572. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.91.3.540 Clark, R., Coleman, A. P., & Novak, J. D. (2004). Brief report: Initial psychometric properties of the Everyday Discrimination Scale in black adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 27, 63-368. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.09.004 Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155-159. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155 Constantine, M. G., Anderson, G. M., Berkel, L. A., Caldwell, L., D., & Utsey, S. O. (2005). Examining the cultural adjustment experiences of African international college students: A qualitative analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52(1), 57- 66. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.52.1.57 Constantine, M. G., Okazaki, S., & Utsey, S. O. (2004). Self-concealment, social selfefficacy, acculturative stress, and depression in African, Asian, and Latin American international college students. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 74, 230-241. doi:10.1037/0002-9432.74.3.242 Cotton, S. J., Dollard, M. F., & Jonga, J. (2002). Stress and student job design: Satisfaction, well being, and performance in University students. International Journal of Stress Management, 9, 147-162. doi:10.1023/A:1015515714410 129 Duru, E., & Poyrazli, S. (2007). Personality dimensions, psychosocial-demographic variables, and English language competency in predicting level of acculturative stress among Turkish international students. International Journal of Stress and Management, 14, 99-110. doi:10.1037/1072-5245.14.1.99 Dion, K. L. (2002). The social psychology of perceived prejudice and discrimination. Canadian Psychology, 43, 1-10. doi:10.1037/h0086899 Edwards-Hewitt, T., & Gray, J. J. (1995). Comparison of measures of socioeconomic status between ethnic groups. Psychological Reports, 77, 699-702. Frazier, P. A., Tix, a. P., & Barron, K. E. (2004). Testing moderator and mediator effects in counseling psychology research. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51(1), 115-134. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.51.1.115 Fritz, M. V., Chin, D., & DeMarinis, V. (2008). Stressors, anxiety, acculturation, and adjustment among international and North American students. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 32, 244-259. doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2008.01.001 Gareis, E. (2012). Intercultural friendship: Effects of home and host region. Journal of Intercultural and International Communication, 5, 1-20. doi:10.1080/17513057.2012.691525 Gee, G. C., Spencer, M. S., Chen, J., & Takeuchi, D. (2007). A nationwide study of discrimination and chronic health conditions among Asian Americans. American Journal of Public Health, 97, 1275-1282. 130 Hartshorne, R., & Baucom, J. (2007). Issues affecting cross-cultural adaptation of international graduate students. Multicultural Learning and Teaching, 2, 78-87. doi:10.2202/2161-2412.1023 Hayes, R. L., & Lin, H. (1994). Coming to America: Developing social support systems for international students. Journal of Multicultural Counseling and Development, 22, 7-16. Hechanova-Alampay, R., Beehr, T. A., Christiansen, N. D., & Van Horn, R. K. (2002). Adjustment and strain among domestic and international student sojourners: A longitudinal study. School Psychology International, 23, 458-474. doi:10.1177/0143034302234007 Hollingshead, W. (1979). The Hollingshead Four-Factor Index of Socioeconomic Status. Unpublished manuscript, Yale University, New Haven, CT. Hyun, J., Quinn, B., Madon, T., & Lustig, S. (2007). Mental health need, awareness, and use of counseling services among international graduate students. Journal of American College Health, 56(2), 109-118. Institute of International Education (2011). Open doors IIENTwork org. Retrieved October 28, 2012, from http: //opendoors.iienetwork.org/ Jackson, E. S., Tucker, C. M., & Herman, K. C. (2007). Health value, perceived social support, and health self-efficacy as factors in a health-promoting lifestyle. Journal of American College Health, 56, 69-74. 131 Jackson, J. S., Brown, K. T., & Kirby, D. C. (1998). International perspective on prejudice and racism. In J. L. Eberhardt & S. T. Fiske (Eds.), Confronting Racism: The problem and the response (pp. 101-135). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Jou, Y. H., & Fukada, H. (1997). Stress and social support in mental and physical health of Chinese students in Japan. Psychological Report, 81, 1303-1312. King, S. (1994). Analysis of electronic support groups for recovering addicts. Interpersonal Computing and Technology: An Electronic Journal for the 21st Century, 2, 47-56. Klomegah, R. Y. (2006). Social factors relating to alienation experienced by international students in the United States. College Student Journal, 40(2), 303-315. Krahe, B., Abraham, C., Felber, J., & Helbig, M. K. (2005). Perceived discrimination of international visitors to universities in Germany and the UK. British Journal of Psychology, 96, 263-281. doi:10.1348/000712605X48296 Lee, J., Koeske, G. F., & Sales, E. (2004). Social support buffering of acculturative stress: a study of mental health symptoms among Korean international students. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 28, 399-414. doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2004.08.005 Lee, J. J., & Rice, C. (2007). Welcome to America? International student perceptions of discrimination. Higher Education, 53, 381-408. doi:10.1007/s10734-005-4508-3 Leong, F. T. L., & Chou, E. L. (1996). Counseling international students. In P. B. Pedersen, J. G. Draguns, W. J. Lonner, & J. E. Trimble (Eds.), Counseling across cultures (pp. 210-242). Honolulu: University Press of Hawaii. 132 Lin, J. G., & Yi, J. K. (1997). Asian international students’ adjustment: issues and program suggestions. College Student Journal, 31, 473-479. Mallinckrodt, B., & Leong, F. T. L. (1992). International graduate students, stress, and social support. Journal of College Student Development, 33, 71-77. Mays, V. M., & Cochran, S. (2001). Mental health correlates of perceived discrimination among lesbian, gay, and bisexual adults in the United States. American Journal of Public Health, 91, 1869-1876. doi:10.2105/AJPH.91.11.1869 Miller, M. J. (2007). A bilinear multidimensional measurement model of Asian American acculturation and enculturation: Implications for counseling interventions. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 54, 118-131. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.54.2.118 Misra, R., Crist, M., & Burant, C. J. (2003). Relationships among life stress, social support, academic stressors, and reactions to stressors of international students in the United States. International Journal of Stress Management, 10, 137-157. doi:10.1037/1072-5245.10.2.137 Mori, S. (2000). Addressing the mental health concerns of international students. Journal of Counseling and Development, 7, 137-144. Muller, T., Gragtmans, K., & Baker, R. (2008). Childhood physical abuse, attachment, and adult social support: Test of a mediation model. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 40, 80-89. doi:10.1037/0008-400X.40.2.80 Panter, A. T., Daye, C. E., Allen, W. R., Wightman, L. F., & Deo, M. (2008). Everyday discrimination in a national sample of incoming law students. Journal of Diversity in Higher Education, 2, 67-79. doi:10.1037/1938-8926.1.2.67 133 Poyrazli, S., Kavanaugh, P. R., Baker, A., & Al-Tamimi, N. (2004). Social support and demographic correlates of acculturative stress in international students. Journal of College Counseling, 7, 73-82. Poyrazli, S., & Grahame, K. M. (2007). Barriers to adjustment: Needs of international students within a semi-urban campus community. Journal of Instructional Psychology, 34(1), 28-45. Poyrazli, S., & Lopez, M. D. (2007). An exploratory study of perceived discrimination and homesickness: A comparison of international students and American students. The Journal of Psychology, 141(3), 263-280. Poyrazli, S., Thukral, R.K., & Duru, E. (2010). International students’ race-ethnicity, personality and acculturative stress. Journal of Psychology and Counseling, 34(1), 25-32. Rajajapaksa, S., & Dundes, L. (2002). It’s a long way home: International student adjustment to living in the United States. Journal of College Student Retention: Research, Theory, and Practice, 4, 15-28. Redfield, R., Linton, R., & Herskovits, M. J. (1936). Memorandum for the study of acculturation. American Anthropologist, 38, 149-152. Roberts, C. B., Vines, A. I., Kaufman, J. S., & James, S. A. (2008). Cross-sectional association between perceived discrimination and hypertension in AfricanAmerican men and women: The Pitt County study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 167, 624-632. doi:10.1093/aje/kwm334 134 Ryder, A. G., Alden, L. E., & Paulhus, D. L. (2000). Is acculturation unidimensional or bidimensional? A head-to-head comparison in the prediction of personality, self identity, and adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 4965. doi:10.1037//0O22-3514.79.1.49 Salem, D. A., Bogat, G. A., & Reid, C. (1997). Mutual help goes on line. Journal of Community Psychology, 25(2), 189-207. Sandhu, D. S. (1994). An examination of the psychological needs of the international students: Implications for counselling and psychotherapy. International Journal for the Advancement of Counselling, 17, 229-239. doi:10.1007/BF01407739 Sandhu, D. S., & Asrabadi, B. R. (1994). Development of an acculturative stress scale for international students: Preliminary findings. Psychological Reports, 75, 435-448. Schmitt, M. T., Spears, R., & Branscombe, N. R. (2003). Constructing a minority group identity out of shared rejection: The case of international students. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 1-12. doi:10.1002/ejsp.131 Sodowsky, G. B., & Lai, E. W. M. (1997). Asian immigrant variables and structural models of cross-cultural distress. In A. Booth, A. C. Crouter, & N. Landale (Eds.), Immigration and the family: Research and policy on U.S. immigrants (pp. 211234). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Sodowsky, G. R., & Plake, B. S. (1992). A study of acculturation differences among international people and suggestions for sensitivity to within group differences. Journal of Counseling and Development, 71, 53-59. 135 Sullivan, C. (2011). Predictors of acculturative stress for international students in the United States (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from http://www.csus.edu/ehost/ Sumer, S., Poyrazli, S., & Grahame, K. (2008). Predictors of depression and anxiety among international students. Journal of Counseling and Development, 86, 429437. Swagler, M. A., & Ellis, M. V. (2003). Crossing the distance: Adjustment of Taiwanese graduate students in the United States. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 50(4), 420-437. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.50.4.420 Utsey, S. O., Bolden, M., Lanier, Y., Williams, O., & Lee, A. (2006). Moderator effects of cognitive ability and social support on the relation between race-related stress and quality of life in a community sample of Black Americans. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 12, 334-346. doi:10.1037/1099-9809.12.2.334 Wadsworth, B. C., Hecht, M. L., & Jung, E. (2008). The role of identity gaps, discrimination, and acculturation in international students’ educational satisfaction in American classrooms. Communication Education, 57(1), 64-87. doi:10.1080/03634520701668407 Ward, C., Bochner, S., & Furnham, A. (2001). The psychology of culture shock (2nd ed.) Philadelphia: Routledge. Wei, M., Heppner, P. P., Mallen, M. J., Ku, T. Y., Liao, K. Y. H., & Wu, T. F. (2007). Acculturative stress, perfectionism, years in the United States, and depression among Chinese international students. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 54, 385-394. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.54.4.385 136 Williams, D. R., Yu, Y., Jackson, J., & Anderson, N. (1997). Racial differences in physical and mental health: Socioeconomic status, stress, and discrimination. Journal of Health Psychology, 2, 335-351. doi:10.1177/135910539700200305 Wilton, L., & Constantine, M. G. (2003). Length of residence, cultural adjustment difficulties, and psychological distress symptoms in Asian and Latin American international college students. Journal of College Counseling, 6, 177-186. Winefield, H. R., Winefield, A. H., & Tiggemann, M. (1992). Social support and psychological well-being in young adults: The Multi-Dimensional Support Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 58, 198-210. Yang, B., & Clum, G. A. (1995). Measures of life stress and social support specific to an Asian student population. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 17, 51-67. doi:10.1007/BF02229203 Ye, J. (2005). Acculturative stress and use of the Internet among East Asian international students in the U.S. Cyber Psychology & Behavior, 8, 154-161. doi:10.1089/cpb.2005.8.154 Ye, J. (2006a). An examination of acculturative stress among Chinese international students, social support, and use of online ethnic social groups. Howard Journal of Communication, 17, 1-20. doi:10.1080/10646170500487764 Ye, J. (2006b). Traditional and online support networks in the cross-cultural adaptation of Chinese international students in the United States. Journal of ComputerMediated Communication, 11, 863-876. doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2006.00039.x 137 Yeh, C. J., & Inose, M. (2003). International students’ reported English fluency, social support satisfaction, and social connectedness as predictors of acculturative stress. Counseling Psychology Quarterly, 16, 15-28. doi:10.1080/0951507031000114058 Ying, Y. (2005). Variation in acculturative stressors over time: A study of Taiwanese students in the United States. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 29, 59-71. doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2005.04.003 Ying, Y., Lee, P. A., & Tsai, J. L. (2000). Cultural orientation and racial discrimination: Predictors of coherence in Chinese American young adults. Journal of Community Psychology, 28, 427-442. doi:10.1002/1520-6629(200007)28:4<427::AID-JCOP5>3.0.CO;2-F Zhang, J., & Goodson, P. (2011). Predictors of international students’ psychosocial adjustment to life in the United States: A systematic review. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 35, 139-162. doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2010.11.011