Level 3: structured observation 質性研究 Qualitative Inquiry in TESOL (P.144-160) Ma3c0102 甘蘋 ( Victoria Kan) Level 3 focuses on structured observation. While recognising the dangers involved in this approach, it also highlights the valuable part such observation can play in fieldwork and provides advice on how to decide whether to incorporate it. Practical advice is offered on designing and applying an observation schedule and potential problems are addressed. The level also includes a section on how to calculate inter-observer agreement. (p.105) Level 3 : Structured observation • One of the most important decisions needed to make is the systematic use of pre-determined categories will play in the approach. • Structured observation is sometimes referred to as systematic observation , but this misleadingly implies that participant observation is not systematic. • • Open observation– the early stages of participant observation where the observer tries to get a general sense of the setting and the activities. Closed observation—observer is strictly coding behavior on a low-inference schedule, or instrument. The hidden dangers of closed observation potential trouble spots from four angles: perspective, design, procedure, and interpretation. 1. Perspective • Strength – to allow researcher to focus on those areas and characteristics that they wish to know more about. • Danger—many novice researchers tend to introduce it at the beginning of their projects and without giving proper consideration to the implications of these for the project as a whole. • Wolcott (1994:159) what concerns him is that electing for such a system can serve to conceal more than it reveals… (p.145) 2. Design • Apply Wolcott’s point to consider the issue of labeling. • Strength— with limited ambitions and with a view to deepening the researcher’s understanding of particular features of behavior ,it can serve the project well • Danger-- attempt to capture precise teaching “activities” in a single set of descriptive terms can produce a plethora of labels and a deal of confusion. • Ex: (Adams 1972) took the trouble to survey the many labels in the area of classroom observation and produced a daunting list of available terms. (p.146) 3. Procedure • Strength—the preparation and administration of an observation schedule has something for everyone: the appeal of the systematic for those who enjoy creating order out of chaos, an opportunity to establish fixed routines. • Danger– the range of its attractions makes it a dangerous instrument: “There is a tendency … to generate lots of bits of data. The problem …one of trying to piece them together to produce an overall picture, or one of trying to find general themes that link fragments of data together.” (Bryman 2001 a: 173) 4. Interpretation • • • • Bryman’s observation points to the associated problem of interpretation. Danger-- accumulative potential of structured observation can undermine its analytic purpose. Become swamped with information that has distorted the shape of the original project. Comparison –quantitative representations are left to stand as explanations in themselves; qualitative researcher needs to remember that description, whether in narrative or categorical form, is not the same as interpretation and takes us below the surface of things. (p.148) Participant and Structured Observation Participant observation Structured observation Orientation Open Closed Foundation Event-based Category - Based Form Narrative Descriptive Observer status Coding Observer-asinstrument Post-observation Observer-throughinstrument Pre-observation Recording Retrospective Cotemporaneous Format Notebook Observation schedule Participant observation Structured observation Replicability Non-replicable Replicable QI status Main or supplementary method Supplementary method • The two approaches can make a potent combination. Standard observation schedules • In practice, most researchers develop their own. • The groundbreaking contribution to the structured observation of classrooms was made by Flanders (1970), who developed a ten-category observation schedule, the Flanders Interaction Analysis Categories (FIAC). • Designed specifically to identify direct and indirect teacher influence, its coverage and simplicity broadened its appeal beyond this relatively narrow focus, so that many subsequent systems have their roots here. Presents the main categories in the FIAC system Teacher talk: (a) indirect influence 1. Accepts feeling 2. Praises or encourages 3. Accepts use uses ideas of pupils 4. Asks questions (b) direct influence 5. Lecturing 6. Giving directions 7. Criticising or justifying authority Pupil talk: 8. Pupil talk--response 9. Pupil talk -- initiation 10. Silence or confusion Deciding whether to use structured observation • From “what do I want to find out?” to “What schedule do I use?” • We can use techniques associated with that to generate new ways of seeing that will in turn prompt fresh questions, a vitally important preliminary because the value of the schedule we eventually devise will depend on the quality of the questions we ask. Contribution of an observation schedule (p. 150) Main Research question Subsidiary question Specific data needed Coding scheme + schedule Working up a schedule • Reasons: Qualitative inquiry generally prefers inductive reasoning, which is why structured observation has to be considered in the light of the wider discovery process. • Its design represents a creative challenge but also offers the prospect of a flexible and evolving system that allows for substantial revision at the trialing stage. Practical considerations in approaching structured observation (Evertson & Green,1985) 1. Research question(s) 5. Observation instrument(s) e.g. category system, descriptive system 2. Focus e.g. particular group(s), event, strategy 6. Observation procedures e.g. When? How often? Number of observers? 3. Setting e.g. classroom, staffroom 7. Analytical procedures e.g. frequency count, event structure 4. “ Slice of reality” 8. Presenting of findings Working up a schedule--basic decisions • Should not be a mechanistic one • The most basic decision of all is what behaviors will feature in the observation. • It is possible to cover all behaviors (FIAC) • Focus on specific behaviors or events (ex: teacher questions or group work) • Four choices are important: 1. Descriptive system or category system—depend on the extent to which numerical information is required and whether the relevant behavior can be broken down into a reasonable number of discrete categories. 2. Rating scale or non-rating scale—Rating scales evaluate behavior according to agreed criteria ( ex: high, medium, low) and rating system are suitable for purposes of evaluation. Two systems can be combined. 3. High-inference categories or low-inference categories—The structured observation involves one observer and is essentially descriptive, highinference categories are perfectly acceptable, but if more than one observer is involved lowinference categories are preferable. 4. Event sampling ( event-based coding)or interval sampling (time-based coding)—In interval sampling behaviors are coded at predetermined intervals (e.g. every three seconds). (p.152) Working up a schedule--Illustrations • Based on rating scale--the first example (Extract 3.12) which involves interval sampling but no rating, coding takes place every five seconds. The best way is not to rely on a watch, but to make a tape on which you record a click every five seconds. • Four minutes of coding on a 10-category system (p153) 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55 60 1 2 2 2 6 6 2 2 6 6 6 6 5 2 2 2 10 2 2 6 2 2 2 10 5 5 3 5 3 3 5 3 3 6 6 4 4 4 6 4 2 2 6 6 6 6 6 5 3 6 6 9 Working up a schedule--Illustrations •Interval sampling would involve identifying activities on one axis an time periods on another , as in Extract 3.13. Example of non-rating system ( interval sampling) 5 10 15 TT ST \/ \/ 20 25 \/ \/ \/ 30 \/ 35 40 45 50 55 \/ \/ \/ \/ 60 \/ Sil \/ Oth Extract 3.14 Example of rating system (event sampling) (p154) Feature Delivery Aspects R Clarity 4 Loudness 4 Speed 2 Appropriateness 2 Comments Speed often too fast. Big ability range in class, but T made no allowances when speaking to weaker Ss. Working up a schedule--Defining the units • It is best to think first about higher order categories and then work out lower order definitions. • Considerations can be applied to all structured observation but are designed with category systems. • Within category: 1. Make sure the category is as clear as possible 2. Check that the category is related to observable behavior 3. Be as precise as possible. 4. Consider the range of each category. (p.155) • Among categories: 1. Ensure that all definitions are clear and exclusive 2. Check that there are no gaps in the coverage of chosen behaviors. 3. The schedule must be practicable. Essential characteristics of an effective category system • • • • Clearly definable categories related to observable behavior Mutually exclusive categories—no overlap The category set is exhaustive The system can be operationalised Some practical problems 1. Observer effect: • You should not underestimate the effect that filling in a form • It is possible to hide or disguise the writing or coding • It is not good to rely on a single observation 2. Expectancy effect: • Should reflect very carefully on the initial assumptions • The higher the degree of inference involved in the observation, the more important it is to be aware of factors that might influence coding. 3. Observer drift: • Awareness can help to reduce it and where teams of coders are involved periodic checks on inter-rater agreement can direct attention to emerging problems. 4. Central tendency: • Only to rating scale and refers to the tendency for opt for something at or near the middle. • Keeping options to a minimum can help to reduce it. Calculating inter-observer agreement • In the case, more than one person is involved • Problem—observers will choose a particular code not on the basis of pre-determined criteria • Straightforward method of making allowance for chance agreement that does not depend on a sophisticated grasp of statistics or probability theory. Known as Cohen’s Kappa (1960), it is based on records of agreement and disagreement. (p.157)