Chapter 1: Introduction Chapter Preview The first chapter introduces the study of human development. The first section defines development, briefly describing the how, why, and who of this definition. The second section identifies five characteristics of the life-span perspective on human development and explains different aspects of the overlapping contexts in which people develop. The story of David illustrates the importance of human plasticity. The ecological-systems approach is Bronfenbrenner’s description of how the individual is affected by, and affects, many other individuals, groups of individuals, and larger systems in the environment. The next section discusses the strategies developmentalists use in their research, including scientific observation, experiments, and surveys. To study people over time, developmentalists have created several research designs: crosssectional, longitudinal, and cross-sequential. The final section discusses several common mistakes that can be made in interpreting research, including the mistake of confusing correlation with causation and the ethics of research with humans. In addition to ensuring confidentiality and safety, developmentalists who study children are especially concerned that the benefits of research outweigh the risks. Chapter Guide I. Defining Development 1. The science of human development seeks to understand how and why people—all people, everywhere, of every age— change over time, and how and why they remain the same. 2. Development is multidirectional, multicontextual, multicultural, multidisciplinary, and plastic. 3. The science of human development is empirical, meaning that it focuses on data, facts, observation, and experimentation. 4. The scientific method consists of five basic steps: (1) formulate a research question, (2) develop a hypothesis, (3) test the hypothesis, (4) draw conclusions, and (5) make the findings available. 5. Replication of research findings verifies the findings and leads researchers to more definitive and extensive conclusions. In replicating research, scientists use different participants. 6. The nature–nurture debate is the question of how much of any characteristic is the result of genes (nature) and how much is the result of experience (nurture). Genes and environment affect every aspect of development. 7. The fact that nature and nurture interact helps clarify another question: whether or not timing is crucial. Research has shown that there are both critical periods and sensitive periods in development. 8. The value of understanding that nature and nurture interact also can be seen in research on the origins of violence in young people. One study found that mistreated boys were more likely to be overly aggressive if they had a particular variation in the MAOA gene (the low-MAOA gene). However, even if they inherited the gene that produces low levels of the enzyme monoamine oxidase A, boys who had not been mistreated tended to become peaceable adults. 9. The emphasis on the interaction between people and within each person is highlighted by the dynamic-systems theory, which stresses fluctuations and transitions. II. The Life-Span Perspective 1. The life-span perspective views human development as multidirectional, multicontextual, multicultural, multidisciplinary, and plastic. 2. Change is apparent in each aspect of life and in every direction. Research shows that some shifts are stage-like and other shifts are gradual. 3. The approach that emphasizes the influence of the systems, or contexts, that support the developing person is Urie Bronfenbrenner’s ecological-systems approach. According to this model, human development is supported by systems at four nested levels: the microsystem (immediate social setting), the exosystem (local, such as school and church), the macrosystem (cultural values, political processes, economic policies, and social conditions), and the chronosystem, which emphasizes the importance of historical time on development. A fifth system, the mesosystem, connects systems, for example, the interface between home and school. As a reflection of the impact of biology on development, Bronfenbrenner renamed his theory bioecological theory. 4. Developmentalists take a broad view of development, recognizing the influence on development of external forces, that is, the context of development. This larger perspective makes it imperative that development be understood in its social context, including its historical and socioeconomic contexts. 5. A cohort is a group of people born within a few years of each other who tend to share certain historical events and cultural shifts. 6. Socioeconomic status (SES) is determined by several overlapping variables, including income, wealth, education, place of residence, and occupation. Although low income obviously limits a person, other SES factors (such as education) can make poverty better or worse. 7. Culture affects development in a multitude of interrelated ways, from whether to cover your mouth when laughing to what to eat for breakfast. 8. An ethnic group is a collection of people who share certain attributes, such as ancestry, national origin, religion, and language. 9. Although race was once defined as a biological category, it is actually a social construction, an idea that is built on shared perceptions, not on objective reality, and, unlike genetic differences, social constructions can change. 10. The value of an interdisciplinary approach to understanding human development can be seen in research on mirror neurons, which are brain cells that respond to the observed actions of others. These neurons, which in the human brain reflect not only the movements but also the intentions, sensations, and emotions of those around us, may help explain some autism, language learning, empathy for other people, and how culture is transmitted. 11. One of the most encouraging aspects of the science of development is that development is characterized by plasticity, or the capability of change. III. Using the Scientific Method 1. There are many ways to test hypotheses. One method is scientific observation of people in their natural environment, in a laboratory setting, or in searches of archival data. Observation is limited in that it does not tell us what causes people to do what they do. 2. Experiments can reveal cause-and-effect relationships by allowing experimenters to observe whether a change in an independent variable affects some specific behavior, or dependent variable. In an experiment, the participants who receive a particular treatment constitute the experimental group; the participants who do not receive the treatment constitute the comparison group (control group). 3. The survey is a quick and direct way to obtain data. However, it is especially difficult to get valid data from a survey: Some people may refuse to answer, and others may give answers to make themselves look better. Survey answers also are influenced by the wording and the sequence of the questions. 4. In cross-sectional research, groups of people who are different in age but similar in all other important ways are compared on the characteristic that is of interest to the researcher(s). One limitation of cross-sectional research is that it is always possible that some variable other than age differentiates the groups. 5. In longitudinal research, the same people are studied over a period of time. Longitudinal research is particularly useful in studying developmental trends that occur over a long age span. 6. Both longitudinal and cross-sectional researchers must bear in mind that research on a cohort may not be valid for people developing in an earlier or later cohort. 7. In cross-sequential research, several groups of people at different ages (crosssectional component) are followed over time (longitudinal component). IV. Cautions from Science 1. Correlation is a number indicating the degree of relationship between two variables. A correlation is positive if both variables tend to increase together or decrease together, negative if one variable tends to increase when the other decreases, and zero if no connection is evident. Correlation does not prove causality. 2. Because numbers can be easily summarized and compared, scientists often rely on data produced by quantitative research. This method may be particularly limiting when researchers describe child development. Also, many developmental researchers use qualitative research that asks open-ended questions. 3. When studying people, scientists take special care to ensure that participation is voluntary and harmless and that the study’s benefits outweigh its costs. They do this by establishing a code of ethics for researchers to follow, which is enforced by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) at most educational and medical institutions.