Title: Effects of Whisker-trimming on GABAA receptors in SI Cortex Or in adult barrel cortex CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION Sensory deprivation has been a productive approach to investigate the effects of environmental stimuli on adult and developing brain. Whereas lack of normal excitatory inputs leaves some cortical neurotransmitter systems unaffected (Goodman et al., 1993; Schlaggar et al, 1993), it can lead to down regulation of the gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA)-ergic system. For instance, the cortex of monkeys, cats, and rats shows particular changes in GABAergic components after different types of sensory or visual deprivation. In the rat somatosensory cortex, deprivation of whisker input results in decreases in GABAergic circuitry elements, such as the number and proportion of GABA-immunoreactive synaptic contacts in layer IV (Micheva and Beaulieu, 1995), glutamic acid decarboxylase (GAD) levels (Akhtar and Land, 1991), and muscimol binding to GABAA receptors (Fuchs and Salazar, 1998). Alterations in specific GABA receptors subunits might help understand GABAergic function. This is the first investigation where whisker deprivation regulates specific GABAA receptor subunits. The Whisker to Barrel System of the Rat The rodent somatosensory whisker to barrel pathway has several features that make it a desirable system to study sensory deprivation. For instance, there is a 1:1 topographic relationship 1 between each of the whiskers in the rat’s face and a group of neurons that constitute a ‘barrel’ in layer VI of Somatosensory Cortex (SI) (Woolsey and Van der Loos, 1970): each barrel responds primarily to one principal whisker. This feature enabled the discovery of cytoarchitectonic (Woolsey and Van der Loos, 1970; Welker and Woolsey, 1974; Van der Loos and Woolsey, 1973) and physiological (Welker, 1971, 1976; Simons, 1978; Simons and Woolsey, 1979) effects of whisker stimulation and/or deprivation, and to investigate the experience-dependent maintenance of synapses (Micheva and Beaulieu, 1996), neurotransmitters (Micheva and Beaulieu, 1995), and neurotransmitter receptors (Fuchs, 1995). Moreover, the SI cortex of rats is accessible to surgical procedures, allowing for a variety of chemical, physiological and mechanical preparations and manipulations, such as measurements of changes of GABA receptor subunits after topical cortical blockade of NMDA receptors (Penschuck et al., 1999). Sensory Deprivation and GABA A Receptors Effects of sensory deprivation on GABAergic cortical circuitry have been widely studied. Pioneer studies on the adult monkey’s visual system showed that depriving visual input from one eye results in decreases of both GABA and its synthesizing enzyme GAD in the deprived cortical neurons (Hendry and Jones, 1986). In the SI cortex of adult rodents, similar effects of deprivation have been observed. First of all, GAD is reduced in deprived barrels after trimming whiskers for 6 weeks beginning in the adult, but not beginning in the neonate (Akhtar and Land, 1991). Physiological studies showed that simply trimming rat whiskers leads to signs of disinhibition in the deprived barrel neurons, such as higher spontaneous activity, and a decreased selectivity to respond to specific angles of whisker deflection (Simons and Land, 1987). These physiological changes remain even after allowing neonatally deprived rats to regrow their whiskers for several 2 weeks, indicating the dramatic, long-lasting effect of neonatal deprivation. What is the chemical basis of these physiological changes? Since GABA is the main inhibitory neurotransmitter in cortex, it was important to consider GABA and its receptors as suitable candidates responsible for these physiological changes. Blocking GABAA receptors with the antagonist bicuculline results in signs of cortical disinhibition (Kyriazi et al, 1996) that are similar to those from deprived barrel cortex. Furthermore, binding of the GABA agonist [3H]muscimol, which selectively binds to GABAA receptors, is reduced in the deprived barrels (Fuchs and Salazar, 1998). This effect was observed in both neonatally and adult deprived rats, and was still present even after allowing the rats to grow their whiskers for ten additional weeks after the trimming period. These overall decreases after deprivation were suggested as a down-regulating mechanism that compensates for the reduced sensory input (Fuchs and Salazar, 1998). Recent studies showed that whisker trimming for 2 months, starting at birth, reduced the numerical density of both intracortical and thalamocortical symmetrical synapses, upon inhibitory neurons in barrel neuropil (Sadaka et al., 2003). GABA inhibitory effects in cortex depend on the types of GABA receptors involved. Inhibition through the GABAA receptor, a chloride channel itself, is relatively fast. These receptors, when activated, increase the chloride conductance of the membrane, affecting transmitter release in the presynaptic neuron, and producing hyperpolarization or shunting inhibition of the postsynaptic neuron (MacDonald and Olsen, 1994; Xi and Akasu, 1996). Through the metabotropic GABAB receptor inhibition is slower and involves changes in potassium and calcium permeability at presynaptic and postsynaptic synapses (Misgeld et al., 1995; Howe et al., 1987; Deisz and Prince, 1989; Deisz et al., 1997). 3 GABA Receptor Subunits and Sensory Deprivation GABAA receptors subunits comprise a family of at least 17 subunits (Davies et al., 1997). Each subunit is expressed in a particular laminar pattern in SI and visual cortex (V1). For instance, in SI and V1, the α1 subunit, which is present in the majority of the GABAA receptors, is densest in layers III-IV (Fritschy et al., 1994). This laminar-specific expression might represent distinct functional areas which can be differentially affected by sensory deprivation paradigms. For instance, while monocular intravitreal injection of TTX in monkey induces a reduction in α1, β2, and γ2 subunit mRNAs subunits levels in deprived visual cortex, it leaves levels of α2, α4, and β1 unchanged (Huntsman et al., 1994; Jones, 1997). In focal cortical malformations induced by neonatal freeze lesions of SI, α1 and α5-GABAA subunits decrease in rat SI (Redecker, 2000). Furthermore, electrolytic lesion of thalamus in the newborn decreases α1 in layers III-IV, but increases α2, α3, and α5 in the same SI layers (Paysan, 1997). When whiskers are trimmed during a critical period of early postnatal development, stimulation of the regrown whiskers causes a degraded tuning of layer II/III receptive fields in the corresponding deprived column (Lendvai et al., 2000; Stern et al., 2001). Similarly, plucking whiskers from birth results in weaker responses of neurons in layers II/III and IV of the related barrel column (Fox, 1992, 1994), but causes stronger responses from neighboring barrel columns (Simons and Land, 1987; Fox, 1992, 1994). Similar deprivation effects are recorded in layer II/III and IV barrel neurons after adult deprivation in rats (Glazewski and Fox, 1996; Wallace and Fox, 1999). However, it has not been determined whether there are decreases in GABAergic components of layers II/IIII that could account for the apparent disinhibition in these layers. GABAA receptors decrease in layer IV of deprived barrel neurons (Fuchs and Salazar, 4 1998). These effects might not only be restricted to this layer, but might be present in layers II and III of the same deprived whisker barrel columns. Moreover, changes in GABAA receptor binding, as assessed by autoradiography, might be paralleled by changes in GABAA receptor subunits. A favorable candidate α1-GABAA subunit, since it is the one that predominates in layers II/III of barrel columns ( ), and is constituent of most GABAA receptors ( ). In the present study, to determine whether sensory deprivation affects GABAA receptors in layers II/III and IV, quantitative autoradiographic tests were performed on adult whisker trimmed rats. To determine whether the same deprivation paradigm affects the α1-GABAA receptor subunits, immunocytochemical methods were used. 5 CHAPTER II MATERIALS AND METHODS Subjects The subjects were Long-Evans hooded rats (Simonsen, Gilroy, CA), maintained on a 12:12-hour light:dark cycle, with food and water available ad libitum. Whiskers from either the middle row C or the other rows ABDE were trimmed for six weeks. Whisker Deprivation Six-week-old rats had whiskers trimmed every other day for 6 weeks, ensuring that their vibrissae were kept shorter than 1 mm. All experimental rats had the mystacial vibrissae in either row C, rows ABDE, or all rows clipped on one or both sides of the face. During the procedure, no anesthetic was used, and rats were hand-held with gentle restraint. These protocols were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of North Texas. Histology Unperfused rats were killed at the end of their deprivation schedules by decapitation. For tangential sections the deprived barrel region was dissected out from the brain, and was flattened and frozen at -44˚C with the heat dissipater of a cryostat (2800 Frigocut N, Reichert-Jung, 6 Cambridge Instruments, Deerfield, IL). Cryostat sections 16 µm thick were cut at -20˚ C tangentially to the pial surface, and were thaw-mounted onto gelatin subbed microscope slides. For coronal sections the whole brain was frozen in -40˚C isopentane for 5 min, transferred to 80˚C isopentane, and cut and mounted as above. Sections were air dried for 0.5 to 3 h and then stored desiccated at -80˚C until their use for either immunochemistry or receptor autoradiography. After receptor autoradiography, some sections were Nissl stained, to examine whether cellular and immunological observations can be paralleled. Other sections were stained for cytochrome oxidase (CYO) activity (Wong-Riley, 1979) to localize barrels and to analyze any similarities between CYO activity and receptor binding. Immunocytochemistry Subunit-specific antisera were used to visualize GABA subunits. The α1-GABAA receptor subunit antisera were raised in rabbit against synthetic peptides derived from mouse subunit cDNA (generously provided by J.M. Fritschy). Prior to the immunological procedure, slides with rat sections were transferred to a 10˚C refrigerator for 5-10 min. Then they were dried in a slide warmer for 45 sec, immersed horizontally for 20 min in 0.5% paraformaldehyde in 0.15 M phosphate buffer (pH 7.4) (Fristchy and Mohler, 1995) for 20 min, and rinsed for 10 min in ice-cold 0.5 M Tris-saline buffer (TBS), pH 7.6. This and all of the following steps were performed with gentle agitation using a rocker table. Slides were then washed in preincubation solution consisting of TBS containing 10% normal goat serum and 0.05% Triton X-100. Sections were and incubated for two days and two nights at 4˚C in primary antibody solution (1:100,000) diluted in TBS buffer containing 10% normal goat serum (NGS). Sections 7 were then washed three times for 1.5 hr in TBS and incubated for 30 min at room temperature in TBS containing 2% NGS and goat-raised anti-rabbit biotinylated secondary antibodies (Jackson Immunoresearch Laboratories Inc., West Grove, PA) diluted 1:300 in TBS pH 7.4. After 1 hr additional washing in TBS, sections were transferred to the avidin-peroxidase solution (Vectastatin Elite kit; Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) for 30 min, and then washed 30 min in TBS. Sections were then stained using two equal volumes of 0.1% diaminobenzidine hydrochloride (DAB) in 50 mM TBS. To one of the volumes of substrate solution 0.02% hydrogen peroxide was added to the sections to start the reaction, and sections were vigorously agitated for 1-2.5 min until the desired color was obtained. The reaction was stopped by transfer into ice-cold TBS. After three more 10 min washes in TBS, sections were dried in a slide-warmer, dehydrated with ascending series of ethanol, cleared in xylenes and coverslipped using DPX. Ligand binding GABAA receptors were assessed using [3H]muscimol (Mower et al., 1986; Schwark et al., 1994). These same sections were previously used to examine changes in layer IV after deprivation (Fuchs and Salazar, 1998). Sections were preincubated 20 minutes at 4oC in 0.31 M Tris-citrate buffer, pH 7.1, and then were incubated for 40 minutes at 4oC in the buffer containing 10 nM [3H]muscimol (12-20 Ci/mmol; DuPont NEN, Boston, MA). Sections then were rinsed twice for 30 seconds in cold buffer, dipped briefly in dH20, and dried in a stream of air. Nonspecific binding was assessed in the presence of 1 mM GABA. Autoradiography 8 The brain sections and tritium standards (Microscale, Amersham) were exposed simultaneously in the same cassette to tritium-sensitive Hyperfilm (Amersham). Following 2-4 month exposure periods, the film was developed with Kodak D-19 and processed according to the manufacturer’s instructions. Data analysis: Immunostained and Nissl stained sections For semiquantitative image analysis, sections were digitized using a video-based computerized image analysis system (MCID, Imaging Research, St. Catherine, Ont., Canada). Measures of relative optical density were obtained from all the tangential α1-GABAAimmunostained sections, from cortical layer I (whenever possible) to layer IV. Samples were taken within a computer-generated circle over each barrel column, allowing for comparisons between deprived and intact rows. Measures from the readily visible barrels in layer IV were taken first. Readings from upper layers were then obtained. Patterns in the positions of radially oriented blood vessels were used to confirm the exact location of each barrel column. Determination of boundaries between layers II/III and IV was aid with a light microscope, taking into consideration cellular differences between granular layer IV and supragranular layer III, and that septa are more conspicuous in layer IV than in layer III. Within each section, the mean ratio of densities in deprived/nondeprived row was determined, using rows B, C, and D only. The average ratio for each subject was then calculated from the section means. Then the average ratio for all subjects was obtained. To test the null hypothesis that this ratio was not different from 1, Student’s t-test (two-tailed) was used. Experimental groups were compared with one another by using analysis of variance with post hoc 9 t-tests. The significance level was 0.05. Percent decrease within each section was calculated as the mean for deprived barrels minus the mean for nondeprived barrels, divided by the mean for nondeprived barrels. Standard error measurements (S.E.M.) were calculated first for each subject. For display, images were contrast-enhanced in pseudocolor. Data analysis: Autoradiographs [3H]Muscimol was quantitatively analyzed using a video-based computerized image analysis system (MCID, Imaging Research, St. Catharines, Ont., Canada). Tritium standards were used to calibrate autoradiographic densities. Ligand binding densities were automatically calibrated by using a best-fit equation based on the plastic tritium standards, which had been calibrated in nCi/mg tissue wet weight based on standards made from rat brain tissue (Fuchs and Schwark, 1993). The analysis of data was done by the same procedures as the immunostained and Nissl stained sections. 10 CHAPTER III RESULTS Effects of Whisker Deprivation on α1-GABAA receptor subunit Coronal sections from control subjects depict the normal laminar distribution for α1GABAA receptor subunit immunostaining, in which layer IV appears the darkest (Fig. 1). Adult rats with whiskers trimmed for 6 weeks showed in tangential sections a clear decrease in optical density immunopositive staining for α1-GABAA receptor subunit in the deprived barrel columns. Deprived columns showed less staining than the adjacent non-deprived ones. This decrease was larger and readily detected in layers II/III of the deprived barrel columns 6% ± 0.6 (P<0.005). The effect in layer IV was smaller, but still significant (-3.3% ± 0.9, P<0.005, Fig. 3, Table 1). Control subjects showed no difference between C and adjacent rows ( 11 ). Nissl Staining Identification of barrel columns boundaries was aided by the differential staining and cell density between barrel septa and hollow. Figure 4( with coronal and tangential sections). Trimming whiskers for 6 weeks in adult rats resulted in an overall decrease in layers II, III, and IV of -8.7% ± 0.9 (mean ± S.E.M., P<0.005) as compared to the adjacent non deprived columns. This decrease was largest in supragranular layers II and III (-11.1% ± 1.5, P<0.005), and smaller in layer IV (-5.6% ± 0.7, P<0.005), where the difference was less obvious. (fig 4, Table 1). In control subjects visual inspection rendered no difference between C and adjacent rows. GABAA receptor autoradiography [3H]Muscimol binding showed an overall reduction of -11.0% ± 0.9 in the deprived columns as compared to the adjacent intact ones (mean ± S.E.M., P<0.001). The decrease was similar for layers II/III (-11.4% ± 0.9, P<0.001), and IV (-10.2% ± 0.9, P<0.005). Comparison between these two percentages from supragranular and granular layers was non significant. Control subjects showed no difference between C and adjacent rows. 12 Cortical Layers [3H] Muscimol Binding II, III, and IV II/III IV Difference (II/III) - IV -11.0 ± 0.9*** -11.4 ± 0.9*** -10.2 ± 0.9*** n.s. α1-GABAA receptor subunit Immunoreactivity -4.9 ± 0.6*** -6.0 ± 0.6*** -3.3 ± 0.9*** * Nissl -8.7 ± 0.9** -11.1 ± 1.5*** -5.6 ± 0.7*** ** Table 1. Percentage decrease in the deprived barrel rows relative to intact rows (mean ± S.E.M.) *P<0.05;**P<0.005; ***P<0.001; n.s., not significant. 13 Effects of Whisker Trimming on Layers II,III, and IV of SI 14 % decrease on deprived vs intact columns 12 10 8 6 4 2 0 α1-GABAA receptor subunit [3H] Muscimol Binding Nissl Fig 1. Effects of whisker trimming on α1-GABAA receptor subunit immunoreactivity, [3H]Muscimol binding, and Nissl staining on deprived barrel columns. After 6 weeks of whisker trimming in adult rats, deprived rows containing layers II, III, and IIV showed significant decreases in all three markers (**p<0.005) compared to adjacent undeprived rows. Each value represents the mean ± S.E.M. 14 14 Percent decrease in deprived barrel layers 12 10 8 6 4 2 0 II/III** IV* α1-GABAA receptor subunit II/III** IV* [3H] Muscimol Binding II/III* IV* Nissl Fig X. Effects of whisker trimming on α1-GABAA receptor subunit immunoreactivity, [3H]muscimol binding, and Nissl staining on deprived barrel columns of layers II/III, and IV of SI of rats. The effects were larger in supragranular deprived layers II/III than in deprived layer IV for all paradigms. For α1-GABAA receptor subunit immunoreactivity the decrease in layers II/III was 6% ± 0.6, P<0.001, and decrease in layer IV was 3.3% ± 0.9, P<0.001. For [3H]muscimol binding the decrease in layers II/III was 11.4% ± 0.9, P<0.001, whereas in layer IV it was 10.2% ± 0.9, P<0.001. For Nissl staining the decrease in layers II/III was 11.1% ± 1.5, P<0.001, and in layer IV it was 5.6% ± 0.7, P<0.001. The difference in percent deprivation effect in supragranular versus granular layer was significant, both for α1-GABAA receptor subunit immunoreactivity (P<0.005) and for Nissl staining (p<0.05). Each value represents the mean ± S.E.M. 15 CHAPTER IV DISCUSSION Effects of whisker trimming on GABAA binding Decreases in GABAA receptors, as found in this study, may participate in the long lasting disinhibition in neurons of deprived barrel columns, such as the increase in spontaneous activity, and the decrease in preference for angle of whisker deflection (Simons and Land, 1987). This respecification of receptive fields can also be mapped using markers for neuronal activity such as 2-deoxyglucose (Kossut et al., 1988; review in Kossut, 1992). It seems that this increased disinhibition arises within the cortex (Simons and Land, 1994), since deprivation effects can be avoided by cortical infusion of muscimol (Wallace et al., 2001). These cortical map modifications seem to be compensatory, brought about by the reduction in sensory input to the brain (Siucinska and Kossut, 1996). Our results are in agreement with previous reports in which sensory deprivation in the adult can modify rat somatosensory cortex (Glazewski and Fox, 1996; Fuchs and Salazar, 1998). Adult plasticity differs from developmental plasticity in fundamental ways. In the adult, experienceregulated plasticity may be a temporal, reversible event, that involves specific changes in synaptic strength, and that may result in synapse formation and elimination (Trachtenberg et al., 2000; Rausell and Jones, 1995; Huntley, 1996). These changes could be expected primarily in the 16 cortico-cortical circuitry (Armstrong-James et al., 1994). It is known that after 21 days of age rats achieve the adult number of synapses per neuron (Micheva and Beaulieu, 1996). During development, however, these changes might be irreversible (Lendvai et al., 2000), and linked to changes in collateralization and arborization of thalamocortical fibers (Antonini and Stryker, 1993). Whisker deprivation changes the excitability of neurons in cortex in a layer-specific manner. Layer IV becomes more sensitive to whisker deprivation around postnatal day 7 (Fox, 1992), and remains plastic in the adult (Huang et al., 1998; Goldreich et al., 1999). Layers II/III reach mature receptive field organization around postnatal day 14 (Stern et al., 2001) and also remain plastic in adulthood (Armstrong-James et al., 1994; Glazewski and Fox, 1996; Lendvai et al., 2000, Sheperd et al., 2003). For instance, [14C] 2-deoxyglucose labeling shows that normal activity decreases in layers II/III and IV of barrel columns corresponding to plucked whiskers in young adult rats. In rats deprived at older age, decreases in metabolic activity are reduced and only measurable in layers II/III, and absent in layer IV (Kossut, 1998; Skibinska et al., 2000). Moreover, plastic changes in responses from adult cortex after peripheral manipulations appear as early as 24 hrs later in layers II/III, but not in layer IV (Diamond et al., 1994). This induced changes occur in layer II/III regardless of changes in layer IV after sensory manipulation and suggest that supragranular layers are indeed a site of experience dependent plasticity in SI (Glazewski and Fox, 1996). Contribution to these changes might occur from layer IV to II/III synapses, since whisker deprivation induces depression at these sites (Allen et al., 2003; Sheperd et al., 2003). The present results show that GABAA receptor binding decreases not only in layer IV (Fuchs and Salazar, 1998), but also in the deprived layers II/III of the same column. While layer IV presents a high degree of functional independence from supragranular layers (Goldreich, et al., 17 1999; Huang et al., 1998), intracortical activity is required for potentiation and depression of sensory responses in barrel cortex (Wallace et al., 2001). Thus, a reduction in GABAA receptors in layers II/III could significantly contribute to observed experience-dependent plasticity (Lendvai et al., 2000; Stern et al., 2001; Fox, 1992, 1994; Simons and Land, 1987; Fox, 1992, 1994; Glazewski and Fox, 1996; Wallace and Fox, 1999; Fuchs and Salazar, 1998). It would be interesting to test whether infusing muscimol in the barrel cortex using an elvax implant has any effect in GABAA receptors from deprived and intact cortical layers II/III and IV. The decrease in GABAA receptors might have major functional consequences. For instance, during development, deprivation causes axonal branches ascending from layer IV to layers II/III to grow exubertantly and make errors, extending beyond their correspondent barrel column (Stern et al., 2001). It seems that a decrease in GABAA receptors could allow the spreading of excitation in layers II/III and thus affect the mechanisms that promote/inhibit axonal growth. In the adult these effects would be less dramatic, once the synaptic connections are established. Alpha1-GABAA receptor subunit immunocytochemistry The present study is the first to show that chronic whisker trimming in adulthood decreases the level of α1-GABAA receptor subunit staining in the corresponding columns. Reductions in α1GABAA receptor subunit have been reported in other cases of sensory deprivation. Monkeys monocularly deprived with intravitreal injection of tetradotoxin for 8-21 days reduces levels of α1, β2, and γ2 subunit mRNA in the deprived visual cortex. This reduction is fairly specific, since mRNA for other subunits (α2, α4, and β1) remain unchanged (Huntsman et al., 1994; Jones, 1997). Whether similar decreases in alpha-1 GABAA receptor occur after neonatal deprivation is not known. Rat SI shows a decrease in α1 and α5-GABAA in focal cortical malformations induced by 18 neonatal freeze lesions of SI (Redecker, 2000). Furthermore, electrolytic lesion of thalamus in the newborn decreases α1 in layers III-IV, but increases α2, α3, and α5 in the same areas (Paysan, 1997). These changes in density of immunoreactive structures could either be a result of specific down-regulation of receptor subunit, or represent a reduction in the inhibitory synaptic contacts, or both. In adult monkeys, blockage of sensory input results in differential up- or down-regulation of GABAA receptor genes (Hendry et al., 1994). Whether similar manipulations modify synaptic contacts is addressed in the next section. Nissl Results In this study, adult whisker trimming results in significant decreases in all layers of deprived barrel columns. This decrease could reflect either cell density changes, reduction of Nissl-stained axons and dendrites, or shrinkage of the deprived columns (or expansion of the undeprived, adjacent ones). Cell density changes have been reported after whisker deprivation. GABA-containing neurons and GABAA receptors from deprived layer IV decrease in number after whisker trimming (Micheva and Beaulieu, 1995b; Fuchs and Salazar, 1998). GABAergic population in barrel cortex represents 10% of the total (Micheva and Beaulieu, 1995b). Furthermore, neonatal all whisker-deprivation reduces the numerical density of presumed inhibitory synapses by 52% (Sadaka et al., 2003). In contrast, when considering all barrel Nisslstained cells, neonatal whisker deprivation does not affect numerical density, but it significantly decreases (13.7%) the intracortical projections in all layers of the deprived columns (Keller and Carlson, 1999). This suggests that our results might represent a decrease in the Nissl-stained projections rather than cell number change. The possibility of shrinkage of deprived barrel size is 19 not supported by studies of neonatal unilateral sensory deprivation (Sadaka et al., 2003). Our results, however, were based on trimming only one row. This different way of depriving whiskers might account for differences in functional and structural changes (Glazewski and Fox, 1996). During development, whisker deprivation leads to abnormal receptive fields in layers II/III (Stern et al., 2001). These abnormalities seem to be caused in part by aberrant ascending axons from layer IV to layer II/III, by changes in inhibition, intrinsic membrane properties of cortical neurons, and modulation of existing synapses by LTP. Some lines of evidence suggest that axonal length within columns develops normally from layer IV to II/III even in absence of normal sensory experience (Bender et al., 2003). However, comparisons between deprived and non-deprived barrel columns show a decrease in connectivity in deprived neurons from layers II/III (Petersen et al., 2004), which results from changes in decreased unitary EPSP amplitudes. Other deprivation studies during critical periods show that dendritic branching in layer II/III neurons changes after deprivation performed during critical periods only (Maravall et al., 2004). Our results were based on deprived/nondeprived ratios. Against the possibility of axonal increase in the adjacent-to-deprived row in layer II/III, is the finding that spine density decreased in the deprived barrels. This reflects a decrease in synaptic density in layer III neurons, the likely site of termination of cortico-cortical inputs, and indicated a decrease in functional synapses between columns that may. In turn, this downregulation may account for the change in cortical representation of the deprived row in these layers (Kossut, 1998). 20 Implications Since sensory manipulation can induce changes in LII/III receptive fields before or without it affecting LIV (Glazewski and Fox, 1996; Stern et al., 2001, the synapses from LIV to LII/III have been hypothesized to be a site of experience-dependent plasticity in SI. Support from Allen, et al, 2003, in which whisker deprivation induces synaptic depression at LIV to L2/3 synapses. Also, Sheperd et, al., 2003 : it alters the functional topography of the L4 to L2/3 projection. Bender et al., 2003, on the other hand looked for the axonal changes from IIIV to II/III after deprivation and found no change. Why do we find a change in Nissl? What does this mean? Are there changes in Nissl reported for Layer IV? Future experiments: - measure decrease in activity in infraorbital nerve after 1 week - compare neonatal vs adult deprivation for alpha 1, and in the neonatal should be larger effect? - See if the effect is reversible in the neonatal and in the adult. - Compare C dperived versus ABDE deprived figures: is there an increase in the C representation in the spare C in the abde clipped animal? For Alpha 1 or the otheres? Kossut reports 65%increase after stimulation of spare C,in layer Iv in 2DGin surgery leaving intact C; no ipsilateral effects of stimulant. Increase present in layer II-III and as well. - Measure width in micrometers of barrels of intact vs deprived?at different laminar levels (see Kossut 98) - How was the significance calculated for II/III vs IV? was it by placing the 21 rqatios of decreases in normal and exp or by comparing ratios II/IIIover IV in normals vs ratio II/III over IV in deprived? If so, then we got a winner!!! (The mech for deprivation affecting IV is intracortical and what is affecting IV is not necessary the same affecting II/III. - If decrease inn II/III is intracortical, then not only GABA, but also some excitatory neurotransmitters might be affected in II/III even when not in IV (NMDA?)(See discussion in Micheva ’95) - The decrease in alpha 1 GABAAR subunit can be caused either by decreased synthesis of receptors or changes in subunit composition. (Lech , monica 2001) - Ask Dr. Fuchs if she has sections with deprived NMDA receptor binding!! - Point out that this study involves young adult (adolescent) rats, 6 weeks old. Their brains might be more plastic than older adults (like older than 6 months, i.e. Glazewski, and Fox, 1996) - Maybe mention the importance of these experiments in the determination of mechanisms that can increase plasticity with therapeutic means, such as decreasing gabaergic inhibition or increasing neuromodulator levels (Morales, B., 2003) REFERENCES - Akhtar, N.D. and Land, P.W., Activity-dependent regulation of glutamic acid decarboxylase in the rat barrel cortex: Effects of neonatal versus adult sensory deprivation. J. Comp. Neurobiol., 307 (1991) 200-213. 22 - Ambardekar, A. V., Ilinksy, I.A., Froestl, W., Bowery, N.G., and Kultas-Ilinsky, K. Distribution and properties of GABAB antagonist [3H] CGP 62349 binding in the rhesus monkey thalamus and basal ganglia and the influence of lesions in the reticular thalamic nucleus. Neuroscience, 93-4 (19) 1339-1347. - Armstrong-James M, Fox K. Spatiotemporal convergence and divergence in the rat S1 ‘barrel’ cortex. J. Comp. Neurol., 263 (1987) 265-281. BUSCAR - Armstrong-James, M., Diamond, M.E., Ebner, F.F. An innocuous bias in whisker use in adult rats modifies receptive fields of barrel cortex neurons. J Neurosc., 14 (1994) 69786991.BUSCAR - Antonini, A., Stryker, M. Rapid remodeling of axonal arbors in the visual cortex. Science, 260 (1993) 1819-1821. BUSCAR - Bender, K.J., Rangell, Juliana, and Ferldman, D.E. Development of columnar topography in the excitatory layer 4 to layer 2/3 projection in rat barrel cortex. J. Neurosc. 23-25 (2003) 8759-8770. - Bowery, N.G., Hill, D.R., Hudson, A.L., Doble, A., Middlemass, D.N., Shaw, J., and Turnbull, M. (-) Baclofen decreases neurotransmitter release in the mammalian CNS by an action at a novel GABA receptor. Nature, 283 (1980) 92-94. - Bowery, N.G., Hudson, A.L., and Price, G.W. GABAA and GABAB receptor site distribution in rat central nervous system. Neuroscience. 20 (1987) 365-383. - Chmielowska, J, Carvel, G.E., and Simons, D.J. Spatial organization of thalamocortical and corticothalamic projection systems in the rat SmI barrel cortex. J.Comp. Neurol. 285 (1989) 325-338. - Chu, D.C.M., Albin, R.L., Young, A.B., and Penney, J.B. Distribution and kinetics of GABAB 23 binding sites in rat central nervous system: a quantitative autoradiographic study. Neuroscience. 34-2 (1990) 341-357. - Davies, P.A, Hanna, M.C, Hales, T.G, Kirknesss, E.F. (1997) Nature (London) 385, 820-823 - Deisz, R.A. and Prince, D.A. Frequency-dependent depression of inhibition in guinea-pig neocortex in vitro by GABAB receptor feed-back on GABA release. J. Physiol. Lond. 424 (1989) 513-541. - Deisz, R.A. Presynaptic and Postsynaptic GABAB receptors of neocortical neurons of the rat in vitro: differences in pharmacology and ionic mechanisms. Synapse. 25 (1997) 62-72. - Deisz, R.A. GABAB receptor-mediated effects in human and rat neocortical neurones in vitro. Neuropharmacology. 38 (1999) 1755-1766). - Diamond, M.E., Huang, W., Ebner, F.F. Laminar comparison of somatosensory cortical plasticity. Science 265: 1885-1888. BUSCAR - Fox, K. A critical period for experience-dependent synaptic plasticity in rat barrel cortex. J. Neurosc. 12 (1992) 1826-1838. - BUSCAR Fox, K. The cortical component of experience-dependent synaptic plasticity in the rat barrel cortex. J. Neurosc. 14 (1994) 7665-7679. BUSCAR - Fritschy, J.M., Paysan, J., Enna, A., Mohler, H. (1994) J. Neurosci. 143, 5302-5324. - Fritschy, J.M, Mohler, H. J. Comp. Neurol., 359 (1995) 154-194 - Fuchs, J.L. and Salazar, E. Effects of whisker trimming on GABAA receptor binding in the barrel cortex of developing and adult rats. J. Comp. Neurol., 395 (1998) 209-216. - Glazewski, S., Fox., Time course of experience-dependent synaptic potentiation and depression in barrel cortex of adolescent rats. J. Neurophysiol., 75 (1996) 1714-1729. BUSCAR - Goodman, C.S, Shatz, C.J. Neuron 10. suppl., (1993) 77-78. 24 - Huntley, G.W., Correlation between patterns of horizontal connectivity and the extent of shortterm representational plasticity in rat motor cortex. Cereb. Cortex, 7 (1996) 143-156. - Hendry, S.H.C. and Jones, E.G. Reduction in number of immunostained GABAergic neurons in deprived-eye dominance columns of monkey area 17, Nature, 320 (1986) 750-753. - Hendry, S.H.C., Huntsman, M.M., Vinuela, A. Mohler, H. de Blas, A.L., Jones, E.G. GABAA receptor subunit immunoreactivity in primate visual cortex: distribution in macaques and humans and regulation by visual input in adulthood. J. Neurosci. 14 (1994) 2383-2401. - Howe, J.R., Sutor, B., and Zieglgansberger, W. Baclofen reduces post-synaptic potentials of rat cortical neurons by an action other than its hyperpolarizing action. J. Physiol. Lond. 384 (1987) 539-570. - Huntsman, M.M., Isackson, P.J., and Jones, E.G. Lamina-specific expression and activitydependent regultation of seven GABAA receptor subunit mRNAs in monkey visual cortex. J. Neurosc. 14-4 (1994):2236-59. - Jones, E.G. Area and lamina-specific expression of GABAA receptors subunit mRNAs in monkey cerebral cortex. Can. J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 75-5 (1997) 452-469. - Jones, ’95 and 98 - Keller, A., Carlson, G.C. Neonatal whisker clipping alters intracortical, but not thalamocortical projections, in rat barrel cortex. J. of Comp. Neurol., 412 (1999) 83-94. - Kiriazy, H.T., Carvell, G.E., Brumberg, J.C., and Simons, D.J. Quantitative Effects of GABA and Bicuculline Methiodide on Receptive Field properties of neurons in real and simulated whisker barrels. Journal of Neurophysiology. 75-2 (1996) 547-560 - Kossut, M., Hand, P.J., Greenberg, J., Hand, C.L. Single vibrissal cortical column in SI cortex of rat and and its alterations in neonatal and adult bivrissa-deafferented animals: a quantitative 25 2DG study. J Neurophysiol.60 (1988) 829-852. - Kossut, M. Plasticity of the barrel cortex neurons. Prog. Neurobiol. 39 (1992) 389-422. - Kossut, M., Experience-dependent changes in function and anatomy of adult barrel cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 123 (1998) 110-116. - Kossut M., and Juliano, S.L., Anatomical correlates of representational map reorganization induced by partial vibrissectomy in the barrel cortex of adult mice. Neuroscience. 92-3 (1999) 807-817. - Lendvai B., Stern, E., Svoboda, K., Experience-dependent plasticity of dendritic spines in the developing rat barrel cortex in vivo. Nature 404 (2000) 876-881.BuUSCAR - Lebedev, M.A., Mirabella, G., Erchova, I., Diamond, M.E. Experience-dependent plasticity of rat barrel cortex: redistribution of activity across barrel columns. Cerebral Cortex. 10-1 (2000) 23-31. - MacDonald, R.L, Olsen R.W.GABAA receptor channels. Annual Review of Neuroscience.17 (1994) 569-602. - Micheva K.D., and Beaulieu, C. Neonatal sensory deprivation induces selective changes in the quantitative distribution of GABA-immunoreactive neurons in the rat barrel field cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 361-4 (1995) 574-84. - Micheva, K.D., and Beaulieu, C. An anatomical substrate for experience-dependent plasticity of the rat barrel field cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 92 (1995) 11834-11838. - Maravall, M., Koh, I.Y., Lindquist, W.B., Svoboda, K., Experience-dependent changes in basal dendritic branching of layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons during a critical period for developmental plasticity in rat barrel cortex. Cerebral Cortex, advances March 28 (2004) 1-10 - Micheva, K.D. and Beaulieu, C. An anatomical substrate for experience-dependent plasticity 26 of the rat barrel field cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 92-25 (1995) 11834-8. - Micheva K.D., and Beaulieu, C. Postnatal development of GABA neurons in the rat somatosensory barrel cortex: a quantitative study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 7-3 (1995) 419-30 - Micheva, K.D. and Beaulieu, C. Quantitative aspects of synaptogenesis in the rat barrel field cortex with special reference to GABA circuitry. ’ J. Comp. Neurol., 373-3 (1996) 340-354. - Micheva, K.D. and Beaulieu, C. Development and plasticity of the inhibitory neocortical circuitry with an emphasis on the rodent barrel field cortex: a review. Can J Physiol Pharmacol. 75-5 (1997) 470-8. - Misgeld, U., Bijak, M., and Jarolimek, W. A physiological role for GABAB receptors and the effects of baclofen in the mammalian central nervous system.. Prog. Neurobiol., 46(4) (1995) 423-62 - Muñoz, A., DeFelipe, J., and Jones, E.G. Patterns of GABA (B)R1a,b receptor gene expression in monkey and human visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 11-2 (2001) 104-113. - Payssan, J., Kossel, A., Bolz,,,, J., and Fritschy, J. Area-specific regulation of γ-aminobutyric acid type A receptor subtypes by thalamic afferents in developing rat neocortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 94 (1997) 6995-7000. - Penschuck, S., Giorgetta, O., and Fritschy, J.M. Neuronal activity influences the growth of barrels in developing rat primary somatosensory cortex without affecting the expression pattern of four major GABAA receptor subunits. Brain Research. 112 (1999) 117-127. - Petersen, C.C.H., Brecht, M., Hahn, T.T.G., Sakmann, B. Synaptic changes in Layer 2/3 underlying map plasticity of developing barrel cortex. Science, 304 (2004) 739-742. - Rausell, E., Jones, E. Extent of intracortical arborization of thalamocortical axons as a determinant of representational plasticity in monkey somatic sensory cortex. J. Neurosci., 15 27 (1995) 4270-4288. - Redecker, C., Luhmann, H.J., Hagemann, G., Fritschy, J., and Witte, W. Differential Downregulation of GABAA receptor subunits in widespread brain regions in the freeze-lesion model of focal cortical malformations. J. Neuroscience. 20-13 (2000) 5045-5053. - Rema, V., Armstrong-James, M., Ebner, F.F. Experience-dependent plasticity is impaired in adult rat barrel cortex after whiskers are unused in early postnatal life. J. Neurosc. 23-1 (2003) 358-366. - Sadaka, Y., Weinfeld, E., Lev, D.L., and White, E. Changes in mouse barrel synapses consequent to sensory deprivation from birth. J. Comp. Neurol,, 457 (2003) 75-86. - Schlaggar, B.L., Fox, K., O’Leary, D.D.M. Postsynaptic control of plasticity in developing somatosensory cortex. Nature (London) 364 (1993) 623-626. - Sheperd, G.M., Pologruto, T.A., Svodoba, K. Circuit analysis of experience-dependent plasticity in the developing rat barrel cortex. Neuron, 38-2 (2003) 277-289. - Skibinska, A., Glazewski, S., Fox, K. Age-dependent response of the mouse barrel cortex to sensory deprivation: a 2-deoxyglucose study. Exp. Brain Res. 132 (2000) 132-134. - Simons, D.J. Response properties of vibrissa units in the rat SI somatosensory neocortex. J. Neurophysiol., 41 (1978) 798-820. - Simons, D.J. and Land, P.W. Early experience of tactile stimulation influences organization of somatic sensory cortex. Nature, 326 (1987) 694-697. - Simons, D.J. and Woolsey, T.A., Functional organization of mouse barrel cortex. Brain Research, 165 (1979) 327-332. - Stern, E.A., Maravall, M., and Svoboda, K. Rapid development and plasticity of layer 2/3 maps in rat barrel cortex in vivo. Neuron, 31-2 (2001) 305-315. 28 - Siucinska, E., Kossut, M. Short-lasting classical conditioning induces reversible changes of representational maps of vibrissae in mouse SI cortex: a 2DG study. Cereb. Cortex 6 (1996) 506-513. BUSCAR - Trachtenberg, J.T., Chen., B.E., Knott. G.W., Feng, G., Sanes., J.R., Welker, E., Svodoba, K., Long-term in vivo imaging of experience-dependent synaptic plasticity in adult cortex. Nature 420 (2002) 788-794. BUSCAR - Van der Loos, H. and Woolsey, T.A. Somatosensory cortex: structural alterations following early injury to sense. Science, 179-71 (1973) 395-398. - Wallace, H.S., Glazewski, S., Fox, K. The role of cortical activity in experience-dependent potentiation and depression of sensory responses in rat barrel cortex. J Neurosc. 21 (2001) 3881-3894.BUSCAR - Welker, C. and Woolsey, T.A., Structure of layer IV in the somatosensory neocortex of the rat: description and comparison with the mouse. J. Comp. Neurol., 158 (1974) 437-453 - Welker, C., Microelectrode delineation of fine grain somatotopic organization of (SmI) cerebral neocortex in albino rat. Brain Research 26-2 (1971) 259-275. - Welker, C. Receptive fields of barrels in the somatosensory neocortex of the rat. J. Comp. Neurol., 166-2 (1976) 173-189. - Woolsey, T.A. and Van der Loos, H. The structural organization of layer IV in the somatosensory region (SI) of mouse cerebral cortex, Brain Research, 17 (1970) 205-242. - Xi, Z X; Akasu, T. Presynaptic GABAA receptors in vertebrate synapses. The Kurume Medical Journal, 43-2 (1996) 115-12 29