Reaction Time Abstract: Motion, which is the process by which something moves from one place to another, is broken down into two categories: scalars and vectors; scalar measurements given magnitude, but vector measurements given magnitude and direction. Distance and displacement are similar in that they give a measurement of the space traveled, but displacement is a vector and therefore has a direction and measures only all of the space traveled when going from going from “point A” to “point B” in a given cycle or pathway. Speed and velocity are similar because they give a measurement of how fast something is traveling, except that velocity is a vector measurement and gives a direction. Acceleration is a vector measurement and is defined as a change in velocity. The human body is divided into many systems, and the system that controls the process of sending, receiving, and processing nerve impulses is the nervous system. When stimuli, changes in the environment, are detected, the body produces a response. The time elapsed between processing a stimulus and giving forth a response is the reaction time. The purpose of the experiment was to choose a variable and see if and how the variable affects the control group’s reaction times. The variable selected was wearing prescription glasses while catching a falling ruler; the hypothesis was that the variable would slow down the reaction time of the test subject. After all data was recorded and charted, the data rejected the hypothesis and therefore the reaction times were quicker when using the variable during the experimental trials. Introduction: Motion is defined as the process by which something moves from one place to another. Motion can be classified three ways: linear, which is motion in a straight line, rotational, which is motion around an axis, and curvilinear, which is a combination of both linear and rotational motion. Motion was first kinematically described by Galileo; Galileo described concepts such as velocity and acceleration. Motion can be described in two ways: scalar and vector (Lerner/Wilmoth, 2008). Scalar and vector measurements both are similar in that they describe magnitude (how much or how far an object travels). Vector measurements are more precise in that they give a direction along with magnitude. Examples of scalar measurements are length, distance, mass, volume, density, temperature, pressure, and speed. Examples of vector measurements include displacement, momentum, acceleration, thrust, velocity, and weight (Benson, 2009). Distance and displacement are two similar, yet very different concepts that deal with motion. Distance, when talking about physics, refers to the extent of space that exists between two points. Distance is a scalar measurement and therefore a direction is not entailed with distance. Displacement is a vector measurement which refers to the extent of space that exists directly from “point A” to “point B”. Besides distance being a scalar measurement and displacement being a vector measurement, distance and displacement differ in what each measure. Distance measures all of the space traveled when going from going from “point A” to “point B” in a given cycle or pathway. Displacement measures the actual distance directly from the beginning to end; if a start point and an end point are one in the same, the displacement would then be 0. Because of this relationship, displacement is always less than or equal to the distance (Elert, unk.). Speed and velocity are like distance and displacement in that they are both very similar, yet very different. Speed refers to the ratio of distance to the change in time. Speed is a scalar measurement and therefore does not have a direction. In the common vernacular, speed refers to how fast an object is traveling. The formula for speed is as follows: speed = distance / ∆t (change in time). Velocity is similar to speed, but velocity refers to the ratio of displacement to change in time rather than distance. Velocity is a vector measurement and therefore has a direction. Like speed, velocity can be referred to how fast something is going when speaking in the common vernacular. The only difference is velocity gives a direction and gives the distance from “point A” to “point B”. The formula for velocity is as follows: velocity = displacement / ∆t (change in time). Speed and velocity are both usually measured in meters per second or kilometers per hour (Elert, unk.). Another concept that involves motion is acceleration. Acceleration in physics terminology is the rate of change of velocity to the change in time. Acceleration refers to the increase of velocity over a period of time, and deceleration refers to the decrease of velocity over a given period of time. In the common vernacular, acceleration is referred to as speeding up, which is slightly different then its true scientific definition. Because acceleration is a change in velocity, acceleration occurs when the direction changes because accleration is also a vector measurement just like velocity. Acceleration can be calculated using the following formula: acceleration = (final velocity – initial velocity) / ∆t; acceleration is measured in m/s2 (Elert, unk.). Gravity, which is the force that exists between all masses in the universe, is known to pull masses down to the ground. For example, when someone jumps, they lift off the ground but return shortly due to the gravity on Earth. The theory of gravity was first proposed by Sir Isaac Newton who began hypothesizing after Newton was hit on the head by a falling apple from a tree above. Gravity makes life possible to be in “real time” as sometimes gravity is referred to, whereas on other planets where gravity either makes movement near impossible to lift off the ground or too little gravity to remain on ground for extended periods of time. This gravity puts forth a uniform gravitational acceleration that occurs with all masses present on Earth. The acceleration due to gravity on Earth is equal to Earth’s gravity because objects will gain velocity as the down ward pressure forces them down ward at a rate of 9.8 m/s2 (Elert, unk.). The human body consists of many smaller “parts” or “sections” which act together to regulate normal health and bodily function. These systems of the body work independently to complete specific tasks that each system was “designed” to complete. When these systems work in a joint effort, the individual systems join together to complete tasks and body functions that could not be achieved independently. These systems include the circulatory system, digestive system, endocrine system, immune system, excretory system, respiratory system, muscular system, skeletal system, reproductive system, and the nervous system. The system responsible for sending, receiving, and processing nerve impulses throughout the body is the nervous system (Goddard, 2009). The nervous system consists of three major sections: the central nervous system, peripheral nervous system, and autonomic nervous system. The central nervous system can de divided into two major sections, the brain and the spinal cord. The brain contains over 100 billion neurons, or brain cells, and is divided into three main sections: the cerebrum, the medulla oblongata, and the cerebellum. The cerebrum is the largest part of the brain and contains several “lobes” which are small sections of the brain to perform specific tasks such as audio comprehension, visual comprehension, hormone control, and logic. These sections are connected by long nerve fibers. The strength of these connections of nerve fibers are currently known to highly influence an individuals intelligence. The cerebellum is attached above the top of the spinal cord tip. The cerebellum is the second largest part of the brain which serves as a “balancer” which keeps muscle tone and bodily balance in proper function. The medulla oblongata is the smallest part of the brain, is located directly on the tip of the spinal cord and is responsible for regulating heartbeat, blood pressure, breathing, and reflex centers for reflexes such as vomiting, coughing, sneezing, and hiccupping. The spinal cord is attached to the brain and runs about halfway down the spine of the human body; its purpose is to connect the brain with the peripheral nervous system (Farr, 2002). The peripheral nervous system is responsible for the relay of nerve impulses that are sent, received, and processed by the central nervous system. As with the brain, the nervous passages are formed from neurons. Sensory input from the body surface, from joint, tendon, and muscle receptors, and from internal organs passes centrally through the roots of the spinal cord; this is the primary concept of the peripheral nervous system. The autonomic nervous system is an offshoot of the peripheral nervous system which provides the normal processes needed to maintain the proper bodily function of all muscles, glands, and organs. For example, when the following occurs, the peripheral nervous system is the main component for the response. When a person touches something hot, the nerve endings on the finger tips realize that the object being touched is very hot. These nerves send impulses through the peripheral nervous system to the brain, which instantaneously sends an impulse back to the nerves on the fingertips, telling the nerves to flinch and back off of the object. The example given demonstrates the concept of stimuli and responses (Farr, 2002). Stimuli, or a stimulus in the singular, are a “detectable change in the environment; that which influences or causes a temporary increase of physiological activity or response in the whole organism or in any of its parts,” (Biology-Online, 2009). Stimuli occur nearly every second of every day, and can sometimes go unnoticed. Stimuli are the outside “forces” that trigger a reaction from the body. Stimuli are the major concept that exist in the senses of the body and include such examples as smells, sounds, feelings, visuals, tastes, etc. Stimuli cause the human body to trigger a response, which is a response produced by the body-either voluntary or involuntarily- in response to the stimulus or stimuli. Responses are necessary in order for the human body to operate as the body is supposed to; reactions protect us from damaging the various sensory organs of the body, such as the ears, nose, mouth, hands, skin, and eyes. The body will send a response to the brain, telling a given person whether or not a noise is too loud for the ears, something is too hot or cold for the skin, too sour or rotten for the mouth/tongue, too much strain on the eyes, etc. The theory of stimulus-response is vital in understanding the thought process and reaction time (Farr, 2002). A reaction, in biological terms, refers to a response in the stimulus-response theory. Stimuli are transformed into impulses by the sensory nerves, which send different frequencies of impulses to the brain. These impulses are registered by the brain and the central nervous system and send back impulses through the peripheral nervous system to the sensory nerves. These stimuli responses generally occur in less than one second, but longer responses, such as verbal responses or responses needing logical reasoning and thought may take longer. The time that spans through this chain reaction is known as the reaction time. There are four main types of responses: simple, go/no-go, discrimination, and choice. Simple responses include performing action when one is told, such as hitting a button when a light flashes. Go/no-go responses include performing an action only under certain conditions, such as hitting a button when a light flashes green, but restraining when the light flashes red. Choice responses require the person to take into consideration both the stimulus and response longer because of different choices of actions with different conditions. An example would be hitting a circular button when a light flashes green, hitting a triangular button when a light flashes red, and hitting a square button when the light flashes yellow. Discrimination responses require an analysis of a pair of items and include such actions as determining which image of two given photos is brighter, newer, etc. Simple responses are almost instantaneous whereas go/ nogo, choice, and discrimination responses require more reaction time to process and choose the correct answer (Kosinski, 2009). Reaction time itself may vary based on many factors that contribute to the varied results of reaction time. Variables that affect reaction time include alertness, age, gender, hormones, hand orientation, direct vs. peripheral vision, practice, exposure to subject being tested, fatigue, dietary habits, distractions, good/poor vision, good/poor hearing, alcohol level, breathing cycle, emotions, drugs, exercise, illness, intelligence, and personality type. Depending on these factors at a given time, reaction times may vary. This is the primary reason why extensive experiments with many trials and variables must be performed to gather accurate data about a person’s reaction time (Kosinski, 2009). The purpose of the initial experiment was to provide a control group for the experimental trials. The purpose of the experiment was to choose a variable and see if and how the variable affects the control group’s reaction times. Each group was to drop a ruler and calculate the time elapsed before catching a falling ruler. The variable selected was the use of prescription-lens glasses to distract and disorientate the subject of the trials. With unclear vision, the test subject would probably show a difference in reaction time as compared to the controlled experiment. Another purpose of the experiment is to see if there is a direct correlation between the amount of times a person texts daily (survey question) and the reaction time of the test subjects. The hypothesis for the experiment is that the application of prescription glasses will disorient the test subject and will therefore have a longer reaction time. The experiment tests the simple response to a stimulus, with only one key stimulus and one known response. The reason that the hypothesis given is suggested is that often people have very different eyesight and prescriptions and therefore, not many of the test subjects will be able to see the ruler drop as clearly as a few will be able to. The hypothesis with regards to the survey question is that the more times a person texts daily, the quicker the reaction time will be, because texting requires a person to respond to text messages quickly; the hypothesis is that the more texts sent will result in lower reaction times because of exposure to quick responses. Materials and Methods: The materials and formulas needed for the experiment include the following: Half-meter stick Prescription-lens eye glasses Scientific calculator {s=(v1)(t) + (1/2)(a)(t2)} where “s” stands for distance, “v1” stands for initial velocity, “a” stands for acceleration, and “t” stands for time. Survey question: How many times do you send texts in an average day? Survey response slips The experiment was divided into three different sections: the control experiment, the experiment with the variable applied, and the survey questionnaire. The control experiment was set up first by dividing the class into smaller groups of four. A half-meter stick was supplied and was held by the person conducting the experiment. Held by the highest number and the 1-cm mark nearest to the floor and furthest from the conductor of the experiment, the test subject was told to prepare himself/herself to catch the ruler. The test subject, using his/her dominant hand, was told by the conductor of the experiment that he/she would drop the ruler at any moment in the next five seconds. After the ruler was dropped, the test subject must catch the ruler as quickly as possible. The process was repeated four times for each group member. The displacement of the ruler was recorded to be used in the formula, s = (v1)(t) + (1/2)(a)(t2), to calculate the total time elapsed between dropping and catching the ruler, or the reaction of time of the test subject; the data was recorded and averaged for later use in data tables. The second section of the experiment began with the process of finding a variable that could be applied to the control experiment with the hopes of having an effect on the reaction time of the test subject. The variable chosen was the application of prescription eyeglasses while catching the ruler; this variable would influence the ability to see the ruler dropping. Each of the other groups selected a different variable; during the actual “variable experimental trials”, each member of each group was to catch the ruler another three or so times, each with a different variable applied. The data was plugged into the formula, s = (v1)(t) + (1/2)(a)(t2), to reveal the reaction times of each person’s trials; the data was recorded and averaged for later use in data tables. The final section of the experiment was to create a survey question that would be applied to the experimental data to discover is there is a correlation between the two. The following question was asked, “How many times a day do you send text messages on average?” Survey response slips were printed out and distributed to each test subject with the possibility of answering: 0-25 times a day, 25-50 times a day, 50-75 times a day, and 100+ times a day. After the surveys were filled out, all of the data was compiled and organized into various separate tables and bar graphs. Data: Table 1: Class Data from Controlled Experiment Name Gender Control Reaction Time (s) Mr. Boylan Male 0.15 Aishvarya Female 0.24 Daniela Female 0.24 Kamil Male 0.2 Joe Male 0.37 Marie Female 0.38 Monique Female 0.14 Sal Male 0.08 Kelly Female 0.17 Tyler Male 0.22 Alex Female 0.07 Chris Male 0.18 Movses Male 0.2 Catherine Female 0.29 Adriana Female 0.42 Mary Grace Female 0.15 Anna Rose Female 0.21 Jovan Male 0.18 Shaminy Female 0.38 Sabrina Female 0.22 Kevin Male 0.21 Johnny Male 0.18 Brian Male 0.22 James Male 0.2 Mike Male 0.09 Table 2: Class Data for Experimental Variable Name Average Reaction Time Reaction time while wearing prescription glasses (s) 0.24 Ruler Displacement while wearing prescription glasses (m) 0.12 Aishvarya Daniela 0.24 0.17 0.17 Sal 0.08 0.05 0.07 Kamil 0.20 0.24 0.22 Joey 0.37 0.22 0.20 Marie 0.38 0.28 0.23 Kelly 0.17 Void* Void* Monique 0.14 0.25 0.22 Sabrina 0.22 0.11 0.13 Mike 0.09 0.15 0.17 0.15 Chris 0.18 0.23 0.21 Tyler 0.22 0.26 0.23 Alex 0.07 0.13 0.16 Shaminy 0.38 0.32 0.25 Brian 0.22 0.10 0.13 Catherine 0.29 0.33 0.25 Mary Grace 0.15 0.30 0.23 Anna Rose 0.21 0.26 0.24 Movses 0.20 0.11 0.15 Jovan 0.18 0.12 0.15 James 0.20 0.19 0.18 Johnny 0.18 0.09 0.14 Adriana 0.42 0.36 0.26 Mr. B 0.15 0.17 0.17 Kevin 0.21 0.35 0.27 *unable to participate due to health issues. Table 3: Survey Responses to Question: “How Many Times in an Average Day Do You Send Texts? Name Gender Average Control Reaction Time (s) Survey Responses 25-50 Mr. B Male 0.15 Aishvarya Female 0.24 Daniela Female 0.24 100+ 50-75 0-25 Kamil Male 0.20 Joe Male 0.37 Marie Female 0.38 Monique Female 0.14 Sal Male 0.08 Kelly Female 0.17 Tyler Male 0.22 Alex Female 0.07 100+ 0-25 0-25 25-50 100+ 100+ 100+ 0-25 Chris Male 0.18 Movses Male 0.20 Catherine Female 0.29 Adriana Female 0.42 Mary grace Female 0.15 50-75 0-25 50-75 0-25 0-25 Anna rose Female 0.21 Jovan Male 0.18 Shaminy Female 0.38 Sabrina Female 0.22 Kevin Male 0.21 Johnny Male 0.18 Brian Male 0.22 James Male 0.20 Mike Male 0.09 100+ 0-25 100+ 25-50 100+ 0-25 25-50 50-75 Bar Graph 1: Class Average Reaction Time Class Average (with Experimental Variable Applied) (s) 0.16 0.22 Class Control Average (s) 0 0.05 0.1 0.15 0.2 0.25 Class Control Average (s) Class Average (with Experimental Variable 0.22 0.16 Average Reaction Time Bar Graph 2: Average Control Reaction Times Grouped By Survey Response 0.19 0.24 0.16 0.24 0 0.05 0.1 0.15 (values in seconds) Discussion: 0.2 0.25 People who text 100+ times a day People who text 50-75 times a day People who text 25-50 times a day People who text 0-25 times a day After the data was compiled and the tables, charts, and trends were analyzed, the data shows that the hypothesis is rejected. The original hypothesis stated that the application of prescription glasses would make seeing the ruler dropping harder and would therefore cause the reaction times to be longer than in the control trials. The results show that the average class reaction time for the experimental trials was 0.16 seconds, whereas the average reaction time during the control trials was 0.22 seconds. The reason for this is mostly due to exposure to the test at hand. During the control experiment, the challenge of catching a falling ruler at a random moment was novelty and challenging. After many trials of practiced, the test subjects were already accustomed to catching the ruler in the quickest possible fashion. The manner in which the subjects caught the ruler and the strategies of the test subjects were in favor of the test subjects based upon the ruler. The survey responses did not show distinct enough differences to say that there is a direct correlation between texting and reaction time; for the most part though, those who text more often seem to have better reaction times than those who don’t text at all. Although the data seems extremely accurate, there are, in fact, several ways that the data/experiment could have had error. The most probable reason for error would be human error with regards to calculations. The calculations for finding the reaction time of each trial required a decent amount of work, and therefore left a decent amount of room for error. Another place for error is the actual displacement of the ruler because there is a high possibility that the displacement of the ruler could have been misread. Literature Cited: Benson, Tom (2009). “Scalars and Vectors” Accessed on March 21st, 2010: http://www.grc.nasa.gov/WWW/K-12/airplane/vectors.html Biology Online (2009). “Stimulus” Accessed on March 23rd, 2010: http://www.biology-online.org/dictionary/Stimulus Elert, Glenn (unk.). “Acceleration” Accessed on March 21st, 2010: http://physics.info/acceleration Elert, Glenn (unk.). “Distance vs. Displacement” Accessed on March 21st, 2010: http://physics.info/displacement/ Elert, Glenn (unk.). “Speed and Velocity” Accessed on March 21st, 2010: http://physics.info/velocity/ Farr, Gary (2002). “The Brain” Accessed on March 22nd, 2010: http://www.becomehealthynow.com/article/bodynervousadvanced/819/1/ Farr, Gary (2002). “Functions of the Nervous System” Accessed on March 22nd, 2010: http://www.becomehealthynow.com/article/bodynervousadvanced/823 Farr, Gary (2002). “The Peripheral Nervous System” Accessed on March 22nd, 2010: http://www.becomehealthynow.com/article/bodynervousadvanced/821/ Goddard, Jennifer (2009). “Study of Body Systems” Accessed on March 22nd, 2010: http://www.fuzz2buzz.com/en/mex/mind-map/hand-drawn/2516/study-body-systems Kosinski, Robert (2009). “A Literature Review on Reaction Time” Accessed on March 23rd, 2010: http://biology.clemson.edu/bpc/bp/Lab/110/reaction.htm Lerner, Ed and Brenda Wilmoth (2008). “Motion” Accessed via Gale Databases on March 21st, 2010: http://galenet.galegroup.com/servlet/SciRC?locID=fres78823&bi=SU&bt=%2522Motion %2522&c=1&t=1&ste=21&docNum=CV2644031485&st=b&tc=63&tf=32