SHORT COMMUNICATION Field observation of two males following a female in the Iberian lynx (Lynx pardinus) during the mating season Jose V. Lopez-Baoa,*, Alejandro Rodrıgueza, Enrique Alesb a Department of Conservation Biology, Estación Biológica de Donana, CSIC, Avda. Marı´a Luisa s/n, 41013 Seville, Spain B-80, 21760 Matalascanas, Spain b With the exception of lion (Panthera leo, L., 1758), cheetah (Acinonyx jubatus, Schreber, 1775), and domestic cat (Felis catus, L., 1758), which exhibit social behaviour (Schaller 1972; Macdonald et al. 1987; Caro 1994) felids are considered solitary species, where contacts between adults take place mainly during the mating season (Kleiman and Eisenberg 1973; Gittleman 1989; Caro 1994). The typical mating system of felids is polygyny (Komers and Brotherton 1997) although in some species it has been described as facultative monogamy (Poole 1995; Komers and Brotherton 1997). Other deviations from the general pattern have been reported with lower frequency (Clutton-Brock 1989; Weisbein and Mendelssohn 1990; Caro 1994; Zheltukhin 2003). The Iberian lynx (Lynx pardinus, Temminck, 1827) has been described as a solitary felid, where home ranges of adults of different sex overlap, but those of adults of the same sex are rather exclusive (Ferreras et al. 1997). Contacts between adult Iberian lynx of the same sex have been rarely recorded. In fact, Ferreras et al. (1997) found such occasions only in 1.14% from among 2186 pairs of simultaneous locations of two adult lynx of the same sex. The Iberian lynx mating system has been defined as being close to monogamy, although some tendency to polygyny was occasionally detected (Ferreras et al. 1997). In this paper, we report on a field observation of two Iberian lynx adult males following together an adult *Corresponding author. E-mail address: jvlb@ebd.csic.es (J.V. Lopez-Bao). female during the mating season. Females breed between 3 and 9 years, giving birth in March–April (Palomares et al. 2005), after a gestation of about 8 weeks. Female F120 (7 years) was observed together with male M136 (8 years, body mass ¼ 15.0 kg) and male M139 (3 years, body mass ¼ 13.5 kg). Genetic analyses indicate that adult males were not first- or second-degree relatives (unpublished data). The two males were resident in this area since December 2005. The exclusive-overlap index (Dunn and Gipson 1977) indicated that 75% of male home ranges were used exclusively by one individual (Fig. 1). F120 had lived in the study area for at least 5 years but, 5 days before the observation reported here, she was released into her home range after being one month in captivity recovering from several injuries. The observation was recorded on 16 February 2006 at 1:30 p.m. in Coto del Rey, Donana National Park (SW Iberian peninsula, 371100 N, 61230 W), an area inhabited by a lynx subpopulation which is normally composed of 3 lynx breeding territories (Palomares et al. 2001). At the time of the observation, all lynx in the area were radiotracked, and the adult cohort was made of three males and three females. Once found by their radio-signals, all three lynx were followed visually from a distance of 500 m. At first observation all lynx were 10 m from each other. The two males stood up on a path being in attitude of non-direct confrontation, with their beards open and their back bended with end hairs. Both males emitted grunts of low intensity and scent-marked repeatedly with urine on the vegetation. This behaviour Coto del Rey area N 16-02-2006, 1:53 pm 16-02-2006, 1:30 pm National Park 16-02-2006, 2:27 pm Females males 1000 0 1000 m Fig. 1. Path made by F120 (continuous segmented line) and males M136 ans M139 (broken segmented line). The 50% kernel home ranges, built with independent radio-tracking fixes between December 2005 and May 2006, are shown for F120 and M139 (western couple of highly overlapping ranges), and for M136 and another adult female (southern couple of highly overlapping ranges). Note that the male path was outside the overlap area of male ranges. was displayed during 10 min in the presence of the female, which lay down on the ground. Afterwards, the female began to walk and both males followed her without interrupting their gestual confrontation, and always maintaining the same order, the older male followed by the younger male. This consistent order might suggest a possible dominance of the first male towards the second. M136 often stopped and showed the described display to M139, which also stopped and sat down observing the other male. At this time, the female, moved with her beards gathered and her tail hidden between her legs. The three lynx, always in the same order and behaviour moved together for 2 km using firebreaks and pine plantations without bushes that allowed us to keep lynx continuously on sight. At this point, the female went away from the males, which continued walking together exhibiting the same display for about 1 km more (Fig. 1). The next day, 17 February 2006, the female and the older male were found together by radio-tracking. The younger male was then 800 m away from them. On another occasion, on 22 February 2006 the female and the younger male were observed copulating, while the older male was 1500 m away from them. Weisbein and Mendelssohn (1990) described how a female caracal (Caracal caracal, Schreber, 1776) copulated with several males in an order that seemed determinated by the age and the physical condition of males. In our case, the oldest and heavier male was apparently the first detected with the female, and the youngest the second. Only a few records of solitary felids have been reported in which males form noncooperative associations to follow a single female during the mating season (Weisbein and Mendelssohn 1990; Caro 1994, Zheltukhin 2003). Zheltukhin (2003), for example, described how one female Eurasian lynx (Lynx lynx, L., 1758) was followed by five males during the oestrus. A captive female Eurasian lynx accepted copulation with several males (Naidenko and Erofeeva 2004). The distribution of females often determines the distribution and social behaviour of males (Emlen and Oring 1977; Wrangham 1980; Gittleman 1989; Caro 1994). In felids, the spatial distribution and density of females determine the mating system (Komers and Brotherton 1997), where polygyny is the rule and facultative monogamy may happen at high population densities (Wassmer et al. 1988, Poole 1995, Ferreras et al. 1997; Say et al. 1999). The apparent dispute of the two Iberian lynx males might have been elicited by a new resource, an adult female suddenly available during the mating season at the boundary between male territories (Fig. 1). The strategy observed here and in the cases of Caracals and Eurasian lynx described before J (solitary felids, relatively high density of males), suggests that mating systems in felids could present exceptions to monogamy and polygyny. Cases of polyandry might probably occur in association with high densities of males and/or low densities of females. Acknowledgements This research was funded by the Spanish Ministry of Education through the Grant CGL2004-00346/BOS, and by the Spanish Ministry of the Environment under the National Parks research programme (Grant 17/ 2005). J.V. Lopez-Bao was supported by the Ministry of Education and A. Rodrıguez by Consejerıa de Innovacion, Junta de Andalucıa. References Caro, T.M., 1994. Cheetahs of the Serengeti Plains: Group Living in an Asocial Species. University of Chicago Press, Chicago. Clutton-Brock, T.H., 1989. Mammalian mating systems. Proc. R. Soc. London B 236, 339–372. Dunn, J.E., Gipson, P.S., 1977. Analysis of radiotelemetry data in studies of home range. Biometrics 33, 85–101. Emlen, S.T., Oring, L.W., 1977. Ecology, sexual selection, and the evolution of mating systems. Science 197, 215–223. Ferreras, P., Beltran, J.F., Aldama, J.J., Delibes, M., 1997. Spatial organization and land tenure system of the endangered Iberian lynx (Lynx pardinus). J. Zool. London 243, 163–189. Gittleman, J.L., 1989. Carnivore Behaviour, Ecology, and Evolution. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, New York. Kleiman, D.G., Eisenberg, J.F., 1973. Comparisons of canid and felid social systems from an evolutionary perspective. Anim. Behav. 21, 637–659. Komers, P.E., Brotherton, P.N.M., 1997. Female space use is the best predictor of monogamy in mammals. Proc. R. Soc. London B 264, 1621-1270. Macdonald, D.W., Apps, P.J., Carr, G.M., Kerby, G., 1987. Social dynamics, nursing coalitions and infanticide among farm cats, Felis catus. Adv. Ethol. 28, 1–64. Naidenko, S.V., Erofeeva, M.N., 2004. Eurasian lynx reproduction and females reproductive strategies. Zool. Zh. 83 (2), 261–269. Palomares, F., Delibes, M., Ferreras, P., Fedriani, J.M., Calzada, J., Revilla, E., 2001. Spatial ecology of iberian lnx and abundance of european rabbits in southwestern spain. Wildlife Monogr. 148, 1–36. Palomares, F., Revilla, E., Calzada, J., Fernandez, N., Delibes, M., 2005. Reproduction and pre-dispersal survival of Iberian lynx in a subpopulation of the Donana National Park. Biol. Conserv. 122, 53–59. Poole, K.G., 1995. Spatial organization of a lynx population. Can. J. Zool. 73, 632–641. Say, L., Pontier, D., Natoli, E., 1999. A genetic analysis of male reproductive success in two contrasted enviroments in the domestic cats (Felis catus L.). Proc. R. Soc. London B 266, 2071–2074. Schaller, G.B., 1972. The Serengeti Lion. A Study of Predator– Prey Relations. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago. Wassmer, D.A., Guenter, D., Layne, J.N., 1988. Ecology of the bobcat in South Central Florida. Bull. Florida State Mus. Biol. Sci. 33, 159–228. Weisbein, Y., Mendelssohn, H., 1990. The biology and ecology of the caracal (Felis caracal) in the Aravah Valley of Israel. Cat News 12, 20–22. Wrangham, R.W., 1980. An ecological model of femalebonded primate groups. Behaviour 75, 262–297. Zheltukhin, A.S., 2003. The Center of the Russian Plain. In: Matyushkin, E.N., Vaisfeld, M.A. (Eds.), The Lynx. Nauka, Moscow, pp. 137–168.