Jose A. Donazar Alejandro Travaini Olga Ceballos Alejandro Rodriguez Miguel Delibes Fernando Hiraldo Effects of sex-associated competitive asymmetries on foraging group structure and despotic distribution in Andean condors Abstract Phenotype-limited interference models assume competitive asymmetries among conspecifics and unequal sharing of resources. Their main prediction is a correlation between dominance status and patch quality: dominant individuals should preferentially exploit better-quality habitats. We tested assumptions and predictions of the phenotype-limited interference model in Andean condors (Vultur gryphus), a New World vulture with strong sexual size dimorphism (males are 30—40% heavier than females). We recorded searching birds in habitats differing in quality: mountains and plains. We also observed scavenging behaviour at 20 sheep carcasses, and videotaped 5 of them. Intraspecific hierarchy at carcasses was based on size: males dominated females and, within each sex, older birds dominated younger ones. Adult males and juvenile females occupied extreme positions in the feeding hierarchy. Aggression was directed at those individuals belonging to lower hierarchical levels. In high-quality areas (mountains), more condors arrived at carcasses. Juvenile females were more often observed searching in low-quality areas (plains), far from breeding areas and main roost sites. GLM analyses of individual behaviour showed that the hierarchy did not influence time of arrival, but low-ranking individuals spent more time at carcasses, especially if the number of condors at arrival was high. Additionally, low-ranking condors spent less time feeding at carcasses J.A. Donazar ()) A. Travaini 1 A. Rodriguez M. Delibes F. Hiraldo Department of Applied Biology Estacion Biologica de Donana (C.S.I.C.), Avda. Ma Luisa s.n. E-41013 Sevilla, Spain e-mail: Donazar@ebd03.ebd.csic.es, Tel.: +34-95-4232340 Fax: +34-95-4621125 O. Ceballos Grupo de Estudios Biologicos Ugarra, Carlos III 19 40 I, E-31002 Pamplona, Spain Present address: 1 Centro de Investigaciones de Puerto Deseado, Universidad Nacional de la Patagonia Austral, Almirante Brown y Colon s/n, Puerto Deseado 9050, St. Cruz, Argentina when individuals of higher hierarchical levels were present. On the other hand, the number of condors present had a positive effect on feeding rates of dominant individuals, probably because of a reduction in individual vigilance. These results support most of the assumptions and predictions of the phenotype-limited distribution model, although a spatial truncated distribution between phenotypes was not observed. Asymmetric feeding pay-off, unequal parental roles and sexual selection constraints could favour sexual divergence in body size in Andean condors. Key words Competitive asymmetries Despotic distribution Foraging behaviour Vultur gryphus Introduction Cooperation in searching for and manipulating food has evolved many times within vertebrates (Wilson 1975). Individual advantages of foraging in groups are related to the increased probability of detecting food, since the area surveyed by groups is larger than that covered by a single animal, and individuals have the opportunity to observe other conspecifics feeding or displaying behaviours related to feeding (Ward and Zahavi 1973; Krebs 1974; Caraco 1981; Pulliam and Caraco 1984; Clark and Mangel 1986; Huntingford and Turner 1987; Benkman 1988). Moreover, when food items are large in size, food manipulation (killing, dismembering and defence against interspecific competitors) can be better achieved among conspecifics than by single individuals (Schaller 1972). Finally, foraging in groups increases the probability of predator detection and avoidance (Gluck 1986; Lima and Dill 1990). On the other hand, the main costs of group foraging have usually been related to decreased food intake per individual as group size increases (Vickery et al. 1991). Different patterns of food sharing among conspecifics have been proposed. The ideal-free distribution model (Fretwell and Lucas 1970; Fretwell 1972) assumes that 56 individuals have similar competitive abilities and move freely among patches in relation to the reward expected for each of them. Consequently, better patches will receive more individuals but the reward would be similar in each visited food site. Phenotype-limited models consider the role of unequal competitive abilities (asymmetries) among individuals (Parker and Sutherland 1986; Milinski 1988; Milinski and Parker 1991; Lindstrom and Ranta 1993). The asymmetries are mainly based on age and body size differences: larger and older individuals dominate in interactions at feeding grounds (Bautista et al. 1995). Competence asymmetries among individuals could have important effects on group structure, rate of food intake and individual spatial distribution in social foraging species (see among others Parker and Sutherland 1986; Milinski 1988; Milinski and Parker 1991; Rita et al. 1996). Studies testing interference models are scarce (but see Monaghan 1980; Goss-Custard et al. 1984; Bautista et al. 1995; Milinski et al. 1995). Among vertebrates, vultures are excellent candidates for studying the social foraging implications of despotic competence (Kirk and Gosler 1994). They exploit animal carcasses, an unpredictable and ephemeral resource; carcasses are located through processes such as information transfer at information centres and local enhancement (Buckley 1996). Individual foragers gather at feeding grounds and strong intraspecific competition arises (Mundy 1982; Wallace and Temple 1987). Morphological constraints and k-related life history traits make vultures appropriate subjects to compare competitive asymmetries related to age since many species experience long periods before sexual maturity. In addition, although intraspecific variation in body size is rare among vultures it can be found among the subspecies coexisting in wintering quarters (Kirk and Houston 1995) or between sexes in the Andean condor Vultur gryphus) (Del Hoyo et al. 1994). The objectives of this paper were as follows. (1) To test the existence of intraspecific competitive asymmetries in Andean condors during scavenging activities. In this species, males weigh 36—37% more than females (11—15 kg vs 8—11 kg; Del Hoyo et al. 1994), and sexual maturity is reached after 6 years of age (Del Hoyo et al. 1994), which can lead to strong intraspecific feeding hierarchies. (2) To study the effects of competitive asymmetries on social foraging strategies, focusing on foraging group structure and foraging rewards. We tested the phenotype-limited interference model (Sutherland and Parker 1985; Ranta et al. 1993; Rita et al. 1996). Model assumptions are that phenotypes differ in dominance status, that pay-off for subordinates decreases when the number of dominants increase, and that relative pay-offs of phenotypes change between patches. The main prediction is a correlation between dominance status and patch quality, with stable and truncated distributions of phenotypes between patches (dominants would be concentrated exclusively in patches of higher quality). Foraging activities were studied in two areas of different quality: mountains (high) and plains (low). In addition, comparisons were made in a single habitat (mountain) during two seasons with different foraging quality: spring (low) and summer (high). Methods Study area and sites This study was conducted in the Neuquen Province, northern Argentinean Patagonia, in an area with a radius of 60—70 km centred in Junin de los Andes (39057’S and 71005’W, 780 m a.s.l.). The weather is cold and dry with a pronounced gradient of rainfall from 800 mm average annually in the mountains to 300 mm on the eastern steppes. The region belongs to the Patagonian phytogeographic province, Western District (Cabrera 1976). The Andean condor, once widely distributed along the Cordilleran Range from Venezuela to Tierra del Fuego, is now rare in most of its range but still relatively abundant in the study area (Del Hoyo et al. 1994). Two zones with different quality of foraging habitats were considered for our study. The criteria for quality were: (1) the distance to Andean condor breeding and roost sites and (2) the possibility to safely exploit the carcasses. Condors are birds at the limit of flight capacity due to their high wing loading (McGahan 1973). Large adult males experience difficulties taking off after feeding (Palermo 1983); occasionally, birds have to climb steep slopes on foot until they are high enough to fly (personal observations). Hence, flat areas are risky for condors since the probability of predation increases. Carnivores have been seen to kill vultures feeding at carrion (Donazar and Ceballos 1988; Mundy et al. 1992) and the capture or killing of scavenging condors by humans is (or has been) not rare (McGahan 1972; Palermo 1983). Carcass availability is not a primary component of habitat quality since carrion abundance does not vary within our study area. Sheep carcasses are the main food resource for condors in the study area (72.1% of appearances, n = 179 pellets, personal observation). Availability of sheep carcasses is very high throughout the whole study area where tens of thousands of sheep head are raised annually (personal observation). We considered two different quality areas. High quality (mountain areas) The mountains (spurs of the Andean range) occupy the northern and western portions of the study area, with the highest peaks reaching 3600 m. Condor roosts and breeding areas are between 1190 and 2000 m a.s.l. (personal observation). Human disturbance is very low: there are some isolated human settlements and a few non-paved roads. Information from sheep farmers suggests that carcass availability is similar in spring and summer. Flight conditions for foraging condors, however, should be much better in the summer due to warmer weather and thermal formation (Pennycuick 1972); consequently, the probability of gaining carcasses (quality of the habitat) would increase during the summer. Low quality (plains) The plains are located in the south-eastern part of the study area at 500—700 m a.s.l., and are dissected by steep valleys. There is a single condor roost site on an isolated hill at 1000 m a.s.l. which is regularly visited by the birds. The human population is larger than in the mountains: there is a paved road and a town with 5000 inhabitants (Junin de los Andes). Foraging behaviour To study the foraging behaviour of Andean condors, we placed sheep carcasses both in a mountain area (Puerto de Pilolil, 1160 m 57 a.s.l.), and in a plain zone (Mendana hills, 840 m a.s.l.). Observations were made between 10 November—20 December (spring) of 1991 and 1992, and between 12 January—10 February (summer) of 1995. To establish the existence of competitive asymmetries, three age classes were considered: juvenile, subadult and adult condors. Adult birds have black plumage with white secondaries and upperwing coverts, and a conspicuous white neck ruff. Juveniles are dull brown (Del Hoyo et al. 1994). Subadult birds look like adults but their secondaries and upperwing coverts are in ochre or grey shades, never pure white. Males were distinguished from females mainly by the presence of a comb of variable size. Unlike Wallace and Temple (1987), we did not attempt to distinguish more age categories: according to our field experience, it is extremely difficult to detect less showy characters (such as iris colour) from several hundred metres away, and impossible to determine accurately the age of each individual when numerous condors gather at carrion, with the birds constantly shifting position. The data recorded in the course of 309 h of systematic observations during spring (66 in the mountains and 243 on the plains) were used to obtain information on the age and sex of the birds that were exploring the ground. Both age class and sex of all the flying condors observed while travelling in the study area during the whole study period were also recorded. Subadults were pooled with juveniles, since assessing characters such as a white neck ruff in flying birds at medium or long distances is difficult. Most of the birds were observed during the spring (n = 56 in the mountains and n = 61 on the plains), while 12 and 17 birds were observed in the summer season in the mountains and on the plains, respectively. The samples of the two periods did not show significant differences in relation to age and sex frequencies (N x M tests, P > 0.05), so we pooled data from spring and summer for further analyses. Condor scavenging behaviour was studied by observing 18 sheep carcasses, 7 placed in the mountain area and 11 on the plains site during the spring season. Two people watched simultaneously, with telescopes, from a car or hides at a minimum of 300 m away from the carcasses, not interfering with the behaviour of the birds. We recorded the age class, the sex, the arrival and departure times for every individual, and all the interactions among individuals, identifying the age and sex of actors and receivers and whether or not the displacement attempt was successful. We also examined 14 h of video film of five sheep carcasses in the mountain area during the summer (January and February 1995) and one carcass on the plain during the spring (December 1992). We recorded: (a) all condor arrivals and departures until the carcasses were fully consumed by the condors or any other avian scavenger species; (b) the total time that every individual spent in feeding activities: every 10 s we recorded if the individual was (1) feeding: i.e. pecking the carcass or with its head towards the carcass below the horizontal level, (2) watching: the condor was less than 1 m away from the carcass, with its head erect above the horizontal level, (3) out: the condor was more than 1 m away from the carrion. Data analysis Determination of feeding hierarchies, and the comparisons between frequencies of condors scavenging in the three studied zones and between scavenging and searching condors were performed through univariate analyses following Wells and King (1980) and Siegel and Castellan (1988). Hierarchies among sex and age categories were studied, on the basis of the observed interactions among the individuals sharing the carcasses, following similar studies on scavenging birds (Alvarez et al. 1976; Wallace and Temple 1987; Hiraldo et al. 1991; Kirk and Houston 1995). For each category we calculated the percentage of aggressive actions performed and won against the other categories. To determine the existence of dominance we compared this frequency with that expected by chance (goodness-of-fit test); one-tailed x2 and Fisher exact tests were employed following Senar et al. (1989). Directionality was studied in the same data. As pseudoreplication is a common problem in studies based on unmarked individuals (Kirk and Houston 1995), we tried to reduce the variance due to individual differences using the filmed carcasses and taking into account the interactions directed for each individual separately. For each bird we obtained one index for each age-sex category with which it interacted. The index checked if the rate of aggression performed by the bird towards individuals of each category was different to those expected by chance. The index was calculated as (number of aggressions directed to category A/total number of aggressions performed)/(number of condors belonging to category A/total number of condors). Index values above 1 represent frequencies of aggression above those expected. When only birds of two age-sex categories were present we did not perform calculations, as the potential aggressor had no choice. The analyses of arrival time, time spent at carcasses and time spent feeding were performed through generalized linear models (GLMs; Nelder and Wedderburn 1972; Dobson 1983; McCullagh and Nelder 1983). It allows us to determine a wide range of relationship between the response and explanatory variables and the use of different error formulations according to data characteristics. We fitted each explanatory variable to the observed data following a modification of the traditional forward stepwise procedure which conducts, through the principle of parsimony, the most minimal adequate model able to describe the data set (Donazar et al. 1993; Bustamante et al. 1997). If a preliminary exploratory analysis suggested a curved response we fitted a quadratic function and a cubic term to improve the model. We also tested square root and logarithmic transformations if the data suggested multiplicative effects. Model criticism was made following Crawley (1993). We characterized each of the response variables as follows. The arrival time was calculated for each condor as the difference (in minutes) between its arrival and that of the first condor. These values were log-transformed and a GLM model with an identity link and assuming normal error was fitted (equivalent to a multiple linear regression). Time spent by individual condors at carcasses was measured in minutes and log-transformed. Finally, the time that the condors spent feeding was calculated by examining the filmed carcasses. The variable was the frequency of 10-s sequences in which the bird was observed feeding. We considered a binomial distribution of errors (denominator = 1) and used a logistic link (equivalent to a logistic regression). The explanatory variables were characterized as follows: (1) Hierarchy: following our results (see below), four categories were considered, from the first dominant to the last subordinate — adult male, juvenile male, adult female, juvenile female. (2) Carcass: each carrion was considered as a different sample. (3) Zone (habitat quality): following the considerations for seasonal, physiographic and food availability conditions we determined three factors — mountain in spring, mountain in summer and plain in spring. (4) Number of condors present when each individual arrived. (5) Number of condors of higher hierarchical level present when each individual arrived. (6) Presence or absence of individuals of higher hierarchical levels. (7) Maximum number of condors simultaneously present during the time that each individual spent at the carcass. (8) Maximum number of condors of higher hierarchical levels simultaneously present during the time spent by the individual at the carcass. By considering the above variables, through GLM models we minimized the pseudorreplication problem linked to the similarity of conditions taking place at each carcass and day, introducing as factors both the carcass and habitat quality variable (zone). Results Numbers of condors and foraging habitat selection During the 66 h of systematic observation in mountain zones, a total of 33 searching condors (0.50 individuals/h) 58 were detected, while during the 243 h of observation on the plain, 51 condors were detected (0.21 individuals/h). A further 51 condors were observed in a non-systematic way. Altogether, we accurately determined the sex and age of 117 birds. In the mountains, 56 searching individuals were observed, of which 21 (37.5%) were adult males, 5 (8.9%) adult females, 19 (33.9%) young males, and 11 (19.6%) young females. On the plain, 61 condors were observed, of which 15 (24.5%) were adult males, 4 (6.6%) adult females, 13 (21.3%) young males, and 29 (47.5%) young females. The comparison of frequencies of each sex and age category observed in both habitats showed significant differences (N x M exact test, P = 0.002). The partitioning of the contingency table revealed that there were no significant differences between the frequencies of male adults and male juveniles (x2 = 0.007, P > 0.9) and between males (adults plus juveniles) and adult females (x2 = 0.034, P > 0.8); however, there were differences between juvenile females and the remaining categories (x2 = 10.100, P < 0.01). In spring and in the mountains a total of 158 condors were observed to land and feed on all 7 sheep carcasses. The total number of landing birds ranged between 5—38 (mean = 20.6, SD = 12.8): 79 (50.0%) were adult males, 13 (8.2%) adult females, 29 (18.3%) young males and 37 (23.4%) young females. The maximum number of simultaneously observed condors ranged between 2—18 (mean = 7.2, SD = 5.2). On the plain, 54 condors arrived at 8 (72.7%) of the 11 monitored sheep carcasses. The total number of landing birds ranged between 2—16 (mean = 7.1, SD = 5.3). An additional 12 scavenging condors were also observed at non-focal carcasses found during this study. In total, of the 66 observed individuals, 18 were adult males (27.3%), 12 adult females (18.2%), 15 (22.7%) young males and 21 (31.8%) young females. The maximum number of simultaneously observed condors ranged between 1—5 (mean = 2.6, SD = 1.5). During the summer and in the mountains, we observed a total of 35 condors at the 5 sheep carcasses (mean = 7.0, SD = 5.4, range: 1—14); 8 (22.9%) were adult males, 11 (31.4%) adult females, 6 (17.1%) young males and 10 (28.6%) young females. The maximum number of simultaneously observed birds at a carcass ranged between 1—6 (mean = 3.2, SD = 1.9). Application of statistical comparisons to the above data revealed that, in spring, significantly more condors landed at carcasses in the mountains than on the plain (Mann-Whitney U-tests, Z = 7.0, P = 0.026); in addition, more condors landed in the mountains in the spring than in the summer, (Z = 5.0, P = 0.048). The same trends were observed regarding the maximum number of condors present at carcasses: spring mountain versus spring plain (Z = 4.5, P = 0.007), spring mountain versus summer mountain (Z = 3.5, P = 0.018). Finally, by comparing sex and age frequencies of searching birds with those of feeding condors (see above and Fig. 1), no significant differences were detected within the mountains (x2 = 6.426, df = 3, P = 0.0926) or plain x2 = 5.507, df = 3, P = 0.1382). It was notable that the male/female proportion in searching birds seen in the mountains during the spring differed markedly from 1:1 for adult birds (21 males/5 females, binomial test, P = 0.004), but not for juveniles (19/11, P = 0.204). The same trend was detected in birds landing at carrion: adults (80/13, P = 0.0001), juveniles (29/37, P = 0.390). On the plain, the trends were similar: searching birds — adults 15/4 (P = 0.020), juveniles 13/29 (P = 0.192); scavenging birds — adults 18/12 (P = 0.363), juveniles 15/21 (P = 0.406). In contrast, during the summer and in the mountains, the proportion of sexes did not differ from 1:1 either in landing adults (8/11, P = 0.648) or juveniles (6/10, P = 0.454). This means that the fraction of adult females increased from spring to summer in birds scavenging in mountain areas (N x M exact test, P = 0.00013). Competitive ability according to sex and age The results obtained from all the interactions showed the existence of a distinct hierarchy (Fig. 2). Adult males directed and won aggressions against all other age and sex classes; subadult males dominated juvenile males, adult females and juvenile females (no simultaneous meetings between subadult males and subadult females took place); juvenile males dominated females of all age categories; adult females dominated over subadult and juvenile females; subadult females dominated over juvenile females, while juvenile females did not dominate any other age category. Males clearly dominated females, independent of age, and within each sex group, older birds were dominants. Regarding directionality, it could be observed (Fig. 3) that adult males attacked the subordinate categories with a similar frequency (Kruskall-Wallis test, KW = 3.073, P = 0.238). In contrast, adult and juvenile females directed their attacks significantly more to juvenile females (KW = 7.200, P = 0.006, and KW = 13.524, P = 0.000, respectively). These results clearly indicate that condors direct their aggression to those individuals of lower hierarchy, except, clearly, those at the end of the hierarchical scale (juvenile females) which only attack individuals of their own category. When we investigated if the rate of aggression (numbers/minute) received by an individual condor of low-ranking categories (females) depended of the number of dominant individuals present, we did not find any significant correlation, and the trends were contradictory: adult females (rs = 0.387, P = 0.327, n = 8), juvenile females (rs = —0.313, P = 0.321, n = 12). Data recorded for juvenile males were too low to conduct a similar analysis. The GLM model on individual arrival time accounted for the 36.3% of the initial deviance, and did not show data overdispersion. Only two variables were included into the model: carcass (70.7% of the explained deviance) and number of condors at arrival (29.3%) 59 Fig. 1 Percentage of condors that were observed searching for and feeding at carrion in mountain and plain areas during spring (AM adult males, JM juvenile males, AF adult females, JF juvenile females) (Table 1). This indicates that time of arrival is highly variable between carcasses and late-arriving individuals encountered a higher number of condors. Number of condors of higher hierarchical level entered initially into the model but its effect disappeared when the number of condors was included. The GLM model dealing with the time spent by individual condors at carcasses accounted for 66.2% of the original deviance and did not show data overdispersion. Variables included were carcass (66.2% of the explained deviance), number of condors at arrival (24.3%), and hierarchy (12.8%) (Table 2). These results indicate that time spent by individual birds was mainly related to the particular characteristics of each carcass. Additionally, within each carcass, the hierarchy had an important influence: dominant individuals (adult males) spent the minimum time at carcasses whereas juvenile females stayed there longer (Fig. 4). Juvenile males and adult females showed a similar trend, intermediate between the two extreme hierarchies. The differences among hierarchies increased when the number of condors present rose; thus, subordinate individuals (juvenile females) were very sensitive and the time they spent at carcasses increased more sharply than in the other categories. The best model obtained for time spent feeding by individual condors accounted for 25.7% of the initial deviance and did not show data overdispersion (Table 3). It included four variables: carcass (28.4% of the explained deviance), hierarchy (14.6%), presence/ absence of condors of higher hierarchy (10.2%), and maximum number of condors during the individual stay (7.2%). Additionally, we found a significant interaction 60 Fig. 2 Number of displacement attempts at carcasses recorded among condors of each age and sex category [x-axis target (receiver) individuals: AM adult males, SBM subadult males, JM juvenile males, AF adult females, SBF subadult females, JF juvenile females). For each bar, successful attempts are in black and unsuccessful ones in white. Note the different y-axis scales for adult males and the other categories. Significance of one-tailed chi-square and Fisher exact tests are shown above bars ( P < 0.05, P < 0.01, P < 0.001, n.s. not significant; samples with n < 4 were not considered for statistical tests) between hierarchy and maximum number of condors (14.6% of the explained deviance). The representation of the predicted feeding probabilities for a given carcass (the first factor included in the analysis) showed that dominant condors spent more time feeding than subordinates. The hierarchical differences were more acute when the number of condors present was high and when there were individuals of higher hierarchical levels (Fig. 5). It is worth noting that subordinate individuals such as juvenile females spent more time feeding than more dominant birds when the number of condors and competitors was very low. 61 Table 2 GLM model for time spent at carcasses by individual condors (log-transformed), using normal error and identity link. The values for the explanatory variable carcassss (factor levels = 20) are not shown Parameter estimate Constant Juvenile males Adult females Juvenile females Number of condors at arrival Residual deviance df —2.030 0.418 0.200 0.413 0.073 SE 0.219 0.151 0.163 0.140 0.014 112.2 200 When the condors stopped feeding they abandoned the carcass but remained nearby. This was shown by significant correlations detected between the frequency of feeding and time spent away from the carcasses (adult males: rs = —0.783, P = 0.003, n = 12; juvenile males: rs = —0.476, P = 0.243, n = 8; adult females: rs = —0.563, P = 0.036, n = 14; juvenile females: rs = —0.685, P = 0.014, n = 12). On the other hand, when we correlated the frequency of feeding and time spent in vigilance attitudes (watching over) we found a significant trend only for adult females (adult males: rs = 0.007, P = 0.983, n = 12; juvenile males: rs = —0.048, P = 0.935, n = 8; adult females: rs = —0.644, P = 0.013, n = 14; juvenile females: rs = 0.172, P = 0.594, n = 12). Discussion Dominance patterns Our results showed clear individual differences in competitive ability, which is a primary assumption of pheFig. 3 Directionality of aggressions performed by individual condors. The index (y-axis) represents the value (mean ± SD) of a fraction which evaluated the observed rate of aggressions with respect to the total number of potential receptors (AM adult males, JM juvenile males, AF adult females, JF juvenile females). Index values above 1 represent frequencies of aggressions above those expected. Sample sizes are shown above and below bars Table 1 GLM model for time of arrival at carcasses of individual condors (log-transformed), using normal error and identity link. The values for the explanatory variable carcassss (factor levels = 17) are not shown Parameter estimate Constant Log (number of condors at arrival + 1) Residual deviance df 2.271 0.747 283.8 195 SE 0.461 0.131 Fig. 4 Time spent at carcasses by individual condors as predicted by a GLM with normal error and identity link. Explanatory variables are hierarchy and number of condors at arrival (AM adult males, JM juvenile males, AF adult females, JF juvenile females) 62 Table 3 GLM model for time feeding at carcasses by individual condors in relation to the total time of observation, using binomial error (denominator = 1) and logistic link. The values for the explanatory variable carcassss (factor levels = 5) are not shown Parameter estimate Constant Juvenile males Adult females Juvenile females No competitors Maximum number of condors Hierarchy x maximum number of condors Residual deviance df SE 1.173 —0.380 —0.231 0.439 0.685 1.078 0.133 0.127 0.107 0.263 0.110 0.219 —0.259 0.070 735.4 35 notype-limited models (Parker and Sutherland 1986). Intraspecific hierarchy at carcasses is based on size: Fig. 5 Frequency of time spent feeding by individual condors as predicted by a GLM with binomial error and logistic link. Explanatory variables are hierarchy, maximum number of condors present during the time spent by the individual at the carcass, and presence/absence of competitors (AM adult males, JM juvenile males, AF adult females, JF juvenile females) larger males were dominant over the smaller females. The effect of age was apparent only within each sex: older birds dominate younger birds. The analyses of interaction directionality and intensity of supplantation attempts revealed that interactions were preferentially directed at lower-ranked individuals within the feeding hierarchy. Very few interactions were aimed at adult males, and when they did occur they came from other males. This hierarchical pattern partially differs from that described by Wallace and Temple (1987) when studying intraspecific condor fighting in Peru. They stated that, on occasion, age could be an important component in hierarchy settlement — a female could dominate a male more than one year her junior. Our findings agree more with studies where the supplanting behaviour in carrion-eating bird species showing sexual dimorphism is related to intraspecific hierarchies defined by body size (Knight and Knight 1988; Garcelon 1990; Hiraldo et al. 1991, Kirk and Houston 1995). In nondimorphic vulture species, intraspecific hierarchy seems 63 to be determined by age and some behavioural factors dominant individuals (i.e. adult males) is higher in the such as hunger (Koford 1953; Anderson and Horwitz mountains (better-quality area). From the results ob1979; Mundy 1982; Elosegi 1989). tained modelling the feeding-rate intake, we could reasonably expect a reduction in the relative pay-offs for subordinate birds (mainly juvenile females) in these Hierarchy and foraging pay-offs zones. Increasing interference in larger conspecific groups and subsequent reduction of food intake by The first assumption of phenotype-limited models is that subordinate individuals has been observed in other sopay-offs for subordinates decrease when the number of cial-foraging species such as turkey vultures (Cathartes dominant condors increases. Our results support this, aura, Kirk and Houston 1995), herring gulls (Larus ar since subordinate individuals obtained less food in the gentatus, Monaghan 1980; Monaghan et al. 1986), oyspresence of dominant birds, as has been observed in tercatchers (Haematopus ostralegus, Goss-Custard et al. many other social-foraging species (see among others: 1984), and mute swans (Cygnus olor, Milinski et al. Baker et al. 1981; Heinrich 1988, 1994; Senar et al. 1989; 1995). Senar 1994; Kirk and Houston 1995 and references therein). In our case, however, and contrary to the observations of other authors (Balph 1977; Bekoff and Habitat segregation Scott 1989), we did not find a direct relationship between the number of competitors and the frequency of ag- The main prediction of the phenotype-limited model is a gression directed to subordinate birds. This seems to be positive relationship between dominance status and due to the fact that when competitors are present, ex- patch quality. Our results support the idea that despotic tremely subordinate condors, especially juvenile females, competence determines patch segregation in the studied remain at the edge of the scavenging group and avoid region. Extreme dominants (adult males) search for food becoming involved in aggressive encounters (see also and scavenge mainly in high-quality areas (mountain) Alonso et al. 1997). In fact, our results show that time whereas extreme subordinates (juvenile females) forage spent feeding and time spent away from the carcass are mainly on the plains. Similar segregation patterns have inversely correlated. This may also explain why subor- been described for other socially foraging species (Modinates tended to spend more time at carcasses when the naghan 1980; Goos-Custard et al. 1984; Bautista et al. number of condors present was high, whereas more 1995; Milinski et al. 1995). As was found in these studdominant individuals, such as adult males, are almost ies, our distribution of phenotypes was not truncated unaffected. A similar pattern of time maximization has between patches, contrary to the predictions of the been described, at the interspecific level, in other scav- model. Our data show, however, that adult males search enging guilds (Hiraldo et al. 1991). Such a strategy by on the plain more frequently than juvenile females extremely subordinate condors would be facilitated by search in the mountains; this would fit a partial truntheir higher flying capabilities, which would permit them cationss (Milinski et al. 1995). This could be due to many to remain for longer periods of time at carcasses. factors, such as imperfect habitat knowledge, unstable We detected a positive effect of the number of con- resource distribution and intraspecific kleptoparasitism dors present on the individual rate of food intake. As (Sutherland and Parker 1986). In addition, other factors occurs at the interspecific level in other scavenger as- such as the presence of dominants (see Houston 1988), semblages (Alvarez et al. 1976), the presence of a larger the amount of carrion available, and the hunger level of number of conspecifics probably determines some feed- the individual could influence the probability of landing ing advantages that compensate for the inhibition ef- at a carcass. In fact it is well known that food-stressed fects. A larger number of condors can better dismember individuals cover larger foraging distances and visit the cadaver, permitting access to a larger amount of suboptimal areas (Village 1982; Bloom et al. 1993, Tella food. Additionally, larger group sizes can reduce the et al., in press). Thus, dominant condors which have not amount of time dedicated to vigilance, as has been ob- detected carcasses could visit plain areas, but the reverse served in other social foraging species (Gluck 1986; Li- (extreme subordinates visiting carcasses) in the mounma and Dill 1990; Alonso and Alonso 1991). An tains would be less frequent, as we observed. It is worth interesting observation, however, is that this effect was emphasizing that our three fitted GLM models acnot detectable for juvenile females. They spent almost counted heavily for the carcassss factor. This supports the same amount of time feeding regardless of the the hypothesis that the conditions occurring at carcasses number of conspecifics present. Juvenile females are can be highly variable. probably not able to benefit from an increase in group There may be an alternative explanation to habitat size since all the birds present (when competitors are not segregation by despotic competition. Focusing their present they would be other juvenile females) can po- searching and scavenging activities in plains, suborditentially compete with them for food. nate condors could avoid eventual resource depletion The second assumption was that the relative pay-offs in the mountains. This benefit could not be realized by of phenotypes change between patches. We have ob- breeding adults, as their range movements would be tained indirect evidence supporting this; the number of limited by nest site attendance. Thus, subordinate birds 64 would compensate for lower pay-offs in mountain areas by exploiting a larger and consequently more profitable area. A similar strategy of young nonbreeding subordinate birds versus breeding adults has been described for other group-foraging species (Heinrich 1988, Donazar et al. 1996; Tella et al., in press). In our opinion, however, our data better fit a scenario of habitat segregation by despotic competence, since non-breeding juvenile males did not show a similar pattern as juvenile females, which would be expected if segregation by constraints linked to breeding area fidelity are operating. Foraging strategies and sex roles Habitat segregation by despotic competence would increase the cost/benefit relationship for subordinate birds since they should travel to more distant areas with a higher mortality risk. A question arises from this scenario: how can these asymmetries be maintained within an evolutionary context? In our opinion, the advantages obtained by foraging extreme dominant birds (adult males) would be compensated by higher costs in other life history traits. Our results show that the fraction of adult females among the scavenging birds in mountain areas close to condor sites is much lower in the spring, when nestlings are still growing, than in the summer, when fledglings are almost grown up (see Palermo 1983 for condor breeding seasons). This suggests that parental sex roles in reproduction can be very different and that the burden of nest and chick care must fall on the females, while the male could be responsible for feeding. A few observations conducted on captive breeding pairs seem to support this hypothesis (Klos 1973, 1984). In addition, Andean condors are known to perform complex courtship behaviours not found in any other species of New or Old World vultures (Gailey and Bolwig 1973), which seems to indicate the existence of selective pressures linked to sexual selection (Hedrick and Temeles 1989; Andersson 1994). As a synthesis of this reasoning, it can be hypothesized that asymmetric advantages in foraging activities and sexual selection constraints could favour divergence in body size between the sexes and explain this unusual pattern of sexual dimorphism among raptors. Acknowledgements. Obdulio Monsalvo, Javier Bustamante, Miguel Pineda and Martin Funes helped with the fieldwork. Our thanks to Gerardo Alena, Gabriel and Miguel Anz, Raul Cordero, Victor Soleno, and the administrators of the Estancias Cerro de los Pinos, Chimehuin, Collon-Cura, Lolen, and Quemquemtreu, who permitted us to work on their land. Ricardo R. Estrella, Jose L. Tella and Manuela G. Forero improved earlier versions of the manuscript. Logistic support was provided by the Centro de Ecologia Aplicada del Neuquen (Argentina); we are very grateful to Alejandro del Valle and Antonio Guinazu for their continuous help. Financial support was provided by the Instituto de Cooperacion Iberoamericana and the Ministerio de Asuntos Exteriores (Spain) through the Programa de Cooperacion Cientifica con Iberoamerica. The second author (A.T.) had a postdoctoral fellowship from the Ministerio de Educacion y Ciencia (Spain). Ignacia de Bustamante, Dougie and Norina Lawson, and Andy Green translated the text into English. References Alonso JC, Alonso J (1991) Costs and benefits of flocking in common cranes. In: Harris J (ed) Proceedings of the 1987 International Crane Workshop. International Crane Foundation, Baraboo, pp 271—276 Alonso JC, Bautista LM, Alonso JA (1997) Dominance and the dynamics of phenotype-limited distribution in common cranes. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 40:401—408 Alvarez F, Arias de Reyna L, Hiraldo F (1976) Interactions among avian scavengers in southern Spain. Ornis Scand 7:215—226 Anderson J, Horwitz RJ (1979) Competitive interactions among vultures and their avian competitors. Ibis 121:505—509 Andersson M (1994) Sexual selection. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ Baker MC, Belcher CS, Deutsch LC, Sherman GL, Thompson DB (1981) Foraging success in junco flocks and the effects of social hierarchy. Anim Behav 29:137—142 Balph MH (1977) Winter social behaviour of dark-eyed juncos: communication, social organization, and ecological implications. Anim Behav 25:859—884 Bautista LM, Alonso JC, Alonso JA (1995) A field test of ideal free distribution in flock-feeding common cranes. J Anim Ecol 64:747—757 Bekoff M, Scott AC (1989) Aggression, dominance, and social organization in evening grosbeaks. Ethology 83:177—194 Benkman CW (1988) Flock size, food dispersion, and the feeding behavior of crossbills. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 23:167—175 Bloom PH, McCrary MD, Gibson MJ (1993) Red-shouldered hawk home range and habitat use in southern California. J Wildl Manage 57:258—265 Buckley NJ (1996) Food finding and the influence of information, local enhancement, and communal roosting on foraging success of North American vultures. Auk 113:473—488 Bustamante J, Donazar JA, Hiraldo F, Ceballos O, Travaini A (1997) Differential habitat selection by immature and adult grey eagle-buzzards Geranoaetus melanoleucus. Ibis 139:322—330 Cabrera AL (1976) Regiones fitogeograficas argentinas. Enciclopedia Argentina de Agricultura y Jardineria, tomo II, fasciculo 1. Editorial ACME SACI, Buenos Aires Caraco T (1981) Risk-sensitivity in foraging groups. Ecology 62:527—531 Clark C, Mangel M (1986) The evolutionary advantages of group foraging. Theor Popul Biol 30:45—75 Crawley MJ (1993) GLIM for ecologists. Blackwell, London Del Hoyo J, Elliot A, Sargatal J (1994) Handbook of the birds of the world, vol 2. Lynx, Barcelona Dobson AJ (1983) Introduction to statistical modelling. Chapman & Hall, London Donazar JA, Ceballos O (1988) Red fox predation on fledgling egyptian vultures. J Raptor Res 22:88 Donazar JA, Hiraldo F, Bustamante J (1993) Factors influencing nest site selection, breeding density and breeding success in the bearded vulture (Gypaetus barbatus. J Appl Ecol 30:504—514 Donazar JA, Ceballos O, Tella JL (1996) Communal roosts of Egyptian vultures (Neophron percnopterus): dynamics and implications for the species conservation. In: Muntaner J, Mayol J (eds) Biologia y conservacion de las rapaces mediterraneas. Sociedad Espanola de Ornitologia, Madrid, pp 189—201 Elosegi I (1989) Vautour fauve (Gyps fulvus), gypaete barbu (Gypaetus barbatus), percnoptere dsEgypte (Neophron per cnopterus): synthese bibliographique et recherches. Acta Biol Mont 3:1—278 Fretwell SD (1972) Populations in a seasonal environment. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ 65 Fretwell SD, Lucas HL Jr (1970) On territorial behavior and other factors influencing habitat distribution in birds. Acta Biotheor 19:16—36 Gailey J, Bolwig N (1973) Observations on the behavior of the Andean condor (Vultur gryphus). Condor 75:60—68 Garcelon DK (1990) Observations of aggressive interactions by bald eagles of known age and sex. Condor 92:532—534 Gluck E (1986) Flock size and habitat-dependent food and energy intake of foraging goldfinches. Oecologia 71:268—272 Goss-Custard JD, Clarke RT, Durell SEA (1984) Rates of food intake and aggression of oystercatchers Haematopus ostralegus on the most and the least preferred mussel Mytilus edulis beds of the Exe estuary. J Anim Ecol 53:233—245 Hedrick AV, Temeles EJ (1989) The evolution of sexual dimorphism in animals: hypotheses and tests. Trends Ecol Evol 4:136—138 Heinrich B (1988) Winter foraging at carcasses by three sympatric corvids, with emphasis on recruitment by the raven, Corvus corax. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 23:141—156 Heinrich B (1994) Dominance and weight changes in the common raven, Corvus corax. Anim Behav 48:1463—1465 Hiraldo F, Blanco JC, Bustamante J (1991) Unspecialized exploitation of small carcasses by birds. Bird Study 38:200—207 Houston DC (1988) Competition for food between Neotropical vultures in forest. Ibis 130:402—417 Huntingford F, Turner A (1987) Animal conflict. Chapman & Hall, London Kirk DA, Gosler AG (1994) Body condition varies with migration and competition in migrant and resident South American vultures. Auk 111:933—944 Kirk DA, Houston DC (1995) Social dominance in migrant and resident turkey vultures at carcasses: evidence for a despotic distribution? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 36:323—332 Klos H-G (1973) Hand-rearing Andean condors at West Berlin Zoo. Int Zoo Yearb 13:112 Klos H-G (1984) Breeding history of the Andean condor at the West Berlin Zoo. Int Zoo Yearb 23:14—16 Knight RL, Knight SK (1988) Agonistic asymmetries and the foraging ecology of bald eagles. Ecology 69:1188—1194 Koford CB (1953) The California condor. Dover, New York Krebs JR (1974) Colonial nesting and social feeding as strategies for exploiting food resources in the great blue heron (Ardea herodias). Behaviour 51:99—134 Lima SL, Dill LM (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 68:619—640 Lindstrom K, Ranta E (1993) Foraging group structure among individuals differing in competitive ability. Ann Zool Fenn 30:225—232 McCullagh P, Nelder JA (1983) Generalised linear modelling. Chapman & Hall, London McGahan J (1972) Behavior and ecology of the Andean condor. PhD thesis, University of Wisconsin. McGahan J (1973) Gliding flight of the Andean condor in nature. J Exp Biol 58:225—237 Milinski M (1988) Games fish play: making decisions as a social forager. Trends Ecol Evol 3:325—330 Milinski M, Parker GA (1991) Competition for resources. In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioural ecology. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 137—168 Milinski M, Boltshauser P, Buchi L, Buchwalder T, Frischknecht M, Haderman T, Kunzler R, Roden C, Ruetschi A, Strahm D, Tognola M (1995) Competition for food in swans: an experimental test of the truncated phenotype distribution. J Anim Ecol 64:758—766 Monaghan P (1980) Dominance and dispersal between feeding sites in the herring gull (Larus argentatus). Anim Behav 28:521—527 Monaghan P, Metcalfe NB, Hansell MH (1986) The influence of food availability and competition on the use of a feeding site by herring gulls. Bird Study 33:87—90 Mundy PJ (1982) The comparative biology of Southern African vultures. Vulture Study Group, Johannesburg Mundy PJ, Butchart D, Ledger J, Piper S (1992) The vultures of Africa. Academic Press, London Nelder JA, Wedderburn RWM (1972) Generalized linear models. J R Stat Soc A 135:370—384 Palermo MA (1983) El condor. Fauna argentina 23. Centro Editor de America Latina, Buenos Aires Parker GA, Sutherland WJ (1986) Ideal free distributions when individuals differ in competitive ability: phenotype-limited ideal free models. Anim Behav 34:1222—1242 Pennycuick CJ (1972) Soaring behaviour and performance of some East African birds observed from a motor-glider. Ibis 114:178— 218 Pulliam HR, Caraco T (1984) Living in groups: is there an optimal group size? In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 125— 148 Ranta E, Rita H, Lindstrom K (1993) Competition versus co-operation: success of individuals foraging alone and in groups. Am Nat 140:42—58 Rita H, Ranta E, Peuhkuri N (1996) Competition in foraging groups. Oikos 76:583—586 Schaller GB (1972) The Serengeti lion. University of Chicago Press, Chicago Senar JC (1994) Vivir y convivir, la vida en grupos sociales. In: Carranza J (ed) Etologia, Introduccion a la ciencia del comportamiento. Universidad de Extremadura, Caceres, pp 205— 233 Senar JC, Camerino M, Metcalfe NB (1989) Agonistic interactions in siskin flocks: why dominants sometimes subordinate?. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 25:141—145 Siegel S, Castellan NJ Jr (1988) Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. New York, McGraw-Hill Sutherland WJ, Parker GA (1985) Distribution of unequal competitors. In: Sibly RM, Smith RH (eds) Behavioural ecology. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 255—274 Tella JL, Forero MG, Hiraldo F, Donazar JA (in press) Conflicts between lesser kestrel conservation and European agricultural policies as identified by habitat use analysis. Conserv Biol Vickery WL, Giraldeau L-A, Templeton JJ, Kramer DL, Chapman CA (1991) Producers, scroungers, and group foraging. Am Nat 137:847—863 Village A (1982) The home range and density of kestrels in relation to vole abundance. J Anim Ecol 51:69—80 Wallace MP, Temple SA (1987) Competitive interactions within and between species in a guild of avian scavengers. Auk 104:290—295 Ward P, Zahavi A (1973) The importance of certain assemblages as information centresss for food-finding. Ibis 115:517—534 Wells H, King JL (1980) A general exact testss for N x M contingency tables. Bull S Calif Acad Sci 79:65—77 Wilson EO (1975) Sociobiology: the new synthesis. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass Communicated by J. Hoglund