1 Self Esteem Instability – Scale Development and Relations to Appraisal and Dispositional Constructs Joseph M. Flaxer Thesis completed in fulfillment of the requirements of the Vanderbilt Honors Program in the Psychological Sciences Under the direction of Professor Craig A. Smith Vanderbilt University April 2012 2 Abstract Past literature indicates that self-esteem may not be a stable entity for all individuals, and there may be some individuals for whom their self-esteem varies across contexts. Research has shown that further exploring the Self Esteem Instability paradigm has the potential to create a much more comprehensive and accurate account of SE that accounts for this variability. Also, it is desirable to develop a viable alternative to the current multiple assessment method of SE-stability that can measure SE-instability in a single assessment. Participants were asked to write about a highly stressful event they had experienced, and then responded to a long survey of questions measuring SE-level, SEinstability, appraisal style and situated appraisal, and a variety of other dispositional constructs. Interaction between SE-instability and SE-level was predicted, as well as relationships of these interactions to blame assignment, coping, and other appraisal and dispositional constructs. Results indicated that a highly reliable measure of SE-instability was produced with strong face validity. Instability was found to relate to a wide variety of constructs in ways that were largely in line with predictions, although the exact relations predicted were not always observed. Implications of these findings are discussed. 3 Self Esteem Instability – Scale Development and Relations to Appraisal and Dispositional Constructs Self-esteem can be defined as the overall evaluation of one’s own worth, value, or importance (Blascovich, & Tomoka, 1991). Current models show that self-esteem is manifested as a readily accessible, stable appraisal of the self that arises from multiple self-evaluations of different personal attributes, resulting in positive and negative feelings about the self. Generally, self-esteem has been conceptualized as an attitude that is a facet of self-concept. Self-esteem (SE) is also studied more specifically as a perceived difference between the ideal and actual self, and also as an adaptive function for self-protection against environmental stressors (Blascovich, & Tomoka, 1991). In the psychology literature, it is widely assumed that SE is trait-like (Blascovich, & Tomoka, 1991). Most measures of SE include perceptions of ability and feelings regarding the self (Smith & Petty, 1995). Past studies have shown that compared to low-SE individuals, high-SE individuals tend to be more confident in their abilities and more confident that their efforts will lead to success, less susceptible to mood swings, more receptive to favorable feedback from interaction partners and less vulnerable to depression (Smith & Petty, 1995). Self-esteem has also been shown to be useful in predicting people's reactions to adverse events. Low-SE individuals, relative to those with high-SE, have been shown to respond to aversive events with greater negative affect, to over-generalize from specific negative feedback to other aspects of their identities, and to lower their self-evaluations when placed in negative moods (Smith & Petty, 1995). Among the many self-esteem correlates, some apparent discrepancies arise when evaluating how people with similar levels of self-esteem respond emotionally to different 4 situations. For example, Baumeister and colleagues (Baumeister, Bushman, & Campbell, 2000; Baumeister, Smart, & Boden, 1996), contend that high self-esteem is often associated with aggressive behavior. In a similar vein, Heatherton and Vohs (2000) report that some individuals with high self-esteem become noticeably disagreeable when others or events threaten their egos. In a review of the SE literature, Baumeister, Campbell, Krueger, and Vohs (2003) concluded that although SE relates to affect and motivation, it is not predictive of such markers of adaptive functioning as academic achievement and popularity (Kernis, Lakey & Heppner, 2008). As such, self-esteem may not be a stable entity for all individuals, and there may be some individuals for whom their self-esteem varies across contexts. Research has shown that further exploring within-person variability in self-esteem has the potential to create a much more comprehensive and accurate account of SE that explores and accounts for this variability. An important advance was the development of the SE-instability paradigm. In the earlier stages of the SE-instability literature, Rosenberg (1986) differentiated between baseline and barometric self-esteem, defined, respectively, as long-term fluctuations in one's self-esteem that occur gradually over an extended time period and short-term fluctuations in one's contextually-based global self-esteem. More recently, Michael Kernis (1993) defined unstable-SE as the magnitude of fluctuations in momentary, contextually-based self-esteem, or, in other words, as the propensity to exhibit variability in self-feelings across time. Although SE-stability in a population falls on a spectrum, for expository ease, it is helpful to refer to four categories based on level and stability of SE, which include high unstable-SE, high stable-SE, low unstable-SE and low stable-SE (Kernis, 1993). Those who exhibit high unstable-SE are thought to experience substantial short-term fluctuations in their high-SE (Kernis, 1993). As a result, these individuals have feelings that are 5 highly vulnerable to change, as their high self-esteem is constantly threatened. Accordingly, these individuals will act to maintain positive self-view by emphasizing positive self-relevant events and obstacles they have overcome, and will act to diminish threats to their positive self view by undermining the legitimacy of the threat and externalizing the source of the threat (Kernis, 1993). On the other hand, those who exhibit high stable-SE feel less threatened by negative self-relevant events, are less prone to employ other-blame, and are less likely to have strong adverse reactions. While less is certain about those with low unstable-SE, it is hypothesized that these individuals will not engage in direct forms of self-enhancement, as do those with high unstable-SE (Kernis, 1993). Rather, these individuals show a tendency to employ self-protective efforts in response to negative self-relevant events. Some of these coping strategies include self-handicapping and excuse-making. These efforts are perhaps adaptive, in that this group appears to be preventing an already low level of SE from staying too low (Kernis, 1993). In contrast, those with low stable-SE will tend to self-blame, cope poorly, and exhibit depressive symptoms. In studying individual emotional responses, it is helpful to refer to the appraisal theory paradigm, which provides a systematic framework for explaining variability in emotional reactions by treating emotion as an adaptive function (Smith & Kirby, 2009). Within this framework, different emotions result from different evaluations of the implications of our circumstances for one’s well-being. Appraisal theory sheds light on important issues in emotional psychology like the kinds of traits, situations and contexts likely to give rise to specific emotions for a particular individual, and as a result, serves as a useful paradigm through which to evaluate differences in emotional responses among those with differing SE-levels and SE-instability. 6 Out of seven appraisal dimensions, motivational relevance and motivational congruence determine how pressing a set of circumstances is to one’s needs and goals and whether the situation helps or hurts these goals. Self-accountability and other-accountability differentiate between guilt and anger, as they determine whether the self or an outside force is to blame for the given set of circumstances. Problem Focused Coping Potential (PFCP), an evaluation of one’s ability to act directly on the situation to make or keep the circumstances consistent with one’s goals, Accommodation Focused Coping Potential (AFCP), an evaluation of one’s ability to psychologically adjust to the situation should it turn out not as desired, and future expectancy, an evaluation of the extent to which one can expect a set of circumstances to improve or worsen, can differentiate between emotions like anxiety, hope, determination and challenge. These appraisals also relate to optimism, perceived stress, and perceived competence (Smith & Lazarus, 1990). No study has evaluated SE-instability in direct relation to appraisal theory, and as such, one purpose of this study was to discover more about the SE-stability paradigm by measuring how participants with different levels of SE and different levels of SE-instability appraise a stressful situation. In the study, participants were asked to recall in detail a stressful incident, and then responded to a series of questions requiring them to reflect on the incident. Primary appraisal measures include the Appraisal Style Questionnaire (ASQ), as well as a Situated Appraisal Measure. The ASQ rates appraisal style on the disposition level, while the Situated Appraisal Measure rates how an individual is appraising a specific situation that he/she is reflecting upon. This study was primarily focused on appraisals of self and other accountability, and appraisals of problem focused coping potential and accommodation/emotion focused coping potential. Along with these appraisal measures, a number of other correlates were 7 investigated, including, anxiety, defensiveness, perceived stress, perceived confidence, optimism, a wide variety of discreet emotions, behavioral and emotion coping, and specific coping techniques. The analysis was focused on main effects of SE-level independent of stability, and SE-instability independent of level; their cross product was also evaluated to find interactions. In measuring SE-stability, the primary challenge was to develop a reliable scale. In the past, Kernis (1993) has measured SE-stability by administering a global self-esteem measure like the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale multiple times over a period of five to seven days, with the instructions asking participants to rate how they feel at the moment. Researchers have then computed the standard deviations of participant scores across these multiple assessments, with a greater standard deviation indicating lower stability of self-esteem. However, to make full use of the subject pool in this investigation, all of the questions had to come from within the single, hour-long survey, and as a consequence, Kernis’ method could not be applied. Plus, it would be useful for future studies to develop a viable alternative to Kernis’ repeated assessments method. Thus, a new measure, the Self-Esteem Instability Scale (SEIS), was developed. The purpose of this scale was to separate out those with high SE-stability from those with low SE-stability, without presorting them into groups based on their appraisals of blame assignment, coping potential and other correlates of interest. This scale was administered as a 15-item Likert-like scale, and then reduced to 10 items based on reliability analysis. As for predictions, it was expected that those with high unstable-SE would show strong attributions of other-accountability with high emotional consequences and weak attributions of self-accountability. As paralleled in the Kernis (1993) literature, it was predicted that attributions of other blame would be to protect a positive self-view. Of note, these predictions hold for 8 negative, stressful circumstances, where the possibility of things reflecting badly on the self is high. They were not predicted to apply to positive circumstances, where self-accountability could lead to self-enhancement. Also, from this group high levels of anxiety were expected, as well as unwillingness to reappraise attitudes, standards and expectations, partly in the form of poor accommodation focused coping. From the group of high stable-SE, weak attributions of other-accountability and strong attributions of self-accountability with little emotional consequence were predicted. As such, these individuals were not expected to show high levels of anger or guilt. In addition, they were expected to exhibit low levels of anxiety, an ability to change attitudes, standards, and expectations, both partly in the form of good accommodation focused coping, and in positive forms of coping with adversity, like accepting failure. Again, these predictions hold for negative, stressful circumstances, where the possibility of things reflecting badly on the self is high. As paralleled in the Kernis (1993) literature, it was predicted that stable-SE would indicate that respondents’ SE-levels would not be threatened by the negative-self relevant event, and these individuals would not feel a need to employ other-blame, and would feel secure and calm in admitting self blame and moving forward in reappraising and coping with the situation. From the group of low unstable-SE individuals, low attributions of other blame were expected. Higher levels of self-blame compared to the levels of those with high self-esteem were expected, but lower levels of self-blame compared to those with low stable-SE was expected. In terms of coping, this group was predicted to employ self-protective strategies like excuse making and other forms of self-handicapping to disengage and move on. In this sense, better accommodation-focused coping was anticipated than among the low unstable-SE individuals. As described in the Kernis (1993) literature, for these negative, stressful circumstances, it was 9 predicted that unlike the high-unstable individuals, the low-unstable individuals would selfprotect their SE in the form of coping and other self protective strategies, as opposed to employing other blame. As previously mentioned, the low unstable-SE, group is characterized by attempting to keep SE-level from dipping too low, rather than protecting a high-SE level. From the group of low stable-SE individuals, high attributions of self-blame, poor accommodation focused coping, and as a result, poorer adaptational outcomes, like lower lifesatisfaction, were predicted. Methods Participants A total of 133 participants (76.7% female) ranging from ages 16-54, (M=22.7; S.D.= 7.8) took the survey. We took advantage of 3 different recruiting methods. Some participants were recruited through the SONA system administered through the Vanderbilt Psychology Department. These participants were Vanderbilt undergraduates looking to fulfill required research participation for psychology course requirements. After signing up for the study these participants were provided a URL that linked directly to the survey. Other participants were recruited through of psychology experiment boards on the web. Interested volunteers were also provided with a URL linked directly to the survey. The third group of participants consisted of acquaintances or relatives of experimenters. These individuals received an e-mail inviting them to participate in the study, with a URL linking to the survey. Procedure and Design Stressor incident: At the beginning of the survey, participants were asked to describe a stressful incident. The prompt read: “There are many things that happen in our lives that cause 10 us extreme stress, such as loss of a loved one, moving to a new place, loss of a job, chronic illness, academic failure, or natural disaster, etc. In the space below, please describe in detail an extremely stressful situation that you have experienced in the past year. Describe what led to this event, what happened, and what you did next. Describe the situation in such a way that a person reading the description would feel stress just from hearing about the situation. Please describe the experience in as much detail as you can. The text box will expand to accommodate your essay.” Participants proceeded through the self-guided survey at their own pace. All information and instructions were self-contained within the survey. The participants were told the survey would consist of approximately 470 items, and required approximately 1 hour to complete. The experiences the respondents described and evaluated were entirely of their own choosing. Participates were told that they were free to discontinue their participation in the survey at any time, and were free to decline to answer any questions they do not wish to answer. Measures Self-Esteem Instability Scale (SEIS) – We developed the SEIS as a potential viable alternative to Kernis’ repeated assessments method of measuring SE-instability. The purpose of this scale was to separate out those with high SE-stability from those of low SE-stability without presorting them into groups based on their appraisals of blame assignment, coping potential and our other correlates of interest. This scale was administered as a 15-item Likert-like scale, and then reduced to 10 items based on reliability analysis (see results section for a description of this reduction process). The final 10-item scale has a Chronbach’s Alpha equal to 0.858. Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale: The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965) is a 10-item self-report measure of global self-esteem. It consists of 10 statements related to overall 11 feelings of self-worth or self-acceptance. The items are answered on a four-point scale ranging from strongly agree to strongly disagree. This scale has demonstrated good reliability and validity across a large number of different sample groups. In the present sample this measure provided evidence of good reliability (Cronbach’s Alpha = .89). Appraisal Style Questionnaire (ASQ): The Appraisal Style Inventory is a two-item 9point scale that includes 12 vignettes of hypothetical situations: 6 positive and 6 negative, 6 achievement-related and 6 affiliative. For each situational prompt, participants are asked to imagine themselves in the situation and to answer 7 questions, assessing the 7 different appraisal dimensions) in response to it. The items from this scale addressed in the results section included self accountability for positive situations (alpha=.790), self accountability for negative situations (alpha=.703), other accountability for positive situations (alpha=.671), other accountrability for negative situations (alpha=.715), an old measure of problem focused coping potential (alpha=.839), a new new measure of PFCP (alpha=.89) and an old measure of accommodation focused coping potential (alpha=.913), and a new measure of AFCP (alpha=.91). New Problem-focused Coping-Potential Scale: The new Problem-focused CopingPotential Scale was designed to measure to what extent participants utilize that particular appraisal style, showing their general potential (or lack thereof) to act on a stressful situation to increase its desirability. It includes 12 Likert-type, dispositional measures of the problemfocused coping potential dimension. An example of a statement that the participants must rate the extent to which they agree with the statement is: “In general, when faced with a stressful situation, I am confident of my ability to deal with it.” Participants then chose the extent to which they either: strongly disagree, somewhat disagree, neither agree nor disagree, somewhat agree, or strongly agree. In the present sample, this scale demonstrated an alpha of .89. 12 New Emotion-focused Coping Potential Scale: The new Emotion-focused CopingPotential Scale was designed to measure to what extent participants use emotion-focused as an appraisal style, which ideally reflects the degree to which the participant has the general potential, or lack of, to handle and adjust to a stressful situation, especially if the situation does not turn out in a desirable manner. It includes 12 Likert-type, dispositional measures of the emotion-focused coping potential dimension. An example of a statement that the participants must rate the extent to which they agree with the statement is: “When I realize a goal is unattainable, I change my goal to make it more reachable.” Or “When something goes wrong, I readjust my priorities.” Participants then chose the extent to which they either: strongly disagree, somewhat disagree, neither agree nor disagree, somewhat agree, or strongly agree. In the present sample, this scale demonstrated an alpha of .91. Situated Appraisal Measure: The Situated Appraisal Measure (Smith, Haynes, Lazarus, & Pope, 1993) uses 9-point scales to assess how an individual is appraising a specific situation that he/she is reflecting upon, based on the seven appraisal dimensions. Each of these dimensions is assessed by a single item. The situated appraisal measures mentioned in the results section include self-accountability, other accountability, problem focused coping potential, and accommodation focused coping potential. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) measures two types of anxiety, state anxiety (S-Anxiety) and trait anxiety (T-Anxiety). We used the trait anxiety part of the scale. T-Anxiety is characterized by anxiety-proneness, shown through individual differences which anxiety, which are reflected through a frequency of anxiety states in past and future probabilities. It is formatted in a 4-pont Likert scale and has a median alpha of .90 (Spielberger & Reheiser, 2009). 13 Constructive-Thinking Inventory - Behavioral Coping: The Constructive-Thinking Inventory (Epstein & Meier, 1989) is a self-report questionnaire that evaluates the habitual cognitive coping assessments participants make in stressful situations. The Behavioral Coping component refers to the tendency participants have to focus on behavioral and taking actions when in stressful situations. This measure provides good evidence of reliability with Cronbach’s Alpha = .84. Constructive Thinking Inventory - Emotional Coping: The Constructive-Thinking Inventory (Epstein & Meier, 1989) is a self-report questionnaire that evaluates the habitual cognitive coping assessments participants make in stressful situations. The Emotion Coping component refers to the tendency participants have to focus on cognitive evaluations and readjustments when in stressful situations. This measure provides good evidence of reliability with Cronbach’s Alpha = .85. COPE Inventory: The COPE Inventory (Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub, 1989) was developed to assess a broad range of coping responses and includes at least two pairs of polaropposite tendencies. Participants reported how they responded when confronted with the difficult and stressful experience. Ratings were made on a four-point scale (1= I usually don’t do this at all, 4= I usually do this a lot). The Inventory consists of 18 different subscales. The subscales included in my results section were the ones were significantly associated with one of more of the SE-constructs. As such, I reported on the subscales labeled: denial (= .77), behavioral disengagement (= .73), social support (= .87), religion (= .96), stoicism (= .51), selfisolation (= .85), and substance use (= .87) Sample items include: “I made a plan of action,” “I learned something from the experience,” and “I said to myself this isn’t real.” All of the 18 subscales had reliabilities between .51 and .96. 14 Marlowe Crowne Social Desirability Scale: Originally intended as a measure of Social Desirability, Marlow-Crowne scale, (Crowne and Marlowe, 1960) this scale was found to be a reliable measure of defensiveness (alpha=0.79). Persons scoring high on this scale appear to underreport, deny, or suppress negative emotions such as anxiety and anger. (Shapiro et al., 1995). DEAL (measure of discrete emotions): Participants were given a number of adjective clusters that described different emotions. Each group of adjectives was meant to convert to a single basic emotion. Participants selected the extent to which they were feeling the emotion during the stressful experience. For each adjective cluster, they were asked if they were experiencing the emotion at the time [of the incident] not at all, moderately, or extremely (Smith, Haynes, Lazarus, & Pope, 1993). The emotion clusters assessed in the results section were the ones that were significantly associated with one or more of the SE-constructs, and included tranquil-calm-serene, regretful-remorseful-sorry, shy-timid-bashful, mad-angry-irate, irritatedannoyed, joyful-happy-glad. Perceived Stress Scale: The Perceived Stress Scale (Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983) is a 14 item self-report instrument with a five-point scale: (0 = never, 1 = almost never, 2 =sometimes, 3 = fairly often, 4 = very often), is an economical and simple psychological instrument to administer, comprehend, and score. It measures the degree to which situations in one’s life over the past month are appraised as stressful. Items were designed to detect how unpredictable, uncontrollable, and overloaded respondents find their lives. The Perceived Stress Scale poses general queries about relatively current levels of stress experienced. All items begin with the same phrase: In the past month, how often have you felt…? In the present sample the Perceived Stress Scale demonstrated an alpha reliability of .88. 15 The LOT (Life Orientation Test): The Life Orientation Test (LOT) was developed to assess individual differences in generalized optimism versus pessimism. The LOT (Scheier & Carver, 1985) consists of eight items, four of which are keyed in a positive direction, and four of which are keyed in a negative direction. Respondents are asked to indicate the extent to which they agree with each of the items, using the following response format: 4 = strongly agree, 3 = agree, 2 - neutral, 1 = disagree, and 0 = strongly disagree. In the present sample, the LOT demonstrated an alpha reliability of .86. Perceived Competence Scale (PCS) - Perceived Competence Scale (Smith, Wallston, & Dobbins, 1991) evaluates one’s perceived ability to effectively interact with one’s environment. It is composed of a four-item measure designed to assess a persons perceived ability to personally accomplish goals that the person deems are important. The PCS has an internal alpha of 0.72. Results Overview of Analyses The results are divided into 4 main sections. Section 1 shows our final SEIS scale, its removed items, and its reliability. Section 2 provides the means and distributions of SE-level and SE-instability. Section 3 shows how are SE-instability, SE-level, and their interaction associated with our appraisal measures, and other correlates of interest. Section 4 shows the regression analysis of the four interactions we found for SE-level and SE-instability, and explains the four graphs that correspond to each interaction. Section 1: SEIS Scale Development 16 For the initial 15-item scale, Cronbach’s alpha was 0.823, indicating strong reliability. Concerns about 5 of the items led us to reduce the scale to 10 items, with Chronbach’s Alpha equal to 0.858. The final 10 item questionnaire consisted of the following items. Reported reliability values are the corrected item-total correlations from this final 10-item scale (alpha=0.858) 1. Despite the highs and lows of the day, I feel satisfied with myself (Item-total r=.547)* 2. My ego is pretty stable (Item-total r=.457)* 3. When I mess up with something, I am inclined to think that I am a failure (Item-total r=0.562) 4. My self-esteem often fluctuates (Item-total r=0.667) 5. When I don’t perform well I feel really bad about myself (Item-total r=0.417) 6. My feelings of self worth usually depend on the highs and lows of the day (Item-total r=0.656) 7. I often feel like my identity is being threatened (Item-total r=0.599) 8. How I think about myself fluctuates, depending on the situation (Item-total r=0.679) 9. I try not to get too intensely involved in competitive activities so it won’t hurt too much if I lose or do poorly (Item-total r=0.463) 10. My self esteem usually remains stable (Item-total r=0.457)* *item reverse coded Five items were removed on statistical grounds, conceptual grounds or both. The removed items included: 1. When I succeed, I want other people to know of my accomplishments. Because this item was closely related to self-aggrandizement and emphasizing positive self-relevant events, a hypothesized consequence of unstable SE, a concern was that inclusion of the item would contribute to confounded analyses. This item was also removed on statistical grounds (corrected item-total correlation = .013, alpha jumps to 0.838 when removed). 2. When I do something wrong, my first impulse is to blame the circumstances Because this item was closely related to appraising other accountability, a hypothesized consequence of unstable SE, a concern was that inclusion of the item would contribute to 17 confounded analyses. This item was also removed on statistical grounds (corrected item-total correlation = 0.293). 3. I tend to make excuses for poor performances. Because this item was closely related accommodation focused coping, a hypothesized consequence of low unstable-SE, a concern was that inclusion of the item would contribute to confounded analyses. 4. I will avoid a task if I don’t think I will do well at it A concern was that this item was a consequence of unstable-SE rather than an indicator of the construct. 5. When I succeed, I feel really good about myself This item was eliminated on statistical grounds based on a poor inter-item correlation (r=-0.073). The best reliability came from items that most generally represent the SE-instability construct. For instance, the strongest item-total correlations came from “how I think about myself fluctuates, depending on the situation, and “my self-esteem often fluctuates.” All in all, the highest loading items on the final 10-item scale most directly reflect the SE-instability construct being measured. This evidence points to the SEIS having good face-validity. Section 2 - means and distributions of SE-level and SE-instability: As shown below in graph 1, SE-instability was negatively correlated with SE level, but there was still a good amount of individual variability. More importantly, as shown in table 1, the overall SE-level of our subject pool was high. This lack of low-SE individuals is important to keep in mind when trying to make sense of the data, as discussed in Section 3. 18 Table 1: Mean SE-level and SE-instability N Minimum Maximum Mean Std. Deviation rosenberg_SE_Scale 97 1.6 5.0 3.9 .78 se_instability_scale 97 1.0 4.6 2.8 .74 Graph 2: SE-Instability as a Function of SE-Level 19 Section 3 - SE-instability, SE-level, and their Interaction: In Table 2, a two-step multiple regression was used to examine the degree to which SElevel and SE-instability distinctly predicted relations hypothesized to be associated with the appraisal measures and other constructs. In the first step used both SE-level and SE-instability were used to predict the outcome measures. This step evaluated each construct differentially. In the second step the cross-product between SE-level and SE-instability was added to see if they interacted in predicting other-blame for negative circumstances, anxiety, accommodation focused coping, or any other appraisal measure or dispositional construct included in our analysis. To prevent problems with multicolinearity, before conducting these analyses both the SE-level and SE-Instability measures were centered on their means, and these centered variables were used in all of the reported multiple regression analyses, and were also used to create the cross-product that was entered in the second step of these analyses. (Aiken & West, 1991). The measures included in table 2 to compare to the SE constructs consisted of all or parts of the following scales: 1. Appraisal Style Questionnaire: measures of accountability and coping potential; 2. Situated Appraisal: measures of accountability and coping; 3. State-Trait Anxiety: measures of trait anxiety; 4. Constructive Thinking Inventory: measures of behavioral and emotional coping; 5. COPE scale: included the specific coping techniques with significant associations with one or more of the SE-constructs; 6. The full Marlowe Crowne Social Desirability Scale to test for defensiveness; 7. The specific discrete emotions from the DEAL scale that were significantly associated with one or more of the SE-constructs; 8. The full Perceived Stress Scale, LOT Optimism Scale, and Perceived Confidence Scale. The interactions that were to associate with other accountability for negative situations, turning to religion, tranquility and regret are further elaborated on in Section 3, table 3. 20 Despite predicting a lot of interaction, only found a few instances of interactions were found, and by and large separate main effects of SE and separate main effects of instability were observed. The prediction that highXunstable-SE would be associated with other blame for negative circumstances was confirmed. However, due to a lack of interaction among SE-level and SE-instability, it cannot conclusively stated that our predictions were confirmed. However, there were many instances in which characteristics that were expected for highXunstable-SE, and characteristics that were not expected for lowXunstable-SE, turned up to characterize SEinstability independent of SE-level. For example, unstable-SE was associated with high anxiety, high perceived stress, high levels of denial, low AFCP, low behavioral and emotion coping, and low optimism. These results led to two possible conclusions. One possible explanation is that SE-instability is a more general, discrete trait that in several respects can be evaluated independently of SE-level. A second viable explanation relates to the high SE-level of the subject pool. Had the sample included a greater number of low SE individuals, and if it is the case that manifestations of unstable-SE do actually differ for low vs. high SE individuals, then perhaps the expected interactions would have been observed to confirm our predictions. 21 Table 2 – Relations of SE-level, SE-Instability and their Interaction to Appraisal and Dispositional Constructs. APPRAISAL MEASURES 1. ASQ - Accountability and Coping Self Accountability_Pos Self Accountability_Neg Other Accountability_Pos Other Accountability_Neg PFCP_Old Scale EMCP_Old Scale PFCP_New Scale AFCP_New Scale 2. Situated Appraisal Self-accountability Other- accountability PFCP EFCP_accomodation EFCP_deal emotionally OTHER CORRELATES 3. Anxiety STAI_Scale 4. Coping: Constructive Thinking Inventory Behavioral Coping Emotional Coping 5. Coping Techniques (COPE Scale) Denial Behavioral disengagement Social support Turning to religion Stoicism Self isolation Substance use 6. Defensiveness Marlowe_Crowne 7. Discrete Emotions Tranquil-calm-serene Regretful-remorseful-sorry Shy-timid-bashful Mad-angry-irate Irritated-annoyed Joyful-happy-glad 8. Other Perceived Stress Scale LOT_Optimism Scale Perceived Confidence Standardized Coefficients (beta) SE-Level SE-Instability 0.443** 0.284* 0.222 0.368** 0.302* ns ns ns ns ns SE-Level 0.174 -0.237 -0.318* -0.286* -0.416*** ns ns ns -0.17 0.149 SE-Instability instabXse See Table 3 instabXse -0.377*** 0.514*** - 0.292* -0.241* -0.292* -0.903*** - ns ns ns ns ns ns 0.227* 0.39** -0.199 0.369** 0.312* 0.213 - - -0.208 0.429** 0.399** -0.288 ns 0.419** 0.234 -0.363* -0.25* 0.48*** 0.642*** 0.45*** -0.25* ns Note, ns=not significant, *p < .1, ** p < .05; *** p < .01. See Table 3 See Table 3 See Table 3 - 22 Section 4 – Regression Analysis of Interactions: Table 3 shows the regression analysis of the SE-level X SE-Instability interactions. In building this table, a regression equation was used to compute predicted values for hypothetical individuals who are high versus low on SE-level and SE-instability. In order to predict the original rating scale, the raw equation was used rather than the standardized equation with Beta weights. The standard deviations of the original variables were 0.777 for SE-level, and 0.737 for SE-instability. The representative values of 1 and -1 are close to, but a little more extreme than using plus or minus 1 standard deviation. Below table 3, the four interactions are depicted graphically and explained. Table 3 Regression equation: measure = constant + (Rosenberg SE)+(SEIS)+(InstabilityXSE) (1 and -1 as representative values) Other Accountability_Neg High SE (1) unstable (1) High SE (1) stable (-1) Low SE (-1) unstable (1) Low SE (-1) stable (-1) Tranquil-calm-serene High SE (1) unstable (1) High SE (1) stable (-1) Low SE (-1) unstable (1) Low SE (-1) stable (-1) Regretful-remorseful-sorry High SE (1) unstable (1) High SE (1) stable (-1) Low SE (-1) unstable (1) Low SE (-1) stable (-1) Turning to religion High SE (1) unstable (1) High SE (1) stable (-1) Low SE (-1) unstable (1) Low SE (-1) stable (-1) Equation 5.454+0.074 (SE)+0.061 (SEIS)+0.504 (SE)(SEIS)= 5.454+0.074 (SE)-0.061 (SEIS)-0.504 (SE)(SEIS) = 5.454-0.074 (SE)+0.061 (SEIS)-0.504 (SE)(SEIS) = 5.454-0.074 (SE)-0.061 (SEIS)+0.504 (SE)(SEIS) = Predicted Values 6.093 4.963 4.937 5.823 20.974+1.958 (SE)-2.293 (SEIS)-9.688 (SE)(SEIS) = 20.974+1.958 (SE)+2.293 (SEIS)+9.688 (SE)(SEIS)= 20.974-1.958 (SE)-2.293 (SEIS)+9.688 (SE)(SEIS)= 20.974-1.958 (SE)+2.293 (SEIS)-9.668 (SE)(SEIS) = 10.951 34.913 26.411 11.621 68.679-4.974 (SE)+4.260 (SEIS)-21.273 (SE)(SEIS) = 68.679-4.974 (SE)-4.260 (SEIS)+21.273 (SE)(SEIS) = 68.679+4.974(SE)+4.260 (SEIS)+21.273 (SE)(SEIS)= 68.679+4.974 (SE)-4.260 (SEIS)-21.273 (SE)(SEIS)= 46.692 80.718 99.186 48.12 1.886-0.396 (SE)-0.297 (SEIS)+0.441 (SE)(SEIS)= 1.886-0.396 (SE)+0.297 (SEIS)-0.441 (SE)(SEIS)= 1.886+0.396 (SE)-0.297 (SEIS)-0.441 (SE)(SEIS)= 1.886+0.396 (SE)+0.297 (SEIS)+0.441 (SE)(SEIS)= 1.634 1.346 2.426 3.02 23 Graph 2: Other Accountability for Negative Situations as a Function of InstabilityXSE Interaction 7 6.5 6 Other accountability for Negative Situations (0-9) 5.823 6.093 5.5 5 4.937 4.963 Unstable Stable 4.5 4 3.5 3 Low SE High SE Instability X SE Graph 2 illustrates the relationship of instabilityXSE to other accountability for negative situations. The instabilityXSE association with other accountability for negative situations shows that for individuals with unstable SE, appraising other accountability increases with increasing SE-level. For stable-SE individuals, appraising other accountability decreases with increasing SE-level. This interaction is a key finding, as it parallels the Kernis (1993) literature and confirms the prediction that individuals with high unstable-SE would appraise other blame under negative-self relevant circumstances (in this case, on the dispositional level). 24 Graph 3: Tranquility as a Function of InstabilityXSE Interaction 40 Tranquility Rating 35 34.913 30 26.411 25 Unstable 20 Stable 15 10 11.621 10.951 5 0 Low High InstabilityXSE The instabilityXSE association with tranquility shows that for high-SE individuals, tranquility in the face of stress increases with increasing SE stability, whereas for individuals with low SE, tranquility decreases with increasing stability. These results indicate that individuals with high stable-SE seemed best equipped to deal with the highly stressful event, whereas individuals with low unstable-SE were least well equipped. When considering the highSE individuals, these results support the prediction that those with high unstable-SE would demonstrate poor accommodation focused coping, and would experience anxiety. When considering the low-SE individuals, the low calmness among those with low unstable-SE is counter to our prediction that those with unstable-low SE would cope better than those with low stable SE and remain calmer. This finding sheds light on the need to learn more about SEinstability among those with low SE in furthering the SE-instability paradigm. Graph 4 Regret as a Function of InstabilityXSE Interaction 25 100 99.186 90 Regret Rating 80.718 80 70 60 50 48.12 46.692 40 unstable stable 30 20 10 0 low high InstabilityXSE The instabilityXSE association with regret shows that for individuals with high-SE, those with unstable-SE show less regret than do those with stable-SE. This finding indicates that the high unstable-SE individuals are perhaps avoiding possible self-criticism by justifying their actions and beliefs. For the low-SE individuals, those with unstable-SE report higher levels of regret than do those with stable-SE. Interestingly, individuals with high stable-SE were also able to express regret about the situation. In general, one might consider the low stable-SE group and the high-unstable-SE group to be the most vulnerable groups. As such, expressing regret would perhaps be most damaging for these two groups. 26 Graph 5: Turning to Religion as a Function of InstabiltyXSE Interaction 3.5 Turn to Religion 3 3.02 2.5 2.426 2 1.634 1.346 1.5 1 0.5 0 low high InstabilityXSE unstable stable 27 The instabilityXSE association with turning to religion shows that for individuals with unstable-SE, turning to religion decreases as SE-level increases. For individuals with stable-SE, turning to religion also decreases as SE-level increases. As depicted, individuals with low-SE were the ones most often turning to religion. Of these low-SE individuals, those with low stableSE turned to religion most. Perhaps this finding sheds light on the differences between the coping techniques of low unstable individuals and low stable individuals. While it was predicted that low unstable-SE individuals would be better at coping, perhaps turning to religion represents a less direct and hands-on type of coping that differentiates those with low stable-SE from those with low unstable-SE. Plus, perhaps for an individual with low unstable-SE, turning to religion represents an act of desperation, as most other forms of coping have failed. Discussion All in all, a highly reliable measure of SE instability has been produced. This measure was shown to be associated with the constructs it was expected it to be related to, such as state anxiety, perceived stress, anger, irritation, denial, behavioral disengagement, stoicism and self isolation. Also, as expected, the measure of SE-instability was negatively associated with accommodation-focused coping, problem focused coping, behavioral and emotional coping, optimism, and seeking social support. Also, the interaction between InstabilityXhigh-SE was associated with other accountability for negative situations on the trait level. While there was no association with defensiveness as measured by the Marlowe-Crowne scale, indications of defensiveness being positively associated with high unstable-SE did come up when considering the negative association between unstableXhigh SE and regret. As previously mentioned, the instabilityXSE association with regret shows that for individuals with unstable-SE, regret 28 decreases as SE-level increases. For individuals with stable-SE, regret increases as SE-level increases. This lack of regret among individuals with high unstable-SE indicates that they are avoiding possible self-criticism by justifying their actions and beliefs. The high reliability of the SEIS is exciting because in certain respects it represents a more useful and viable alternative to Kernis’ repeated assessments method. Although the Kernis strategy by definition is highly valid in evaluating actual variation in self-esteem, with multiple assessments across multiple occasions it can be an awkward and expensive strategy to employ and is impractical to use in a broad array of studies. Plus, beyond the inconvenience and cost of the assessment, the accuracy of the Kernis assessment depends on the different contexts in which SE is assessed. For example, a key reason for SE-instability differences is how an individual responds to negative and positive feedback. If the multiple assessments do not involve feedbackrelevant contexts, the Kernis method might systematically underestimate instability. Conversely, if highly feedback-relevant contexts happen to be assessed for other individuals, the estimate of SE-instability might be systematically too high. For the purpose of this study and for future studies, it was highly desirable to develop a relatively brief measure that could be assessed in a single administration, and that incorporated a consistent level of feedback related contexts. In terms of our findings, first and foremost, there were many instances in which characteristics expected for highXunstable-SE, and characteristics not expected for lowXunstable-SE, turned up to characterize SE instability independent of SE-level. For example, unstable-SE was associated with high anxiety, high perceived stress, high levels of denial, low AFCP, low behavioral and emotion coping, and low optimism. One possible explanation for this finding is that SE-instability is a more general, discrete trait that in several respects can be evaluated independently of SE-level. However, the SE-level of our subject pool was high with a 29 mean of 3.86 (SD=0.777), and this might be why separate main effects were found for SE-level and SE-instability, and few interactions. Had the sample included more lower SE individuals, and if it is the case that manifestations of unstable-SE do actually differ for low vs. high SE individuals, then perhaps the expected interactions would have been observed. Furthermore, it is important to explain the interaction of SE-instability and SE-level when compared to other accountability for negative situations. As previously mentioned, the limited sample of low-SE individuals likely prevented the expected interactions. Plus SE instability was negatively correlated with SE-level (-0.67**), meaning that SE-instability was highest in individuals with low-SE. Also, this interaction on the dispositional level did not appear on the situated appraisal level. Yet, one can still contend this interaction on the dispositional level remains a valid and important finding. By looking at the scatter plot of SE instability as a function of SE level (graph 1), one can reasonably argue that there was still a wide enough variety of SE-instability among those with high and low SE in order to obtain an interaction, especially when considering that other accountability for negative situations was a primary predictor of unstableXhigh-SE. Plus, other accountability was not the only situated appraisal measure that was not significant. In fact, none of the situated appraisal measures were significantly associated with instabilityXSE, instability alone, nor SE-level alone. A likely explanation for this occurrence was the fact that participants were asked to reflect on a traumatic experience for their situated measures. Perhaps the high level of trauma caused blame assignment to have little ambiguity, wiping out differences between those with different levels SE-instability. In sum, while a lot of interaction between SE instability and SE level was predicted, only found a few instances of interactions were found, and by and large separate main effects of 30 instability and separate main effects of SE-level were found. More specifically, many of the characteristics expected to be associated with highXunstable SE, and to not to be associated with lowXunstable SE, were found to be associated with unstable-SE independent of SE-level. An interaction that was found confirmed our prediction based in the Kernis literature that highXunstable SE would be associated with other accountability for negative circumstances. This finding indicates that the SEIS is a valid scale for measuring SE-instability. Furthermore, the lack of low-SE individuals in our subject pool indicates that the characteristics found to associated with unstable SE that were predicted for highXunstable SE are still likely representative of individuals with high, unstable SE. Based on the subject pool, there is less that can be concluded about instability among those with low-SE. Also, the possibility is still open that certain manifestations of unstable-SE are general traits that are independent of SE level, especially in relation to anxiety and coping. As such, future research should aim to discover more about individuals with low-unstable SE, and further strengthen the validity of the SEIS. One route would be to obtain a subject pool with low-SE individuals. Perhaps it would be useful to study a depressed population to see if this population consists mostly of individuals with low stable-SE, or if there are individuals in this population with unstable SE, to identify what the distinguishing characteristics are. Also, in order to more accurately test for significant results in situated appraisal measures, it would be beneficial to replicate the study by asking that participants reflect on stressful event, rather than specifically on a highly stressful event. On a related note, it would also be beneficial to evaluate and code the written paragraphs in which participants described their respective traumatic event, as researchers could create a method to categorize the kind of trauma or stressor. It would be helpful to separate achievement vs. affiliative oriented events. Also, researchers could use a 31 Lingistic Inquiry Word Count (LIWC) analysis as a convenient and objective method. This method gives a count of the various types of words written by the participant. Potentially useful categories might include the amount of blame words, positive words and negative words that respondents use in describing a stressful event. In contrast to the current study, it could also be of high value to create a controlled experiment in which participants are put in a stressful situation, as opposed to the retrospective method that was used. Last but not least, there are some simple methods that would help to confirm the validity of the SEIS. For instance, as previously mentioned, this survey took about an hour to complete, and incorporated a number of measures that were not used in this specific analysis. It would be helpful to administer the SEIS within the context of a shorter study to ensure that participants would be focused and discriminatory in answering the SE-based questions. Plus, a simple retest of the SEIS would allow for increased power from a bigger “N.” Last but not least, it would be useful to compare the SEIS directly to the Kernis multiple assessments method. A simple study could involve first administering the SEIS, and then administering the multiple assessments measure as Kernis did using the Rosenberg SE Scale. One could compare how the two measures correlate to each other and to other relevant scales of interest. In sum, this study further investigated the contention that there are likely individuals for whom their self-esteem varies across contexts, and also developed a viable alternative to the current multiple assessment method of SE-stability that can measure SE-instability in a single assessment. Results indicated that a highly reliable measure of SE-instability was produced with strong face validity. The few instances of expected interactions being found indicate that to some degree, SE-instability is a general, discrete trait that can be evaluated independently of SE-level. 32 Yet, had the sample included a greater number of low SE individuals, perhaps the expected interactions would have been observed to confirm more of the predictions based in the Kernis (1993) literature. Future direction for the study of the SE-instability paradigm is promising, with many concrete avenues available to increase the validity of the SEIS, confirm predictions related to individuals with high unstable-SE, and discover more about unstable-SE in individuals with low SE-levels. 33 References Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Baumeister, R. F., Bushman, B. J., & Campbell, W. K. (2000). Self-esteem, narcissism, and aggression: Does violence result from low self-esteem or from threatened egotism? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 141–156. Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J. I., & Vohs, K. D. (2003). Does high self-esteem cause better performance, interpersonal success, happiness, or healthier lifestyles? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 4, 1–44. Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation of threatened egotism to violence and aggression: The dark side of high self-esteem. Psychological Review, 103, 5–33. Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A theoretically-based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 267283. Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 385-396. Crowne, Douglas P. Marlowe, David. A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, Vol 24(4), Aug 1960, 349-354 Epstein, S., & Meier, P. (1989). Constructive Thinking: A broad coping variable with specific components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 332-350. Heatherton, T. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2000). Interpersonal evaluations following threats to self: Role of self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 725–736. Kernis, Michael H., Cornell, David P, Sun, Chuen-ru, Berry, Andrea. There's More to Self- 34 Esteem Than Whether It Is High or Low: The Importance of Stability of Self-Esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Volume 65, Issue 6. 1993. Pages 11901204. Kernis, Michael H. Measuring Self-Esteem in Context: The Importance of Stability of Self-Esteem in Psychological Functioning. 7 September 2005 Kernis, H. Michael, Lakey, E. Chad, Heppner, L. Whitney. Secure Versus Fragile High SelfEsteem as a Predictor of Verbal Defensiveness: Converging Findings Across Three Different Markers. Journal of Personality, Vol. 76, No. 3. (June 2008), pp. 477-512 Robinson, John P., Wrightsman, Lawrence S., and Andrews Frank M. Measures of Personality and Social Psychological Attitudes. Chapter 4 – Measures of Self Esteem. Blascovich, Jim and Tomoka, Joseph, 1991. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Rosenberg, M. (1986). Self-concept from middle childhood through adolescence.In J.Suls& A. G.Greenwald (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on the self: Vol. 2. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J. R., & Snyder, S. S. (1982). Changing the World and Changing the Self: A Two-Process Model of Perceived Control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42(1), 5-37. Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health: Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychology, 4, 219-247. Shapiro, D., Goldstein, I., & Jamner, L.(1995). Effects of anger/hostility, defensiveness, gender, 35 and family history of hypertension on cardiovascular reactivity. Psychophysiology, 32, 425-435. Smith, C.A. & Lazarus, R.S. (1990). Emotion and Adaptation. Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research, 609-637. Smith, C. A., Dobbins, C. J. and Wallston, K. A. (1991), The Mediational Role of Perceived Competence in Psychological Adjustment to Rheumatoid Arthritis. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 21: 1218–1244. Smith, C. A., Haynes, K. N., Lazarus, R. S., & Pope, L. K. (1993). In search of the “hot” cognitions: Attributions, appraisals, and their relation to emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 916-929. Smith, C.A. & Lazarus, R.S. Appraisal components, core relational themes, and the emotions. Cognition and Emotion, Vol. 7, Iss. 3-4, 2008 Smith, C.A. & Kirby, L.D. (2009) Putting appraisal in context: Toward a relational model of appraisal and emotion. Cognition & Emotion, 23:7,1352 -1372. Smith, Stephen M. & Petty, Richard E. Personality moderators of mood congruency effects on Cognition: The role of self-esteem and negative mood regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol 68(6), Jun 1995, 1092-1107 Spielberger, C. D. and Reheiser, E. C. (2009), Assessment of Emotions: Anxiety, Anger, Depression, and Curiosity. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 1: 271–302.