Running Head: CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES Associations of Maternal Macro- and Micro-Level Communication Styles and Child Emotions During Parent-Child Discussions About Children’s Cancer Leighann Hughart Thesis completed in partial fulfillment of the requirements of the Honors Program in Psychological Sciences Under the Direction of Dr. Bruce Compas Vanderbilt University April, 2011 CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 2 Acknowledgements I would like to extend my appreciation to those who graciously and selflessly contributed to my thesis-writing experience. In particular, I would like to recognize the efforts of all individuals on the Coping and Communication team at Vanderbilt, whose tireless and diligent work have enabled the study to function beyond its potential. I am sincerely grateful to have had the opportunity to work with such an intelligent group of individuals. Their motivation to help others is evident in their continual production of excellent work, which is always met with great enthusiasm. I am especially grateful to Dr. Bruce Compas, whose immense knowledge of and unequivocal passion for research granted me the privilege of having an invaluable source of information, advice and support towards my efforts. His wisdom of and dedication to his work are truly inspiring. Finally, I would like to thank my family and friends for their endless encouragement and support throughout this process. CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 3 Abstract Previous research indicates that children with pediatric cancer may be at risk for both short-term and long-term emotional difficulties including anxiety and depression. Parent communication may guide a child in successfully coping with stressful experiences related to the diagnosis and treatment of cancer, which can ultimately influence the child’s ability to cope and adjust to challenges posed by the illness. The current study aims to identify what aspects of maternal parent communication patterns may heighten or relieve child pediatric cancer patients’ anxiety about cancer. Sixty-two mother-child dyads of families with children diagnosed with cancer were recruited to participate in this two-site study. Mothers and children 10 years of age or older were asked to complete questionnaire packets pertaining to their experience with the illness. All families who completed the packets were then recruited to participate in a videorecorded observation including a communication task that involved a cancer-related discussion. Parent communication techniques and child emotions evident in the conversation were coded. Results indicate that mothers’ linguistic structures of their responses were related to their general communication styles and that these ultimately predicted their child’s mood. CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 4 Introduction Parent-child communication is essential to providing children with examples and information of how to effectively cope with life stressors, including the challenges and stresses associated with chronic or potentially fatal illnesses. Effective communication about stressful events is difficult, as parents must not only adjust to the situation but also formulate and execute their explanations comprehensibly to their child. Families with children diagnosed with cancer face numerous stressors that are further complicated by the treatment and uncertainty of the illness. Previous findings indicate that parental full disclosure to a child about his or her diagnosis, prognosis, and information regarding treatment may play a role in reducing anxiety or distress in pediatric patients (e.g., Chesler et al., 1986; Slavin et al., 1982). The current study utilized recorded mother-child discussions about cancer to examine specific components of parent-child communication from a broad perspective of parenting style and a more micro-level of linguistic responses to child utterances with the aims of understanding the relationships between parenting style, parent communication, and child emotions during these interactions. Parent-Child Communication Socialization is fundamental for a child’s psychological development and ability to function in society. Previous studies have led developmental psychologists to recognize that parents are among the strongest determinants of this process (Maccoby et al., 1992). Furthermore, a parent’s communicative abilities significantly affect the quality of their relationship and influence on their child. Maccoby et al. (1992) argue the “initial phases of parent—child bonding […] are based on affectively charged parent-child exchanges,” suggesting that these effects begin early in the child’s development and continue throughout their maturation. CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 5 One active area of research on parental socialization and parent-child communication has focused on adolescents’ risk for substance use and abuse. For example, considering the parent’s role as teacher, Kelly et al. (2002) conducted a study exploring the relationship between youth drug involvement and parent-child communication. Examining whether parental influence is the most effective tool in preventing drug use, this study examined how parent communication of values regarding drug abuse affected their child’s abuse rate. Previous studies indicate that youth from families who practice frequent, open (bidirectional), and positive communication are less likely to become involved in drugs (Kelly et al., 2002). This form of communication encourages expressions of both parent and child perspectives and fosters the reinforcement of conventional standards of conduct. By exercising active involvement in conversations, children feel engaged in understanding behavioral norms (Kelly et al., 2002). Results of the Kelly et al. (2002) study indicate that children who experienced more parental sanctions were less likely to experiment with drugs (Kelly et al., 2002). Because children view their parents as important and credible sources of information, frequent and deliberate conversations about the dangers of substance abuse are essential in lessening the likelihood of drug experimentation. Therefore, open communication encourages children to ask more questions and increases child involvement in decisions, which previous studies suggest affects their psychological adjustment (Kelly et al., 2002). Thus, it is the parent’s responsibility to socialize their child, which heavily relies upon communicative abilities. Through communication, the parent can successfully teach the child how to effectively respond to the inevitable challenges encountered throughout maturation. CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 6 Parent-Child Communication about an Illness The dual parental role as an information source and an influential factor in their child’s emotional and psychological development reinforces parental communication as an essential agent in mediating potential life stressors a child may experience in his or her lifetime. One major stressor a child may face is a chronic or potentially fatal illness. Because parents are the gatekeepers of information between their children and physicians, appropriate communication techniques about a child’s illness have remained an important but still under studied issue in the medical realm, as reflected in two opposing views on the optimal way for parents to communicate with their children about illness. Previously, in the 1950s and 1960s, advocates of the protective approach prevailed arguing that detailed knowledge about an illness and its prognosis, including the possibility of death, would heighten a child’s anxiety or fear and potentially inhibit their coping abilities (Chesler et al., 1986). Furthermore, supporters of this view argued that young children were concrete thinkers, cognitively incapable of understanding the medical information associated with their illness, therefore deeming disclosure unnecessary. However, by hiding information about their child’s illness, parents risk the possibility that children will learn about their illness from other sources, such as siblings, teachers, hospital staff, and peers. A child who is unable to discuss or receive information pertaining to their illness is not necessarily protected from heightened anxiety or fear; their discovery of the diagnosis and prognosis of their illness from sources other than parents is potentially detrimental to their psychological adjustment. Rather, they may view their illness as a subject of secrecy or a forbidden topic for discussion (Clarke et al., 1986). Chesler et al. (1986) discuss reports of children who were aware of the prognosis of their illness but who had parents unwilling to disclose information pertaining to the condition CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 7 and consequentially experienced loneliness and isolation (Chesler et al., 1986). Hidden or guarded disclosure may deny instrumental tools necessary to establish an honest relationship with a child, which ultimately denies him opportunities for hope, as “an air of mystery implies reason for despair” (Chesler et al., 1986). Due to the inconsistency of the guarded or closed method of communication, the open approach to communication about a child’s illness gained popularity, as advocates assert that honest discussion of diagnosis and prognosis will lead to better psychosocial adjustment in both the patient and the family members (Slavin et al., 1982). Additionally, previous research credits open communication with enhancing the coping skills of both the child facing the illness as well as the general mental health of the family (Chesler et al., 1986). Psychological adjustment to an illness may be more successful if a patient and his or her family are able to exercise effective coping skills with the stressors and instability familiar to the illness experience. Beyond the qualitative and quantitative aspects of disclosure about an illness to a child, parental communication is further examined on at least two different levels; i.e., analysis of parent-child interactions can involve both macro- and micro-level perspectives. Macro-level approaches measure the general style of parent-child conversations, such as parents’ overall levels of warmth and responsive listening to their child during a discussion about an illness. This level of analysis provides a broad measure of communication patterns and styles within an observed interaction. Macro- level analyses focus on moment-to-moment exchanges between parents and children and are important to understanding parent-child interactions, as parent-child discussions not only affect the child’s psychological adjustment to their illness but also may influence aspects of their health. For example, in a study by Miller and Drotar (2007) analyzing diabetic children and their decision-making and treatment adherence, findings measuring macro- CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 8 level communicative patterns indicate that higher levels of positive parent communication were associated with better treatment adherence. Negative communication was associated with family decisions characterized by pessimism, inability to consider all options and perspectives, and failure to take responsibility (Miller & Drotar, 2007). Furthermore, previous studies suggest that during problem-solving tasks, negative statements made between children with diabetes and their parents were associated with increased problem-solving difficulty (Miller & Drotar, 2007). Therefore, higher levels of negative parent communication increase the risk that adolescents are less likely to ask for help or engage in conversations that require decision-making. Miller and Drotar (2007) note that this failure to seek help and participate in joint decision-making with parents is potentially harmful to a child’s well-being, as active input from the child is associated with positive psychological adjustment. Similarly, Martin et al. (1998) also studied children with diabetes and found that the illness was better maintained when parents were “more emotionally supportive, had better resolved their grief about their child’s diabetes, were less sad and angry, and in parent-child dyads were better able to resolve conflicts” (Martin et al., 1998). Ultimately, frequent, open and positive communication regarding a child’s illness is associated with increased positive psychological adjustment and better maintenance of the illness. To measure communication styles during parent-child interactions about an illness, global communication patterns have been examined. These patterns can be assessed through a number of constructed observational coding systems, such as the Iowa Family Interaction Rating Scale (IFIRS; Melby & Conger, 2001), the System for Coding Interaction and Family Functioning (SCIFF; Lindahl & Malik, 2001), the Interaction Behavior Code (IBC; Prinz, Foster, Kent, & O’Leary, 1979), and the McMaster Interaction Coding System (MICS; Dickstein, CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 9 Hayden, Schiller, Seifer, & San Antonio, 1994), which are geared towards communication in pediatric populations (Dunn et al., in press). The current study utilized the Iowa Family Interaction Rating Scales (IFIRS; Melby & Conger, 2001), which is a macro-level coding scheme constructed to evaluate non-verbal and verbal behavioral and emotional elements during parent-child interactions. This coding system has been designed and validated by parent-child dyads, which included children of various ages (Melby & Conger, 2001). The range of ages facilitates applicability to a wide span of pediatric patients. Additionally, the IFIRS system has been applied to a variety of different samples of parent-child dyads (Melby &Conger, 2001), which establishes support for its use in the current study. Rather than recording a bifurcated decision of whether or not a behavior occurred throughout the observed interaction, this model uses a global rating scale for each code, ranging from 1-9, to assess each code’s presence in the communication style of the focal. This design allows for parametric statistical analyses and the potential to capture wider ranges of individual differences in parent-child communication and parenting styles. The second perspective of analysis of parent-child expressions during an interaction is on a micro-level. Micro-level analyses include detailed and comprehensive analysis of both the content of parent-child utterances and their linguistic and syntactic structures. For example, following a child’s utterance, micro-level evaluation of a parent utterance may consider whether they maintained or changed the topic of their child’s statement, and whether they expanded upon it or repeated it. Previous language studies define particular responses to child utterances in micro-level contingency categories (e.g., Saxton, 2005; Lasky & Klopp, 1982; Hart & Risley, 1992). CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 10 The current study focuses on two types of micro-level parental responses to their children: parental reflections and expansions of child utterances (Hart & Risley, 1992). Parents’ repeating of a child’s utterance has been a frequent component in micro-level analysis of parent communication and several terms have been utilized to describe this communication mechanism. While the current study refers to parents’ repeated content of child utterance as reflections, Saxton (2005) identifies these responses as “recasts,” noting its common use in parent-child conversations. Other terms refer to recasts as repeats, repetitions, and imitations (e.g., Lasky & Klopp, 1982; Fey et al., 1999). The broad category term used by Saxton (2005) is characterized as a response that “expands, deletes, permutes, or otherwise changes the [child utterance] while maintaining significant overlap in meaning” (Saxton, 2005). This response provides numerous advantages to the parent in executing effective communication with the child. Because the parent is discussing a topic of interest to the child, parental recasts allow the parent to be more confident in receiving and maintaining the child’s attention (Saxton, 2005). Saxton (2005) also asserts that recasts ensure “a strong likelihood that the child will comprehend at least part of what is being said to them, since lexical items are being reflected back to them from their own utterance” (Saxton, 2005). Lasky and Klopp (1982), whose study classifies recasts as imitations, defines this category as a “repetition of a preceding utterance,” which includes an exact imitation, or “word for word repetition of utterance”; additionally, the term extends to circumstances involving expanded imitations, or utterances that are “[repetitions] with additions” (Lasky &Klopp, 1982). Saxton (2005) and Lasky and Klopp’s (1982) definitions of recast and imitation introduce a supplemental code to recasts. Referred to in the current study as expansions, this code has been defined as a response to an utterance that “[retains] the [child’s] words in the order given, CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 11 and [adds] those functions that will result in well-formed simple sentence that is appropriate to the circumstances” (DePaulo & Bonvillian, 1978). In expansions, the parent preserves the child’s meaning while also altering grammar and adding words or content. Previous studies suggest that expansions and reflections may promote conversation among parent-child dyads (Nelson et al., 1996; Hart & Risley, 1992). Classifying these terms as “repetitions, expansions, and extensions,” Hart and Risley (1992) viewed these types of responses as measures of the parent’s positive feedback to the child. Nelson et al. (1996) argue that recasts are natural and conversational, thus promoting the continuation of a conversation. Similarly, Saxton (2005) notes “that parents do not need to be prompted or trained to recast their children’s speech. Instead, recasting seems to fall out naturally from the constraints imposed by the challenge of talking to a linguistically naïve interlocutor,” which indicates its facilitation of flowing conversation (Saxton, 2005). Micro-level analysis of parental communication also includes measuring the length and complexity of the parent’s utterance, which have shown to depend on child age (Snow, 1972). DePaulo and Bonvillian (1978) suggest the importance of expansions pertaining to a child’s age in their review of studies examining micro-level communication patterns between parents and children. For example, Nelson et al. (1973) found that children exposed to expansions involving both grammatically incomplete and complete utterances performed better than the control group on imitating sentences and level of predicate complexity. Similar findings from a study performed by Fey et al. (1999) regarding recasts (reflections) suggest that this mechanism facilitates language learning and more complex syntax in children (Fey et al., 1999). Recasts may simplify child-processing demands and allow the child to compare the adult’s grammar with child’s own grammar, which may resolve discrepancies in their grammar. Because expansions CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 12 and reflections may potentially assist in language development and comprehensibility of parental utterances, their frequency of use by the parent may differ with age, as parental language changes with their gauging of their child’s capability of understanding. Current literature recommends open communication between parents and children who are battling a chronic or potentially fatal illness; additionally, parental communicative approaches utilizing reflections and expansions may promote more conversation from the child and assist in their understanding of complex content. However, understanding the potential link between micro-analytic communication styles and child emotions about certain significant childhood illnesses, such as cancer, remains unanswered. Additionally, no research exists in these populations examining the relationship between macro- and micro-levels of communication, such as whether the parent’s parenting style and communicative approach will direct them to use particular linguistic styles during discussions about stressful situations. Parent-Child Communication about Cancer Because pediatric cancer remains the leading cause of death by disease among children ages 1-14 years-old in the United States, psychological adjustment to this illness, its treatment, and the overall experience has been the focus of considerable research. In 2007, 10,400 children between one and fourteen years old were diagnosed with cancer, and it is predicted that 1,545 of these children will die from the illness (Edwards, 2009). However, death rates have declined dramatically in the past 20 years, as the 5-year survival rates for all pediatric cancers increased from 58.1 percent in 1975-1977 to 79.6% in 1996-2003 (Edwards, 2009). The increase in survival rates and treatment options has led to research on the adjustment of current pediatric cancer patients as well as long-term survivors. Adjustment entails measuring the child’s CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 13 externalizing and internalizing behavior problems, fear, anxiety, self-esteem, global functioning, family functioning, and their determinant factors. Parent-child communication about cancer arises as an important process of interest, as the uncertainty of cancer and its treatment and prognosis affect what the parent will ultimately communicate to the child about their illness. Clarke et al. (2005) assert that parents’ planning and strategizing about parent-child discussions concerning cancer is crucial; their findings indicate, “good psychosocial adjustment is related to a child’s early knowledge of the diagnosis,” as children who were informed about their illness at diagnosis exhibited better adjustment compared to those who were either misinformed initially or notified at a later stage (Clarke et al., 2005). Clarke et al. (2005) also suggested that parents who practiced open communication and provided more information to the child about his or her illness may better equip their child with effective coping abilities; these children “understand the importance of taking medication, feel able to discuss their worries and concerns with parents, and trust their family and health professionals” (Clarke et al, 2005). However, while parents must consider the quality and amount of information about cancer they disclose, previous studies address the importance of parents to exercise ageappropriate communication when providing information about an illness. Parents are then left with the difficult task of gauging their child’s capacity for understanding the information they provide (Chesler et al., 1986). The age of the child is significantly related to the amount of information about the illness, its treatment and prognosis disclosed by the parent, with younger children given less information than older children (Chesler et al., 1986). Snow (1972) conducted a study measuring the length of parent utterances and the quantity of speech and found that mothers spoke to younger children in shorter, less elaborate statements (Snow 1972). CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 14 Additionally, parents report that the amount of information they disclosed depends on what they feel their child is cognitively able to understand and absorb (Chesler et al., 1986). Child comprehensive evaluation by the parent is essential. For example, Clarke et al. (2005) found that refusal and lack of adherence to treatment in children with cancer were associated “with a lack of understanding, and poor communication regarding diagnosis and treatment,” suggesting that parents must find effective forms of communication to successfully explain the complexities of cancer (Clarke et al., 2005). Because cancer treatments and the terms used to describe them are complex, the task of determining an age-appropriate approach to these discussions becomes further complicated. Little research has focused specifically on parent-child interactions about cancer; however, because children rely heavily on parental explanations, close examination of parental utterances is crucial in understanding how to avoid disconnect during these conversations. Unfortunately, research on the psychological adjustment of pediatric cancer patients has had a number of limitations, and major inconsistencies exist in the findings. Bennett (1994) stated that while 9 to 14% of children suffer from chronic illnesses, “considerable disagreement exists […] as to whether children with such disorders are at increased risk for adjustment problems” (Bennett 1994). Arguments concerning psychological adjustment among children with illnesses are complicated, as studies must determine whether children with differing chronic illnesses are equally at risk of poor psychosocial adjustment. Furthermore, Bennett (1994) asserts, “children with chronic medical problems have been found to have increased rates of behavioral and emotional problems in some but not all studies” (Bennett 1994). For example, Clarke et al. (2005) reported that 84.1% of the children in their study “showed negative behavioral (temper tantrums, feeling agitated, immature, boisterous, uncooperative, and CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 15 manipulative) or mood (anxious, needy, withdrawn) changes” (Clarke et al., 2005). However, Eiser et al. (2000) found that survivors of pediatric cancer showed no signs of problems as reflected in measures of anxiety, depression or self-esteem when compared to the normative population (Eiser et al., 2000). To strengthen the understanding of psychological adjustment among pediatric cancer patients, Bruce (2005) suggests further investigation should include “assessment of discrete cancer populations, [assessment of] coping styles and life-threatening illness, [assessment of] parent-child interactions, and [the profiling of] trauma-related symptoms over time” (Bruce 2005). For example, by measuring time since diagnosis, researchers can determine if pediatric patients exhibit different behaviors throughout their experience with the illness. Without controlling these variables, findings will remain limited and ambiguous about cancer patients’ adjustment patterns. Finally, studies analyzing psychosocial adjustment in pediatric cancer patients have utilized different measures via questionnaire packets and surveys. Without standardized measures, results remain difficult to synthesize and interpret. Most studies merely assess data from questionnaires, but do not integrate any form of observational data or interviews, which would enable thorough analysis of linguistic and syntactic structure within a conversation. Furthermore, no research has been conducted examining the relationship between parental communication styles, parental linguistic structure, and emotions of pediatric cancer patient when discussing the illness. Literature considering parent child communication on a broad macro-level and a meticulous micro-level and whether this in turn affects emotions of children with cancer is nonexistent. CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 16 Current Study As noted above, parent-child communication can be measured on macro and micro levels. Because mothers are typically the primary caregivers and the source of communication and emotional support, the current study considered parent-child interactions about cancer from each perspective. Currently, no literature exists examining parent communication and parenting style. This study linked macro-level analysis of parenting style with a micro-level analysis of parent communication and expected that parental expansions and reflections of child utterances would be positively associated with a parent communicative style of higher warmth/support, communication, and listener responsiveness. Furthermore, based on current literature, open and honest communication is positively associated with better psychological adjustment. Because of this evidence, I hypothesized that certain parental contingencies to child utterances affect their child’s emotions during discussions about cancer. The current study examined whether a relationship exists between the linguistic and syntactic structure of maternal utterances and a child’s emotions during these interactions. I hypothesized that use of maternal expansions and reflections would associate with scores of maternal warmth, listener responsiveness, and communication. Additionally, I hypothesized that these parental communicative approaches would associate with higher levels of child positive mood and less anxiety and sadness from the child during these discussions about cancer. Method Participants In this two-site study, mothers were recruited from the Pediatric Hematology/Oncology units of the Vanderbilt Children’s Hospital and the Columbus Children’s Hospital-Ohio State University. Potential participants included families with children between the ages of 5 and 17 CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 17 years-old who had recently been diagnosed with cancer. Because in the study is focused on learning whether a relationship exists between cancer-related stress and time of diagnosis, families recruited as soon as possible following initial diagnosis. Mothers and children were approached 3 to 6 weeks post diagnosis and were considered eligible to participate if the child did not exhibit any learning deficits or have any additional serious medical condition separate from cancer. Currently, the study has data available for 62 mother and child interactions coded on both the macro- and micro levels, which provides information about the mothers’ general communicative styles as well as the specific linguistic and syntactic structure of their utterances. Additionally, these coded interactions account for the child’s mood throughout the dyad’s observed interaction. Thus, the current sample available for analysis includes 62 mothers between the ages of 24 and 72 (M = 38.37 SD = 8.43) and children with cancer ages 5 to 17 (M = 10.44, SD = 3.97). Within this sample of children with cancer, the distribution of diagnoses is generally representative. Leukemia represents the majority (40.0%) of the sample, with diagnoses of Lymphoma (17.6%), brain tumors (8.0%) and other solid tumors (e.g., osteosarcoma, neuroblastoma, Wilm’s tumor) (33.6%) following. Table 1 provides demographic information for both mothers and children. Demographic information for one remaining mother-child dyad is not available. All mothers and children participated during the initial weeks or months of the active phase of the child’s treatment. Measures Parental Responses and Contingency. To measure parental responses and contingency to child utterances, our research team developed a coding system assessing numerous aspects regarding micro-level analysis of linguistic and syntactic structure. During an interaction, CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 18 following a child’s utterance, the parent’s next two utterances are coded on the basis of two factors: (1) their topic and conversation maintenance, and (2) their contingency to the child’s utterance. For example, following a child’s utterance, does a parent maintain or change the topic of conversation? Because mothers constituted the majority of our sample and are typically the primary caregiver and source of information for a child, the current study analyzed segments of discussions about cancer among mother-child dyads and coded maternal responses to child utterances. Data analyses specifically examined maternal reflections and expansions, which comprise two of the ten micro-analytic contingency codes defined in the coding system developed for this study. Maternal reflections include those utterances that strictly repeat the content of the child’s utterance. Similarly, maternal expansions qualify as repetitions to the child’s utterance, but with added semantic or syntactic content. Codes in the coding system not represented in the current analyses consider other components of parental utterances, such as whether their responses to a child cause the child to reevaluate or reframe his or her emotions and perspective surrounding the topic of conversation. This coding system allowed our team to quantify the degree of parental contingency to a child’s response and allow us to measure whether certain methods of communication (e.g., reflecting or expanding upon a statement) are associated with communicative abilities among the mother-child dyad and the child’s emotions during the interaction. Transcripted versions of interactions were double-coded by a primary and a reliability coder. To contingency code the interactions, coders were required to pass a detailed, written exam with a 90%. Additionally, coders were required to practice on previously coded transcripts and discuss them with experienced coders. All coded transcripts were subjected to double- CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 19 coding, which were afterwards discussed by the primary and reliability coders, who resolved discrepancies by providing examples and clarifications from the coding manual to support a particular code. Mean percent agreement was calculated for each contingency code. For reflections, percent agreement averaged at 90% while expansion percent agreement was at 69%. Maternal parenting and communication style. Codes for emotions and communication will be a subset of those included in the Iowa Family Interaction Rating Scales (IFIRS; Melby et al., 1998) to assess levels of and styles of communication and emotions for parents and children. The IFIRS is a global observational coding system that evaluates both verbal and non-verbal behaviors during exchanges between parents and children, as well as affective aspects of each participant in the interaction. Rated on a scale of 1 (not at all characteristic) to 9 (highly characteristic), behaviors and emotions are assessed based on their frequency, intensity, and proportions during the exchange. Behaviors and emotions that are absent are represented by a score of 1 while prevalent behaviors are scored as a 9. For example, an individual rated 1 on Positive Mood displayed “no examples or evidence of Positive Mood;” an individual rated 9 “frequently” was “happy, optimistic, content, positive about self and life in general” during the interaction (Dunn et al., 2010). Fifteen codes were rated for both children and mothers, with an additional 9 codes rated for the mothers alone. Parent codes are organized into three categories: emotion, dyadic interaction, and parenting. Maternal communication style was evaluated using selected IFIRS codes measuring her expressiveness, listener responsiveness, and supportiveness throughout the interaction. Expressiveness was indicated in the mother’s scores on communicative abilities throughout the conversation (e.g., does she provide clear explanations and clarifications for her statements and reasoning?). Listener responsiveness measured how attentive the mother is to CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 20 the child’s utterances. Supportive communication was represented by the mother’s warmth and encouragement extended to the child during the interaction. The IFIRS has been validated against reports from self, sibling, and parent-report using correlation analysis and confirmatory factor analysis (Melby & Conger, 2001) and previous studies indicate the system’s strong inter-rater reliability, internal consistency, and test/retest reliabilities (Melby & Conger, 2001; Ge et al., 1996; Melby et al., 1995). There is some evidence that racial differences both in coders as well as subjects are important to consider in observational coding (Melby et al., 2003) and the study took steps to monitor this possibility. Videotapes of the interactions were coded by a primary coder and compared with the codes of a reliability coder. Coders were required to pass an in-depth written assessment of the IFIRS coding definitions and conventions with at least 90% correct. Additionally, coders must have achieved at least 80% reliability on practice tapes that had previously been coded by experienced coders. Each recorded interaction was independently coded by two trained observers, who watched the interactions a total of five times: once to become familiar with the interaction, twice more to code one participant (e.g., the mother) and finally two more times to code the other participant. The focal coded first was determined by a coin flip. Codes were then compared between the two coders: when ratings with a code were one point off from each other, the higher score was documented as the consensus score. With codes that were two or more points different, coders discussed the examples that led them to assign that score and determined a consensus score by referring to specific examples and considering their intensity in the interaction. Inter-rater reliability was 78%. Observed child emotions. Observed child emotions during the interaction were examined using a combination of the IFIRS system codes assessing their positive affect, anxiety, CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 21 prosocial behavior, and sadness throughout the discussion. Sadness measures the degree of despondence, unhappiness, sadness, regret and depressive symptoms evident in an individual during the task. Anxiety conveys the prevalence of nervousness, fear, tension, stress, worry and concern. Positive mood analyzes the frequency and intensity of positive, uplifting and optimistic statements in an individual’s verbal and non-verbal (e.g., smiling, laughing) behavior. Finally prosocial behavior indicates an individual’s cooperation, maturity and engagement in the conversation, essentially indicating their ability to promote conversation in the exchange. Procedure In this two-site study, the Institutional Review Board approved the protocol for each site. Eligible families included those with children between the ages of 5 and 17 who were recently diagnosed with cancer. The families were typically approached 3 to 6 weeks post diagnosis. Children with any significant learning deficits and additional serious medical conditions aside from cancer were exempted from the study. Patients between the ages of 10 and 17 years-old and all parents of participating families were recruited to participate in Phase I of the study, which involved completion of a questionnaire battery. Participating children completed an assent form while participating parents signed a consent form. The questionnaire battery included measures for both children and the parents. These measures required the participant to provide demographic information, medical information, reports of emotional and behavioral problems, cancer-specific worries and fears, perceived stress, psychological distress (e.g., depression), information about coping methods, family environment information, and information about their parent-child communication experiences. Following completion of Phase I, families were approached and asked to participate in a videotaped observational session (Phase II). CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 22 To address the limitations of previous studies, the second phase of the study provided a valuable supplement to gaining insight on the effectiveness of strong parent-child communication about cancer and its impact on the child’s psychological adjustment to the illness. All observational sessions at the Vanderbilt site took place at the Vanderbilt Children’s Hospital. During this phase, the patient and his or her parent were asked to participate in two tasks measuring how they communicate. The first task was a puzzle task and was meant to provide a sense of how the dyad communicates in general. The parent and child were placed at a table and separated by a divider hindering visibility of one another. The parent was given a completed version of a puzzle (which varied based on the child’s age) and told they must guide their child to construct the same puzzle purely through verbal instruction. They were given 5 minutes to complete the task and were unable to view the result until 5 minutes has passed. The second task required that the parent and child engage in a conversation about cancer, its treatments, and their personal experiences with the illness. The participants were given a card with prompt questions and were asked to engage in discussion for fifteen minutes. Prior to and following this discussion, both the parent and child were asked to complete pre- and postquestionnaires asking them how they were feeling and how much they talk about cancer with one another. After the discussion, the participants were also debriefed and asked how they felt about the conversation. Phase III asked those parents and eligible children who completed the Phase I questionnaire battery to complete a similar questionnaire packet twelve months after their recruitment. Families received compensation for each completed phase of the study. CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 23 Data Analytic Approach Means and standard deviations for mother communication and linguistic styles and child emotions were calculated and are presented in Table 2. Analyses of the primary research questions were conducted using Pearson correlations and linear multiple regression analyses. Results Preliminary Analyses To examine the representativeness of the current sample of mothers who have children with cancer, independent sample t-tests were run for mothers, with available demographic data, who completed the observation phase (n = 54) and for those who only completed the questionnaire packets (n = 155). Missing demographic data for 8 of the mothers account for the discrepancy in the larger sample size reported for in the other subsequent data analyses. Mothers’ available demographic data were used to examine mother age, education level, marital status, annual family income, child age, and child gender as potential influential factors. Furthermore, mother scores from scales in the questionnaire packets utilized to measure posttraumatic stress disorder and anxiety were examined. Results indicated no significant differences in either score of PTSD, t (151) = -1.51, ns, or of depression, t (153) = -1.38, ns, for mothers who did and did not complete the observation. Likewise, there were no significant differences for mother age, t (153) = 1.33, ns; annual family income, t (148) = 1.95, ns; marital status, t (153) = 1.02, ns; age of child, t (155) = 0.35, ns; or child gender, t (155) = -1.45, ns. However, analyses indicated a significant difference in the education status of mothers, t (153) = 2.85, p < .01, as those who chose to complete the observation were slightly more educated than those who did not. CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 24 Descriptive Statistics Means and standard deviations were calculated for IFIRS scores and total number of expansions and reflections throughout each mother-child interaction about cancer. For contingency codes, mothers’ mean use of reflections (M = 8.33, SD = 8.70) were higher than mothers’ mean use of expansions (M = 5.03, SD = 3.78). Means for mother IFIRS codes were highest for the communication code (M = 7.18, SD .89) and lowest for listener responsiveness (M = 6.70, SD = 1.15). Additionally, means and standard deviations were calculated for child IFIRS scores. These calculations for child emotions during discussions about cancer indicated that the child sadness mean score was highest (M = 5.60, SD = 1.40), while the child listener responsiveness mean score was lowest (M = 5.37, SD = 1.27). Table 2 presents all mother and child variables for which means and standard deviations were calculated. Maternal Macro- and Micro-level Communication styles Correlations between mother communication style and contingency codes during discussions about cancer. Correlations between maternal macro-level parenting and communication styles and micro-level maternal utterances are represented in Table 2. During the mother-child conversations about cancer, mothers’ use of total reflections was significantly positively correlated with mother listener responsiveness (r = .47, p < .01) and communication scores (r = .30, p < .05). Mothers’ use of total expansions throughout the interaction was significantly positively correlated with all mother communication styles: mother listener responsiveness (r = .38, p < .01), mother communication (r = .28, p < .05), and mother warmth/support (r = .27, p < .05). Mothers’ use of reflections was not significant with mother warmth/support (r = .24, ns). CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 25 Maternal Communication and Linguistic Styles and Child Emotion Correlations between mother IFIRS and contingency scores and child emotions during a discussion about cancer. Correlation analyses were conducted to consider all levels of maternal communicative patterns and their associations with different child emotions during cancerrelated discussions. Mother expansions were significantly positively associated with child positive mood (r = .36, p < .01). However, no significant associations were found for mother expansions and child anxiety (r = -.08, ns) and child sadness (r = -.19, ns). Furthermore, mother reflections were not significant with any of the child mood codes: child positive mood (r = .15, ns), child anxiety (r = -.08, ns), and child sadness (r = -.04, ns). Regarding mother parenting and general communicative style, mother warmth and supportiveness was significantly positively correlated with child positive mood (r = .42, p < .05) but not for child anxiety (r = -.03, ns) or child sadness (r = -.04, ns). Likewise, mother communication and listener responsiveness showed similar results. Mother communication was significantly positively associated with child positive mood (r = .42, p < .01), but showed no significant relationship with child anxiety (r = -.02, ns) or child sadness (r = -.16, ns). Correlations for mother listener responsiveness indicated a significant positive association with child positive mood (r = .46, p < .01) and no significant associations with child anxiety (r = -.07, ns) or child sadness (r = -.07, ns). These correlations are available in Table 4. Multiple Linear Regression Analyses Because correlation analyses indicated child positive mood as the only child emotion significantly associated with mother communication patterns, multiple linear regression analysis was facilitated to examine their roles as predictors of child positive mood during parent-child conversations about cancer. Furthermore, mothers’ use of expansions was the only contingency CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 26 code to significantly positively correlate with child positive mood. Thus, because the IFIRS codes are highly inter-correlated, three separate multiple linear regression analyses were conducted to measure mother IFIRS codes and mother expansions as predictors of child positive mood. Standardized beta-weights and semi-partial correlations were calculated with mother IFIRS codes held constant and child positive mood as the dependent variable. In the second step of the regression analysis, results indicate that mothers’ use of expansions significantly predicted child positive mood ( = .270, p < .05) when added to mother communication scores. Similarly, second-step regression analysis revealed that mothers’ expansions significantly predicted child positive mood ( = .321, p < .05) when added to mother warmth/support. However, the magnitude of mothers’ use of expansions did not predict positive mood when added to listener responsiveness but remained a small effect ( = .225) and approached significance (p < .074). Semi-partial correlations were squared to indicate the percentage of variance in the child’s positive mood by the following predictors: Mother Listener Responsiveness and Mother Expansions (sr2 = .043); Mother Communication and Mother Expansions (sr2 = .067); and, Mother Warmth/Support and Mother Expansions (sr2 = .067). These results are presented in Table 5, which provides beta-weights for solely mother IFIRS scores and child positive moods, as well as beta-weights with mother IFIRS scores and mothers expansions added as predictors in child positive mood. Discussion Pediatric cancer can be an extremely stressful illness for both children and their parents. For the parent, the loss of control over their child’s well-being challenges their role as caretaker. This traumatic change occurs at a time when their duties to protect their child are most crucial. Because children typically view their parents as a primary source of knowledge, they look to CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 27 their parents to provide information about a situation that is foreign and frightening. A child with cancer is suddenly thrown into a chaotic world of doctor’s appointments, periods of inpatient stays and prolonged painful procedures and treatments, which are masked by numerous complex terms. Such complexities and uncertainties pertaining to treatment and prognosis heighten the mystery and threatening nature of cancer. Thus, parent-child discussions about cancer provide a vital opportunity to shape the child’s perception and understanding about the illness. The previous approach to provide the child with little information about the illness and its treatments in an attempt to protect them from anxiety or distress is now being refuted in several studies (e.g., Chesler et al., 1986). Rather, the current literature (i.e., Clarke et al., 2005; Cline et al., 2006) promotes open and honest conversation that involves disclosure to the child about their cancer. Because parent-child communication is essential during stressful experiences, the current study meticulously examined specific aspects of maternal macro- and micro-level communication styles and whether they associate with child mood throughout a cancer-related discussion. In examining conversational mechanisms utilized by parents, the current study aimed to shed light on the relationships between parenting style, parental communication, and children’s emotional responses, as no research exists addressing the influence of these factors in children’s experience with cancer. Furthermore, prevailing literature fails to investigate if particular parenting styles will correlate with specific linguistic approaches. The current study delves into a fraction of the several components that construct language and general communication style and how these factors may influence child mood. Results of the study provided valuable information pertaining to the relationship of parent general communication approaches and the structural aspects of their language. These findings CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 28 supported the hypothesis that mother’s with pro-social communication styles would be positively correlated with their use of expansions and reflections. Analyses revealed that mothers who were better listeners and more effective communicators utilized more reflections of their child utterances during the discussion about cancer. While the association between responsive listening and repetition of content reveal the obvious conclusion that strong listening skills are required to repeat an utterance verbatim, the relationship with higher communication scores necessitates a more conceptual interpretation. Previous studies (i.e., Nelson et al., 1996) argue that reflections are natural and conversational. They promote the continuation of a conversation by receiving and maintaining their child’s attention and ensuring their comprehensibility, since it is the same lexical items from the child’s own utterances that are used (Saxton, 2005). Comprehensibility among participants in an interaction is a fundamental aspect of the communication code in the IFIRS rating system (Melby and Conger, 2001). Thus, a parent whose conversational style clarifies what the child is saying will receive a higher communication score: the use of reflections may function as a clarification device. Mothers whose contingent responses involved more expansions were also rated as better listeners and more effective communicators. However, these mothers were additionally perceived as warmer and more supportive towards their children during the interactions. Expansions as communication mechanisms indicate that the parent has acknowledged the child’s utterance by not only repeating their content, but also facilitating their own contributions relative to the statement. This approach exemplifies strong communicative and listening skills, and also may represent a mother’s attempt to connect with her child by conveying a sense of understanding. Rather than refute or attempt to reframe a child’s utterance, an expanded response relies upon both the mother’s preservation of her child’s meaning and her contribution to the CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 29 utterance, which may communicate supportiveness. The significant correlations between the macro- and micro-levels of parent communication styles are crucial to interconnecting the components of communication and to linking different parenting styles to deeply rooted levels of linguistic structure. For example, correlations from the current study suggest that parents who are warmer, better listeners and better communicators are more likely to use expansions, or, conversely, that mothers who more frequently utilized expansions were perceived as warmer with stronger listening and communication skills. To determine methods of easing the stress of cancer, it is important to examine aspects of parent communicative approaches that affect child mood. Results in the current study determined which general maternal communication styles and contingent responses to child utterances would predict child mood during the cancer-related discussion. Among measures of child positive mood, anxiety, and sadness, child positive mood emerged as the only emotion code to significantly correlate with mother communication styles. Furthermore, between the two contingency codes, only mother expansions were associated with child positive mood and mother communication styles. These correlations are essential in implying that particular parental communicative approaches are related to child emotions. Such implications are further supported by the regression analyses, which provide strong evidence that mothers who are warm and effective communicators will more frequently expand on their child’s utterances and will evoke higher positive emotions from their child throughout the conversation. Although listener responsiveness and expansions were not significantly predictive of child positive mood, it is noteworthy that they approached significance. As previously mentioned, child participation in discussions about their illness is significantly positively associated with their adjustment to the illness (Slavin et al., 1982). Thus, the current study’s findings suggest that CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 30 these parental linguistic and conversational techniques are effective communication skills because they encourage, acknowledge and support active participation from their child, which ultimately contributes to their child’s emotions in these discussions. Despite several findings, various limitations exist in the current study. Similar to previous literature, which fails to incorporate multiple forms of data collection, the data analyses of the current study only utilized information obtained from the observation phase. To better determine whether macro- and micro-level communication components affect child anxiety and sadness, future analyses of this sample should incorporate data from scales in the questionnaire packets, which measure PTSD and depression for parents and children. Furthermore, the current study focused on maternal responses to child utterances. To generalize these relationships among parental macro- and micro-level communication components, father responses and communication styles must also be examined. Additionally, the total number of expansions and reflections coded in the discussion were considered, rather than their percentages relative to other contingency codes present in the conversation. Therefore, lengths of conversations were unaccounted for, such as those with fewer parental utterances or sequences unavailable to code. Ultimately, as open and honest discussions appear to lessen child anxiety about an illness (Chesler et al., 1986) and parental emotional support correlates with better maintenance of a child’s illness (Martin et al., 1998), the current study indicates that effective communication skills and warmth will predict positive emotions from children during conversations about cancer. Furthermore, it suggests that expansions, which are also predictors of child positive mood, appear to be a mechanism that conveys these general communication techniques. Since parents are expected to be their child’s source of comfort and protection, these findings reveal the potential to ease their stress with cancer-related discussions by utilizing certain linguistic CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 31 structures to better communicate and convey support. Subsequent analyses should evaluate whether other parent communication styles and contingent responses to child utterances are related and if they are significant predictors of child mood. Teaching parents which linguistic structures will translate to a particular parenting style (i.e., responsive listening, supportiveness, etc.) and will benefit their child’s mood may influence their child’s overall psychological adjustment to the illness. Finally, because previous studies (i.e., Chesler et al., 1986; Snow, 1972) suggest that parents formulate their syntactic structures on their beliefs of their child’s language capabilities, future research examining whether parent macro- and micro-level communication techniques differ with children’s age would likely be beneficial. CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 32 References Achenbach, T.M., & Rescorla, R.A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms and Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families. Aldridge, A.A., Roesch S.C. (2007). Coping and adjustment in children with cancer: a meta-analytic study. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 30, 2, 115-129. Bennett, D. S. (1994). Depression among children with chronic medical problems: a meta-analysis. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 19, 2, 149-169. Brage, D., Meredith, W., & Woodward, J. (1993). Correlates of loneliness among midwestern adolescents. Adolescence, 28, 111, 685-693. Bruce, M. (2006). A systematic and conceptual review of posttraumatic stress in childhood cancer survivors and their parents. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 233-256. Chesler, M.A., Paris, J., & Barbarin, O.A. (1986). “Telling” the child with cancer: parental choices to share information with ill children. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 11, 4, 497-516. Clarke, S., Davies, H., Jenney, M., Glaser, A., & Eiser, C. (2005). Parental communication and children’s behaviour following diagnosis of childhood leukaemia. Psycho-oncology, 14, 274-281. Cline, R. J. W., Harper, F. W. K., Penner, L. A., Peterson, A. M., Taub, J. W., & Albrecht, T. L. (2006). Parent communication and child pain and distress during painful pediatric cancer treatments. Social Science & Medicine, 63, 883-898. Dahlquist, L. M., Power, T. G., Cox, C. N. & Fernbach, D. J.(1994). Parenting and child CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 33 distress during cancer procedures: a multidimensional assessment. Children's Health Care, 23, 3, 149-166. DePaulo, B. M., & Bonvillian, J. D. (1978). The effect on language development of the special characteristics of speech addressed to children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 7, 189-211. Dickstein, S., Hayden, L. C., Schiller, M., Seifer, R., & San Antonio, W. (1994). The family mealtime interaction coding system. Unpublished coding manual, Brown University School of Medicine, Providence, RI. Dunn, M. J., Rodriguez, E. M., Miller, K. S., Gerhardt, Vannatta, K., Saylor, M., Scheule, M., Compas, B. E. (in press, 2011). Direct observation of mother-child communication in pediatric cancer: Assessment of verbal and nonverbal behavior and emotion. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. Edwards, B., Ward, E., Kohler, B. A., Eheman, C., Zauber, A. G., Anderson, R. N., Jemal, A., Schymura, M. J., Lansdorp-Vogelaar, I., Seeff, L. C., van Ballegooijen, M., Goede, L., Ries, L. A. G. (2009). Annual report to the nation on the status of cancer, 19752006, featuring colorectal cancer trends and impact interventions (risk factors, screening, and treatment) to reduce future rates. Cancer, 116, 544-573. Eiser, C., Hill, J. J. & Vance, Y. H. (2000). Examining the psychological consequences of surviving childhood cancer: systematic review as a research method in pediatric psychology. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 25, 6, 449-460. Erickson, S.J., & Steiner, H. (2001). Trauma and personality correlates in long term pediatric cancer survivors. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 31, 3, 195-213. CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 34 Fey, M. E., Krulik, T. E., Frome Loeb, D. (1999). Sentence recast use by parents of children with typical language and children with specific language impairment. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 8, 273-286. Ge, X., Best, K. M., Conger, R. D., & Simmons, R. L. (1996). Parenting behaviors and the occurrence and co-occurrence of adolescent depressive symptoms and conduct problems. Developmental Psychology, 32, 717-731. Hart, B. & Risley, T. R. (1992). American parenting of language-learning children: persistent differences in family-child interactions observed in natural home environments. Developmental Psychology, 28, 6, 1096-1105. Jemal, A., Siegel, R., Ward, E., Murray, T., Xu, J. & Thun, M. J. (2007). Cancer statistics, 2008. CA: Cancer Journal for Clinicians, 57, 43-66. Kazak, A.E., Alderfer, M., Rourke, M.T., Simms, S., Streisand, R., & Grossman, J.R. (2004). Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and posttraumatic stress symptoms (PTSS) in families of adolescent childhood cancer survivors. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 29, 3, 211-219. Lasky, E. Z. & Klopp, K. (1982). Parent-child interactions in normal and language-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 47, 7-18. Lim, J., Wood, B. L., Miller, B. D. (2008). Maternal depression and parenting in relation to child internalizing symptoms and asthma disease activity. Journal of Family Psychology, 22, 264-273. Lindahl, K. M., & Malik, N. M. (2001). The system for coding interactions and family functioning. In P. Kerig, & K. M. Lindahl (Eds.), Family observational coding systems: Resources for systemic research (pp. 77–91). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 35 Maccoby, E. E. (1992). The role of parents in the socialization of children: An historical overview. Developmental Psychology, 28, 6, 1006-1017. Martin, M. T., Miller-Johnson, S., Kitzmann, K. M., Emery, R. E. (1998). Parent-child relationships and insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus: observational ratings of clinically relevant dimensions. Journal of Family Psychology, 12, 1, 102-111. Melby, J. N., & Conger, R. D. (2001). The Iowa Family Interaction Rating Scales: Instrument Summary. In P. Kerig and K. Lindahl (Eds.), Family observational coding systems: Resources for systemic research. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Melby, J. N., Ge, X., Conger, R. D., & Warner, T. D. (1995). The importance of task in evaluating positive martial interactions. Journal of Marriage & of the Family, 57(4), 981-994. Melby. J. N., Hoyt, W. T., & Bryant, C. M. (2003). A generalizability approach to assessing the effects of ethnicity and training on observer ratings of family interactions. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 20, 171-191. Miller, V.A., & Drotar, D. (2007). Decision-Making competence and adherence to treatment in adolescents with diabetes. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32, 2, 178-188. Nelson, K. E., Camarata, S. M., Welsh, J., Butkovsky, L., Camarata, M. (1996). Effects of imitative and conversational recasting treatment on the acquisition of grammar in children with specific language impairment and younger language-normal children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 850-859. Noll, R. B., Gartstein, M. A., Vannatta, K., Correll, J., Bukowski, W. M., & Davies, W. H. (1999). Social emotional, and behavioral functioning of children with cancer. Pediatrics, 103,1, 71-78. CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 36 Noll, R. B. & Kupst, M. J. (2007). Commentary: the psychological impact of pediatric cancer hardiness, the exception or the rule? Journal of Pediatric Psychology 32, 9, 1089– 1098. Prinz, R., Foster, S. L., Kent, R. N., & O’Leary, K. D. (1979). Multivariate assessment of conflict in parent-adolescent dyads. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 12, 691–700. Saxton, M. (2005). ‘Recast’ in a new light: insights for practice from typical language studies. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 21, 23-38. Slavin, L. A., O’Malley, J. E., Koocher, G. P., Foster, D. J. (1982). Communication of the cancer diagnosis to pediatric patients: Impact on long-term adjustment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 2, 179-183. Snow, C. (1972). Mothers’ speech to children learning language. Child Development, 43, 2, 549-565. CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 37 Table 1. Demographic Information for Mother and Child Participants Mother Age [mean (SD)] 38.37 (8.43) Race [n (%)] White 85.5 African-American 9.7 Other 3.2 Ethnicity [n (%)] Hispanic/Latino Not Hispanic/Latino 1.6 77.4 Annual Family Income [n (%)] < $25,000 25.8 $25,001 – $50,000 21.0 $50,001 – $75,000 9.7 $75,001 – $100,000 12.9 > $100,000 29.0 Education [n (%)] Some high school Graduated high school Graduated technical school 4.8 21.0 3.2 Some college 24.3 Graduated college 32.3 One or more years graduate school 14.5 CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 38 Marital Status [n (%)] Single 4.8 Married 66.1 Divorced 8.1 Separated 4.8 Remarried 3.2 Widowed 4.8 Living with someone 8.1 Gender [n (%)] Child Male 58.1 Female 41.9 Age [mean (SD)] 10.44 (3.97) Diagnosis Type [n (%)] Leukemia Lymphoma Brain Tumor Other Solid Tumors 40.0 17.6 8.0 33.6 CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 39 Table 2. Means and Standard Deviations for Variables. M (SD) Mother Expansions 5.03 (3.78) Mother Reflections 8.33 (8.70) Mother WM 5.96 (1.59) Mother LR 6.70 (1.15) Mother CO 7.18 (0.89) Child PM 5.32 (1.65) Child SD 5.60 (1.40) Child AX 5.37 (1.27) Note: Mother EXP = Mother Total Expansions; Mother REFL = Mother Total Reflections; Mother WM = Mother Warmth/Supportiveness; Mother LR = Mother Listener Responsiveness; Mother CO = Mother Communication; Child PM = Chile Positive Mood; Child AX = Child Anxiety; Child SD = Child Sadness. CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 40 Table 3. Correlations among Mother Communication Styles and Linguistic Structure of Utterances. Mother Total Reflections Mother Total Expansions Mother Listener Responsiveness .47** .38** Mother Communication .30* .28* Mother Warm/Support .24 .27* ** p < .01 * p < .05 CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 41 Table 4. Correlations among mother communication styles and linguistic structures of utterances, and child emotions during discussions about cancer. Mother EXP Mother REFL Mother WM Mother CO Mother LR Mother EXP 1 Mother REFL .54* 1 Mother WM .27* .24 1 Mother CO .28** .30* .63** 1 Mother LR .38* .47* .55** .76** 1 Child PM .36** .15 .42* .42** .46** Child AX -.08 -.08 -.03 -.02 -.07 Child SD -.19 -.04 -.04 -.16 -.07 ** p < .01 * p < .05 Note: Mother EXP = Mother Total Expansions; Mother REFL = Mother Total Reflections; Mother WM = Mother Warmth/Supportiveness; Mother CO = Mother Communication; Mother LR = Mother Listener Responsiveness; Child PM = Chile Positive Mood; Child AX = Child Anxiety; Child SD = Child Sadness. CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 42 Table 5. Multiple linear regression analysis of mother macro- and micro- level communication styles and child emotions during parent-child conversations about cancer. Child PM Standardized t sr2 .447 3.840 .200 Mother LR .362** 2.926 .112 Mother EXP .225 1.819 .043 .322 2.667 .095 Mother CO .322** 2.667 .095 Mother EXP .270* 2.242 .067 .398 3.337 .158 Mother WM .321** 2.661 .094 Mother EXP .270* 2.242 .067 Block 1 R2 .200 Mother LR Block 2 R2 .043* Block 1 R2 .159 Mother CO Block 2 R2 .067 Block 1 R2 .159 Mother WM Block 2 R2 .067 **p < .01 *p < .05 Note: Mother EXP = Mother Total Expansions; Mother REFL = Mother Total Reflections; Mother WM = Mother Warmth/Supportiveness; Mother CO = Mother Communication; Mother CANCER-RELATED COMMUNICATION STYLES 43 LR = Mother Listener Responsiveness; Child PM = Chile Positive Mood. Child PM is the dependent variable. Each Mother IFIRS code was tested as a significant predictor of Child PM with Mother EXP partialled out and added as a second step; part correlations were then calculated for Mother EXP and squared to show percentage of variance of the predictors on Child PM.