Running head: CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING Children’s Optimism and Coping with Stress When Living with a Parent with Depression Susanna Crowell Under the Direction of Dr. Bruce Compas Vanderbilt University CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 2 Abstract In the U.S. approximately 16% of people are affected by depression in their lifetime and it is estimated that depression will become the leading cause of disability worldwide by 2020 (Monroe & Reid, 2009). In order to help prevent depression in children who live with parents with depression, Compas et al. (2009) created and tested an intervention that involved family group cognitive behavioral therapy. Within this intervention the children were taught coping skills to deal with stress associated with their parents’ depression in order to help reduce depressive symptoms in these children. The current study found that optimism is positively correlated with positive coping strategies in children. Further, the findings show that optimism plays a role in lowering children’s affective and anxiety problems on its own, separate from coping. Implications for interventions to enhance children’s coping with the stress of living with a depressed parent are highlighted. CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 3 Children’s Optimism and Coping with Stress When Living with a Parent with Depression Depression affects approximately 16% of people in the United States in their lifetime and it is estimated to become the leading cause of disability in 2020 worldwide (Monroe & Reid, 2009). To put these numbers into perspective, nearly one out of every five people in the United States will suffer from Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) at some point in their lives (Rakow et al., 2009). It is imperative to better understand the characteristics and causes of depression in order to help those who have depression and to help those who are at risk for developing this disorder. One group who is at significant risk for developing depression is children who live with parents who suffer from depression (Fendrich, Warner, & Weissman, 1990). Not only are children of depressed parents more likely than children who live with non-depressed parents to develop depression but they are also more likely to develop other internalizing and externalizing forms of psychopathology (Compas et al., 2009). Understanding processes that increase risk vs. processes that promote resilience among children of depressed parents is a high priority. Parental Depression There are biological, social and psychological risk processes that have been shown to contribute to the risk of children living with a depressed parent for developing psychopathology (Goodman & Gotlib, 1999). For instance, Goodman and Gotlib found that children living with a depressed mother were more likely to develop depression or other disorders due to one or more of the following factors: heritability of depression, stressful home environment, depressed mother’s behavior and affect, and/or innate dysfunctional neuroregulatory mechanisms. However, there were moderators found that possibly reduce the risk of the children developing CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 4 depression or other disorders including the father’s availability and mental health; the timing of the mother’s depressive episodes; and the child’s affect, gender, and intellectual/social-cognitive skills. Several of these risk factors and moderators were examined in the current study. Research on depression has shown that often parents with depression use parenting styles that create a stressful environment for the child (Jaser et al., 2008). For instance, during a depressive episode parents typically are extremely irritable and intrusive with their children or they are distant and unavailable (Dietz et al., 2008). A parent can express neglect or distancing behaviors by not being active in their child’s life (e.g., not knowing what grade their child is in or their child’s best friend’s name). These same parenting styles often continue even after depressive episodes end (Jaser et al., 2008). Either extreme (intrusive or neglecting) on the parenting spectrum creates an environment where the child feels uncertain and faced with stress about the interactions with their parent and their home life (Dietz et al., 2008). Too often this chronically stressful environment that the children are living in leads them to have high levels of anxiety, depression, and other emotional and behavioral problems. In order for the children to adapt to living with a parent with depression they need to develop effective ways to cope with these sources of stress. Children’s Coping Compas et al. (2009) examined the consequences that children living with a depressed parent deal with while focusing on ways in which children can cope with the stress that is created from living with a parent with depression. Coping is adaptive, episodic, and interactional (Skinner & Zimmer-Gembeck, 2009). This means that coping is adaptive based on our developmental stage in life while also working in episodes and is interactional through momentto-moment situations. Compas et al. (2001) define coping as “conscious and volitional efforts to CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 5 regulate emotion, cognition, behavior, physiology, and the environment in response to stressful events or circumstances.” Also, coping is part of two broader processes of responses to stress: automatic and controlled responses. Automatic responses include how the body automatically and physiologically responds to stress, for example increased heart rate. Coping involves the conscious, controlled efforts that a person makes to deal with stress. Coping is a controlled process and can involve attempts at either primary control (e.g., problem solving) or secondary control (e.g., cognitive restructuring, positive thinking, acceptance, and distraction). Secondary control coping is an adaptive form of coping that is useful when dealing with uncontrollable stressors. Because children living with a depressed parent have little or no control over their parents’ behavior or their parents’ depression, secondary control coping may be especially adaptive and can include cognitive restructuring in which the children reframe their parent’s depression as something they can not solve or change. Alternatively, children can think positively about their parent’s depression by remembering that the depressive episodes don’t last forever. Another way children can use secondary control coping skills is through acceptance and distraction. This type of coping occurs when the children admit to themselves that their parent is depressed but decide to find something else to occupy their thoughts with rather than worrying about their depressed parent. Through an investigation of a preventive intervention to teach coping skills to children of depressed parents, Compas et al. (2010) were able to discover factors that protect children from developing depression, internalizing and externalizing problems along with other mental health problems. Specifically, research in the field of depression has established that parental depression can increase the risk of their children for developing depression and children’s coping is one factor that can help lower this risk (Sarigiani, Heath, & Camarena, 2003). Previous research has CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 6 shown that children with depressed parents who use secondary control coping skills, such as cognitive restructuring, positive thinking, acceptance, and distraction are less likely to develop symptoms of internalizing and externalizing problems (Jaser et al., 2005). It is known that not all children of depressed parents develop problems such as internalizing and externalizing or other psychological disorders. For example, Jaser et al. (2009) have shown that coping plays a major role in lowering the risk of children developing these problems. The ways in which children cope with the stress of living with a parent with depression may be an important factor in why some children develop depression or other issues while some develop fewer problems. Optimism The role of coping and important correlates of coping in children of depressed parents has not been fully identified. One important factor related to coping that has not been studied in children of depressed parents is the tendency to hold optimistic vs. pessimistic beliefs about the self and the world. Optimism is “an individual difference variable that reflects the extent to which people hold generalized favorable expectancies for their future” (Carver, Scheier, & Sergerstrom, 2010). Typically optimists and pessimists differ in their view and expectancies of the future, those with more positive outlooks on the future are considered optimists while those who have a more negative outlook on the future are considered to be pessimists. In studies of optimism in adults it has been shown that optimists and pessimists differ in multiple ways (Carver & Connor-Smith, 2010). In the face of adversity optimists are more likely to have confidence and persevere while pessimists are more likely to approach the situation with hesitation and doubt. Optimists are more likely to be physically and mentally healthier than pessimists. Optimism has been seen as a way for resilience in stressful life events. CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 7 Optimism is often measured in research by the LOT-R, a 10-item questionnaire (Scheier & Carver, 1992). Only 6 of the items are scored to measure a person’s level of optimism and 4 filler questions. Three items are reversed scored in order to ensure reliability and validity of the participants’ answers. Questions from the LOT-R include: “In uncertain times, I usually expect the best”; “If something can go wrong for me, it will”; and “I’m always optimistic about my future.” Optimism and Coping Optimism has been found to be related to coping in other populations and may be important in children of depressed parents. For example, optimism was linked to active coping efforts in women dealing with breast cancer prior to surgery (Carver et al., 1993). However, relatively little research has examined the relations between optimism and coping in children, leaving several important questions unanswered. More specifically, what role does optimism play in a child’s ability to cope with stress when raised by a parent with depression? Does the use of certain coping strategies cause children to be more optimistic and thus less depressed? Or do the children who are more optimistic cope better and are thus less depressed? Or is it the combination of coping and optimism that leads to the least depressive symptoms when living with a depressed parent? A recent review by Carver and Conner-Smith (2010) examined the relationship between personality and coping. One of the aspects of personality that they analyzed was optimism. They reviewed studies that have compared people’s types of coping to their levels of optimism. The different types of coping they examined were engagement coping, disengagement coping, and emotion-focused coping. Engagement coping includes accepting the problem or stress and coping through self-regulation and controlled expressions of emotion or efforts to solve the CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 8 problem or address the source of stress. This is what Compas et al. (2001) describe as primary control and secondary control forms of coping. Disengagement coping occurs when the person denies or ignores the stress and stressor completely. Emotion-focused coping is when a person focuses on either avoiding the stressors that cause them negative emotion or managing the emotions they feel, not necessarily managing the stressor itself. Connor-Smith and Carver found that engagement coping was positively correlated with optimism and disengagement coping was negatively correlated with optimism. Also, emotion-focused was not significantly correlated with optimism. Carver and Conner-Smith (2010) concluded that optimism is directly related to a person’s “expectancy value motivational tradition” which is related to the way in which a person copes with stress. This “expectancy-value tradition in motivational” theory means that the amount of effort a person dedicates to something is determined by their perceived likelihood of success in their endeavor. This suggests that a person’s motivation and coping levels are related to what they expect to come from a situation. Therefore, when a person has a higher level of optimism they feel that the outcome of a situation is going to be more positive than a less optimistic person would believe and they may have more confidence and motivation to use engagement forms of coping. It has also been shown that optimism leads to better overall well-being for people (Scheier & Carver, 1992). Thus, those who have higher optimism levels are not going to experience as high levels of stress as those who are less optimistic. This process may be due to the fact that people who are more optimistic have less overall stressful feelings because they typically believe that everything will turn out all right in the end. This means that the more optimistic people typically have lower anxious and depressive symptoms. In addition, the CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 9 findings about optimism and coping were replicated and validated by the Carver and ConnerSmith (2010) review mentioned previously. In another study looking at the role of optimism in a population of women recently diagnosed with breast cancer it was found that dispositional optimism, measured by the LOT-R, had a positive effect on women shortly after being diagnosed and at 6 months after diagnosis (Epping-Jordan et al., 1999). Higher levels of optimism were negatively correlated with emotion-focused disengagement coping at all time points, but not significantly correlated with emotion-focused engagement coping. However, when assessed at 3 months after diagnosis it appeared that the dispositional optimism was not a predictor of depressive or anxious symptoms. What accounts for this change in the role of optimism in the midst of this population of women’s stress? Would this same pattern occur in children of depressed parents? Also, would optimism only reduce disengagement coping in children as it did in adults in this study? Or could optimism also improve engagement coping in children even though it was not seen in adults in this study? Optimism and Coping in Children and Adolescents Sawyer, Pfieffer, and Spence (2009) provide evidence that an optimistic thinking style can lower the risk of depressive symptoms in adolescents dealing with stress and negative life events. Adolescents who experienced negative life events but used positive coping strategies and had optimistic thinking styles showed less depressive symptoms. Optimism and coping were both related to symptoms of depression. This suggests that optimism and positive coping styles can help children and adolescents from developing depression. However, in this study the researchers used their own 12-item scale to assess the adolescents’ optimism thinking styles. For future research it would be useful to use the LOT-R questionnaire, which has been shown to be a reliable measure of optimism in this age group. CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 10 Ey et al. (2005) successfully measured optimism in elementary aged children and concluded that it is critical to determine the importance of optimism and pessimism in children and adolescents. Because optimistic thinking has been shown to have a positive effect on adults, it is important to understand the role of optimism in children, especially children at higher risks of developing depression. As discussed above, important research has been done on the role of optimism in at risk populations of adults, but there has not been any research done on the role of optimism in at risk children and adolescents of parents with depression. Overall, there is still much research to be done on the role of optimism and coping in relation to depression. The findings about coping and the risk that children with depressed parents are at continue to replicate themselves making the results even more reliable and valid. At this time, it is understood that coping can help a child from developing severe internalizing and externalizing problems but it is not fully understood how the coping decreases these risks. It has also been shown that optimism can improve coping in adults, but there have not been any studies that look specifically at the effects of optimism on children’s ability to cope with stress. Current Study The current study addressed several questions about the relations between optimism and coping in a sample of parents with a history of depression and their children. Are children’s levels of optimism related to lower levels of anxiety and depression? Is children’s use of secondary control coping to lower levels of anxiety and depression and higher levels of optimism? And does children’s use of secondary control coping account for the relation between optimism and anxiety and depression? The more that is understood about how children cope and how optimistic they are can help lead researchers to a way in which to teach those children that are not as optimistic and who don’t cope as well to cope better. This will lead to fewer children CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 11 developing problems in the future and hopefully will lead to a lower percentage of depressed adults in the future. Methods Participants Participants for the intervention study from which the data was collected represented a varied sample from Nashville, Tennessee, Burlington, Vermont and the surrounding areas of both cities (see Table 1 for demographic statistics). There were 111 parents who either currently have or have had previous major depressive disorder during the lifetime of their child(ren) and 155 children of these parents from the areas. Of the parents with a history of depression there were 95 mothers (mean age of 41.2, SD = 6.8) and 16 fathers (mean age = 48.3, SD = 8.2). The families were randomly placed into one of two conditions: the cognitive-behavioral or written information. The two conditions did not differ significantly on any of the demographic variables. The children participants ranged in age from 9 to 15-years-old and included 70 girls (mean age = 11.5, SD = 2.0) and 85 boys (mean age = 11.3, SD = 2.0). The educational level of the parents included less than high school (7.2%), completion of high school (8.1%), some college (31.5%), college degree (27%), and graduate education (26.1%). The racial and ethnic breakdown of the participants was representative of the areas in Tennessee and Vermont from which the participants were from based on the 2000 U.S. Census data. This breakdown included 86% Euro-American, 5.8% African-American, 2.7% HispanicAmerican, 1% Asian-American, 1% Native American, and 3.9% mixed ethnicity. As for the socioeconomic status of the participating families their annual family income ranged from an average of less than $5,000 to over $180,000, with a median annual income of $40,000. Also, CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 12 64% of parents were married, 21.6% were divorced, 3.6% separated, 9.0% had never married, and 1.6% were widowed. Measures There are five different measures that each participant completed that are relative to the questions of optimism and coping including the RSQ, the LOT, the CBCL, and the YSR. Children’s responses to stress. The RSQ (Responses to Stress Questionnaire) measures the children’s responses to stress and their coping through a 57-item questionnaire (ConnorSmith, Compas, Wadsworth, Thomsen, & Saltzman, 2000). This measure was found to be reliable and valid in assessing a person’s coping and overall responses to stress. Confirmatory factor analyses have identified five factors on the RSQ measures; for the current study the scales that were included in the analyses were primary control coping, secondary control coping, involuntary engagement coping, and disengagement coping. Primary control coping and secondary control coping factors are both types of engagement coping while disengagement coping involves cognitive and behavioral efforts to orient away from the source of stress and/or one’s emotions. Primary control coping is demonstrated when the child tries to solve the problem that is causing him/her stress. Secondary control coping can be seen if the child tries to reason through the problem and think positively about it. Involuntary engagement coping can be seen when a child experiences rumination or intrusive thoughts. Disengagement coping is when the child denies the existence of the problem and refuses to accept the need to cope. The RSQ measures these types of coping by asking the children questions such as “When your mom or dad is sad or depressed what do you usually do?” Primary Control Coping is significantly correlated with Secondary Control Coping and Involuntary Engagement but not with the other scales (Connor-Smith et al., 2000). CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 13 Disengagement Coping was correlated with both Involuntary Engagement and Involuntary Disengagement but not correlated with either Primary or Secondary Control Coping. Finally, Involuntary Engagement and Disengagement were significantly correlated. Moderate to strong correlations among different types of coping are common in cross-sectional coping research because most individuals use multiple coping strategies, and higher levels of distress are associated with more coping of all types. Children’s levels of optimism. The LOT-R (Life Orientation Test-Revised) measures the children’s levels of optimism through a 10-item questionnaire (Scheier & Carver, 1992). Herzberg, Glaesmer, & Hoyer (2006) tested the validity and reliability of the LOT-R and found it to be a successful measure of optimism through a factor analysis. The following are the items that comprise the optimism scale: (1) In uncertain times, I usually expect the best. (2) If something can go wrong for me, it will. (3) I’m always optimistic about my future. (4) I hardly ever expect things to go my way. (5) I rarely count on good things happening to me. (6) Overall, I expect more good things to happen to me than bad. There are also four other filler questions that do not count towards to evaluation of a person’s optimism level. Children’s internalizing and externalizing symptoms. The CBCL is the Child Behavior Check List that asks parents to report on their perception of their child’s level of stress and problems (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). This measure is used to assess the children’s anxiety and depression symptoms along with internalizing and externalizing problems. The CBCL includes a 118-item checklist of problem behaviors that parents rate as not true (0), somewhat or sometimes true (1), or very true or often true (2) of their child in the past 6 months. The YSR is the Youth Self Report, which asks the children themselves to report their levels of stress and problems of their own (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). Reliability and validity of the CBCL and CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 14 YSR are well established (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). Internal consistency for the scales used in this study ranged from = .84 to = .94 for the CBCL and from = .84 to = .90 for the YSR. Test–retest reliability ranged from r = .82 to r = .91 for the CBCL and from r = .74 to r = .89 for the YSR. Internal consistency in the current sample ranged from = .78 to = .91 for the scales used in this study. Design and Procedure Compas et al. (2009) recruited and enrolled a sample of 111 families in a randomized controlled trial testing the efficacy of a preventive intervention for parents with depression and their 9 to 15 year-old children. Over the course of 2 years, data were collected at baseline, after 2 months, after 6 months, after 12 months, and finally after 24 months. The families were divided into two different groups, either the family group cognitive behavior therapy group or a written information group. Both groups were administered the tests and data was collected from all participants. The current study used data from the baseline assessment prior to families participation in the preventive intervention. The larger study examined the success of cognitive behavioral therapy on the parents’ depressive symptoms and the children’s level of stress in relation to coping (Compas et al., 2009). As stated before the group cognitive behavioral therapy was successful. Due to the nature of this large data set it can be used to investigate the children’s optimism in the face of adversity in addition to studying the effectiveness of the family cognitive behavioral preventive intervention. Also, what needs to be further investigated are the children’s responses to the Life Orientation Test and see how that is correlated with the depressive symptoms and coping results. These data are relevant to our understanding of optimism in children of depressed parents and their coping strategies. As stated above, optimism can lead to a child’s coping style and thus CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 15 lower their depressive symptoms. The hypothesis to be examined further is that children who are coping with the stress of living with a parent with depression will better cope if they have higher levels of optimism. Previous research has shown this to be true for adults dealing with cancer and other types of physical illness but no study has specifically looked at children dealing with a chronic stressor, such as living with a parent with depression (Scheier & Carver, 1992). Data Analyses The main analyses include Pearson correlations and linear multiple regression to examine relations among measures of children’s optimism, coping and emotional/behavioral problems. Results Descriptive statistics for the measures used in the current study are presented in Table 2. At baseline T scores on the YSR and CBCL scales were examined to provide a normative reference point for this sample (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). Mean T scores on the YSR and CBCL were, respectively, 55.12 and 58.00 for anxiety problems and 56.05 and 60.14 for affective problems. The mean scores for affective and anxiety problems on the YSR and the CBCL are approximately one-half to one standard deviation above the normative mean, in the range of T = 55-60. These scores indicate that, as expected, this is a sample of children at risk for more significant problems, as reflected by moderately elevated mean T scores. Due to the fact that the LOT-R has not been used extensively to measure optimism in children and adolescents there is not a norm or an established average mean score for this measure. The correlations in Table 3 demonstrate the relationships between the children’s responses to the RSQ, the LOT-R, the YSR, and their parents’ responses to the CBCL. Children’s responses to the LOT-R were positively correlated with their responses to the RSQ regarding primary and secondary control coping (i.e., r = .34 for primary control coping and r CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 16 =.40 for secondary control coping). This means that more optimistic children were also higher on positive coping strategies. In addition, children’s responses to the LOT-R were negatively correlated with their responses to the RSQ regarding involuntary engagement (e.g., emotional and physiological reactivity) and disengagement coping (i.e., r = -.29 for involuntary engagement and r = -.30 for disengagement coping). This indicates that not only was optimism positively related to positive coping strategies but it was also negatively related to negative coping strategies. There are also significant negative correlations in Table 3 between optimism and affective problems in the children. Specifically, the children’s reports of affective problems on the YSR were negatively correlated r = -.36 significantly with optimism. This pattern was also evident with anxiety problems as reported by the YSR, optimism was negatively correlated r = .30 with anxiety problems in children. Parents’ reports on the CBCL did not yield as consistent correlations with optimism as did the YSR. This is likely due to the fact that the parent rather than the children completes the CBCL. The correlations between the children’s affective problems based on the CBCL and optimism were modestly significantly correlated r = -.15. However, the correlations between the children’s anxiety problems based on the CBCL and optimism were not significant. Table 4 demonstrates the associations of the measures of optimism and coping with children’s affective problems through a linear regression analysis. In the first equation, optimism accounted for 13% of the variance in children’s affective problems. When secondary control coping was added optimism remained a significant predictor, however it only accounted 6% of the variance while secondary control coping accounted for 10% of the variance. Then in the third step disengagement coping was added and there was not a significant change in the CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 17 variance. But in the fourth step when primary control coping was added and there was an unexpected suppressor effect with disengagement coping and the change in variance was again significant. In this fourth step optimism and disengagement coping each accounted for 3% of the variance while secondary control coping and primary control coping each accounted for 10% of the variance. When accounting for all four variables (e.g., optimism, secondary control coping, disengagement coping, and primary control coping) optimism accounted for the smallest effect on affective problems in the children, however it is still at a significant level. In Table 6, the associations of the measures of optimism and coping with children’s anxiety problems through a linear regression analysis are displayed. Optimism accounted for 9% of the variance in children’s anxiety problems. When secondary control coping was added optimism remained a significant predictor, however it only accounted 3% of the variance while secondary control coping accounted for 8% of the variance. Then in the third step disengagement coping was added and there was not a significant change in the variance. But in the fourth step when primary control coping was added and there was an unexpected suppressor effect with disengagement coping and the change in variance was again significant. In this fourth step optimism insignificantly accounted for 1% of the variance in children’s anxiety symptoms. Secondary control coping accounted for 8% of the variance, disengagement coping accounted for 3% of the variance, and primary control coping each accounted for 9% of the variance. When accounting for all four variables (e.g., optimism, secondary control coping, disengagement coping, and primary control coping) optimism accounted for the least effect on anxiety problems in the children. The findings in Table 5 and Table 7 present the results of linear regression analyses predicting children’s affective problems and anxiety symptoms as reported by parents on the CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 18 CBCL. Problems with statistical significance most likely due to the fact that these results were based on the CBCL, which the parent completes, rather than the children themselves. However, in Table 5 the second step change in variance in the regression equations predicting affective problems on the CBCL was significant. Optimism accounted for 1% of the variance when combined with secondary control coping, which accounted for 2% of the variance. Discussion My research questions focused on the relationship between coping and optimism. The findings from the current study suggest that the children who are most optimistic are more likely to cope more effectively with the stress related to living with a depressed parent, meaning that optimistic children use more secondary control coping. More optimistic children report lower levels of anxiety and depression symptoms, and the ways that children cope partially accounted for the association between optimism and symptoms. The correlations between the children’s responses to the LOT-R, RSQ, YSR, and CBCL showed the relationship between children’s levels of optimism and their coping strategies. Optimism (as measured by the LOT-R) was positively correlated with secondary control coping (as measured by the RSQ) and negatively correlated with affective and anxiety problems (as measured by the YSR and the CBCL). Also, optimism was negatively correlated with involuntary engagement coping and disengagement coping. Thus, children who had higher levels of optimism, also had lower affective and anxiety problems and higher levels of secondary control coping. Based on the linear regression analysis optimism and coping both matter in children’s affective and anxiety problems. As more coping strategies were added to the equation, optimism accounted for a smaller percentage of variance in affective and anxiety problems. Nevertheless, CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 19 some optimism was shown to account for how children cope with the stress of living with a parent with depression. It may be inferred from this data that children who are more optimistic are less depressed and anxious. Therefore, optimism could be a protective factor for developing depression for children whose parents suffer from depression. Also, as the regression analysis demonstrated there are some levels of optimism that can stand alone, apart from coping, to lower anxiety and affective problems in the children. Therefore, it may be beneficial to hold an optimistic view on life for children at risk for depression. There has been very little research done on optimism in children and what implications and conclusions can be gathered from measuring optimism in children. Aside from Ey et al. (2005), researchers have not measured optimism in children, and consequently researchers have not measured what it means for children to be more or less optimistic. This study is the first known research on the relationship of optimism and coping in children dealing with the stress of living with a parent with depression. These children are at a higher risk of developing depression and are dealing with a chronic stressor. It is important to measure if optimism can help children cope better who are at an increased risk of developing depression prior to measuring if optimism is beneficial for a child at an average risk of developing depression. Based on the children’s self-reports on the YSR and LOT-R through the linear regression analysis, optimism is related to lower anxiety and depression but this relationship is partially accounted for by their use of primary and secondary control coping. However, these effects were not as strong when using the CBCL to measure the child’s anxiety and affective symptoms which suggests that some of these effects may be due to the use of the same reporting method CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 20 (i.e., children’s self reports) to assess optimism, coping, affective problems, and anxiety symptoms. This is an important area of psychology to study because so many people are affected by depression and it is imperative that we find a way to prevent it in the future. The results of this proposed study will help psychologists better understand how children cope with stress and the role children’s optimism plays in their coping. Research has already established that an individual’s optimism may help them cope better, but this current study looks specifically at children and analyzes the process by which this happens in more depth. This study does have limitations, however; it only involves children who are at risk for depression because they live with parents who have depression. The study does not include all types of children including those who are not at an apparent risk for depression. There is a need in the future research in this area to use longitudinal designs to see if these results are stable over time. This is something that can be done in the context of the Compas et al. (2009) preventative intervention study in which these data were collected. The current study only used baseline data collected prior to the preventative intervention. There are more data that range through 2 years after baseline that can be analyzed in the future to better understand the role of optimism and coping in children living with depressed parents. This study opens up the possibility for more research in the future that could include all types of children, at an increased risk of developing depression or not at risk (i.e., those living with a depressed parent and those living with parents who don’t suffer from depression). Thus, in the future researchers can discover if optimism helps all children to better cope with stress or just those who are at a higher risk for depression early in life. The current study functions to CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING determine how optimism may lower the risk of depression in children and adolescents, and is a necessary step for future preventive interventions for depression. 21 CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 22 References Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for ASEBA School- Age Forms and Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Re- search Center for Children, Youth, and Families. Barber, B. K., Olsen, J. A. & Shagle, S. (1994). Associations between parental psychological control and behavioral control and youth internalized and externalized behaviors. Child Development, 65, 1120-1136. Carver, C. & Conner-Smith, J. (2010). Personality and coping. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 679-704. Carver, C. S., Pozo, C., Harris, S. D., Noriega, V., Scheier, M. F., Robinson, D. S., Ketcham, A. S., Moffat, F. L., & Clark, K. C. (1993). How coping mediates the effect of optimism on distress: A study of women with early stage breast cancer. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 375-390. Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., Sergerstrom, S. C. (2010). Optimism. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 879-889. Compas, B. E. (2009). Coping, regulation, and development during childhood and adolescence. New Directions for Children and Adolescent Development, 124, 87-99. Compas, B. E., Forehand, R., Keller, G., Champion, A., Reeslund, K. L., McKee, L., Fear, J. M., Colletti, C. J. M., Hardcastle, E., Merchant, M. J., Roberts, L., Potts, J., Garai, E., Coffelt, N., Roland, E., Sterba, S. K., & Cole, D. A. (2009). Randomized clinical trial of a family cognitive-behavioral preventive intervention for children of depressed parents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, 1009-1020. CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 23 Compas, B. E., Potts, J., Reising, M. M., Reeslund, K. L., Williamson, J. A., Garai, E., & Forehand, R. (2010). Resilience in children of parents with a history of depression: Coping and the Regulation of Positive and Negative Affect. Manuscript submitted for publication. Connor-Smith, J. K., Compas, B. E., Wadsworth, M. E., Thomsen, A. H. & Saltzman, H. (2000). Responses to stress in adolescence: Measurement of coping and involuntary stress responses. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 976-992. Dietz, L. J., Birmaher, B., Williamson, D. E., Silk, J. S., Dahi, R. E., Axelson, D. A.,…Ryan, N. D. (2008). Mother-child interactions in depressed children and children at high risk and low risk for future depression. American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47, 574-582. Epping-Jordan, J. E., Compas, B. E., Osowiecki, D. M., Oppedisano, G., Gerhardt, C., Primo, K., & Krag, D. N. (1999). Psychological adjustment in breast cancer: Processes of emotional distress. Health Psychology, 18, 315-326. Ey, S., Hadley, W., Allen, D. N., Palmer, S., Klosky, J., Deptula, D., …Cohen, R. (2005). A new measure of children’s optimism and pessimism: The youth life orientation test. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 548-558. Fendrich, M., Warner, V. & Weissman, M. M. (1990). Family risk factors, parental depression, and psychopathology in offspring. Developmental Psychology, 26, 40-50. Herzberg, P. Y., Glaesmer, H. & Hoyer, J. (2006). Separating optimism and pessimism: A robust psychometric analysis of the revised life orientation test (LOT–R). Psychological Assessment, 18, 433-438. CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 24 Jaser, S. S., Champion, J. E., Reeslund, K. L., Reising, M. M., & Compas, B. E. (2009). “Coping and positive affect in the adolescent children of mothers with and without a history of depression.” Manuscript Under Review. Jaser, S., Fear, J. M., Reeslund, K. L., Champion, J. E., Reising, M. M., & Compas, B. E. (2008). Maternal sadness and adolescents’ responses to stress in offspring of mothers with and without a history of depression. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 37, 736-746. Monroe, S. M. & Reid, M. W. (2009). Life stress and major depression. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 68-72. Rakow, A., Forehand, R., McKee, L., Coffelt, N., Champion, J., Fear, J., & Compas, B. (2009). The relationship of parental guilt induction to child internalizing problems when a caregiver has a history of depression. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 18, 367-377. Sarigiani, P. A., Heath, P. A., & Camarena, P. M. (2003). The significance of parental depressed mood for young adolescents’ emotional and family experiences. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 23, 241-266. Sawyer, M. G., Pfieffer, S., & Spence, S. H. (2009). Life events, coping, and depressive symptoms among young adolescents: A one-year prospective study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 117, 48-54. Scheier, M. F. & Carver, C. S. (1992). Effects of optimism on psychological and physical wellbeing theoretical overview and empirical update. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 16, 201-228. Skinner, E. A. & Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J. (2009). Challenges to the developmental study of coping. New Directions for Children and Adolescent Development, 124, 5-17. CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 25 CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING Table 1 Participants’ Demographics Race White-not Latino/Hispanic Black or AfricanAmerican Asian Latino or Hispanic American Indian or Alaska Native Mixed - Specify: Other Specify: Gender Male Female Age 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 Frequency (n=242) 180 Percent 32 13.2 7 5 2.9 2.1 1 .4 16 1 6.6 .4 121 121 50.0 50.0 55 38 29 37 36 22 24 1 22.7 15.7 12.0 15.3 14.9 9.1 9.9 .4 74.4 26 CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 27 Table 2 Descriptive Statistics Measure LOT-R RSQ Primary Control Coping RSQ Secondary Control Coping RSQ Disengagement Coping RSQ Involuntary Engagement YSR Anxiety Problems (T score) YSR Affective Problems (T score) CBCL Anxiety Problems (T score) CBCL Affective Problems (T score) N 234 239 Mean 14.02 .17 Min 1.00 .07 Max 24.00 .30 Standard Deviation 4.50 .04 238 .24 .10 .37 .05 .00 239 .20 .12 .31 .03 .00 239 .22 .14 .32 .04 .00 233 55.12 50.00 78 6.78 45.90 233 56.05 50.00 80 7.25 52.58 234 58.00 50.00 77 7.52 56.49 234 60.14 50.00 81 7.85 61.56 Variance 16.79 .00 CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 28 Table 3 Correlations- Child Baseline 1 2 1 -.34** 2 3 LOT-R RSQ-Primary -Control Coping 3 RSQ-Secondary .40** .22** -Control Coping 4 RSQ-.29** -.31** -.71** Involuntary Engagement Coping 5 RSQ-.30** -.64** -.28** Disengagement Coping 6 YSR-Affective -.36** -.37** -.42** Problems 7 YSR-Anxiety -.30** -.34** -.36** Problems 8 CBCL-Anxiety -.02 .01 -.12 Problems 9 CBCL-Affective -.15* -.13* -.17* Problems **Correlation is significant at the .01 level * Correlation is significant at the .05 level 4 5 6 7 8 9 -- -.07 -- .46** .18** -- .44** .16* .68** -- .23** -.10 .16** .37** -- .18** .07 .36** .34** .48** -- CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 29 Table 4 Linear Regression Baseline Regression Equations Predicting Affective Problems on the YSR in Children at Baseline Equation 1—Affective Problems Final R2= .30*** Block 1: R2 change = .13*** Optimism -.36*** sr2 .13 Block 2: R2 change = .09*** Optimism Secondary Control Coping -.23*** -.32*** .06 .10 Block 3: R2 change = .001 Optimism Secondary Control Coping Disengagement Coping -.23*** -.32*** .03 .05 .09 .00 Block 4: R2 change = .08*** Optimism Secondary Control Coping Disengagement Coping Primary Control Coping -.17** -.32*** -.20** -.37*** .03 .10 .03 .10 Note: = standardized beta; sr2 = semi-partial correlation squared; Optimism Measured by LOTR; Primary, Secondary, and Disengagement coping measured by RSQ *p<.05 **p<.01 ***p<.001 Affective Problems measured by YSR CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 30 Table 5 Linear Regression Baseline Regression Equations Predicting Affective Problems on the CBCL in Children at Baseline Equation 1—Affective Problems Final R2= .05 Block 1: R2 change =.02* Optimism -.15* sr2 .02 Block 2: R2 change = .02 Optimism Secondary Control Coping -.10*** -.14 .01 .02 Block 3: R2 change = .00 Optimism Secondary Control Coping Disengagement Coping -.10 -.14 -.00 .01 .02 .00 Block 4: R2 change = .00 Optimism Secondary Control Coping Disengagement Coping Primary Control Coping -.08 -.14 -.08 -.13 .01 .02 .00 .01 Note: = standardized beta; sr2 = semi-partial correlation squared; Optimism Measured by LOTR; Primary, Secondary, and Disengagement coping measured by RSQ *p<.05 **p<.01 ***p<.001 Affective Problems measured by CBCL CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 31 Table 6 Linear Regression Baseline Regression Equations Predicting Anxiety Problems on the YSR in Children at Baseline Equation 1—Anxiety Problems Final R2= .23* Block 1: R2 change = .09*** Optimism -.30*** sr2 .09 Block 2: R2 change = .07*** Optimism Secondary Control Coping -.18** -.29*** .03 .08 Block 3: R2 change = .00 Optimism Secondary Control Coping Disengagement Coping -.18* -.28*** .03 .03 .07 .00 Block 4: R2 change = .08*** Optimism Secondary Control Coping Disengagement Coping Primary Control Coping -.12 -.28*** -.20** -.37*** .01 .08 .03 .09 Note: = standardized beta; sr2 = semi-partial correlation squared; Optimism Measured by LOTR; Primary, Secondary, and Disengagement coping measured by RSQ *p<.05 **p<.01 ***p<.001 Anxiety Problems measured by YSR CHILDREN’S OPTIMISM AND COPING 32 Table 7 Linear Regression Baseline Regression Equations Predicting Anxiety Problems on the CBCL in Children at Baseline Equation 1—Anxiety Problems Final R2= .03 Block 1: R2 change = .00 Optimism -.02 sr2 .00 Block 2: R2 change = .01 Optimism Secondary Control Coping .03 -.13 .00 .01 Block 3: R2 change = .02* Optimism Secondary Control Coping Disengagement Coping -.00 -.15* -.14* .00 .02 .02 Block 4: R2 change = .00 Optimism Secondary Control Coping Disengagement Coping Primary Control Coping .00 -.15* -.18* -.07 .00 .02 .02 .00 Note: = standardized beta; sr2 = semi-partial correlation squared; Optimism Measured by LOTR; Primary, Secondary, and Disengagement coping measured by RSQ *p<.05 **p<.01 ***p<.001 Anxiety Problems measured by CBCL