Parental Hostility Parental Warmth and Hostility in Parent-Child Interactions in Families of Depressed Parents Elissa Gerfen Vanderbilt University 1 Parental Hostility 2 Abstract The purpose of this study is to examine the effects of parental hostility and warmth on the children of depressed parents. The study includes parents with a history of Major Depressive Disorder and their children as participants. Parents and their children were filmed responding to pre-determined questions about positive and negative events in their families during two 15-minute interactions. The focus of the study is on levels of aggression and externalizing behaviors of the children in relation to the levels of parental hostility and warmth. The results demonstrated a positive correlation between parental hostility and child aggression and externalizing. Conversely, the results demonstrated a negative correlation between parental warmth and child aggression and externalizing behavior problems. Further, the regression analyses demonstrated a stronger association between parental hostility than parental warmth with children’s behavior problems. Parental Hostility 3 Hostility in Parent/ Child Interactions in Families with Depressed Parents Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) is among the most common psychological disorders. Further, rates of depression are highest among individuals from families with a history of depression. While the pathway of development of depression is not clear, environmental pressures such as living with a parent suffering from depression can increase the chance of the children developing depression themselves. Studies have found that stressful relationships are both the cause and the result of depression (Hammen, 2003) and therefore interpersonal relationships between depressed parents and their children are an important focus of research in regard to the transmission of depression. Research has suggested that as many as 45% of children of depressed parents will develop major depression by adolescence (Jacob & Johnson, 2001). A follow-up study found that the peak age of onset of MDD is from 15-20 years and children of depressed parents have a five times higher rate of developing MDD than children of non-depressed parents (Weissman et al., 1997). These high rates of the development of depression in children of depressed parents demonstrate the importance of studying the interpersonal relationships that occur within interactions of the child and parent. There have been numerous studies conducted on the different aspects of parenting in depressed parents that lead to child depression. For example, Radke-Yarrow et al. (1994) noted that the prototypic depressed mother can have symptoms including anxious and depressive affect, irritability, helplessness, emotional unavailability, and absorption in self. Parental impairments include high rates of dysphoric mood, low rates of positive affect, negative views of the child, high rates of criticism, and low involvement or conversely over involvement with the child (e.g., Jaser et al., 2005; Lovejoy et al., 2000, Parental Hostility 4 Radke-Yarrow et al., 1994). While some studies have suggested a “prototypic depressed mother” there are no set patterns for the specific combination of impairments or characteristics that a parent will display; each parent has a unique pattern and range of numbers of these. A meta-analysis by Lovejoy et al. (2002) examines previously conducted studies on maternal depression and parenting behavior. These studies were broken down into negative coercive behaviors, positive behaviors, and disengagement. Mothers demonstrate a wide range of parenting behavior difficulties when in an episode of depression according to qualitative review of research (Lovejoy et al., 2000). These impairments disrupt the parent’s abilities to successfully parent and it has been found that these impairments persist beyond episodes of depression. For a parent to be considered currently depressed they must either be currently experiencing an episode of depression or have had an episode within the last 12 months (Lovejoy et al., 2000). Parents who have a history of depression must have had at least one episode of depression that fulfilled the DSM-IV diagnosis. Although the effects of depression endure, larger effect sizes appear for negative and coercive behavior in parents who are in episodes of depression than in parents who have a history of depression but who are not currently depressed. The same holds true for demonstrations of sadness, disengagement, and antisocial behavior during both positive and negative interactions (Jaser et al., 2008). All of these parental impairments lead to risks for children of depressed parents and ultimately an increased chance of children developing depression and other mental health problems. Parental depression and the resulting impairments in parenting ability can interfere with their children’s emotional development. Children of depressed parents Parental Hostility 5 may receive relatively little help from their parents in understanding as well as regulating their own feelings and emotions. They also have mixed expectations regarding care giving. These “maladaptive representations of social relationships” lead to emotional insecurities and may provide examples for increased risk of psychopathology development in children of depressed parents (Cummings, 1995). Two of the ways that parental depression manifests itself in relation to the child is through lack of involvement (withdrawal) or over involvement (intrusiveness) (Jaser et al., 2005). Parental intrusiveness can take a hostile tone with the child. Past research has demonstrated the importance for examining the effects of parents who display both hostility and depression. Alpert et al. (2003) demonstrated that parents who are depressed and express “anger attacks” relates to children who have externalizing problem (e.g., attention problems, delinquency, and aggression). This study also hints at an increase in the recurrence of MDD episodes associated with the presence of hostility. Alpert et al. (2003) unfortunately had a relatively small sample size and included children who were experiencing subclinical symptoms. Many of the parents were also currently experiencing an episode of depression, which may have worsened the effects of the hostility. As discussed before, the effects of MDD continue to affect the lives of those involved even when a parent is not experiencing an episode. Therefore it is important to examine whether a parent who is not experiencing an episode of MDD will still have the same effects when hostility is involved. Similar rates of externalizing symptoms in children have been found even when parental anger and hostility is directed at someone other than the child. For instance, internalizing (e.g., anxiety and depression) and externalizing problems are related to Parental Hostility 6 exposure to interparental or marital conflict (Papp, Cummings, & Goeke-Morey, 2002). A longitudinal study demonstrated marital hostility related to increases in the internalizing of children and conversely, husband’s marital withdrawal related to increases in the internalizing symptoms in those same children (Katz & Gottman, 1993). Sturge-Apple, Davies, and Cummings (2006) expanded on this research and found that while interparental hostility did not directly affect child adjustment, families were not immune to the effects of hostility. Rather they interpret their findings on the basis of the “spillover hypothesis” with interparental hostility affecting parental withdrawal, which in turn affects child adjustment. Expanding on the research that multiple risk factors lead to harmful effects on child development (e.g., Appleyard et al., 2005; Forehand et al., 1998) recent research that the existence of both marital conflict and parental depression in the household will increase a child’s risk for emotional insecurity (Kouros, Merrilees, & Cummings, 2008). This suggests that if children who are affected by parental depression and marital hostility suffer from emotional difficulties, children who encounter direct hostility would suffer as well. For instance, in a study of over reactive mothers, Kouros, et al. (2008) found a correlation with externalizing behaviors in infants (2-3 year-olds). Cummings and colleagues found that those children who have past experiences with marital conflict in the home are more apt to pick up on when their parent use constructive ideas in conflict resolution in their conversations (Cummings et al., 2007). This relates to the studies that propose that when parental warmth is present, it negates the effects of parental hostility, on children’s externalizing behaviors (Shaw, et al., 1998). This study also notes the importance of the inverse relationship, that when a parent exhibits a lack of warmth, it can cause an increase in children’s externalizing behaviors, Parental Hostility 7 which may be due to children’s inability to recognize consequences for their behavior (O’Leary et al., 1999). However, little research has been done with regards to parental hostility in depressed parents. Ongoing research at Vanderbilt University and the University of Vermont provide the current data of direct observation of parent-child interactions. Direct observation of parent-child interactions in this study may provide information about the parent’s role in the functioning of their offspring. In particular, the role of parental hostility and parental warmth are important to this for understanding the influence of parental hostility on children’s behavior. Since both parental depression and parental hostility directly contribute to stress for children. I hypothesize that parents who demonstrate hostility will have children who demonstrate more externalizing behaviors such as oppositional defiance and aggression. Second, I also hypothesize that those parents who demonstrate high levels of warmth will have children with inversely related externalizing scores. Thirdly, I will examine the interaction of the levels of warmth and hostility in the parent and the effects on the child. Method Participants The study included 103 parents (91 mothers and 12 fathers) and 137 children (66 girls and 71 boys) of these parents as participants. Each parent must have experienced at least one episode of Major Depressive Disorder in their lifetime, but may not currently meet the DSM-IV criteria Major Depressive Disorder, Psychotic Disorder, nor bipolar disorder. The children must be between the ages of 9 and 15-years-old (M=11.45) and may not have a current substance abuse problem or any history of conduct disorder. This Parental Hostility 8 age range paralleled previous studies on the children of depressed parents (e.g., Anderson & Hammen, 1993; Rudolph & Hammen, 2000). In addition, the children may not be experiencing a current episode of depression. The participants were all part of a larger multi-site longitudinal study, which evaluates an intervention technique (Compas et al., 2002). Measures Parents and children both completed assessments of children’s behavior problems. The parents completed the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), which is a 118-item checklist assessing the child’s behaviors (Abenbach & Rescorla, 2001). The children completed the Youth Self Report (YSR), the adolescent counterpart to the CBCL (Abenbach & Rescorla, 2001). The YSR is a self-report designed for 10 to 16-year-olds, designed for them to rate their own functioning over the past 6-months for 112 items. In particular, this study used the Aggressive Behavior and Externalizing scales to address the question of behavior changes in the child. Data are reported as normalized T scores, based on separate norms for age and sex, but raw scores were used in all analyses to allow for maximum variance. Parents completed the Structured Clinical Diagnostic Interview (SCID). This interview assesses both the current and past psychiatric diagnosis. The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II) was used to assess the levels of depression in the parent. This measure may demonstrate data that differs from the SCID. The interactions were coded using the Iowa Family Interaction Rating Scales (IFIRS). This rating scale is meant to code the observational data. Coders must reach a level of reliability before they can begin to code these taped interactions. Two trained Parental Hostility 9 coders code each video. This inter-rater reliability helps to reestablish the reliability of the IFIRS system. The consensus of the two coders also helps to resolve differences in the coding. Each parent is coded for 23 different codes and each child is coded for 14 codes, each on a 9-point scale. This study focused on the codes for hostility and warmth in the parents. The IFIRS code for hostility measures the degree to which the parent displays hostile, angry, critical, disapproving and/or rejecting behavior toward their child during the interaction. The code for warmth measures the degree to which the parent displays or expresses appreciation, care, concern, liking, or support for their child during the interaction. Procedure. Following the collection of parent and child reports, the parent and child will complete a direct observation task. The task includes two 15-minute filmed interactions between the parent and the child. The parent and child were given a set of questions for each fifteen-minute interaction. Task 1 focused on a positive activity that the parent and child had shared and the Task 2 focused on a recent time of depression or stress for the parent that caused stress to the child. Both topics came from questionnaires that the parent and child filled out at the beginning of the experiment. Two trained coders code each fifteen-minute interaction separately before reaching consensus. Each coder views the video for a total of five times, once for a preliminary viewing and twice for both the parent and the child. Following coding, consensus is reached with several trained coders to ensure reliability. Results Descriptive Statistics Parental Hostility 10 Means and standard deviations for aggressive children’s aggressive symptoms and externalizing behaviors are presented for children’s self-reports and parents’ reports, as well as the consensus codes for parents’ hostility and warmth, in Table 1. All scores presented are either the raw sum scores or the consensus scores for the direct observations. Correlations Pearson correlations were used to test the hypotheses regarding the relations between children’s aggressive symptoms and externalizing behaviors against parental hostility and warmth. The correlations for the independent variables of parent’s hostility and warmth and the dependent variables of aggressive and externalizing behaviors are presented in Table 2. Parents and children (on the CBCL and YSR, respectively) reported positively correlating scores for aggressive and externalizing behaviors. Aggressive behavior had a correlation of .474, p < .001 and externalizing behavior had a correlation of .480, p < .001. This demonstrates an inter-rater reliability between the parent and the child for scoring the child. Since both the CBCL and YSR scores for externalizing behaviors include their respective scores for aggressive symptoms, the two scores predictably have a positive correlation. The correlations between the dependent variables and Parental Hostility scores are consistent with the first hypothesis that parents who demonstrate hostility will have children with increased levels of aggressive symptoms and externalizing behaviors. Both scores of the CBCL aggression (r = .42 and .31) externalizing problems (r = -.42 and .35) and the YSR aggression (r = .26 and .28) and externalizing problems (r = .28 and .27) had Parental Hostility 11 a significant positive correlation with Parent Hostility in both Task 1 and Task 2 (p < .002). Similarly, the correlations between the dependent variables and the Parental Warmth scores are consistent with the second hypothesis that parents who demonstrate warmth will have an inverse relationship with the levels of aggression and externalizing behaviors in their children. In concordance with the second hypothesis, all correlations had a significantly inverse relationship (r’s ranged from -.19 to -.31, all p < .05), except for the correlation between Parental Warmth, Task 1, consensus code and the YSR score for Aggressive Behavior (r = -.15, p = .07). Multiple Linear Regressions of the Effects of Parental Hostility and Warmth A multiple linear regression analysis was used to examine the interaction effects that Parental Hostility and Warmth have on the aggressive and externalizing behaviors. The results from this multivariate analysis indicated that in most cases Parental Hostility remained significant, even when taking into account for Parental Warmth (Table 3). In two instances Parental Warmth remained significant. These instances included in predicting for CBCL Aggressive and CBCL Externalizing, Parental Warmth, Task 1 achieved a significant value (t = -2.14, p = .04; t = -2.67, p < 01). Thus, both parental warmth and hostility were significant predictors of children’s aggressive and externalizing problems, and there is some evidence that hostility is a stronger predictor than warmth. Discussion The first hypothesis was that parents who demonstrate higher levels of hostility would have children who have correlating high levels of aggression and externalizing. In Parental Hostility 12 accordance with previous research done by Papp et al. (2002) with regards to the relationship between interparental hostility and increases in externalizing behavior problems in their children, all of the correlations between observed parental hostility on both a positive and a stressful task and the parent and child reports of children’s aggression and externalizing were significantly and positively correlated. The second hypothesis was that parents who demonstrate higher levels of warmth would have children with lower levels of aggression and externalizing problems. All but one of the correlations (parental warmth on the positive task and children’s self-reports of aggression) demonstrated this inversely significant relationship (p < .05). These results suggest that both parental hostility and parental warmth have a direct relationship with their children’s levels of aggression and externalizing problems. Therefore, I examined the relationship between parental hostility and warmth and the effects of this relationship on the aggression and externalizing of the children. Warmth remained significant after taking into account parental hostility in only two instances---on the positive task with parent reports of children’s aggressive and externalizing behavior problems. This implies a possible effect resulting from the difference in task. Contrary to Franck and Buehler’s (2007) findings that warmth was not a buffer from the negative effects of marital conflict, this study suggests that warmth may actually be able to provide some sort of buffer. Further research will need to examine the effects of warmth in a longitudinal study. The fact that hostility continued to be significant over warmth in five of the other six regressions, demonstrates the importance of studying parental hostility in parents with a history of depression Parental Hostility 13 This study had several limitations that will need to be addressed as the study proceeds beyond the baseline data collection. This particular analysis was only a crosssectional examination of this population and therefore the causal direction is not clear due to this research design. That the parents’ hostility is the cause for these aggressive and externalizing behaviors is more of an inference. It is plausible that the parental hostility is in reaction to the aggressive and externalizing behaviors that the children exhibit. A longitudinal examination of the participants will provide more evidence toward causality. Fortunately, the study will continue beyond the cross-sectional level and is currently scheduled to follow these families for two years. Another aspect that limited the experiments was the fact that the parents were heterogeneous regarding whether they were in an episode of depression or not. The fact that some parents may have been in an episode of depression at the time of the baseline interactions may have skewed their levels of hostility. However, as demonstrated maladaptive parenting behaviors persist beyond an episode of depression but should still be addressed in future studies (Lovejoy et al., 2000). This experiment also had several strengths. One of the main strengths is the sample size. This large sample size was comparable to or larger than many of the studies previously conducted on the subject of marital hostility and was able to add to the statistical power of the analyses. Another strength was the multiple methodologies used in evaluating the variables. Both the parents and the children reported on the offspring’s Aggressive Behavior and Externalizing. This use of multiple methodologies helped to eliminate reporter bias. In addition, the direct observation component brought with it a different aspect of the parents actually interacting with their children. The strength of the Parental Hostility 14 inclusion of the direct observation could be seen in the correlations between the interactions and the dependent variables across the two separate tasks. A third strength is the fact that we identified an at risk group of children. These children, as demonstrated by their T scores, have higher levels of aggression and externalizing behavior problems than the normal population. In conclusion, this study provides a basis for demonstrating the need of further research into the role parental hostility plays in the aggressive and externalizing behaviors of the offspring of depressed parents. The data demonstrated a significant correlation between hostility and children’s behaviors, which lends itself to a possible area of interest when conducting interventions for at risk children. This study demonstrated another example of the importance of warmth in affecting a decrease in aggressive and externalizing behaviors. Parental Hostility Table 1. Descriptive Variables. Variables CBCL Aggressive Behavior T score CBCL Externalizing T score YSR Aggressive Behavior T score YSR Externalizing T score Parent WM, Task 1, Consensus Code Parent WM, Task 2, Consensus Code Parent HS, Task 1, Consensus Code Parent HS, Task 2, Consensus Code Mean 56.89 53.95 55.76 50.29 4.90 4.70 3.48 3.82 Std. Deviation 7.67 10.95 7.51 10.78 1.53 1.87 1.79 2.03 N 132 132 134 134 135 131 135 131 15 Parental Hostility 16 Table 2. Correlations of Parent’s Child Reports and Children’s Self Reports of Aggressive Symptoms and Externalizing Behaviors and Consensus Codes on Parents Hostility and Warmth. Variable 1. CBCL Aggressive Behavior 2. CBCL Externalizing 3. YSR Aggressive Behavior 4. YSR Externalizing 5. Parent WM, Task 2 6. Parent WM, Task 1 7. Parent HS, Task 2 8. Parent HS, Task 1 *p<.05. **p<.01 1 --.98** .47** .48** -.28** -.26** .42** .31** 2 --.46** .48** -.29** -.31** .42** .35** 3 --.96** -.28** -.15 .26** .25** 4 ---.29** -.19* .28** .27** 5 --.44** -.48** -.28** 6 ---.36** -.31** 7 --.43** 8 --- Parental Hostility 17 Table 3. Multiple Linear Regression of the Effects of Parental Hostility and Warmth on the Aggressive and Externalizing Behaviors of their Children. Step R2 β at Entry t Predicting CBCL Aggressive 1. Parent HS, Task 1 .09 .31 3.82** 2. Parent HS, Task 1 .25 2.94** Parent WM, Task 1 .12 -.18 -2.14** 1. Parent HS, Task 2 2. Parent HS, Task 2 Parent WM, Task 2 Predicting CBCL Externalizing 1. Parent HS, Task 1 2. Parent HS, Task 1 Parent WM, Task 1 1. Parent HS, Task 2 2. Parent HS, Task 2 Parent WM, Task 2 Predicting YSR Aggressive 1. Parent HS, Task 1 2. Parent HS, Task 1 Parent WM, Task 1 1. Parent HS, Task 2 2. Parent HS, Task 2 Parent WM, Task 2 Predicting YSR Externalizing 1. Parent HS, Task 1 2. Parent HS, Task 1 Parent WM, Task 1 1. Parent HS, Task 2 2. Parent HS, Task 2 Parent WM, Task 2 *p<.05. **p<.01 .18 .18 .12 .16 .18 .19 .06 .07 .07 .08 .07 .08 .08 .11 .42 .37 -.10 5.37** 4.14** -1.11ns .35 .27 -.22 4.36** 3.31** -2.67** .42 .37 -.11 5.39** 4.09** -1.28ns .25 .23 -.08 4.76** 2.64** -.91ns .26 .16 -.20 3.12** 1.73ns -2.13ns .27 .23 -.11 3.32** 2.72** -1.30ns .28 .18 -.21 3.34** 1.89ns -2.20ns Parental Hostility 18 References Abenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles. Burlington: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families. Appleyard, K., Egeland, B., van Dulmen, M. H., & Sroufe, L.A. (2005). When more is not better: The role of cumulative risk in child behavior outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 235-245. Alpert, J. E., Petersen, T. et al. (2003). Behavioral and emotional disturbances in the offspring of depressed parents with anger attacks. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 72, 102-106. Brennan, P. A., Hammen, C., Katz, A. R., Le Brocque, R. M. (2002). Maternal depression, paternal psychopathology, and adolescent diagnostic outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 1075-1085. Cummings, E. M. (1995). Security, emotionality, and parental depression: A Commentary. Developmental Psychology, 31, 425-427. Forehand, R., Biggar, H., & Kotchick, B. A. (1998). Cumulative risk across family stressors: Short- and long-term effects for adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 119-128. Franck, K. L., & Buehler, C. (2007). A family process model of marital hostility, parental depressive affect, and early adolescent problem behavior: The roles of triangulation and parental warmth. Journal of Family Psychology, 21, 614-625. Hammen, C., & Brennan, P. A. (2003). Severity, chronicity, and timing of maternal depression, and risk for adolescent offspring diagnoses in a community sample. Parental Hostility 19 Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 253-258. Jaser, S. S., Fear, J. M., Reeslund, K. L., Champion, J. E., Reising, M. M., & Compas, B. E. (2008). Maternal sadness and adolescents’ responses to stress in offspring of mothers with and without a history of depression. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 37, 736-746. Jaser, S. S., Langrock, A. M., Keller, G., Merchant, M. J., Benson, M. A., Reeslund, K. L., Champion, J. E., & Compas, B. E. (2005). Coping with the stress of parental depression II: Adolescent and parent reports of coping and adjustment. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 34, 193-205. Johnson, S. L., & Jacob, T. (2001). Sequential interactions in the parent-child communications of depressed fathers and depressed mothers. Journal of Family Psychology, 15, 38-52. Katz, L. F., & Gottman, J. M. (1993). Patterns of marital conflict predict children’s internalizing and externalizing behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 29, 940-950. Kouros, C. D., Merrilees, C. E., & Cummings, E. M. (2008). Marital conflict and children’s emotional security in the context of parental depression. Journal of Marriage and Family. 70, 684-697. Lovejoy, M. C., Graczyk, P. A., O’Hare, E., & Neuman, G. (2000). Maternal depression and parenting behavior: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 561-592. O'Leary, S. G., Slep, A. M. S., Reid, M. J. (1999). A longitudinal study of mothers' Parental Hostility 20 overreactive discipline and toddlers' externalizing behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 27, 331–341 Papp, L. M., Cummings, E. M., & Goeke-Morey, M. C. (2002). Marital conflicts in the home when children are present versus absent. Developmental Psychology, 38, 774-783. Radke-Yarrow, M., Zahn-Waxler, C., Richardson, D. T., Susman, A., & Martinez, P. (1994). Caring behavior in children of clinically depressed and well mothers. Child Development, 65, 1405-1414. Shaw, D. S., Winslow, E. B. Owens, J. I., Vondra, J. F. Cohn and R. Q. Bell. (1998). The development of early externalizing problems among child from low-income families: A transformational perspective, Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 95–107. Sturge-Apple, M. L., Davies, P. T., & Cummings, E. M. (2006). Impact of hostility and withdrawal in interparental conflict on parental emotional unavailability and children’s adjustment difficulties. Child Development, 77, 1623-1641. Weissman, M. M., Warner, V., Wickramaratne, P., Moreau, D., & Olfson, M. (1997). Offspring of depressed parents: 10 years later. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 932-940.