Esther Dabagyan SED 625 11/28/06 Dr. Rivas Current Event # 4 My fourth current event is about a research paper tilted The Features of Peer Argumentation in Middle School Students’ Scientific Inquiry, by Heekyong Kim and Jinwoong Song. The main focus on the paper was to answer the following questions: 1. 2. 3. 4. What are the sources of evidence used in students’ arguments? Which strategies are used in students’ argumentation? How does critical discussion proceed? Which types of discussion are found in critical discussion? (Kim and Song, 2005) The researchers focused on eight eighth graders; two boys and six girls in a middle school in Seoul, Korea. The students conducted an open inquiry activity in small groups of two and three where they had to write a report for peer review and defend their report through a critical discussion. The whole study took place over a 2 month period where the eight students were given surveys, recorded with audio and video and asked about their experience. None of the students had conducted an open inquiry before where they had to decide on a problem and devise their own procedure. They also had never written a report for their peers nor had a critical discussion about their findings. However, all eight students had middle to high achievement levels in science, thus giving them a comfortable level of scientific background knowledge with which to have good discussion with. The same can not be said about most of my students. The findings were that the majority of evidence used by students during arguments was personal evidence and not “authoritative” evidence, such as evidence from external literature or the teacher. It was found that students used a variety of strategies during argumentation including criticism, repetition, cutting short of an opponent, challenging, muttering, negotiation, changing the topic, suggesting and explicating closing the topic. The discussions proceeded typically in four stages, focusing the argument, exchanging information and details, debating and closing the argument. Most debate resulted in a coexistence of ideas where groups acknowledges that there may be more than one possible answer or confrontation where the groups rejected the other group’s outcome. Consensus was not reached by the groups very of then. I found it interesting that the focus of the paper was not to gage the quality of argumentation but to discover the process. However, at the same time, there were not enough logistical details, such as how much time exactly was given for students to write their group reports, to implement this in my own classroom. It is clear that open inquiry lessons were designed to help students argue; however the 2 month timeline seems very unrealistic, especially when the process is described at cyclic, allowing students to go back and redo their experiments after their critical discussions and peer-reviews of reports. One problem the students had was finding a topic or a problem to investigate. The authors admit that this stage actually took half the allowable time for the entire process. I have also noticed this to be a problem with my own students. Each group did a different inquiry experiment however; they were given guidelines for their reports. It seems that of all the three groups, they each had the opportunity to report to two different groups, and listen to two different groups. It is unclear as to what the third group did while the other two where in the process of peer review and argumentation since the events are described as two group events. Was the third group allowed to observe or were they removed from the class? The excerpts from their discussions seem very advanced, I do not believe that the students would be as calm and thoughtful had the debate been in front of a class of thirty-five instead of their two peers. Ultimately, this study speaks volumes to me, in that in shows me that I must build into my labs the opportunity for my students to analyze and reflect on their data. And while there seems to be one predominant method of argumentations, the study states that there were many complex types of processes also, where the stages they identified were mixed up. This seems very true to real science, where there isn’t just one way to conduct an experiment, but many. I’m unsure whether this study is positing that peer argumentation is the best way to force students to reflect and analyze their data, but it certainly seems to be leaning that way. If perhaps they had cast a wider net, thus increasing their sample size, their finding would be more exciting and relevant. I’m not sure how my English language learners would handle a report for peer review, let a lone a debate about their findings in an experiment. If I were to implement something similar in my classroom, it would have to be for only very few labs and in a guided inquiry experiment rather than open inquiry. Kim and Song reported that the students expressed interest in reforming their hypothesis and most even went back after their peer review and performed their experiment again, controlling more variables and re-working their methods. When I read this, it was a science teacher’s dream come true that students would actually desire to modify their experiments to such detail, and the education in this is invaluable but really – who has the time? How can we as teachers come up with the time for students to re-do labs, especially after we give then the time to present and argue the lab reports in front of their peers. The paper certainly makes the point that when students must defend and support their claims against a peer’s critique, they are held to a higher standard of reflection. However, until a wider study is done that shows how the argumentative process translates to scores on standardized tests and closing achievement gaps, I do not see many time-constrained teachers adopting this feature of inquiry based learning.