The Understandings of Consequence Project is supported by The National Science Foundation. Click here to visit our Teacher Resource Website. Click here to learn more about the Understandings of Consequence Fall Institute 2006! Research shows that students have difficulty achieving deep understanding of many fundamental science concepts, for instance, the nature of matter, pressure, density, and electrical circuits to name but a few. After students have presumably learned the scientific explanations, they often revert back to their initial explanations. The Understandings of Consequence Project has demonstrated that part of the problem arises from differences in how students and scientists think about cause and effect. Scientific explanations often require students to structure knowledge in ways that contradict their expectations about the nature of how causes and effects behave. Such explanations can involve: causal mechanisms that are inferred or abstract; causal patterns that extend beyond linear and unidirectional to cyclic, reciprocal, and non-sequential; correspondences between causes and effects that are in various respects probabilistic; and causal agents that are decentralized and involve aspects of emergence. These are ways of thinking that students typically are not familiar with. Thus students attempt to assimilate information about complex concepts into simplistic causal structures, which ultimately distort the information. In order to achieve deep understanding of scientific explanations, students need to learn the levels of these dimensions that fit the level of explanation needed. We have developed a taxonomy of causal models to guide these teaching and learning efforts. We have also developed a taxonomy of epistemological "moves", such as comparing more than one model and being alert to possible gaps in one's explanation, that serves scientific inquiry and lead to more complex conceptions. Through a series of intervention studies, the project demonstrated that impacting students' assumptions about the nature of causality is effective in helping students restructure their knowledge and achieve scientific understandings. We engaged students in activities designed to reveal the causal structure of a concept and in explicit discussion about the nature of causality involved. For instance, in exploring the role of density in sinking and floating, students are shown a small piece of candle that sinks when placed in a liquid and are asked to predict what will happen when a big piece of candle is placed in another container of liquid. When it floats, students start to explore what is going on. The outcome and ensuing discussion pushes them beyond a linear, feature-based causality of "the weight makes it sink" to an interactive causality, where they begin to focus on the liquid and the object and recognize the causal pattern is a relationship between greater and lesser density of objects and liquid. The papers below elaborate our findings for each topic of study. Our recent papers discuss our findings on transfer. In general, we found good evidence that students can transfer their understanding of structurally similar causal forms between science concepts with and without support and that they can transfer their understanding about the nature of causality to structurally nonsimilar causal forms with support. The Understandings of Consequence Curriculum Units on Density, Ecosystems, Electrical Circuits, and Air Pressure are available at our Teacher Resource Website. Some of these units are featured on the Essential Science Series airing on the Annenberg CPB channel (http://www.learner.org). With NSF support (#ESI-0455664), we are currently collaborating with the Harvard Smithsonian Science Media Group to develop an interactive website and professional development materials for teachers. Teachers interested in hearing more about it and possibly becoming involved in the design and test phases of the project should contact us. Phase 4 Project Team: Principal Investigator: Tina Grotzer Advisor: David Perkins Research Assistants: Rebecca Lincoln Dorothy MacGillivray* Sarah Mittlefehldt Kiki Donis Rebekah Gould Belinda Basca Carolyn Houghton Collaborating Teachers: Rich Carroll Lucy Morris Val Tobias MaryAnn Bernstein Dave Thibault Michael Edgar Lynda Verity Gini MacAuley Kim Piccolo Jim Stanger Elaine Sheehan Eric Buchovecky Eileen Kenneally Margot Sudbury Rose Honey Sun Kim Daniel Record Kristen Record Susie Shaw Maritess Panlilio (Harvard) Jennifer James (Harvard) Melanie Pincus (MIT) Katya Levitan-Reiner (Smith) Alice Yang (Harvard College) Graduate Research Assistants: Becky DeVito Gina Ritscher Kelly Leahy Laura Jung Sun Kim Daniel Record Kristen Record Susie Shaw Rose Honey Scientific Advisors: Sue Mattson (Harvard Smithsonian Center for Astrophysics) Yossi Snir (University of Haifa, Israel) Carlos Vasco (Nacional University, Bogota, Colombia) Roger Sudbury (MIT Lincoln Labs) Science Media Group Collaborators: Matt Schneps Nancy Finkelstein Alex Griswold Clive Grainger Education Development Center (EDC) Outside Evaluator: Daphne Miner Programmers: Lee Campbell Ben Fu, MIT Media Lab Undergraduate Research Assistants: Mickey Muldoon Maritess Panlilio Jennifer James Melanie Pincus (MIT) Katya Levitan-Reiner (Smith College) Alice Yang * italics denote people who were involved in an earlier phase of the project Selected Publications and Presentations Documents are in PDF Format. To download Adobe Acrobat Reader, click here: Grotzer, T.A., & Lincoln, R. (in press). Educating for "intelligent environmental action" in an age of global warming, in S. Moser & L. Dilling (Eds.) Communicating Urgency--Facilitating Social Change: Strategies for Dealing with the Climate Crisis. National Center for Atmospheric Research (NCAR). Perkins, D.N., & Grotzer, T.A. (2005). Dimensions of causal understanding: The role of complex causal models in students' understanding of science. Studies in Science Education, 41, 117-166. Grotzer, T.A., Houghton, C.A., Basca, B., Mittlefehldt, S., Lincoln, R., & MacGillivray, D. (2005). Causal patterns in density: Lessons to infuse into air pressure units. Cambridge, MA: Project Zero, HGSE. DeVito, B., & Grotzer, T.A. (2005, April). Characterizing Discourse in Two Science Classrooms by the Cognitive Processes Demonstrated by Students and Teachers. Paper presented at the National Association of Research in Science Teaching (NARST) Conference, Dallas, TX. Grotzer, T.A. (2005, April). Transferring Structural Knowledge about the Nature of Causality to Isomorphic and Non-Isomorphic Topics. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association (AERA) Conference, Montreal, Quebec. Grotzer, T.A. (2004, October). Putting everyday science within reach: Addressing patterns of thinking that limit science learning. Principal Leadership,16-21. Grotzer, T.A., & Sudbury, M. (2004). Causal patterns in simple circuits. President and Fellows of Harvard College for Project Zero, Harvard Graduate School of Education, Cambridge, MA. Basca, B.B., & Grotzer, T.A. (2003). Causal patterns in air pressure-related phenomena. President and Fellows of Harvard College for Project Zero, Harvard Graduate School of Education, Cambridge, MA. Grotzer, T.A. (2003). Learning to understand the forms of causality implicit in scientific explanations. Studies in Science Education. 39, 1-74. Grotzer, T.A., & Basca, B.B. (2003). Helping students to grasp the underlying causal structures when learning about ecosystems: How does it impact understanding? Journal of Biological Education, 38,(1)16-29. Grotzer, T.A. (2003, March). Transferring structural knowledge about the nature of causality: An empirical test of three levels of transfer. Paper presented at the National Association of Research in Science Teaching (NARST) Conference, Philadelphia, PA. Mittlefehldt, S., & Grotzer, T.A. (2003, March). Using metacognition to facilitate the transfer of causal models in learning density and pressure. Paper presented at the National Association of Research in Science Teaching (NARST) Conference, Philadelphia, PA. Ritscher, R., Lincoln, R., & Grotzer, T.A. (2003, March). Understanding density and pressure: How students' meaning-making impacts their transfer of causal models. Paper presented at the National Association of Research in Science Teaching (NARST) Conference, Philadelphia, PA. Grotzer, T.A. (2002). Causal patterns in ecosystems. Cambridge, MA: Project Zero, Harvard Graduate School of Education. Grotzer, T.A. (2002). Expanding our vision for educational technology: Procedural, conceptual, and structural knowledge. Educational Technology, 42(2) 52-59. Basca, B.B., & Grotzer, T.A. (2001, April). Focusing on the nature of causality in a unit on pressure: How does it affect students understanding? Paper presented at the annual conference of the American Educational Research Association, Seattle, WA. Bell, B., Carroll, R., & Grotzer, T.A. (2000, April). How causal models can help or hinder an understanding of force and motion concepts. Paper presented at the National Science Teachers Association (NSTA) Conference, Orlando. Bell-Basca, B., Grotzer, T.A., Donis, K., & Shaw, S. (2000, April). Using domino and relational causality to analyze ecosystems: Realizing what goes around comes around. Paper presented at the National Association of Research in science Teaching, New Orleans, LA. Donis, K., & Grotzer, T.A. (2000, April). Teaching about domino and cyclic causality to help students understand ecosystems. Paper presented at the National Science Teachers Association (NSTA) Conference, Orlando. Edgar, M., & Grotzer, T.A. (2000, April). Causal dimensions that create difficulty in understanding evolution. Paper presented at the National Association for Research in Science Teaching (NARST) Conference, New Orleans, LA. Grotzer, T.A. (2000, April). How conceptual leaps in understanding the nature of causality can limit learning: An example from electrical circuits. Paper presented at the annual conference of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans, LA. Grotzer, T.A., & Perkins, D.N. (2000, April). A taxonomy of causal models: The conceptual leaps between models and students’ reflections on them. Paper presented at the annual conference of the National Association for Research in Science Teaching, New Orleans, LA. Grotzer, T.A., & Sudbury, M. (2000, April). Moving beyond underlying linear causal models of electrical circuits. Paper presented at the annual conference of the National Association for Research in Science Teaching, New Orleans, LA. Houghton, C., Record, K., Bell, B., & Grotzer, T.A. (2000, April). Conceptualizing density with a relational systemic model. Paper presented at the National Association for Research in Science Teaching (NARST) Conference, New Orleans, LA. Perkins, D.N., & Grotzer, T.A. (2000, April). Models and moves: Focusing on dimensions of causal complexity to achieve deeper scientific understanding. Paper presented at the annual conference of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans, LA. Sudbury, M., Grotzer, T.A., & Bell, B. (2000, April). Helping students learn about electricity by examining their causal stories. Paper presented at the National Science Teachers Association (NSTA) Conference, Orlando. Grotzer, T.A., & Bell, B.B. (1999). Negotiating the funnel: Guiding students toward understanding elusive generative concepts. In L. Hetland & S. Veenema (Eds.), The Project Zero Classroom: Views on Understanding. Cambridge, MA: Project Zero, Harvard Graduate School of Education. Click here to link to papers from Complex Causality and Conceptual Change, American Educational Research Association (AERA) Symposium, April 2001. This project is supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant No. REC-9725502 to Tina Grotzer and David Perkins. Any opinions, findings, conclusions or recommendations expressed here are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect those of the National Science Foundation. Brandi Turnbow 9/20/06 Understandings of Consequence Article Review Brief Summary The article’s basic premise is that students have difficulty achieving deep understanding of many fundamental science concepts, and that after students have learned the scientific explanations, they revert back to their initial explanations. The article states that the Understandings of Consequence demonstrates that the way students think about cause and effect is the reason for the reversion. Students learn through scaffolding and framework structure that forces them to fit what they learn into a framework that already exists, there by “distorting” the desired outcomes to fit their prior knowledge. The researchers have created the taxonomy of causal models to help guide teaching and learning, and ask both teachers and learners to compare multiple models and look at gaps in explanations. The researchers found that they could help restructure students understanding of causality and further their understanding. They also found that students could apply their understanding about causality to other non-similar situations. Analysis The article seeks to solve what I see as a big problem in science education. Restructuring the way students take in, process, and interpret what they are exposed to, makes a big difference in the information they walk away with. It also affects the way they apply what they have learned to new concepts. The focus on cause and effect is such a big deal because that is the essence of science, and it is hard to teach, and as a student difficult to understand. The table of taxonomy of causal models breaks down the four types of causal models and seeks to give the instructor tools to break down and better explain how each model is different, and therefore allows students to see detailed differences and thereby restructuring their processing of causal problems. The taxonomy of epistemological "moves" breaks down and allows teachers to see defined relationships in the progression of a scientific model, how it performs, and the analyzed outcomes. This can better help teachers to explain how to interpret how a model performs, and what different performances by different models means. Article’s strengths and weaknesses In my opinion the strengths of this article is the amount of research put into this project. The research team is very large and members of the team are very highly respected researchers who have been working in the field of educational research for nearly thirty years. The collaborating institutions such as Harvard, and the Smithsonian are highly regarded as top of the field. Not only does the article develop a highly detailed, easy to understand taxonomy, based on classroom research, but it also sites many earlier research projects to back up the validity of the taxonomies. The only weakness I see in the article is that I would have liked more real life examples. The floating candle example is good, but as a reader I would like more examples to help strengthen my own understanding of how you can apply the taxonomies in the classroom. Implications The implications of this research could drastically improve science education by helping teachers better breakdown, and explain one of the basic driving forces of scientific discovery, cause and effect. Reflection Seeing that I started this homework at ten thirty at night, after working and schooling a fourteen-hour day. I’m not sure I was really able to full grasp and internalize the true beauty and essence of the article. Though, now that I have discovered this research, I will definitely delve deeper when my brain is working and apply it to how I plan my structure and use student’s prior knowledge. I have attended Harvard’s PZ institutes and found them to be the driving force behind many my methods of successful teaching. So yes I really trust what they have to say. Thus putting this research into practice will be well worth the time and effort.