et al.Catherine E. Snow, for Learning About Science Academic Language and the Challenge of ReadingDOI: 10.1126/science.1182597 , 450 (2010); 328Science This copy is for your personal, non-commercial use only. . clicking herecolleagues, clients, or customers by , you can order high-quality copies for yourIf you wish to distribute this article to others . herefollowing the guidelines can be obtained byPermission to republish or repurpose articles or portions of articles including high-resolution figures, can be found in the onlineUpdated information and services, http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/328/5977/450 version of this article at: found at: can berelated to this articleA list of selected additional articles on the Science Web sites http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/328/5977/450#related-conten t http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/328/5977/450 #otherarticles , 1 of which can be accessed for free: cites 9 articlesThis article http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/328/5977/450# otherarticles 1 articles hosted by HighWire Press; see: cited byThis article has been on May 13, 2010 www.sciencemag.orgDownloaded from (this information is current as of May 13, 2010 ): The following resources related to this article are available online at www.sciencemag.org : subject collectionsThis article appears in the following http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/collection/educ ation Education CopyrightAmerican Association for the Advancement of Science, 1200 New York Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005. (print ISSN 0036-8075; online ISSN 1095-9203) is published weekly, except the last week in December, by theScienceregistered trademark of AAAS. is aScience2010 by the American Association for the Advancement of Science; all rights reserved. The title S c i e n c e , L a n g u a g e , a n d L it e r a c y their l earners ’ st r u g g l e s t o c o m e t o t e r m s w i t h unfamiliar l anguage, discourse patterns, and the often f ormidable conventions of science ( 27 ). References1. P. Fensham, Science Education Policy-Mak ing: Eleven Emerging Issues [Com missioned by United Nations Educational, Scienti fic, and Cultura l Organization (UNESCO) Section for Science, Technical, and Vocational Education; Paris; 2008] .2. S . P . N o r r i s , L . M . P h i l l i p s , Sci. Ed uc. 87 ,224 (2003).3. L. D. Yore, G. L. Bisanz, B. M. Hand, In t. J. Sci . Edu c. 25 , 689 (2003).4. B. M. Hand, V. Prain , L. D. Yore, in Writi ng as a Learning Tool: Integrating Theory and Practice , P. Tynjälä, L. Mason, K. Lonka, Eds. (Kluwer, Dordrec ht, Netherla nds, 2001), pp. 105 –129.5. L . D . Y o r e , D . F . T r e a g u s t , Int. J. S ci. E du c. 28 ,291 (2006).6. H. Alidou et al ., Optimizin g Learning and E du cation in Africa—the L anguag e Factor: A Stock Tak in g Re search on Mother Tongue and Bilingual Education in Sub-Saharan Africa (U N ESCO In s ti tu t e f o r E d u c a ti o n , Lib r e vi ll e, G a b on , 2 00 6) .7. N. R. Romanc e, M. R. Vitale, J. Res. Sci. Teach. 29 , 545 (19 92).8. N. R. Romanc e, M. R. Vitale, in Linking Sci ence and Literacy in the K – 8 Classroom , R. Douglas, PERSPECTIVE M. P. Klentschy, K. Worth, W. Binder , Eds. [National Scienc e Teacher s Association (NSTA) Press, Arlington, VA, 2006] , pp. 391 – 405.9. N. R. Romance, M. R. Vit ale, NSF/IERI Science IDEAS Project (Techn ical Report 0228353- 0014, Florida Atlanti c Univers ity, Boca Raton, FL, 2008).10. G. Cervetti, P. Pearson, M. Bravo , J. Barber, in Linking Sci ence and Literacy in the K– 8 Classroom , R. Douglas, M. Klentschy, K. Worth, Eds. (NSTA Press, Arlington, VA, 2006) , pp. 221 – 224.11. O. M. Amaral, L. Garrison, M. Klent schy, Biling. Res. J. 26 , 213 (2002). 12. M. Revak, P. Kuerbis, pap er presented at the Inter national Meeti ng of the Association for Science Te acher E du cation, S t. Lou i s, MO, 9 to 12 J anuary 200 8.13. B. Hand, Ed., Science Inquiry, Argument and Language: A Case for the Science Writing Heuristic (Sense Publis hers, Rotte rdam, Neth erlands, 2007).14. M. Gunel, B. Hand, V. Prain, Int. J. Sci. Math. Educ. 5 , 615 (2007). 15. M. A. McDermo tt, B. Hand, paper presente d at the Inter national Meeti ng of the Association for Science Teacher Education , St. Louis, MO, 9 to 12 January 2008.16. M. Setati, J. Adler, Y. Reed, A. Bapoo , Lang. Edu c. 16 , 128 (2002).17. Depar tment of Education , National Education Quality Initiative and Edu cation, Science and Skills Developm ent Academic Language and the Challenge of Reading for Learning About Science Catherine E. Snow L Grade 6 Systemic Evaluation (Department of Edu cation, Pretoria , South Africa, 2005) .18. V. Rodse th, Bilingualism and Multi lingualism in Education (Centre for Continuing Development, Johannes burg, 1995).19. C. Ballenger, Lang. Edu c. 11 , 1 (1997 ). 20. P . W e b b , D . T r e a g u s t , Res. Sci. Educ . 36 ,381 (200 6). 21. M. G. Villanueva, P. Webb, Afr. J. Res. Math. Sci. Technol.Educ. 12 , 5 (2008). 22. P. Webb, Y. Willia ms, L. Meiring, Afr. J. Res. Mat h. Sci.Technol. Edu c. 12 , 4 (2008). 23. P. Webb, Int. J. Enviro n. Sci. Educ. 4 , 313 (20 09). 24. C. R. Nesbit, T. Y. Hargrove, L. Harrelson, B. Maxey,Implementing Science Notebooks in the Primary Grades: Sc ience A ctivities J ournal (Heldref, Washington, DC, 2003).25. D. August, T. Shanah an, Eds., Developi ng Literac y in Second-Lan guage Learners: Report of the National Literacy Panel on Language-Mi nority Children and You th (Lawrence Erlb aum, Mahwah, NJ, 2006) .26. J. Cummins , Appl. Linguist. 2 , 132 (1981). 27. D. Hodson , Teaching and Learni ng About Science:Language, Theories, Methods, History, Traditions and Values (Sense Publishers, Rotte rdam, Netherland s, 2009). 10.1126/science.1 182596 is no single variety of educated American English. Academic language features vary as a function of discipline, topic, and mode (written versus oral, for example), but there are certain common characteristics that distinguish highly academic from less academic or more conversational language and that make academic language— even well-wri tten, careful ly c onstructed, and professionally edited academic language—difficult to comprehend and even harder to produce ( 8 ). arts (mostly narratives) suggests that the comprehension of “academic language” may be one source of the challenge. So what is academic language? A iteracy scholars and s econdary teachers alike are puzzled by the frequency w ith which s tudent s who read words accuratelyand fluently have trouble comprehending text (1, 2). Such students have m astered what was traditionally considered the m ajor obstacle t o reading success: the depth and complexity of the English spelling system. But many middleand high-school students are less able to convert their word-reading skills into comprehension w hen confronted with texts in s cience (or m ath or social studies) than they are when confront ed with texts of fiction or discursi ve essays . The greater diff iculty of science, math, and social studies texts than of texts encountered in English language Harvard Graduate School of Educatio n, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA. c a d e m i c l a n g u a g e i s o n e o f t h e t e r o f a c a d e m i c l a n g u a g e a r e u s i n g a h i g h d e n s i t y o f a v o i d i n g e n s u r i n g r e d u n d a n c y ; p r e c i s i o n o f gDownloaded from i n c f o o n r c 2010 VOL 328 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org m 450 23 APRIL i a s t e i n o e n s s b , e a a r c i h n i g e v w e o d r d b s y , SPECIAL SECTION From http://www.lowrider.com/forums/10-Under-the-Hood/topics/183-HP-vs-torque/posts (spelling as in the original posting) Often times guys get caught up in the hype of having a big HP motor in their lolo. I frequently get asked whats the best way to get big numbers out of their small block. The answer is not HP, but torque. "You sell HP, you feel torque" as the old saying goes. Most of us are running 155/80/13 tires on our lolo's. Even if you had big HP numbers, you will *never* get that power to the ground, at least off the line. I have a 64 Impala SS 409, that i built the motor in. While it is a completely restored original (I drive it rolling on 14" 72 spoke cross laced Zeniths), the motor internals are not. It now displaces 420 CI, with forged pistons and blalanced rotating assembly. The intake, carb and exhaust had to remain OEM for originality's sake, and that greatly reduces the motors potential. Anyway, even with the original 2 speed powerglide, it spins those tires with alarming ease, up to 50 miles per hour! In my 62, I built a nice 383 out of an 86 Corvette. I built it for good bottom end pull, since it is a lowrider with 8 batteries. And since it rides on the obligitory 13's, torque is what that car needs. It pulls like an ox right from idle, all the way up to its modest 5500 redline. But I never take it that high, as all the best power is from 1100 to 2700 RPM. So when considering an engine upgrade, look for modifications that improve torque. That is what your lolo needs! Posted by Jason Dave, Sept 2009 Jason you are right on bro. I have always found an increase in torque placement has not only provided better top end performance but also improved gas mileage in this expensive gas times. Posted by Gabriel Salazar, Nov 2009 From http://www.tutorvista.com/content/physics/physics-iii/rigid-body/torque.php Fig. 1. Examples of nonacademic text (Lowrider, top) and academic text (TutorVista, bottom). derives from m embership in a community th a n s p e c if i c c la i m s . Ma i n t a in i n g th committed t o a shared epistemology; this e i mp e r s o n a l authoritative s tance creates (“Jason you are r ight on bro ”). stance is expressed t hrough a r eduction a distanced tone that is often puzzling to Though both t he Lowrider authors are in the use of personal pronouns, a adolescent r eaders and is extremely difficult for writing t o i nform, they are not preference for epistemically warranted adolescents to emulate in wr i ti n g . assuming the i mpersonal aut evaluations (such as “ri g o r o u s s t u d yPerhaps the simplest basis f or comparing horitative voice that is characteristic of ” and “qu e s t i o n a b l e a n a l y s i s the Lo wr i d e r a n d Tu t o r Vi st a t e x t s academic language. They claim t heir ”)overpersonallyexpressi ve evaluations (s i s t o c o n si d e r h o w rare in other authority t o provide information about uch as “great st udy ” and “funky analys contexts are the words t hey use most the advant age of torque over is”), and a focus on general rather horsepower adjust ments on t he basi s of personal experience. The scientist’sauthoritativestance,ontheoth erhand, Lever arm Torque is the product of the magnitude of the force and the lever arm of the force. What is the significance of this concept in our everyday life? Dependence of torque on lever arm To increase the turning effect of force, it is not necessary to increase the magnitude of the force itself. We may increase the turning effect of the force by changing the point of application of force and by changing the direction of force. Hinge Axis of rotation Let us take the case of a heavy door. If a force is applied at a point, which is close to the hinges of the door, we may find it quite difficult to open or close the door. However, if the same force is applied at a point, which is at the maximum distance from hinges, we can easily close or open the door. The task is made easier if the force is applied at right angles to the plane of the door. When we apply the force the door turns on its hinges. Thus a turning effect is produced when we try to open the door. Have you ever tried to do so by applying the force near the hinge? In the first case, we are able to open the door with ease. In the second case, we have to apply much more force to cause the same turning effect. What is the reason? The turning effect produced by a force on a rigid body about a point, pivot or fulcrum is called the moment of a force or torque. It is measured by the product of the force and the perpendicular distance of the pivot from the line of action of the force. force = Force x Perpendicular distance of the pivot from the force. The unit of moment of force is newton metre (N m). In the above example, in the first case the perpendicular distance of the line of action of the force from the hinge is much more than that in the second case. Hence, in the second case to open the door, we have to apply greater force. M o m e n t o f a on May 13, 2010 www.sciencemag.orgDownloaded from www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 328 23 APRIL 2010 451 Science, Language, and Literacy frequently. The rarest words used i n t he Science t eachers are not generally well Lowrider text are t he special term “lol o ” prepared t o help t heir students penetrate and its alternative form “low r ider, ”“up g r a the linguistic puzzles that science texts d e , ”“carb, ”“HP, ”“exhaust,”“spin, ” and present. They of course recognize that “torque. ” Only two words from the Academic teaching vocabulary i s key, but typically Word List (10 ), a list of words used focus on t he science vocabulary ( the frequently across academic texts of diff bolded w ords in the t ext), often without erent disciplines, appear in this passage. recognizing that those bolded words are The Tu t o r Vi s t a t e x t r a r e w o r d s i n defined with general-purpose academic words t hat s tudent s also do not know. c l u d e “magnitude,” “perpendicular, ”“lever,”“pivot, ”“hinge,”“fulcrum, ” and Consider the TutorVi st a definition of “torque,” and it uses t he academic words torque: “To r q u e i s t h e p r o d u c t o f t h e magnitude of the force and the lever arm “tas k , ”“maxi mum ,”“significance,” and of the force. ” Many 7th graders are “illust rati on. ” The difference i n word s election reflects t he convention i n t he unfamiliar with the t erms “magnitude ” and more academic text of presenting precise “lever ”;andsome proportion will think t hey understand “product,” information in a dense, concise manner. Nominalizations are a grammatical pr ocess of converting entire s entences (such as “Gutenberg invented the printing press ”)intophrasesthat can then be embedded in other s entences (such as “Gutenberg ’s invention of the printing press revolutionized the dissemination of information”). Nominalizations are crucial to the conciseness expected in academic language. I n t he Tu t o r Vi s t a s e n t e n c e “We m a y i n c r e a s e t h e t u r n ing eff ect of the f orce by changing the point of application of f orce and by changing the direction of force,”“application” and “direction” are nominalizations representing entire pr opositions. “Application ” is shorthand for “where w e apply,” and “direction ” is shorthand for “how we direct.” Thus , although this sentence has t he same apparent s tructure as “We c a n g e t a s m i l e f r o m a b a b y by changing his diaper and by patting his back,” the processing load is much higher. “Increase” in the original s entence i s a verb referring to a r elation between two quantities, whereas “get ” in the baby-sentence adaptation refers to an action or effect in the r eal world. “Diaper ” and “back ” are physical entities subjected to actions, whereas “application ” and “direction” are themselves actions that have been turned into nouns. Part of the complexity of academic language derives f rom the fact that we use t he syntactic structures acquired f or talking about agents and actions to talk about entities and relations, without recognizing t he challenge that t hat transition poses to the reader. I n particular, in science classes we m ay expect students t o process t hese sentences without explicit instruction i n t heir structure. “force,” and “arm ” without r ealizing t hat t hose terms are being used i n technical, academic ways here, w ith m eanings quite di ff erent from t hose of daily l ife. Yet this definition, with its s ophisticated and unfamiliar w ord m eanings, i s t he basis f or all the rest of the TutorVista exposition: the trade-off between magnitude and direction of force. Efforts to he lp s tudent s understand science cannot ignore their need to understand the words used to write and talk about science: the allpu rpo s e a ca dem i c w or ds a s wel l as t he d isci pline specifi c ones. Of course some students acquire academic vocabulary on their own, if they read widely and if their comprehension skills are strong enough to support inferences about the meaning of unknown words (11 ). The fact that many adolescents prefer reading Web sites to books (12 ), however, somewhat decreases access to good models of academic language even for those interested in technical topi cs. Thus, t hey have f ew opportunities to learn the academic vocabulary that is c rucial across their content-area learning. It is also possible t o explicitly teach academic vocabulary t o m iddleschool s tudents. Word Generation i s a middleschool program developed by the Strategic Education Research Partnership t hat embeds allpurpose academic words i n i nteresting topics and provides activities for use in math, s cience, and social studies as well as English l anguage arts classes i n w hich the target w ords are used ( see t he We b s i t e f o r e x a m p l e s ) ( 13 ). Among the academic w ords taught in Word Generation are t hose used to make, assess, and defend claims, s uch as “data,”“hypothesis,”“affi r m ,”“convince,”“disprove,” and “interpret. ” We d e s i g n e d Wo r d G e n eration to f ocus on dilemmas, because these promote discussion and debate and provide motivating contexts for s tudents and teachers t o use t he target words. For example, one week is devoted to the t opic of whether junk food should be banned from sc hools, and another t o whether physician-assisted suicide should be l egal. Discussion i s i n itse lf a key contributor to science learning ( 14) and to reading comprehension (15 , 16 ). Words l earned through explicit teaching are unlike ly to be retained i f t hey are taught in lists r ather t han embedded i n m eaningful texts and i f opportunities to use t hem i n discussion, debate , and writing are not provided. It is unrealistic to expect all m iddle- or precision of word choice, and use of highschool s tudents t o become proficient nominalizations to refer t o complex processes producers of academic language. M any —reflect the need to present complicated graduate students still struggle t o m anage t he ideas in effi cient ways. Students m ust be abl authoritative s tance, and t he self-presentation e to r ead texts that use these features if they as an expert t hat justifies it, i n their writing. And are t o become independent learners of it is i mportant to note t hat not all f eatures science or s ocial s tudies. T hey m ust have associated with the academic writing style ( suchaccess to t he all-purpose academic a s the use of passive voice, impenetrability of vocabulary that i s used t o talk about prose constructions, and indifference t o literary knowledge and that they will need to use i n m niceties) are desirable. But the ce ntral fe atures aking t heir own arguments and evaluating of a cade m ic lan guage —grammatical others’ arguments. Mechanisms for t eaching t embeddings , s ophisticated and abstract hose words and the ways that scientists use t vocabul ary, hem s hould be a part of the s cience curriculum. Collaborations between desi gners of science curricula and l iteracy scholars are needed to develop and evaluate methods fo r h el p i n g s t ud e n t s m a s t e r t h e l a n gu a g e of s ci e nc e at the u nd ergraduate and h igh-school levels a s well as at the mi ddle-school level that Word Generation is currently serving. Advancing Adolescent Literacy, Time to Act: An Agenda for Advan cing Adolescent Literacy for College and Career Success (Carnegie Corporation of New York, New York, 2010); http:/ /carnegie.org/ fileadmin/ Media/Pu blications/PDF/t ta_Main.pdf. 2. J. Johnson, L. Martin-Hans en, Sci . Scope 28 , 12 (2005). 3. M. A. K. Halliday , paper presente d at the Ann ual Internation al Language in Education Conferen ce, Hong Kong, December 1993. 4. M. J. Schleppegrell, Linguist. Educ. 12 , 431 (2001). 5. M. A. K. Halliday , J. R. Martin, Writing Science : Literac y and Discursive Power (Univ. of Pittsburgh Press, Pittsburgh, PA, 1993). 6. A. Bailey, The Language Demands of School: Putting Academic English to the Test (Yale Univ. Press, New Haven, CT, 2007). 7. R. Scarcella, Academic English : A Conceptual Framework (Technical Report 2003-1, Univ. of California Linguistic Minority Research Institu te, Irv ine, CA, 2003) . 8. C. Snow, P. Uccelli, in The Cambridge Handbook of Literacy , D. Olson , N. Torrance , Eds. (Cambridge Univ. Press, New York, 2008), pp. 112 – 133. 9. Z. Fang, Int. J. Sci . Educ. 28 , 491 (2006).1 0 . A . C o x h e a d , T E S O L Q . 3 4 , 213 (2000 ). 11. J . L a w r e n c e , Read. P sy chol. 30 ,445 ( 2009). 12. E. B. Moje, M. Ov erb y, N . T ysvaer, K . Mo rri s, H arv. E d u c . Rev. 78 , 107 (2008). 13. C. Snow, J. Lawre nce, C. Wh ite, J. Res. Educ. Effectiven ess2, 325 (2009 ); www.wordgener ation.org. 14. J. Osborne, Science 328 , 463 (20 10). 15. J. Lawrence, C. Snow, in Handbook of Reading ResearchIV, P. D. Pearson, M. Kamil. , E. Moje, P. Affle rbach, Eds. (Routledge Education, London, 2010). 16. P. K. Murph y, I. A. G. Wilkinson, A. O. Soter, M. N. Hennessey, J. F. Alexander, J. Edu c. Psychol. 62, 387 (2009 ). 17. P r e p ar a t io n o f t h is p ap e r w as m ad e p os si b l e b y co l la b o r at i on s s u p p o r t e d b y t h e St r at e g i c Ed u ca t io n R e s e ar ch P ar t ne r s hi p an d re s e a rc h f u n d e d b y t he Sp e nc e r F o u n d at i o n, t he W i ll i am an d F l or a H e w le tt F o u nd at i on , t h e Ca rn e g i e Co r p o ra t io n o f N e w Y o rk , an d t he I ns t i t u t e f o r E d u c at i on Sc i e n ce s t hr o u g h t h e Co u n ci l o f G r e at Ci t y S ch oo l s. T ha nk s al s o t o w ww . Tu t or V i st a . co m f o r p e rm i ss i on to re p r in t t he ir le s s on o n t o r q u e . 10.1126/science.1 182597 23 APRIL 2010 VOL 328 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org452 mag.orgDownloaded from References and Notes1. Carnegie Council on