1 WHEN JUSTICE PROMOTES INJUSTICE: WHY MINORITY LEADERS WHO ACT JUSTLY EXPERIENCE MORE BIAS THAN THOSE WHO ACT UNJUSTLY ABSTRACT Accumulated knowledge on organizational justice leaves little reason to doubt the notion that organizational members benefit when supervisors adhere to interpersonal justice rules. However, upon considering how justice behaviors influence subordinates’ cognitive processes, we predict that interpersonal justice has a surprising, unintended negative consequence. Supervisors who violate interpersonal justice rules trigger subordinates to search for reasons why their supervisors are threatening them, causing subordinates to be more attuned to supervisors’ individual characteristics and therefore unlikely to use stereotypes when evaluating them. In contrast, supervisors who adhere to interpersonal justice rules allow subordinates to divert attention away from them, leading subordinates’ judgments of their supervisors to be influenced by stereotypes. Consistent with these predictions, in a survey we found that minority supervisors faced bias relative to Caucasian supervisors when supervisors adhered to—but not when they violated— interpersonal justice rules. We replicated this effect in an experiment and established that it is explained by an alternating pattern of stereotype activation and inhibition: participants viewed minority supervisors to be more deceitful than Caucasians when supervisors adhered to—but not when they violated—interpersonal justice rules. We then conducted exploratory analyses and identified one factor (unit size) that mitigates this troubling pattern. 2 Accumulated knowledge on organizational justice points clearly to the conclusion that organizational stakeholders fare better when supervisors exhibit interpersonal justice. When supervisors adhere to interpersonal justice rules (i.e., treating subordinates respectfully when making resource allocation decisions), subordinates experience increased organizational commitment and greater job satisfaction (Cohen-Charash & Spector, 2001; Colquitt et al., 2013). Likewise, supervisors benefit because just interpersonal behavior engenders higher quality social exchange relationships, which ultimately elicit stronger employee task performance and more helping behavior (for a review, see Colquitt et al., 2013). The practice of adhering to interpersonal justice rules also prevents various forms of injury. In particular, it allows subordinates to experience reduced levels of threat and uncertainty (Van den Bos, 2001), leaving them unencumbered as they engage in their work. Above all, interpersonal justice rules represent behaviors that are viewed almost universally as ethical and virtuous (Folger & Cropanzano, 1998). In short, an adherence to interpersonal justice rules not only positively influences employee wellbeing, supervisor outcomes, and the exchange relationship between supervisor and subordinate (Colquitt, LePine, Piccolo, Zapata, & Rich, 2012; Masterson, Lewis, Goldman, & Taylor, 2000), but also reflects the presence of noble-minded authority and the absence of exploitation and threat. As a consequence of these reinforcing benefits, there exists little reason to question the notion that organizations and their members fare better when supervisors adhere to, rather than violate, interpersonal justice rules. However, an unintended, negative consequence of interpersonal justice rule adherence emerges when considering how one of the key benefits of justice—protection from threat—is likely to influence the attention of subordinates. When subordinates do not construe their supervisors as sources of illicit harm, such as when supervisors adhere to interpersonal justice rules, subordinates are likely to shift their attention away from their supervisors and toward other foci (such as their co-workers). When perceivers do not pay close attention to a target individual’s 3 distinguishing characteristics, stereotypes can influence judgment if the target individual belongs to a stigmatized minority group (Bargh, 2013; Fiske, 1998). Thus, the consequence of interpersonally just treatment—a lack of attention to supervisors—may lead subordinates’ fairness judgments to be influenced by stereotypes if their supervisors are stigmatized minority group members (e.g., if they are Black or Hispanic). The upshot is that when supervisors adhere to interpersonal justice rules, increased stereotyping will cause supervisors from stigmatized ethnic groups to be perceived, on average, as less fair than Caucasian supervisors. In contrast, violations of interpersonal justice rules cause subordinates to pay careful attention to their supervisors because subordinates view them as sources of threat (Skitka, 2003). This decreases the likelihood of stereotype usage, causing minority supervisors and Caucasian supervisors to be evaluated in the same, albeit negative, way. In short, we expect a troubling pattern: bias against minority supervisors occurs when they treat subordinates respectfully rather than disrespectfully. With a research design that iterates back and forth between field and lab data—a template that reflects “full-cycle research” (Chatman & Flynn, 2005)—we test the prediction that subordinates will exhibit bias by (1) helping minority supervisors less than Caucasian supervisors by engaging in less extra-role behavior and (2) hurting minority supervisors more than Caucasian supervisors by engaging in more undermining behavior. Consistent with the arguments above, we predict that this unequal treatment will occur when supervisors adhere to—but not when they violate—rules related to the form of justice most directly tied to supervisor actions (interpersonal justice). We also test the cognitive mechanism that drives this effect (the alteration between stereotype activation and inhibition): when supervisors violate interpersonal justice rules, the stereotype that minorities are more deceitful than Caucasians (Burton, Greenberger, & Hayward, 2005; Ogbu, 1985) will be inhibited, and when supervisors adhere to interpersonal justice rules, this stereotype will be activated to explain why minority supervisors are, on average, perceived as less fair than Caucasian supervisors. To cap our effort, we conduct exploratory analyses and 4 identify a situation (when supervisors oversee a small number of subordinates) that mitigates stereotyping by increasing the likelihood that subordinates will be aware of their supervisors’ individual characteristics, even when supervisors adhere to interpersonal justice rules. By using research on attention to integrate theory on stereotyping and justice, we redress several distinct limitations in the organizational justice literature. While recent research has suggested that certain forms of justice (e.g., procedural) can be depleting, interpersonal justice is presumed to have only positive consequences (Johnson, Lanaj, & Barnes, 2014). Our focus on an unintended consequence of interpersonal justice thus suggests that important nuance should be introduced into the literature, complicating the conventional view that interpersonal justice is uniformly beneficial. Above all, although it is known that positive actions occasionally have unintended, negative consequences, our core finding represents an even more paradoxical situation in which an ostensibly good phenomenon may ultimately undermine itself. That is, justice can promote injustice. This core finding has the greatest potential for addressing limitations associated with one of the most widespread views in research on organizational justice, which is that supervisors and subordinates operate based on principles of social exchange (Colquitt, 2012). Subordinates are traditionally assumed to be the most vulnerable to unjust treatment within exchange relationships. When treated poorly, they often have no recourse because they lack power, and therefore must remain in a disadvantaged position relative to their more powerful bosses. In contrast, since supervisors are in a position of power, it stands to reason that they will be the beneficiaries of positive treatment and hard work from their subordinates as long as they treat them well. However, we argue that minority supervisors are in a similarly vulnerable position. As they act more justly, they may experience more bias. In this way, we provide initial evidence for a perspective that challenges an assumption from social exchange theories of organizational justice, which is that supervisors are likely to “get back” what they “put in” to their relationships with subordinates. 5 Additionally, we inform research on the cognitive underpinnings of fairness judgments. In particular, our findings address a limitation in fairness heuristic theory (Lind, 2000; Lind & Van den Bos, 2002). According to this theory, subordinates rely on thin slices of justice-relevant behavior to quickly make global judgments about whether their supervisors are generally fair or unfair (Lind, 2000; Lind & Van den Bos, 2002). We argue that justice rule adherence is likely to trigger this type of heuristic-based processing more than justice violations. Further, we suggest that scholars have not acknowledged an important downside to the rapidly formed judgments that subordinates make in the context of justice rule adherence: they are vulnerable to unintended influences from stereotypical thinking. We also contribute to the literature on stereotyping and racial bias in several ways. Our findings address a limitation in the literature on stereotyping, which is the underappreciated reality that the actions of minority supervisors may be a source of stereotyping. That is, members of stigmatized groups may unknowingly set themselves up to be the target of stereotyping when they treat others with respect. We also spotlight an often overlooked form of racial bias: that which is directed up the organizational hierarchy from subordinates toward supervisors—a form of bias that is unique because subordinates do not have the benefit of authority. Additionally, our incorporation of research on the activation and inhibition of stereotypes into theory on organizational justice sheds new light on why bias against minority leaders remains so stubborn. Research and legal proceedings have demonstrated numerous occurrences of bias against minority supervisors in industries as varied as health care, finance, and sports (Anderson, 1993; Shavers, Klein, & Fagan, 2012; Smith, 1999). A consequence is the presence of a glass ceiling: even though they make up over 30% of the labor force, Black and Hispanic leaders represent less than 3% of all Fortune 500 CEOs (diversity.com; bls.gov). If so many forces—including norms, ethical standards, and an ingrained desire to simply treat others respectfully—lead minority supervisors to exhibit the very behaviors that are most likely to perpetuate bias against them, then bias is highly 6 likely to persist (King, 2004; Pager & Shepherd, 2008). Indeed, since supervisors adhere to interpersonal justice rules far more than they breach them, the mechanisms that we illuminate may help explain why minority supervisors experience a general pattern of bias. We culminate our analysis with a discussion of practical implications. First, our work can be used to inform the trend toward using subordinates’ perceptions of overall supervisor fairness in surveys used in corporate settings. Compared to perceptions of justice rule adherence, overall fairness perceptions better capture the overarching “my supervisor is not fair!” impression (Colquitt, 2012). However, our results suggest that subordinates who have supervisors from one race may use their supervisors’ justice behaviors to inform their overall fairness judgments in a way that is inconsistent with subordinates who have supervisors from another race. Second, our findings can be used to construct recommendations for how to reduce bias against minority supervisors. Whereas acting justly has long been considered a virtue with clear rewards, for minorities it may be a double-edged sword: violating justice rules causes harm for subordinates and for supervisors, yet adhering to justice rules can cause bias to be perpetuated. This “lose-lose” scenario raises questions about vicious cycles in which all courses of action available to minority supervisors may result in harmful outcomes. In the face of these troubling implications, we discuss how findings from our exploratory analyses illuminate possible ways for organizations to break the pattern in which minority supervisors’ just treatment of their subordinates leads to unjust outcomes. BACKGROUND AND HYPOTHESES In Figure 1, we present a theory that maps out the antecedents and consequences of fairness perceptions for minority versus Caucasian supervisors. In our theorizing, we adopt the widely accepted distinction between justice rules, which are circumscribed behaviors associated with accepted norms of supervisor conduct, and overall fairness perceptions, which are general impressions that subordinates have regarding their supervisors’ overall fairness (Colquitt, 2012). 7 We craft our arguments under the assumption that subordinates base perceptions of their supervisors’ overall fairness on observations of their supervisors’ justice-relevant behavior (Colquitt, 2012; Cropanzano, Byrne, Bobocel, & Rupp, 2001). As shown in Figure 1, we position overall fairness perceptions as a key mediator of the relationship between race and bias, and justice rule adherence as a moderator of this mediating relationship. Before building our theoretical model and hypotheses, we clarify several issues related to our terminology and assumptions. ---------------------------------Insert Figure 1 About Here ---------------------------------Terminology and Assumptions The notion of “bias” presupposes that there is a reference point against which minority supervisors experience disadvantage. In the remainder of our article, we refer to bias as actions that disadvantage minority supervisors relative to Caucasian supervisors. Second, we use the term “minority supervisors” to refer only to supervisors from two minority groups—Blacks and Hispanics. Although there are nuanced differences in the precise type of stigma that Blacks and Hispanics face, the evidence we review below shows that members from both of these races experience the most stigma and stereotyping in organizations, and that the specific manifestation of this stigma is similar for both ethnic groups. Third, we use the terms “race” and “ethnicity” interchangeably (www.aaanet.org/gvt/ombdraft.htm). Finally, since 90% of employees have only one supervisor (Brody, Rubin, & Maume, 2014), our theory is relevant to subordinates with a single supervisor. Organizational justice is often conceptualized as adherence to four types of justice rules: distributive (equity, equality, and need-based rules used to determine work outcomes) (Adams, 1965; Homans, 1961); procedural (decision making process rules such as consistency, accuracy, and bias-suppression) (Leventhal, 1980; Thibaut & Walker, 1975); informational (truthfulness and justification-based rules used to explain decision-making processes); and interpersonal (respect 8 and propriety-based rules of interpersonal treatment) (Bies & Moag, 1986). We expect that our proposed effect will be restricted to interpersonal justice rule adherence. When compared to the other three justice dimensions, supervisor discretion is greatest for interpersonal justice (Scott, Colquitt, & Paddock, 2009). Whereas cues that pertain to distributive, procedural, and even informational justice rules often arise from ambiguous, impersonal, or indirect sources, interpersonal justice involves direct treatment received from a supervisor, such as dignity, politeness, and respect. In fact, interpersonal justice is arguably the rule least constrained by formalized policies and practices within the organization, and therefore most dependent on supervisor intentions (Scott et al., 2009). Although interpersonal and informational justice rules are often examined concurrently, unlike interpersonal justice, informational justice rule adherence is not always under the supervisor’s control. For example, a supervisor may be forced to either provide or withhold information from his or her subordinates as a matter of company policy (Gilliland & Schepers, 2003). As a result, subordinates are more likely to construe interpersonal justice rule adherence as being inextricably linked to a specific supervisor when compared to adherence to other forms of justice, including informational justice. This is important because ultimately it is this attribution process that leaves supervisors prone to stereotyping. In addition, since they derive from ongoing patterns of interpersonal treatment, encounters related to interpersonal justice rules can occur on a daily basis, making them particularly salient and strong (Bies, 2005). Given that our arguments center on how subordinates evaluate supervisors’ personal qualities, we isolate interpersonal justice in our theorizing. Unless otherwise specified, our usage of “justice” can be interpreted as referring to interpersonal justice. Further, we focus on entity-based justice in our theorizing (Cropanzano, Byrne, Bobocel, & Rupp, 2001), because we are interested in assessments made about individual supervisors. 9 The Influence of Supervisor Race and Interpersonal Justice on Fairness Perceptions Supervisors vary in the extent to which they exhibit justice rules—ranging from little adherence (i.e., frequent and/or strong justice violations) at one pole to strong adherence (i.e., rare or nonexistent justice violations) at the other pole. Although some justice scholars have acknowledged that “perception can be at variance to reality” (Lind, 2001: 66), the literature still seems to imply a relatively straightforward relationship between supervisors’ actual adherence to justice rules and subordinates’ perceptions of overall fairness, such that supervisors who adhere more strictly to justice rules are perceived to be fairer (Ambrose & Schminke, 2009; Colquitt & Shaw, 2005; Kim & Leung, 2007; Lefkowitz, 2009). We suggest that this correspondence does not always hold. Further, the occasions when it does not are systematically predictable. When supervisors breach interpersonal justice rules, subordinates naturally perceive a threat to their welfare. This causes them to devote more attention to the process of understanding their supervisors’ intentions and motives (Kunda, 1990). Thus, subordinates are likely to vigilantly monitor supervisor behavior in order to detect the extent to which their welfare is threatened (MacLeod & Mathews, 1988; Mogg & Bradley, 2002). In this way, vigilance is self-protective. It is also a natural response, as negative events trigger more sensemaking and a greater need for attribution than positive events (Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Finkenauer, & Vohs, 2001). In particular, negative interpersonal treatment triggers active search rather than automatic processing (Posten & Mussweiler, 2013). As noted by Roberson (2006: 179): Consistent with the findings of attributional research which suggest that negative and unexpected events prompt attributional search more often than do positive events (Hastie, 1984, Pyszczynski and Greenberg, 1981 and Wong and Weiner, 1981), outcome valence and expectancy violation have been identified as triggers for attributional activity related to fairness evaluations…researchers have suggested that unfair treatment, which can be characterized by unfavorable outcomes or rule violation, will initiate similar processes (Rutte & Messick, 1995). After experiencing unfairness, people will engage in information-seeking behavior to determine the cause of such treatment. 10 Similarly, Mayer and Gavin (2005: 876) note that “[i]f the manager begins to take actions that could damage the employee’s interests, by closely monitoring the manager, the employee can more quickly take action to lessen the negative effects of the manager’s influence.” This line of thinking is consistent with fairness theory (Folger & Cropanzano, 2001), which suggests that just behavior leads to more superficial and automatic cognitive processing than unjust behavior. When an individual focuses intently on a particular stimulus, object, or person, he or she is more likely to make attributions that are specific to its individuating characteristics rather than rely on general notions about the category to which it belongs (Pratto & Bargh, 1991). Thus, when an employee is compelled to narrow attention to a source of threat or injury (a supervisor who is violating justice rules), he or she will be more attuned to the supervisor’s individual characteristics. As a result, subordinates with supervisors from stigmatized social groups are unlikely to be influenced by stereotypes, whereby a stereotype is “a generalization about a group” that “reflects faulty thought processes or overgeneralization, factual incorrectness, inordinate rigidity, an inappropriate pattern of attribution, or a rationalization for a prejudiced attitude or discriminatory behavior” (Dovidio, Brigham, Johnson, & Gaertner, 1996: 80). Rather than forming an impression based on the supervisor’s membership in a social group, harmful behavior causes subordinates to form an impression based on individual characteristics of the supervisor, reducing the likelihood that subordinates with supervisors from minority ethnic groups evaluate their supervisors differently than subordinates with Caucasian supervisors (Fiske & Neuberg, 1990). Along these lines, in a comprehensive review of the literature, Blair (2002: 250) observed that “[a] well-supported finding is that stereotypes dominate unless the perceiver spends more time learning about the person’s unique attributes.” Upon distinguishing among different ways that attention can influence stereotyping, Blair (2002: 251-252) drew the following conclusion: …many studies have found that stereotyping is more likely when perceivers cannot or do not pay attention to the target…processes that counteract stereotypes can only operate with 11 sufficient attention…therefore a lack of attention makes it highly likely that judgment and behavior will be biased by automatic stereotypes. Consistent with the evidence just reviewed, subordinates who experience interpersonal justice violations from supervisors who are stigmatized minorities are likely to evaluate their supervisors in the same way as subordinates who experience interpersonal justice violations from Caucasian supervisors. Note that we do not suggest that subordinates form “accurate” or “unbiased” judgments. Such an assessment is difficult—perhaps impossible—to make, given the subjective nature of fairness (Folger & Cropanzano, 1998) as well as prior research suggesting that individuals often fall victim to irrational judgment when experiencing threat (Staw, Sandelands, & Dutton, 1981). Rather, our argument is that any biases that do exist will not be contingent on ethnic stereotypes. Therefore, evaluations of supervisors will not vary depending on whether subordinates have minority or Caucasian supervisors when justice violations have occurred. In contrast, the lack of threat associated with respectful, dignified treatment allows subordinates to direct their limited attentional resources to other foci, such as task responsibilities, relationships with colleagues, and personal obligations (Kanfer & Ackerman, 1996). When attention is diverted away from a stimulus, object, or person, a perceiver is still capable of forming judgments about it, however these judgments are largely developed outside the purview of careful processing (Fiske, 1998; Petty & Cacioppo, 1986). This reality of cognition makes subordinates’ judgments susceptible to beliefs that are unrelated to supervisors’ individual characteristics, such as ingrained conceptions about stigmatized groups to which the supervisor may belong (e.g., ethnic stereotypes) (Pratto & Bargh, 1991). Further, Unkelbach, Forgas, and Denson (2008: 1410) investigated the role of affect on stereotyping, noting that “[a]ccording to recent affect-cognition theories…positive affect should trigger a more top-down, spontaneous and assimilative processing style, increasing the influence of pre-existing schemata and stereotypes on responses.” Since interpersonally just treatment triggers positive affect (Colquitt et al., 2013), stereotypes are likely to influence evaluation when supervisors adhere to interpersonal justice rules. 12 Thus, pre-existing stereotypes that distinguish minorities from Caucasians are likely to (1) persist for supervisors who adhere to interpersonal justice rules, and (2) be inhibited for supervisors who violate interpersonal justice rules. The notion that stereotyping can shift between these two situations adapts and extends the idea that stereotypes are not rigid, but instead can be activated to influence general beliefs and perceptions only in certain circumstances (Blair, 2002; Kunda & Spencer, 2003). This activation and inhibition is typically done rapidly and subconsciously. Our interest lies in how stereotypes influence comparisons between minority and Caucasian supervisors—a focus that is consistent with the notion that stereotypes usually refer to generalized judgments of the inferiority of certain racial groups relative to other racial groups. Although minority supervisors may be perceived as less fair when they violate justice rules than when they adhere to them, our core argument is that they are not perceived as less fair than Caucasian supervisors when interpersonal justice rules are violated. This is because subordinates are attuned to qualities that characterize supervisors as individuals (e.g., their personalities) rather than qualities that indicate social group membership (e.g., race), and, in turn, subordinates are less vulnerable to forming impressions based on supervisors’ social category membership. Instead, we suggest—somewhat paradoxically—that subordinates perceive minority supervisors to be less fair than Caucasians only when supervisors adhere to interpersonal justice rules. Hypothesis 1: Supervisor race will interact with supervisor justice rule adherence in predicting perceptions of overall supervisor fairness, such that minority supervisors will be perceived as less fair than Caucasians when supervisors adhere to interpersonal justice rules, but not when they violate interpersonal justice rules. Since stereotypes that negatively differentiate minority supervisors from Caucasian supervisors are activated when supervisors adhere to interpersonal justice rules rather than violate them, the stereotypes that do emerge must be consistent with actions that are positive (polite, respectful, and dignified treatment) (Bies & Moag, 1986). On the surface, this may appear to be a contradiction of sorts. How can minority supervisors from stigmatized groups be seen as worse than Caucasian supervisors only in conditions when good things happen? To explain this tension, 13 one stereotype appears particularly relevant: the belief that stigmatized minorities are more deceitful than Caucasians. When supervisors act rudely or disrespectfully, the recipients of such treatment are likely to assume that supervisors genuinely feel derision toward them, as there is little to be gained socially by acting disrespectfully toward others when such feelings are not sincere. In contrast, supervisors who treat subordinates graciously may have ulterior motives. Perhaps they display politeness and respect in an ingratiating manner in order to gain favor (Liden & Mitchell, 1988), or they seek buy-in from subordinates in order to maintain harmony in the workplace, build political clout, and create the appearance that their hold on power is legitimate (Ferris et al., 2007). Indeed, people often make the inaccurate, overgeneralized judgment that minorities from stigmatized groups are more likely than Caucasians to act politely as an instrument to attain valued ends, such as gaining political capital (Ogbu, 1985). This is consistent with research suggesting that one of the most prevalent stereotypes of minorities is that they are deceitful. In an overview of how Blacks have been viewed historically in the U.S., Holt (1986: 312) noted that it has long been common to believe that “blacks are deceitful.” Outside the United States, Adejunmobi (2009: 312) has similarly observed that a stereotype that “blacks are deceptive” persists. Indeed, nearly half of participants in one study labeled Blacks as deviant, which is a key corollary of deceitful behavior (Henricks, 2011). McCrate (2006) traces the history of this stereotype to Blacks being fashioned as poorly disciplined and dishonest, with the implication being that they must rely on deceptive— rather than diligent—routes to success. As a result of this historical inertia, the view of Blacks as deceitful remains an enduring stereotype (Burton et al., 2005; Devine & Elliot, 1995; Sigall & Page, 1971; Vala, Pereira, Lima, & Leyens, 2012; Wittenbrink, Judd, & Park, 1997). Similarly, Hispanics are stereotyped as “treacherous, deceitful, cunning” (Basler, 2010: 104; see also Berg, 1990). In one study, the stereotype of deceitfulness was seen as more relevant to Mexicans than any other negative stereotype by three different social groups (white Americans, 14 Vietnamese, and Chinese) (Burton et al., 2005). Furthermore, Burton et al. (2005) found that deceitfulness was the only negative attribute that White Americans considered to be closely entwined with the identity of Mexican-Americans (African-Americans were viewed as the second most deceitful social group by Whites). In another study, Hispanics were viewed as deceitful because they were thought to be likely to lie or obscure information (Evans & Michael, 2014). Other stereotypes reinforce the reality that Hispanics are likely to be viewed as deceitful. In one sample, more than half of participants viewed Hispanics as ethically deviant (Henricks, 2011), and in another study the second most common stereotype of Hispanics was that they were legally deviant (Levy, Stroessner, & Dweck, 1998). Since Hispanics are considered to be deviant, as well as lazy and unintelligent (Levy et al., 1998), a warped assumption is that they must rely on cunning and guile—rather than intelligence and hard work—to succeed. In line with evidence suggesting that both Blacks and Hispanics are likely to be stereotyped as deceitful, subordinates are likely to perceive that positive interpersonal treatment from a minority supervisor is the result of someone who is conniving and insincere. Given that individuals in work settings are known to sometimes mask their true feelings when their roles call for constant displays of respect toward others (Brotheridge & Grandey, 2002), subordinates of minority supervisors may have even greater reason to believe that respectful actions are not authentic. Whereas subordinates may explain just treatment from stigmatized minorities with the stereotype of deceitfulness, Caucasian supervisors are unlikely to be pegged as deceitful when they adhere to justice rules, because this stereotype does not exist for Caucasians (Burton et al., 2005; Ogbu, 1985).1 In turn, the belief that minority supervisors are deceitful is likely to negatively influence fairness perceptions. To this point, Burton et al. (2005) found that deceitfulness was the most condemned of twenty-eight stereotypes of minorities, including aggression, arrogance, 1 This stereotype may exist for other minorities, such as Asians, yet it is less powerful for these social groups than for Blacks and Hispanics (Cheryan & Bodenhausen, 2000). Therefore, our main comparison point involves Blacks and Hispanics versus Caucasians. 15 selfishness, and laziness. This suggests that the perception that a supervisor is deceitful is likely to have a particularly negative effect on a subordinate’s judgment of his or her supervisor’s overall fairness (i.e., how generally virtuous the supervisor is). Consistent with our arguments on alternating patterns of stereotype activation and inhibition, we predict that the attribution that minority supervisors are less fair than Caucasian supervisors will be explained by perceptions that minorities from stigmatized groups are more deceitful than Caucasians. We expect that this relationship will hold when supervisors adhere to interpersonal justice rules, but not when they violate them. See the dashed lines in Figure 1. Hypothesis 2: The relationship between supervisor race and fairness perceptions will be (1) mediated by the perception that the supervisor is deceitful and (2) conditional on interpersonal justice, such that the perception that minority supervisors are more deceitful than Caucasian supervisors will mediate the relationship between supervisor race and fairness perceptions when supervisors adhere to interpersonal justice rules, but not when they violate interpersonal justice rules. To further crystallize the logic behind Hypotheses 1 and 2, it is useful to consider why we posit that race is an antecedent while interpersonal justice serves as a moderator (see Figure 1). Race influences fairness perceptions through stereotyping because the content of any racial stereotype is a direct result of a supervisor’s race. In contrast, we posit that interpersonal justice rule adherence is a moderator because it provides the necessary condition for this stereotype to be activated to influence subordinates’ fairness perceptions. In this vein, interpersonal justice does not influence the content or social meaning of the stereotype of deceitfulness, but rather provides the circumstance (reduced attention) that facilitates the onset of stereotyping and, in turn, the attribution process that leads to the perception that supervisors are unfair. Consequences of the Activation and Inhibition of Stereotypes Although subordinates do not have the authority to punish supervisors directly and are unlikely to substantially deviate from their work responsibilities because the consequences of such action are likely to be too severe, they may exhibit bias against minority supervisors by 16 undercutting the social exchange relationship. There are two basic ways that subordinates can do this. First, they can help minority supervisors less than Caucasians via reduced extra-role behavior (Farh, Podsakoff, & Organ, 1990; Lee & Allen, 2002). Second, they can harm minority supervisors more than Caucasians via increased undermining. Consistent with past research (Cohen-Charash & Spector, 2001; Colquitt, Conlon, Wesson, Porter, & Ng, 2001), greater perceived fairness is often associated with more positive outcomes. This would suggest that increased fairness perceptions will boost extra-role helping behavior and decrease undermining. When integrating this logic with that of Hypothesis 1 (subordinates of minority supervisors will perceive less fairness than subordinates of Caucasian supervisors only when supervisors of both races adhere to interpersonal justice rules), we expect that subordinates with minority supervisors will engage in less helping behavior and more undermining than subordinates of Caucasian supervisors only in the context of interpersonal justice rule adherence, because they perceive minority supervisors as less fair than Caucasian supervisors. This leads us to make the following prediction (see the solid lines in Figure 1): Hypothesis 3: The relationship between supervisor race and bias will be (1) mediated by fairness perceptions and (2) conditional on interpersonal justice, such that fairness perceptions will mediate the relationship between supervisor race and bias when supervisors adhere to justice rules, but not when they violate them. We predict that subordinates of minorities will perceive less supervisor fairness than subordinates of Caucasians only when supervisors adhere to interpersonal justice rules (Hypothesis 1), and that this interactive effect will be explained by minority supervisors being perceived as more deceitful than Caucasian supervisors (Hypothesis 2). Further, we predict that minority supervisors will experience more bias than Caucasian supervisors when they adhere to interpersonal justice rules, and that this effect will be explained by perceptions of overall supervisor fairness (Hypothesis 3). When combining each of these effects, we predict an expanded causal chain with two mediating effects and one moderating effect. Specifically, we expect the relationship between perceptions of deceit and perceptions of fairness to mediate the relationship 17 between supervisor race and both helping and undermining (i.e., bias), and adherence to interpersonal justice rules to moderate the path between perceptions of deceit and perceptions of fairness. This prediction constitutes an integration of all of the relationships shown in Figure 1 (both the solid and dashed lines). Hypothesis 4: The relationship between supervisor race, perceptions of deceit, fairness perceptions, and bias in helping behavior/undermining (supervisor race perceptions of deceit fairness perceptions bias) will be conditional on whether supervisors adhere to interpersonal justice rules, such that the relationship will exist when supervisors adhere to justice rules, but not when they violate them. We first test our theory with field data (study 1) and then replicate this effect by testing it in a controlled experiment (study 2). In this experiment we also extend the findings of study 1 to test the role of stereotyping. Finally, we revisit our field data to understand how our study 1 and study 2 findings work together to explain how bias against minority supervisors persists in the context of justice and how the troubling effects we find can be mitigated. STUDY 1: FIELD SURVEY Method Sample and procedure. In order to gain access to full-time employees from a diverse set of industries (e.g., financial services, banking, information technology, engineering, telecommunications, healthcare, and pharmaceuticals), we asked 149 business school students to each recruit two potential participants for an online survey, of whom 117 (79%) agreed to do so. This type of sampling procedure is widely used (e.g., Eddleston, Veiga, & Powell, 2006; Mayer, Nishii, Schneider, & Goldstein, 2007; Morgeson & Humphrey, 2006). We required participants to have an immediate supervisor who was willing to complete an online survey.2 In an effort to increase response rates, we sent out reminder emails one and two weeks after initial contact. This 2 To ensure anonymity, all identifying information was removed and the data were transmitted via Secure Sockets Layer (SSL). In exchange for their participation, both subordinates and supervisors were given an equal chance of winning one of five $100 Visa gift cards. To address potential problems associated with participant eligibility, we asked for business email addresses, contacted potential participants directly, and visually inspected the data for multiple indicators of suspicious responses. 18 resulted in responses from 239 subordinates (93%) and 202 supervisors (85%), leading to a total of 185 subordinate-supervisor pairs. Of those supervisors, 82.2% were Caucasian, 3.2% were Hispanic, and 5.4% were African American. The average age was 38 years (SD = 13.15) for subordinates and 45 years (SD = 11.37) for supervisors, respectively. 51% of subordinates and 41% of supervisors were female. Since our theory has the clearest implications for Caucasians, Blacks, and Hispanics, we removed supervisors from other ethnic groups. Although the relatively small number of minority supervisors in our sample is consistent with published work on minority leaders and power positions (Elvira & Town, 2001; Hekman et al., 2010), we took several steps to improve confidence in our estimates (as described below). Measures. We use three sources of data (objective personal characteristics, supervisor perceptions, and employee perceptions) to reduce the possibility of common method variance. We collected both supervisor-reported and subordinate-reported perceptions of how well supervisors adhered to interpersonal justice rules (i.e., respect and propriety) when carrying out decision making procedures. All scales described below meet thresholds of reliability put forth by Kline (2013). Supervisors completed four items adapted from Colquitt’s (2001) interpersonal justice scale assessing their level of interpersonal justice rule adherence (Zapata, Olsen, & Martins, 2012) on a scale from 1 (“To a Very Small Extent”) to 5 (“To a Very Large Extent”). Sample items include: “Do you treat this subordinate in a polite manner?,” and “Do you treat this subordinate with respect?” (.65). Subordinates indicated their perceptions of interpersonal justice received from supervisors using four items from Colquitt’s (2001) scale (1 = “To a Very Small Extent” to 5 = “To a Very Large Extent”). Sample items include: “Has he/she treated you in a polite manner?,” and “Has he/she treated you with respect?” (.89). We measured subordinate fairness perceptions using a scale developed by Rodell and Colquitt (2009). The survey asked subordinates to indicate their agreement with a series of statements about their supervisor’s overall level of fairness. Sample items include: “How often 19 does your immediate supervisor act fairly toward you?” (1= “Almost Never” to 5 = “Almost Always”), and “To what extent do you believe that your immediate supervisor is fair to you?” (1= “To a Very Small Extent” to 5= “To a Very Large Extent”) (.89). We used a three-item scale developed by Liden and Maslyn (1998) pertaining to extra-role behavior that a subordinate is willing to invest in order to help his or her supervisor (e.g., “I am willing to apply extra efforts, beyond those normally required, to meet my supervisor’s work goals”; “I do work for my supervisor that goes beyond what is specified in my job description”) on a scale from 1 (“Strongly Disagree”) to 5 (“Strongly Agree”) ( .77). Finally, supervisor race was dummy-coded 0 for Caucasian and 1 for minorities. Controls. We controlled for several factors that may affect the extent to which subordinates would be either biased toward supervisors or likely to stereotype them. We controlled for the supervisor’s tenure in the organization (in months), which should influence his/her social ties and political capital. We controlled for the number of months the subordinate has reported to the supervisor. We also accounted for dissimilarity between the supervisor and the subordinate on the factors of race, age, and gender.3 When all variables were entered into the model, listwise deletion resulted in the removal of 18 cases. Results and Discussion Descriptive statistics are shown in Table 1. We should note that the means for interpersonal justice are consistent with past survey research (e.g., Colquitt et al., 2012; Johnson et al., 2014).4 3 We also created dummy codes to represent all 28 industries in the sample. None of them had a significant influence on the outcome. Since the number of dummy codes was so large, we excluded these fixed effects to preserve a reasonable variable-to-observation ratio. However, results remained consistent when these fixed effects were included. 4 Given the relatively small number of minority supervisors in our data, we first checked our data for outliers. Analyses using both multiple item and multiple construct identification techniques strongly suggested that there are no error outliers in the dataset (Aguinis, Gottefredson, & Joo, 2013). In addition, we conducted sensitivity analyses by broadening the criteria we used for minorities by including Native Americans and Asians (which effectively doubles the number of minority supervisors). Given that minorities other than Hispanics and Blacks are not as stigmatized as Hispanics and Blacks (Burton et al., 2005), the results are unlikely to be as strong when Native Americans and Asians are included. However, because they are still more stigmatized than Caucasians with respect to deceit (Burton et al. 2005), the pattern of results should remain consistent. We discuss the results of these sensitivity analyses below. 20 ------------------------------------------Insert Tables 1 and 2 About Here ------------------------------------------We tested our hypotheses with hierarchical regression (see Table 2). To test our prediction that supervisor race would interact with supervisor justice rule adherence to predict employee perceptions of fairness (Hypothesis 1), we corrected for the skew of the dependent variable by taking its natural log. To account for the likelihood that supervisors’ self-reported perceptions of their adherence to justice rules and subordinates’ perceptions of their supervisors’ adherence to justice rules are both vulnerable to biases, we included both measures in our model simultaneously as separate indicators of supervisor adherence to interpersonal justice rules. The interaction term was significant for both subordinate-reported justice rule adherence, B = -.10, t = -2.10, p < .05, and supervisor-reported justice rule adherence, B = -.22, t = -2.30, p < .05. We calculated simple slopes for the combined effects of subordinate-reported and supervisor-reported measures and used the variance-covariance matrix of regression coefficients to ascertain standard errors. In line with our expectations, subordinates perceived minority supervisors to be less fair than Caucasian supervisors when they adhered to interpersonal justice rules (B = -.23, t = -3.79, p < .001), but not when they violated them (p = .39). When considered independently, the subordinate-reported justice and supervisor-reported justice measures had the same substantive patterns of significance for their respective simple slopes (see Figures 2a and 2b for plots of these interactions). ------------------------------------------------Insert Figure 2 and Table 3 About Here ------------------------------------------------Finally, we tested our prediction that a positive relationship between supervisor race and subordinate extra-role behavior would be (1) mediated by fairness perceptions and (2) conditional on interpersonal justice rule adherence, such that perceptions of fairness would explain why subordinates help minority supervisors less when supervisors adhere to interpersonal justice rules, but not when they violate them (Hypothesis 3). Consistent with the solid lines in Figure 1, we 21 tested for mediated moderation, or a Stage 1 conditional indirect effect (Hayes, 2013). Similar to our test of Hypothesis 1, we included both supervisor-rated and subordinate-rated perceptions of justice rule adherence as indicators of the moderator, and we report effects of the combined tests of both ratings (see Table 3). Analyses demonstrated that 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals for the indirect effect (B = -.07) excluded zero (95% CI: -.23, -.01) when supervisors adhered to justice rules, however when supervisors violated interpersonal justice rules, 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals for the indirect effect (B = .02) did not exclude 0.5 Overall, the results from this study provide support for our argument that minority supervisors would only experience bias from subordinates when they adhered to interpersonal justice rules, and that the relationship between supervisor race and extra-role behavior would be mediated by fairness perceptions.6 Further, the sample used in our analysis (supervisor-subordinate dyads from a wide range of employers in various industries) suggests that our findings are generalizable. Perhaps most troubling is that we found this bias when including measures of subordinates’ own perceptions of their supervisors’ adherence to interpersonal justice rules (see 5 We took several steps to ensure that our results are robust. First, when we include Native Americans and Asians, the race and interpersonal justice rule adherence interaction is significant for supervisor-rated justice and marginally significant for subordinate-rated justice. In addition, the full conditional indirect effect is supported. Second, our usage of bootstrapping allows inferences on indirect effects to be drawn from smaller samples than other approaches (Shrout & Bolger, 2002). Third, our multi-source data reduced the likelihood of common method variance. Fourth, we tested whether the positions of fairness perceptions and extra-role behavior (fairness perceptions extra-role behavior) could be reversed, such that extra-role behavior fairness perceptions. However, confidence intervals for the conditional indirect effect in this model did not exclude 0 either when supervisors adhered to interpersonal justice rules or when they violated them. Fifth, supervisor race was not related to interpersonal justice rule adherence. Sixth, although there could be some aspect of bi-directional causality in our data, we ruled out the possibility that race predicts perceptions of interpersonal justice rule adherence through overall fairness perceptions (race overall fairness perceptions perceptions of interpersonal justice). Seventh, we considered theoretical perspectives related to the possibility that overall justice perceptions drive perceptions of justice rule adherence (Lind, 2001). Fairness theory (Folger & Cropanzano, 2001) couches justice rules as antecedents to overall fairness perceptions. Further, Colquitt (2012: 532) noted that “although this overall fairness could serve a number of roles, it is often discussed as ‘theoretically downstream’ from the specific justice dimensions …[F]rom this perspective, distributive, procedural, interpersonal, and informational justice serve as antecedents of overall fairness, with overall fairness then serving as an antecedent of attitudinal and behavioral outcomes.” 6 Of the four justice rule dimensions, we assert that only adherence to interpersonal justice rules influences stereotyping and fairness perceptions in the ways posited in our theory. To verify this, we conducted two separate supplementary analyses. First, we reran all of the above analyses controlling for both subordinate-reported and supervisor-reported measures for procedural, distributive, and informational justice rules. Results were consistent when these factors were included. Second, we substituted each of the justice rule dimensions for interpersonal justice and performed the same analyses. None of them replicated our findings for any of the hypotheses. 22 Figure 2a). The implication is that even when subordinates perceive that minority supervisors adhere to interpersonal justice rules to the same degree as is reported by subordinates of Caucasian supervisors, minority supervisors are still rated as less fair than Caucasian supervisors and, in turn, their subordinates are less willing to go above and beyond for them. This study is not without limitations. Although our measures and analyses reduced some concerns related to reverse causality, the research design remains vulnerable to spurious effects (though this should not impact our hypothesized interactions). Also, although we used scale items and scale anchors consistent with existing research on justice in study 1, the anchor values make it difficult to delineate clearly between zones of justice adherence and justice violation. However, our conclusions can be preserved since behavior can be interpreted as less just and more just in a relative sense. In addition, we examined entity-based rather than event-based interpersonal justice rule adherence (for a review, see Cropanzano et al., 2001). We were unable to focus on a specific justice event since we surveyed supervisor-employee dyads across several organizations. However, this is consistent with a number of field studies that have included questions in which subordinates are asked how frequently their supervisors adhere to justice rules in general rather than during a specific event (McFarlin & Sweeney, 1992; Moorman, 1991; Rupp & Cropanzano, 2002; Scott, Colquitt, & Zapata-Phelan, 2007). Given our focus on the justice dimension most intertwined with the supervisor (i.e., interpersonal justice) (Bies, 2005; Scott et al., 2009) and our emphasis on global attributions made about supervisor fairness, an entity-based focus was most appropriate. It is also worth noting that Colquitt et al. (2013) demonstrated that meta-analytic effect sizes do not depend on whether attributions are made about an event or entity. Finally, although we took steps to ensure that our conclusions based on the number of minority supervisors in study 1 were valid, the number of minorities could make our results vulnerable to noise. To establish causal inference, rule out alternative explanations, and mitigate some of the limitations of our field study, we conducted a controlled laboratory experiment that manipulates 23 supervisor race and a supervisor-initiated justice event (allowing for a roughly even number of Caucasian and minority supervisors). By manipulating (in)justice, we provide participants with the initial justice-relevant information needed to inform overall fairness perceptions, and by using random assignment we minimize differences across conditions. We also expand our focus by explicitly testing for the role of stereotyping and investigating whether our findings generalize from withholding benefits from minority supervisors to undermining them. STUDY 2: EXPERIMENT Method Sample and design. 307 undergraduate students enrolled in introductory organizational behavior and human resource management classes at a university in the United States participated in the experiment as one option to fulfill a course requirement. The final sample size was dictated in part by a university rule that all students in the course must have the opportunity to enroll in the experiment. The sample was 63% Caucasian, 5% Hispanic, 20% Asian/Pacific Islander, 7% African American, and 5% “Other.” 53% of the participants were female. The experiment utilized a two-by-two between-subjects factorial design: interpersonal justice (adherence/violation) by supervisor race (Caucasian/minority). To reflect the field survey, we included both Blacks and Hispanics in the minority supervisor condition. However, the experimental manipulation that we describe below required us to manipulate Blacks separately from Hispanics. Procedures. When the participants arrived, we instructed them to type their names into the computer and informed them that we were interested in studying cognitive performance in an anagram task. We told participants that they would be paired with another participant from a local university via networked computers. The students were told that the strongest performer on an initial trial would be designated the supervisor while the other participant would continue to solve anagrams. The supervisor’s role would be to grade the participant’s anagram tasks in order to determine his/her final performance. In actuality, there was no participant at the other institution. 24 Each participant was told that he/she scored lower than the participant from the other university, and thus the participant from the other university would be the supervisor for the remainder of the task. To increase believability regarding our use of a student supervisor, we told participants that we were interested in understanding how people work with supervisors who do not belong to the same institution and who they know little about. After the anagrams from the initial trial were purportedly graded, we introduced our manipulation of supervisor race. Supervisor race manipulation. A full-cycle research design mandates that the characteristics of a controlled experiment should mirror the characteristics of the external setting (Chatman & Flynn, 2005). Since our study 1 analyses included Caucasians, Blacks, and Hispanics, we included the same ethnic groups in the experiment. We manipulated supervisor race through a pre-programmed message to participants, informing them that they would continue in their role as participants and that their supervisor had either a Caucasian-sounding name (Todd/John) or a minority-sounding name (Tyrone/Juan). Given that all participants were asked to provide their names in the beginning of the experiment, it was credible that the supervisor’s name was given at this point. Although including pictures could have strengthened our race manipulation, we sought to manipulate race with a minimum possible number of cues in order to reduce the likelihood that participants would be distracted or guess the purpose of the study (Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2002). Previous research suggests that certain names are perceived to be principally used by certain ethnicities (Bertrand & Mullainathan, 2004; Milkman, Akinola, & Chugh, 2012; Rosette, Leonardelli, & Phillips, 2008), therefore we manipulated race using only names. We paired “Tyrone” and “Todd” based on Rosette et al. (2008: 766-767), who validated the use of these names in a depiction of CEOs in a study that examined bias in favor of Caucasian leaders. Milkman, Akinola, and Chugh (2012) discovered that “Juan” is viewed as one of the two most highly representative names of the Hispanic ethnic group. Further, “Juan” is the name of many prominent Hispanic leaders in government and business. “John” is highly representative of 25 the Caucasian racial group, has the same number of letters as “Juan,” and etymologically is the Caucasian equivalent of the name “Juan.” Participants were randomly assigned to each condition with the following message, in which participants either saw “Todd,” “John,” “Tyrone,” or “Juan” in the bracketed space: Based on your performance on the last set of anagrams, you have been assigned the role of: Participant. The assigned leader for the current run of the study is: [leader name]. The leader will be responsible for grading your anagrams, which will determine your bonus eligibility. Manipulation of justice rule adherence. Given that the supervisors purportedly controlled performance evaluation and compensation (participants scoring in the top 25% received a bonus $5 Starbucks gift card), participants were likely to be sensitive to justice or injustice. During the task, the supervisors and participants were able to send a message to each other. When coupled with justice-related decisions, the presence of this interaction made interpersonal justice relevant. After completing the second anagram task, the supervisor sent a message to the participant, which, in reality, was pre-determined by the experimenters. This message manipulated interpersonal justice, which relates to displays of politeness, dignity, and respect in the context of resource allocation decisions (Bies, 2005; Bies & Moag, 1986). Participants in the interpersonal justice rule adherence condition received the following message: “I assume the potential bonus is important to you so I want you to know that I’m taking my job of grading your anagrams seriously.” Participants in the interpersonal justice rule violation condition received the following message: “My job as the leader is tougher than solving these easy anagrams. Your potential bonus is the last thing on my mind.” After receiving this message, the participants completed a final anagram task consisting of 12 anagrams, and were then asked to fill out the survey questionnaire while the supervisor purportedly compiled the results of the anagram tasks. Embedded within the questionnaire was an attention check that asked participants to indicate whether they had received a message (i.e., our manipulation) from the supervisor. The eleven participants who indicated that 26 they had not received a message from the supervisor were removed from the analyses, leaving a final sample of 296 participants. Mediators. We measured the stereotype of deceitfulness by asking participants, “To what extent do you think the leader is deceptive?” using the following scale (1 = “Strongly Disagree” to 5 = “Strongly Agree”). Stereotype research involving interpersonal interactions between pairs of individuals (such as in this experiment) presents a dilemma. While stereotypes involve overgeneralizations about social groups, our experiment involved a participant encountering a single individual. Prior research has resolved this dilemma by (1) ensuring that participants are aware of a target’s social group membership, (2) employing questionnaire items about the target (in this case, the supervisor) and then (3) extrapolating the existence of stereotyping from between-groups differences that emerge in the analyses (Blair, 2002). This way, questionnaire items tap into the actual experience of participants (an individual level experience) while the analyses tap into the construct of stereotyping (overall differences in how group members are perceived). We also assessed participants’ fairness perceptions utilizing a three-item scale developed by Zapata-Phelan et al. (2009), which adapted the items we used in Study 1 to the experimental context (.97). Dependent variables. To extend beyond the study 1 finding on the withholding of extrarole behavior, we sought to assess whether subordinates would inflict harm on minority supervisors via social undermining. Undermining involves “behavior intended to hinder, over time, a worker’s ability to establish and maintain positive interpersonal relationships, work-related success, and favorable reputation” (Duffy, Ganster, Shaw, Johnson, & Pagon, 2006: 105). We used the three categories invoked in Duffy et al.’s (2006) above-stated definition (reputation, interpersonal relationships, and work-related success) as a framework to guide our identification of three factors that are vulnerable to being undermined. Because blame spreads negative information about a supervisor’s character and motivations, casting blame toward a supervisor will undermine 27 that supervisor’s reputation. In addition, given that trust is one of the most central elements of functional relationships, a subordinate’s expressions of distrust will undermine the supervisor’s ability to establish positive interpersonal exchange relationships. Finally, since rewards and credit are central to career advancement, the discouragement of a supervisor’s ability to receive rewards will undermine his or her work-related success. To measure blame-throwing, the participants were given items adapted from a scale developed by Nicklin, Greenbaum, McNall, Folger, and Williams (2011) that pertained to whether they would explicitly blame the supervisor or hold him responsible for how their performance was assessed (e.g., “the leader should have done something different”) (.78). Second, to assess outward displays of distrust, we gave participants a four-item scale on distrust, adapted from Mayer and Gavin (2005) (e.g., “I really wish I had a good way to keep an eye on the leader”) (.77). Finally, after the task was completed, participants were given the opportunity to recommend (1 = “Strongly Disagree” to 5 = “Strongly Agree”) to the experimenter whether the supervisor should be eligible for the bonus. To increase the likelihood that participants would believe that they could actually influence the awarding of the supervisor’s bonus, we highlighted the fact that the supervisor had not completed any anagrams during the final performance trial, so the only way to assess his/her performance was to survey the other participant. Manipulation checks. In order to determine whether our interpersonal justice rule adherence manipulation was successful, we asked participants to indicate the extent of their agreement (1 = “Strongly Disagree” to 5 = “Strongly Agree”) with three items (e.g.., “The leader gave me feedback in a respectful manner”) (.92). To check the effectiveness of our supervisor race manipulation, we asked participants to describe what race they thought the leader is. Participants could select one of the following options: Caucasian, Black or African American, Hispanic, Native American, Asian/Pacific Islander, or Other. At the end of our study we asked participants (1) whether they thought they had guessed the purpose of the study, and (2) if so, what 28 they thought the purpose was. None of the participants guessed that the purpose of the study had to do with the supervisor’s race. Results and Discussion We ran a multivariate analysis of variance to check our manipulations and ensure that there were no spillover effects. Participants identified whether they experienced interpersonal justice rule adherence or violations in a way consistent with our manipulations, F(1,289) = 152.74, p < .001. Participants also identified the race of the supervisor in a way consistent with our manipulations, F(1,289) = 30.89, p < .001. In addition, the justice manipulation did not predict perceptions of supervisor race (p = .83) and the race manipulation did not predict perceptions of interpersonal justice (p = .56). For all analyses, we tested models both including and excluding participant race. We report results for models in which participant race is included. However, all results remain robust when leaving participant race out of the model. Descriptive statistics are shown in Table 4. To test whether the findings from study 1 would replicate, we ran an analysis of variance. As with Study 1, our prediction that supervisor race would interact with supervisor adherence to interpersonal justice rules in predicting fairness perceptions (Hypothesis 1) was supported, F(1,287) = 5.81, p < .05. Further, tests of simple effects revealed that there was a marginally significant discrepancy in fairness perceptions when supervisors adhered to interpersonal justice rules (B = -.25, t = -1.86, p = .06), but not when they violated interpersonal justice rules. Caucasian supervisors who adhered to interpersonal justice rules were evaluated as fairer (M = 3.84) than minority supervisors who adhered to interpersonal justice rules (M = 3.59). In contrast, the difference in fairness perceptions between Caucasian supervisors (M = 3.37) and minority supervisors (M = 3.58) was not significant when they violated interpersonal justice rules. Thus, results provide partial support for our prediction that when supervisors adhered to interpersonal justice rules, subordinates would perceive minority supervisors to be less fair than Caucasian supervisors. See Figure 2c for a plot of this interaction. 29 --------------------------------Insert Table 4 About Here --------------------------------We then tested our prediction that this interaction would be explained by the stereotype that minorities are more deceitful than Caucasians (Hypothesis 2). In line with Figure 1, we tested this with moderated mediation, or a stage 2 conditional indirect effect, whereby adherence to interpersonal justice rules moderated the indirect effect of supervisor race stereotype activation fairness perceptions (Hayes, 2013). In essence, this analysis tested whether a pre-existing stereotype is activated only when minority supervisors adhere to justice rules. Consistent with this prediction, when supervisors adhered to interpersonal justice rules, analyses demonstrated that bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals for the indirect effect (B = -.09) excluded zero (95% CI: .02, -.19). When supervisors violated interpersonal justice rules, analyses demonstrated that bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals for the indirect effect (B = -.05) did not exclude 0. We then sought to test the prediction (Hypothesis 4) that the relationship between stereotype activation and fairness perceptions would mediate the relationship between supervisor race and undermining when supervisors adhered to interpersonal justice rules, but not when they violated them. This constitutes the full test of Figure 1. To test this hypothesis, we conducted a conditional path analysis. We first tested whether this effect held for blame-throwing. When supervisors adhered to interpersonal justice rules, analyses demonstrated that bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals for the indirect effect (B = .03) excluded zero (95% CI: .01, .09). When supervisors violated interpersonal justice rules, bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals for the indirect effect did not exclude 0. We then tested whether this effect held for displays of distrust. When supervisors adhered to interpersonal justice rules, bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals for the indirect effect (B = .02) excluded zero (95% CI: .01, .07). When supervisors violated interpersonal justice rules, bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals for the indirect effect did not exclude 0. Finally, we tested whether this indirect effect would hold for whether participants 30 recommend that leaders not be given bonuses. When supervisors adhered to interpersonal justice rules, bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals for the indirect effect (B = .04) excluded zero (95% CI: .01, .13). When supervisors violated interpersonal justice rules, bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals for the indirect effect did not exclude 0. In sum, and as shown in Table 5, the conditional path analysis was supported for all three types of undermining, providing strong support for our theoretical model. ---------------------------------Insert Table 5 About Here ---------------------------------In this study we replicated all of the findings from study 1 while extending them in several important ways. First, we established causality. Second, we manipulated supervisor justice rule adherence and violation rather than relying on self-reported or other-reported perceptions. Third, we showed that our theory generalized from decreasing extra-role behaviors to inflicting harm via undermining. Fourth, we established the explanatory role of stereotype activation and inhibition by demonstrating that stereotyping emerged when supervisors adhered to justice rules, but not when they violated them. In the context of justice rule adherence, individuals used ingrained beliefs about the inferiority of minority supervisors to color their perceptions of fairness and, in turn, engage in undermining. However, stereotyping (and the link between stereotyping, fairness perceptions, and bias) disappeared when they needed to attend to supervisor behavior in order to make sense of why supervisors violated interpersonal justice rules. To be clear, although justice violations suppress the use of stereotypes, given the well-known downsides of justice rule violations (Colquitt et al., 2013), we do not suggest that minorities should violate justice rules. There are limitations with this study. Although our experiment allows us to make causal inferences, it relied on a supervisor who was not physically present. Though electronic communication is now one of the most prevalent forms of interpersonal interaction in the workplace, such exchanges lack various characteristics that make interactions rich. We also 31 suggest caution with interpreting the simple slope in our test of Hypothesis 1. The significance test (p = .06) suggests that there is a 6% possibility of the finding being due to chance rather than the 5% possibility that serves as conventional protocol in the social sciences (Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003). COMPLETING THE CYCLE: REVISITING STUDY 1 DATA Consistent with full-cycle research, we use the findings from the experiment to re-interpret the field data from Study 1 (Chatman & Flynn, 2005). The Relationship between Stereotype Activation/Inhibition and Overall Bias Against Minorities A key nuance of our theory is that stereotypes do not reverse when supervisors violate interpersonal justice rules. That is, minorities are never perceived as fairer than Caucasians (or, more precisely, less unfair than Caucasians). Rather, stereotypes are simply attenuated (a buffering effect), such that minorities and Caucasians are evaluated in the same way. This nuance has critical implications for the perpetuation of stereotypes and bias against minority supervisors. Since stereotypes persist in some situations and merely disappear in others, the upshot is that bias in how minority supervisors are perceived relative to Caucasian supervisors is, on balance, perpetuated. Even if some situations cause bias against minority supervisors to disappear, the net effect is that stereotype activation causes biased perceptions to persist. This is especially likely to be the case when considering the prevalence of justice rule adherence relative to the prevalence of justice rule violations. Probing the mean levels of justice rule adherence in our survey data, we found that about 15% of supervisors failed to adhere to interpersonal justice rules somewhat frequently.7 This frequency aligns with other research that has found similar baseline levels of injustice in organizations (King, 2004; Pager & Shepherd, This was calculated by assessing the proportion of respondents who responded “somewhat” to “to what extent does the supervisor engage in [justice behavior]?” 7 32 2008). When coupled with our findings, this large adherence-to-violation ratio means that subordinates may discriminate against the majority of minority supervisors. These patterns led us to expect an overall indirect effect in our survey data: subordinates will perform extra-role behaviors for Caucasian supervisors more than minority supervisors, and this effect will be mediated by perceptions of fairness. Consistent with this prediction, analyses demonstrated that bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals for the indirect effect between race, perceptions of fairness, and helping behavior (B = -.04) excluded zero (95% CI: -.10, -.01). This finding suggests that subordinates generally exhibit bias against minority supervisors because they deem them to be less fair than Caucasians. How Can Stereotyping and Bias from Just Treatment be Mitigated? As noted earlier, Blair (2002: 250) observed that “[a] well-supported finding is that stereotypes dominate unless the perceiver spends more time learning about the person’s unique attributes.” Thus, stereotyping is reduced when subordinates view supervisors in terms of their unique, individuating characteristics (e.g., personality) rather than in terms of over-generalizations related to their social categories (i.e., racial stereotypes). For this reason, the relationship between interpersonal justice rule adherence and stereotyping may be attenuated by factors that help increase subordinates’ awareness of their supervisors’ individual characteristics. A key factor that determines the extent to which subordinates get to know their supervisors individually is how closely they work with them. One of the primary drivers of the intensity of interaction patterns is the organization’s structure. In particular, interaction intensity is dependent on how many subordinates supervisors oversee (i.e., the size of supervisors’ units). Supervisors who directly oversee small work units will have more time to spend with each subordinate and fewer competing demands. Given the opportunity to build strong relationships with subordinates, supervisors who oversee small units are likely to take a keen interest in each subordinate and assume responsibility for their development and success. By building an intense working relationship, subordinates are 33 likely to be more familiar with their supervisors’ unique personal qualities. Even if subordinates who have minority supervisors do not pay close attention to their supervisors when they adhere to justice rules (and therefore do not pose a threat), subordinates’ prevailing familiarity with the idiosyncratic characteristics of their supervisors will likely override attributions that would otherwise be made purely on the basis of stereotypes. Overall, then, it could be the case that supervisors with small units are not subjected to stereotyping when they exhibit justice. Using the study 1 survey data, we found results consistent with this possibility. With our usage of a variable capturing unit size, listwise deletion resulted in a sample size of 158. We found a three-way interaction between race, interpersonal justice rule adherence, and unit size on overall fairness perceptions for both subordinate perceptions of interpersonal justice rule adherence, B = -.01, t = -1.98, p = .05, and supervisor perceptions of interpersonal justice rule adherence, B = -.03, t = -2.17, p < .05. In both cases, the interaction between race and interpersonal justice was significant in large work units (about 22 members) and not significant in small work units (about 2 members). Note that we did not find an overall main effect for unit size. That is, it is not necessarily better for supervisors to work in smaller, more intimate environments. Instead, the influence of unit size is best understood in the moderating role we identify above. GENERAL DISCUSSION Our findings are consistent with the logic that when supervisors adhere to interpersonal justice rules, subordinates feel less threat from their supervisors and turn their attention toward other foci (Mayer & Gavin, 2005; Roberson, 2006), ultimately leading them to rely on stereotypical beliefs about minorities. Across a survey and an experiment we provided support for the notion that this stereotyping leads to bias against Blacks and Hispanics, suggesting that our findings apply to multiple stigmatized racial minority groups. To shed light on this troubling result, we integrated two constructs from organizational justice (interpersonal justice rule 34 adherence and fairness perceptions) and two constructs from research on racial bias (stereotyping and upward-directed bias). The findings drawn from our integrated framework contribute back to research in both of these domains. Contributions to Theory on Organizational Justice A new look at social exchange perspectives on justice. Social exchange is perhaps the most frequently used theoretical framework for understanding the beneficial effects of organizational justice rule adherence (Colquitt, 2012). Drawing from prior research on power asymmetry in the context of social exchange, it stands to reason that (1) possessing authority over others and (2) treating others respectfully typically protects against harmful treatment from those same people (Colquitt et al., 2001). When both of these forces are combined, it would appear that supervisors who exhibit interpersonal justice will be in a strong position to get back what they invest in their relationships with subordinates (Cropanzano & Mitchell, 2005). Indeed, research has found that subordinates generally reciprocate their supervisors’ adherence to interpersonal justice rules by displaying positive attitudes and behaviors (e.g., supervisor-focused citizenship) (Colquitt et al., 2013). By identifying occasions when the expectation that supervisors will get back what they put in to the exchange relationship goes unfulfilled, we add important nuance to social exchange-based perspectives on justice. Rather than giving more to subordinates and then getting better treatment in return, minorities encounter a troubling reality. When compared to Caucasian supervisors, minority supervisors who give more often get worse treatment in return. Further, the nature of this lack of reciprocation suggests that justice has the potential not only to lead to negative outcomes, but to undermine itself: supervisor behaviors that are taken for granted as representing justice lead to subordinate behaviors that impair the ability for minority supervisors to be treated the same way as Caucasian supervisors. Given the power inherent in their positions in the organizational hierarchy, the prospect that respectful supervisors experience more bias from their subordinates than disrespectful supervisors is surprising. If this finding is confirmed in additional studies across 35 diverse settings and samples, then it can challenge the longstanding consensus that supervisors who treat subordinates respectfully will be the beneficiaries of positive outcomes. In this vein, it would suggest that there are important boundary conditions to perspectives on organizational justice that are informed by social exchange. Our findings also suggest that theory on attention can help determine when and why supervisor-subordinate relationships may not be characterized by reciprocal exchange. When subordinates do not pay attention to their supervisors, they engage in stereotyping and ultimately do not reciprocate their supervisor’s just behavior. Since it represents the mechanism that likely causes stereotyping, attention can also be used to understand which situational factors (e.g., unit size) attenuate bias. In sum, attention provides a unified theoretical framework for incorporating factors from the literature on racial bias and organizational structure into theory on social exchange to understand (1) what explains and (2) what attenuates the negative consequences of adherence to interpersonal justice rules. In this way, an attention-based view provides a parsimonious and integrative way to unpack the cognitive and situational mechanisms that characterize social exchange relationships in the context of justice and racial diversity. The downside of the fairness heuristic. Although subordinates are capable of consciously distinguishing between specific facets of supervisor justice behavior (e.g., procedural versus distributive versus interpersonal), scholars have suggested that the attention required to make such fine-grained assessments is likely to be depleting. As a result, subordinates often form global judgments about whether their supervisors are generally fair or unfair (Lind & van den Bos, 2002). Known as the fairness heuristic, this judgment process occurs rapidly and subconsciously, such that subordinates use thin slices of justice-relevant behavior (e.g., respectful actions) to make global assessments of supervisor fairness (Lind, 2001). One major downside of this type of high level, automatic processing is that it remains vulnerable to distorted thought patterns, such as stereotypes. Although all perceptual measures, including justice measures “can be at variance to reality” (Lind, 2001: 66), the ramifications of this downside have not been fully acknowledged in 36 the justice literature. Our findings suggest that, when individuals do not have a reason to pay careful attention to their supervisors (when they experience a lack of threat), the fairness heuristic is more likely to be employed, subsequently allowing stereotypes to seep in and bias fairness judgments. This updated view of the bias inherent in fairness judgments explains why subordinates’ fairness perceptions are sometimes warped and, ultimately, when and why subordinates direct biased behavior toward minority supervisors. Contributions to Theory on Stereotyping and Racial Bias in Supervisor Evaluations Our findings provide a novel view of both stereotyping and racial bias. With respect to stereotyping, we have demonstrated why stereotyping remains prevalent in seemingly benign environments—wherein minorities are stereotyped more when they treat others with respect. Although it is known that inattention drives stereotyping, we have identified an enabler of inattention (lack of threat from supervisors) that is novel because it is nested in supervisor behavior. This, in turn, suggests that supervisors are unwittingly participating in the very behavior that unleashes stereotyping against them. With respect to theory on racial bias, we helped shed light on a phenomenon that has remained largely uninvestigated in research associated with discrimination: upward-directed bias. This form of bias reflects how minority supervisors face a disconcerting, paradoxical tension between the power and status they have earned while climbing the organizational hierarchy and the lack of power and status they have inherited by virtue of their membership in historically stigmatized ethnic groups. Our results corroborate the prediction that stereotype activation perpetuates two different ways that subordinates exhibit bias against minority supervisors relative to Caucasian supervisors: providing less help and engaging in more undermining. Given that a supervisor’s success largely depends on his or her legitimate hold on power, upward-directed bias is likely to be debilitating for minorities. In addition to examining a different form of racial bias, we uncovered a novel causal process that helps to explain the well-documented stubbornness of racial bias in organizational 37 settings. There remains an incomplete understanding of why racial bias remains so prevalent in spite of the social, ethical, and institutional forces that work against it. Since (1) interpersonal justice rules are relevant to many interactions, and (2) supervisors adhere to interpersonal justice rules in most of these interactions, an extension of our finding would be that bias persists against most minority supervisors most of the time. Consistent with this idea, we reported an indirect effect demonstrating that minorities generally experience more bias than Caucasian supervisors because they are perceived to be less fair. In sum, the pressure on supervisors to adhere to interpersonal justice rules paradoxically undercuts the ideal of a just meritocracy. Since the type of behavior that triggers bias is deemed to be inherently good, bias is likely to sustain. Managerial Implications We uncovered the disturbing pattern that supervisors are more likely to be stereotyped when they treat their subordinates with dignity than with derision. In this way, interpersonal justice rule adherence is a double-edged sword for minorities, who can either act unjustly and be treated equally, or act justly and be treated unequally. Given the numerous benefits that interpersonally just supervisors provide for their organizations and the various deleterious outcomes that result from unjust behavior (Colquitt et al., 2013), we do not in any way suggest that justice violations are preferable to justice adherence. Rather, we suggest that our identification of this “no win” situation for minorities puts the onus on organizations, which are faced with multi-layered questions regarding fairness and equity. Our finding that unit size mitigates the influence of interpersonal justice on racial bias can help surface potential solutions because it suggests that structural and situational factors can influence the psychological mechanism (inattention to supervisors’ individual characteristics) that triggers stereotyping (Blair, 2002). Given that direct attempts to change employees’ beliefs are challenging since stereotypes are often deeply ingrained, interventions that focus on the structural and situational attributes that influence attention may be especially useful. When assigning supervisors fewer direct reports is not possible, other alterations to the physical environment can 38 make the presence of supervisors more salient. For instance, supervisors can be located more proximally to subordinates. Organizational routines can also have an impact. To illustrate, frequent interaction gleaned from one-on-one meetings or small group caucuses may make supervisor characteristics more apparent. As another alternative, routines that lead to relationship building may cause supervisors to reveal personal characteristics that make their individuality more readily come to mind. For example, after-hour social activities or outings that involve partners and family members may allow subordinates to see their supervisors in a different light. As a final way to redress the troubling pattern identified in this paper, we suggest that the design of the survey instruments used within organizations to assess supervisor fairness be reconsidered. For instance, when individuals assess supervisors in 360 degree feedback surveys, there are often questions about overall fairness. Even in settings such as education, instructor evaluation forms frequently include questions that tap into global perceptions of instructor fairness. To take another example, two thirds of the calculations for the annual list of the Fortune 100 Best Places to Work are based on an employee assessment called a trust index, which assesses, among other things, how fairly supervisors treat employees. An overreliance on global measures of fairness is problematic because our data indicate that when employees are asked to make assessments about overall fairness, employees who have supervisors from one race make judgments in ways that are inconsistent with employees who have supervisors from another race. We therefore suggest that company surveys include questions that tap into specific justice behaviors rather than ask employees to make high-level, overarching assessments about the fairness of their supervisors. In support of this view, the interaction between race and interpersonal justice rule adherence was not significant for the interpersonal justice manipulation check in study 2 (p = .953). Perhaps this consistency is due to the nature of the questions, which lead subordinates to focus intently on specific behaviors rather than rely on general impressions, which are more vulnerable to the influence of stereotypes. 39 Limitations and Future Directions There are natural limitations extending from our paper’s scale and scope. Although our survey sample included supervisor-subordinate dyads from various organizations and industries, lending the results a strong element of generalizability, we did not focus on a single work context, nor did we include multiple subordinates from each organization in our sample. This prevents the identification of the role of group and interpersonal factors in shaping the relationship between interpersonal justice rule adherence and fairness perceptions (Lind, Kray, & Thompson, 2001). Future research can assess such social dynamics by accounting for when bias may snowball due to social contagion effects and when it may be buffered by third-parties who speak out against it (Rosette, Carton, Bowes-Sperry, & Hewlin, 2013). In addition, we examined two stigmatized racial groups concurrently even though they are different in several respects. That said, there is strong support for Blacks and Hispanics both being characterized by the stereotype of deceit (e.g., Burton et al., 2005). More importantly, our results generalize to a broader construct (stigmatized group membership) rather than a single racial group. In terms of our study 1 methodology, we did not test a within-person design in which individuals had multiple supervisors of different races. As a result, we cannot unequivocally claim that the same individual would treat supervisors differently based on race. However, about 90% of employees have a single dominant supervisor in organizations (Brody et al., 2014), therefore limiting this concern and suggesting that our results do possess external validity. Finally, we did not test multiple supervisor-subordinate pairs from the same organization. There are a number of potentially fruitful avenues for future research. As one alternative, scholars could examine whether expectations for unjust behavior vary based on supervisor race. If expectations are lower for minority supervisors, then employees may “see” more injustice (Shapiro & Kirkman, 2001). Of all possible directions, perhaps the most promising involves the role of attention. Our findings suggest that attention can be used to incorporate factors from research domains (stereotyping and organizational structure) that (1) have to date been 40 disconnected and (2) have not been extensively considered in tandem with justice. As a key component of organizational life, attention influences many distinct factors—from communication to decision making to information transfer (Ocasio, 1997). Since interpersonal justice appears to be relevant to such a foundational organizational construct, we suggest that interpersonal justice rule adherence may be associated with a more expansive set of psychological, social, and institutional forces than previously thought. By putting a broader array of factors in play, an attention-based view of interpersonal justice increases the potential precision with which scholars can explain the influence of interpersonal justice on important outcomes while expanding its scope and relevance to new areas of inquiry. This increases the likelihood that important new insights will emerge. Further, since such insights would stem from a common mechanism, they could be understood in a theoretically integrated fashion. CONCLUSION For supervisors from stigmatized minority groups, the standards associated with interpersonal justice rules are a double-edged sword. If supervisors violate interpersonal justice rules, then subordinates suffer and the subordinate-supervisor relationship is damaged. Yet if they adhere to interpersonal justice rules, then they operate at a disadvantage relative to Caucasian supervisors. When supervisors treat their subordinates justly, they paradoxically set themselves up to face bias in return. Findings from our exploratory analyses suggest that this bias can be mitigated when characteristics in the organizational context create the conditions needed for subordinates to get to know their supervisors personally. Yet despite this potential mitigating factor, we found that minorities experience more overall bias than Caucasian supervisors, and that this bias is perpetuated by the lack of threat that subordinates experience following positive interpersonal treatment. Hence, our results suggest the existence of a troubling reality: justice can promote injustice. 41 REFERENCES Adams, J. S. 1965. Inequity in social exchange. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 2. New York: Academic Press. Adejunmobi, M. 2009. Claiming the field: Africa and the space of indian ocean literature. Callaloo, 32(4): 1247-1261. Aguinis, H., Gottfredson, R. K., & Joo, H. 2013. Best-practice recommendations for defining, identifying, and handling outliers. Organizational Research Methods, 16: 270-301. Ambrose, M. L., & Schminke, M. 2009. The role of overall justice judgments in organizational justice research: A test of mediation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(2): 491-500. Anderson, D. 1993. Cultural diversity on campus: A look at intercollegiate football coaches. Journal of Sport and Social Issues, 17(1): 61-66. Bargh, J. A. 2013. Social psychology and the unconscious: The automaticity of higher mental processes. New York: Psychology Press. Basler, C. R. 2010. Latinos/as through the lens. In J. Sutherland, & K. Feltey (Eds.), Cinematic sociology: Social life in film: 99-108. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. 2001. Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5(4): 323-370. Berg, C. R. 1990. Stereotyping in films in general and of the Hispanic in particular. Howard Journal of Communications, 2(3): 286-300. Bertrand, M., & Mullainathan, S. 2004. Are Emily and Greg more employable than Lakisha and Jamal? A field experiment on labor market discrimination. The American Economic Review, 94(4): 991-1013. Bies, R. J. 2005. Are procedural justice and interactional justice conceptually distinct? In J. Greenberg, & J. A. Colquitt (Eds.), Handbook of organizational justice: 85-112. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. Bies, R. J., & Moag, J. S. 1986. Interactional justice: Communication criteria of fairness. Research on Negotiation in Organizations, 1(1): 43-55. Blair, I. V. 2002. The malleability of automatic stereotypes and prejudice. Personality & Social Psychology Review, 6(3): 242-261. Brody, C. J., Rubin, B. A., & Maume, D. J. 2014. Gender structure and the effects of management citizenship behavior. Social Forces, 92(4): 1373-1404. 42 Brotheridge, C. M., & Grandey, A. A. 2002. Emotional labor and burnout: Comparing two perspectives of “people work”. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 60(1): 17-39. Burton, M. L., Greenberger, E., & Hayward, C. 2005. Mapping the ethnic landscape: Personal beliefs about own group’s and other groups’ traits. Cross-cultural Research, 39(4): 351379. Chatman, J. A., & Flynn, F. J. 2005. Full-cycle micro-organizational behavior research. Organization Science, 16(4): 434-447. Cheryan, S., & Bodenhausen, G. V. 2000. When positive stereotypes threaten intellectual performance: The psychological hazards of “model minority” status. Psychological Science, 11(5): 399-402. Cohen-Charash, Y., & Spector, P. E. 2001. The role of justice in organizations: A meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 86(2): 278-321. Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S., & Aiken, L. 2003. Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3 ed.). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Colquitt, J. A. 2001. On the dimensionality of organizational justice: A construct validation of a measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(3): 386-400. Colquitt, J. A. 2012. Organizational justice. In S. W. J. Kozlowski (Ed.), The oxford handbook of organizational psychology, Vol. 1: 526-547. New York: Oxford University Press. Colquitt, J. A., Conlon, D. E., Wesson, M. J., Porter, C. O., & Ng, K. Y. 2001. Justice at the millennium: A meta-analytic review of 25 years of organizational justice research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(3): 425-445. Colquitt, J. A., LePine, J. A., Piccolo, R. F., Zapata, C. P., & Rich, B. L. 2012. Explaining the justice–performance relationship: Trust as exchange deepener or trust as uncertainty reducer? Journal of Applied Psychology, 97(1): 1-15. Colquitt, J. A., Scott, B. A., Rodell, J. B., Long, D. M., Zapata, C. P., Conlon, D. E., & Wesson, M. J. 2013. Justice at the millennium, a decade later: A meta-analytic test of social exchange and affect-based perspectives. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98(2): 199-236. Colquitt, J. A., & Shaw, J. C. 2005. How should organizational justice be measured? In J. Greenberg, & J. A. Colquitt (Eds.), Handbook of organizational justice: 113-152. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates Publishers. 43 Cropanzano, R., Byrne, Z. S., Bobocel, D. R., & Rupp, D. E. 2001. Moral virtues, fairness heuristics, social entities, and other denizens of organizational justice. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 58(2): 164-209. Devine, P. G., & Elliot, A. J. 1995. Are racial stereotypes really fading? The Princeton trilogy revisited. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21: 1139-1150. Dovidio, J. F., Brigham, J. C., Johnson, B. T., & Gaertner, S. L. 1996. Stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination: Another look. In N. C. Macrae, C. Stangor, & M. Hewstone (Eds.), Stereotypes and stereotyping, Vol. 276: 319. New York: The Guilford Press. Duffy, M. K., Ganster, D. C., Shaw, J. D., Johnson, J. L., & Pagon, M. 2006. The social context of undermining behavior at work. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 101(1): 105-126. Eddleston, K. A., Veiga, J. F., & Powell, G. N. 2006. Explaining sex differences in managerial career satisfier preferences: The role of gender self-schema. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(2): 437-445. Elvira, M., & Town, R. 2001. The effects of race and worker productivity on performance evaluations. Industrial Relations: A Journal of Economy and Society, 40(4): 571-590. Evans, J. R., & Michael, S. W. 2014. Detecting deception in non‐native English speakers. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28(2): 226-237. Farh, J. L., Podsakoff, P. M., & Organ, D. W. 1990. Accounting for organizational citizenship behavior: Leader fairness and task scope versus satisfaction. Journal of Management, 16(4): 705-721. Ferris, G. R., Treadway, D. C., Perrewé, P. L., Brouer, R. L., Douglas, C., & Lux, S. 2007. Political skill in organizations. Journal of Management, 33(3): 290-320. Fiske, S. T. 1998. Stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology, Vol. 1 and 2: 357–411. New York: McGraw-Hill. Fiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. L. 1990. A continum of impression formation, from category―based to individuating processes: Influences of information and motivation on attention and interpretation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 23: 1-74. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. 44 Folger, R., & Cropanzano, R. 2001. Fairness theory: Justice as accountability. In J. Greenberg, & R. Cropanzano (Eds.), Advances in Organizational Justice, Vol. 1: 1-55. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Folger, R. G., & Cropanzano, R. 1998. Organizational justice and human resource management. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Hayes, A. F. 2013. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press. Hekman, D. R., Aquino, K., Owens, B. P., Mitchell, T. R., Schilpzand, P., & Leavitt, K. 2010. An examination of whether and how racial and gender biases influence customer satisfaction. Academy of Management Journal, 53(2): 238-264. Henricks, K. 2011. The soft material significance of race: An examination of white stereotypes. Sociological Insight, 3: 146-168. Holt, E. 1986. "A Coon Alphabet" and the comic mask of racial prejudice. Studies in American Humor, 5: 307-318. Homans, G. C. 1961. Social behavior: Its elementary forms. New York: Harcourt, Brace, and World. Johnson, R. E., Lanaj, K., & Barnes, C. M. 2014. The good and bad of being fair: Effects of procedural and interpersonal justice behaviors on regulatory resources. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99: 635-650. Kanfer, R., & Ackerman, P. L. 1996. A self-regulatory skills perspective to reducing cognitive interference. In I. G. Sarason, G. R. Pierce, & B. R. Saraon (Eds.), Cognitive interference: Theories, methods, and findings: 153-171. New York: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates. Kim, T. Y., & Leung, K. 2007. Forming and reacting to overall fairness: A cross-cultural comparison. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 104(1): 83-95. King, R. 2004. The corporate antitrust problem. Workforce Management: 22. Kline, P. 2013. Handbook of psychological testing. London: Routledge. Kunda, Z. 1990. The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108(3): 480-498. Kunda, Z., & Spencer, S. J. 2003. When do stereotypes come to mind and when do they color judgment? A goal-based theoretical framework for stereotype activation and application. Psychological Bulletin, 129(4): 522-544. Lee, K., & Allen, N. J. 2002. Organizational citizenship behavior and workplace deviance: The role of affect and cognitions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(1): 131-142. 45 Lefkowitz, J. 2009. Promoting employee justice: It's even worse than that. Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 2(2): 221-225. Leventhal, G. S. 1980. What should be done with equity theory? New approaches to the study of fairness in social relationships. In K. J. Gergen, M. S. Greenberg, & R. H. Willis (Eds.), Social exchange: Advances in theory and research: 27-55. New York: Springer. Levy, S. R., Stroessner, S. J., & Dweck, C. S. 1998. Stereotype formation and endorsement: The role of implicit theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6): 1421-1436. Liden, R. C., & Maslyn, J. M. 1998. Multidimensionafity of leader-member exchange: An empirical assessment through scale development. Journal of Management, 24(1): 43-72. Liden, R. C., & Mitchell, T. R. 1988. Ingratiatory behaviors in organizational settings. Academy of Management Review, 13(4): 572-587. Lind, E. A. 2000. Fairness heuristic theory: Justice judgments as pivotal cognitions in organizational relations. In J. Greenberg, & R. Cropanzo (Eds.), Advances in organizational justice. Lexington, MA: New Lexington. Lind, E. A. 2001. Fairness heuristic theory: Justice judgments as pivotal cognitions in organizational relations. Advances in organizational justice, 56: 88. Lind, E. A., Kray, L., & Thompson, L. 2001. Primacy effects in justice judgments: Testing predictions from fairness heuristic theory. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 85(2): 189-210. Lind, E. A., & Van den Bos, K. 2002. When fairness works: Toward a general theory of uncertainty management. In B. M. Staw, & R. M. Kramer (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior, Vol. 24: 181-223. MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. 1988. Anxiety and the allocation of attention to threat. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40(4): 653-670. Masterson, S. S., Lewis, K., Goldman, B. M., & Taylor, M. S. 2000. Integrating justice and social exchange: The differing effects of fair procedures and treatment on work relationships. Academy of Management Journal, 43(4): 738-748. Mayer, D., Nishii, L., Schneider, B., & Goldstein, H. 2007. The precurors and products of justice climates: Group leader antecedents and employee attitudinal consequences. Personnel Psychology, 60(4): 929-963. Mayer, R. C., & Gavin, M. B. 2005. Trust in management and performance: Who minds the shop while the employees watch the boss? Academy of Management Journal, 48(5): 874-888. 46 McCrate, E. 2006. Why racial stereotyping doesn’t just go away: The question of honesty and work ethic. Working paper no. 115, University of Massachusetts. Amherst, MA. McFarlin, D. B., & Sweeney, P. D. 1992. Research notes. Distributive and procedural justice as predictors of satisfaction with personal and organizational outcomes. Academy of Management Journal, 35(3): 626-637. Milkman, K. L., Akinola, M., & Chugh, D. 2012. Temporal distance and discrimination: An audit study in academia. Psychological Science, 23(7): 710-717. Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. 2002. Selective orienting of attention to masked threat faces in social anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40(12): 1403-1414. Moorman, R. H. 1991. Relationship between organizational justice and organizational citizenship behaviors: Do fairness perceptions influence employee citizenship? Journal of Applied Psychology, 76(6): 845–855. Morgeson, F. P., & Humphrey, S. E. 2006. The Work Design Questionnaire (WDQ): Developing and validating a comprehensive measure for assessing job design and the nature of work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(6): 1321-1339. Nicklin, J. M., Greenbaum, R., McNall, L. A., Folger, R., & Williams, K. J. 2011. The importance of contextual variables when judging fairness: An examination of counterfactual thoughts and fairness theory. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 114(2): 127-141. Ocasio, W. 1997. Towards an attention‐based view of the firm. Strategic Management Journal, 18: 187-206. Ogbu, J. U. 1985. A cultural ecology of competence among inner-city Blacks. In M. B. Spencer, G. K. Brookins, & W. R. Allen (Eds.), Beginnings: The social and affective development of Black children: 45–66. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Pager, D., & Shepherd, H. 2008. The sociology of discrimination: Racial discrimination in employment, housing, credit, and consumer markets. Annual Review of Sociology, 34: 181-209. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. 1986. The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 19: 123-205. New York: Academic Press. Posten, A.-C., & Mussweiler, T. 2013. When distrust frees your mind: The stereotype-reducing effects of distrust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105(4): 567-584. 47 Pratto, F., & Bargh, J. A. 1991. Stereotyping based on apparently individuating information: Trait and global components of sex stereotypes under attention overload. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 27(1): 26-47. Roberson, Q. M. 2006. Justice in teams: The activation and role of sensemaking in the emergence of justice climates. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 100(2): 177-192. Rodell, J. B., & Colquitt, J. A. 2009. Looking ahead in times of uncertainty: the role of anticipatory justice in an organizational change context. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(4): 989-1002. Rosette, A. S., Carton, A. M., Bowes-Sperry, L., & Hewlin, P. F. 2013. Why do racial slurs remain prevalent in the workplace? Integrating theory on intergroup behavior. Organization Science, 24(5): 1402-1421. Rosette, A. S., Leonardelli, G. J., & Phillips, K. W. 2008. The White standard: racial bias in leader categorization. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(4): 758-777. Rupp, D. E., & Cropanzano, R. 2002. The mediating effects of social exchange relationships in predicting workplace outcomes from multifoci organizational justice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 89(1): 925-946. Scott, B. A., Colquitt, J. A., & Paddock, E. L. 2009. An actor-focused model of justice rule adherence and violation: the role of managerial motives and discretion. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(3): 756. Scott, B. A., Colquitt, J. A., & Zapata-Phelan, C. P. 2007. Justice as a dependent variable: subordinate charisma as a predictor of interpersonal and informational justice perceptions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(6): 1597-1609. Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. 2002. Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin. Shapiro, D. L., & Kirkman, B. L. 2001. Anticipatory injustice: The consequences of expecting injustice in the workplace. In J. Greenberg, & R. Cropanzano (Eds.), Advances in organizational justice: 152-178. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Shavers, V. L., Klein, W. M., & Fagan, P. 2012. Research on race/ethnicity and health care discrimination: Where we are and where we need to go. American Journal of Public Health, 102(5): 930-932. 48 Sigall, H., & Page, R. 1971. Current stereotypes: A little fading, a little faking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18(2): 247-255. Skitka, L. J. 2003. Of different minds: An accessible identity model of justice reasoning. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7(4): 286-297. Smith, D. B. 1999. Health care divided: Race and healing a nation: University of Michigan Press. Staw, B. M., Sandelands, L. E., & Dutton, J. E. 1981. Threat-rigidity effects in organizational behavior: A multilevel analysis. Administrative Science Quarterly, 26(4): 501-524. Thibaut, J. W., & Walker, L. 1975. Procedural justice: A psychological analysis. Hillside, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Unkelbach, C., Forgas, J. P., & Denson, T. F. 2008. The turban effect: The influence of Muslim headgear and induced affect on aggressive responses in the shooter bias paradigm. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(5): 1409-1413. Vala, J., Pereira, C. R., Lima, M. E. O., & Leyens, J. 2012. Intergroup time bias and racialized social relations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(4): 491-504. Van den Bos, K. 2001. Uncertainty management: The influence of uncertainty salience on reactions to perceived procedural fairness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(6): 931-941. Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. 1997. Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(2): 262-274. Zapata-Phelan, C. P., Colquitt, J. A., Scott, B. A., & Livingston, B. 2009. Procedural justice, interactional justice, and task performance: The mediating role of intrinsic motivation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 108(1): 93-105. Zapata, C. P., Olsen, J. E., & Martins, L. L. 2012. Social exchange from the supervisor’s perspective: Employee trustworthiness as a predictor of interpersonal and informational justice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 121(1): 1-12. 49 FIGURE 1 Theoretical Model Supervisor adherence to interpersonal justice rules Supervisor race Perceptions of fairness Bias against supervisor Perception of supervisor deceit a . Solid lines represent effects tested in both Study 1 and Study 2. Dotted lines represent effects tested only in Study 2. 50 FIGURE 2 Interaction between Race and Interpersonal Justice on Overall Fairness Perceptions Figure 2a: Study 1 Employee-rated Perceptions Figure 2b: Study 1 Supervisor-rated Perceptions 4.94 5 5 4.67 4.69 Caucasian supervisor Minority supervisor 4.01 4 3 Perceptions of Fairness 4.25 Perceptions of Fairness 4.62 4.36 2 4.08 4 Caucasian supervisor Minority supervisor 3 2 Supervisors Violate Justice Rules Supervisors Adhere to Justice Rules Supervisors Violate Justice Rules Supervisors Adhere to Justice Rules Figure 2c: Study 2 Experimental Manipulation Perceptions of Fairness 5 Caucasian supervisor 3.84 4 3.58 3.59 3.37 Minority supervisor 3 2 Supervisors Violate Justice Rules a Supervisors Adhere to Justice Rules . In all three figures, the difference between Caucasian and minority supervisors is significant when supervisors adhere to justice rules, but not when they violate them. In Figure 2c, this difference is marginally significant. b . For each plot, the variance-covariance matrix of regression coefficients was used to ascertain the standard errors. c . In figures 2a and 2b, values of interpersonal justice are taken at one standard deviation above and below the mean. d . The pattern for supervisor-rated perceptions was plotted with data from the coefficient from each interaction term when controlling for subordinate-rated perceptions. Likewise, the pattern for subordinate-rated perceptions was plotted with data from the coefficient from each interaction term when controlling for supervisor-rated perceptions. e . In Figure 2c, 95% confidence intervals are indicated with vertical bars. f . The means in Figure 2c are adjusted for the covariates listed in the method section. g . Although analyses were conducted with the natural log of fairness perceptions, the plots were created with the original metric in order to allow the means to be interpreted according to the original 1 to 5 scale. 51 TABLE 1 Study 1 Descriptive Statistics Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 1. Helping Behavior 4.17 0.65 2. Perceptions of overall supervisor fairness 4.61 0.62 .21** 3. Supervisor race 0.08 0.27 -.04 -.21** 4. Subordinate reported interpersonal justice 4.52 0.64 .19* .60** -.13 5. Supervisor reported interpersonal justice 4.64 0.48 .16* .03 .05 .19* 6. Supervisor-subordinate age difference -6.47 13.38 .03 -.09 .17* -.02 .08 7. Supervisor-subordinate gender difference 0.22 0.42 .07 -.04 -.05 -.12 -.02 -.02 8. Supervisor-subordinate race difference 0.18 0.39 .02 .07 .27** .01 .08 -.03 .01 * .04 -.11 -.15 .06 -.04 .00 -.11 .15 .07 -.13 9. Supervisor tenure (months) 117.27 96.99 -.05 .06 -.15 10. Length of subordinate-supervisor relationship 39.27 46.72 -.04 .01 .04 a . N = 167 . *p < .05 **p < .01 b 5 6 7 8 9 .40** 52 TABLE 2 Study 1: Results of Regression Analysis Perceptions of Overall Supervisor Fairness Variable B SE(B) t Constant 0.89 0.12 7.74** Controls: Supervisor tenure Length of subordinate-supervisor relationship Supervisor-subordinate age difference Supervisor-subordinate gender difference Supervisor-subordinate race difference 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.01 0.05 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.02 0.03 -0.22 0.47 -0.71 0.32 1.96 Main Effects: Supervisor race Subordinate-reported interpersonal justice Supervisor-reported interpersonal justice 1.33 0.15 -0.01 0.50 0.02 0.02 2.64** 8.64** -0.53 Interaction Effects: Supervisor race x Subordinate-reported interpersonal justice Supervisor race x Supervisor-reported interpersonal justice -0.10 -0.22 0.05 0.09 -2.10* -2.30* R2 . Note: N = 167. b . Race was dummy-coded 0 = Caucasian and 1 = ethnic minorities. c . *p < .05 **p < .01 a 0.40** 53 TABLE 3 Study 1: Conditional Indirect Effects of Supervisor Race on Helping Behavior via Perceptions of Overall Supervisor Fairness Interpersonal Justice Rule Adherence (+1 SD) Interpersonal Justice Rule Violation (-1 SD) Coefficient 95% Confidence Interval -0.07 0.02 (-0.23, -0.01) (-.10, .23) Note: a . N = 167. . Race was dummy-coded 0 = Caucasian and 1 = ethnic minorities. c . Values of independent variable are one standard deviation above and below the mean. b 54 Variable 1. Blame-throwing 2. Displays of distrust 3. Recommendation against bonus 4. Perceptions of overall supervisor fairness 5. Supervisor race 6. Interpersonal justice 7. Subordinate race (Caucasian) 8. Subordinate race (Black) 9. Subordinate race (Asian) 10. Subordinate race (Other) 11. Presumed race a . N = 296. . *p < .05 **p < .01 b M 3.01 3.22 2.93 3.60 0.50 0.52 0.64 0.06 0.19 0.04 0.09 TABLE 4 Study 2 Descriptive Statistics SD 1 2 3 0.81 0.75 .42** 1.26 .47** .50** 0.85 -.38** -.31** -.34** 0.50 .08 .03 .02 ** ** 0.50 -.36 -.28 -.30** 0.48 -.02 -.03 -.02 0.25 .06 .04 .09 0.39 -.04 -.01 -.03 0.21 -.08 -.04 -.04 0.29 -.02 -.02 -.02 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 -.03 .14* .03 .07 -.09 .02 .12* .01 .06 -.07 -.07 .05 -.12* .02 -.08 .05 .07 .05 -.35** -.65** .06 -.12* -.13* .01 .06 -.02 .17** .04 55 TABLE 5 Study 2: Conditional Indirect Effects of Supervisor Race on Bias via Serial Mediation through Perceptions of Supervisor Deceit and Overall Supervisor Fairness Blame-throwing Displays of distrust Recommendation against bonus B 95% Confidence Interval B 95% Confidence Interval B 95% Confidence Interval Interpersonal Justice Rule Adherence .03 (.01, .09) .02 (.01, .07) .04 (.01, .13) Interpersonal Justice Rule Violation .02 (-.001, .06) .01 (-.001, .05) .02 (-.001, .08) a . N = 296. Race was dummy-coded 0 = Caucasian and 1 = ethnic minorities. . Values of independent variable are one standard deviation above and below the mean. b