Traditional paper-pencil assessments (subset of problem types shown below): Christine Massey,1 Philip J. Kellman,2 Zipora Roth,1 Ji Y. Son,2 Timothy Burke 2 & Warren Longmire1 1University of Pennsylvania & 2University of California, Los Angeles Naming shaded pieces/Shading pieces Judging Fraction Magnitude Perceptual learning (PL): experience-induced changes in the ability to extract information. Declarative • Experts selectively perceive problemknowledge Procedural (e.g., knowledge relevant features as well as higher-level concepts, (e.g., how to) relationships. facts) • Even when explicitly instructed about Perceptual Learning higher-level relationships, novices often ignore (e.g., pattern them or cannot recognize them in new cases. recognition & structural • PL is crucial in the development of expertise intuitions) but largely neglected in K-12 instruction. The learning focus: creating and iterating unit fractions to produce new quantities (integers, fractions, and mixed numbers with the same denominator). Current Research Questions: (1) Can PL be systematically accelerated in mathematics – in particular, in the domains of fractions and measurement? (2) Can PL interventions (short varying interactions w/ critical relational structures) improve performance on traditional assessments? Variation in Trial Difficulty 1. Parent/offspring value: may be integer (1 or >1) or fraction (<1, improper, or mixed fraction) 2. # of conceptual steps: • One-step: No chunking to slice or clone. • Two-steps: The cloning Students have limited interpretations of fractions: e.g., the common part-whole interpretation (3 out of 5 slices of a pizza) is hard for students to generalize to improper and equivalent fractions, and to operations with fractions. Iterable 1/5 unit (but only in relation to the whole 1 unit) 1 unit 5/5 units 7/7 or 7/5 units? Each trial involves one parent quantity (given). and one offspring quantity (to be created). User controls interactive “slicing” and “cloning” tools. of the offspring requires chunking to create one whole out of 1/5 units; then adding on another three 1/5 units. • Control participants (7th graders) completed the most relevant fraction/measurement units taught in their curriculum before testing (7 weeks) • PLM participants (6th graders) received all treatment & tests except last delayed posttest prior to these curriculum units. Group Spring 08 Spring 08 6th graders, Intervention group (n=53 at start, 40 at end) Completed S&C1 PLM Baseline/pretest Immediate on S&C1 and S&C2 subscales Posttest on both subscales (Posttest 1) 7th graders, Uninstructed control (n=21) Baseline on S&C1 and S&C2 subscales Fall 08 After 5 month delay from PLM, Delayed Posttest on both subscales (Posttest 2) Fall 08 Naming shaded pieces/Shading pieces Circle pairs representing equal amounts. Word Problems Short Number Sentences Word Problems Criteria for completion of S&C 2 PLM • 8 categories (5 active slice/clone categories; 3 passive slice/active clone categories); 8/10 correct in each category • 70% learned to criterion in the 8 categories of SC2 • Average # of categories learned to criterion: 6.8 5 month delay one Write another fraction that represents the same amount. 5.5 month delay 2 conceptual steps to clone The learning focus: equivalent fractions involving different denominators Each trial involves one parent quantity (given) and two different offspring quantities (to be created). Incorrect Slice Response Correct Slice Response Shown feedback; given another slice opportunity before going on to clone. One offspring is shown cloned; subjects must clone to create the remaining offspring. S&C1 Intervention Effect sizes Posttest vs. Pretest: 2.03 Posttest vs. Control: 3.45 S&C2 Intervention Delayed Posttest vs. Pretest: 1.75 Delayed Posttest vs. Control: 2.79 S&C1 Intervention Effect sizes Posttest vs. Pretest: .84 Posttest vs. Control: 1.14 S&C2 Intervention Delayed Posttest vs. Pretest: 1.03 Delayed Posttest vs. Control: 1.40 • PL technology can facilitate structure discovery and structure mapping, offering an important complement to declarative and procedural instruction. • PL technology may accelerate fluency in processing key structures, reducing cognitive load and facilitating more advanced problem solving. Spring 09 Completed S&C2 PLM Immediate Posttest on both subscales (Posttest 3) Short Number Sentences Criteria for completion of S&C 1 PLM • 11 categories (one-step/low difficulty: 4 categories; two-step/high difficulty: 7 categories); 8/10 correct in each category • 100% learned to criterion in the 11 categories of SC1 • Average # of categories learned to criterion: 11 Example S&C 2 Trial The two modules presented here (Slice&Clone 1 and 2) are designed to help students come to understand fractions as representing quantities, in which the denominator of the fraction defines an iterable unit and the numerator of the fraction specifies a given number of iterations of that unit. Division/Multiplication of fractions (symbolic notation) Traditional paper-pencil assessments (subset of problem types shown below): Final Delayed Posttest on only S&C2 subscale • PL techniques show great promise even in high-level, symbolic domains such as mathematics −− including notoriously difficult areas such as fractions and measurement. Acknowledgements The researchers would like to thank Hye Young Cheong (Upenn) and Joel Zucker (UCLA) for assistance on this project. Special thanks go to the Feltonville School of Arts and Sciences. This research was supported by the Institute of Education Sciences, US Department of Education through Grant R305H06070 to UCLA. The opinions expressed are those of the authors and do not represent views of the US Department of Education.