7 Computer Modeling in Basal Ganglia Disorders José Luis Contreras-Vidal

advertisement
Computer Models of Atypical Parkinsonism
95
7
Computer Modeling in Basal Ganglia Disorders
José Luis Contreras-Vidal
1. INTRODUCTION
The last two decades have witnessed an increasing interest in the use of computational modeling
and mathematical analysis as tools to unravel the complex neural mechanisms and computational
algorithms underlying the function of the basal ganglia and related structures under normal and neurological conditions (1–3). Computational modeling of basal ganglia disorders has until recently
been focused on Parkinson’s disease (PD), and to a smaller scale, Huntington’s disease (HD) (4–6).
However, with the advent of large-scale neural network models of frontal, parietal, basal ganglia, and
cerebellar network dynamics (7–9), it is now possible to use these models as a window to study
neurological disorders that involve more distributed pathology such as in atypical parkinsonian disorders (APDs). Importantly, computational models may also be used for bridging data across scales
to reduce the gap between electrophysiology and human brain imaging (10), to integrate data in all
areas of neurobiology and across modalities (11), and to guide the design of novel experiments and
intervention programs (e.g., optimization of pharmacotherapy in PD) (12).
This chapter aims to present the paradigm of computational modeling as applied to the study of
PD and APDs so that it can be understood for those approaching it for the first time. Therefore,
mathematical details are kept to a minimum; however, references to detailed mathematical treatments are given. The brain, in its intact state, is a very complex dynamical system both structurally
and functionally, and many questions remain unanswered. However, theoretical and computational
neuroscience approaches may prove to be a very valuable tool for the neuroscientist and the neurologist in understanding how the brain works in health and disease.
The Modeling Paradigm
Computational models of brain and neurological disorders vary widely in the focus and scale of
the modeling, their degrees of simulated biological realism, and the mathematical approach used
(13). A combination of “top-down” and “bottom-up” approaches to computational modeling in neuroscience is depicted in Fig. 1, which shows the series of steps that need to be taken to study brain
disorders computationally. One first has to measure the behavioral operating characteristics (BOCs),
such as movement kinematics, average firing rates of homogenous cell populations, or neurotransmitter levels under intact conditions. The next step is to construct a mathematical model based on the
behavioral, anatomical, neurophysiological, and pharmacological data concerned with the production of the BOCs. The specified model should be capable of reproducing the measured behavior. The
modeled output is compared with the initial BOCs, leading to model refinement and further computer
simulations until a reasonable match between the measured and the modeled BOC can be achieved.
From: Current Clinical Neurology: Atypical Parkinsonian Disorders
Edited by: I. Litvan © Humana Press Inc., Totowa, NJ
95
96
Contreras-Vidal
Fig. 1. The paradigm of neural network modeling.
Although models are usually developed to demonstrate the biological plausibility of a particular
preexisting hypothesis, or to show how computational algorithms may be implemented in neural
networks, the same models can be used to generate and test new ideas by analyzing the emergent
properties of the model—model properties that cannot be explained by the operation or the wiring of
individual model components alone. Moreover, by lesioning the intact structural components of the
model, manipulating its dynamic mechanisms, or altering its inputs, it is possible to characterize the
neuropathology of the disease and the associated changes in the behavior. It is then also possible to
test the effect of surgical or pharmacological interventions to counteract the effects of lesions or
disease (2,12). Thus, computational modeling of neurological disorders involves a succession of
modeling cycles to first account for neural mechanisms underlying the normative data, followed by
the characterization and simulation of diseased mechanisms and abnormal behavior.
Modeling PD and APDs
Although a comprehensive review of the literature on modeling of basal ganglia function in health
and disease is not possible because of space limitations, the reader is referred to the reviews of Beiser
et al. (1), Ruppin et al. (2), and Gillies and Arbuthnott (3) for a detailed account. These efforts have
focused on explaining the roles of the basal ganglia in procedural learning (14–17), action selection
(18–20), serial order (21–23), and movement planning and control (24–27). Unfortunately, although
these models have provided insights on the abnormal mechanisms underlying PD, few efforts have
been put forward on modeling APDs. Thus, one goal of this chapter is to describe how existing
models of cortical, basal ganglia, and cerebellar dynamics can be used to model and simulate APDs.
In the next section, we briefly review the neuropathology in APDs. The reader is referred to Chapters 1,
4, 8, and 18–20 of this book for detailed reviews and discussion of the underlying neuropathology.
Neuropathology of the APDs
It is widely recognized that the differential diagnosis of APDs presents some difficult problems.
First, clinical diagnostic criteria are often suboptimal or partially validated (28); and second, the
Computer Models of Atypical Parkinsonism
97
usefulness of routine brain-imaging techniques, such as magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), is still
debatable because of issues such as specificity, sensitivity, variations in technical parameters, and the
experience of radiologists and neurologists (29). Nevertheless, modern MRI methodologies are useful to differentiate PD from APDs, as brain MRI in PD is grossly normal. Moreover, MRI studies
suggest specific regional brain markers associated with the underlying pathology in APDs, such as
multiple system atrophy (MSA), progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP), and corticobasal degeneration (CBD). Importantly, as these data can be used as a starting point for modeling purposes (Fig. 1),
we briefly summarize the MRI findings.
Multiple system atrophy of the parkinsonian type (MSA-P) appears to be characterized by
putaminal atrophy, T2-hypointensity, and “slit-like” marginal hyperintensity (29–32), whereas MSA
of the cerebellar type (MSA-C) usually is accompanied by atrophy of the lower brainstem, pons,
middle cerebellar peduncles, and vermis, as well as signal abnormalities in the pontine and the middle
cerebellar peduncles (30,31,33,34). Mild-to-moderate cortical atrophy is also common to both MSAP and MSA-C (29,35).
PSP is usually characterized by widespread cell loss and gliosis in the globus pallidus, subthalamic nucleus, red nucleus, substantia nigra, dentate nucleus, and brainstem, and often cortical atrophy and lateral ventricle dilatation (29). These patients also show decreased 18F-flurodopa in both
anterior and posterior putamen and caudate nucleus, and an anterior–posterior hypometabolic gradient (36). On the other hand, CBD may be differentiated by the presence of asymmetric frontoparietal
and midbrain atrophy (29).
Interestingly, cortical atrophy in APDs, particularly in cases involving damage of frontoparietal
networks, is consistent with reports of ideomotor apraxia, which can also be seen in PD patients when
their pathology is coincident with cortical atrophy (36–39). In particular, patients with PD and PSP,
who showed ideomotor apraxia, had the greatest deficits in movement accuracy, spatial and temporal
coupling, and multijoint coordination (38). However, as has been noted elsewhere, it remains to be
seen if the kinematic deficits observed in this patients are characteristic of apraxia or are caused by
elementary motor deficits (40). Given the pathological heterogeneity seen in patients with APDs, and
even in PD, it seems that computer modeling would be a useful tool to study how damage to frontoparietal networks and their interaction with a dysfunctional basal ganglia and/or cerebellar systems
may increase the kinematic abnormalities beyond some threshold that would result in the genesis of
limb apraxia.
MODELING STUDIES
In this section we present two examples of computational models of basal ganglia disorders. The
first example presents a model formulated as a control problem to investigate akinesia and bradykinesia in PD, whereas the second example, formulated as a large-scale, dynamic neural network model
involving frontoparietal, basal ganglia, and cerebellar networks, investigates the effects of PD and
PSP on limb apraxia.
Modeling Hypokinetic Disorders in PD
Contreras-Vidal and Stelmach proposed a neural network model of the sensorimotor corticostriato-pallido-thalamo-cortical system to explain some movement deficits (the “BOCs” in the modeling cycle) seen in PD patients (24). This model, originally specified as a set of nonlinear differential
equations, proposed the concept of basal ganglia gating of thalamo-cortical pathways involved in
movement preparation (or priming), movement initiation and execution, and movement termination
(Fig. 2A). In this model, three routes for cortical control of the basal ganglia output exist: activation
of the “direct pathway” opens the gate by disinhibition of the thalamic neurons (route 1), whereas
activation of both a slower indirect (through GPe, route 2) pathway and a faster direct cortical (route 3)
pathway closes the gate through inhibition of thalamo-cortical neurons.
98
Contreras-Vidal
Fig. 2. (A) Three pathways for control of pallidal output by cortical areas. B) Block diagram of the corticobasal ganglia-thalamo-cortical network of Contreras-Vidal and Stelmach (24). The basal ganglia output (GPi)
gates activity at the ventro-lateral thalamus (VLo), thus modulating cortico-cortical communication in the “vector integration model” that progressively moves the endeffector (PPV) to the target location (TPV). The basal
ganglia gating allows priming and controls the speed of change from limb present position (PPV) to target
position (TPV). Filled circles imply inhibitory connections, whereas arrows imply excitatory connections.
In this theoretical framework, activation of pathway 1 would result in activation of thalamo-cortical motor circuits leading to initiation and modulation of movement, whereas activation of pathway 2
would lead to breaking of ongoing movement. Activation of pathway 3 would facilitate rapid movement switching, or prevent the release of movement. The model postulated a role of the pallidal
output for generating an analog gating signal (at the thalamus) that modulated a (hypothesized) cortical system that vectorially computed the difference between a target position vector and the present
limb position vector, the so-called Vector-Integration-To-Endpoint (VITE) model (Fig. 2B) (41).
In this computational model, the basal ganglia output had the role of gating the onset, timing, and
the rate of change of the difference vector (DV), therefore controlling movement parameters such
as movement initiation, speed, and size. Moreover, the proposed basal ganglia gating function
allows three control modes: preparation for action or priming (gate is closed), initiation of action
(gate starts to open), and termination of action (gate starts to close). Simulations of this model demonstrated a single mechanism for akinesia, bradykinesia, and hypometria, and a correlation between
degree of striatal dopamine depletion and the severity of the PD signs.
In the terminology of control theory, it can been shown that the sensorimotor cortico-basal ganglia
network of Contreras-Vidal and Stelmach (24) can be approximated as a proportional + derivative
controller (Equation 1) with time-varying position (Gp; Equation 2) and velocity (Gv; Equation 3)
gains. The system described by the second-order differential equation (1) represents the motion of
the endpoint (P) toward the target (T) under the initial position and velocity conditions P = P0 and
dP0/dt = 0, respectively.
d
2
P = Gv d P – G p P – T
dt
dt
2
(1)
Computer Models of Atypical Parkinsonism
G p = α G0
99
t
1+t
Gv = 1 + t 2 – α
1+t
(2)
(3)
where α represents an integration rate and G0 is a scaling constant and t is the time in sec. The
system given by Equations 1–3 is based on the experimentally testable assumption that pallidal signals modulate the ventro-lateral thalamus (VLo) such that the pattern of thalamic output has a sigmoidal shape or is gradually increasing as a function of time as in Equation 4. This assumption implies
that either a compact set of pallidal neurons progressively decrease their firing rate as a function of
time before movement onset, therefore gradually disinhibiting the motor thalamus, or an increasing
number of (inhibitory) pallidal cells, controlling the prime movers, are depressed as the movement
unfolds.
VL o = G 0
t
1+t
(4)
Figure 3 shows the mean cumulative distributions of onsets (decreases and increases) for GPi neurons prior to movement onset obtained from a set of neurophysiological studies published between
1978 and 1998 (Table 1) in which information about the timing of internal globus pallidus (GPi)
neurons was available. The plot shows a gradual buildup or decrease of the mean cumulative distribution of neurons whose activations were either enhanced or depressed prior to movement onset. Overall,
increases in GPi activity were predominant (48% and 26% for increases and decreases, respectively). It
is likely that the small percentage of GPi depressions prior to movement onset reflects the focused
activation of prime mover muscles required to produce an arm movement. Interestingly, the mean
cumulative distribution shows an increasing number of GPi neurons that are depressed, which supports
the foregoing model’s assumption.
Computer simulations of the set of equations (1–4) show that smaller-than-normal position and
velocity gains can lead to bradykinesia (Fig. 4). Therefore, it is hypothesized that a function of basal
ganglia networks is to compute these time-varying position and velocity gains. Low gains would
prevent appropriate opening of the pallidal gate leading to poor activation of thalamo-cortical pathways involved in movement, whereas high gains may lead to excess or unstable movement. Interestingly, Suri and colleagues also used a dynamical linear model to model basal ganglia function and
their simulations suggested that low gains in the direct and indirect pathways resulted in PD motor
deficits (26).
In addition, reduced movement amplitudes may be caused by premature resetting of these gains
that prevent movement completion. Indeed, we have shown that smaller-than-normal (in duration
and size) pallido-thalamic signals produced by dopamine depletion in the basal ganglia can reproduce micrographic handwriting (12).
Modeling Abnormal Kinematics During Pointing Movement and Errors in Praxis
in PD and APDs
To study the kinematic aspects of movement in basal ganglia disorders, we have performed computer simulations of the finger-to-nose task and a “bread-slicing gestural task”; the latter task is used
commonly in testing for ideomotor apraxia (38). These two tasks require the integration and transformation of sensory information into motor commands for movement, and therefore successful task
completion depends on accurate spatial and temporal organization of movement.
100
Contreras-Vidal
Fig. 3. Mean cumulative (SE) distributions of activations and depressions of GPi activity obtained from set
of studies in Table 1.
Table 1
Cumulative Distribution of Onsets for GPi Cells
Initial Timing of Discharge (%) Prior to:
–150 ms
Ref
42a
43b
44d
45
46e
47g
48h
49i
Location
C (m. N)
NAc
DL EP
NA
VL
VL (m. 2)
DL EP
DL/VL
Mean %
Total
–100 ms
–50 ms
0 ms
EMG
7
—
22
—
—
—
57
—
0
—
14
—
—
—
16
—
27
—
45
—
10f
19
57
17
0
—
29
—
4
3
16
17
40
2
48
—
—
81
63
25
7
2
31
—
—
14
18
53
53
—
53
38
29
84
66
62
13
—
34
26
12
15
19
91
20
7
37
11
2
37
NA
NA
10.8
3.8
21.9
8.6
32.4
15.6 48.1 26.3
cells
#
0
7
24
14
1
7
NA
NA
15
35
114
36
41
68
28
18
14.3 6.63
—
355
, increase;, decrease; C, centered; DL, dorsolateral; EMG, onset of first muscle activity in prime
movers; EP, entopenduncular nucleus (GPi-like structure in felines); GPi, internal globus pallidus; m., monkey; NA, data not available; VL, ventrolateral. All times with respect to movement onset, except EMG data.
Notes: a biphasic stimulation trains (from Fig. 8), b EMG recordings were made at separate times from the
single-cell recordings; c recording was done from “full extent of GP” and percentages of discharges were
divided equally in increases and decreases; d estimated from Fig. 9 (61%: , 39%: ); e for VisStep task
(71% [29%] GPi increased [decreased] their discharge overall); f estimated from Fig. 5, by counting number of samples during time interval and dividing by the maximum number of conditions (four); g data
estimated from Fig. 6C (85%: , 15%: ); h estimated from Fig. 4 (assumes uniform distributions of 78%
activations in EP); i data estimated from Fig. 3B.
Computer Models of Atypical Parkinsonism
101
Fig. 4. Simulations of normal (thick lines) and Parkinson’s disease (thin lines) movement control. Joint
position (ramp trajectories) and velocity (bell-shaped trajectories) for three different position and velocity gain
settings are shown (α = 10; G0 = 10; normal: Gp = Gv = 1.0; PD1: Gp = Gv = 0.6; PD2: Gp = Gv = 0.2).
Reducing the gain resulted in slower movement (bradykinesia).
Figure 5 depicts a schematic diagram of frontal, parietal, basal ganglia, and cerebellar networks
postulated to be engaged in the learning and updating of sensorimotor transformations for reaching to
visual or proprioceptive targets. The model is based on an extension of the computational models of
Bullock et al. (9) and Burnod et al. (8), and recent imaging and behavioral experiments (50–53) that
suggest distinct roles for fronto-parietal, basal ganglia, and cerebellar networks. In the model, visual
and/or proprioceptive signals about target location and end-effector position are used to code an
internal representation for target (T) and arm (Eff), presumably in posterior parietal cortex (PPC).
These internal representations are compared to compute a spatial difference vector (DVs) as in the
cortical “vector integration model” depicted in Fig. 2B. The DV’s outflow must be transformed into
a joint rotation vector (DVm) in order to guide the end-effector to the target. This spatial direction-tojoint rotation transformation is an inverse kinematic transformation and computationally, it can be
learned through certain amount of simultaneous exposure to patterned proprioceptive and visual
stimulation under conditions of self-produced movement—referred to as “motor babbling” (8,9).
The cortical network just described is complemented by two subcortical networks connected in
two distinct, parallel, lateral loops, namely, a frontostriatal network and a parieto-cerebellar system.
The former provides a learned bias term that rotates the frame of reference for movement, whereas
the latter provides a correction term that is added to the direction vector whenever the actual direction
of movement deviates from the desired one. The frontostriatal network is modeled as an adaptive
search element that performs search and action selection using reinforcement learning (15). This
system uses an explicit error (dopamine) signal to drive the selection and the reinforcement/punishment mechanisms used for learning new procedural skills. It searches and evaluates candidate
visuomotor actions that would lead to acquisition of a spatial target. The cerebellar component is
modeled as an adaptive error-correcting module that continuously provides a compensatory signal to
drive the visuomotor error (provided by the climbing fibers) to zero (7).
102
Contreras-Vidal
Fig. 5. Diagram of hypothesized networks involved in sensorimotor transformation for reaching. See text for
description.
Simulating the Finger-to-Nose Test in Control, PD, and APD
The model summarized in the previous discussion was implemented in Matlab Simulink (following the mathematical equations in refs. 8 and 9) to investigate the effects of PD and APDs in the
spatial and temporal characteristics of repetitive pointing movement to the nose (a kinesthetic target).
Figure 6 depicts the movement trajectories and the joint angles for a simulated arm with three joints
(shoulder, elbow, wrist). The simulation assumed horizontal movements at the nose level, and therefore the paths are shown as two-dimensional plots. As expected, in the virtual control subject, the
finger-to-nose trajectories were slightly curved and showed high spatial and temporal accuracy. The
joint excursions reflected the repetitive nature of the task with most of the movement performed by
elbow flexion/extension followed by the shoulder and to a lesser degree by the wrist.
To simulate PD, the gain of the basal ganglia gating signal was decreased by 30% with respect to
that in the intact system. In addition, Gaussian noise (N(0,1)*30) was added to the internal representation of the hand to account for kinesthetic deficits reported in PD patients (degradation of the
kinesthetic representation of the spatial location of the nose would have had the same effect) (54).
Computer simulations of the PD network showed decreased movement smoothness and increased
spatial and temporal movement variability. Moreover, the joint rotations were slower and progressively decreased in amplitude. Thus, the simulation is consistent with reports of deficits in the production of multiarticulated repetitive movement in PD.
Following the observations of cortical atrophy in frontal and parietal areas in APD summarized in
subheading Neuropathology of the APDs, we modeled this disease in two steps: First, we added noise
(uniform distribution between 0 and 1.5) to the adaptive synaptic weights of the frontoparietal network in the intact (“control”) system; second, we pruned the synaptic weights by randomly setting
50% of the weights to zero; and third, the gain of the basal ganglia gating signal was reduced to 50%
of the control value. Simulation of the APD network model showed that the movement trajectories
Computer Models of Atypical Parkinsonism
103
Fig. 6. Simulations of the finger-to-nose test in normal, PD, and APD models. Movement trajectories and
joint angles are shown. See videoclip for real-time simulation.
were severely affected. The virtual subject showed misreaching to the nose and produced movement
paths that varied widely in space. This was reflected in the joint excursions, which were characterized by noisy and asymmetrical flexion and extension ranges, as well as some periods of joint immobility. Thus, simulation of APD had the largest effect on the spatial and temporal aspects of movement
coordination.
104
Contreras-Vidal
Simulating the Bread-Slicing Gesture in Control, PD, and APD
Recent clinical studies suggest that apraxia may be explained in terms of damage to a frontoparietal network involved in reaching and prehension (38). According to Rothi, ideomotor apraxia is
characterized by “impairment in the timing, sequencing, and spatial organization of gestural movements” (55). Studies involving patients with PD and APDs, including PSP and CBD, indicate that
combined involvement of basal ganglia and cortical networks may underlie the presence of apraxia in
these patients (37,38). Moreover, in a group of studies by Leiguarda and colleagues, they found that
patients with PD, PSP, and CBD, who tested positive for apraxia in the clinical test, showed larger
kinematic abnormalities than those patients who do not show apraxia on clinical examination. However, as discussed initially by Roy, it is still a matter of debate whether these kinematic abnormalities
are a result of an apraxia-like impairment or to basic motor control deficits (40). For example, do the
basic motor control deficits seen in these clinical populations explain the abnormal kinematic deficits
seen in patients with ideomotor apraxia?
In the computer simulations that follow we focus on production errors, which are associated with
deficits in the programming and execution of gestural movements, like the bread-slicing task. Simulations of higher-order content errors would require modeling of frontal areas related to ideational or
conceptual processing, which are not included in the current model. In the model simulations, it is
assumed that the planning and control components are disturbed because of the pathology in frontoparietal networks.
Figure 7 shows the performance in the ‘bread slicing’ task in a normal virtual subject (Control),
and after simulated PD and APD. Both diseases were simulated as in subheading Modeling
Hypokinetic Disorders in PD. In the control simulation the slicing gesture, depicted as a continuous
dark line, reflected a tight, curvilinear trajectory, which was primarily a result of the repetitive, periodic, shoulder rotations. Elbow and wrist joint angles also showed sustained, periodic patterns albeit
of smaller amplitude. Simulation of the parkinsonian network resulted in reduced spatial and temporal accuracy of the slicing movement. This was accompanied by slower and asymmetrical joint oscillations that were maximal for the shoulder joint, but considerably reduced for the wrist joint. In fact,
as the slicing gesture was generated, the wrist joint was progressively rotated from ~18° to ~38° (see
geometry of the arm in top panel of Fig. 7). Simulation of PSP resulted in large production errors as
evidenced by distorted spatial trajectories, some of which left the spatial area for the simulated bread
surface. The profiles of the joint angles also resemble those reported in recent studies (see e.g., Fig. 5
in ref. 38). For example, the angular changes of virtual patients with PD and APDs are smaller,
irregular, and distorted compared to the simulated control subject.
CONCLUSIONS
Models of basal ganglia disorders have been advanced in the last two decades. These models have
mostly focused on PD, in part because of the computational burden of simulating large-scale models
that include multiple brain areas. However, recent computational work on frontal, parietal, cerebellar, and basal ganglia dynamics involved in sensorimotor transformations for reaching provide a
window to study widespread pathologies as those seen in APDs. These models can inform the experimentalist about the various potential sources of movement variability in various types of tasks (simple
vs sequential, pantomime vs real tool use, kinesthetic vs visual cueing, etc.). Moreover, simulated
lesions and interventions can be used to test hypotheses and guide new experiments.
For example, we modeled the pharmacokinetics and the pharmacodynamics using a neural network model parameterized by the measurements of the handwriting kinematics of PD patients (12).
Other mathematical treatments of the dose–effect relationship of levodopa and motor behavior treat
the motor control system as a black-box system and do not provide insights on the mechanisms,
although they may still be useful for therapy optimization purposes (56). On the other hand, a biologically inspired, albeit highly simplified neural network model of cortico-basal ganglia dynamics
Computer Models of Atypical Parkinsonism
105
Fig. 7. Simulations of the bread-slicing gesture commonly used to test ideomotor apraxia in basal ganglia
disorders. Simulations show movement trajectories and joint angles (shoulder, elbow, and wrist) for normal,
PD, and APD simulations. See videoclip on accompanying DVD for real-time simulation.
during normal and neurological conditions would be valuable to assess which parameters might be most
amenable to therapeutic intervention, including individual optimization of pharmacological therapy,
while at the same time informing about abnormal mechanisms.
Limitations of Current Models of Basal Ganglia Disorders
APDs are caused by various diseases, therefore involving a complex neuropathophysiology and a
wide spectrum of abnormal behavioral signs. Currently, there is no mathematical or conceptual model
that can explain all the symptoms based on abnormal mechanisms within the basal ganglia, cerebellum, and/or cortical areas. This limitation is partially because of the necessary simplification of the
neurobiology required to make the model computationally or mathematically tractable for formal
investigation. Thus, it is likely that some aspect of any proposed model may be wrong; however, the
likelihood of this happening can be minimized by developing a step-by-step reconstruction of the
system under study, with each step required to account for additional sources of neural or motor
106
Contreras-Vidal
variability. Moreover, manipulations of model components, such as simulated lesions or deep brain
stimulation paradigms, are also oversimplifications of the processes occurring in the brain in response
to such events.
We hope that computational models of basal ganglia disorders will be developed further to help
elucidate and account for the complexity of the clinical symptoms seen in PD and APDs, in such a
way that these models can guide further theoretical and applied research and foster interactions
between clinicians, modelers, and neuroscientists.
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
A controversial question in apraxia research is the relevance of ideomotor apraxia for real-life
action (57). Earlier reports suggested that patients with apraxia may use single objects appropriately
even when they are unable to pantomime their use (58,59), whereas other have found the same types
of spatiotemporal errors in both object pantomime and during tool use (58) or reported that subjects
with ideomotor apraxia, assessed by gesture pantomime, had more errors with tools while eating than
matched controls (61). The large-scale neural network presented in this chapter may help to elucidate
any potential relationship of ideomotor apraxia to skilled tool use in naturalistic situations. Extension
of the large-scale neural network model to include visual and tactile signals produced by the sight and
on-line interaction with the object would be critical to answer this question. Another important direction for future work relates to the integration of stored movement primitives (so-called gesture engrams) and dynamic movement features required to produce and distinguish a given gesture from
others. Although the simulations of the “slicing” gesture in anapraxic neural network model presented in this chapter were based on degraded sensorimotor transformations for movement, it should
also be possible to evaluate the effects of disruptions in the on-line sequential (dynamic) processes
underlying the repetitive nature of the slicing task, which were intact in the present simulations.
Finally, the hypothetical effects of neuromotor noise in different model components could also be
assessed through neural network simulations.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The author thanks Shihua Wen, University of Maryland’s Department of Mathematics, for running the simulations shown in Figs. 6 and 7. The author’s computational work summarized herein has
been supported in part by INSERM and the National Institutes of Health.
FIGURES
Figures were generated in Matlab and filtered using Abobe Illustrator.
MEDIA
Procedure Used to Generate the MPEG Files:
In the CD disk, there are six mpeg files, the file names represent the simulated tasks, which are
described below. The frames in format .avi were generated by the computational model written in
MatLab's Simulink. The .avi files are generated by a shareware named "HyperCam" produced by
Hypererionics Technology (www.hperionics.com). The mpeg files were obtained using the freeward
softward "avi2meg" written by USH (www.ush.de). All the movies are recorded at a rate of 30 frames
per second.
Description of Movies:
Finger-to-nose Control (duration: 1:48): Simulation shows the end-pont trajectories during the
finger-to-nose task. A three segments arm is shown. These segments are linked by the shoulder,
elbow, and wrist joints. This simulation of an intact (Control) network shows almost linear trajectories.
Computer Models of Atypical Parkinsonism
107
Finger-to-nose PD: This movie shows the performance of the computer model after simulating
severe PD. In this simulation, the movement becomes slower, discrete, and noisy. Moreover, some of
the targeted movements do not reach the nose.
Finger-to-nose APD: This movie shows the finger-to-nose simulations after damage to the frontoparietal network involved in sensorimotor trnsformations for reaching, which simulated APD. The
end-point trajectories become irregular, coarse, fragmented, and dyscoordinated.
Slicing Control: This clip shows a simulation of the "bread slicing" gesture as seen from directly
above. The rectangle represents the loaf of bread. Note that there were not constrains on the way the
virtual arm was supposed to produce the gesture. Note that during the repetitive movement the wrist
joint was initially slightly flexed, but gradually became extended. This illustrates the fact that in a
redundant arm there are many possible arm configurations that can be used to move the end point to
a spatial target or along a given spatial trajectory.
Slicing PD: The PD simulation shows reduced range of motion for all joints, discontinuous movements, and a difficulty in generating the repetitive slicing gesture. In this particular simulation, the
wrist is in the extended position from the onset of the movement.
Slicing APD: This video shows the slicing gesture task after simulated APD. This simulation
shows production errors as the arm fails to move the end point along the desired slicing trajectory.
This resulted in highly disrupted spatial and temporal organization.
REFERENCES
1. Beiser DG, Hua SE, Houk JC. Network models of the basal ganglia. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1997;7:185–190.
2. Ruppin E, Reggia JA, Glanzman D. Understanding brain and cognitive disorders: the computational perspective. Prog
Brain Res 1999;121:ix–xv.
3. Gillies A, Arbuthnott G. Computational models of the basal ganglia. Mov Disord 2000;15:762–770.
4. Amos A. A computational model of information processing in the frontal cortex and basal ganglia. J Cogn Neurosci
2000;12:505–519.
5. Lorincz A. Static and dynamic state feedback control model of basal ganglia-thalamocortical loops. Int J Neural Syst
1997;8:339–357.
6. Wickens JR, Kotter R, Alexander ME. Effects of local connectivity on striatal function: stimulation and analysis of a
model. Synapse 1995;20:281–298.
7. Contreras-Vidal JL, Grossberg S, Bullock D. A neural model of cerebellar learning for arm movement control: corticospino-cerebellar dynamics. Learn Mem 1997;3:475–502.
8. Burnod Y, Grandguillaume P, Otto I, Ferraina S, Johnson PB, Caminiti R. Visuomotor transformations underlying arm
movements toward visual targets: a neural network model of cerebral cortical operations. J Neurosci 1992;12:1435–1453.
9. Bullock D, Grossberg S, Guenther FH. A self-organizing neural model of motor equivalent reaching and tool use by a
multijoint arm. J Cogn Neurosci 1993;5:408–435.
10. Tagamets MA, Horwitz B. Interpreting PET and fMRI measures of functional neural activity: the effects of synaptic
inhibition on cortical activation in human imaging studies. Brain Res Bull 2001;54:267–273.
11. Horwitz B, Poeppel D. How can EEG/MEG and fMRI/PET data be combined? Hum Brain Mapp 2002;17:1–3.
12. Contreras-Vidal JL, Poluha P, Teulings HL, Stelmach GE. Neural dynamics of short and medium-term motor control
effects of levodopa therapy in Parkinson’s disease. Artif Int Med 1998;13:57–79.
13. Bower JM. Modeling the nervous system. TINS 1992;15:411–412.
14. Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague R. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 1997;275:1593–1599.
15. Contreras-Vidal JL, Schultz W. A predictive reinforcement model of dopamine neurons for learning approach behavior. J Comput Neurosci 1999;6:191–214.
16. Nakahara H, Doya K, Hikosaka O. Parallel cortico-basal ganglia mechanisms for acquisition and execution of
visuomotor sequences—A computational approach. J Cogn Neurosci 2001;13:626–647.
17. Suri R, Schultz W. Learning of sequential movements by neural network model with dopamine-like reinforcement
signal. Exp Brain Res 1998;121:350–354.
18. Contreras-Vidal, JL. The gating functions of the basal ganglia in movement control. In: JA Reggia, E Ruppin, DL
Glanzman (eds.). Progress in Brain Research. Disorders of Brain, Behavior and Cognition: the Neurocomputational
Perspective. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1999:261–276.
19. Humphries MD, Gurney KN. The role of intra-thalamic and thalamocortical circuits in action selection. Network
2002;13:131–156.
108
Contreras-Vidal
20. Gurney K, Prescott TJ, Redgrave P. A computational model of action selection in the basal ganglia. II. Analysis and
simulation of behaviour. Biol Cybern 2001;84:411–423.
21. Beiser D, Houk J. Model of cortical-basal ganglia ganglionic processing: encoding the serial order of sensory events.
J Neurophysiol 1998;79:3168–3188.
22. Fukai T. Sequence generation in arbitrary temporal patterns from theta-nested gamma oscillations: a model of the basal
ganglia-thalamo-cortical loops. Neural Netw 1999;12:975–987.
23. Berns GS, Sejnowski TJ. A computational model of how the basal ganglia produce sequences. J Cogn Neurosci
1998;10:108–121.
24. Contreras-Vidal JL, Stelmach GE. A neural model of basal ganglia–thalamocortical relations in normal and Parkinsonian movement. Biol Cybern 1995;73:467–476.
25. Connolly CI, Burns JB, Jog MS. A dynamical-systems model for Parkinson’s disease. Biol Cybern 2000;83:47–59.
26. Suri RE, Albani C, Glattfelder AH. A dynamic model of motor basal ganglia functions. Biol Cybern 1997;76:451–458.
27. Borrett DS, Yeap TH, Kwan HC. Neural networks and Parkinson’s disease. Can J Neurol Sci 1993;20:107–113.
28. Litvan I. Recent advances in atypical parkinsonian disorders. Curr Opin Neurol 1999;12:441–446.
29. Yekhlef F, Ballan G, Macia F, Delmer O, Sourgen C, Tison F. Routine MRI for the differential diagnosis of Parkinson’s
disease, MSA, PSP, and CBD. J Neural Transm 2003;110:151–169.
30. Schrag A, Kingsley D, Phatouros C, et al. Clinical usefulness of magnetic resonance imaging in multiple system atrophy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1998;65:65–71.
31. Schrag A, Good CD, Miszkiel K, et al. Differentiation of atypical parkinsonian syndromes with routine MRI. Neurology 2000;54:697–702.
32. Kraft E, Schwarz J, Trenkwalder C, Vogl T, Pfluger T, Oertel WH. The combination of hypointense and hyperintense
signal changes on T2-weighted magnetic resonance maging sequences: a specific marker of multiple system atrophy?
Arch Neurol 1999;56:225–228.
33. Savoiardo M, Strada L, Girotti F, Zimmerman RA, Grisoli M, Testa D, Petrillo R. Olivopontocerebellar atrophy: MR
diagnosis and relationship to multisystem atrophy. Radiology 1990;174:693–669.
34. Savoiardo M, Grisoli M, Girotti F, Testa D, Caraceni T. MRI in sporadic olivopontocerebellar atrophy and striatonigral
degeneration. Neurology 1997;48:790–792.
35. Horimoto Y, Aiba I, Yasuda T, et al.Cerebral atrophy in multiple system atrophy by MRI. J Neurol Sci 2000;173:109–112.
36. Fahn S, Green PE, Ford B, Bressman SB. Handbook of Movement Disorders. London: Blackwell Science, 1997.
37. Leiguarda RC, Marsden CD. Limb apraxias: higher-order disorders of sensorimotor integration. Brain 2000;123:860–79.
38. Leiguarda R, Merello M, Balej J, Starkstein S, Nogues M, Marsden CD. Disruption of spatial organization and interjoint
coordination in Parkinson’s disease, progressive supranuclear palsy, and multiple system atrophy. Mov Disord
2000;15:627–640.
39. Litvan I. Progressive supranuclear palsy and corticobasal degeneration. Baillieres Clin Neurol 1997;6:167–185.
40. Roy EA. Apraxia in diseases of the basal ganglia. Mov Disord 2000;15:598–600.
41. Bullock D, Grossberg S. Neural dynamics of planned arm movements: emergent invariants and speed-accuracy properties during trajectory formation. Psychol Rev 1988;95:49–90.
42. Anderson ME, Horak FB. Influence of the globus pallidus on arm movements in monkeys. III. Timing of movementrelated information. J Neurophysiol 1985;54:433–448.
43. Brotchie P, Iansek R, Horne MK. Motor function of the monkey globus pallidus. 1. Neuronal discharge and parameters
of movement. Brain. 1991;114:1667–1683.
44. Cheruel F, Dormont JF, Amalric M, Schmied A, Farin D. The role of putamen and pallidum in motor initiation in the
cat. I. Timing of movement-related single-unit activity. Exp Brain Res 1994;100:250–266.
45. Georgopoulos AP, DeLong MR, Crutcher MD. Relations between parameters of step-tracking movements and single
cell discharge in the globus pallidus and subthalamic nucleus of the behaving monkey. J Neurosci 1983;3:1586–1598.
46. Mink JW, Thach WT. Basal ganglia motor control. I. Nonexclusive relation of pallidal discharge to five movement
modes. J Neurophysiol 1991;65:273–300.
47. Nambu A, Yoshida S, Jinnai K. Movement-related activity of thalamic neurons with input from the globus pallidus and
projection to the motor cortex in the monkey. Exp Brain Res 1991;84:279–284.
48. Neafsey EJ, Hull CD, Buchwald NA. Preparation for movement in the cat. II. Unit activity in the basal ganglia and
thalamus. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1978;44:714–723.
49. Turner RS, Anderson ME. Pallidal discharge related to the kinematics of reaching movements in two dimensions.
J Neurophysiol 1997;77:1051–1074.
50. Contreras-Vidal JL, Buch ER. Effects of Parkinson’s disease on visuomotor adaptation. Exp Brain Res 2003;150:25–32.
51. Buch ER, Young S, Contreras-Vidal JL. Visuomotor adaptation in normal aging. Learn Mem 2003;10:55–63.
52. Inoue K, Kawashima R, Satoh K, et al. A PET study of visuomotor learning under optical rotation. Neuroimage
2000;11:505–516.
53. Balslev D, Nielsen FA, Frutiger SA, et al. Cluster analysis of activity-time series in motor learning. Hum Brain Mapp
2002;15:135–45.
Computer Models of Atypical Parkinsonism
109
54. Klockgether T, Borutta M, Rapp H, Spieker S, Dichgans J. A defect of kinesthesia in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord
1995;10:460–465.
55. Rothi LJG, Ochipa C, Heilman KM. A cognitive neuropsychological model of limb praxis. Cogn Neuropsychol
1991;8:443–458.
56. Hacisalihzade SS, Mansour M, Albani C. Optimization of symptomatic therapy in Parkinson’s disease. IEEE Trans
Biomed Eng 1989;36:363–372.
57. Buxbaum LJ. Ideomotor apraxia: A call to Action. Neurocase 2001;7:445–458.
58. Poizner H, Mack L, Verfaellie M, Rothi LJ, Heilman KM. Three-dimensional computer graphic analysis of apraxia.
Neural representations of learned movement. Brain 1990;113:85–101.
59. Liepmann H. The left hemisphere and action. London, Ontario: University of Western Ontario, 1905.
60. De Rensi E, Motti F, Nichelli P. Imitating gestures. A quantitative approach to ideomotor apraxia. Arch Neurol
1980;37:6–10.
61. Foundas AL, Macauley BL, Raymer AM, Maher LM, Heilman KM, Gonzalez Rothi, LJ. Ecological implications of
limb apraxia: evidence from mealtime behavior. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1995;1:62–66.
Download