Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 1 Middle School Students’ Experiences with Symbolism Introduction to the study Background Whenever we think something and express or communicate about it, we need some tools. In doing algebra, symbols provide one such tool with which we can think and represent our thoughts and ideas. Not only are symbols a tool for representation, they have also played a critical role in developing algebra. If we consider generality as what makes algebra most different from arithmetic, the beginning of algebra is historically traced back to ancient Mesopotamia and Egypt. In spite of almost four thousand years of history of algebra, the history of symbols had not begun until the 16th century. Before then, algebraic ideas were stated rhetorically, and special words, abbreviations, and number symbols were used as notations. It was Vieta who used symbols purposefully and systematically after some mathematical symbols (e.g. , , ) were introduced with letters used for unknowns (Kline, 1972). Since Vieta, algebra has rapidly developed from a science of generalized numerical computations, to a science of universal computations and then into a science of abstract structures thanks to symbolism (Sfard, 1995). However, symbolism is one of the major difficulties for young students in learning algebra even though symbolism made it possible to study abstract structures in algebra by expressing complicated mathematical ideas succinctly. Hiebert et al. (1997) explained that the difficulties in dealing with symbols as a learning tool were attributed to the fact that “meaning is not inherent” in symbols (p. 55). They insisted that meaning is not attached to symbols automatically and without meaning symbols could not be used effectively. So students should construct meaning for and with symbols as they actively Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 2 use them. The National Council of Teachers of Mathematics’ (2000) Algebra Standard also encouraged using symbols as a tool to represent and analyze mathematical situations and structures in all grade levels. Specifically, students in Grades 6 – 8 are recommended to have … extensive experience in interpreting relationships among quantities in a variety of problem contexts before they can work meaningfully with variables and symbolic expressions. An understanding of the meanings and uses of variables develops gradually as students create and use symbolic expressions and relate them to verbal, tabular, and graphical representations. Relationships among quantities can often be expressed symbolically in more than one way, providing opportunities for students to examine the equivalence of various algebraic expressions (p. 225-226). In this recommendation, NCTM put emphasis on using problem contexts to help students develop meaning for symbols and appreciate quantitative relationships. In line with the issues mentioned above, the present study is intended to provide insight into students’ experiences with symbolism. In particular, the educational purpose of this study is to inform mathematics educators of how students construct meaning of symbols and learn mathematical concepts with symbols so that mathematics educators can enhance students’ learning of mathematics with symbols. Research questions In the abstract development of algebra with systematic symbolism, Wheeler (1989) argued that abstract algebra sacrificed the implicit meanings for its applicability Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 3 unlike rhetorical and syncopated algebra (cited in Kieran, 1992). For instance, Diophantus, as a syncopated algebraist, created numeral expressions like 10 – x and 10 + x and multiplied them to get 100 – x2 as if they were numbers like 2 and 3 to solve his word problem. As he denoted the letter x as an unknown but fixed value in the context of the problem, he could keep the meaning of x for its applicability explicitly. However, he might have not obtained the notion of variables, which abstract algebra achieved (Sfard & Linchevski, 1994). As Kieran (1992) elaborated, symbolic language made algebra more powerful and applicable by eliminating “many of the distinctions that the vernacular preserves” and inducing the essences (p. 394). However, the powerful yet decontextualized language brought difficulties for young students who were beginning to learn algebra: Thus, the cognitive demands placed on algebra students included, on the one hand, treating symbolic representations, which have little or no semantic content, as mathematical objects and operating upon these objects with processes that usually do not yield numerical solutions, and, on the other hand, modifying their former interpretations of certain symbols and beginning to represent the relationships of word-problem situations with operations that are often the inverse of those that they used almost automatically for solving similar problems in arithmetic (Kieran, 1992, p. 394). In fact, many researchers (e.g. Kieran, 1992) have studied students’ difficulties in manipulating symbols as mathematical objects and modifying their interpretations of symbols. Also some studies (e.g. Stacey & MacGregor, 1997) were conducted to investigate how meaning for symbols could be developed. Hiebert and Carpenter (1992) Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 4 reviewed such literature and summarized that making meaning for symbols could develop connections between symbols and other representation forms. In analyzing two primary functions of symbols of a public function and a private function, they insisted that connections between symbols were required for a public function, involved in representing something known already for communication, and connections with other representation forms were necessary for a private function, involved in organizing and manipulating ideas as objects (p. 73-74). This analysis led to my curiosity about early algebraic students’ experiences with symbolism. My initial curiosity included sporadic questions like; how do young students interpret mathematical symbols?, in what ways do they use symbols?, do they feel the need of symbols?, what do they want to represent with symbols?, in what ways do their understanding of symbols affect learning mathematical concepts?, and so on. Inspired by these questions, the present study will investigate how middle school students develop algebraic reasoning with symbols while doing mathematical activities. The following questions will guide this study: 1. How do students make sense of symbols used in mathematical activities? 2. How do students’ mathematical concepts develop and evolve as they use symbols throughout mathematical activities? I presume that students’ prevalent experiences with symbolism occur in classroom learning situations and the learning experience includes teacher’s lecture, reading mathematics books, doing hands-on activities, observing how teacher and other students use symbols, and discussion with other students. Under these circumstances, we can never simply assume that students understand symbols in the way that each activity means to provide. So I would like to investigate how students make sense of symbols Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 5 used in classroom activities through the first question. The second question focuses on students’ understandings of mathematical concepts that certain symbols are intended to represent. For instance, y = mx + b is a symbol as a whole or a symbolic representation that describes linear relationship between two variables of x and y. Also m and b are symbols for the slope and the y-intercept in the linear relationship, respectively. Embedded mathematical ideas in the symbol or symbolic representation are what students need to learn ultimately. Since I believe that students’ understanding of mathematical concepts cannot occur once and for all, I expect students’ understanding of certain mathematical ideas to evolve throughout various activities. So I would like to investigate how students’ mathematical concepts develop as they engage in mathematical activities. Theoretical framework This section includes theoretical perspectives with which the present study will be guided. The first part is allotted to my perspectives on learning mathematics in general, and the second part is to show briefly what theoretical perspectives on symbolism were considered that eventually lead to the adoption of the procept model (Tall et al., 2001). Perspectives on learning Perspectives on learning concern statements of subjectivity, that will inform and affect all the activities of the present study in general, rather than establish a theoretical framework. In particular, this section mainly includes my current personal beliefs formed through learning and teaching mathematics and studying mathematics education as a graduate student. Since the beliefs will provide lenses and constraints with which I design the present study and interpret all possible phenomena the study will bring, I Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 6 believe that it is worthwhile to state here. My perspectives on learning are quite parallel to what radical constructivists assume as underlying principles: (1) knowledge is not passively received but built up by the cognizing subject, and (2) the function of cognition is adaptive and serves the organization of the experiential world, not the discovery of ontological reality (von Glasersfeld, 1995, p. 18). Although the way I interpret the principles might not be as identical as most radical constructivists presume, these inform my view of mathematics learning. What I believe about learning mathematics are: A learner does not receive mathematical knowledge passively. A learner constructs his own knowledge. Learning occurs through experiences. Knowledge is primarily personal. As an elaboration, I believe that a learner does not receive mathematical knowledge passively. Even without taking a constructivist perspective that a learner actively constructs mathematical knowledge, I have seen evidences of my belief. For example, I had learned mathematics via lectures and fortunately most of my mathematics teachers helped me understand mathematical concepts. When I discussed or worked on problems with my classmates who had shown similar mathematical abilities and performances, I could find differences in how we understood a certain topic and strategies we used. If we were receivers of knowledge, we should barely find differences in knowledge or understanding of it. I think the differences came from what we did in our own mind. My second belief is that a learner constructs his own knowledge. What I mean by “construct” is not necessarily the same as constructivist perspective. Construction means neither invention like what professional mathematicians do nor isolated construction without Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 7 help. Back in my example aforementioned, I believe that my friends and I constructed our own mathematical knowledge mainly with the teacher’s help. We could not absorb what the teacher told us, but instead each of us tried to make sense of mathematics around us. In other words, we made sense of all mathematics from previous learning, textbook, everyday life experiences, communication with classmates, and teacher’s explanation in order to fit all of them together. Third, I believe that learning occurs through experiences. What I mean by experiences is mostly the learner’s interaction with his/her environment, and examples of experiences related to learning include learning activities, reading books, communicating with others, listening to teachers, reflecting and so on. Finally, I believe that knowledge is primarily personal. In fact, mathematical knowledge definitely had social and cultural aspects not only because mathematics has been built historically and culturally throughout a long period of time but also because most of the mathematical experiences we have occur in school settings with a teacher and a class of students. Although we develop mathematical knowledge or meaning as a community in classroom, it is ultimately the learner who makes choices of whether the developed knowledge will be included meaningfully into his or her knowledge structure and whether the knowledge will be actively used for mathematical activities like solving problems. Perspectives on symbolism In order to study students’ experiences with symbolism, the present study needs to decide which symbols will be focused on and how to define them. Cobb (2000) seemed to define the term symbol broadly such as: Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 8 …to denote any situation in which a concrete entity such as a mark on paper, an icon on a computer screen, or an arrangement of physical materials is interpreted as standing for or signifying something else (p. 17). Following his definition, I could use two distinct erasers pretending they were cars in order to explain a traffic accident that I experienced. By moving the two erasers, I could explain how my car was hit by the other. Here the erasers were symbols since they were concrete entities in the accident situation and stood for cars. In addition, Janvier et al. (1992) differentiated the erasers and the car that the erasers stood for as “signifier or referent” and “signified or referenced”, respectively. Unlike symbols, the signified or referenced (what symbols stand for) is not limited to a concrete thing. It could be an action, an idea or a concept, depending on context. Although accepting Cobb’s (2000) notion of symbols and the concepts of signifier (or referent) and signified (referenced) by Janvier et al. (1992), the present study will focus on standard mathematical written symbols since mathematical activities considered in this study will happen in classroom contexts. Unlike the previous example of everyday-life symbols, it is not always clear to tell what mathematical written symbols stand for. For example, a fraction 3/4 is a mathematical symbol, or signifier, but its signified is not as clear as that of the erasers in the previous example. In various contexts, 3/4 could stand for a fair share of sharing 3 apples among 4 children, a ratio of 3 out of 4, or an operator as in 3/4 of something. Even when removing a specific context, 3/4 still could refer to the abstract concept of fraction. This example shows two faces of symbols as process and concept. It seems the frequent case that symbols as a vehicle to signify process are prioritized, and after the process is familiarized enough they can be used as Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 9 objects carrying concepts. Mason’s (1980) spiral model also explained the shift of symbol uses as: from confidently manipulable objects/symbols, through their use to gain a ‘sense of’ some idea involving a full range of imagery but at an inarticulate level, through a symbolic record of that sense, to a confidently manipulable use of the new symbols, and so on in a continuing spiral (cited in Mason, 1987, p. 74-75). In particular, Mason (1980) not only explained the shift of symbol uses between as process and as concept but also showed the continuous acquisitions of new symbol uses with confidently usable symbols. However, both the dichotomous uses of symbols and the acquisition of symbols as concept after process seem artificial since I believe process and concept are mingled together so that the demarcation is not clear and they grow together. Thus, I believe, building on Mason’s (1980) spiral model, Gray and Tall (1991, cited in Tall et al., 2001, p5.) introduced the notion of procept. Tall et al. (2001) elaborated as: It [procept] is now seen mainly as a cognitive construct, in which the symbol can act as a pivot, switching from a focus on process to compute or manipulate, to a concept that may be thought about as a manipulable entity. We believe that procepts are at the root of human ability to manipulate mathematical ideas in arithmetic, algebra and other theories involving manipulable symbols. They allow the biological brain to switch effortlessly from doing a process to thinking about a concept in a minimal way (p. 5). Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 10 Therefore, rather than studying whether symbols are used as process or concept, they considered a powerful way of using symbols to switch between process and concept flexibly. Then they developed three different performance levels showing how students used symbols. They are the procept, process and procedure levels. To distinguish procedure and process, they meant procedure as “a specific sequence of steps carried out a step at a time” and process as “in a more general sense any number of procedures which essentially have the same effect” (Tall et al., 2001, p. 7). In order to show what is expected at each level, the following figure from Tall et al. (2001, p.8) is provided: Spectrum of outcomes procedural To DO routine mathematics accurately proceptual To perform mathematics flexibly & efficiently To THINK about mathematics symbolically Procept Process (es) Procedures Progress Process Procedures Procedure Sophistication of development Figure 1: A spectrum of performances in the carrying out of mathematical processes (Tall et al., 2001, p.88) Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 11 In progressing up to the proceptual level, students can choose the most suitable process under a given situation having more options and then “Being able to think about the symbolism as an entity allows it to be manipulated itself, to think about mathematics in a compressed and manipulable way, moving easily between process and concept” (Tall et al., 2001, p. 88). So this model will guide the present study theoretically so that through this model I can see students’ experiences with symbolism throughout their mathematical activities. Methods The present study will be conducted within the activities of a NSF-funded project, Coordinating Students’ and Teachers’ Algebraic Reasoning (CoSTAR). The project purposely studies “teachers’ and students’ understandings of shared classroom interactions and ways that teachers and students work together to shape the teaching and learning of middle-school algebra” (Izsak et al., 2002, p. 2). Whereas CoSTAR investigates both teachers’ and students’ algebraic reasoning, the present study will be conducted from only students’ perspectives, so that some activities (such as classroom interactions) that will be approached through both teachers’ and students’ perspectives in CoSTAR, will be interpreted only from the viewpoint of the students in this study. Participants in the present study will be selected from students in a rural middle school that provides the research site for CoSTAR. It is anticipated that participants of the present study will be students in grade 7 in the fall of 2003 and they will begin to learn algebra with materials from the Connected Mathematics Project (CMP; Lappan et al., 2002). Students in grade 7 will study 6 to 8 units throughout the whole academic year, Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 12 but the first unit, “Variables and Patterns: Introducing Algebra”, will be the mathematical content of the present study. In the unit, students will learn how to represent a changing situation in different ways. Eventually, students will describe patterns of change relating one variable to another verbally, in tabular form, graphically, and symbolically. As the data collection method, a teaching experiment will be adopted since the goal of the present study fits with this method. According to Steffe and Thompson. (2000), researchers through teaching experiments aim to construct models of students’ mathematics and so they supposedly look behind what students say and do in order to understand their mathematical realities (p. 269–270). In the teaching experiment, I will play a role of teacher-researcher, who will try to understand students’ experiences with symbolism and to build models of the students’ symbolic activity with the notion of procept (Tall et al., 2001). Participating students will work in pairs in the teaching experiment since they can provoke each other’s thinking through interactions and by reflecting on their own thinking relative to that of the other. Two pairs of students are expected to participate in the present study. As a personnel element of teaching experiments, an observer will support the teacher-researcher providing feedback from planning each teaching episode to the whole process of the teaching experiment. Tasks in the teaching experiment will be designed to understand students’ symbolism and also to extend their experiences with symbolism based on what they do during the class activities and their written performances. Each teaching episode will be video-recorded with two cameras, which will provide a whole picture of interactions among the teacher-researcher and a pair of students and a focused view of students’ work. To generate the primary data source, the Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 13 views from both cameras will be synchronized and digitized. Then the video data will be analyzed iteratively. Informed by iterative videotape analyses (Lesh & Lehrer, 2000), data analyses in the present study will go through several interpretation cycles. The first interpretation cycle will include debriefing from on-the-scene observers right after each teaching episode. Notes and feedback from both the teacher-researcher and an observer will be included. The second interpretation cycle will produce observation notes as replaying videotape of a teaching episode before conducting the next episode. The third cycle will produce written transcripts for each teaching episode. The last interpretation cycle will produce analyses of all the teaching episodes for each pair of students and across the pairs. Unlike the three previous interpretation cycles, the fourth cycle will be repeated to produce interpretations of students’ activities from the theoretical perspectives of the present study. Analysis of Students interviews As a part of the activities of the project CoSTAR, I had three interview sessions with a pair of students, Ansley and Aisha (pseudonyms), who were in grade 6 in the spring of 2003. The major mathematical content was the operations of fractions. In the first interview session, they were mainly asked about adding and subtracting fractions with number line representations and fraction strips. They had learned number line representations of addition and subtraction in class just before the interview. Also fraction strips were used when introducing fractions and when estimating fractional sums and differences. In the second interview session, students were questioned about what they did on the test taken about a week before. In the last interview session, the main task was multiplying fractions in the form of ‘fraction of fraction’ with an area model. In Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 14 order to increase an accuracy of equal partitioning and help students’ thinking, the computer software, “Fraction Bars”, was provided for students in the third interview session. Although the time constraint did not allow me to analyze the students interview data through the complete interpretation cycles described in the above section, I could find a couple of themes about students’ symbolism across the interview data. Below I will present those assertions that I made through the data analysis. First, students appeared to prioritize procedures with which they processed a certain task. Although they had learned various procedures for a task, they did not seem to consider the multiple procedures as equal choices. Instead, they would rather rank the procedures and resort to the best procedure that they picked. For addition and subtraction of fractions, students had learned at least three different methods such as quantitative reasoning with fractions strips, using number line representations, and using common denominators. However, they put the priority on the method of using common denominators. When I had the first interview session, I asked students to add and subtract fractions with number line representations. Also fraction strips were available for students to partition a number line and draw a certain length of segment. When asked to solve 1/5 + 2/5 on a number line, Aisha drew a 1/5-long-segment and a 2/5-long segment using the fifth fraction strip and she answered 3/5 instantly saying, “because 2 plus 1 is 3 and you just put 5 instead of adding”. For the question of 1/2 + 1/3, she drew a 3/6-long-segment and a 2/6-long segment with the sixths fraction bar instead of using the halves strip and the third strip and found the answer by adding numerators. Seemingly, Aisha was confident with adding and subtracting fractions using common Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 15 denominators and considered number line representation as a visual form to show the procedure rather than another procedure of operation. As converting improper fractions into proper ones, they could process through division, subtraction, reasoning quantitatively, and using multiplication facts, but they thought that the division method was the most powerful. Even when converting 7/6, both students divided 7 by 6 to obtain 1 1/6. Asked whether they had another way to convert, Aisha told that she could use subtraction, but she added soon that subtraction method did not always work. She appeared to think that she could only use subtraction method when an improper fraction is less than 2 rather than considering a repeated subtraction for an improper fractions bigger than 2. Then I reminded them of another method as showing a video clip where their teacher drew a picture of 7/6 as one whole and 1/6. For the picture, Aisha explained to Ansley that she knew 7/6 is more than 1 and so she had a leftover of 1/6. However, her explanation was not quite quantitative reasoning such as 7/6 means seven 1/6s and because six 1/6s make a whole 7/6 is the same as one whole and a 1/6, that is, 1 1/6. So Dr. Olive provided a problem of 1/6 oz. bags of spice and asked a series of questions about how many bags were needed to make 1/2 oz., 5/6 oz., and 7/6 oz. And he finally asked how much of ounces seven 1/6-ounce spice bags were. After answering correctly, Ansley said, “I think that one is easier than the other ways”, but she continued, “it is easier to understand but division is quicker”. Then I showed Ansley’s conversion of 4 and 13/10 into 6 and 1/10 using division on her test (see Picture 1.). Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 16 Picture 1: Ansley’s conversion of 4 and 13/10 She did a long division of 13 divided by 10 correctly and answered 6 and 1/10 mistakenly. She told in the interview that she did not know why she did so. This might show her that division method is not as easy as some other methods. However, both students did not change their preference of division method for conversion even after admitting another method was easier to understand and the burden of long division. Second, both students showed a strong preference of symbolic manipulations for operating with fractions. So when they were required to use other procedures, they tended to make sense of them and process them by referring to their results that they obtained through symbolic manipulations. In the second interview, I asked Aisha to explain her number line representation of 3/5 – 5/10 on the test. Her written work showed a correct symbolic manipulation as below but her number line representation was not supportive (see Picture 2.). Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 17 Picture 2: Aisha’s number line representation of 3/5 – 5/10 Based on her comment, “I solve first and draw it, draw it backward”, I inferred that she found the answer 1/10 first and tried to represent involving fractions of 3/5, 5/10, and 1/10 somehow on the number line. Instead realizing that a 3/5-long segment is a combination of a 5/10-long segment and a 1/10-long segment, she put 5/10 on the segment beginning at 5/10 and ending at 6/10 on the number line. Then the interview revealed that she did not consider the length of the segment. Even when she learned what number line representations meant, she failed to think flexibly as she could take a 5/10long segment from a 3/5-long segment and have a 1/10-long segment left either in the front or at the end of the 5/10-long segment. Other supporting evidence was found in the third interview when they worked on fractions of fractions. In the third interview session, the main task was; Greg bought 2/5 of a square pan of brownies that had only 7/10 of the pan left. a. Draw a picture of brownie pan before and after Greg bought his brownies. b. What fraction of a whole pan did Greg buy? They had the task as homework after learning how to find fractions of fractions with pictorial representations. One representation was with thermometers partitioning horizontally or vertically and the other was with brownie pans partitioning horizontally and vertically. Unlike my expectation that the context and pictorial representations would help them think, they had a difficulty to understand the context and draw the pictures. It took more than half of the interview session for them to get a brownie pan with each 1/10 of 7/10 of a brownie pan partitioned into fifths (see Picture 3.). Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 18 Picture 3: A brownie pan with partitioning each 1/10 of 7/10 into fifths Then Aisha finally told Ansley to color 2 small pieces in each 1/10 bar. Ansley seemed to pay little attention to what she was doing and followed Aisha’s direction. When asked to explain, Aisha said, “I thought the answer’s 14/50. I solved and I started counting down, which gave me 5 and across 7. So I just colored 2 bars. 7 times 2 is 14” and added, “I get my answer first and then showed how I got it”. So apparently Aisha understood or remembered that the answer to a fraction of a fraction was obtained by multiplying numerators and denominators of the two fractions, and tried to make sense of the pictorial representation. As a finding of the brief data analysis, I concluded that both students had not yet reached the process level in the spectrum in Figure 1. Although they had learned various procedures for each operation as adding and subtracting fractions, converting improper fractions, and multiplying fractions, they did not perform the mathematical processes flexibly and efficiently by selecting a best procedure under a specific problem setting. Instead, they adhered to a certain procedure for a mathematical process. Commonly both students preferred symbolic manipulation for each operation, but the preference does not mean that they have reached the proceptual level either so that they can think about Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 19 mathematics symbolically. So, the findings informed me that both students were in between the procedural level and the process level since they could perform at least a procedure for each mathematical process confidently and they could understand and perform some other procedures although they did not use them flexibly and efficiently. This suggested that the procept model might have room for elaboration or modification to explain how students perform a mathematical process. Therefore, in the proposed study, I anticipate elaborating the procept model in students’ learning of variables and patterns or modifying the model if it is necessary. Interview Tasks In this section, I will describe the interview schedules and the purpose of each interview task. The interview schedules will be set according to how students’ learning in class progresses and the interview tasks will be based on the unit to be investigated in the present study, “Variables and Patterns: Introducing Algebra”. The unit has 5 investigations, but the last investigation will not be included in the interview tasks since the investigation is about how to use a graphing calculator to generate a table or a graph, not to use a graphing calculator as an investigating tool for the mathematical topic. For each of four investigations, two interview sessions will be conducted; one in the middle of the investigation and the other at the end of the investigation. The first interview session in each investigation will mainly focus on how students make sense of symbolism in class activities and the second one will focus on students’ mathematical concepts through problem solving activities. The problems to be used in the second interview session of each investigation will be adopted from those in the “Application, Connections, Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 20 Extensions” sections in CMP. Below, I describe the purpose of tasks in each interview session with example problems for each second interview session. 1. Investigation 1: Variables and coordinate graphs The main tasks are to describe a situation with two variables, to identify variables in a situation, to identify which variable is independent and which is dependent, and to plot the graph of two variables. Example: The convenience store across the street from Metropolis School has been keeping track of their popcorn sales. The table below shows the total number of bags sold beginning at 6:00 A. M. on a particular day. a. Make a coordinate graph of these data. Which variable did you put on the x-axis? Why? b. Describe how the number of bags of popcorn sold changed during the day. Explain why these changes may have occurred. Time Total bags sold Time Total bags sold 6:00 A. M. 0 1:00 P. M. 58 7:00 A. M. 3 2:00 P. M. 58 8:00 A. M. 15 3:00 P. M. 62 9:00 A. M. 20 4:00 P. M. 74 10:00 A. M. 26 5:00 P. M. 83 11:00 A. M. 30 6:00 P. M. 88 noon 45 7:00 P. M. 92 2. Investigation 2: Graphing changes Students are here to make a table and/or a graph after reading a narrative of a changing situation, to interpret data given in a table and a graph, and to compare tabular, graphic and narrative representations of the situation. Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 21 Example: Make a table and a graph of (time, temperature) data that fit the following information about a day on the road: We started riding at 8 A. M. The day was quite warm, with dark clouds in the sky. About midmorning the temperature dropped quickly to 63F, and there was a thunderstorm for about an hour. After the storm, the sky cleared and there was a warm breeze. As the day went on, the sun steadily warmed the air. When we reached our campground at 4 P. M. it was 89F. 3. Investigation 3: Analyzing graphs and tables The interview tasks are to search for patterns of change in a graph and a table, to describe a situation with verbal rules, and to predict a change. Example: Recall that the perimeter of a rectangle is the sum of its side lengths. a. Make a table of all the possible whole-number values for the length and width of a rectangle with a perimeter of 24 meters. b. Make a coordinate graph of your data from part a. Put length in the x-axis and width on the y-axis. c. Describe what happens to the width as the length increase. d. Would it make sense to connect the points in this graph? Explain your reasoning. 4. Investigation 4: Patterns and Rules Students are to show their understanding of the relationship between rate, time, and distance, to identify and represent rates in a table and a graph, to express patterns in symbols. Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 22 Example: Sean just bought a new CD player and speakers from the Audio Source for $315. The store offered Sean an interest-free payment plan that allows him to pay in weekly installment of $25. a. How much will Sean still owe after one payment? After two payments? After three payments? b. Using n to stand for the number of payments and A for the amount still owed, write an equation for calculating A for any given value of n. c. Use your equation to make a table and a graph showing the relationship between n and A. d. As n increases by 1, how does A change? How is this change shown in the table? On the graph? e. How many payments will Sean have to make in all? How is this shown in the table? How is this shown on the graph? In particular, the first interview sessions in each investigation will provide an answer to the first research question of the present study – how do students make sense of symbols used in mathematical activities?, and the second interview sessions in each investigation will mainly provide evidences of how students’ mathematical concepts develop and evolve as they use symbols throughout mathematical activities. Specifically, a series of thought process maps adopted from Tall et al. (2001, p.11) will show students’ development and evolvement of their mathematical concepts. As an example of a thought process map, Figure 2 shows how Aisha solved 1/2 + 1/3 on a number line. Jeonglim Chae Prospectus To find a common denominator of 2& 3 Solve 1/2 + 1/3 on a number line To find equivalent fractions with denominator 6 6 1 2 To find the sum with symbol manipulation 3/6 + 2/6 = 5/6 1/2 = 3/6 1/3 = 2/6 3 4 ooooo strategic sub-goal symbol manipulation link to the goal 1, 2, 3, … Draw two segments of lengths 2/6 and 3/6 successive sub-goals Figure 2: Aisha’s strategies for solving 1/2 + 1/3 on a number line Refer to symbol manipulation 23 Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 25 Reference Cobb, P. (2000). From representations to symbolizing: introductory comments on semiotics and mathematical learning. In P. Cobb, E. Yackel, & K. McClain (Eds.), Symbolizing and communicating in mathematics classrooms: perspective on discourse, tools, and instructional design (pp. 17-36). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Hiebert. J., et al. (1997). Making sense: teaching and learning mathematics with understanding. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Hibert, J., & Carpenter, T. P. (1992). Learning and teaching with understanding. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 65-97). New York: Macmillan. Izsak, A., Findell, B., Olive, J., & Orrill, C. (2002). Coordinating students’ and teachers’ algebraic reasoning. Unpublished project proposal. Janvier, C., Girardon, C., Morand, J. (1992). Mathematical symbols and representations. In P. S. Wilson (Ed.), Research ideas for the classroom: high school mathematics (pp. 79-102). New York: Macmillan. Kieran, C. (1992). The learning and teaching school algebra. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 390-419). New York: Macmillan. Kline, M. (1972). Mathematical thought from ancient to modern times. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Lappan, G. et al. (2002). Connected Mathematics. Glenview, IL: Prentice Hall. Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 26 Lesh, R., & Lehrer, R. (2000). Iterative refinement cycles for videotape analyses of conceptual change. In A. E. Kelly & R. A. Lesh (Eds.), Handbook of research design in mathematics and science education (pp. 665-708). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Mason, J. H. (1987). What do symbols represent? In C. Janvier (Ed.), Problems of representation in the teaching and learning of mathematics (p.73-81). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2000). Principles and standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: NCTM. Sfard, A. (1995). The development of algebra: confronting historical and psychological perspectives. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 14, 15-39. Sfard, A., & Linchevski, L. (1992). The gains and the pitfalls of reification – the case of algebra. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 26, 191-228. Stacey, K., & MacGregor, M. (1997). Ideas about symbolism that students bring to algebra. Mathematics Teacher, 90, 110-113. Steffe, P. L., & Thompson, P. W. (2000). Teaching experiment methodology: underlying principles and essential elements. In A. E. Kelly & R. A. Lesh (Eds.), Handbook of research design in mathematics and science education (pp. 267-306). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Tall, D. et al. (2001). Symbols and the bifurcation between procedural and conceptual thinking. Canadian Journal of Science, Mathematics and Technology Education, 1(1), 81-104. Jeonglim Chae Prospectus 27 von Glasersfeld, E. (1995). Radical constructivism: a way of knowing and learning. London, UK: The Falmer Press. Wheeler, D. (1989). Contexts for research on the teaching and learning of algebra. In S. Wagner & C. Kieran (Eds.), Research issues in the learning and teaching of algebra (pp. 278-287). Reston, VA: NCTM.