Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 1 Running Head: KNOWLEDGE FOR TEACHING FRACTION MULTIPLICATION Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication Andrew Izsák The University of Georgia Department of Mathematics and Science Education 105 Aderhold Hall Athens, GA 30602-7124 (706) 542 - 4045 izsak@uga.edu Word Count: 18, 800 Acknowledgements I would like to thank Ms. Archer, Ms. Reese, and their students for allowing me to be part of their classrooms; members of the CoSTAR project for a range of assistance conducting the research; and John Olive, Les Steffe, and anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on previous drafts. The research reported in this article was supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant No. REC-0231879. The opinions expressed are those of the author and do not necessarily reflect the views of NSF. Correspondence should be addressed to Andrew Izsák, Department of Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 2 Mathematics and Science Education, The University of Georgia, 105 Aderhold Hall, Athens, GA, 30602-7124. Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 3 Abstract The present study contrasts mathematical knowledge that two 6th-grade teachers apparently used when teaching fraction multiplication with the Connected Mathematics Project materials. The analysis concentrated on those tasks from the materials that use drawings to represent fractions as length or area quantities. Examining the two teachers’ explanations and responses to their students’ reasoning over extended sequences of lessons led to a theoretical frame that emphasizes relationships between the teachers’ unit structures and pedagogical purposes for using drawings. The choice of topic, fraction multiplication, and the methods developed for the study extend research on knowledge specialized for teaching particular mathematics topics, sometimes referred to as mathematical knowledge for teaching. The methods should be of interest to those investigating teachers’ knowledge of mathematics and related disciplines. Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 4 Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication Research on teacher knowledge has expanded from studies of teachers’ subjectmatter knowledge of various content areas to the organization of teachers’ knowledge for teaching particular content to students (e.g., Ball, 1991; Ball, Lubienski, & Mewborn, 2001; Borko & Putnam, 1996; Davis & Simmt, 2006; Ma, 1999; Sherin, 2002; Shulman, 1986). This expansion follows a generation of research that did not find clear connections between teacher knowledge and student achievement and reflects increasing awareness that teachers need not just content knowledge that many educated adults have, but also knowledge specialized for teaching particular topics (see Ball et al., 2001, for a review of relevant literature). In light of this insight, current discussions of teacher knowledge are often framed in terms of subject matter, pedagogical, and pedagogical content knowledge (e.g., Borko & Putnam, 1996). When introducing the notion of pedagogical content knowledge, Shulman emphasized knowledge of students’ thinking about particular topics, typical difficulties that students have, and representations that make mathematical ideas accessible to students. Within mathematics education, Ball and colleagues (Ball & Bass, 2000; Ball et al. 2001) have used a term related to pedagogical content knowledge, mathematical knowledge for teaching, to emphasize the mathematics that teachers might use to accomplish tasks central to their practice. Such tasks include using curricular materials judiciously, choosing and using representations, skillfully interpreting and responding to students' work, and designing assessments. In the most visible line of current Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 5 research on mathematical knowledge for teaching, Hill, Schilling, and Ball (2004) reported a multiple choice instrument that reliably measures elementary teachers’ knowledge of number concepts; operations; and patterns, functions, and algebra. Hill, Rowan, and Ball (2005) demonstrated through a large-scale study that first- and thirdgrade teachers’ knowledge of these topics, as measured by the same instrument, had an effect size comparable to SES on student gain scores on CTB/McGraw-Hill’s Terra Nova measures of student achievement. The gains occurred over a one-year period in urban and suburban schools serving higher poverty populations. These results established a positive correlation between teacher knowledge and student achievement, a link that proved elusive in prior research. At the same time, elaborating mathematical knowledge for teaching particular topics remains a central challenge. The present report uses contrasting case studies of two teachers to elaborate aspects of mathematical knowledge for teaching fraction multiplication, a topic central to the elementary curriculum but that research on teachers’ knowledge has yet to investigate closely. In contrast to recent research that has administered tests to large populations of teachers (Hill, Rowan et al., 2005; Hill, Shilling et al., 2004), the present study examines mathematical knowledge that the two teachers’ used for one category of tasks included in the definition of mathematical knowledge for teaching— interpreting and responding to students’ work. By concentrating on particular cognitive structures, the analysis will be more narrow in focus than recent studies that have examined not only knowledge, but also beliefs and goals that teachers employ during lessons (e.g., Schoenfeld, 1998, 2000, in press). Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 6 Data for the present study came from sixth-grade classrooms in which the teachers and students used a combination of fractions units from Connected Mathematics (CMP; Lappan, Fey, Fitzgerald, Friel, & Phillips, 2002) and draft revised units that have since been published as part of Connected Mathematics 2 (Lappan, Fey, Fitzgerald, Friel, & Phillips, 2006). These are reform-oriented materials whose development was supported by the National Science Foundation. The units ask teachers and students to use manipulatives and drawings to reason about problem situations in which fractions are embedded, often as length or area quantities. Teachers are to use students’ strategies to help them develop general numeric methods. The four drawings shown in Figure 1 for representing 2/3 of 3/4 are all based on work produced by students in the case study classrooms. The range of approaches for representing parts of parts motivates the overarching research question for the present study: What mathematical knowledge did the teachers use when examining such examples for opportunities to build general numeric methods? When looking for such opportunities, teachers would need to decide which of the drawings suggest correct answers for the current problem and which suggest methods that could extend readily to further examples such as 3/5 of 4/7. Furthermore, building general numeric methods from students’ strategies would require many teachers to adapt in response to student strategies that were novel to them. I use the word adapt to mean adjustments that teachers would make in their thinking about the content as a consequence of interacting with students. Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 7 Figure 1 About Here The next section of the article positions the current study with respect to two bodies of research, research on teachers’ understanding of fraction multiplication and research on unit structures required to reason about fractions as quantities. The review will point out different perspectives on knowledge found in these bodies of research. The theoretical frame presented in the following section will articulate a perspective on knowledge that builds more directly on some of these perspectives than others. In particular, it will incorporate descriptions of unit structures as well as further aspects of knowledge not well represented in past research. Then, after a brief description of the classroom context, I will describe the data collection and analysis methods in detail because an important part of the research was the construction of methods that afforded coordinated analyses of knowledge that teachers and students evidenced over sequences of lessons in which they participated together. Such methods were essential for developing accounts of knowledge that teachers used when responding to their students’ reasoning about problem situations. Finally, I will proceed with the two case studies, examining one teacher at a time. Background Teachers’ Knowledge of Fraction Multiplication Research on teachers’ knowledge has examined fraction division and decimal multiplication more closely than fraction multiplication. Research on fraction division Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 8 has reported that teachers can confuse situations that call for dividing by a fraction with ones that call for dividing by a whole number or multiplying by a fraction (Armstrong & Bezuk, 1995; Ball, 1990; Borko et al., 1992; Ma, 1999; Sowder, Philipp, Armstrong, & Schappelle, 1998). Ball asked 10 preservice elementary and 9 preservice secondary teachers to generate situations that would illustrate 1 3/4 ÷ 1/2. Seventeen could compute the correct answer but only five were able to generate an appropriate word problem or situation. Five others generated situations that would illustrate 1 3/4 ÷ 2, one generated a situation that would illustrate 1 3/4 x 2, and eight were unable to generate any situation, correct or incorrect. Ma reported similar results for U.S. teachers after including the same task in her investigation of 23 U.S. and 72 Chinese inservice teachers’ understandings of core mathematics topics taught in elementary grades. Borko et al. reported a case in which a teacher candidate, Ms. Daniels, was asked by a student to explain the invert and multiply rule for fraction division. The class had just computed the answer to 3/4 ÷ 1/2. Ms. Daniels generated a situation and area representation that illustrated 3/4 x 1/2, realized that her example showed fraction multiplication instead of division, and was stumped. Sowder et al. reported that a middle-grades teacher, Cynthia, had trouble making appropriate connections between operations on fractional quantities and the arithmetic operations of multiplication and division (pp. 49-50). Ma’s (1999) study not only confirmed constraints reported by Ball (1990) on U.S. teachers’ performance, but also provided a more elaborated description of teachers’ mathematical knowledge. In interviews, she asked U.S. and Chinese teachers to solve Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 9 mathematics problems embedded in teaching scenarios. Topics included subtraction with regrouping, multidigit multiplication, and fraction division. Ma developed the idea of knowledge packages to explain the vividly stronger performance of the Chinese teachers. A knowledge package consists of numerous subtopics that are connected to one another and that support the teaching of the larger topic. To illustrate with the example most relevant to the present study, Ma described Chinese teachers’ knowledge packages for fraction division in terms of the meanings of addition, multiplication, and division with whole numbers; the concepts of inverse operation, fraction, and unit; and the meaning of multiplication with fractions (Ma, 1999, p. 77). Ma found that the Chinese teachers in her sample had more elaborate knowledge packages than the U.S. teachers, which allowed them to move flexibly among subtopics when explaining how they would help students connect new topics to prior topics. A second set of studies about teacher knowledge of rational numbers has built upon a different perspective on knowledge that states people have intuitive models for arithmetic operations (Fischbein, Deri, Nello, & Marino, 1985). The proposed model for multiplication is repeated addition, which implies that the multiplier or operator needs to be a whole number and supports the notion that multiplication always makes larger. Fischbein et al.’s results were on fifth-, seventh-, and ninth-grade students, but further studies have extended the results to elementary school teachers (Graeber & Tirosh, 1988; Graeber, Tirosh, & Glover, 1989; Harel & Behr, 1995; Post, Harel, Behr, & Lesh, 1991; Tirosh & Graeber, 1989). In these studies, teachers have had a harder time solving word problems in which the multiplier was a decimal less than one. For instance, Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 10 Graeber and colleagues (Graeber & Tirosh, 1988; Graeber et al., 1989) administered to 129 preservice elementary teachers a written test consisting of 26 word problems adapted from those used by Fischbein et al. They found that a higher percentage of the preservice teachers solved multiplication word problems correctly when the multiplier was a whole number and often used division inappropriately when working on problems like the following: One kilogram of detergent is used in making 15 kilograms of soap. How much soap can be made from .75 kilograms of detergent? (Graeber & Tirosh, 1988, p. 264) A few further examples in the literature suggest that teachers may also have a hard time using drawings to explain the product of two rational numbers. Eisenhart et al. (1993) reported that during an interview the same Ms. Daniels began, but could not complete, an explanation for .7 x 2.35 using a rectangular region. Armstrong and Bezuk (1995) gave inservice middle school teachers a word problem for which computing 1/3 of 3/4 would be appropriate. The teachers recognized that the situation called for fraction multiplication but had a hard time explaining their thinking, drawing diagrams to match algorithmic solutions, and understanding the appropriate unit or whole for the problem. Sowder et al. (1998) reported difficulties that a middle-grades teacher, Linda, had explaining fraction multiplication with an area model during interviews and classroom instruction (p. 83-87). Finally, Ball et al. (2001) present a classroom vignette in Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 11 which a teacher struggled to explain products of decimals using dimensions and areas of rectangles. The studies summarized in this section have emphasized errors and other constraints on teachers’ performances, primarily on tests and in interview settings. Such results suggest that, beyond knowledge of computation procedures, teachers’ understandings of fraction multiplication (and division) is limited. Those studies that have described teachers’ mathematical knowledge in more detail have delineated subtopics or have gathered evidence for the intuitive model of multiplication as repeated addition, but they have not examined teachers’ cognitive structures for reasoning about fractional quantities. Yet, reasoning with such quantities is essential for interpreting and evaluating different approaches to representing parts of parts, such as those shown in Figure 1. The next section summarizes results of further research, much of it done with students, that has examined cognitive structures for reasoning with fractional quantities. These results will inform the theoretical frame developed subsequently. Unit Structures Conceptual units of various types have played a central role in research on children’s understandings of whole and rational numbers. The most important point in the following discussion is the distinction between two and three levels of units, which I explain first in the context of whole numbers by summarizing Steffe’s (1988, 1994) analyses of emerging multiplication schemes.i According to Steffe, a child who can form Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 12 two levels of units can understand a whole number such as five simultaneously as five separate units (one level of unit) and as one group of five understood to be a single entity (the whole group is a second level of unit) (Figure 2a). Steffe states that such a child has formed composite units. Through interiorization and coordination of composite units, a child can produce three levels of units by nesting composite units within composite units. As an example, a child with such an operation can assimilate a display of 20 blocks as a single group (the whole group is one level) composed of five units (a second level), where each of the five units is also a composite unit composed of four separate units (a third level) (Figure 2b). Reasoning with three levels of units requires attending to all three levels simultaneously. Figure 2 About Here In subsequent work, Steffe and Olive (Olive 1999; Olive & Steffe, 2001; Steffe 2001, 2003, 2004) have examined how elementary students modified their knowledge of whole-number counting and multiplication to construct knowledge of fractions when engaged in tasks involving lengths and rectangular areas. The students in Steffe’s and Olive’s reports solved tasks by coordinating two and three levels of units and by using three mental operations called disembedding, iterating, and partitioning. For the present article, iterating and partitioning will be the most important, and I illustrate them in the next paragraph. Differences in students’ ability to coordinate levels of units and contexts in which students used their disembedding, iterating, and partitioning Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 13 operations led to significant differences in learning. The most successful students constructed two new operations, recursive partitioning and splitting, that were fundamental to the construction of schemes for commensurate fractions (e.g., 1/3 is equivalent to 5/15), improper fractions, fraction composition (e.g., finding 1/3 of 1/4), and addition of unit fractions. I describe recursive partitioning to illustrate the role three levels of units can play in reasoning about fractions and because it will be important to one of the case studies reported below. Steffe (2003, 2004) has defined recursive partitioning to be taking a partition of a partition in the service of a non-partitioning goal. Consider a task that asks for the result of taking 1/4 of 1/3 using lengths. Students might first construct the requested quantity by partitioning a unit length into three pieces (reasoning with two levels of units), narrowing their attention to just the first of those pieces, and partitioning that piece into four further pieces (reasoning with two levels of units a second time) (Figure 3a). Determining the size of the resulting piece is a nonpartitioning goal, and students could accomplish this in more than one way. Students might simply iterate the resulting piece and count to see that 12 copies exhaust the original unit (Figure 3b). This solution involves reasoning with two levels of units a third time (one unit containing 12 twelfths). Alternatively, students might recursively partition by subdividing each of the remaining thirds into four pieces (Figure 3c). In contrast to the first solution, recursive partitioning involves reasoning with three levels of units in which four of the smallest units are nested within each mid-level unit, and so there must be 3 x 4 = 12 units in the whole unit. Note that recursive partitioning Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 14 incorporates aspects of distribution because each of the thirds is repartitioned into the same number of smaller units. Figure 3 About Here In a second line of research also focused on the formation and reformation of nested levels of conceptual units, members of the Rational Number Project have examined five rational number subconstructs—part-whole, quotient, ratio number, operator, and measure (e.g., Behr, Harel, Post, & Lesh, 1992). Of particular relevance to the present study is the operator construct, in which a fraction is thought of as a function applied to a number, object, or set. Behr and his colleagues (Behr, Harel, Post, & Lesh, 1993) decomposed the operator construct further into three subconstructs— duplicator and partition-reducer, stretcher and shrinker, and multiplier and divisor. The role of unit structures in the first two subconstructs is most important for the present discussion. When reading the descriptions below, those familiar with research on multiplication and division will notice that the duplicator and partition-reducer subconstruct is based on partitive division, and the stretcher and shrinker construct on quotitive division. Behr et al. (1993) used the problem 3/4 of 8 to contrast the duplicator and partition-reducer subconstruct with the stretcher and shrinker subconstruct. In the duplicator and partition-reducer subconstruct, the numerator duplicates by specifying how many copies to make of the original set. In the present example, three sets of eight Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 15 objects are created (Figure 4a). The denominator partitions by determining among how many sets the resulting individual objects are shared—in this example 24 objects are shared among four sets—and reduces by taking one of the resulting sets of six objects as the answer. Behr et al. also explained that one could partition first and then duplicate. Notice that the operations of duplicating and reducing transform the number of composite units but not the size of those units. Figure 4 About Here In contrast, the operations of stretching and shrinking do act on sizes of units. Here, the numerator stretches by exchanging each of the eight original objects for composite units of three objects (Figure 4b). The denominator determines the size of the groups into which the resulting individual objects are rearranged—in this example 24 objects are rearranged into groups of four—and shrinks by exchanging the resulting six groups of four for six groups of one. Behr et al. (1993) also explained that one could shrink first and then stretch. Note that, like recursive partitioning, the stretcher and shrinker construct contains aspects of distribution in the sense that the numerator and denominator operate on each subunit. In a subsequent study, Behr, Khoury, Harel, Post, and Lesh (1997) interviewed 30 preservice elementary teachers on the operator subconstruct. This is the only other study of which I am aware that analyzed unit structures to which teachers attend when reasoning about fraction multiplication. The teachers were given 8 bundles of 4 sticks Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 16 and asked to complete three tasks. The first task was simply to show a pile that has 3/4 as many sticks. For the second and third tasks, the teachers were asked to imagine that each bundle of sticks represented boards that one carpenter used on a job in one day. The second task stipulated that only 3/4 of the carpenters came to work one day, and the third stipulated that all eight carpenters came to work but only worked 3/4 of a day. In both cases the teachers were asked to arrange the boards for the carpenters. The researchers sought to (a) characterize the strategies the preservice teachers used as duplicator and partition-reducer, stretcher and shrinker, or other and (b) determine the conceptual units that the teachers formed and transformed during their solutions. They reported that even though the intent of the third problem was to elicit stretcher and shrinker strategies (for instance by taking 3/4 of each bundle), only 12 of the documented strategies were of this type. Nineteen of the remaining strategies were consistent with the duplicator and partition-reducer construct. In these cases, teachers tended not to offer a stretcher and shrinker strategy even when the interviewers attempted to elicit such strategies. Apparently these teachers found it hard to distribute fractions as operators across their conceptual units, which in turn suggested constraints on the flexibility with which they formed and transformed conceptual units when using sticks to find a fraction of a whole number. The present study contributes in several ways to the bodies of research summarized above. First, as already mentioned, research on teachers’ knowledge of rational numbers has examined fraction division and decimal multiplication more Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 17 closely than fraction multiplication. The few studies that do exist suggest that teachers have trouble explaining fraction multiplication when fractions are understood as quantities such as lengths or areas, but these same studies have not examined the underlying knowledge that could account for such performances. The present study concentrates on fraction multiplication and elaborates the underlying mathematical knowledge that two teachers evidenced when reasoning with fractions as quantities. Second, research that has described cognitive structures for reasoning about fractions as quantities has highlighted, among other things, conceptual units organized into multi-level structures. The present study will demonstrate that teachers need to reason explicitly and flexibly with nested levels of units if they are to respond to the variety of quantitative structures, such as those shown in Figure 1, that their students might assemble. The analysis will demonstrate that the capacity to distribute fraction operations across conceptual units is central to such flexibility. Third, the present study examines not only teachers’ unit structures, but also the pedagogical purposes for which they used drawings that depict visually perceivable features of problem situations. To the best of my knowledge, researchers have not delineated different goals, or purposes, for which teachers might use drawings during lessons on fraction arithmetic or other topics. I focus on the use of drawings in teaching not only because discussions of pedagogical content knowledge and mathematical knowledge for teaching often make explicit reference to representations, but also because reform-oriented curricula in the United States are placing new demands on teachers and students to interpret and reason with a variety of inscriptions. The Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 18 theoretical frame developed in the next section includes a discussion of different possible purposes. Theoretical Frame In many respects, the perspective on knowledge I take in the present study is consistent with others found in research on cognition in mathematical and scientific domains (e.g., Izsák, 2004, 2005; diSessa, 1988, 1993; Piaget, 1970; Schoenfeld, Smith, & Arcavi, 1993; Steffe, 1988, 1994, 2001). I emphasize three characteristics. First, knowledge elements are often more fine grained than statements found in textbooks. Summaries presented above of past research include descriptions of knowledge at different grain sizes. To illustrate, Ma (1999) included the meaning of multiplication, the concept of fraction, and the meaning of multiplication with fractions when elaborating the knowledge package for fraction division; but, research by Steffe (1988, 1994, 2001, 2003, 2004) and by members of the Rational Number Project (e.g., Behr, Harel et al., 1993; Behr, Khoury et al., 1997) decomposed such meanings and concepts into numerous smaller cognitive structures. By attending to teachers’ nested unit structures, I examine knowledge at a grain size similar to that analyzed by Steffe and members of the Rational Number Project. Second, knowledge elements are diverse, which means that a person’s knowledge elements related to a particular domain are not only numerous but also of different types. In the present study, nested quantitative units and pedagogical purposes for using drawings (described in more detail below) are different types of knowledge. Third, knowledge is context sensitive and often tacit. A person Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 19 who uses a given piece of knowledge when reasoning about one situation may not use that same piece again in further situations where, from an observer’s perspective, it might be applied productively. Furthermore, a person may only evidence some knowledge elements by responding to situations through combinations of talk, gesture, and inscription. The perspective on knowledge just outlined implies that developing accounts of knowledge used by teachers while teaching requires inferring diverse, fine-grained knowledge elements that teachers activate (sometimes in response to students’ comments and questions) and how those elements function together as teachers try to accomplish particular goals. Inferring knowledge elements is delicate because a teacher may have, but not use, a particular piece of knowledge for such reasons as (a) the teacher does not recognize a given situation as one where the piece of knowledge could be applied productively or (b) connections among other activated knowledge elements lead the teacher to consider but then not use a piece of knowledge. From my perspective, if a teacher has but, for whatever reason, does not use a piece of knowledge when responding to a range of situations that arise in the course of teaching—situations where that knowledge could be applied productively—then for that teacher the knowledge is not an instance of knowledge for teaching. In the analysis below, I restrict my claims to knowledge that teachers appear to use when responding to their students. I stop short of making claims about what they may or may not know more generally. The theoretical frame developed here emerged through analyses of two teachers’ enactments of the CMP materials and, in particular, why the two teachers were more Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 20 adaptive in response to some of their students’ comments and strategies than others. The analysis led to three categories that serve to highlight the diversity of knowledge elements that they used—numerical aspects of multiplication, unit structures, and pedagogical purposes for using drawings. First, teachers evidenced several ideas related to numerical aspects of multiplication. Examples included declarative statements about whole-number factor-product combinations, that multiplication is the same as repeated addition, and that a fraction times a number is the same as a fraction of the number. Although each of these examples could have been anticipated, there was one interesting wrinkle. Despite one teacher’s assertion that the words “of” and “times” meant the same thing, she and her students apparently attended to parts of wholes when thinking about a fraction times a number but to parts of parts when thinking about a fraction of a number: Each word appeared to direct this teacher’s and at least some of her students’ attention in different ways. Thus, I will not use the words “of” and “times” interchangeably and will be careful to use the same word that the teachers, students, books, and interviewers used in a given instance. Second, as explained in the next section, the CMP materials provide extensive opportunities to reason about quantities when developing fraction arithmetic, and hence opportunities for teachers and students to employ their unit structures. Both teachers appeared comfortable with the parts-of-a-whole interpretation of fractions and the associated two levels of units. The extension of fractions from parts of wholes to parts of parts created opportunities to reason with three-level unit structures, but I emphasize that tasks involving parts of parts only created opportunities to reason with Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 21 three-level structures. As the first solution to 1/4 of 1/3 discussed above illustrates, in some cases it is possible to determine the result of taking part of a part by reasoning with just two levels of units. Analyses of classroom interactions presented later in this article will examine instances where a teacher had trouble responding to students’ reasoning about parts of parts, apparently because she focused on just two levels of units. Further analyses will demonstrate that the ability to reason with three levels of units explicitly is necessary but not sufficient if teachers are to adapt in response to the variety of ways that their students might assemble three-level structures in a given problem situation. The underlying flexibility with unit structures needed to compare alternative structures that students might produce, such as those suggested in Figure 1, rests in turn on reasoning with drawn versions of distribution. The third category of knowledge, pedagogical purposes for using drawings, has not been discussed widely in past research. I take drawings to be a particular kind of inscription distinct from formal symbolic notations. The distinction between drawings and symbolic notations as different types of inscriptions is important because reformoriented materials, like CMP, often use drawings to help students develop meaning for formal notations and procedures for transforming those notations. A person must interpret or use an inscription to reason about a situation before it makes sense to talk about the inscription as representing. Comparing the two teachers’ enactments of CMP to my interpretation of the materials led to four different pedagogical purposes for which a teacher might use drawings of quantities to solve fraction arithmetic problems. The first use is simply to Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 22 illustrate solutions also arrived at using an alternate method, such as a numeric computation. In this case, a teacher could use the computed answer as a guide when developing an interpretation of the drawing. The second use is to infer a computation method first by using drawings to determine solutions for various problems and then by looking for patterns in the resulting numerical answers (for fraction multiplication the pattern might be products of numerators are numerators of products and products of denominators are denominators of products). The third use for drawings is to deduce a computation procedure from the structure of drawn quantities. In the case of fraction multiplication, a teacher might focus on his or her own understandings of the whole, parts of the whole, and parts of parts of the whole to determine a general computation procedure. For instance, a teacher might partition a unit square horizontally to represent one fraction and repartition the square vertically to represent the second. No matter what fractions are used, this method generates a smaller array representing the part-of-the-part nested inside a larger array representing the whole. The number of pieces in the smaller array is always determined by the product of numerators (a x b in Figure 5), and that in the larger array is always determined by the product of denominators (c x d in Figure 5). Therefore, a general computation procedure can be based on products of numerators and products of denominators. The fourth use is to adapt how one represents structures of quantities in response to students’ thinking by (a) inferring students’ understandings of the whole, parts of the whole, and parts of parts of the whole and (b) attending to the Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 23 variety of ways that students might begin assembling three-level unit structures as evidenced by their explanations and drawings. Figure 5 About Here I have already mentioned two reasons for attending to teachers’ pedagogical purposes for using drawings: (a) Definitions of pedagogical content knowledge and mathematical knowledge for teaching make explicit reference to representations, and (b) reform-oriented curricula in the United States often place new demands on teachers and students to interpret and reason with a variety of inscriptions. A third reason is that the case studies presented below suggest that the unit structures teachers employed shaped the purposes for which they used drawings when teaching fraction multiplication. The first case is of a teacher who reasoned primarily with just two levels of units and used drawings to illustrate solutions. The second case is of a teacher who evidenced more consistent attention to three-level structures and used drawings to infer a computation method. (A teacher who focused primarily on just two levels of units might also be able to use drawings to determine solutions without first calculating answers using numeric methods but consistent, explicit attention to three-level structures would make this easier.) The third use for drawings would require the ability to explicitly produce three levels of units, and the fourth use would require the ability to produce such structures flexibility so as to compare alternative approaches for producing three-level structures. Explicit attention to drawn versions of distribution Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 24 supports such flexibility. Although I claim that core mathematical understandings shaping how the two teachers’ enacted the CMP materials and responded to their students over sequences of lessons can be organized into the three categories of numeric aspects of multiplication, unit structures, and pedagogical purposes for using drawings, I do not claim that these categories capture all knowledge that the teachers might have used during their fraction multiplication lessons. Connected Mathematics and Fraction Multiplication The CMP materials shaped, but did not determine, the perspectives on fraction multiplication and the purposes for drawings that the teachers used during their lessons. The first fractions unit, Bits and Pieces I, introduces various interpretations of fractions, decimals, and percents, including parts of a whole, measures of quantities, and indicated division. The materials include fraction strips, number lines, and rectangular regions for representing fractional quantities. Fraction strips are made of paper subdivided by folding. The length of the entire strip is intended to represent one whole. The second fractions unit, Bits and Pieces II, develops fraction arithmetic by building on the interpretations and drawings introduced in Bits and Pieces I. Each case study teacher taught from a slightly different draft of the revised Bits and Pieces II unit that has since been published (Lappan et al., 2006). At different points, the overarching mathematical goals stated in the teacher’s edition appear consistent with the second, third, and fourth uses for drawings discussed above. The goals include developing Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 25 ways to model sums, differences, products, and quotients using fraction strips, number lines, and rectangular areas; looking for and generalizing patterns in numbers (the infer purpose); and developing algorithms (possibly the infer, deduce, or adapt purpose). The introduction to the teacher’s edition states that Bits and Pieces II does not teach a preferred algorithm. Rather, students are to develop solution strategies for problems in which fractions and operations on fractions are embedded. Teachers are to develop their students’ strategies into general algorithms (the adapt purpose). Pierce Middle School, Ms. Archer, and Ms. Reese The present case study was conducted as part of the Coordinating Students’ and Teachers’ Algebraic Reasoning (CoSTAR) project. The project conducts coordinated research on teaching and learning in grades six through eight at Pierce Middle School. At the beginning of the study, the district had replaced traditional instructional materials focused on skill in computation with the standards-based CMP (Lappan et al., 2002) materials. Pierce Middle School is in a rural community outside of a large southern city. At the time of the study, the school had approximately 700 students who were racially and economically diverse: 35% were African American, 63% were White, and 42% qualified for free lunch. The first teacher, Ms. Archer, ii was in her first year as a full-time teacher, but she was not new to teaching. She reported several previous non-teaching careers as well as teaching in the district as a long-term substitute, mostly in high school classrooms. For one assignment, she had taught the CMP seventh-grade Stretching and Shrinking unit in Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 26 Pierce Middle School. Ms. Archer had a bachelor’s degree in mathematics and, at the time of the study, was enrolled in a 2-year alternative certification program for teachers who have been hired by a district and have a bachelor’s degree in an appropriate field. The program assumes that teachers have the requisite content knowledge, emphasizes other aspects of teacher preparation such as teaching methods, and requires that certification candidates have an on-site mentor. Ms. Archer reported that her own experiences as a student had emphasized drill and repetition, and her collegiate mathematics appeared rusty when she reported having to “brush up” on functions for an exam of content required for licensure. The second teacher, Ms. Reese, was confident in her teaching. Prior to the study, she had taught algebra to seventh-, eighth-, and ninth-grade students for approximately 10 years. Ms. Reese reported that her high school classes had focused on “traditional mathematics” and algorithms, had rarely used manipulatives, and had included drawn pictures only occasionally to introduce a topic or when there was confusion. Ms. Reese was in her second year of CMP implementation, but at the time of the study was teaching Bits and Pieces II for the first time. Izsák, Tillema, & Tunç-Pekkan (in press) described Ms. Reese in greater detail. CMP is an ambitious program that embeds mathematical ideas in problem situations, and teachers often need significant support when first using the materials in their classrooms. Prior to the present case studies, the district had provided limited professional development opportunities to support teachers at Pierce Middle School as they transitioned to reform-oriented materials. Ms. Archer and Ms. Reese participated Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 27 in monthly, after-school professional development sessions provided by the CoSTAR project but had to rely primarily on their existing mathematical understandings when implementing the CMP materials. Of particular relevance to the present study, both teachers reported that solving fraction arithmetic problems with drawings was new for them. Both teachers seemed earnest about using the CMP materials successfully with their students and combined aspects of more traditional and more reform-oriented instruction. Data and Methods The CoSTAR project gathered data that afforded coordinated analyses of understandings that the teachers and their students had of lessons in which they participated together. We started by videotaping continuous sequences of enacted lessons over entire units. When taping lessons, we only observed and did not interact with teachers or students about the content. To the extent possible, our intent was to document instruction in natural settings. Because comments made by teachers and students during lessons were often insufficient for inferring with much confidence the understandings that they were using, we augmented the lesson data with interviews based on tasks and events from the observed lessons. We then analyzed both the lesson and the interview data to infer knowledge that teachers and students may have used to make sense of the lessons. We used two cameras to videotape one teacher’s instruction daily during the same class period. One researcher set the first camera in the back of the classroom and Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 28 recorded the entire class, adjusting the levels of ceiling and wireless microphones to hear all students during whole-class discussion and to zero in on conversations between the teacher and individual students during group work. A second researcher used the second camera to record written work, staying at the back of the classroom to record the whiteboard during whole-class discussion and shadowing the teacher to record papers that the teacher discussed with students at their desks during group or individual work. This method of recording fit well with the perspective on knowledge outlined above because it captured talk, gesture, and inscription as evidence for more explicit and more tacit knowledge that supported students’ reasoning and teachers’ responses to their students’ reasoning. Later the same day, we combined the video and audio from the two cameras using an audiovisual mixer to create a restored view (Hall, 2000). A restored view is a wider view of activity than can typically be captured with an individual camera but is still a selective view that reflects the researcher’s perspective. From the restored view, we created a lesson graph, a document that parsed the lesson into episodes. Examples of what we took to be episodes include a whole-class discussion of one problem and a conversation between the teacher and a student during group or individual work. The lesson graph included the start and end times of each episode, a brief description of the problem being discussed, key comments made by the teacher and students, and screen shots of any written work that was being discussed. We used the lesson graphs to identify episodes where teachers and students discussed drawings of quantities, as opposed to numeric methods. For the interviews, Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 29 we gave priority to those lesson episodes where teachers and students apparently struggled to make sense of how the other used or interpreted drawings. Such episodes provided good opportunities to investigate what teachers and students attended to in a particular drawing and how they understood the underlying topic. I emphasize that we could not always anticipate when such difficulties might arise and that being present in the classroom on a daily basis maximized our opportunities to capture episodes that served as effective prompts in interviews. I interviewed pairs of students from the same classes in which we gathered the lesson videos (four pairs in Ms. Archer’s classroom and three in Ms. Reese’s). Each teacher helped identify pairs that represented as best as possible a cross section of achievement, a balance between boys and girls, and the racial composition of the class. I interviewed each pair once a week for approximately 50 minutes starting the second week of lesson taping. My overarching goal was to gather data on students’ understanding of the content and how those understandings shaped their interpretations of the lesson excerpts. I used problem-solving tasks and lesson video excerpts as prompts in semistructured interviews (Bernard, 1994, Chapter 10). Typically, I prepared tasks that included both the original tasks in the lesson excerpts and extensions of those tasks. Students worked the tasks using pencil and paper and watched the lesson excerpts on a laptop computer. I elicited students’ reasoning on the tasks and interpretations of the lesson excerpts, but did not correct mistakes or offer instruction. I used one camera to record students, and one to record written work and viewed lesson excerpts. Project Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 30 members created a restored view that captured much of what the students said, wrote, and watched and created interview graphs similar to the lesson graphs described above. I used the graphs to locate excerpts for use in teacher interviews that revealed aspects of students’ mathematical thinking and difficulties not evidenced during lessons. Finally, other project members conducted weekly, hour-long interviews with the teachers to gain access to their interpretations of the CMP activities, their understandings of their students, and the pedagogical decisions they made. One project member interviewed Ms. Reese, and a second interviewed Ms. Archer. The interviews were also semistructured and based on a combination of lesson and student interview video excerpts selected in consultation with me. The consultations insured that lesson excerpts used in the student interviews were also used in the teacher interviews. Teacher interviews occurred after a completed cycle of student interviews and almost always within one week of the original lessons. When preparing teachers to view a given lesson or student interview excerpt, the interviewers stated the mathematics task central to the excerpt. If the task was an extension I had used in student interviews, teachers were sometimes asked to work the task before watching the excerpt. As initial prompts, teachers were often asked to discuss what mathematics was most important in the task, reconstruct what they may have been thinking at a particular point during a lesson, interpret students’ reasoning, or consider how they might respond to a particular student comment or question made during a lesson or interview. As the case studies progressed, teachers developed a sense Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 31 of the questions we were asking and sometimes responded spontaneously to lesson and student interview excerpts. Follow up questions often directed a teacher’s attention to particular aspects of a drawing or a particular student comment that we thought was central to understanding how she and her students understood a given task. The interviewers used one camera to record the teacher and one to record viewed lesson clips. Project members created a restored view that captured much of what the teacher and interviewer said and watched and created interview graphs similar to the lesson graphs described above. We collected data on Ms. Reese first, and only during her enactment of Bits and Pieces II. This began mid-March and continued to mid-May of 2003. The entire case study consisted of 30 taped lessons, 20 interviews distributed over three pairs of students, and nine teacher interviews. Roughly, the first month concentrated on fraction addition and subtraction and the second month on fraction multiplication. The present report concentrates on the 11 lessons, five student interviews, and two teacher interviews that focused on fraction multiplication. (Izsák et al., in press, analyzed the lessons and interviews that focused on fraction addition.) Although the present article concentrates on the second month of instruction, claims made here that Ms. Reese could reason with three-level unit structures are informed by the full dataset. In analyzing data from Ms. Reese’s classroom, questions emerged about how she and her students had discussed partitioning during the preceding Bits and Pieces I unit. Thus, the next year we collected two rounds of data in Ms. Archer’s classroom. The first round occurred in late January through February of 2004 and consisted of 13 lessons Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 32 from Bits and Pieces I, eight interviews distributed over four pairs of students,iii and four teacher interviews. These data contained numerous examples in which Ms. Archer and her students determined products of fractions and whole numbers. The second round occurred in late April through May of 2004 and consisted of 17 lessons from Bits and Pieces II, 17 student interviews distributed over the same four pairs of students, and five teacher interviews. In contrast to Ms. Reese, Ms. Archer touched on fraction multiplication during her fraction addition lessons as well as her two lessons focused exclusively on products of fractions using rectangular regions. We followed up on instances of fraction multiplication instruction in 11 of the student interviews and three of the teacher interviews. Thus, the analyses below are based on significant corpora of data from each classroom. At the end of data collection several episodes stood out in which Ms. Archer had struggled when using drawings to respond to students. A particularly vivid example, discussed in detail later in the article, occurred when she introduced her students to products of proper fractions using number lines. This example raised questions about Ms. Archer’s reasoning with fractional quantities and the pedagogical purposes for which she used drawings, questions that provided initial direction for the analyses presented below. I reviewed all of the lesson videos from Ms. Archer’s classroom chronologically for statements, gestures, or inscriptions that evidenced ideas I understood to be related to multiplication of rational numbers. I found comments about using whole-number multiplication to guide partitioning the whole; statements about the meanings of Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 33 multiplication, fractions, and fraction multiplication; numeric computations with fractions; and discussions of drawings that, to me, showed parts of parts. The chronology was important to preserve in case the knowledge Ms. Archer used shifted over the course of the unit, a distinct possibility given that she was teaching fraction arithmetic with drawings for the first time. To be confident that my list of statements, gestures, and inscriptions was complete, I went through the 30 lesson videos twice and a second member of the CoSTAR project also went through the lesson videos to check the list. Next, I conducted a chronological analysis of knowledge that Ms. Archer evidenced through statements, gestures, and inscriptions during her interviews. Here I used verbatim transcriptions, as well as videos. I coordinated my analysis of knowledge she evidenced during lessons and interviews using methods similar to those described by Cobb and Whitenack (1996) and Schoenfeld et al. (1993) for analyzing longitudinal sets of video recordings. These methods are based on analyzing small episodes, generating hypotheses about those episodes, analyzing further episodes for evidence that might support or undermine any of the initial hypotheses, and modifying hypotheses as necessary to knit local accounts into consistent global accounts. I used the same approach when analyzing the data from Ms. Reese’s classroom and interviews. Given the delay of up to a week between the initial lesson taping and following teacher interview, I used the interview data with great care when supplementing the lesson data for evidence of the mathematical knowledge that each teacher used when interacting with her students. I was most confident using the interview data when the Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 34 teachers evidenced similar understandings in both contexts—for instance, when they gave similar explanations for how to solve a problem first during a lesson and then during an interview. In cases where the teachers evidenced possibly different understandings in the lesson and interview contexts, I considered the possibilities that they accessed different knowledge in the two contexts, that my initial attributions needed to be refined or discarded, and that the teachers had learned something during the time between the lesson and the interview. To illustrate, a teacher might evidence different knowledge in the two contexts if she discussed aspects of the mathematics during her interview that she did not discuss during her lesson. If she reported that it did not occur to her to discuss those aspects during her lesson, then the data suggested that she accessed different knowledge in the two contexts. If her comments during the interview suggested a different interpretation of the original lesson data, then I changed my account of the knowledge she used. If she had explicitly rejected a certain problemsolving approach during an earlier lesson, but then acknowledged the approach could work during a subsequent interview, the data suggested she had learned something. Finally, I examined the teacher’s editions of the CMP units to determine which mathematical ideas they emphasized and how they presented the use of drawings. I compared my understanding of the materials to my understanding of the observed lessons to gauge where each teacher’s explanations were consistent with, or diverged from, those in the materials. This was important because the materials could influence explanations the teachers gave to students, and hence the knowledge they appeared to access during classroom interactions. Instructional goals stated in the materials might Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 35 lead a teacher to de-emphasize certain mathematical aspects that she understood about a given task. In such cases, without an understanding of the materials, I might under represent her mathematical knowledge for teaching. In other cases, where a teacher’s explanations were particularly close to those in the materials, I might over represent her mathematical knowledge for teaching if she simply repeated something she had read. Results on Ms. Archer from Bits and Pieces I The analysis of Ms. Archer proceeds chronologically to gather evidence for the unit structures she used when responding to her students. The initial Bits and Pieces I lessons concentrated on methods for partitioning fraction strips and for computing fractions of whole numbers. Further lessons concentrated on numeric methods and shed less light on the unit structures that Ms. Archer and her students apparently used to reason about fractions as quantities. I will make three main points. First, several tasks afforded opportunities to reason with three levels of units, but whether Ms. Archer did so remained unclear: Her comments made explicit reference to just two levels of units. Second, Ms. Archer and her students discussed two different strategies for determining fractions of whole numbers, a primary one that she and her students discussed most often, as well as a student’s alternative. Ms. Archer’s quick recognition of the alternative demonstrated her capacity to adapt to a student’s reasoning in one context. Third, if Ms. Archer attended to three levels of units, she did not do so consistently in cases where discussing a third level explicitly could have helped answer students’ questions. Data Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 36 from Bits and Pieces I left open the possibility that Ms. Archer could reason explicitly with three-level unit structures but, for whatever reason, did not. Subsequent data from Bits and Pieces II would provide examples where she clearly struggled to relate three levels of units successfully. Whole-Number Multiplication and Partitioning The Bits and Pieces I unit opens with a set of problems about fund-raisers and introduces thermometers as representations for recording progress toward fund-raising goals. Students are asked first to make fraction strips by folding 8 1/2 inch strips of paper into halves, thirds, fourths, and so on up to twelfths. For prime numbers, there are few alternatives to estimating directly the size of one piece and folding to check, a strategy that emphasizes two levels of units. Composite numbers afford opportunities to use knowledge of whole-number multiplication, to partition in stages, and to reason with three levels of units. Ms. Archer clearly attended to whole-number multiplication. The first day she made the following announcement to the whole class after students had been creating fractions strips for several minutes: “Some of you need to think about multiples, products, same stuff we had in Prime Time.iv Even though we are dealing with parts of a whole, we can still apply some of the things that we learned in Prime Time.” A few minutes later, she asked a student who had made thirds but was struggling to make ninths, “So if you fold it in thirds, and I know that three times three is nine, so what would I have to divide the thirds into?” (Ms. Archer also made a similar comment about dividing fifths into fifteenths at one point during a subsequent Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 37 interview.) The second day Ms. Archer listened as Damien demonstrated to the class how he used factors to make a twelfths strip. He explained that half of 12 is 6, and that half of 6 is 3, as he folded his strip in half, in half again to make fourths, and then in thirds to make twelfths. Whether Ms. Archer attended to three levels of units at these moments, however, remained unclear. When summarizing the activity of creating fraction strips, she referred to whole-number factor-product combinations two times, each time saying that even numbers were easier because you could start by folding the strip in half. These comments may have been influenced by a similar statement in the teacher’s edition and by Damien’s explanation. Ms. Archer did not discuss connections between wholenumber factor-product combinations and taking partitions of partitions more generally, but no task or comment in the CMP materials suggested that developing such connections was a central instructional goal. Her concluding remarks to the class emphasized that the strips were going to be used as measuring devices and that students should remember to find equal-sized parts of a whole, comments that emphasized two levels of units. Ms. Archer emphasized the same two points when discussing these initial lessons during her interviews. These data left open the possibility that Ms. Archer had capacities to reason with three levels of units that remained largely latent. Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 38 Two Strategies for Determining Fractions of Whole Numbers The next tasks in the CMP materials ask students to solve problems about fundraising using thermometers that show progress toward specified goals. The thermometers are also 8 1/2 inches so that students can measure them with their folded strips. Ms. Archer’s students began determining fractions of whole numbers when finding how many dollars had been raised at different points in several fund-raisers. Over the course of two and a half days, students worked on 11 such problems in groups and presented their solutions to the whole class. The vast majority of solutions relied on determining the dollar value of one equal part of the whole number (interpreting fractions as parts of a whole) and then using multiplication or repeated addition to determine the final answer. As one example, Angie determined 3/5 of $300 by finding that 1/5 was $60 and then calculating 3 x $60 = $180. Ms. Archer would use an analogous strategy, applying repeated addition or multiplication to a unit fraction, when introducing products of proper fractions during her subsequent enactment of Bits and Pieces II. One student, Damien, used an alternative strategy when determining 11/12 of $240. He found that one of 12 equal parts would be $20 but, instead of using repeated addition to build up to 11/12, he subtracted one part from the whole (i.e., $240 – $20 = $220). Ms. Archer first observed Damien’s approach while he worked at his desk and commented, “Oh. That is a good idea. Hold that thought so you all might share that with the class.” During subsequent whole-class discussion, Ms. Archer called on Damien to present his solution. Unfortunately, a debate emerged because some Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 39 students had got $216 as the final answer using tenths strips. Ms. Archer ran out of time and did not resume the discussion the next day. Nevertheless, that she complimented Damien and remembered to call on him at the end of class provided evidence that she understood his approach. As with class discussions about folding strips, whether Ms. Archer attended to three-level unit structures during class discussions about fractions of whole numbers remained unclear. She referred to just two levels when emphasizing a whole divided into equal parts but may have attended to a third level if, for example, she considered 300 divided into five groups, each of which consists of 60 ones. The data were ambiguous, however, because the smallest level of unit was the one, and she may not have attended to it explicitly. If she did attend to three-level structures here, data from subsequent lessons discussed below made clear that she did not always assemble threelevel structures readily. Thus, it is possible that Ms. Archer reasoned with three-level structures like that shown in Figure 2 but not in Figure 3. I will handle this uncertainty by describing Ms. Archer as reasoning primarily with two levels of units. Struggling to Respond to Students The final episode from Bits and Pieces I that I discuss occurred two weeks after those presented above. In the interim, we recorded two days during which students used thermometers to answer questions about fund-raising goals. The discussions focused on calculating fractions of whole numbers and emphasized numeric methods. Then Ms. Archer took several days off from work for personal reasons. When we Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 40 returned to her classroom, she had completed one lesson that used fraction strips to understand equivalent fractions. This lesson afforded opportunities to discuss nested levels of units but, if we missed such conversations, they would have been very different from those we observed our first day back. On that day, Ms. Archer and her students transferred the locations of fractions from all of the strips (halves, thirds, fourths, and so on up to twelfths) onto one number line. Students lined up the left end of their strips with the zero on the number line and made tick marks corresponding to each fold, but they did not make explicit relationships among different levels of units. Our second day back provided the first example in which we observed Ms. Archer attempt but then abort the use of drawings when responding to students’ questions. Students were comparing 2/3 to 3/4 to determine whether 2/3 was closer to 1/2 or to 1. Some said that 2/3 and 3/4 were the same distance from 1 because both were one piece away from making a whole. In an apparent attempt to direct students’ attention toward the size of each fraction, Ms. Archer drew two squares and emphasized that both were the same size. She subdivided the squares and shaded “three of four equal parts” and “two of the three,” again making explicit reference to two levels of units. Some students continued to say that the two fractions were the same. Rather than introduce a third level of unit that subdivided both thirds and fourths in her drawing, Ms. Archer switched to numeric computations, asked for the “least common multiple of 4 and 3,” and determined equivalent fractions. Figure 6 shows the white board as it appeared at the end of her explanation. Note that in writing 3 x 3/4, Ms. Archer asked how many times 4 went into 12 and then multiplied 3 times 3 Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 41 to get 9. Her discussion of 4 x 2/3 was similar. She then asked if 8/12 or 9/12 was bigger. Many students called out, “three fourths.” Figure 6 About Here These lesson data provided initial evidence that if Ms. Archer could produce three-level structures, she did not always do so readily. Further evidence that she did not consider three-level structures during the episode came from a subsequent interview. Ms. Archer watched an interview excerpt in which Emily and Angiev reviewed the lesson excerpt summarized above. The students were asked if, starting with 3/4, Ms. Archer could have made thirds. Angie explained that Ms. Archer “could not make thirds exactly” but could make twelfths. Ms. Archer commented, “She’s right. I could have, but I wasn’t thinking about that at the time.” Data from the initial Bits and Pieces I lessons, during which students created fractions strips, made clear that Ms. Archer connected whole-number multiplication to partitioning, but left unclear whether she attended to three levels of units. Data from this last lesson suggested that if she could produce three-level structures, they were not a central part of her mathematical knowledge for teaching. During the next three lessons that we recorded, Ms. Archer and her students continued using fraction strips to place fractions on number lines and answered such questions as how do you decide if a fraction is between 0 and 1/2, between 1/2 and 1, or greater than 1? The discussions focused on comparing numerators and Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 42 denominators. During the final two Bits and Pieces I lessons that we recorded, Ms. Archer and her students discussed computation methods for multiplying fractions and whole numbers that were based on repeated addition. The remaining Bits and Pieces I lessons that we did not record developed connections among fractions, decimals, and percents. Ms. Archer Uses Number Lines to Explain Products of Proper Fractions The Bits and Pieces II unit begins with lessons on estimating sums of fractions using comparisons to 0, 1/2, and 1 as a guide. The unit then introduces fraction addition through problems about buying and selling land in a rectangular town that is 1 mile by 2 miles. The task includes a map that shows the land divided among several people. Students are told that there are 640 acres in one square mile and are asked to find the fraction of one square mile and the number of acres that each person owns. The strategy Ms. Archer emphasized was similar to the primary one students used to answer questions about fund-raisers: Find how many acres correspond to an appropriate unit fraction and then use multiplication or repeated addition to determine the number acres a given person owns. Ms. Archer’s comments continued to emphasize equal parts of a whole (two levels of units) and that a fraction of a number means a fraction times a number. Immediately following these lessons, Ms. Archer used the number line to introduce products of proper fractions. Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 43 Using Two Levels of Units to Reason About Parts of Parts Bits and Pieces II intends for teachers and students to work first with rectangular regions when studying products of proper fractions, but Ms. Archer began instead with an example that used number lines. The problem was to find one fifth times two thirds and, as Ms. Archer explained in a subsequent interview, the drawing came from the teacher’s edition (see Figure 7). The problem was not situated in any particular context. Ms. Archer’s explanations of the solution, combined with her difficulties responding to a student’s question, provided stronger evidence than did the Bits and Pieces I data that she reasoned primarily with two levels of units Figure 7 About Here Ms. Archer began her explanation with the drawing already completed. She reminded her students that a fraction of a number means a fraction times the number and pointed out that the interval from 0 to 1 was divided into three parts. Next, she covered all but the first third, and students commented that it was divided into five parts. Ms. Archer then reminded students that the problem asked about two thirds: So we have one fifth here (pointing to the shaded fifth of the first third) and we have one fifth here (pointing to the shaded fifth of the second third.) But this one fifth (pointing to the shaded fifth of the first third) really is the same as what if we look across the whole number line? Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 44 Emily explained that the answer was one fifteenth because “in the whole number line there’s five equal parts in each section and you add them all together.” Ms. Archer agreed, pointed again to the first shaded piece, and said this “really would be one of fifteen equal pieces.” Although Emily’s comment suggested attention to three levels of units, Ms. Archer’s comments referred explicitly to two levels at a time, interpreting each shaded portion first as one fifth and then as one fifteenth. A moment later, Ms. Archer had trouble responding to one student. Jeff may not have attended to the distinction between one fifth of a whole and one fifth of a third because he stated, “If you wanted one fifth, it would change to, it would be like three fifteenths.” Jeff’s tone implied confusion, perhaps about discussing one shaded piece both as one fifth and as one fifteenth. Ms. Archer did not address Jeff’s confusion directly. Instead, she read a sentence on her overhead that also appears in the teacher’s edition: “Each one fifth of a third is one fifteenth so that two parts marked would be two fifteenths.” (I did not use these data to attribute three-level unit structures to Ms. Archer because the explanation came verbatim from the teacher’s edition.) Jeff still seemed confused when he stated once more that one fifth would be three fifteenths. Ms. Archer may have thought Jeff attended to all three thirds because she stressed that this problem required looking at just the first two. She then confirmed the answer by multiplying numerators together and denominators together. Ms. Archer concluded her demonstration by stating that “the number line shows why” and moved on to lessons that introduced addition and subtraction of fractions and mixed numbers. The methods Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 45 Ms. Archer and her students discussed during these subsequent lessons were numeric and so provided little access to their unit structures. Persistent Difficulty Responding to Jeff The lesson data left unclear why Ms. Archer inserted the number line example, what she understood about using the number line to solve fraction multiplication problems, and what she meant by “showing why.” Furthermore, from my point of view, the classroom discussion appeared to leave Jeff’s question unanswered. I interviewed Jeff and Emilyvi about this lesson excerpt and, during her next interview, Ms. Archer watched portions of the lesson and the student interview. To prepare Ms. Archer for watching the lesson excerpt, the interviewer asked, “What was your goal with showing the multiplication at this point?” She answered “to give [students] a taste of what we’re going to be doing and to show them how there are other ways that things can be done.” She explained that her students already knew where 1/3 was on the number line and how to “divide things into parts.” Finally, she said that she had not known how to use number lines to multiply fractions and found the example interesting. These comments summarized her reasons for inserting the number line example and suggested why she did not have students work further problems as part of the lesson. When the interviewer showed the lesson excerpt, including Jeff’s comment that 1/5 was the same as 3/15, Ms. Archer had difficulty relating three levels of units appropriately. Talking over the video, she interjected: Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 46 Maybe I should’ve said sixth fifteenths to let him know we talking about three fifteenths plus three fifteenths. Yeah. And that would’ve helped him to understand that now I’m listening to it.…He’s right. That’s three fifteenths, but we’re concerned about two thirds. So therefore we have to do three fifteenths plus three fifteenths, which is six fifteenths. And when you reduce it, it still gonna give you two fifteenths. When the interviewer followed up by asking, “Where are the three fifteenths you think he’s thinking of?,” Ms. Archer first suggested that Jeff might be thinking of a fraction equivalent to 1/5, then suggested that he might be thinking of a common denominator for 1/3 and 1/5, and finally said she did not know. One possible explanation for Ms. Archer’s comments is that she understood the one fifths to be of the whole and, in a momentary lapse, assumed that 6/15 could be reduced to 2/15. A second possible explanation is that she understood each one fifth to be of one third and meant three fifteenths of one third plus three fifteenths of one third makes six fifteenths of one third. Either way, the possibility that Ms. Archer could establish an explicit three-level structure in which five fifteenths make up each of three thirds of a whole seemed less likely a moment later: When the interviewer suggested that Jeff might be confused by the distinction between a fifth of a whole and a fifth of a third, Ms. Archer asserted once more that 3/15 plus 3/15 made 6/15 which “would give you two fifteenths.” The next video clip that the interviewer played came from an interview I conducted with Jeff and Emily. In preparation for watching the student interview, Ms. Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 47 Archer solved the extension problem 3/5 of 2/3, correctly. She first calculated 3/5 x 2/3 = 6/15 and 3/15 + 3/15 = 6/15, and then shaded three fifths of the first third and three fifths of the second third. Next, Ms. Archer watched portions of the student interview during which Jeff and Emily first produced incorrect solutions to 3/5 of 2/3, also on the number line, and then reviewed the original lesson segment. (Their mistakes are tangential to the present analysis.) At one point, I asked the students why Ms. Archer shaded the way she did. Jeff proposed rearranging the two shaded pieces side by side in the first third and explained, “You’re really thinking of two thirds, I mean, two fifths of a third.” When Ms. Archer was asked, “Do you think that would work as well?,” she replied “No” and explained: This is one third by itself (pointing to the first third) and this is another third by itself (pointing to the second third). And the concept is, was then one fifth times two of three parts. So, you gotta have the two of three parts. You can’t just put both of them down in there, then you won’t get what you’re supposed to have. Ms. Archer insisted that each third be treated separately once more near the end of the interview. When the interviewer asked how she would build on what the students appeared to understand after one lesson on fraction multiplication with number lines, Ms. Archer said give other examples and that “if it was gonna be three fifths (i.e., 3/5 of 2/3), then you gotta do three sections of two separate things.” Thus, in both examples, Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 48 Ms. Archer appeared to use the parts-of-a-whole interpretation twice in succession— once to establish thirds and once to establish parts of one third—and then applied repeated addition to the result. Note that this interpretation of Ms. Archer’s reasoning is consistent with that of her reasoning for fractions of whole numbers during her Bits and Pieces I lessons. Ms. Archer’s rejection of Jeff’s alternative approach to 1/5 of 2/3, was in contrast to compliments she paid Damien several weeks earlier when he devised a different method for solving 11/12 of $240. A quick thought experiment based on these data illustrates potential connections between teachers’ unit structures and using drawings to adapt. Flexibility with just two levels of units might suffice to adapt in response to Damien’s method but not to Jeff’s. Understanding that Jeff’s proposed diagram for 2/5 of 1/3 could also be interpreted as 1/5 of 2/3 would require sufficient flexibility with three-level unit structures to see a length of 2/3 divided into ten equal pieces, the first two of which were shaded. Recognizing that Jeff’s proposed diagram and Ms. Archer’s could both represent 1/5 of 2/3 would require additional flexibility supported by a visual understanding of the distributive property formally symbolized here as: 1/5 x 2/3 = 1/5 x 1/3 + 1/5 x 1/3. Ms. Archer, however, reported earlier during the same interview that she had not thought about the distributive property during the original lesson. Finally, as a brief aside during the middle of the interview, Ms. Archer was asked about “the value of showing [students] both the algorithm and the diagram.” She explained, “I think the diagram showed to me, it showed them why one fifth times two Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 49 third is two fifteenths, you know, and I just wanted to show them why.” In the next exchange she continued: [The number line] is just another way of doing things, you know, which I’ve often told them there’s more than one way to do math problems. You know, if it, that might help some of the students to understand things better. They may wanna do it that way. What Ms. Archer meant by “why” remained unclear at this point, but her tendency to compute answers before using lengths to represent parts of parts, combined with her difficulties producing three levels of units appropriately, suggested that she would have trouble using drawings for purposes other than illustrating solutions first arrived at through alternative numeric methods, the first use for drawings discussed above. Data presented in the next section provided further evidence that her difficulties with three-level unit structures precluded further uses for drawings. Ms. Archer Uses Rectangular Regions to Explain Products of Proper Fractions Ten days later Ms. Archer arrived at the CMP lessons intended to introduce products of proper fractions. She continued telling students that the drawings “show why” but initially blurred the distinction between part of a part and part of a whole when interpreting problems as finding one fraction times another. These data suggested that Ms. Archer continued to focus primarily on just two levels of units to reason about fraction multiplication, and they continued to raise questions about what she meant when saying that drawings “show why.” Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 50 Blurring Part of a Part and Part of a Whole The CMP materials use rectangular regions to introduce products of proper fractions and begin with the example 1/2 of 2/3. Ms. Archer began her introduction with just a few minutes left in the class period. She presented the problem as 1/2 times 2/3 and had students tell her to multiply the numerators together and the denominators together. Having computed 2/6, she then showed her students two transparencies, one showing a unit square partitioned into thirds with two parts shaded and one showing a second unit square partitioned into halves with one part shaded. She laid one unit square over the other as shown in Figure 8a, reproducing a drawing in the teacher’s edition. She asked students how many parts were in the square and told students to “look at the area that has the most lines in it.” This direction was similar to one in the teacher’s edition to look at where “the shaded sections overlap.” Figure 8 About Here Ms. Archer returned to the same example the next day, reproduced the drawing of two thirds from the previous lesson, and asked the class how to show two thirds times one half. Several of the students’ struggled, and their attempts suggested that they viewed both two thirds and one half as being of one whole. Ms. Archer stated once more that the drawing in Figure 8a showed “why,” and told students to attend to “the condensed area where the lines cross.” Jeff, the same student who asked about the number line, asked what to do with the remaining three shaded parts. In her response, Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 51 Ms. Archer began referring to the double shaded region as “half of two thirds,” but Jeff remained confused. A few moments later, Ms. Archer had students work on a series of problems in the book that used squares to represent brownie pans and that asked for parts of parts. As Ms. Archer worked with students at their desks, a few wanted to show 1/2 of 2/3 by double shading one of their thirds (similar to Figure 8c). At first Ms. Archer redirected these students towards drawings similar to the one shown in Figure 8a, but later she demonstrated before the entire class two ways to show 1/2 of 2/3. The first solution was based on cross partitioning but showed more clearly that the 1/2 was just of the 2/3, not the whole (see Figure 8b); the second solution double shaded one third (see Figure 8c). Ms. Archer used hand gestures to show that the two “parts” from the first method could be rearranged to coincide with “half” in the second method. Thus, for this problem, she apparently adapted her perspective on parts of parts in response to her students’ thinking and may have reasoned with three levels of units. For the balance of the lesson, students used rectangular regions to determine 3/4 of 1/2. Classroom data collection ended here because the school year was about to end, and Ms. Archer moved on to fraction division. Adaptive Representation of Parts of Parts Begins to Emerge In conjunction with the member of the CoSTAR project who was interviewing Ms. Archer, I planned a final interview to examine whether Ms. Archer might accept as correct further ways that her students represented parts of parts and what she meant Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 52 when she told students that drawings show “why.” The prompts included reproductions of students’ class work for 3/4 of 1/2 shown in Figure 9 and excerpts from the final round of student interviews. Ms. Archer’s responses suggested strongly that when examining students’ work she focused primarily on whether the picture showed part of a part and the correct numerical answer. I emphasize that Ms. Archer’s use of the part-of-a-part language during her final interview may have been shaped by her experiences helping students during the most recent lessons. Thus, I did not use these data to infer understandings she may have used in prior lessons. Figure 9 About Here The interviewer asked Ms. Archer to review the set of examples in Figure 9 and to indicate which ones students could use “to understand why the answer to three fourths of one half is three eighths.” Ms. Archer indicated the first or second. She stated that Angie’s method (Figure 9a) was what she had taught, but that Damien’s (Figure 9b) was also possible. These data suggested that Ms. Archer now accepted as correct more than one representation of parts of parts, but initially she rejected Theo’s solution (Figure 9c): That’s not right. Wait a minute. Four, eight, twelve, sixteen (counting parts in the whole). That’s not right. Well, hold up. Three, sixteen. Two, four, six, eight, sixteen. Four, eight, wait, four, eight, twelve, sixteen. Two, Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 53 four, six (counting double shaded parts). Six, sixteen; three, eight. Okay. All right. I see what he did. After a few exchanges about the first two examples, Ms. Archer considered Theo’s work once more, “One, two, three, four, five, six of sixteen and that’s three eighths. Yeah.” Rather than analyzing relations among nested levels of units to see if Theo had constructed 3/4 of 1/2, Ms. Archer apparently relied on reducing his numerical answer to the one she knew. For the balance of the interview, the interviewer replayed excerpts and showed written work from the final round of student interviews. A primary goal for the student interviews was to determine how they might extend Ms. Archer’s demonstrated solution for 1/2 of 2/3 to other examples. Ms. Archer’s response to data from the student interviews provided some evidence that she was beginning to attend more flexibly to parts of parts and evidence for what she might mean by “why.” During one interview, Damien and Theo solved 2/5 of 3/4 using brownie pans. Figure 10a shows how Damien first divided the square into four parts. When he said that he was not sure how to “divide into five,” Theo suggested drawing vertical lines. Damien built on Theo’s suggestion, subdivided each fourth into five parts (Figure 10b), and got stuck once more. Finally, Theo suggested taking two parts from each of the three shaded fourths (Figure 10c) and explained that the answer would be 6/20 because there are “four pieces divided up into five.” For me, the students’ drawings and explanations evidenced their production of three levels of units and distributive reasoning. Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 54 Figure 10 About Here The interviewer set up the clip by simply asking Ms. Archer to watch the students work together on 2/5 of 3/4. At the point that Damien subdivided the fourths into five parts (Figure 10b), Ms. Archer talked over the video and interjected, “Since he’s done it that way, it’ll work, you know. He’s just gotta shade in two of each of those fifths, which would give him, like, six twentieths, I think.” Although Ms. Archer also evidenced distributive reasoning when anticipating Theo's suggestion, she still had trouble describing the drawing appropriately. When the interviewer followed up by asking, “Does that show why?,” Ms. Archer answered, “That is six twentieths of three fourths. Yeah. That’s right.” For the next example, the interviewer simply showed Ms. Archer a reproduction of Emily’s work on the same problem. Emily partitioned the brownie pan vertically into four parts and shaded three. She then repartitioned the three shaded fourths vertically into five parts and double shaded two of the resulting parts (Figure 10d). Although Emily’s construction showed part of a part correctly, her resulting drawing made it difficult if not impossible to relate the double shaded region to the whole. When asked if Emily’s approach was useful for determining “why it works,” Ms. Archer thought for 15 seconds before commenting: Yeah. ‘Cause all she’s done is shown, what is that? One, two, I think, it’s gonna be, what, three fifths? No, no, no, no. What is it? [Ms. Archer Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 55 reduced 6/20 to 3/10 in the margin] …. Let’s see. How could she get three tenths out of this? One, two. Well, yeah, I guess it could work because if you count, it should be ten across when you get through counting and then she would’ve done, this probably would be three if she put another thing here. You know what I’m saying? And, that would be the same concept. That Ms. Archer sought to get a known numerical answer “out of” the drawing gave a clearer picture than did previous data of what she meant by “why.” Her comments focused on whether Emily might have arrived at the correct answer, not on whether Emily’s drawing would allow her to relate multiple levels of nested units. These data are consistent with using drawings to illustrate solutions also arrived at through an alternate method, the first pedagogical use for drawings discussed above. To summarize, Ms. Archer may have produced three-level structures in a limited set of situations, but the data suggested strongly that, if she did, such structures were not central to her mathematical knowledge for teaching fraction arithmetic. This, in turn, precluded further uses of drawn representations discussed above. Ms. Archer’s response to Damien’s method for determining 11/12 of $240 demonstrated that she could adapt in response to a student’s reasoning in a case where reasoning primarily with two levels of units was sufficient. Moreover, her focus on two levels of units, her connection between a fraction of a number and times the number, and her connection between multiplication and repeated addition were sufficient to use drawn Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 56 representations for illustrating solutions to several fraction multiplication problems from the Bits and Pieces I and Bits and Pieces II units. Although Ms. Archer became somewhat more adaptive in response to her students’ reasoning with parts of parts at the end of her enactment of Bits and Pieces II, data from both units provided strong evidence that she did not produce appropriate three-level structures when responding to her students’ reasoning across a range of situations. Results on Ms. Reese from Bits and Pieces II In analyzing data on Ms. Archer, a central challenge was determining whether she reasoned with a third level of unit. The data on Ms. Reese’s unit structures were clearer. Recall that she used a slightly different draft revision of the Bits and Pieces II unit than did Ms. Archer. Only the version Ms. Reese used included lessons on adding and subtracting fractions using the number line. Ms. Reese’s enactment of these lessons provided several examples in which she used recursive partitioning, and hence reasoned with three levels of units. vii Her multiplication lessons provided additional examples in which she used recursive partitioning. Furthermore, her capacity to reason with units was sufficient to infer a computation procedure based on answers to numerous problems, the second pedagogical use for drawings discussed above. Yet, she still had difficulty responding to some of the ways that her students produced nested unit structures. Here the central challenge for analysis was determining whether Ms. Reese attended to drawn versions of distribution. I proceed chronologically through her six lessons on products of fractions. Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 57 Representing Parts of Parts More Than One Way Using Rectangular Regions In contrast to Ms. Archer, Ms. Reese introduced products of proper fractions as the CMP materials intended—with brownie pans. She began with the example 1/2 of 2/3 and a drawing similar to that used by Ms. Archer. She introduced squares as drawings of brownie pans, asked students how to draw two thirds of the brownie pan, and followed a student’s suggestion to partition the square horizontally (Figure 11a). She then asked how to represent 1/2 of 2/3, and one student said that half would be one third because “to get two thirds you have to have one third plus one third.” Ms. Reese accepted the solution immediately and said half of two thirds would be one of the two shaded pieces (note the similarity to Figure 8c). She told students that the goal was to figure out how to solve this problem using an algorithm and proceeded to divide the square vertically (Figure 11b). Ms. Reese then asked students to “shade half” and after a moment shaded half of the entire rectangle (Figure 11c). Like Ms Archer, Ms. Reese reproduced the drawing in the teacher’s edition. She said that the double shaded part represented half of two thirds and emphasized that there were no brownies in bottom left hand corner. She then asked what the two pieces stood for as a fraction, and a student said two sixths. Ms. Reese wrote “1/2 of 2/3 is 2/6,” asked if the two solutions were the same, and called on students until one said that 2/6 could be reduced to 1/3. Ms. Reese finished the lesson with a second brownie pan problem that demonstrated 3/4 of 1/2 is 3/8. Figure 11 About Here Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 58 Struggling to Respond to Students’ Approaches Although Ms. Reese accepted different student approaches for representing 1/2 of 2/3, she had trouble responding to students’ reasoning the very next day. The central issue was creating the first partition of the brownie pan. Ms. Reese reviewed the previous lesson and had students work at their desks on the following problems: 1/3 of 5/6, 2/3 of 3/4, 1/4 of 1/3, and 1/4 of 2/5. At one point, she worked with a student whose representation for 1/3 of 5/6 was similar to that shown in Figure 12c. The student explained that the answer was 2/6 because the whole was broken into three pairs of smaller rectangles. Even though she arrived at an incorrect answer, the student may have reasoned with three levels of units if she took the whole pan as a unit that was divided into thirds, each of which was further divided in half, making sixths. Figure 12 About Here Ms. Reese responded, “Something is going on here that is not right. I have got to think about this a minute.” She questioned whether the student had shaded 5/6 and counted the shaded pieces to check. That Ms. Reese counted the five shaded pieces suggested that she was genuinely stuck. The student continued to explain when Ms. Reese interrupted, “Oh. I gotcha. OK. So you just didn’t divide it up again. You just put like this is one third (pointed to two pieces), that’s one third (pointed to another two Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 59 pieces), and that’s one third (pointed to the last two pieces). OK. That’s fine.” Apparently, Ms. Reese attended to three levels of units, even though she did not address the problem with the student’s work. A few moments later, Ms. Reese observed that another student had a similar incorrect solution and stopped the class so that she could compare two strategies for determining 1/3 of 5/6. The following discussion suggested that she may have figured out, at least in part, what was wrong. Ms. Reese began by telling the class that it was alright to have different answers so long as they were equivalent fractions. She demonstrated her solution first, pointing out that she drew lines in just one direction to partition the brownie pan into sixths (Figure 12a). She shaded five pieces, said she wanted to take a third of just those pieces, and apparently attempted to produce three levels of units when she continued: I am thinking out loud. I am acting like a kid. I was thinking at first when I saw this, well, maybe two of these could make up one of the thirds where that would be like one third (pointed to the top two pieces), that would be two thirds (pointed to the next two pieces) but Oh Oh (pointed to the fifth shaded piece) there’s only one left. So that’s not going to work. Ms. Reese used this impasse as motivation for partitioning her brownie pan vertically into thirds (Figure 12b). She shaded the first third of the whole, and reminded students Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 60 that the bottom piece was not included. She counted five double shaded pieces and, with some prompting about the size of the pieces, students said the answer was 5/18. Ms. Reese then recounted the alternative solution by partitioning a second brownie pan as shown in Figure 12c and told the class that she remembered reading in the teacher’s guide that “it wont always work if you divide like this,” meaning cross partitioning for the first fraction.viii Although comparing the two approaches provided an opportunity to talk about taking a third of each sixth, and hence a drawn version of distribution, Ms. Reese did not do so. Instead, she had students examine the class solutions thus far: 1/2 of 2/3 is 2/6, 3/4 of 1/2 is 3/8, and 1/3 of 5/6 is 5/18. After students offered a tentative pattern based on multiplying numerators together and denominators together, Ms. Reese asked if the second proposed solution to 1/3 of 5/6 (Figure 12c) fit the pattern. Some students commented that 2/6 was equivalent to 6/18. Ms. Reese pointed out that the two methods gave truly different answers but did not state which one was correct. (Ms. Reese reported working problems in her lessons ahead of time and probably knew the correct answer.) This discussion provided initial evidence of Ms. Reese’s use of drawings to infer a computation method, the second use described above. As Ms. Reese had students continue to work with their partners on the remaining problems, one discussion about drawing 2/3 of 3/4 suggested constraints on her attention to parts of parts. Jack and Kate had similar work (Figure 13a and 13b). Kate explained that she had divided her brownie pan into fourths. To me it appeared that Kate had continued by shading two of the three fourths, correctly, but Ms. Reese Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 61 commented that Kate was going to run into the same problem illustrated in Figure 12c and had her start over. Jack partitioned a new brownie pan into fourths all in one direction. As he did so, Ms. Reese commented, “OK. We need to take two thirds of that. You might be able to answer it right there like that. I’m not sure.” When Jack shaded two of the three fourths (Figure 13c), Ms. Reese said, “That will work.” Figure 13 About Here Ms. Reese may have overlooked Kate’s correct solution to 2/3 of 3/4 because she was genuinely stuck when two students found that 1/3 of 5/6 was 2/6 and because the teacher’s edition warned against cross partitioning for the first fraction. The data evidenced Ms. Reese’s capacity to reason with three level of units in some situations but left unclear whether she attended to drawn versions of distribution. The third day, Ms. Reese and the students used area models to solve several more problems by partitioning horizontally for one fraction, partitioning vertically for the second, and determining how many parts were in the whole and in the part of a part. The day after that, she introduced linear representations for fraction multiplication. I discuss those data next because the final interview with Ms. Reese examined the area examples just discussed and a linear example discussed below. Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 62 Products of Fractions Using Thermometers Recall that Ms. Archer skipped the Bits and Pieces II lessons that used thermometers to develop fraction multiplication because she was running out of days in the school year. In contrast, Ms. Reese used thermometers to find several examples of parts of parts and to infer a computation method. In so doing, she used recursive partitioning, and thus produced three levels of units. Furthermore, she clearly attended to drawn instantiations of the distributive property, even though she did not discuss the property explicitly with her students. These lesson data raised questions about Ms. Reese’s attention to drawn versions of distribution. Recursive Partitioning and Using Drawn Representations to Infer a Computation Method As she had done with rectangular regions, Ms. Reese had students use lengths to determine parts of parts. When students came upon apparent counterexamples to the emerging numerical pattern based on products of numerators and products of denominators, she had them consider equivalent fractions. The first problem Ms. Reese posed was couched in a context where students had raised two thirds of their $720 fundraising goal in four days. Ms. Reese sketched a fundraising thermometer and shaded two thirds. She asked students for the fraction that had been raised each day. One student suggested dividing the shaded two thirds into four parts, and Ms. Reese did so (Figure 14a). Another student said the answer was one sixth because the unshaded part should be divided into two pieces. Ms. Reese partitioned the unshaded part and wrote “1/4 of 2/3 = 1/6” underneath. Although this answer appeared Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 63 inconsistent with the emerging pattern, she solved the problem a second time using rectangular areas to show that 1/4 of 2/3 was also 2/12 (Figure 14b). Students then explained that 1/6 and 2/12 were equivalent fractions. Figure 14 About Here For the second and third problems, Ms. Reese led the class through a similar sequence of steps, and the questions she posed suggested attention to recursive partitioning. For the second problem, 1/3 of 1/2, she partitioned one half into three parts and had a student explain that the second half should also be divided into three parts to see that 1/3 of 1/2 is 1/6. For the third problem, 2/5 of 1/2, she partitioned one half into five parts, shaded two, and had a student explain that the second half should also be divided into five parts to see that 2/5 of 1/2 is 2/10. Attention to Drawn Instantiations of the Distributive Property During her fourth lesson, Ms. Reese’s comments evidenced attention to the distributive property in the context of finding a fraction of a mixed number. The problem was to determine 1/3 of 2 1/2 pounds of cheese. Ms. Reese drew three thermometers, shaded two and half, and divided each piece into thirds (Figure 15a). A student suggested dividing everything into sixths, which Ms. Reese did. Ms. Reese then commented that she needed “to get a third of each thing.” She shaded a third of the half and, as a rhetorical move to invite students’ contributions, said that she was not sure Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 64 how to get a third of the wholes. A student suggested shading two pieces, and Ms. Reese shaded two pieces of each whole (Figure 15b). Ms. Reese counted five double shaded pieces, and students said the pieces were sixths. Figure 15 About Here Ms. Reese’s instruction was interrupted by state testing. Afterwards, several lessons relied largely on numeric methods to develop products of mixed numbers. The one place Ms. Reese used drawings was to help students convert mixed numbers to improper fractions. Two weeks after the cheese example, when reviewing for test, Ms. Reese used lengths and rectangular areas for determining products of fractions once more. During the review, she may still not have attended to distribution in the context of rectangular areas: She stopped a student who began working on 1/3 of 5/6 by cross partitioning a brownie pan into sixths and said, “Remember we had a long discussion in class about why you cannot cut them this way and this way because your answer might be close to the right answer but it is not going to be the right answer.” Classroom data collection ended here because the school year was about to end, and Ms. Reese moved on to fraction division. Ms. Reese Explains Her Lessons and the Distributive Property During Interviews When analyzing lesson and interview data, I pursued two hypotheses for why Ms. Reese directed students away from cross partitioning brownie pans for just one Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 65 fraction even in cases where students appeared, from my perspective, to progress towards correct answers. The first was that the incorrect 1/3 of 5/6 is 2/6 solution, combined with comments in the teacher’s edition, were sufficient for Ms. Reese to understand that partitioning horizontally for one fraction and vertically for the second was more reliable, even if she was still figuring out why. The second was that Ms. Reese understood that the distributive property could be used to make either approach to partitioning work but did not want to have that discussion with her students. That Ms. Reese demonstrated a drawn instantiation of the distributive property in the subsequent cheese problem context, combined with comments that she wanted to direct students toward multiplying numerators and multiplying denominators with a minimum of confusion raised the possibility that she had, but chose not to use, relevant knowledge. Data from the final interview did not allow clear discrimination between these hypotheses but did provide evidence for three claims. First, Ms. Reese was less confident about drawing versions of the distributive property in some contexts than others. The interviewer first asked about two examples from lessons in which students used numeric methods incorrectly when multiplying mixed numbers. In both cases, the students only multiplied the whole numbers together and the fractions together (e.g., 3 1/3 x 4 2/3 = 12 2/9). Ms. Reese recognized the mistake as one that several students in each of her classes had made and at one point asked, “So how could we represent that with pictures to come up with a …?” These data demonstrated constraints on Ms. Reese’s understandings of drawn versions of distribution, at least in cases of the (a + b)(c Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 66 + d) pattern. The interviewer reminded Ms. Reese that they had discussed how to use rectangular regions for this purpose earlier in the year during a professional development session. (Recall that the CoSTAR project conducted professional development for teachers at Pierce Middle School). After a discussion about what sixth-grade students might be able to understand about the distributive property, the interview brought up the cheese problem discussed above and asked, “What is one thing you might hope that [students] do?” Ms. Reese’s answer was consistent with explanations she gave during the lesson summarized above and appeared to reflect a much clearer understanding about drawn versions of the distributive property in this context: I would hope that they would see that, first of all, if you’ve got two wholes and a half more, that they, a third of that amount total, so you could make it easy and do a third of the half and figure that out, and then you could do a third of the two wholes and figure that out and combine those two. Second, by the time of the interview, Ms. Reese could describe the role of distribution in the 1/3 of 5/6 example but expressed reservations about discussing this with students. The interviewer set up the lesson excerpt described above by simply saying, “You put four problems: 1/3 of 5/6, 2/3 of 3/4, 1/4 of 1/3 and 1/4 of 2/5 and then you start moving around the room.” He then showed the excerpt in which a student used brownie pans to determine that 1/3 of 5/6 is 2/6. Ms. Reese first recalled, “We talked about why it gets, you get close to the answer but its not exactly the right Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 67 answer because you’re not dividing the pieces in the right number of pieces.” An exchange later she elaborated: “So she’s not actually taking a third of each piece of the five sixths.” Ms. Reese restated this explanation at a few other points during the same interview. Although she understood why the student’s approach did not work by the time of her last interview, Ms. Reese also commented, “I wasn’t really sure what she was thinking. I was thinking well this isn’t 5/6, well yes it is 5/6. Well why, you know, well she’s taking two pieces of the whole thing but not of the 5/6.” This comment lent some weight to the hypothesis that during the initial lesson Ms. Reese did not use her understanding of distribution to diagnose the student’s difficulty. Third, Ms. Reese appeared still to be developing flexibility with three levels of units when the interview replayed the lesson excerpt in which Jack and Kate worked on 2/3 of 3/4. The interviewer asked, “Would it have been okay if they had started with 3/4 doing horizontally and vertically?” Ms. Reese looked at Jack’s initial work reproduced in Figure 13a and stated, “As long as they had made each piece into thirds and shaded two of each piece. But see, he just shaded all of that.” Ms. Reese described taking two pieces from each fourth several other times. When the interviewer asked if one needs to divide each fourth into three pieces, Ms. Reese first said “Yes,” then asked for clarification of the question, thought for a moment, and finally pointed out that Jack could have taken just two of the three shaded pieces. These data suggested that Ms. Reese’s ability to produce three levels of units, though sufficient to infer numeric methods, was not sufficiently flexible to respond quickly and securely to the variety of ways that her students might assemble such structures during lessons. Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 68 To summarize, in contrast to Ms. Archer, Ms. Reese apparently employed threelevel unit structures effectively at several points during her lessons, for instance when motivating the need to partition rectangular regions in separate directions for each fraction (Figure 12) and when recursively partitioning linear representations. Thus, reasoning with three-level unit structures appeared to be part of her mathematical knowledge for teaching fraction multiplication with drawings. At the same time, the knowledge remained tacit and was used only in some contexts. Ms. Reese’s use of three-level structures was sufficient to use drawings for inferring a general numeric method for multiplying proper fractions with proper fractions and proper fractions with improper fractions. Although she attended to drawn versions of distribution when reasoning with linear representations, attending to drawn versions of distribution also when reasoning with area representations would have supported greater flexibility with three-level unit structures. Such flexibility could have supported, in turn, fuller adaptation in response to the range of ways her students began to assemble such structures. Discussion Analyses of data on Ms. Archer and Ms. Reese led to a frame for describing mathematical knowledge for teaching in one domain, fraction multiplication. The frame was informed by a perspective on knowledge that emphasizes diverse, fine-grained knowledge elements. The three categories—numerical aspects of multiplication, unit Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 69 structures, and pedagogical purposes for using drawings—highlight the diversity. Comparing the range of structures identified in the present study to a single construct such as the “meaning of multiplication with fractions” used by Ma (1999, p. 77) illustrates the finer grain size. I emphasize that describing such diverse, fine-grained knowledge elements was motivated primarily by the desire to explain why the two teachers adapted in response to their students’ thinking more readily in some situations than others. The frame emphasizes relations between teachers’ unit structures and purposes for using drawings. That Ms. Archer attended primarily to two levels of units explained why she could adapt in response to Damien’s strategy for determining 11/12 of $240 but not to Jeff’s questions about fraction multiplication on the number line. Furthermore, when enacting the Bits and Pieces I and Bits and Pieces II units, her primary attention to just two levels of units constrained the pedagogical purposes for which she might use drawings to illustrating numeric answers. Ms. Reese provided clearer evidence of reasoning with three levels of units, and she could apparently use lengths and areas to determine answers without using numeric computations as a support. Although her facility with three-level unit structures was sufficient to infer a numeric method for computing products of fractions, she was not sufficiently flexible to adapt in response to some examples of her students’ reasoning. Thus, her capacity to reason with fractional quantities also placed constraints on the pedagogical purposes for which she might use drawings, including building general numeric methods from students’ strategies as called for by the CMP materials. Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 70 Taken together, the two case studies suggest that teachers’ capacity to produce three levels of units is necessary but not sufficient for interpreting and evaluating the variety of ways that students might begin to evidence multi-level structures when using drawings to reason about fraction multiplication situations. Attention to drawn versions of distribution is also essential. This result, combined with that reported by Behr, Khoury, et al. (1997) in which preservice elementary teachers found it hard to distribute fractions as operators across their conceptual units (see discussion above), raises questions for future research about the extent to which teachers recognize the distributive property when it is not represented symbolically as a(b + d) = ab + ad. The ability to recognize distribution when represented in different ways seems an important aspect of mathematical knowledge for teaching whole-number and fraction arithmetic with drawings and manipulatives. Further research should continue examining the extent to which teachers recognize distribution when represented in different ways. Explicit, flexible attention to three levels of units may be central mathematical knowledge for teaching further topics where fractions are treated as quantities. For instance, the examples in which Ms. Archer struggled to compare 2/3 and 3/4 (Figure 6) and to recognize Theo’s strategy for representing 3/4 of 1/2 (Figure 9c) illustrate how attending to only two levels of units can hamper using drawings to generate equivalent fractions. The capacity to generate equivalent fractions, in turn, is central to comparing, adding, and subtracting pairs of fractions with unlike denominators. Again, Izsák et al. (in press) analyze the role of recursive partitioning, and hence three levels of units, in Ms. Reese’s fraction addition and subtraction lessons. Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 71 Explicit, flexible attention to three levels of units can also support using length or area quantities to determine answers to fraction division problems. Here, flexible reasoning with nested levels of units requires not only attention to drawn versions of distribution but also the capacity to make different choices of unit for each level as a solution unfolds. To see this, consider again the problem used by Ball (1990) and Ma (1999) discussed above. To generate a situation that illustrates 1 3/4 ÷ 1/2, one might ask how many halves are in 1 3/4 and generate an initial unit structure in which an interval of length 1 3/4 is the largest unit, the interval from 0 to 1 is the mid-level unit, and intervals of length 1/2 are the smallest unit. Because the intervals of length 1/2 do not partition the length of 1 3/4 evenly, answering how many halves are in 1 3/4 requires a new unit structure in which a length of 1 3/4 is again the largest unit, but intervals of length 1/2 are now the mid-level unit and intervals of length 1/4 the smallest unit. At this stage, the interval from 0 to 1 moves to the background and is important primarily for describing sizes of pieces, not multiplicative relations among the levels used directly to answer the question. Answering how many halves are in 1 3/4 requires recognizing that the smallest interval is half of mid-level interval and so there are 3 1/2. Sowder et al. (1998) reported a group of middle-grades teachers who had trouble making such choices for units when using drawings to reason through the problem 1 ÷ 3/5 (pp. 56-59). Future research on teachers’ mathematical knowledge for teaching whole and rational number arithmetic, particularly in contexts where numbers are treated as measures of quantities, should examine the extent to which other Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 72 populations of teachers also have trouble reasoning explicitly and flexibly with nested levels of units. Results of the present study extend past research on teachers’ mathematical knowledge not only by examining how they employ unit structures when responding to students over sequences of lessons, but also by teasing apart different pedagogical purposes for which teachers might use drawings. That little is known about teachers’ pedagogical purposes for using drawings is a significant omission in research on teacher knowledge, especially given the increasing use of reform-oriented materials in classrooms. Results from this study raise the possibility that curriculum developers, teachers, and researchers may not be communicating clearly about the role drawn representations can play in reform-oriented mathematics instruction. Recall that the CMP materials suggested three of the four purposes for using drawings discussed as part of the theoretical frame. Meanwhile, comments made by Ms. Archer, Ms. Reese, and other teachers with whom the CoSTAR project has worked suggest that they understand the purpose of multiple representations and strategies to be providing alternatives from which each student can understand and use just one. This is different from having students work with multiple representations to develop an integrated knowledge base, for instance by using drawings to develop meaning for operations with formal symbolic notations. The notions of illustrating, inferring, deducing, and adapting may serve as useful tools for analyzing the design of reform-oriented materials and for future research on teaching and learning with such materials. Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 73 Finally, methods used in the present study may be of interest to those studying teachers’ knowledge of other topics in mathematics. The methods do not replace those used in other studies of teachers’ knowledge, but they do respond differently to some of the challenges studying mathematical knowledge that teachers use in practice. Some of these challenges are that (a) classroom observations alone can make it hard to discriminate between competing hypotheses about what knowledge a teacher may be using, (b) teachers’ self-reports of what they and their students think and do during lessons may be unreliable, and (c) teachers may use knowledge when responding to items on survey instruments designed to measure teachers’ knowledge, but not use that knowledge when responding to their students’ thinking during lessons. The methods used in the present study start with classroom events, particularly those where teachers appear to have trouble responding to their students’ thinking, and then try to gather further evidence for the knowledge that teachers may be using. Interviews provide additional data on students’ reasoning both about the original task discussed in class and about extensions of those tasks. Further interviews provide data on teachers’ reasoning about the original task discussed in class, about extensions of those tasks, and about their students’ thinking. Examples of extension tasks used in the present study include asking Ms. Archer to solve 3/5 of 2/3 with the number line and asking students to determine 2/5 of 3/4 with rectangular regions. Combining lesson and interview data in this way can help discriminate between competing hypotheses about teachers’ knowledge with greater confidence, determine to what extent teachers’ self-reports appear consistent with classroom events (from researchers’ perspectives), and lead to Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 74 insights about teacher cognition that can help us better understand explicit and tacit mathematical knowledge that teachers’ use when responding to their students’ thinking during instruction. Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 75 References Armstrong, B., & Bezuk, N. (1995). Multiplication and division of fractions: The search for meaning. In J. T. Sowder & B. P. Schappelle (Eds.), Providing a foundation for teaching mathematics in the middle grades (pp. 85-119). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. Ball, D. L. (1990). Prospective elementary and secondary teachers' understanding of division. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 21(2), 132-144. Ball, D. L. (1991). Research on teaching mathematics: Making subject-matter knowledge part of the equation. In J. Brophy (Ed.), Advances in research on teaching (Vol. 2). Teachers' knowledge of subject matter as it relates to their teaching practice (pp. 1-48). Greenwich, CN: JAI Press. Ball, D. L., & Bass, H. (2000). Interweaving content and pedagogy in teaching and learning to teach: Knowing and using mathematics. In J. Boaler (Ed.), Multiple perspectives on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 81-104). Westport, CT: Ablex. Ball, D., Lubienski, S., & Mewborn, D. (2001). Research on teaching mathematics: The unsolved problem of teachers' mathematical knowledge. In V. Richardson (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching (4th Ed.). Washington, D.C.: American Educational Research Association. Behr, M., Harel, G., Post, T., & Lesh, R. (1992). Rational number, ratio, and proportion. In D. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 296-333). New York: Macmillan. Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 76 Behr, M., Harel, G., Post, T., & Lesh, R. (1993). Rational numbers: Toward a semantic analysis—emphasis on the operator construct. In T. Carpenter, E. Fennema & T. Romberg (Eds.), Rational numbers: An integration of research (pp. 13-47). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Behr, M., Khoury, H., Harel, G., Post, T., & Lesh, R. (1997). Conceptual units analysis of preservice elementary school teachers' strategies on a rational-number-asoperator task. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 28(1), 48-69. Bernard, H. (1994). Research methods in anthropology (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Borko, H., Eisenhart, M., Brown, C., Underhill, R., Jones, D., & Agard, P. (1992). Learning to teach hard mathematics: Do novice teachers and their instructors give up too easily? Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 23(3), 194-222. Borko, H., & Putnam, R. (1996). Learning to teach. In D. Berliner & R. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 673-708). New York: MacMillan. Cobb, P., & Whitenack, J. W. (1996). A method for conducting longitudinal analyses of classroom videorecordings and transcripts. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 30(3), 213-228. Davis, B., & Simmt, E. (2006). Mathematics-for-teaching: An ongoing investigation of the mathematics that teachers (need to) know. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 61(3), 293-319. diSessa, A. A. (1988). Knowledge in pieces. In G. Forman & P. Pufall (Eds.), Constuctivism in the Computer Age (pp. 49-70). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 77 diSessa, A. A. (1993). Toward an epistemology of physics. Cognition and Instruction, 10(2&3), 105-225. Eisenhart, M., Borko, H., Underhill, R., Brown, C., Jones, D., & Agard, P. (1993). Conceptual knowledge falls through the cracks: Complexities of learning to teach mathematics for understanding. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 24(1), 8-40. Fischbein, E., Deri, M., Nello, M. S., & Marino, M. S. (1985). The role of implicit models in solving verbal problems in multiplication and division. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 16(1), 3-17. Graeber, A., & Tirosh, D. (1988). Multiplication and division involving decimals: Preservice elementary teachers' performance and beliefs. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 7, 263-280. Graeber, A., Tirosh, D., & Glover, R. (1989). Preservice teachers' misconceptions in solving verbal problems in multiplication and division. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 20(1), 95-102. Hall, R. (2000). Videorecording as theory. In A. E. Kelly & R. A. Lesh (Eds.), Handbook of research design in mathematics and science education (pp. 647-664). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Harel, G., & Behr, M. (1995). Teachers' solutions for multiplicative problems. Hiroshima Journal of Mathematics Education, 3, 31-51. Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 78 Hill, H., Rowan, B., & Ball, D. L. (2005). Effects of teachers' mathematical knowledge for teaching on student achievement. American Educational Research Journal, 42(2), 371-406. Hill, H., Schilling, S., & Ball, D. L. (2004). Developing measures of teacher's mathematics knowledge for teaching. The Elementary School Journal, 105(1), 11-30. Izsák, A. (2004). Students' coordination of knowledge when learning to model physical situations. Cognition and Instruction, 22(1), 81-128. Izsák, A. (2005). “You have to count the squares”: Applying knowledge in pieces to learning rectangular area. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 14(3), 361-403. Izsák, A., Tillema, E., & Tunç-Pekkan, Z. (in press). Teaching and learning fraction addition on number lines. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education. Lappan, G., Fey, J. T., Fitzgerald, W., Friel, S. N., & Phillips, E. D. (2002). Connected Mathematics. Glenview, IL: Prentice Hall. Lappan, G., Fey, J. T., Fitzgerald, W., Friel, S. N., & Phillips, E. D. (2006). Connected Mathematics 2. Boston, MA: Prentice Hall. Ma, L. (1999). Knowing and teaching elementary mathematics: Teachers’ understanding of fundamental mathematics in China and the United States. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Olive, J. (1999). From fractions to rationale numbers of arithmetic: A reorganization hypothesis. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 1(4), 279-314. Olive, J., & Steffe, L. (2001). The construction of an iterative fractional scheme: The case of Joe. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 20(4), 413-437. Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 79 Piaget, J. (1970). Piaget’s theory. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (3rd ed., pp. 703-732). New York: Wiley. Post, T., Harel, G., Behr, M., & Lesh, R. (1991). Intermediate teachers' knowledge of rationale number concepts. In E. Fennema, T. Carpenter & S. Lamon (Eds.), Integrating research on teaching and learning mathematics (pp. 177-198). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. Schoenfeld, A. (1998). Toward a theory of teaching-in-context. Issues in Education, 4(1), 1-94. Schoenfeld, A. (2000). Models of the teaching process. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 18(3), 243-261. Schoenfeld, A. (in press). On modeling teachers’ in-the-moment decision-making. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education Monograph Series. Schoenfeld, A. H., Smith, J., & Arcavi, A. (1993). Learning: The microgenetic analysis of one student's evolving understanding of a complex subject matter domain. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 55-175). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Sherin, M. (2002). When teaching becomes learning. Cognition and Instruction, 20(2), 119150. Shulman, L. S. (1986). Those who understand: Knowledge growth in teaching. Educational Researcher, 15(2), 4-14. Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 80 Sowder, J., Philipp, R., Armstrong, B., & Schappelle, B. (1998). Middle-grade teachers’ mathematical knowledge and its relationship to instruction: A research monograph. Albany: State University of New York Press. Steffe, L. (1988). Children's construction of number sequences and multiplying schemes. In J. Hiebert & M. Behr (Eds.), Number concepts and operations in middle grades (pp. 119-140). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics; Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Steffe, L. (1994). Children's multiplying schemes. In G. Harel & J. Confrey (Eds.), The development of multiplicative reasoning in the learning of mathematics (pp. 3-39). Albany: State University of New York Press. Steffe, L. (2001). A new hypothesis concerning children's fractional knowledge. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 20(3), 267-307. Steffe, L. (2003). Fractional commensurate, composition, and adding schemes learning trajectories of Jason and Laura: Grade 5. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 22(3), 237-295. Steffe, L. (2004). On the construction of learning trajectories of children: The case of commensurate fractions. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 6(2), 129-162. Tirosh, D., & Graeber, A. (1989). Preservice elementary teachers' explicit beliefs about multiplication and division. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 20, 79-96. Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 81 Footnotes 1. Throughout his work, Steffe explains cognition and learning using theoretical constructs based on the work of Piaget. He describes cognitive structures in terms of schemes and operations and learning mechanisms that include interiorization, assimilation, and accommodation. 2. All names are pseudonyms. 3. A graduate student conducted three interviews in my presence with one of the student pairs during the first round of data collection and two more interviews in my presence with the same pair of students during the second round. 4. Prime Time is a CMP unit that develops elementary number theory concepts including factors, products, prime numbers, and composite numbers. 5. These students were the ones the graduate student interviewed in my presence. 6. Jeff replaced Emily’s first interview partner, when Angie elected to stop being interviewed. 7. Izsák et al. (in press) analyzed these lessons. 8. The Bits and Pieces II Teacher’s Guide does state that cross partitioning “does not lead to the kind of partitioning that suggests multiplication of numerators and denominators” (p. 8, Lappan, Fey, Fitzgerald, Friel, & Phillips, 2006). Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 82 Figure Captions Figure 1. Four ways to draw 2/3 of 3/4. Figure 2. (a) The number 5 understood with two levels of units. (b) The number 20 understood with three levels of units. Figure 3. Determining 1/4 of 1/3. (a) Constructing part of a part. (b) Using iteration and two levels of units. (c) Using recursive partitioning and three levels of units. Figure 4. Determining 3/4 of 8. (a) Using the duplicator and partition-reducer subconstruct (b) Using the stretcher and shrinker subconstruct. Figure 5. Using a drawing to determine a general numeric method for computing products of proper fractions. Figure 6. Ms. Archer’s white board when comparing 3/4 and 2/3. Figure 7. The number line showing 1/5 of 2/3 that Ms. Archer copied from the prepublication version of Bits and Pieces II Teacher's Guide (Lappan, Fey, Fitzgerald, Friel, & Phillips, 2006). Figure 8. Ms. Archer uses brownie pans to show 1/2 of 2/3. (a) Her first drawing. (b) Her second drawing. (c) Her third drawing. Figure 9. Reproductions of students' drawings for 3/4 of 1/2. (a) Angie. (b) Damien. (c) Theo. Figure 10. Students’ drawn solutions to 2/5 of 3/4. (a) Damien divides the pan into four and shades three parts (reproduction). (b) Damien divides each fourth into five parts (reproduction). (c) Theo shades two parts in each shaded fourth (students’ original work). (d) Emily creates all partitions vertically (reproduction). Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 83 Figure 11. Ms. Reese determines 1/2 of 2/3. (a) She draws 2/3 of a brownie pan. (b) She divides the square vertically. (c) She determines 1/2 of 2/3 is 2/6. Figure 12. Ms. Reese demonstrates two approaches to 1/3 of 5/6. (a) She partitions horizontally. (b) She partitions vertically. (c) She presents two students’ approach. Figure 13. Three drawings of 2/3 of 3/4. (a) Jack’s initial work (reproduction). (b) Kate’s initial work (reproduction). (c) Jack’s revised work (reproduction). Figure 14. Ms. Reese determines 1/4 of 2/3 two ways. (a) She draws a thermometer and divides the shaded 2/3 into four parts. (b) She solves the problem a second time using rectangles. Figure 15. Ms. Reese shows 1/3 of 2 1/2 is 5/6 (a) She draws 1/3 of 2 1/2 pounds of cheese. (b) She divides each whole into sixths and shades five pieces. Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 84 Figure 1 TOP Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 85 Figure 2 TOP (a) (b) Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 86 Figure 3 TOP (a) (b) (c) Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 87 Figure 4 TOP Duplicate Reduce (a) Stretch Shrink (b) Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 88 Figure 5 TOP d b a b c a b ab of c d cd Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 89 Figure 6 TOP Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 90 Figure 7 TOP Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 91 Figure 8 TOP (a) (b) (c) Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 92 Figure 9 TOP (a) (b) (c) Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 93 Figure 10 TOP (a) (b) (c) (d) Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 94 Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 95 Figure 11 TOP (a) (b) (c) Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 96 Figure 12 TOP (a) (b) Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 97 (c) Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 98 Figure 13 TOP (a) (b) (c) Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 99 Figure 14 TOP (a) (b) Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 100 Figure 15 TOP (a) (b) Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching Fraction Multiplication 101 i Throughout his work, Steffe explains cognition and learning using theoretical constructs based on the work of Piaget. He describes cognitive structures in terms of schemes and operations and learning mechanisms that include interiorization, assimilation, and accommodation. ii All names are pseudonyms. iii A graduate student conducted three interviews in my presence with one of the student pairs during the first round of data collection and two more interviews in my presence with the same pair of students during the second round. Prime Time is a CMP unit that develops elementary number theory constructs iv including prime numbers, composite numbers, factors, and products. v These students were the ones the graduate student interviewed in my presence. vi Jeff replaced Emily’s first interview partner, when Angie elected to stop being interviewed. vii Izsák et al. (in press) analyzed these lessons. viii The Bits and Pieces II Teacher’s Guide does state that cross partitioning “does not lead to the kind of partitioning that suggests multiplication of numerators and denominators” (p. 8, Lappan, Fey, Fitzgerald, Friel, & Phillips, 2006).