Exp Brain Res (2003) 151:387–404 DOI 10.1007/s00221-003-1481-x RESEARCH ARTICLE S. B. Bortolami · P. DiZio · E. Rabin · J. R. Lackner Analysis of human postural responses to recoverable falls Received: 7 August 2002 / Accepted: 11 March 2003 / Published online: 13 June 2003 ! Springer-Verlag 2003 Abstract We studied the kinematics and kinetics of human postural responses to “recoverable falls.” To induce brief falling we used a Hold and Release (H&R) paradigm. Standing subjects actively resisted a force applied to their sternum. When this force was quickly released they were suddenly off balance. For a brief period, !125 ms, until restoring forces were generated to shift the center of foot pressure in front of the center of mass, the body was in a forward fall acted on by gravity and ground support forces. We were able to describe the whole-body postural behavior following release using a multilink inverted pendulum model in a regime of “small oscillations.” A three-segment model incorporating upper body, upper leg, and lower leg, with active stiffness and damping at the joints was fully adequate to fit the kinematic data from all conditions. The significance of our findings is that in situations involving recoverable falls or loss of balance the earliest responses are likely dependent on actively-tuned, reflexive mechanisms yielding stiffness and damping modulation of the joints. We demonstrate that haptic cues from index fingertip contact with a stationary surface lead to a significantly smaller angular displacement of the torso and a more rapid recovery of balance. Our H&R paradigm and associated model provide a quantifiable approach to studying recovery from potential falling in normal and clinical subjects. Keywords Motor control · Posture · Modeling · Balance · Falling S. B. Bortolami ()) · P. DiZio · E. Rabin · J. R. Lackner Ashton Graybiel Spatial Orientation Laboratory, Brandeis University, Waltham, MA 2454-9110, USA e-mail: simborto@brandeis.edu Tel.: +1-781-7362033 Introduction Our goal was to study postural control performance during sudden exposure to off-balance conditions. Quantitative study of sudden off-balance conditions is relevant for understanding the events preceding falling and the functioning of short-latency postural restabilization mechanisms. We present a paradigm and a model, which have general implications regarding the structure and functions of the posture controller. Research on posture has employed a variety of techniques to evaluate postural control and many of these have relevance for understanding falling and short latency postural response mechanisms. We describe the advantages and limitations of some of the key techniques before describing our approach and its potential. Most approaches fall within several broad categories: moving platform posturography, pushes or pulls of the body, self-generated perturbations, vertical drops, and releases from a leaning posture. Considerable insight into postural control mechanisms and their derangement due to various pathologies has been gained with these methods. Moving platform posturography Platforms that can displace and tilt allow the evaluation of postural responses to linear or angular displacement of the support surface (Nashner 1971, 1987; Nashner and Forssberg 1986). Platform perturbations are extensive in magnitude and time, for example, 8 cm over 450 ms (see Horak and MacPherson 1996). Electromyographic (EMG) responses in the leg muscles occur within 70–90 ms (Horak and MacPherson 1996; Horak and Nashner 1986). With platform perturbations, normal subjects use three general “strategies” depending on perturbation magnitude and direction and the area of the support surface under the feet. The ankle strategy is used when the platform moves backward under the feet or rotates downward under the toes. Subjects extend their feet to move the center of pressure (cp) forward and to drive the center of mass 388 backwards under the feet; there is little rotation at other joints (Horak and Nashner 1986; Nashner 1977). A hip strategy is employed when the base of support is so narrow that little force can be exerted with the toes and when large platform perturbations occur (Horak and Nashner 1986; Nashner and McCollum 1985). Stepping is a third strategy that may be used with very large perturbations and is a way to avoid falling (Horak and MacPherson 1996). The response to platform perturbations differs depending on “central set,” for example, whether the perturbation is predictable, and whether vision is permitted (Horak and Nashner 1986). Pushes and pulls on the body Perturbations applied to the torso at force levels below those toppling the body or forcing a step constitute a way of studying integrative coordination of upper and lower limbs (Cordo and Nashner 1982; McIllroy and Maki 1995; Schieppati et al. 1995a, 1995b; Holt et al. 2000). EMG latencies in muscles counteracting predictable and unpredictable lateral or forward forces applied at the waist are approximately 75 ms (Elger et al. 1999; Gilles et al. 1998). EMG latencies in arm and hip muscles are similar, but close analysis suggests that they are under parallel, centrally generated control rather than a single general control pattern (Wing et al. 1997). Short latency, compensatory EMG activity (approximately 50–70 ms) appears also when perturbations are delivered by a backpack apparatus that perturbs the trunk during quiet stance or locomotion (Dietz 1992, 1996). When the perturbations applied to the body are increased in magnitude, subjects fall unless they make corrective movements. For example, if subjects standing on one leg are subjected to a progressively increasing or suddenly destabilizing force on their torso, they exhibit a protective hop (Roberts 1975). If they are standing on both legs, they take a compensatory step (or steps) to restore balance (Luchies et al. 1994, 1999; Pai and Patton 1997, Pai et al. 1998; Patton et al. 1999; Pidcoe and Rogers 1998). Exposed to sudden backward pulls, young subjects (mean age 22 years) take a single protective step whereas elderly (mean age 73) subjects take multiple short, low steps (Luchies et al. 1994). Thus, restoring strategies either relate to maintaining upright posture with ankle or hip movements or involve stepping. Studies involving exposing the body to multiple medial-lateral pushes at the trunk or pelvis at random, but at relatively low force levels (below those initiating a protective step) raise the possibility that muscle stiffness rather than reflexive compensation may be the key component of restabilization (Rietdyk et al. 1999). Predictive models (static and dynamic) have been developed to characterize, based on an inverted pendulum model, the boundaries for center of mass position and velocity limits, before a protective step must be initiated (Pai and Patton 1997; Patton et al. 1999). The relative contribution of stiffness control to quiet standing is a matter of some debate. Winter and colleagues (1996, 1998, 2001) have proposed an inverted pendulum model with the muscles serving as tunable springs to drive the center of pressure in phase with the center of mass. Morasso and Schieppati (1999) have challenged this approach arguing that the relationship between the center of mass and center of pressure is a fact of physics and not the result of control patterns, and that ankle stiffness per se is insufficient to stabilize the body. They propose active control by the CNS and point to the potential role of ankle proprioception and foot somatosensation in allowing for anticipatory control. Self-generated perturbations Movements of the arms and other parts of the body can potentially destabilize posture. However, a variety of anticipatory postural compensations occurs when subjects make voluntary movements that have consequences for their postural stability, for example, pulling on a handle or raising an arm (Cordo and Nashner 1982; Marsden et al. 1981). These anticipatory compensations have the consequence of counteracting the impending torques on the body and the shifts in center of mass. These studies are insightful in illuminating the wide range of compensations that must be accompanying virtually all aspects of voluntary movement and locomotion (Bouisset and Zattara 1981, 1988; Cordo and Nashner 1982; Crenna and Frigo 1991; Gelfand et al. 1971; Gurfinkel and Latash 1979; Gurfinkel and Osovets 1972; Gurfinkel et al. 1971, 1988; Marsden et al. 1978, 1981; Massion 1984; Massion and Dufosse 1988; Nardone and Schieppati 1988; Nashner and Forssberg 1986; Sanes and Jennings 1984). Recently it has been suggested that even during quiet stance, a feed forward ankle joint stiffness strategy is employed, based on predicting the loading pattern, to control sagittal plane sway (Gatev et al. 1999). Anticipatory postural compensations are also present for ball catching (Lacquaniti and Maoli 1989; Massion 1992; Paulignan et al. 1989) and for self-induced perturbations such as releasing a load (Aruin and Latash 1995, 1996; Aruin et al. 2001; Beek and Santvoord 1996). Vertical drops Studies of actual falling have been carried out in which subjects are suspended a short distance above the ground by a handle that they grasp (Greenwood and Hopkins 1976, 1980; Melvill Jones and Watt 1971; Wicke and Oman 1982). When the handle is suddenly released by an electromagnet the subject falls under the action of gravity. A pulley and weight system has also been used to drop subjects at fractional g-levels (Greenwood and Hopkins 1976, 1980). EMG activity occurs within 60–80 ms following release in the leg muscles involved in decelerating the body on landing as well as in other, 389 nonpostural muscles. This generalized response likely represents an otolith related activation because labyrinthine-defective subjects do not show those early EMG responses. By contrast, labyrinthine-defective subjects show normal postural responses if the support surface on which they are standing is displaced (Diener and Dichgans 1988; Dietz 1996). Release from a leaning posture A series of studies has evaluated the ability to take a step to regain balance following release from various degrees of forward tilt. Subjects lean forward while their body is supported by a harness that takes up a controlled fraction of their weight, thus allowing them to assume various extents of body tilt. At a random time, the harness is released. The body then gathers momentum as it is accelerated downward by gravity and the subjects try to take a forward step to regain balance. A safety harness prevents impact with the floor if they are unable to step. These studies have shown important age differences in recovery as well as gender differences (Thelen et al. 1997; Wojcik et al. 1999). EMG latencies in the muscles involved in postural recovery range from 73–114 ms and do not show any age-related dependency of functional significance (Thelen et al. 2000). Common drawbacks of current perturbation techniques Most perturbation paradigms for studying posture apply the perturbation to a specific part of the body. For example, moving platforms initiate a perturbation at the feet that is conveyed upward through successive segments of the body. Perturbations at the torso, either externally applied or self-generated by arm movements, affect the head and the hips first and then upper legs and lower legs and feet. Hip perturbations affect body segments successively both downwards and upwards. These types of perturbations have many advantages in terms of allowing successive segmental contributions to be identified. They do, however, complicate the physical modeling and characterization of the postural compensations. The technique of dropping the test subject has the advantage that all segments of the body are simultaneously accelerated downward together during the brief period of free fall preceding the landing. However, the recovery of balance after landing is difficult to characterize and quantify. Another issue is the suddenness of the perturbation. Most techniques, other than releasing from an offvertical posture or vertical drops, must displace or deform the body posture. This requires time and force, which for practical reasons cannot be extremely large. Consequently most perturbations induce a slow postural response. The celerity of the stimulation is a key factor because it reflects on the kind and order of the pathways responding to the perturbation. Most posture studies also lack the use of analytical and numerical models specific to their paradigms. The hold & release paradigm Our goal was to employ a technique for creating a simultaneous and sudden acceleration of all body segments about the feet using an approach amenable to mathematical characterization so that we could model the pattern of postural recovery. We wanted our paradigm to share key characteristics with falling onset since posture control, by its nature, functions to avoid falling. This would allow us to determine whether stiffening is a mechanism employed in recovering from falling. In addition, we wanted to be able to quantify the magnitude of the perturbation and to be able to scale its magnitude across trials and different subjects to give the paradigm flexibility and ease of implementation. The experimental paradigm we adopted (Hold & Release, or H&R; Rabin et al. 2000) is illustrated in Fig. 1. It is a variant of a procedure developed in the clinical setting by Barin and Stockwell (1983; see also Stockwell 1983; Krebs et al. 2001). In our situation a subject stands upright and actively resists a horizontal force applied to the sternum. The force is suddenly withdrawn (within 30–40 ms) and the subject is projected forward pivoting at the ankles, knees and hips because the center of foot pressure was displaced toward the heels by the holding force on the sternum. An important characteristic of the H&R is that it mimics, in a functionally relevant and repeatable manner, what happens during unexpected loss of balance such as tripping or loss of footing. A large number of falls, some investigators suggest the majority, occur because of stumbling or tripping on obstacles during walking (Blake et al. 1988; Campbell et al. 1990; Overstall et al. 1977; Ruberstein et al. 1988). In both tripping and H&R the body is initially nearly upright and has forward momentum, but the center of foot pressure is misaligned with the center of mass. Figure 2 schematizes the onset of falling after tripping and the associated residual momentum of the body. During walking all body segments move with an average speed equal to the gait velocity, including the feet. Just after the moment of tripping or loss of footing, if the fall is physically recoverable, one or both feet reacquire the footing (zero velocity at the foot/feet). However, the rest of the body not only is still moving forward with a residual velocity but also has the center of mass and center of foot pressure misaligned and momentarily “out of control.” Thus the body pitches forward or sideways pivoting about the ankles acted on by the acceleration of gravity, the ground reaction forces, and the acquired momentum. In H&R when the hold force on the sternum is released, the body rapidly acquires angular momentum about the ankles. It is this angular momentum (not the holding force) and the sudden misalignment of the center of mass and center of foot pressure which 390 Fig. 1 Illustration of the Hold & Release paradigm. Hold: the experimenter applies a steady force, H, to the standing subject’s sternum. The ankle, knee, and hip muscles produce torques (t) moving the center of foot pressure toward the heels in order to oppose the force applied at the sternum. Release: the force is suddenly withdrawn and the offset between the center of foot pressure and the center of mass propels the body forward. This is the state illustrated in Fig. 2 for natural tripping or loss of balance. Recoil: the fall is arrested after a latency by motor control and skeletomuscluar systems moving the center of foot pressure Fig. 2 During walking all body parts move with an average velocity (V). After tripping on an unexpected obstacle, the rest of the body is still moving forward with a residual velocity and the center of mass and center of foot pressure are misaligned and momentarily “out of control.” In our schematization, we can think of walking as a pendulum on a moving trolley, and the standing posture as a fixed upright pendulum. Tripping and subsequent reacquisition of footing can be thought of as transforming the first system (a pendulum on a moving platform) to the second system (a pendulum on a nonmoving platform) quasi-instantaneously. Succinctly, the mechanism of tripping is to instantaneously switch the mechanical nature of posture and introduce in it both a misalignment of the center of mass and center of pressure and a horizontal velocity pulse that postural control must dissipate constitute the perturbation that needs to be accommodated to maintain balance. We also wanted to determine whether a linear multisegment inverted pendulum model would be adequate to describe the recovery of balance after H&R. Such a model would have the advantages of linearity, which permits a relatively simple mathematical characterization, and of scalability of the response parameters across differently sized perturbations. The model would also forward. Settled: posture finally settles with a transient response. The internal forces at the joints are only represented partially for clarity. Specifically, we represented only the joint torques of the multibody linkage applied from the feet to the shanks, from the shanks to the thighs, and so forth. Internal forces per se do not affect the motion of the center of mass directly but only the configuration of the body. Internal forces are used to deform the body in relationship to the ground to generate shear forces and changes in center of foot pressure to make gravity and ground support move the center of mass in a desired manner allow us to determine whether reflexive mechanisms producing stiffness and energy dissipation at the body’s joints is a viable CNS scheme for recovering from falling onset. Many studies (see Fregly 1979) have demonstrated the attenuating influence of vision on postural sway during quiet stance. Therefore we included conditions with and without sight of the surroundings to see how this would affect the performance of the posture controller during sudden off-balance conditions. We also included a condition involving light touch of the hand at mechanically nonsupportive force levels. Such light contact with a stable surface greatly attenuates body sway during passive stance (Clapp and Wing 1999; Holden et al. 1987, 1994; Jeka and Lackner 1994, 1995; Jeka et al. 1998; Rabin et al. 1999). Force levels of about 0.4 N are spontaneously adopted by subjects (Holden et al. 1994; Jeka and Lackner 1994, 1995), a value corresponding to the maximum dynamic sensitivity range of the somatosensory receptors in the fingertip (Westling and Johansson 1987) and force levels as low as 0.05 N have some stabilizing effect (Lackner et al. 2001). Labyrinthinedefective subjects cannot balance heel-to-toe for more than a few seconds if their eyes are closed. However, they can stand as stably as normal subjects when light touch of their finger with a stable surface is allowed. In fact, they are more stable than normal subjects (Lackner et al. 1999). Consequently we thought light touch might also enhance recovery of equilibrium during dynamic tasks. 391 Materials and methods Subjects Ten subjects, nine men and one woman, ranging in age from 19 to 56 years participated after giving informed consent to a protocol approved by the Brandeis University Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects. All were healthy, physically active, and without musculoskeletal or neurological problems that could compromise their balance. Apparatus A Kistler force plate (model 9261A) was used to record the standing subject’s anterior-posterior center of foot pressure (cpx), with a relative uncertainty less than 1% (see Figliola and Beasley 1995). An Optotrak" system (Northern Digital) was used to monitor the positions of infrared emitting diodes attached to the subject’s shoulder, hip, knee, and ankle with an uncertainty voxel (volume element) less than 0.5#0.5#0.5 mm. An instrumented, hand-held dynamometric probe with a flat contact surface was used to apply a holding force to the subject’s chest and to provide the time of release. A second Kistler force plate (model 9286, relative uncertainty less than 1% and noise less than 0.02 N rms) was positioned at waist height in front of the subject to measure the force applied by the right index fingertip in conditions involving finger contact. All recording instruments received a signal allowing data synchronization within 2 ms. EMG signals were recorded in two subjects from the tibialis anterior, gastrocnemius, biceps femoris, and rectus femoris muscles by means of a MT8 EMG device (MIE Medical Research, UK). The EMG signals were bandpass filtered at 10–500 Hz and sampled at 1300 Hz. zeroed prior to the start of a trial and its signals were not further treated for bias. Data were averaged across repetitions separately for each condition (EONT, ECNT, ECFT) and subject. The resulting data (30 sets) were then analyzed and compared using the algorithms described in the numerical model section. Results and analysis The postural response to H&R for an ECNT trial from one subject is shown in Fig. 3. Figure 3 displays traces of shoulder, hip, and knee displacements in the format ready for numerical analysis. (The traces are extracted from the measured data starting from the release point; see also Fig. 4). Each trace initially reaches some peak value before rebounding and finally settling within about 5–10 s. Table 1 presents the means and standard deviations of the values of peak deflections (pinnacles of the humps in Fig. 3) of the shoulder, hip, and knee across all subjects for the three different conditions. These displacement values all correspond to body segment rotations of a few degrees. The force plate data shown in Fig. 4A–4C for an ECFT trial show how center of pressure varies during the H&R perturbation. The center of foot pressure is by definition zero when the posture is settled after recovery, as it is elucidated in the modeling sections. The steplike shape of the center of foot pressure response (see Fig. 4A) is the signature of the H&R paradigm. Procedure Subjects stood barefoot in the standard Romberg posture, feet side by side but not touching, attempting to stand as straight and still as possible. The experimenter pushed against the subject’s sternum with the dynamometer with a steady force level. The force level was adjusted according to the size and the strength of the subject in order to produce a roughly comparable sway response in all subjects; it ranged from 10–40 N. The subject actively resisted this force attempting to maintain a straight upright posture. Within the next 10 s the force was suddenly and without warning withdrawn, and the subject attempted to regain equilibrium as rapidly as possible without shifting or lifting his or her feet. Each subject participated in three types of conditions: eyes open, no touch (EONT); eyes closed, no touch (ECNT); eyes closed, forward touch (ECFT). In the touch condition the subject touched lightly ("1 N) the force plate with his or her right index finger. This level of force is insufficient for significant mechanical stabilization of sway but provides a sensory spatial cue that reduces sway relative to no touch conditions (Holden et al. 1994; Jeka and Lackner 1994). In the conditions not involving touch the subject held his or her finger above the force plate. Subjects were given two practice trials for each condition prior to the start of the experiment proper. There were four repetitions per condition and the trial order was randomized for each subject. The duration of the trials was 25 s. All data channels aside from the EMGs were sampled at 120 Hz. Data reduction Raw data from the stance force plate, the Optotrak", and the finger force plate were synchronized and then box-car filtered with zero phase shift at 30 Hz bandwidth. The instant of probe release was used to synchronize data across trials, conditions, and subjects. The stance force and postural position recordings at rest after release (see “settled” in Fig. 1) were used as zero baselines for the force plate and Optotrak" marker recordings. The finger touch plate was Fig. 3 Horizontal displacements of the knee, hip, and shoulder for a typical eyes closed, no touch (ECNT) trial. The ankle is used as the origin. The stick figure on the right hand side shows the Optotrak" marker locations relative to the measurement traces. Time zero corresponds to the moment of release illustrated in Fig. 1. The traces are in a format ready for numerical analysis. (The traces are extracted from the measured data by cutting before the release point). Each trace rises initially to some peak value (humps) before rebounding and finally settling within about 5–10 s. Table 1 presents the means and standard deviations of the values of peak deflections (pinnacles of the humps) across all subjects for the three different experimental conditions 392 Fig. 4 Raw data sample from an H&R eyes closed forward touch (ECFT) trial. A The panel shows the holding force and the center of foot pressure. The center of foot pressure shift cpx (the difference between the cpx values at equilibrium prior to release and at equilibrium after release) is used as a normalization factor across trials and subjects. The holding action is considered completely withdrawn when the measured force crosses the zero value. The panel shows head, shoulder, hip and knee displacements complemented by the respective traces of velocities and accelerations. The body displacement traces demonstrate that the upright posture starts to accelerate forward, “fall,” instantaneously after the onset of the release as is dictated by Newton’s second law. Touch forces from the fingertip are small and not of significantly supportive values. B The variables of the previous panel are complemented here with the recording of the ground shear force. Its good constancy for 125 ms after release onset confirms that for a brief time interval after release the subject “falls” forward unstopped and without the possibility of sensing the fall from the foot soles. EMG recordings are presented from the tibialis anterior, gastrocnemius, biceps femoris, and rectus femoris muscles. C The variables in B have been dilated in time to allow for better identification of the course of action after release 393 Table 1 The upper three lines indicate the mean peak marker displacements and standard deviations in cm of knee, hip, and shoulder sway in the three experimental conditions (see Fig. 3 for visualization): eyes open no touch (EONT), eyes closed no touch (ECNT), eyes closed forward touch (ECFT). The variation cpx (cm) indicates the displacement of cpx between the hold and settled positions for each condition. The bottom three lines show the knee, hip, and shoulder displacements for the experimental conditions normalized in relation to thecpx displacement (cpx ) for each condition. See also Fig. 6. The entries (cm/cm) represent marker displacement in relation to cpx displacement (n=10) Knee sway (cm) Hip sway (cm) Shoulder sway (cm) cpx (cm) Norm. knee sway (cm/cm) Norm. hip sway (cm/cm) Norm. shoulder sway (cm/cm) EONT ECNT ECFT 0.6€0.9 1.8€1.5 3.5€1.6 4.8€0.9 0.12€0.19 0.38€0.32 0.73€0.34 0.5€0.8 1.6€1.2 3.5€1.4 5.2€1.1 0.10€0.16 0.30€0.24 0.67€0.27 0.6€0.7 1.7€0.8 2.3€0.8 4.1€1.0 0.14€0.16 0.41€0.20 0.56€0.19 Conceptual model Before entering a more complex level of analysis of the H&R paradigm we recapitulate the paradigm as follows. – Hold (see Figs. 1, 5): The experimenter applies a force to the subject’s sternum. As a consequence the center of foot pressure (cpx) is offset (backwards relative to the subject) by an amount that is a function of the holding force and the height of the point of application. – Release (see Figs. 1, 5): After release the body accelerates forward because the center of foot pressure, whose action had been opposing the force applied by the experimenter, is still offset with respect to the center of mass. Thus, the body is momentarily in a state of falling. – Recoil (see Fig. 1): After a delay new joint torques shift the cpx in the direction opposite to its offset during the hold period to arrest the fall and ultimately bring about the recovery shown in Fig. 3. – Settled (see Fig. 1) is the reestablished normal upright posture. The size (cpx ) of the cpx shift from the holding phase plateau (see Fig. 4A) to the settled phase plateau is directly proportional to the magnitude of the H&R perturbation and can be used with sufficient approximation to normalize the body displacement and force data because, as is shown below, the system is in a regime of linearity. The center of foot pressure normalization factors (cpx ) and resulting normalized peak marker displacements are shown in the bottom four lines of Table 1. Figure 6 shows stick figures drawn with the normalized marker displacements of Table 1 to allow visualization of the multisegment behavior at the peak of the body deflection across conditions. It can be seen that the ECFT condition is characterized by a greater degree of out-of-line sway of Fig. 5 Layout of the variables involved in the analytical model of the H&R paradigm and definitions for the conceptual analysis of the H&R paradigm. O is the point of equilibrium of the center of mass and center of foot pressure during the settled phase after n P release (see Fig. 1); it was chosen as center of momenta. m ¼ mi i¼1 is the total mass of the body and mi is the mass of each single body segment where n is the number of body segments. N is the vertical component of the ground support forces, which is considered applied at the center of foot pressure cpx. S is the shear component of the ground support forces; g equal to %9.81 m/s2 is the acceleration of gravity; G equal to mg is the total weight of the body. H is the holding force and d the holding height. n n P P x ¼ 1=m mi xi and y ¼ 1=m mi yi ffi l are the coordinates of the 1 1 center of mass of the system. J is the angular coordinate of the motion of the center of mass the three body segments than the no touch conditions, EONT and ECNT. This is attributable to the smaller displacement of the torso when forward touch of the hand is allowed, and represents a significant difference from the other two conditions. (See comparison of different conditions: normalized maximum sway amplitude, below.) Analytical model We analyze the four phases of the H&R paradigm individually here. Figure 5 shows the layout of the system and variables for this analysis. 394 Release When the body is released, Fig. 4A–C shows that between the total decay of the holding force and the initial variation in the cpx a minimum of 90 ms elapses. For this period of time the system’s dynamics is described by the following equations obtained from Eq. 1 by removing the holding force H. Also introducing Eq. 2 we obtain: ! € Gx % Gcpx ¼ Io J ð3Þ S ¼ m€x Fig. 6 Peak marker values normalized using of the factor cpx to allow for comparison across trials, subjects, and conditions. The normalization factor cpx is the variation in the center of pressure from hold to settled as shown in Fig. 4A. Abscissa Centimeters of produced deflection per centimeter of displacement of the center of pressure. Plotted values are also reported in Table 1 Hold For the holding phase we can write the equilibrium equations of the system taking advantage of Newton’s laws of mechanics as follows: 8 € ffi 0 < %Hd þ Gx þ Ncpx ¼ I0 ðtÞJ ð1Þ N þ mg ¼ m€y ffi 0 : SþH ¼ m€x ffi 0 Where: O is the point of equilibrium of the center of mass and center of foot pressure during the settled phase after release (see Fig. 1), which was chosen as center of momenta; m is the total mass of the body; N is the vertical component of the ground support forces, which is considered applied on the center of foot pressure cpx; S is the shear component of the ground support forces; g is the acceleration of gravity (%9.81 m/s2); G equal to mg is the total weight of the body; H is the holding force and d the holding height; I0 is the instantaneous moment of inertia with respect to O; x and y are the coordinates of the center of mass of the body; and J is the angular coordinate of the center of mass motion with respect to the center of momenta O. We can make the following determinations because the angular movements are of the order of magnitude J!€10& or less as indicated by the displacement values in Table 1: €y ¼ oð€xÞ y_ ¼ oð_xÞ y ¼ oðxÞ l ffi constant: ð2Þ The term o(f) means infinitesimal of higher order and l is the constant value approximation of the vertical coordinate of the center of mass y. In order to simplify the treatment further we can assume that the inertia of the upright body could be approximated with I0!ml2 where l is height of the center of mass (we lumped the whole body mass at the center of mass). In addition, we can take advantage of the small displacement relationship x ffi %lJ because segment movements are small ("10&): ! € þ g J ¼ %Gcpx ¼ %mkgk ) kcpx k J l ð4Þ € S¼ %ml2 J Equation 4 is the equation of an inverted pendulum (unstable) and is always applicable regardless of the multi-degree of freedom nature of the system if the motion of the system is analyzed as motion of the center of mass (see Goldstein et al. 2002). Equation 4 is also applicable to all remaining phases of the paradigm. In the general case the moment of inertia of the system becomes time varying, and eventually it must be differentiated together with the angular velocity during the calculation of the inertia forces. (This component is neglected here because the differential of the moment of inertia is an infinitesimal of higher order during H&R). During the release phase, the forcing component on the right hand side of upper Eq. 4 is a constant pushingover torque since the misalignment of the center of foot pressure with respect to the center of mass remains unchanged for at least 90 ms due to the slower reaction time of the CNS (see Fig. 4C). Such a constant pushover torque is a function only of the offset cpx of the center of mass with respect to the center of pressure, which in turn is determined by the magnitude of the holding force H. The quantity cpx can be reliably measured by means of the force plate the subject stands on since it is the difference between the values of the center of foot pressure during the hold phase and the settled phase. During the 90 ms after release before the cpx changes position the system accelerates under gravity (mkgk ) kcpx k is constant) and builds up speed even though it is moving very little. Figure 4C shows that the shear force S remains approximately constant as well, as predicted by Eq. 4. This means that when released, the center of mass is accelerating forward freely, i.e., “falling,” with an acceleration equivalent to the removed holding force H until much later when the center of pressure starts to react. Figure 4C shows that within 50– 60 ms from the onset of release the EMG activity in the 395 leg muscles changes with the gastrocnemius (gastrocnemius is the first to react) and the biceps femoris increasing and the tibialis anterior and the rectus femoris decreasing in activation (see also Figure 8). Due to the muscle activation dynamics, at least another 50–70 ms elapses before the ankles and knees start to extend and the center of foot pressure begins to move (see Figure 4C). This synergy moves the center of foot pressure forward. To break the fall, the cpx needs to go ahead of the center of mass. A mirror symmetric synergy later moves the center of foot pressure in the opposite direction. Succinctly, we can say that after release the body’s joint torques are reversed from the ones of the holding phase and begins to shift the cpx forward, a process taking approximately 90 ms. In the interim the body is falling forward under the pull of gravity and the action of ground reaction forces. This reaction time is comparable across subjects, so we can estimate the " peak velocity prior to the recoil as _J0 ! mkgk ) kcpx k ml2 (from the impulse theorem of " mechanics), which yields J_ 0 / cpx l2 Recoil The calculated velocity J_ 0 dictates the amount of energy that is acquired during the off-balance interim, “fall,” generated by the release. During the recoil phase, postural control must not only realign the center of mass with the center of foot pressure, but in the process it must dissipate this acquired velocity J_ 0 . This energy can only be dissipated by controlling the center of foot pressure and the center of mass misalignment in phase contrast to the sway velocity J_ to perform negative work. The CNS skill in doing this is revealed by the shape of the transient (see Figure 3) rather than by the magnitude of the sway response, i.e., fast vs. slowly decaying transient. The perturbation introduced by the H&R depends on the magnitude of center of mass%center of foot pressure misalignment and the J_ 0 acquired after the release. Both are determined by the holding force, H, and are quantified by the measure cpx . Therefore the data across trials and subjects can be normalized by using the parameter cpx (the linearity of the H&R dynamics is demonstrated below) and importantly the holding force need not be measured or controlled. The only requirement is that the maximum sway produced by the release is not so excessive as to elicit a step. In summary, the normalized peaks of the sway (see Fig. 6) and the rapidity of restoration of the upright posture (damping, see Fig. 3) are two of the parameters yielded by the H&R that provide quantification of CNS performance during sudden offbalance exposure. Settled After about 5–10 s from release, the subject’s posture reaches a position of equilibrium. We adopt this as a measurable experimental zero. This phase is regular standing posture. In order not to overload the reader with technical details, further features of analysis are discussed in Appendices A and B Neuromuscular mechanisms of the H&R The H&R paradigm introduces a velocity and an offbalance disturbance to posture in a quantifiable manner. This allows for addressing the neuromuscular mechanisms engaged in posture restabilization following H&R. Table 1 and Fig. 6 show that with the H&R paradigm movements are small so that from a geometrical standpoint the body is in a regime of “small oscillation.” This means that the biomechanics of the upright posture is linearizable. However, the control of posture can still be a convoluted nonlinear problem if the neurophysiology of the posture control is greatly nonlinear. In the introduction we advanced the hypothesis that H&R posture control is a reflexive control mechanism performing as a spring and damper (otherwise called PD controller) of which the damping component is set up differently across conditions. Moreover, characteristics of the controller may be tuned during the falling phase. If our intuition is correct, we expect the upright posture to perform during the H&R as a multi-degree of freedom oscillator subjected to an impulsive perturbation. Numerical model: identification of the neuromuscular control mechanisms To determine whether the body exposed to H&R actually behaves as we hypothesize we attempted to fit the measured multidegree of freedom sway traces of Fig. 3 with a linear multidegree of freedom oscillator model. Appendix B presents the mathematical basis for modeling our data in terms of the equations of the following multidegree of freedom oscillator: M€x þ C x_ þ Kx ¼ 0 ð5Þ Appendix B shows that the measured properties of the biomechanics combined with the properties of the hypothesized joint control scheme yield a multidegree of freedom system with stiffness and damping at the joints, which is mathematically describable by Eq. 5. The joint stiffness and damping are produced both by the mechanical properties of the body and by the modulatable and tunable reflexive pathways of postural control, which are here identified. From linear ordinary differential equation theory we know that the general solution of such equations is a combination of simple components Fj, j=1,..,3: 396 8 9 < a11 = F1 ¼ a21 ea1 t sinðb1 t þ g1 Þ; : ; a31 8 9 < a12 = F2 ¼ a22 ea2 t sinðb2 t þ g2 Þ; : ; a32 8 9 < a13 = F3 ¼ a23 ea3 t sinðb3 t þ g3 Þ : ; a33 ð6Þ Each component of the response, or modal component, j, is in turn identified by the set of two parameters aj and bj, which represent, respectively, the rate of decay and pulsation of the component. For each modal component the parameters aij, i, j=1,..,3, (unscaled eigenvectors) define the shapes of the components (e.g., en bloc movement or zig-zag movement). We searched for a set of the parameters aij, ai, bi, and gi, i, j=1,..,3, to be entered into Eq. 6, that would fit the experimental recordings xi, i=1,..3 (i.e., knee, hip, and shoulder sway). The fit of the postural responses (knee, hip, and shoulder) of the ten subjects for the three different test conditions yielded 30 sets of parameters. The search was conducted in trial and error manner via nonlinear minimization using a zero order searching method (Neddler and Mead 1965) and the following cost function: # T# # 3 Z # 3 X X # # aj t G¼ aij e sinðbj t þ gj Þ# dt #x i % # # i¼1 j¼1 o 2 Data fit was achieved with only two terms of Eq. 6. Including the third term in the numerical minimization did not contribute a closer fit of the data. Examples of data fitting in one subject are shown in Fig. 7A–C. The average of the rms values of the residuals after model fitting over the 30 sets of data was approximately 1.1 mm with a standard deviation of 0.4 mm. These rms residual values are a small fraction (5%) of the size of the grand average of the peak sway over different markers, conditions and subjects, which was approximately 20 mm (see Table 1). The rms values of the residuals were small in each condition: 1.1 mm for EONT, 1.4 mm for ECNT, and 0.8 mm for ECFT. The good fit of the model to our experimental data confirms the hypothesis that posture behaves linearly during the H&R paradigm and that the model equations of a multiple degrees of freedom oscillator as laid out in Appendix B are applicable. This Fig. 7 Fit of the linear model with one subject’s measured, knee, hip, and shoulder sway for each of the three experimental conditions averaged across the four repetitions per condition (EONT eyes open no touch; ECNT eyes closed no touch; ECFT eyes closed forward touch). A, B and C the measured whole body H&R responses are fully overlapped by the predictions of the multilink model of Fig. 10. The dynamics model simulates the three traces simultaneously as calculated displacements of the body markers following release. The grand average, across the different subjects and conditions, of the rms values of the residuals of the model fit is 5% of the peak deflection of approximately 20 mm (see also Table 1). This indicates that the model fits the experimental data very well and in turn that postural control behaves as a linear control mechanism providing reflexive stiffness and damping to the body’s joints 397 Table 2 Frequency (w) and damping (x) characteristics of the two modes that were sufficient to describe the experimental data of the three conditions. The parameters w represent the frequencies of oscillation of the harmonic components of the recovery transient of the Hold and Release (see also Fig. 3) and the parameters x indicate the rates of energy dissipation of the components. The H&R approach to posture can be compared to plucking the strings of a guitar. In such a case the parameters w would identify the strings (notes), which were plucked and the parameters x the damping characteristics causing them to fade out at different rates (n=10) w1 (Hz) x1 w2 (Hz) x2 EONT ECNT ECFT 0.24€0.12 0.89€0.16 0.50€0.27 0.66€0.24 0.23€0.10 0.68€0.27 0.50€0.22 0.47€0.30 0.22€0.22 0.92€0.13 0.60€0.33 0.74€0.25 Table 3 Eigenvalues of the two modes for the three experimental conditions derived using Kung’s eigenvalue realization algorithm (n=10) Kung’s algorithm is applicable to virtually ideal mathematical systems. The good agreement of the algorithm’s calculations and the results shown in Table 2 corroborates that the H&R is amenable to close form mathematical modeling and that the analytical and numerical treatments presented here are sound w1 (Hz) w2 (Hz) EONT ECNT ECFT 0.16€0.05 0.42€0.17 0.16€0.05 0.42€0.17 0.15€0.06 0.52€0.25 means that the posture controller during H&R supplies appropriate stiffness and damping joint characteristics by means of somatosensory afferent feedback. The identified model parameters were converted into the form of central frequencies wj, i=1,..,3, characteristics xj, ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffidamping ffi qffiffiffiffiffiand i=1,..,3, where wi ¼ a2i þ b2i and xi ¼ jai =wi j: wj and xj were averaged over subjects and are presented in Table 2. The parameters wj represent the frequencies of oscillation of the harmonic components of the recovery transient of Fig. 3 while xj is proportional to the rates of energy dissipation of the components. The H&R approach to posture can be compared to plucking the strings of a guitar. In such a case the wj would identify the strings (notes), which were plucked, and xj the effect of each one’s friction causing them to fade out at different rates. Numerical model verification To confirm the calculations of our minimization algorithm we performed additional system identifications on the sway data using the eigenvalue realization algorithm developed by (Kung 1979). Kung’s algorithm allows for identification of the system eigenvalues when only the impulsive response of the system is available and the forcing input is zero. Table 3 shows that the averaged eigenvalues produced by the eigenvalue realization algorithm calculations over the different subjects and conditions are in overall agreement with the results of Table 2. Considering that Kung’s algorithm is applicable to almost ideal mathematical systems, the good agreement of the algorithm’s calculations and our results corroborates that the H&R paradigm is amenable to close form mathematical modeling and that our analytical and numerical treatments are sound. Comparisons of different conditions: normalized maximum sway amplitude The linearity of the H&R dynamics that we have demonstrated allows normalization of the marker displacement values of each condition’s data set in relation to the cpx shift between the hold and settled phases (see Table 1). Normalized deflections permit valid comparisons to be made across trials and conditions with different perturbation magnitudes. An analysis of variance was conducted and post hoc analyses with Bonferroni corrections were carried out to identify the source of significant differences. These analyses indicated that with forward touch (ECFT) there was significantly less angular displacement of the trunk, i.e., the trunk was kept significantly closer to the vertical relative to the two no touch conditions (ECNT, EONT), p<0.05 all comparisons. There were no significant differences between the ECNT and EONT conditions for ankle, knee, hip, or shoulder displacements. In addition, the peak postural displacements in ECNT and EONT could be characterized within a good approximation by a single link inverted pendulum pivoting at the ankle. Comparisons of different conditions: central frequency and damping The identified central frequencies and damping factors were compared with an analysis of variance to test for effects of the experimental conditions on sway. We also ran post-hoc pairwise comparisons. The two modal frequencies were indistinguishable across conditions but the power was low. This suggests that joint stiffness properties may not be affected by the presence of visual or tactile cues. Such a result was expected, since stiffness is mostly determined by muscle properties and activation levels. Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests of normality revealed that damping factors do not meet the requirements for parametric analysis. Therefore we compared damping factors with the Wilcoxon test, which indicated that the damping of both modes is increased by the presence of touch or vision, each p<0.05. Therefore the presence of tactile cues at the fingertip or of vision aids the dissipation of the energy introduced by the perturbation. The peak vertical touch force, averaged across subjects and repetitions, was 1.1€0.6 N and the fore-aft finger shear force was 0.7€0.3 N. These forces at the finger are much too low to provide significant mechanical stabilization of the torso (see Holden et al. 1994). Figure 4A shows the fingertip touch forces for a typical trial. 398 physiological terms this means that during exposure of the body to sudden off balance conditions reflexive mechanisms provide specific stiffness and damping characteristics to the joints of the body that are not simply the result of passive muscles mechanics. Figure 4 panels B and C show rectified EMG recordings for one subject in a forward touch condition, in which clear bursts are present, which indicates reflexive control of posture. Figure 8 shows the EMG recordings taken in an eyes closed without touch trial from another subject. The recordings from the leg muscles are aligned to show the strong pattern of muscle synergies that is responsible for the viscoelastic (stiffness and damping) response predicted by our modeling and numerical analysis. First principles dictate that postural control can only resort to synchronization of muscle contractions in phase contrast with muscle stretching in order to dissipate energy. The results of this investigation indicate that with the aid of haptic cues the postural controller is more effective in dissipating the energy of the perturbation. These two facts combined mandate that the muscle synergies (patterned muscle contractions) shown in Fig. 8 are finely timed with respect to each other in a context specific manner. Discussion Fig. 8 Example of muscle activation (EMG) in the leg muscles of a subject during Hold & Release from an EONT trial (arrow: time of release). Differential recordings of surface EMG were made from two pairs of muscles: (a) tibialis anterior and gastrocnemius lateralis and (b) biceps femoris (long head) and rectus femoris. The first pair flexes and extends the ankle, and the second pair act on both knee and hip. The data were band-pass filtered at 10–500 Hz and sampled at 1300 Hz. The envelope of the rectified signals is presented with the signals offset to zero in the hold period. This allows for easier visualization of the resulting activity changes. After release, there is bursting of all four muscles with peaks at different latencies. These bursts are the initial responses that arrest the body’s forward motion. When the body is settled, the gastrocnemius and biceps femoris are more active than during the hold period while the tibialis and biceps femoris are less active. In the transient period between bursting and settling, there are multiple cycles of oscillation in the EMG patterns of all the muscles. This pattern indicates that the harmonic nature of the multilink postural model is correct Muscle synergies and touch The very close agreement between the postural sway data and the fit of the presented model corroborate the thesis that posture control behaves as a linear controller. In Great interest lies in understanding the nature of postural restabilization following sudden loss of balance. Our approach was to study postural responses to recoverable falls created experimentally to mimic key features of tripping or loss of footing and at the same time to allow easy mathematical treatment. We wanted to assess whether recovery from being suddenly off-balance was handled by a posture controller regulating stiffness and damping properties of the body’s joints. In addition, we wanted to gauge whether haptic and visual cues affected the performance of such a controller. Finally, the conjunction of a technique for inducing recoverable falls and a model for analyzing them provides the potential for easy and portable clinical use. We demonstrated experimentally and analytically that by using a H&R paradigm we are able to induce sudden loss of balance and a brief period of forward falling. The peak angular displacements at the shoulders, hips, and knees were small following release. This allowed us to model the resulting biomechanical behavior of the body as a multilink, inverted pendulum in a regime of overall small oscillations. We found that a three-link, invertedpendulum model described the experimental data very well under all of our experimental conditions. Importantly, a compound linear inverted-pendulum model adequately described the behavior of all our subjects. This means that the postural response to H&R could be characterized using manageable linear systems theory. The linearity of the paradigm dynamics is very convenient for data scaling, data modeling, and system analysis. However, the real significance lies in the implications for the nature of the neuromuscular mech- 399 anisms responsible for balance recovery following release. Posture dynamics consists of biomechanics and control. Because the biomechanics is linearizable, the demonstrated linearity of the whole H&R response indicates that stiffness and damping (the linear PD controller we hypothesized above) is an excellent approximation of the neurophysiological characteristics of the posture controller at the joint level. In addition, we demonstrated that the stiffness and damping values are produced by reflexive mechanisms and not by passive muscle properties alone. Moreover, stiffness, and damping are with good approximation constant across the recoil. This implies that starting with the sudden release some mechanism determines the gains, set points, and phases of the sensory-motor feedback to be used across the whole recovery. The question is what mechanism produces this behavior. Figure 4 panels A–C show a representative case of H&R with touch cues from the fingertip. Note that the center of foot pressure and the ground shear remain unchanged for 125 ms after the onset of release. EMG activity increases 55 ms after onset of release in the gastrocnemius and biceps femoris muscles (see Fig. 4C). Before the EMG activations only acceleration, velocity, and position of the body segments (head included) show variations. A vestibular elicited response with 60–80 ms latencies is a possible basis for the EMG activity (Greenwood and Hopkins 1976, 1980); however, muscle spindle information is available sooner. Considering that muscle spindles have a low threshold and large afferent fibers, it is likely that within a few milliseconds after release spindle feedback signals could drive corrective responses. Another possibility is a predictive response. However, in such a case it would be difficult to explain why the response would take 55 ms to develop instead of occurring sooner after release in order to avoid exposing the body to falling. (In Appendix A, we discuss what happens to the center of foot pressure variation if the release is anticipated and how this can be used in the H&R paradigm to discard predicted and therefore unsuccessful trials.) Figure 9 presents a simplified schema of the neuromuscular mechanism underlying joint control. It shows how spinal circuitry and supraspinal modulation can perform postural joint control and how this could account for our findings. Descending supraspinal commands, a and b, drive a spinal servos. The common mode of the supraspinal drives (a+b) sets or modulates the stiffness and damping of the muscle pair around the joint. The differential drive (a%b) is proportional to the net joint torque, and it can provide additional set gain feedback or modulated feedback proportional to velocity offsets (damping) or position (stiffness) or force (impedance, omitted here) through the mediation of higher centers (supraspinal mechanisms). Spindle afferents and other sensory signals are mediated and integrated at a supraspinal level and relayed to the spinal servos by means of the a and b drives. This schema provides desired Fig. 9 A simplified schema of the neuromuscular mechanism of joint actuation. The a s represent the combined action of the spinal motor-neurons of the agonist-antagonist muscle pair around a joint. Afferent fibers II and Ia encode respectively joint position J and displacement rate J_ errors. Their activity modulates the spinal a motor servo and also projects to supraspinal centers where it is integrated with other sensory afferents. The kinematics variables J and J_ not only feed back on the spinal a motor servo through the muscle spindles but also modulate the muscle mechanics of the joint. J is the input to the force-length combined characteristic of the agonist-antagonist joint pair; and, J_ determines the combined viscous characteristic (Hill) of the agonist-antagonist joint pair. gs represents the descending static drives which encode the desired set point or motion reference of the joint. The a and b drives are the descending supraspinal commands to the a spinal servos. Golgi tendon organs, interneurons, and many other components of the spinal circuitry are omitted because they are beyond the scope of the figure. The common mode of the supraspinal drive (a+b) sets or modulates the stiffness and damping of the muscle around the joint in an open-loop, feedforward manner. Stiffness and damping are also produced by spindle feedback (under the drive of the descending gs, static, discharges: reference), which inhibits or excites the a spinal servos proportional to position and velocity (mostly) errors. The differential drive (a%b) determines the net joint torque and can provide additional set gain feedback or modulated feedback proportional to velocity, position, or force through mediation of supraspinal mechanisms. Spindle afferent and other sensory signals projecting centrally are integrated and interpreted by supraspinal mechanisms, which adjust the a and b drives to provide desired modulation of mechanical properties not achievable solely by the a spinal circuitry modulation of mechanical properties otherwise not achievable solely by the a spinal circuitry. Our findings suggest (1) that the common (a+b) drive and reflexive spindle pathways are probably set to provide constant stiffness since we find nop significant frequency ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi changes across conditions (w ¼ K=m, where K is stiffness and m is mass), and (2) that the g drives and/or the differential (a%b) drive are modulated by higher centers to provide joint velocity feedback (torque) appropriately timed to produce effective damping of body sway during sudden off-balance conditions. This sensory-motor integration scheme possibly could be tuned before release; however, its precise configuration and parametrical settings might also be adjusted within a few tens of milliseconds following release. In summary, we have demonstrated that postural recovery following H&R has a reflexive viscoelastic 400 behavior. Figure 8 shows the muscle synergies responsible for this finding. Patterned activation of the gastronemius-biceps femoris pair vs. the tibialis anterior-rectus femoris pair (and other muscles) allows for the control of the fore-aft motion of the center of foot pressure. If reflexive and descending control mechanisms were not involved, the motion of the perturbed posture would resemble that of a rocking chair and would extinguish very slowly. The stiffening and relaxing of the muscles must actually be timed in phase contrast to their stretching and shortening to produce dissipation of sway (kinetic energy). From an external point of view, this means that the center of foot pressure is adjusted in phase contrast to the sway velocity so that external forces create negative work on the body. The light touch force on the finger in touch trials is not responsible for the triggering of the EMG responses because it shows variations at the same time as the earlier EMG responses. However, light touch makes a difference in how rapidly the muscle synergies succeed in restoring balance. This suggests that there ae two separate mechanisms, one ruling the early part of the response and another the later phase (see also Denier and Dichgans 1986). Alternatively, there might be only one mechanism which utilizes all available information in a sensory fusion manner. Further analysis and experimentation are needed to clarify this issue. The touch cues also enabled the subjects to have smaller peak deflections of the torso than in the absence of touch. As discussed above, linear behavior carries the convenience that the magnitude of the postural response is proportional to the magnitude of the initiating perturbation. Consequently, the shift, cpx , of the center of foot pressure position between the hold phase and the settled posture (see Fig. 4A) can be used as a scaling factor to normalize the postural data across trials. This simplifies the H&R paradigm by not requiring identical perturbations in every trial and it makes the comparison and statistical treatment of data across conditions much simpler. An additional advantage is that the H&R paradigm can be used in patient populations using low hold force levels or in strong and healthy subjects using high force levels without affecting the validity of the later analyses. The H&R paradigm gives insights into what happens during unintended falls and potential clues with regard to possible prevention strategies. Even more importantly H&R allows for quantification of recovery performance from falling onset. Existing perturbation paradigms in which the support surface is tilted and/or translated yield important insights into postural control but are not specific models of natural off-balance events and falling onset. Posture platforms translate or rotate to induce a misalignment between the center of foot pressure and the center of mass, however, trials start with the two aligned. Friction between the platform and the subject’s feet during platform translation provides the force that constitutes the perturbation. The soles of the feet are therefore stimulated in the process and contribute somatosensory cues during the course of the perturbation that are not specific to an off-balance condition, but only to the posture platform. Moreover, a relatively long period of time is commonly used to carry out the perturbation, about 450 ms (see Horak and McPherson 1996). Such a period is long enough to stimulate and engage feedback loops from short to long, as well as volitional responses (see Brooks 1986). With stance surface perturbations the disturbance is also transmitted from the bottom up, linkby-link, through the entire chain of the body making the analysis of the overall process difficult. The H&R paradigm bypasses this first phase of joint flexion/ extension and starts with the center of foot pressure and center of mass already misaligned. Paradigms involving push and release from a hold force applied laterally or frontally at the waist (see Wing et al. 1993, 1995) by means of electromechanical devices involve much longer time courses (200 ms) for force removal than the simple manual releases in the H&R (35– 40 ms). A perturbation applied to the hip yields generally greater angular displacement at the hip than at the shoulder (Wing et al. 1995), which is the consequence of nonuniform and nonsimultaneous stimulation of the whole mechanical body chain. Wing et al. (1995) have implemented a releasing from push paradigm in which a mechanical apparatus applies a force laterally or frontally to the subject’s waist which can then be diminished to zero over an ffi200 ms period. This procedure is not analogous to H&R because the body is partially mechanically constrained during the 200 ms in which the push force is eliminated. In H&R, the release takes approximately 35 ms and the EMG synergies are activated within approximately 55 ms of the onset of the release (see Fig. 4A–C). By the time 200 ms has elapsed, the primary activity to restore balance has already been completed. In summary, we have introduced a H&R paradigm and conceptual and mathematical model for studying restoration of balance from recoverable falls. These tools allow quantitative measurements of postural performance to be interrelated with underlying physiological mechanisms. Acknowledgements We thank Dr. Joel Ventura and Dr. Stefano Castallani for their valuable assistance and advice and Dr. Todd Hudson for his review of our statistics. This research was supported by National Aeronautics and Space Administration grant, NASA NAG9-1263. Appendix A In our treatment, release has been addressed as instantaneous. Figure 4A shows that the force withdrawal requires about 35 ms. To reconcile this apparent contradiction we can compare the rise time of the posture response with the time of force withdrawal. From the H&R data presented above we can see that the fastest mode affecting the body’s displacements, xi, has a central frequency of approximately 0.55 Hz. Taking advantage of the Parseval theorem we can calculate an approximate rise time of 0.73 s for the sway response (quasi-analytical relationship tr ! 0.4/B; see also Kuo 1991, pp. 335–337, 571). Thus, instantaneous means that the rise time of the system is much larger than the duration of the perturbation. A withdrawal on the order of magnitude of 100 ms would still be fast enough to be considered instantaneous. 401 The Hold force is applied manually therefore it varies a few percent about its average value during the Hold period (Fig. 4C). Even if it varied as much as 10–20% the force fluctuation still would not be critical because the physical perturbation to postural control in H&R is not the holding force but the angular momentum acquired during the brief fall after release. Manual application of the force features high compliance, which guarantees that the means of application of the force would not “drive” the body as in pushing paradigms where stiff actuators are rigidly attached to the body. The only requirement of the paradigm is to produce a center of foot pressure transient comparable with the one of Fig. 4A. A steplike variation in the center of foot pressure and of the ground shear force guarantees that there was not cueing on the part of the experimenter. If the experimenter fails to catch the subject by surprise, the trace of the center of foot pressure differs from steplike and turns into oscillations or a hump. The steplike center of pressure transition indicates brief falling which in turn indicates surprise and the absence of this feature can be used as a criterion to discard unsuccessful trials. The dynamics and control of the H&R paradigm is linear in nature allowing for scalability of the results. However, the question is what is the scaling factor. Our analysis indicates that the center of foot pressure shift cpx is a good choice because it can be easily measured and it is proportional to the perturbation magnitude. Even though the H&R mathematical treatment is not compromised, differences between the settled posture and the initial hold posture affect the validity of cpx as scaling factor. Our experience with the H&R paradigm indicated that subjects, with good approximation, assume the same initial and final postures, which is very desirable. However, in cases where a great variability in final vs. initial posture has occurred the experimenter would probably need to integrate the cpx measure with other parameters in order carefully to describe the center of foot pressure and center of mass misalignment and the forward momentum, which constitute the H&R perturbation. Appendix B ”Small oscillations” of a triple inverted pendulum (or other stable mechanical system) occur when the system moves in a contained fashion about an equilibrium position with small angular displacements of its links (e.g., "10&). This allows sin (J) to be equated to J, and cos (J) to be equated to 1. These conditions were met by the kinematic data for all three links measured in our experiment. This allowed us to approximate the kinetic (T) and potential (V) energies as follows: 1 1 T ¼ q_ T H q_ ! ðq_ % q_ o ÞT ðHo þ H 0 ðq % qo ÞÞðq_ % q_ o Þ 2 2 1 ! ðq_ % q_ o ÞT Ho ðq_ % q_ o Þ þ oðq % qo Þ3 2 V ¼ Vo þ 1 @2V ðq % qo Þ2 þ oðq % qo Þ3 2 @q2 where @V=@qjq¼qo ¼ 0 because q0 is the position of equilibrium, q=(J1, J2, J3). A layout of the system and of the geometrical variables treated in this section is presented in Fig. 10. The Lagrange function of the system results in: 1 1 L ¼ T % V ffi q~_ T H0 ~q_ þ g~qT P0 ~q 2 2 where ~q is equal to (q–q0) and the matrices H0 and P0 are as follows: Fig. 10 The triple inverted pendulum model. The xi, i=1,..3, indicate the measured set of variables (also represented in Fig. 3). From the measured variables xi, i=1,..3, the model implicitly relates the angular variables Ji, i=1,..,3, utilized for determining the multilink postural dynamics H0 ¼ 2 m1 a21 þ m2 ð‘1 þ a2 Þ2 þ m3 ð‘1 þ ‘2 þ a3 Þ2 þ I1 þ I2 þ I3 4 %m2 a2 ð‘1 þ a2 Þ % m3 ð‘2 þ a3 Þð‘1 þ ‘2 þ a3 Þ % I2 % I3 m3 a3 ð‘1 þ ‘2 þ a3 Þ þ I3 %m2 a2 ð‘1 þ a2 Þ % m3 ð‘2 þ a3 Þð‘1 þ ‘2 þ a3 Þ % I2 % I3 m2 a22 þ m3 ð‘2 þ a3 Þ2 þ I2 þ I3 %m3 a3 ð‘2 þ a3 Þ % I3 3 %m3 a3 ð‘1 þ ‘2 þ a3 Þ þ I3 %m3 a3 ð‘2 þ a3 Þ % I3 5 m3 a23 þ I3 2 Po ¼ 4 %m1 a1 þ m2 ð‘1 þ a2 Þ % m3 ð‘1 þ ‘2 þ a3 Þ m2 a2 þ m3 ð‘2 þ a3 Þ %m3 a3 3 m2 a2 þ m3 ð‘2 þ a3 Þ %m3 a3 %m2 a2 % m3 ð‘2 þ a3 Þ m3 a3 5; m3 a3 %m3 a3 where m1–3 are the masses of the lower leg, upper leg and upper body, respectively, a1–3 are the distances from ankle, knee and hip to each segment’s center of mass, and ‘1%3 are the lengths of the segments. Thus, applying the Lagrange equations we obtain: ~ q % gP~ q ¼ %KD ~ q_ % KP ~ H€ q ð7Þ where the terms in KD and KP are the zero and first order approximations of the torques at the joints provided by the postural control mechanism. Our experimental observations provide measures of the displacements of the segments in the sagittal plane. To convert the Lagrangean coordinates qi into our experimental coordinates xi, we used the following transformation (for small oscillations): 402 2 dx ¼J¼4 dq ‘1 ‘1 þ ‘2 ‘1 þ ‘2 þ ‘3 3 0 0 %‘2 05 %ð‘2 þ ‘3 Þ ‘3 ð8Þ The coordinates xi, i=1,..,3, depicted in Figs. 3 and 10, are the horizontal displacements (sways) of respectively knee, hip, and shoulder in the sagittal plane; the ankle is assumed as origin. Substituting Eq. 8 within Eq. 7 we obtain the following: ðJ %1 ÞT HJ %1€x þ ðJ %1 ÞT KD J %1 x_ þðJ %1 ÞT ½KP % gP+J %1 x ¼ 0 In conclusion, the system dynamics assume the following form: M€x þ Cx_ þ Kx ¼ 0 ð9Þ Equation 9 is the equation of a multidegree of freedom oscillator. Since the upright posture is stable, it means that the matrices KP and KD are constrained to yield a stable triplet M, C, K. The general solution of Eq. 9 is a combination of simple components Fj, j=1,..,3. (These equations have been anticipated in the main text as Eq. 6.) 8 9 < a11 = F1 ¼ a21 ea1 t sinðb1 t þ g1 Þ; : ; a31 8 9 < a12 = F2 ¼ a22 ea2 t sinðb2 t þ g2 Þ; : ; a32 8 9 < a13 = F3 ¼ a23 ea3 t sinðb3 t þ g3 Þ : ; a33 ð10Þ Each component or modal response, j, is identified by the set of two parameters aj and bj, which represent respectively the rate of decay and pulsation of the component. For each modal response the parameters aij, i, j=1,..3, (unscaled eigenvectors) embed the shapes of the component contribution (e.g., en bloc movement or zig-zag movement) of the links and the extent of the component’s contribution (scaling factor). The case-by-case solution depends on the initial conditions (x0, ẋ0), which affect the phase parameters gij, i=1,..3, and the scaling of the parameters aij, i, j=1,..3j. The intrinsic nature of the system determines the parameters aj and bj, and the shapes of the modal responses (mutual ratios of the aij within the same component). The rate of decay a is an important parameter because the greater its magnitude the quicker is the fading over time of the related contribution to the overall response. Low values of the a parameter relate to slowly recovering behavior. For easing the comparison of experimental conditions the parameters aj and b were converted into the frequency and damping representation (wj, xj) treated in the results section, which carry overall the same physical meaning. References Aruin AS, Latash ML (1995) The role of motor action in anticipatory postural adjustments studied with self-induced and externally triggered perturbation. Exp Brain Res 106:291– 300 Aruin AS, Latash ML (1996) Anticipatory postural adjustments during self-initiated perturbations of different magnitude triggered by a standard motor action. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 101:497–503 Aruin AS, Shiratori T, Latash ML (2001) The role of action in postural preparation for loading and unloading in standing subjects. Exp Brain Res 138:458–466 Barin K, Stockwell CW (1983) A mathematical model of human postural control in the sagittal plane. In: Vestibular and visual control on posture and locomotor equilibrium. 7th International Symposium of the International Society of Posturography, Houston, pp 29–33 Beek PJ, van Santvoord AAM (1996) Dexterity in cascade juggling. In: Latash ML, Turvey MT (eds) Dexterity and its development. Erlbaum, Mahwah, pp 377–392 Blake AJ, Morgan MJ, Dallosso H, Ebrahim SBJ, Arie THD, Fentem PH, Bassey EJ (1988) Falls by elderly people at home: prevalence and associated factors. Age Ageing 17:365–372 Bouisset S, Zattara M (1981) A sequence of postural movements precedes voluntary movements. Neurosci Lett 22:263–270 Bouisset S, Zattara M (1988) Anticipatory postural adjustments and dynamic asymmetry of voluntary movement. In: Gurfinkel VS, Joffe ME, Massion J, Roll JP (eds) Stance and motion. Facts and concepts. Plenum, New York, pp 177–183 Brooks WB (1986) The neural bases of motor control. Oxford University Press, Oxford Campbell AJ, Borrie MJ, Spears GF, Jackson SL, Brown JS, Fitzgerald JL (1990) Circumstances and consequences of falls experienced by a community population 70 years and over during a prospective study. Age Ageing 19:163–171 Clapp S, Wing AM (1999) Light touch contribution to balance in normal bipedal stance. Exp Brain Res 125:521–524 Cordo PJ, Nashner LM (1982) Properties of postural adjustments associated with rapid arm movements. J Neurophysiol 47:287– 302 Crenna P, Frigo C (1991) A motor programme for the initiation of forward-oriented movements in humans. J Physiol (Lond) 437:635–653 Diener HC, Dichgans J (1986) Long loop reflexes and posture. In: Bles W, Brandt T (eds) Disorders of posture and gait. Elsevier, London pp 41–51 Diener HC, Dichgans J (1988) On the role of vestibular, visual and somatosensory information for dynamic postural control in humans. In: Pompeiano O, Allum JHJ (eds) Progress in brain research, vol 76. Elsevier, London, pp 253–262 Dietz V (1992) Human neuronal control of automatic functional movements-interactions between central programs and afferent input. Physiol Rev 72:33–69 Dietz V (1996) Interaction between central programs and afferent input in the control of posture and locomotion. J Biomech 29:841–844 Elger K, Wing A, Gilles M (1999) Integration of the hand in postural reactions to sustained sideways force of the pelvis. Exp Brain Res 128:52–60 Figliola R, Beeasley D (1995) Theory and design for mechanical measurement, 2nd edn. Wiley, New York Fregly AR (1974) Vestibular ataxia and its measurement in man. In: Kornhuber HH (ed) Handbook of sensory physiology. II. Psychophysics applied aspects and general interpretations. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 321–268 Gatev P, Thomas S, Kepple T, Hallett M (1999) Feedforward ankle strategy of balance during quiet stance in adults. J Physiol (Lond) 5114:915–928 Gelfand JM, Gurfinkel VS, Tsetlin ML, Shik ML (1971) Problems in the analysis of movements. In: Gurfinkel VS, Fomin SV, Tsetlin ML (eds) Models of the structural-functional organization of certain biological systems. MIT Press, London, pp 330– 345 Gilles M, Wing AM, Kirker SGB (1999) Lateral balance organization in human stance in response to a random or predictable perturbation. Exp Brain Res 124:137–144 Goldstein H, Poole CP, Safko JL (2002) Classical mechanics, 3rd edn. Prentice-Hall, New York– Greenwood R, Hopkins A (1976) Muscle responses during sudden falls in man. J Physiol (Lond) 254:507–518 Greenwood R, Hopkins A (1980) Motor control during stepping and falling in man. In: Desmedt JE (eds) Spinal and supraspinal mechanisms of voluntary motor control and locomotion. Karger, New York, pp 294–309 403 Gurfinkel VS, Latash ML (1979) Segmental postural mechanisms and reversals of muscle reflexes. Agressologie 20 [Suppl B]: 145–146 Gurfinkel VS, Osovets SM (1972) Dynamics of equilibrium of the vertical posture in man. Biofizika 17:478–485 Gurfinkel VS, Kots YM, Paltsev YJ, Feldman AG (1971) The compensation for respiratory disturbances of the erect posture of man as an example of the organization of inter-articular interaction. In: Gurfinkel VS, Fomin SV, Tsetlin ML (eds) Models of the structural-functional organization of certain biological systems. MIT Press, London, pp 382–395 Gurfinkel VS, Lipshits MI, Lestienne FG (1988) Anticipatory neck muscle activity associated with rapid arm movements. Neurosci Lett 94:104–108 Holden M, Ventura J, Lackner JR (1987) Influence of light touch from the hand on postural sway. Soc Neurosci Abstracts 13:348 Holden M, Ventura J, Lackner JR (1994) Stabilization of posture by precision contact of the index finger. J Vestib Res 4:285–301 Holt RR, Simpson D, Jenner JR, Kirker SG, Wing AM (2000) Ground reaction force after a sideways push as a measure of balance in recovery from stroke. Clin Rehabil 14:88–95 Horak FB, MacPherson JM (1996) Postural orientation and equilibrium. In: Rowell LB, Shep JT (eds) Handbook of physiology, sect 12. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 255– 2292 Horak FB, Nashner LM (1986) Central programming of postural movements: adaptation to altered support-surface configurations. Neurophysiology 55:1369–1381 Jeka JJ, Lackner JR (1994) Fingertip contact influences human postural control. Exp Brain Res 100:495–502 Jeka JJ, Lackner JR (1995) The role of haptic cues from rough and slippery surfaces on human postural control. Exp Brain Res 103:267–276 Jeka JJ, Oie K, Sch'ner G, Dijkstra TMH, Henson E (1998) Position and velocity coupling of postural sway to somatosensory drive. J Neurophysiol 79:1661–1674 Krebs DE, McGibbon CA, Goldvasser D (2001) Analysis of postural perturbation responses. IEEE Trans Rehabil Eng 9:76– 80 Kung S (1979) A new identification and model reduction algorithm via singular value decompositions. IEEE 705–714 Kuo BC (1991) Automatic control systems. Prentice-Hall, New York Lackner JR, DiZio P, Jeka JJ, Horak FB, Krebs D, Rabin E (1999) Precision contact of the fingertip reduces postural sway of individuals with bilateral vestibular loss. Exp Brain Res 126:459–466 Lackner JR, Rabin E, DiZio P (2001) Stabilization of posture by precision touch of the index finger with rigid and flexible filaments. Exp Brain Res 139:454–464 Lacquaniti F, Maioli C (1989) The role of preparation in tuning anticipatory and reflex responses during catching. J Neurosci 9:1134–1148 Luchies CW, Alexander NB, Schultz AB, Ashton-Miller J (1994) Stepping responses of young and old adults to postural disturbances: kinematics. J Am Geriatr Soc 42:506–512 Luchies CW, Wallace D, Pazdur R, Young S, DeYoung AJ (1999) Effects of age on balance assessment using voluntary and involuntary step tasks. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 54A:M140–M141 Marsden CD, Merton PA, Morton HB (1978) Anticipatory postural responses in the human subject (abstract). J Physiol (Lond) 275:47P–48P Marsden CD, Merton PA, Morton HB (1981) Human postural responses. Brain 104:513–534 Massion J (1984) Postural changes accompanying voluntary movements. Normal and pathological aspects. Hum Neurobiol 2:261–267 Massion J (1992) Movement, posture and equilibrium: interaction and coordination. Progr Neurobiol 38:35–56 Massion J, Dufosse M (1988) Coordination between posture and movement: why and how? News Physiol Sci 3:88–93 McIllroy WE, Maki BE (1995) Early activation of arm muscles follows external perturbation of upright stance. Neurosci Lett 184:177–180 Melvill Jones G, Watt DGD (1971) Muscle control of landing from unexpected falls in man. J Physiol (Lond) 219:709–727 Morasso P, Schieppati M (1999) Can muscle stiffness alone stabilize upright standing? J Neurophysiol 83:1622–1626 Nardone A, Scheippati M (1988) Postural adjustments associated with voluntary contraction of leg muscles in standing man. Exp Brain Res 69:469–480 Nashner LM (1971) A model describing vestibular detection of body sway. Acta Otolaryngol (Stockh) 72:429–436 Nashner LM (1977) Fixed patterns of rapid postural responses among leg muscles during stance. Exp Brain Res 30:13–24 Nashner LM (1987) A systems approach to understanding and assessing orientation and balance disorders. Neurocom Intl Nashner LM, Forssberg H (1986) Phase-dependent organization of postural adjustments associated with arm movements while walking. Neurophysiology 55:1382–1394 Nashner LM, McCollum G (1985) The organization of human postural movements: a formal basis and experimental synthesis. Behav Brain Sci 8:135–172 Neddler JA, Mead R (1965) A simplex method for function minimization. Computer J 7:308–313 Overstall PW, Exton-Smith AN, Imms FJ, Johnson AL (1977) Falls in the elderly related to postural imbalance. BMJ 1:261–264 Pai YC, Patton J (1997) Center of mass velocity-position predictions for balance control. J Biomech 30:3447–433544 Pai YC, Rogers MW, Patton J, Cain TD, Hanke TA (1998) Static versus dynamic predictions of protective stepping following waist-pull perturbations in young and older adults. J Biomech 31:1111–1118 Patton JL, Pai YC, Lee WA (1999) Evaluation of a model that determines the stability limits of dynamic balance. Gait Posture 9:38–49 Paulignan Y, Dufosse M, Hugon M, Massion J (1989) Acquisition of coordination between posture and movement in a bimanual task. Exp Brain Res 77:337–3448 Pidcoe PE, Rogers MW (1998) A closed-loop stepper motor waistpull system for inducing stepping in humans. J Biomech 31:377–381 Rabin E, Bortolami SB, DiZio P, Lackner JR (1999) Haptic Stabilization of posture: changes in arm proprioception and cutaneous feedback for different arm orientations. J Neurophysiol 82:3541–3549 Rabin E, Bortolami SB, DiZio P, Lackner JR (2000) Modeling the impulse response of human upright posture. Soc Neurosci Abstract 64:18 Rietdyk S, Patla AE, Winter DA, Ishac MG, Little CE (1999) NACOB presentation CSB New Investigator award. Balance recovery from medio-lateral perturbations of the upper body during standing. North American Congress on Biomechanics. J Biomech 32:1149–1158 Roberts TDM (1975) Reflex contributions to the assessment of the vertical. Acta Astronaut 2:59–67 Rubenstein LZ, Robbins AS, Schulman BL, Rosado J, Osterweil D, Josephson KR (1988) Falls and Instability in the elderly. J Am Geriatr Soc 36:266–278 Sanes JN, Jennings VA (1984) Centrally programmed patterns of muscle activity in voluntary motor behavior of humans. Exp Brain Res 54:23–32 Schieppati M, Nardone A, Corna S (1995a) Do secondary spindle afferent fibres play a role in the late response to stretch of leg muscles in humans? In: Taylor A, Gladden MH, Durbaba R (eds) Alpha and gamma motor systems. Plenum, New York Schieppati M, Nardone A, Corna S (1995b) Time course of setrelated changes in muscle response to stance perturbation in humans. J Physiol (Lond) 487:787–796 Stockwell CW (1983) Conceptual models of human postural control. In: Vestibular and visual control on posture and locomotor equilibrium. 7th International Symposium of the International Society of Posturography. Houston, pp 22–28 404 Thelen DG, Wojcik LA, Schultz AB, Ashton-Miller JA, Alexander NB (1997) Age differences in using a rapid step to regain balance during a forward fall. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 52:M8–M13 Thelen DG, Muriuki M, Schultz AB, Ashton-Miller JA, Alexander NB (2000) Muscle activities used by young and old adults when stepping to regain balance during a forward fall. J Electromyogr Kinesiol 10:93–101 Westling G, Johansson RS (1987) Responses in glabrous skin mechanoreceptors during precision grip in humans. Exp Brain Res 66:128–140 Wicke RW, Oman CM (1982) Visual and graviceptive influences on lower leg EMG activity in humans during brief falls. Exp Brain Res 46:324–330 Wing A, Goodrich S, Virji-Babul N, Jenner JR, Clapp S (1993) Balance evaluation in hemiparetic stroke patients using lateral forces applied to the hip. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 74:292–299 Wing AM, Clapp S, Burgess-Limerick R (1995) Standing stability in the frontal plane determined by lateral forces applied to the hip. Gait Posture 3:38–42 Wing A, Flanagan JR, Richardson J (1997) Anticipatory postural adjustments in stance and grip. Exp Brain Res 116:122–130 Winter DA, Prince F, Frank JS, Powell C, Zabjek KF (1996) Unified theory regarding A/P and M/L balance in quiet stance. J Neurophysiol 75:2334–2343 Winter DA, Patla AE, Prince F, Ishac M, Gielo-Perczak K (1998) Stiffness control of balance in quiet stance. J Neurophysiol 80:1211–1221 Winter DA, Patla AE, Pietdyk S, Ishac MG (2001) Ankle muscle stiffness in the control of balance during quiet standing. J Neurophysiol 85:2630–2633 Wojcik LA, Thelen DG, Schultz AB, Ashton-Miller JA, Alexander NB (1999) Age and gender differences in single-step recovery from a forward fall. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 54:M44– M50