Ecology, 78(3), 1997, pp. 782–798 q 1997 by the Ecological Society of America WHY ARE SOME FRUITS TOXIC? GLYCOALKALOIDS IN SOLANUM AND FRUIT CHOICE BY VERTEBRATES MARTIN L. CIPOLLINI1 AND DOUGLAS J. LEVEY Department of Zoology, University of Florida, 223 Bartram Hall, Gainesville, Florida 32611 USA Abstract. We examined the influence of secondary metabolites (glycoalkaloids) of Solanum fruits on fruit consumption by seed dispersers and seed predators. Our goal was to determine the degree to which secondary metabolites might explain fruit–frugivore interactions that heretofore have not been explained by other fruit-related variables (e.g., color, nutrient content, seediness, etc.). Using feeding trails with real fruits and artificial fruit pulp media, we tested two sets of hypotheses, one relating to the specificity of secondary metabolites and the other to intra- and interspecific variation in secondary metabolite content. First, the Directed Toxicity Hypothesis was tested against its alternative, the General Toxicity Hypothesis. The Directed Toxicity Hypothesis posits that fruit pulp secondary metabolites are directed primarily toward organisms that destroy seeds following fruit consumption, remaining nontoxic to organisms that disperse seeds. The General Toxicity Hypothesis states that these secondary metabolites are general in their effects. Second, the Removal Rate Hypothesis was tested against its alternative, the Nutrient–Toxin Titration Hypothesis. The Removal Rate Hypothesis posits that profitable fruits, with high natural rates of removal, contain only low levels of secondary metabolites as defenses against seed predators and pathogens. In contrast, the Nutrient–Toxin Titration Hypothesis states that profitable fruits are better able to afford protection by high levels of secondary metabolites because high nutrient levels compensate for deterrent effects of secondary metabolites. We compared Solanum carolinense, with high levels of secondary metabolites (glycoalkaloids) in ripe fruit, and S. americanum, with negligible levels, conducting four sets of feeding trials: (1) Real Fruit trials to determine the preferences of our study animals for real fruit of each plant species; (2) Fruit Mimic trials using artificial fruit pulp media to determine the relative effects of nutrient and glycoalkaloid content on fruit preferences; (3) Interaction trials using artificial fruit pulp media to examine relative and interactive effects of the two main glycoalkaloids in our fruits ( a-solasonine and a-solamargine); and (4) Nutrient–Toxin Titration trials using artificial fruit pulp media to determine how much nutrient content might compensate for deterrent effects. Both seed predators and seed dispersers preferred the low-glycoalkaloid S. americanum fruits and were deterred by glycoalkaloid levels typical of ripe S. carolinense fruits. Because of the generality of the deterrent effects, we rejected the Directed Toxicity Hypothesis. Additionally, we found no evidence for differences between the two glycoalkaloids in their deterrent effects, and no evidence of synergism between them. Nutrient effects were generally small; we found no evidence that nutrient variation could compensate for the deterrence produced by levels of glycoalkaloids found in ripe S. carolinense fruits. Ripe fruits of the presumably more profitable, preferred S. americanum contain much lower levels of glycoalkaloid. Taken together, our results support the Removal Rate Hypothesis. Secondary metabolites of ripe fleshy fruits are an important determinant of fruit use by frugivores. We suggest that a primary function of these metabolites is defense against pests and pathogens. Thus, these organisms should be considered an important influence on fruit–seed disperser interactions. Key words: antifungal compounds; antiherbivore compounds; chemical defenses; fruit–seed disperser interactions; fruit pulp nutrients; secondary plant metabolites; seed dispersal; Solanum americanum; Solanum carolinense. INTRODUCTION A central issue in studies of fruit–seed disperser interactions is the degree to which reciprocal selection Manuscript received 23 August 1995; revised 16 May 1996; accepted 11 July 1996; final version received 2 August 1996. 1 Present address: Biology Department, 430 Berry College, Mount Berry, Georgia 30149 USA. pressures between fruiting plants and frugivores have influenced morphological, chemical, and behavioral traits in each group (Herrera 1985, 1986, Jordano 1987, Howe 1992, Stiles and Rosselli 1992). A basic assumption is that predictable differences in quality of seed dispersal among groups of frugivores can explain variation in fruit traits (Schupp 1992). Yet, most attempts to delineate such groups have been only mar- 782 April 1997 EFFECTS OF GLYCOALKALOIDS IN SOLANUM FRUITS ginally successful or have not fully incorporated phylogeny (e.g., Gautier-Hion et al. 1985, Debussche et al. 1987, Debussche and Isenmann 1989, Willson et al. 1989, Gautier-Hion 1990). In fact, recent studies using rigorous phylogenetic null models have failed to support the notion that frugivores influence variation in fruit traits (Herrera 1987, 1992, Jordano 1987, Bremer and Eriksson 1992). Although some data suggest congruence between the type of fruit ingested and the digestive physiology of particular frugivores (e.g., Martinez del Rio et al. 1989), these studies cannot be taken as proof that frugivores have influenced fruit traits. Not only might the converse be true (frugivores may adapt to independently evolving fruit traits), but ecological ‘‘fitting’’ processes might also explain such patterns, since animal species might utilize only those fruits to which they have been pre-adapted (cf., Janzen 1985). We suggest that most previous studies of fruit–seed disperser interactions have been limited by focusing solely upon vertebrate seed dispersers. Many potentially detrimental organisms may compete with legitimate seed dispersers for fruit resources (Janzen 1977, Herrera 1982). Their effects on the evolution of fruit traits will be different than, and even opposite to, the effects of seed dispersers. Such organisms include vertebrate seed-predators, insect fruit- and seed-predators, and microbial fruit-rot agents. In contrast with organisms that defecate, or otherwise disperse, a majority of ingested seeds in viable condition, such organisms kill nearly all of the seeds in the process of fruit consumption and digestion. Viewed in this context, plants are faced with an evolutionary dilemma: how to make fruits simultaneously attractive to seed-dispersing frugivores and unattractive to all other frugivores. Fruit secondary metabolites are probably involved as mediators of this conflict (Janzen 1977, Herrera 1982, Sorensen 1983, Cipollini and Stiles 1992). Understanding the nature of fruit secondary metabolites and their effects upon various fruit consumers will help to resolve an apparent evolutionary paradox: ripe fruits that are apparently poisonous to potential dispersers (e.g., Atropa belladonna L. and some Solanum spp.). In the same way that recognition of defensive secondary metabolites revolutionized studies of plant–herbivore and plant–pathogen interactions (Rhoades and Cates 1976, Rhoades 1979, 1985), we hope that study of such compounds will yield a new perspective on fruit–seed disperser interactions. Aside from several theoretical papers (Herrera 1982, Izhaki and Safriel 1989, 1990, Mack 1990, Sedinger 1990), this area has been practically ignored. In fact, with few exceptions, secondary metabolite composition of ripe fruit remains unexplored (Goldstein and Swain 1963, Dement and Mooney 1974, Janzen 1975, 1979, 1983, Wrangham and Waterman 1983, Ballington et al. 1988, Gargiullo and Stiles 1991, Cipollini and Stiles 1993). Here, we develop and test alternative hypotheses about the role of glycoalkaloids in Solanum fruits. Gly- 783 coalkaloids, which are virtually restricted to the Solanaceae, are composed of C27 steroid aglycones to which various branched carbohydrate moieties are attached (Ripperger and Schreiber 1981). The best known compounds are the potato glycoalkaloids, a-solanine and a-chaconine (Gregory 1984, Van Gelder 1990), but the structurally related compounds a-solasonine and asolamargine occur more broadly among Solanum species (Ripperger and Schreiber 1981). Glycoalkaloids can be highly toxic to vertebrates; in some species, their concentration in ripe fruit (up to 7% of dry mass) should be high enough to cause lethal effects in a 1– 2 kg vertebrate after consumption of ,10 fruits (based on an estimate of a 100–1000 mg/kg lethal dose in clinical and case studies with domestic animals and humans; Zitnak 1979, Van Gelder 1990). To our knowledge, neither toxicity tests nor feeding-preference trials have been conducted on wild animals using these or any other glycoalkaloids. To test our hypotheses, we used feeding trials on two species of birds (American Robin, Turdus migratorius; Northern Bobwhite, Tetrao virginianus) and two species of mammals (Virginia opossum, Didelphis marsupialis; deer mouse, Peromyscus maniculatus). These species were selected because they are common consumers of Solanum fruits (Martin et al. 1951; M. Cipollini, personal observations) and because they presumably differ in their treatment of seeds. American Robins and opossums are likely to be seed dispersers, whereas Northern Bobwhites and deer mice are likely to be seed predators. We first conducted seed viability tests on defecated seeds to confirm these presumed differences in seed treatment. We then conducted one set of feeding trials with real fruit to determine natural preferences, and three sets of feeding trials with artificial fruits to tease apart the influence of nutrient and glycoalkaloid content on preference patterns. Hypotheses We present two sets of hypotheses. The first set addresses the specificity of fruit pulp secondary metabolites, which may depend on how predictably frugivores differ in their treatment of seeds. The second set addresses whether or not removal rates of fruits and pulp nutrient content can explain patterns of intra- and interspecific variation in secondary metabolites of fruit pulp. Secondary metabolite specificity: alternative hypotheses 1. Directed Toxicity Hypothesis.—Predictable variation among fruit consumers in their effects upon seed dispersal has resulted in selection for specificity in the toxicity of secondary metabolites of ripe fruit. Thus, secondary metabolites of unripe fruit should be directed toward all consumers, whereas those of ripe fruit should be directed only toward nondispersing consumers (seed predators). A form of this hypothesis was 784 MARTIN L. CIPOLLINI AND DOUGLAS J. LEVEY proposed initially by Janzen (1975, 1979), who suggested that secondary metabolites within fruit pulp may ‘‘filter out’’ or repel only groups of consumers that negatively affect seed dispersal. This hypothesis is distinct from the ‘‘optimal defense theory’’ (Rhoades and Cates 1976), in that no relationship is presumed between ‘‘generalist’’ and ‘‘specialist’’ herbivores and type of secondary metabolite (e.g., ‘‘quantitative’’ vs. ‘‘qualitative’’ chemicals). Fruit–frugivore interactions differ fundamentally from plant–herbivore interactions because of the positive effects of fruit consumption by particular animals (seed dispersers), which can contrast markedly with the negative effects of other organisms (seed predators). 2. General Toxicity Hypothesis.—Unpredictable or indistinguishable variation among fruit consumers in their effects upon seed dispersal has resulted in general toxicity of secondary metabolites within ripe fruit. Thus, seed-dispersing and non-seed-dispersing frugivores will be affected equally and adversely by pulp secondary compounds in both ripe and unripe fruit. This hypothesis suggests that retention of toxins in ripe fruits represents a trade-off between defense against damaging organisms and attraction of dispersers (Herrera 1982). In short, ‘‘diffuse’’ interactions between frugivores and fruiting plants constrain selection for directed toxicity. Although little is known about the biological activities of the compounds found in our study fruits (asolasonine and a-solamargine), particularly in a natural context, we note that toxic effects of the structurally similar potato glycoalkaloids solanine and chaconine may differ, and that toxicity thresholds may vary among vertebrate (Zitnak 1979, Baker et al. 1989), insect (Gregory 1984, Tingey 1984), and fungal species (McKee 1959, Nes et al. 1983, Rowan et al. 1983, Gregory 1984, Roddick 1986, 1987, Van Gelder 1990). Although glycoalkaloids vary in their toxicity among groups of animals (thus lending credence to the Directed Toxicity Hypothesis), their initial mode of activity is somewhat general. Initial toxic symptoms are assumed to result from sterol binding (cholesterol, ergosterol, phytosterol) and loss of lipid membrane integrity (Alauddin and Martin-Smith 1962, Zitnak 1979, Nes et al. 1983, Gregory 1984, Tingey 1984, Sidhu and Oakenfull 1986, Roddick 1987). For vertebrates, arthropods, and fungi alike, sterol binding results in altered membrane permeability and tissue damage (Zitnak 1979, Roddick 1986). However, variation among organisms in their sensitivity to glycoalkaloids may depend upon the binding affinity of the glycoalkaloid for particular sterols, as well as the quality and quantity of sterol within the cell membrane (Roddick 1987, Roddick et al. 1990, Keukens et al. 1992). Secondary toxic symptoms presumably result from interference with acetylcholinesterase function, which can produce fatal neurological effects in vertebrates (Zitnak 1979, Roddick 1989). Because nearly all studies of glycoalkaloid Ecology, Vol. 78, No. 3 toxicity have used intravenous or intraperitoneal injection (Van Gelder 1990), the mechanisms by which vertebrates might vary in their overall sensitivity to naturally occurring dietary glycoalkaloids are unclear. Intra- and interspecific variation in ripe fruit chemistry: alternate hypotheses 1. Removal Rate Hypothesis.—The degree to which a particular plant defends its fruits against pests and pathogens via secondary metabolites depends primarily on the removal rate of ripe fruit. Fruits that are removed quickly and predictably by frugivores should be under little selection pressure for persistence. Conversely, fruits that are removed slowly and unpredictably should be under greater selection pressure for defense. Thus, we predict that plants bearing fruits of high nutritional quality or fruits that are, for other reasons, removed quickly and predictably, will show lower levels of secondary metabolites than those plants bearing fruits that are removed slowly. 2. Nutrient–Toxin Titration Hypothesis.—Due to palatability reduction that results from secondary metabolites, the level of secondary metabolites within ripe fruit should be positively related to nutritional quality. As a means of maintaining acceptability, low-nutrient fruits should be under selection to retain low levels of unpalatable secondary metabolites. Conversely, fruits with high nutritional quality should be profitable enough to allow the retention of higher levels of defenses. Thus, we predict that plants bearing fruits with high nutritional quality will show higher levels of secondary metabolites than plants with fruits of low nutritional quality. Additionally, feeding trials with frugivores will show that high nutritional quality can compensate for high levels of toxins. MATERIALS AND METHODS Plant species An advantage of working with Solanum is that secondary metabolites in this genus are well known, due to the agricultural importance of several species (e.g., potato, S. tuberosum L., and tomato, S. lycopersicum L.). Of particular interest is the propensity for immature Solanum fruits to accumulate high levels of alkaloids (1–7% dry mass); levels that may or may not drop during fruit maturation. In particular, although most ripe Solanum fruits contain little or no glycoalkaloid (Briggs et al. 1961, Schrieber 1963, Carle 1981), fruits of several species retain levels nearly equivalent to, or even exceeding, that of unripe fruits (e.g., S. melongena L., Aubert et al. 1989a, b; S. tuberosum L. and S. xanthocarpum, Zitnak 1979; S. viarum and S. lacinatum, Miller and Davies 1979; S. aculeatissimum (khasianum) Jacq., Mahato et al. 1980, Nes et al. 1980; S. pseudocapsicum L., Mangla and Kamal 1989). We focus on two contrasting Solanum species that are abundant in central Florida, S. americanum Mill, April 1997 EFFECTS OF GLYCOALKALOIDS IN SOLANUM FRUITS and S. carolinense L.. Both species are annual or shortlived perennials common in agricultural and other disturbed landscapes throughout the southeastern United States. In comparison with S. americanum, S. carolinense fruits are relatively low in sugar (3–4% vs. 5– 6% wet mass) and water content (80–90% vs. 90–95% wet mass; M. Cipollini and D. Levey, unpublished data). The large (ø10–20 mm in diameter), yellow fruits of S. carolinense ripen in late fall and winter (October through December) and are classified by White (1989) as low-quality and mammal-dispersed fruits. Martin et al. (1951) indicate that S. carolinense is consumed by both birds and mammals. S. americanum ripens its small (5–9 mm in diameter), black fruits in the summer and early fall (July through October) and is probably dispersed primarily by birds (Martin et al. 1951), although small mammals also consume the fruits (Martin et al. 1951; M. Cipollini, personal observations). Both species have large numbers of seeds, 40–170 seeds per fruit, but seeds of S. americanum (ø1.5–1.7 mm in diameter) are smaller than those of S. carolinense (ø2.2–2.7 mm in diameter; United States Department of Agriculture 1974). Our alkaloid estimates (M. Cipollini, D. Levey, and R. Bushway, unpublished data) indicate that unripe fruits of both species contain 10–30 mg/g dry mass of total glycoalkaloid, primarily a-solasonine and a-solamargine. However, ripe fruits of S. americanum are substantially lower in alkaloid content (,1 mg/g) than those of S. carolinense (10–30 mg/g). Fruits and seeds of S. americanum and S. carolinense were collected from wild populations in Maryland and Florida in 1991–1993. To provide sufficient fruit samples for feeding trials and chemical analyses, we propagated 20 seedlings derived from a single maternal plant from each of 10 wild collections of each species (200 total plants of each species). Plants were germinated and grown in a greenhouse, and then transplanted to a tilled experimental field (Austin Cary Memorial Forest, Alachua County, Florida). Animal species American Robins (N 5 14) were mist-netted in January, 1995 near Waldo, Florida. They are typically frugivorous at this time of year, and were maintained on a fruit-based diet (Denslow et al. 1987). Northern Bobwhites (N 5 14), 10 wk old, were purchased from a local breeder who regularly outbreeds captive stock with wild populations. We fed them Purina Game Bird Chow (Purina Mills, St. Louis, Missouri) and did not conduct experiments with them until they were mature, several months later. Deer mice (N 5 12) were obtained from a captive population (D. Dewsbury, University of Florida) that outbred with wild stock less than three generations ago. They were fed LabDiet #5001 (Purina Mills, St. Louis, Missouri). Opossums (N 5 9) were trapped in December 1994 on the University of Florida campus and maintained on Purina Cat Chow (Purina 785 Mills, St. Louis, Missouri). All animals were provided with food and water ad libitum, except immediately before a feeding trial, when food was removed for 8 h. They were kept on a 12 h:12 h L:D cycle (reversed for mammals) and a constant temperature of 238C. Feeding trials with birds were conducted under fluorescent light, whereas those with mammals were conducted during ‘‘dark’’ hours under red light. Our animal handling and experimental protocol was reviewed and approved by the University of Florida’s Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (Approval #1336). These animal species were selected for several reasons: (1) they are reported consumers of Solanum fruits (Martin et al. 1951), or have been observed feeding upon Solanum fruits in the field (M. Cipollini, personal observations); (2) a priori observations (M. Cipollini, unpublished data) suggested that they would differ dramatically in their treatment of seeds (American Robins and opossums are primarily seed dispersers; deer mice and Northern Bobwhites are primarily seed predators); (3) they were easily captured and maintained under laboratory conditions; and (4) they could be trained readily to consume ‘‘artificial fruit pulp’’ diets based upon agar. Extraction and purification of glycoalkaloids We isolated a-solasonine and a-solamargine by column chromatography of crude glycoalkaloid extracts from oven-dried samples of Solanum aculeatissimum (provided by M. Weissenberg, Volcani Center, Israel), S. carolinense, and S. americanum. Finely milled pulp samples were pre-extracted with petroleum ether and dichloromethane to remove fats, waxes, and pigments. We then extracted the fruit pulp residue to exhaustion with hot ethanol, and rotoevaporated the ethanol extract to dryness. The dried ethanol extract was taken up with 1% acetic acid, the pH was shifted to ø11.5 using 30% ammonia, and the extract was heated to 708C for 15 min. After the ammonified extract had cooled at 48C overnight, we extracted the crude glycoalkaloid precipitate using chloroform. We rotoevaporated the crude glycoalkaloid fraction to dryness, dissolved it in watersaturated l-butanol, and fractionated it on a 5 3 70 cm chromatography column (neutral alumina). Fractions (50 mL) eluted with water-saturated l-butanol were collected and monitored for purity using TLC (chloroform : methanol : 1% ammonia (2:2:1); silica gel: Dragendorf’s reagent). Pure fractions co-chromatographing with authentic a-solasonine and a-solamargine (authenticated using TLC, HPLC, and FABMS by M. Weissenberg, Volcani Center, Israel) were collected and rotoevaporated to dryness. Purity of the final extracts was verified using HPLC (R. Bushway, unpublished data). In all feeding experiments, we used citric acid to adjust the pH of artificial fruits containing these glycoalkaloids to 5.7, the average pH of S. americanum and S. carolinense fruits (M. Cipollini, unpublished data). We do not know if pH affects toxicity of gly- 786 MARTIN L. CIPOLLINI AND DOUGLAS J. LEVEY coalkaloids for vertebrates, but we felt that it was prudent to control for pH, because taste perception of Solanum glycoalkaloids is pH-dependent in humans (spicy-hot at high pH, bitter at low pH; Zitnak 1979), and glycoalkaloids have been noted to have pH-dependent effects in their antifungal and sterol-binding activities (McKee 1959, Roddick et al. 1990, Fewell and Roddick 1993). Seed passage trials To document differences in seed treatment among our frugivore species, we fed three individuals of each species a known quantity of S. americanum and, later, S. carolinense fruits that had been bulk-collected from many plants at our field site. Seed numbers in these feedings were estimated from seed number : fruit mass ratio determined on subsamples of these fruits. We collected all feces 24 h after fruit consumption, washed them through a 0.5-mm mesh screen, and counted the number of superficially intact seeds. Subsequent observations indicated that nearly 100% of seeds passed through the gut of all studied animals within 24 h. Additionally, tests using tetrazolium HCl (Hauser and Morrison 1964) confirmed that seeds we classified as intact contained viable embryos. We analyzed these data using factorial ANOVA to determine the effect of Animal Species, Fruit Species, and their interaction on the arcsine-transformed proportion of seeds passed intact. Real Fruit trials We first addressed the General Toxicity vs. Directed Toxicity Hypotheses via feeding trials using natural S. americanum and S. carolinense fruits. Similar patterns of consumption of these two species by seed predators and seed dispersers would support the General Toxicity Hypothesis, whereas different consumption patterns would probably support the Directed Toxicity Hypothesis. We presented ø50 g of fresh fruit to eight individuals of each species. Trials lasted 15 min, after which we recorded fruit mass consumed, Mc (N 5 6– 8 trials for each individual frugivore–fruit species combination). Trials were run in ‘‘sets,’’ so that within each set, each frugivore was presented a random sequence of eight fruit samples, four of which were S. americanum and four S. carolinense. We analyzed preferences using a multivariate model, repeated-measures ANOVA, with ln(Mc 1 1) as the dependent variable and Fruit Species and Trial as within-factors variables (SuperANOVA; Abacus Concepts, 1989). For these and subsequent ANOVAs, the dependent variable (Mc) was ln-transformed prior to statistical analysis to reduce positive mean variance relationships. Additionally, for these and subsequent feeding trials, we ran at least three sets. If, after three sets, any main effect was marginally significant (0.05 , P # 0.10), we ran one or two additional sets and recalculated the statistics. Ecology, Vol. 78, No. 3 This type of experimental design assumes that trials within a set are random and independent, yet trials were randomized sequentially for each frugivore within each set. Consumption of a particular fruit type within any trial might depend upon the fruit types experienced in prior feeding trials. In order to reduce possible dependence among trials, we allowed a 15-min waiting period between successive feeding trials and randomly presented fruit types to each frugivore. This assured us that any trial with S. americanum had an equal probability of being preceded by a trial with S. americanum or S. carolinense. The type of short-term, single-choice feeding trial approach we applied is a compromise between multiple-choice tests and long-term, single-choice tests. Multiple-choice tests, where two or more food items are presented simultaneously, may exacerbate preference differences and may also suffer from lack of independence (Peterson and Renaud 1989, Roa 1992). Randomization of presentation order in short-term, single-choice trials presumably minimizes the problem of dependence. Although relatively free from problems of dependence, long-term, single-choice experiments could underestimate preferences because animals may eventually consume the less preferred foods, either as hunger develops or after the more preferred foods have been consumed. Artificial fruit pulp trials Real Fruit feeding trials were meant to assess fruit preferences in the absence of confounding variables present in the field (cf. Moermond and Denslow 1983, Levey et al. 1984). Nevertheless, we lacked control over many other fruit characteristics that may affect fruit choice but were not of direct interest to us (e.g., color, size, seediness). To control such variables and, thereby, focus on how nutrients and secondary metabolites influence fruit preference, we used artificial fruit pulp diets. To reduce possible effects of habituation, 50–75% of the animals used for Real Fruit trials were replaced by ‘‘naive’’ animals that we assumed had little or no prior experience with these fruits. Fruit Mimic trials.—The purpose of these trials was to establish the chemical basis of preference patterns. We mimicked the pulp chemistry of ripe and unripe fruit in ‘‘nontoxic’’ (2) agars of each species, using artificial components (carbohydrates, lipids, protein, water, pH, and fiber; Table 1). Otherwise identical ‘‘toxic’’ (1) agars had the glycoalkaloids a-solasonine (hereafter SA) and a-solamargine (hereafter SM) added in proportions that reflected their concentrations in unripe or ripe fruits (Unripe (1) or Ripe (1) agars; Table 1). Concentrations of various constituents were determined by prior chemical analyses of unripe and ripe fruits of each study species (M. Cipollini, D. Levey, and R. Bushway, unpublished data). Feeding trails were conducted and analyzed as for Real Fruit trials, except that Agar Type was substituted for Fruit Species, April 1997 EFFECTS OF GLYCOALKALOIDS IN SOLANUM FRUITS 787 TABLE 1. Chemical composition of artificial fruit agars used in this study. Data are mass (g) of each component used to formulate 100 g of media (‘‘1’’ denotes presence of glycoalkaloids and ‘‘2’’ denotes their absence). SUG† CHO‡ LIP§ PRO\ FIB¶ SM# SA†† Water A) Fruit Mimic Trials S. carolinense Ripe (1) Ripe (2) Unripe (1) Unripe (2) 3.75 3.75 2.94 2.94 1.50 1.50 0.70 0.70 0.75 0.75 0.70 0.70 1.05 1.05 1.12 1.12 7.35 7.35 7.90 7.98 0.264 0.000 0.255 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.008 0.000 85.00 85.00 86.00 86.00 S. americanum Ripe (1) Ripe (2) Unripe (1) Unripe (2) 5.39 5.39 1.12 1.12 0.70 0.70 0.32 0.32 0.35 0.35 0.28 0.28 0.56 0.56 0.48 0.48 0.00 0.00 1.64 1.64 0.005 0.000 0.041 0.000 0.003 0.000 0.047 0.000 93.00 93.00 96.00 96.00 4.00 4.00 4.00 4.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 0.58 0.58 0.58 0.58 0.50 0.50 0.50 0.50 4.00 4.00 4.00 4.00 0.000 0.100 0.200 0.000 0.200 0.100 0.000 0.000 90.00 90.00 90.00 90.00 C) Nutrient–Toxin trials Low (1) 1.20 4.00 Average (1) 7.20 High (1) 4.00 Control (2) 0.30 1.00 3.20 1.00 0.15 0.58 0.85 0.58 0.15 0.50 0.85 0.50 8.20 4.00 0.00 4.00 0.100 0.100 0.100 0.000 0.100 0.100 0.100 0.000 90.00 90.00 90.00 90.00 Agar type B) Interaction trials SA (1) SA 1 SM (1) SM (1) Control (2) Note: All media were prepared using 2% agar solution held at 708C prior to the addition of alkaloid. Final pH was adjusted to 5.7 with citric acid. Concentrations of various constituents were determined by prior chemical analysis of unripe and ripe fruits of each study species (M. Cipollini, D. Levey, and R. Bushway, unpublished data). † Sugars (glucose : fructose : sucrose, 2:2:1). ‡ Complex carbohydrates (starch : pectin, 3:1). § Lipids (peanut oil : corn oil, 1:1). \ Protein (soy protein isolate). ¶ Fiber (milled pure cellulose fiber). # a-solamargine. †† a-solasonine. and each set included two replicates of each Agar Type 3 Individual Frugivore combination. We tested all frugivores except opossums, which were excluded because of strong negative effects of glycoalkaloids and the lack of influence of nutrient content on their response toward toxic (1) agars (see Results). When Agar Type was a significant main effect, we made the following planned comparisons: (1) vs. (2) agars within each species for each ripeness state; ripe (1) agars between species; and ripe (2) vs. unripe (2) agars. The first comparison examined whether or not glycoalkaloids influence consumption, the second comparison validated the Real Fruit trials, and the third examined the effect of ripening, independent of glycoalkaloids, on consumption. Interaction trials.—These trials were designed to determine whether the two alkaloids (SA and SM) differed in their activity toward the frugivores and to determine whether there was an interaction (synergism) in their effects at levels typical of ripe S. carolinense fruits. Such synergisms are important to explore, since both SA and SM are commonly present in Solanum fruits and synergism between these glycoalkaloids is known to occur (e.g., Roddick et al. 1990). We formulated a nontoxic (2) agar that approximated average levels of carbohydrate, lipid, protein, water, pH, and fiber (Table 1). Toxic (1) agars had SA and SM added at overall levels of 2% dry mass, a concentration typical of S. carolinense and other toxic ripe Solanum species (M. Cipollini, D. Levey, and R. Bushway, unpublished data). The SA:SM ratio was varied among the three (1) agars: (1) SA only; (2) SA 1 SM, 1:1; and (3) SM only (Table 1). We conducted Interaction trials for all four animal species and analyzed the data as described for Fruit Mimic trials. Planned comparisons were: ‘‘SA only’’ and ‘‘SM only’’ vs. ‘‘SA 1 SM,’’ and ‘‘SA only,’’ ‘‘SM only,’’ and ‘‘SA 1 SM’’ vs. ‘‘Control.’’ The first contrast tests for interaction: whether or not the average consumption of SA and SM agars alone differs from consumption of the agar containing both SA and SM. The second contrast examines the average influence of glycoalkaloids on consumption. Nutrient–Toxin Titration trials.—These trials more specifically addressed the Nutrient–Toxin Trade-off vs. the Removal Rate Hypotheses by focusing on frugivore response to variation in nutrient levels while alkaloid levels were held constant. We formulated a nontoxic (2) agar identical to that used in the Interaction trials, and three toxic (1) agars containing 2% dry mass glycoalkaloid (1:1, SA:SM; Table 1). We varied the overall MARTIN L. CIPOLLINI AND DOUGLAS J. LEVEY 788 Ecology, Vol. 78, No. 3 RESULTS Seed Passage trials Based upon the proportion of seeds they passed intact, we classified American Robins and opossums as seed dispersers, and Northern Bobwhites and deer mice as seed predators. The latter two species destroyed 85– 95% of seeds they consumed, whereas the former destroyed few or none (Fig. 1). This classification is not meant to imply that Northern Bobwhites and deer mice are strictly seed predators and ineffective dispersers of the viable seeds they defecate, only that their treatment of seeds is generally less beneficial to Solanum plants than treatment by American Robins and opossums. Factorial ANOVA results (Table 2) support this dichotomy, with a significant Animal Species effect, but nonsignificant Fruit Species and interaction effects. Real Fruit trials FIG. 1. Results of Seed Passage trials. Data are means 6 1 SE for the proportion of seeds ingested that were passed in viable condition (N 5 3 individuals of each animal species). Identical letters above vertical bars indicate animal species that did not differ in their seed passage rate for a particular fruit species, based upon one-way ANOVA (P # 0.05). nutrient concentration in the three (1) agars as follows: Low (1), 70% reduction in overall nutrient content; Average (1), nutrient concentration equal to the control; and High (1), a 70% increase in overall nutrient content (Table 1). Different amounts of fiber compensated for varying levels of nutrients. Final nutrient concentrations fell within the range reported for temperate fleshy fruits (Stiles 1980, Stiles and White 1982, White 1989). We conducted Nutrient–Toxin Titration trials for all four animal species and analyzed the data as described for Fruit Mimic trials. Planned comparisons were: Low (1) vs. Average (1) and High (1) vs. Average (1), which tested whether nutrient levels below or above average influence consumption when glycoalkaloid content is held constant. Consumption rates of S. carolinense were not significantly elevated in sets run with open fruits, and this treatment effect was thus eliminated from the ANOVA models. Northern Bobwhites, deer mice, and opossums strongly preferred S. americanum over S. carolinense fruits; American Robins showed a similar, but marginally significant (P 5 0.085), preference (Fig. 2). Consumption rates were 4–11 times greater for S. americanum than for S. carolinense. Consumption rates varied significantly among trials for all species (Table 3), but not in a consistent manner for any species. The Trial 3 Fruit Species interaction was not significant for any species. For all species except American Robin, these results demonstrate that fruit species have a strong and consistent influence on consumption. Artificial fruit pulp trials Fruit Mimic trials.—Agar type strongly influenced consumption rate for all three species (P , 0.001; Table 4). Trial and Agar Type 3 Trial terms were not significant for Northern Bobwhites and deer mice, but were significant for American Robins. Robins were much more variable than the other species in their feeding pattern. Individuals varied in their consumption rates, and consumption rates varied among sets. How- TABLE 2. Results of factorial ANOVA for Seed Passage trials. The dependent variable was the arcsine-transformed proportion of seeds passed intact in feces. df 3 11.184 Fruit Species (S. carolinense vs. S. americanum) 1 0.008 0.31 Animal 3 Fruit Species 3 0.148 1.94 25 0.634 Error **** P # 0.0001. Type III F Source Animal species (American Robin vs. opossum vs. Northern Bobwhite vs. deer mouse) SS 147.02**** EFFECTS OF GLYCOALKALOIDS IN SOLANUM FRUITS April 1997 FIG. 2. Results of Real Fruit trials. Data are means 6 1 for the relative amount of fruit mass consumed per trial (i.e., mass of a particular fruit consumed divided by the total mass of all fruits consumed; N 5 8 individuals of each animal species). Matching lowercase letters indicate nonsignificant differences between plant species (P . 0.05; planned contrasts). Where the same letter appears in upper and lowercase, the difference is significant (P , 0.05). SE ever, an examination of the temporal sequence of preference patterns for robins revealed no evidence that choices expressed in early trials were consistently different from those expressed in later trials. Planned contrasts of means demonstrated that Northern Bobwhites and deer mice generally consumed significantly lower amounts of agars containing toxins (Fig. 3). For Northern Bobwhites, three of four comparisons between agars identical except for the presence or absence of glycoalkaloids were significant at P , 0.0001, and the remaining comparison at P 5 0.08 (Fig. 3). Deer mice showed the same pattern. Both species failed to distinguish between agars that mimicked ripe S. americanum with and without glycoal- 789 kaloids. Robins did not display any significant preference for agars without glycoalkaloids ( P 5 0.55 6 0.35, mean 6 1 SE, N 5 4 comparisons); they appear less sensitive to them. For each fruit species and ripeness state, however, robins consistently consumed (1) agars at lower levels than (2) agars (Fig. 3). All three species significantly preferred agars mimicking ripe S. americanum fruits over those mimicking ripe S. carolinense (ripe ‘‘1’’ agars: all P , 0.002, planned comparisons; Fig. 3). These results confirm those of the Real Fruit trials, in which S. americanum was strongly preferred by Northern Bobwhites and deer mice, and slightly preferred by American Robins. Planned comparisons of ripe S. americanum and S. carolinense agars lacking glycoalkaloids (‘‘ 2’’ agars) were insignificant for Northern Bobwhites and deer mice (P 5 0.09 and 0.48, respectively) and significant for American Robins (P 5 0.0.16; Fig. 3). These results suggest that the higher concentrations of glycoalkaloids in ripe S. carolinense fruits were more important in determining preference for S. americanum than were differences in nutrient content, since two of the three animal species were able to distinguish between the two fruit species only when glycoalkaloids were present. Interaction trials.—The main effect of Agar Type was highly significant for all four animal species (Table 5). In every case, this was due to significantly higher consumption of the nontoxic control agar, relative to the three toxic agars (all P , 0.001, planned comparisons; Fig. 4). Measured consumption (in grams) was 4–14 times greater for control agars than for the other agars. Consistent with results of the Fruit Mimic trials, American Robins displayed the lowest difference between consumption of these agar types, indicating that they were least sensitive to the presence of glycoalkaloids. Opossums were the most sensitive. Set and Set 3 Agar Type terms were significant for opossums and Northern Bobwhites. For Northern Bobwhites, consumption of control agars consistently increased from one set to the next. For opossums, consumption of control agars varied widely among sets, with no discernible trend. In both cases, the significant interaction term was due to the large variation in con- TABLE 3. Results of repeated-measures ANOVA for Real Fruit feeding trials. The dependent variable was the ln-transformed fruit mass eaten per trial. Results are presented separately for each species. Data are F values based upon Type III sums of squares. American Robin Source F Fruit species (S. carolinense vs. S. americanum) Set (repeated) Fruit Species 3 Set Opossum df F 4.0 1 1.7* 15 0.9 15 Northern Bobwhite df F 105.6**** 1 3.5*** 11 1.2 11 * P # 0.05; ** P # 0.01; *** P # 0.001; **** P # 0.0001. Deer mouse df F 96.8**** 1 29.3*** 1 3.0*** 15 2.4** 14 1.4 15 0.8 14 df MARTIN L. CIPOLLINI AND DOUGLAS J. LEVEY 790 Ecology, Vol. 78, No. 3 TABLE 4. Results of repeated-measures ANOVA for Fruit Mimic feeding trials using agars with (1) or without (2) alkaloid. The dependent variable was the ln-transformed agar mass eaten per trial. Results are presented separately for each species. Data are F values based upon Type III sums of squares. American Robin Northern Bobwhite Deer mouse F df F df F Agar Type† 4.3*** 7 27.5**** 7 13.4**** Set (repeated) 3.1** 9 0.8 5 1.2 5 Agar Type 3 Set 2.0**** 63 0.9 35 0.7 35 Source df 7 * P # 0.05; ** P # 0.01; *** P # 0.001; **** P # 0.0001. † S. carolinense ripe (1) vs. S. carolinense ripe (2) vs. S. carolinense unripe (1) vs. S. carolinense unripe (2) vs. S. americanum ripe (1) vs. S. americanum ripe (2) vs. S. americanum unripe (1) vs. S. americanum unripe (2). sumption of control agars relative to consistently low and relatively equal consumption of the other agar types. Although they differed in magnitude of response, both species that we classified as seed dispersers (robins and opossums) were deterred by glycoalkaloid at levels found in ripe fruits. We found no evidence that seed predators (Northern Bobwhites and deer mice) were targeted specifically by these compounds. We also found no evidence of synergism between the two glycoalkaloids in their deterrent effects. In particular, consumption of the SA 1 SM agar did not differ from the average consumption of the SA-only and SM-only agars (all P . 0.5, planned comparisons; Fig. 4). Nutrient–Toxin Titration trials.—If nutrient concentration can compensate for the deterrence produced by glycoalkaloid concentrations typical of ripe S. carolinense fruits, we would expect to find strong differences in consumption of toxic (1) agars that differ dramatically in overall nutrient content. Although Agar Type was by far the most important determinant of agar consumption for all species (all P , 0.0001; Table 6) in Nutrient–Toxin Titration trials, most variation in consumption among agar types was due to strong preferences for control (2) agars over (1) agars containing glycoalkaloids and varying amounts of nutrients (all P , 0.0001, planned comparisons; Fig. 5). As in the Interaction trials, opossums were most sensitive to presence of glycoalkaloids; they consumed ø27 times more control agar than the most preferred toxic agar. Deer mice were least sensitive (consuming two times more control agar), followed by American Robins (2.6 times more control agar) and Northern Bobwhites (3.7 times). Average consumption rate of (1) agars containing glycoalkaloids was ranked identically by all species: High nutrient content . Average nutrient content . Low nutrient content. However, with one exception (High (1) vs. Average (1) for deer mice; P , 0.05), differences in consumption were not significant in pairwise tests (High vs. Average and Low vs. Average; planned comparisons.) Thus, only deer mice responded significantly to vari- ation in nutrient composition of toxic agars, and not enough to fully compensate for the negative effect of glycoalkaloid (Fig. 5). For all species, we found no significant differences between Low (1) and Average (1) agars. Feeding rates on High (1) agars were about twice the feeding rates on Low (1) agars. Nevertheless, feeding rates for all species on High (1) agars were still ,50% of those on nontoxic (2) control agars. Given the large magnitude of nutrient variation in the toxic agars tested, ranging from a 70% reduction in nutrient content in Low (1) to a 70% increase in High (1), these results further support the conclusion that glycoalkaloid content is a much more important determinant of agar consumption than is nutrient content. Set effects were significant for opossums, deer mice, and Northern Bobwhites (Table 6). As in the Interaction trials, Northern Bobwhites increased their consumption of control agars during the course of the sets. Opossums and deer mice showed high but inconsistent differences among sets in consumption of control agars. Consumption of other agars did not vary in a consistent manner from one set to the next, except for Northern Bobwhites, which greatly increased their consumption of the high-nutrient agar in two of the last three sets. Largely as a result, Northern Bobwhites had a significant Agar Type 3 Set interaction term (P , 0.0001; Table 6). DISCUSSION Overview of results All four animal species, both seed predators and seed dispersers, were deterred by glycoalkaloid levels and combinations typical of ripe Solanum fruits. Although American Robins seemed to be less deterred by glycoalkaloids than were the other species in some trials (e.g., Real Fruit and Fruit Mimic trials), results of other trials (e.g., Interaction and Nutrient–Toxin Titration trials) showed strong negative effects of glycoalkaloids upon robins. These results lead us to reject the Directed Toxicity Hypothesis. Deterrent effects of glycoalkaloids were so strong that they generally overrode the April 1997 EFFECTS OF GLYCOALKALOIDS IN SOLANUM FRUITS 791 effects of nutrient content in preference trials; even highly rewarding artificial pulp was usually rejected if glycoalkaloids were present. Thus, we also reject the Nutrient–Toxin Titration Hypothesis. Our general conclusion is that glycoalkaloids in ripe Solanum fruits are best explained by the General Toxicity and Removal Rate Hypotheses. Real fruit and artificial fruit preferences The frugivores we tested generally consumed Solanum americanum fruits at rates 4–10 times higher than they consumed S. carolinense fruits. More important, an animal we classified as a potential seed disperser (opossum) was as strongly deterred from feeding on the high-alkaloid S. carolinense as were those animals we classified as seed predators (Northern Bobwhite, deer mouse). Although American Robins were not significantly deterred from feeding on S. carolinense, they fed at a rate about four times lower for S. carolinense relative to S. americanum in these trials. It seems that our experimental design was not sufficient to pick up this preference by robins for S. americanum. When the data were analyzed using an alternative factorial model that included effects of individual frugivores, the preference of American Robins for S. americanum was significant (results not shown). These results argue against the Directed Toxicity Hypothesis, although as discussed previously, aspects of fruit physical and chemical design other than fruit toxicity may have influenced the fruit preferences of these organisms. Thus, we used feeding trials with artificial fruits to isolate nutrient and toxin effects. In general, results of Fruit Mimic trials supported the hypothesis that both Northern Bobwhites and deer mice were affected negatively by glycoalkaloids. Effects of nutrient level on consumption were generally small, except for American Robins, which responded negatively to low-nutrient agars mimicking S. carolinense fruit pulp. Alkaloids in S. carolinense toxic agars were nearly 100% SA, whereas S. americanum toxic agars contained ø50% each of SA and SM. Unripe S. americanum toxic (1) agars (at 0.08% of wet mass) had an effect similar to that of S. carolinense toxic (1) agars (ø0.26% of wet mass), possibly the result of chemical synergism. Such synergism has been demonstrated for these two alkaloids in their effects upon lipid membrane disruption (Roddick et al. 1990), albeit at concentrations nearly five times higher than those employed in our tests. But, because the results of our ← FIG. 3. Results of Fruit Mimic trials. Data are means 6 1 SE for the ln-transformed mass of agar consumed per trial (N 5 8 individuals per animal species). Letters indicate planned comparisons that apply within species only (see Fig. 2). For any particular fruit species–fruit state combination, (2) indicates no added alkaloid, whereas (1) indicates addition of alkaloid. For agar compositions, refer to Table 1. MARTIN L. CIPOLLINI AND DOUGLAS J. LEVEY 792 Ecology, Vol. 78, No. 3 TABLE 5. Results of repeated-measures ANOVA for Interaction feeding trials. The dependent variable was the ln-transformed agar mass eaten per trial. Results are presented separately for each species. Data are F values based upon Type III sums of squares. American Robin Source Agar type† Northern Bobwhite Opossum Deer mouse F df F df F df F df 25.2**** 3 93.8**** 3 16.4**** 3 43.5**** 3 Set (repeated) 1.1 7 8.9**** 5 4.2** 5 1.1 5 Agar Type 3 Set 1.4 21 4.9**** 15 2.5** 15 0.9 15 * P # 0.05; ** P # 0.01; *** P # 0.001; **** P # 0.0001. † ‘‘SA only’’ vs. ‘‘SA 1 SM’’ vs. ‘‘SM only’’ vs. ‘‘Control’’. Interaction trials provided no support for synergism with respect to the deterrence by these compounds, we do not think that synergism explains the similarity in the consumption rate of toxic (1) unripe S. americanum and S. carolinense agars. For all species, agars containing 1:1 mixtures of SA and SM were consumed at rates similar to those of agars containing only one of the two compounds. Although sample sizes were too FIG. 4. Results of Interaction trials. Data are means 6 1 SE for the relative amount of artificial fruit agar consumed per trial (i.e., mass of a particular agar consumed divided by the total mass of all agars consumed; N 5 8 individuals of each animal species). Letters indicate planned comparisons as in Figs. 2 and 3, except that underlined letters define members of a group whose mean response was compared to the matching letter not underlined. For agar compositions, refer to Table 1. EFFECTS OF GLYCOALKALOIDS IN SOLANUM FRUITS April 1997 793 TABLE 6. Results of repeated-measures ANOVA for Nutrient–Toxin Titration feeding trials. The dependent variable was the ln-transformed agar mass eaten per trial. Results are presented separately for each species. Data are F values based upon Type III sums of squares. American Robin Source Agar Type† F 36.6**** Set (repeated) 0.7 Agar Type 3 Set 2.0** Opossum Northern Bobwhite df F df F 3 80.3**** df Deer mouse F df 3 34.8**** 3 35.8**** 3 7 2.9* 5 7.3**** 7 6.9*** 5 21 1.5 15 3.1**** 21 1.5 15 * P # 0.05; ** P # 0.01; *** P # 0.001; **** P # 0.0001. † ‘‘Low Nutrient (1)’’ vs. ‘‘Average Nutrient (1)’’ vs. ‘‘High Nutrient (1)’’ vs. ‘‘Control’’. small for statistical tests, there were no obvious differences between the responses of naive and experienced animals in successive sets of feeding trials. These observations suggested that the preferences exhibited by these animals were either innate or learned rapidly upon initial presentation of choices. Because results of our repeated feeding trials were consistent, we have assumed that learning was not an important component of the choices made by our frugivores. Nevertheless, we recognize that feeding deterrence might be learned in animals as a result of a single toxic (nonfatal) dose of a chemical (e.g., Robbins 1978). If this were true for our feeding trials, our conclusions would remain basically unaltered. Whether innate or rapidly learned, aversions to these toxins greatly affected choices made by these frugivores. We believe that glycoalkaloids in our artificial fruit pulp closely mimicked the effects of naturally occur- FIG. 5. Results of Nutrient–Toxin Titration trials. Data are means 6 1 SE for the relative amount of artificial fruit agar consumed per trial (refer to Fig. 4). Letters indicate planned comparisons as in Fig. 4. For agar compositions, refer to Table 1. 794 MARTIN L. CIPOLLINI AND DOUGLAS J. LEVEY ring glycoalkaloids in fruit, and that these glycoalkaloids are a primary determinant of fruit acceptability to frugivores. In support, we note that these animals preferred S. americanum over S. carolinense fruits by approximately the same ratio that they preferred agars mimicking S. americanum fruits over those mimicking S. carolinense fruits. Fruits of native or naturalized Solanum species similar to S. americanum (e.g., S. luteum Mill., S. nigrum L., S. sarrachoides Sendt. in Mart., and S. ptychanthum Dun. ex DC.) are similar in alkaloid profile to fruits of S. americanum (Carle 1981, Tamboia et al. 1996). These fruits are eaten by captive birds and mammals with as much relish as are fruits of S. americanum (Barnea et al. 1990, Tamboia et al. 1996; M. Cipollini, unpublished data). On the other hand, several exotic species of large, yellow-fruited Solanum are similar to S. carolinense in that they are high in glycoalkaloids in the ripe state (e.g., S. viarum Dun., S. aculeatissimum Jacq., and S. mammosum L.). Although large domestic animals such as cattle seem to play some role in the dispersal of these introduced weeds (e.g., Mullahey and Colvin 1993), wild animals tend to avoid these fruits, which persist for long periods of time on plants without being removed (M. Cipollini, personal observations). As with S. carolinense, such ‘‘toxic’’ fruits tend to be consumed and dispersed in Florida primarily by mammals such as whitetail deer (Odocoileus virginianus), raccoons (Procyon lotor), Virginia opossums and feral hogs (Sus scrofa) (White 1989; Mullahey and Colvin 1993; M. Cipollini, personal observations). Taken together, these observations suggest that the results of our comparison of S. americanum and S. carolinense are probably generalizable to a broader array of Solanum species than we were able to compare experimentally. Although robins, which are potential dispersers of Solanum fruits, seemed less affected by glycoalkaloids, oppossums, our other potential dispersers, were most affected. Additionally, we have tested Cedar Waxwings (Bombycilla cedrorum), also potential dispersal agents, and found them to be equally deterred as were the animals tested in this study (D. J. Levey and M. L. Cipollini, unpublished data). Evidence that both seed predators and seed dispersers were affected negatively by toxins in Fruit Mimic trials provides direct support for the General Toxicity Hypothesis. These results were further supported by strong negative effects of glycoalkaloids on feeding rates of all species in both Interaction and Nutrient–Toxin Titration trials, regardless of the identity of the alkaloids. The generally small effects of nutrient composition seen in both Fruit Mimic trials and Nutrient–Toxin Titration trials suggest that, although nutrient composition may play a role in influencing consumption, the effects of glycoalkaloids are overriding. We found little evidence that increased nutrient content can compensate for toxin content at the levels found in toxic Solanum fruits such as S. carolinense. Thus, our results Ecology, Vol. 78, No. 3 are not consistent with the Nutrient–Toxin Titration Hypothesis, but are consistent with the Removal Rate Hypothesis. That is, fruits high in nutrient content, otherwise profitable (e.g., low fiber content), or quickly removed by dispersers should contain relatively low amounts of toxins. S. americanum fruits were generally preferred over those of S. carolinense in Real Fruit trials, and ripe S. americanum fruit-mimicking agars were preferred by one of the frugivores tested (American Robins). Our field observations over four years indicate very low and sporadic dispersal rates for S. carolinense (most fruits generally fall to the ground and rot), but relatively consistent removal rates for S. americanum. As predicted, S. americanum fruits have negligible levels of glycoalkaloid in ripe fruits (generally ,0.2% of dry mass), whereas S. carolinense fruits have high levels (1–3%) in ripe fruit. Selection for persistence in temperate-zone fruits Different fruiting phenologies and consequent requirements for persistence may ultimately explain differences in alkaloid content of S. americanum and S. carolinense. In temperate North America, S. americanum and other members of the S. nigrum complex generally ripen fruits in the summer and early fall, when dispersal rates by resident and migrant frugivorous birds are high (Thompson and Willson 1979). Conversely, S. carolinense fruits ripen in late fall and may persist throughout the winter into early spring (M. Cipollini, personal observation). Dispersal by frugivorous birds and mammals may be very unpredictable and sporadic for fruits at this time (White and Stiles 1990). Thus, differences in the removal rate of these species in the field may reflect extrinsic differences in the temporal dynamics of the frugivore community. Like several other North American plants (e.g., Ilex opaca Ait., Vaccinium macrocarpon Ait., Rhus typhina L., and Juniperus virginiana L.; White 1989), S. carolinense fruits may have to persist for long periods of time, ‘‘waiting’’ for visitation by dispersal agents. During this time, they presumably require protection against damaging agents such as fungi and insect pests. As previously discussed, glycoalkaloids are known to be toxic and to act as feeding deterrents for generalist invertebrate herbivores, and they are known to have strong antifungal effects (McKee 1959, Allen and Kuc 1968, Allen 1970, Roddick 1987, Cipollini and Levey 1997). Thus, retaining such general toxins may be selectively advantageous by preserving fruits during periods of unpredictable and/or sporadic dispersal. A similar pattern has been found for at least two sets of species: (1) temperate ericaceous plants (Cipollini and Stiles 1992, 1993), and (2) temperate Ilex species (Gargiullo and Stiles 1991). In both cases, relative to species that ripen in late fall and winter, summer or early-fall species were removed at higher rates by seed dispersers, tended to be preferred by frugivorous birds, and contained lower amounts of (or less active) sec- April 1997 EFFECTS OF GLYCOALKALOIDS IN SOLANUM FRUITS ondary metabolites. Feeding trials using artificial fruit pulp agars mimicking ericaceous species demonstrated that preferences for summer species were based on both nutrient and secondary metabolite content (Cipollini and Stiles 1993). Alternative hypotheses Although our experiments suggest that secondary metabolites in ripe fruits represent a trade-off between defense against pests/pathogens and palatability for dispersal agents, other explanations are not precluded. For instance, Murray et al. (1994) suggest that chemicals within the fruits of another solanaceous plant (Witheringia solanceae Remy) have a laxative effect and enhance seed passage rates in Black-faced Solitaires (Myadestes melanops) feeding on these fruits. Rapid seed passage is correlated with lower seed damage during gut passage. We note that one of the first symptoms of Solanum glycoalkaloid toxicity is irritation of the gut lining and diarrhea (Van Gelder 1990). Also, Sorensen (1983), Barnea et al. (1993), and Izhaki and Safriel (1989) suggest that fruit pulp chemicals may be present within ripe fruits as a means of inducing frugivores to leave fruiting plants early in a feeding bout, enhancing dispersal of seeds away from the parent plant. In S. carolinense, reproduction within a given habitat is strongly dominated by clonal growth via rhizomes (Nichols et al. 1991). The occurrence of these plants only in highly disturbed areas such as farm fields and riverbeds suggests that dispersal into and out of these ephemeral habitats may be extremely important. Thus, glycoalkaloids of ripe fruits may play a role in inducing consumers to seek alternative food sources after consuming one or a few fruits. Nonadaptive explanations for the presence of glycoalkaloids and other secondary metabolites in ripe fruits should also be considered. Immature fruits commonly accumulate secondary metabolites, many of which decrease substantially with ripening (Goldstein and Swain 1963, Dement and Mooney 1974, Eltayeb and Roddick 1984a, b). Plants often recover these chemicals from senescent tissues, but may vary in their ability to do so. Interspecific variation in the secondary metabolites of ripe fruits might, thus, be a nonadaptive result of differing patterns of plant senescence. We note that, for our study species, this explanation is unlikely. Fruit production is indeterminate, with continuous asynchronous ripening, in the ‘‘nontoxic’’ S. americanum, whereas it is determinate, with synchronous, postsenescent ripening in the ‘‘toxic’’ S. carolinense. In conclusion, we have demonstrated that secondary metabolites of ripe fleshy fruits may strongly affect fruit choice by vertebrates. The magnitude of their effects, overriding large differences in nutrient content, may help to explain why previous studies on the influence of fruit characters on fruit choice have yielded inconsistent or inconclusive results (e.g., Sorensen 1983, Johnson et al. 1985). If fruit secondary metab- 795 olites are not considered in such studies, fruit preferences based on nutrient content may be difficult to detect. More generally, we hope that our results spur further research into the implications of secondary metabolites of fleshy fruits. Our results suggest that studies of interactions between fruiting plants and seed dispersers need to include the influences of other frugivores, including seed predators, fruit pests, and pathogens (cf. Herrera 1984, 1989, Borowicz 1988, Buchholz and Levey 1990, Cipollini and Stiles 1993). We suggest that if such frugivores did not impinge upon the fruit– seed disperser mutualism, patterns of fruit ripening and secondary metabolite content would be vastly different, as would interactions among fruiting plants and their seed dispersers. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We thank R. Bushway for HPLC analysis of glycoalkaloids, L. Baggett, I. Cipollini, M. E. Cipollini, S. Cipollini, W. Deckard, P. Rey Zamora, W. Silva, T. Tamboia, and G. Williams for field and lab assistance, C. Restrepo for statistical advice, C. Augspurger, C. Herrera, W. Karasov, and T. Stiles for helpful comments and criticism of the hypotheses and methods of this research, D. Schultz and S. Brooker for logistical assistance at Austin Cary Memorial Forest, M. Weissenberg for glycoalkaloid standards and isolation methods, C. Sacchi, D. Whigham, J. O’Niell, M. Gargiullo, and R. Frank for fruit samples, Mr. and Mrs. Garrett for permission to mist-net birds on their property, J. Davis for assistance with animal care facilities, D. Dewsbury for providing field mice, M. Sunquist for use of live traps, L. Brower and B. McNab for use of animal cages, and M. Debussche and an anonymous reviewer for helpful comments and criticism. This work was supported by NSF grant DEB-9207920 and an NSF Research Experiences for Undergraduates Supplemental Grant. LITERATURE CITED Abacus Concepts. 1989. SuperANOVA: accessible general linear modeling. Abacus Concepts, Berkeley, California, USA. Alauddin, M., and M. Martin-Smith. 1962. Biological activity in steroids containing nitrogen atoms. II. Steroid alkaloids. Journal of Pharmacy and Pharmacology 14:469– 495. Allen, E. H. 1970. The nature of antifungal substances in the peel of Irish potato tubers, Phytopathologie Zeitschrift 69:151–159. Allen, E. H., and J. Kuc. 1968. a-Solanine and a-Chaconine as fungitoxic compounds in extracts of Irish potato tubers. Phytopathology 59:776–781. Aubert, S., M. C. Daunay, and E. Pochard. 1989a. Saponosides steroidiques de l’aubergine (Solanum melongena L.). I. Interet alimentaire, methodologie d’analyse, localisation dans le fruit. Agronomie 9:641–651. Aubert, S., M. C. Daunay, and E. Pochard. 1989b. Saponosides steroidiques de l’aubergine (Solanum melongena L.). II. Variations des teneurs liees aux conditions de recolte, aux genotypes et a la quantite de graines des fruits. Agronomie 9:751–758. Baker, D. C., R. F. Keeler, and W. Gaffield. 1989. Pathology in hamsters administered Solanum plant species that contain steroidal alkaloids. Toxicon 27:1331–1337. Ballington, J. R., W. B. Kirkman, W. E. Ballinger, and E. P. Maness. 1988. Anthocyanins, aglycone, and aglyconesugar content in the fruits of temperate North American species of four Sections in Vaccinium. Journal of the American Society of Horticultural Science 113:746–749. 796 MARTIN L. CIPOLLINI AND DOUGLAS J. LEVEY Barnea, A., J. Harborne, and C. Pannell. 1993. What parts of fleshy fruits contain secondary compounds toxic to birds and why? Biochemical Systematics and Ecology 21:421– 429. Barnea, A., Y. Yom-Tov, and J. Friedman. 1990. Differential germination of two closely related species of Solanum in response to bird ingestion. Oikos 57:222–228. Borowicz, V. A. 1988. Do vertebrates reject decaying fruit? An experimental test with Cornus amomum fruit. Oikos 53: 74–78. Bremer, B., and O. Eriksson. 1992. Evolution of fruit characters and dispersal modes in the tropical family Rubiaceae. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 47:79–95. Briggs, L. H., R. C. Cambie, and J. L. Hoare. 1961. Solanum alkaloids. Part XV. The constituents of some Solanum species and a reassessment of solasodamine and solauricine. Pages 4645–4649 in R. Manske, editor. The alkaloids. Academic Press, New York, New York, USA. Buchholz, R., and D. J. Levey. 1990. The evolutionary triad of microbes, fruits, and seed dispersers: an experiment in fruit choice by Cedar Waxwings, Bombycilla cedrorum. Oikos 59:200–204. Carle, R. 1981. Investigations on the content of steroidal glycoalkaoids and sapogenins within Solanum sect. Solanum (5sect. Morella) (Solanaceae). Plant Systematics and Evolution 138:61–71. Cipollini, M. L., and D. J. Levey. 1997. Antifungal activity of Solanum fruit glycoalkaloids: implications for frugivory and seed dispersal. Ecology 78:799–809. Cipollini, M. L., and E. W. Stiles. 1992. Relative risks of microbial rot for fleshy fruits: significance with repect to dispersal and selection for secondary defense. Advances in Ecological Research 23:35–91. Cipollini, M. L., and E. W. Stiles. 1993. Fruit rot, antifungal defense, and palatability of fleshy fruits for frugivorous birds. Ecology 74:751–762. Debussche, M., J. Cortez, and I. Rimbault. 1987. Variation in fleshy fruit composition in the Mediterranean region: the importance of ripening season, life-form, fruit type, and geographical distribution. Oikos 49:244–252. Debussche, M., and P. Isenmann. 1989. Fleshy fruit characters and the choices of bird and mammal seed dispersers in a Mediterranean region. Oikos 56:327–337. Dement, W. A., and H. A. Mooney. 1974. Seasonal variation in the production of tannins and cyanogenic glucosides in the chaparral shrub, Heteromeles arbutifolia. Oecologia 15: 65–76. Denslow, J. S., T. C. Moermond, and D. J. Levey. 1987. A synthetic diet for fruit-eating birds. Wilson Bulletin 99: 135–136. Eltayeb, E. A., and J. G. Roddick. 1984a. Changes in the alkaloid content of developing fruits of tomato (Lycopersicon esculentum Mill.). I. Analysis of cultivars and mutants with differing ripening characteristics. Journal of Experimental Botany 35:252–260. Eltayeb, E. A., and J. G. Roddick. 1984b. Changes in the alkaloid content of developing fruits of tomato (Lycopersicon esculentum Mill.). II. Effects of artificial acceleration and retardation of ripening. Journal of Experimental Botany 35:261–267. Fewell, A. M., and J. G. Roddick. 1993. Interactive antifungal activity of the glycoalkaloids a-solanine and a-chaconine. Phytochemistry 33:323–328. Gargiullo, M. B., and E. W. Stiles. 1991. Chemical and nutritional differences between two bird-dispersed fruits: Ilex opaca and Ilex verticullata. Journal of Chemical Ecology 17:1091–1106. Gautier-Hion, A. 1990. Interactions among fruit and vertebrate fruit-eaters in an African tropical rain forest. Pages 219–232 in K. Bawa and M. Hadley, editors. Reproductive Ecology, Vol. 78, No. 3 ecology of tropical forest plants. Man and the biosphere series, Volume 7. Parthenon Press, Paris, France. Gautier-Hion, A., J.-M. Duplantier, R. Quris, F. Feer, C. Sourd, J.-P. Decoux, G. Dubost, L. Emmons, C. Erard, P. Hecketsweiler, A. Moungazi, C. Roussilhon, and J.-M. Thiollay. 1985. Fruit characters as a basis of fruit choice and seed dispersal in a tropical forest vertebrate community. Oecologia 65:324–337. Goldstein, J. L., and T. Swain. 1963. Changes in tannins in ripening fruits. Phytochemistry 2:371–383. Gregory, P. 1984. Glycoalkaloid composition of potatoes: diversity and biological implications. American Potato Journal 61:115–122. Hauser, E. J., and J. H. Morrison. 1964. The cytochemical reduction of nitro blue tetrazolium as an index of pollen viability. American Journal of Botany 51:748–752. Herrera, C. M. 1982. Defense of ripe fruit from pests: its significance in relation to plant–disperser interactions. American Naturalist 120:218–241. . 1984. Avian interference of insect frugivory: an exploration into the plant–bird–fruit pest evolutionary triad. Oikos 42:203–210. . 1985. Determinants of plant–animal coevolution: the case of mutualistic dispersal of seeds by vertebrates. Oikos 44:132–141. . 1986. Vertebrate-dispersed plants: why they don’t behave the way they should. Pages 5–18 in A. Estrada and T. H. Fleming, editors. Frugivores and seed dispersal. Dr. W. Junk Publishers, Dordrecht, The Netherlands. . 1987. Vertebrate-dispersed plants of the Iberian peninsula: a study of fruit characteristics. Ecological Monographs 57:305–331. . 1989. Vertebrate frugivores and their interaction with invertebrate fruit predators: supporting evidence from a Costa Rican dry forest. Oikos 54:185–188. . 1992. Interspecific variation in fruit shape: allometry, phylogeny, and adaptation to dispersal agents. Ecology 73:1832–1841. Howe, H. F. 1992. Specialized and generalized dispersal systems: Where does ‘‘The Paradigm’’ stand? Vegetatio 107: 3–14. Izhaki, I., and U. N. Safriel. 1989. Why are there so few exclusively frugivorous birds? Experiments on fruit digestibility. Oikos 54:23–32. Izhaki, I., and U. N. Safriel. 1990. Weight losses due to exclusive fruit diet: interpretation and evolutionary implications. A reply to Mack and Sedinger. Oikos 57:140–142. Janzen, D. H. 1975. Ecology of plants in the tropics. Arnold, London, UK. . 1977. Why fruits rot, seeds mold, and meat spoils. American Naturalist 111:691–713. . 1979. New horizons in the biology of plant defenses. Pages 331–350 in G. A. Rosenthal and D. H. Janzen, editors. Herbivores, their interaction with secondary plant metabolites. Academic Press, New York, New York, USA. . 1983. Physiological ecology of fruits and their seeds. Pages 625–655 in O. Lange, P. S. Nobel, C. B. Osmond, and H. Ziegler, editors. Physiological plant ecology III. Springer-Verlag, Berlin, Germany. . 1985. On ecological fitting. Oikos 45:308–310. Johnson, R. A., M. F. Willson, J. N. Thompson, and R. I. Bertin. 1985. Nutritional values of wild fruits and consumption by migrant frugivorous birds. Ecology 66:819– 827. Jordano, P. 1987. Patterns of mutualistic interactions in pollination and seed dispersal: connectance, dependence asymmetries, and coevolution. American Naturalist 129: 657–677. Keukens, E. A., T. de Vrije, C. H. Fabrie, R. A. Demel, W. M. Jongen, and B. de Kruijff. 1992. Dual specificity of April 1997 EFFECTS OF GLYCOALKALOIDS IN SOLANUM FRUITS sterol-mediated, glycoalkaloid-induced membrane disruption. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta 1110:127–136. Levey, D. J., T. C. Moermond, and J. S. Denslow. 1984. Fruit choice in neotropical birds: the effect of distance between fruits on preference patterns. Ecology 67:844–850. Mack, A. 1990. Is frugivory limited by secondary compounds in fruits? Oikos 57:135–138. Mahato, S. B., N. P. Sahu, A. N. Ganguly, R. Kasai, and O. Tanaka. 1980. Steroidal alkaloids from Solanum khasianum: application of 13C NMR spectroscopy to their structural elucidation. Phytochemistry 19:2017–2020. Mangla, M., and R. Kamal. 1989. Steroidal sapogenins and glycoalkaloids from berries of Solanum pseudocapsicum L. at different stages of growth. Indian Journal of Experimental Biology 27:370–371. Martin, A. C., H. S. Zim, and A. L. Nelson. 1951. American wildlife and plants. Dover New York, New York, USA. Martinez del Rio, C., W. H. Karasov, and D. J. Levey. 1989. Physiological basis and ecological consequences of sugar preferences in Cedar Waxwings. Auk 106:64–71. McKee, R. K. 1959. Factors affecting the toxicity of solanine and related alkaloids to Fusarium caeruleum. Journal of General Microbiology 20:686–696. Miller, L. M., and M. E. Davies. 1979. The characteristics of solasodine accumulation in Solanum khasianum C. B. Clarke var. chatterjeeanum Sengupta and S. laciniatum Ait. grown under field conditions in Birmingham. Pages 231– 235 in J. G. Hawkes, R. N. Lester, and A. D. Skelding, editors. Biology and taxonomy of the Solanaceae. Academic Press, London, UK. Moermond, T. C., and J. S. Denslow. 1983. Fruit choice in Neotropical birds: effects of fruit type and accessibility on selectivity. Journal of Animal Ecology 52:407–420. Mullahey, J. J., and D. L. Colvin. 1993. Tropical soda apple: a new noxious weed in Florida. Fact Sheet WRS-7, Florida Cooperative Extension Service, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, USA. Murray, K. G., S. Russell, C. M. Picone, K. Winnett-Murray, W. Sherwood, and M. L. Kuhlman. 1994. Fruit laxatives and seed passage rates in frugivores: consequences for plant reproductive success. Ecology 75:989–994. Nes, W. D., E. Heftmann, I. R. Hunter, and M. K. Walden. 1980. Determination of solasodine in fruits of Solanum khasianum by a combination of chromatofuge and highpressure liquid chromatography. Journal of Liquid Chromatography 3:1687–1696. Nes, W. D., G. A. Saunders, and E. Heftmann. 1983. A reassessment of the role of steroidal alkaloids in the physiology of Phytophthora. Phytochemistry 22:75–78. Nichols, R. L., J. Cardina, and T. P. Gaines. 1991. Growth, reproduction, and chemical composition of horsenettle (Solanum carolinense). Weed Technology 5:513–520. Peterson, C. H., and P. E. Renaud. 1989. Analysis of feedingpreference experiments. Oecologia 80:82–86. Rhoades, D. F. 1979. Evolution of plant chemical defense against herbivores. Pages 4–55 in G. A. Rosenthal and D. H. Janzen, editors. Herbivores, their interaction with secondary plant metabolites. Academic Press, New York, New York, USA. . 1985. Offensive–defensive interactions between herbivores and plants: their relevance in herbivore population dynamics and ecological theory. American Naturalist 125:205–236. Rhoades, D. F., and R. G. Cates. 1976. Toward a general theory of plant antiherbivore chemistry. Recent Advances in Phytochemistry 10:168–213. Ripperger, H., and K. Schreiber. 1981. Solanum steroid alkaloids. Pages 81–192 in R. H. F. Manske and R. G. A. Rodrigo, editors. The alkaloids, Volume XIX. Academic Press, New York, New York, USA. 797 Roa, R. 1992. Design and analysis of multiple-choice feeding-preference experiments. Oecologia 89:509–515. Robbins, R. J. 1978. Poison-based taste aversion learning in deer mice (Peromyscus maniculatus bairdi). Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 92:642–650. Roddick, J. G. 1986. Steroidal alkaloids of the Solanaceae. Pages 201–222 in W. G. D’Arcy, editor. Solanaceae. Biology and systematics. Columbia University Press, New York, New York, USA. . 1987. Antifungal activity of plant steroids. Pages 286–303 in G. Fuller and W. D. Nes, editors. Ecology and metabolism of plant lipids. American Chemical Society, Washington, D.C., USA. . 1989. The acetylcholinesterase-inhibitory activity of steroidal glycoalkaloids and their activities. Phytochemistry 28:2631–2634. Roddick, J. G., A. L. Rijnenberg, and M. Weissenberg. 1990. Membrane-disrupting properties of the steroidal glycoalkaloids solasonine and solamargine. Phytochemistry 29: 1513–1518. Rowan, D. D., P. E. MacDonald, and R. A. Skipp. 1983. Antifungal stress metabolites from Solanum aviculare. Phytochemistry 22:2102–2104. Schreiber, K. 1963. Isolierung von Solasodin–Glykosiden aus Pflanzen der Gattung Solanum L. Kulturpflanze 11: 451–501. Schupp, E. W. 1992. Quantity, quality, and the effectiveness of seed dispersal by animals. Vegetatio 107:15–30. Sedinger, J. S. 1990. Are secondary compounds responsible for negative apparent metabolizability of fruits by passerine birds? A comment on Izhaki and Safriel. Oikos 57:138– 140. Sidhu, G. S., and C. Oakenfull. 1986. A mechanism for the hypocholesterolaemic activity of saponins. British Journal of Nutrition 55:643–649. Sorensen, A. E. 1983. Taste aversion and frugivore preference. Oecologia 56:117–120. Stiles, E. W. 1980. Patterns of fruit presentation and seed dispersal in bird-disseminated woody plants in the eastern deciduous forest. American Naturalist 116:670–687. Stiles, E. W., and D. White. 1982. Additional information on temperate bird-disseminated fruits: response to Herrera’s comments. American Naturalist 120:823–827. Stiles, F. G., and L. Rosselli. 1992. Consumption of fruits of the Melastomataceae by birds: how diffuse is coevolution? Vegetatio 107:57–74. Tamboia, T., M. L. Cipollini, and D. J. Levey. 1996. An evaluation of vertebrate seed dispersal syndromes in four species of black nightshade (Solanum Sect. Solanum). Oecologia 107:522–532. Thompson, J. N., and M. F. Willson. 1979. Evolution of temperate fruit–bird interactions: phenological strategies. Evolution 33:973–982. Tingey, W. M. 1984. Glycoalkaloids as pest resistance factors. American Potato Journal 61:157–167. United States Department of Agriculture. 1974. Seed characteristics of 42 economically important species of Solanaceae in the United States. Technical Bulletin Number 1471, Agricultural Research Service, United States Department of Agriculture, Washington, D.C., USA. Van Gelder, W. M. 1990. Chemistry, toxicology, and occurrence of steroidal glycoalkaloids: potential contaminants of the potato (Solanum tuberosum L.). Pages 117–156 in A.-F. M. Rizk, editor. Poisonous plant contamination of edible plants. CRC Press, Boca Raton, Florida, USA. White, D. W. 1989. North American bird-dispersed fruits: ecological and adaptive significance of nutritional and structural traits. Dissertation. Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, New Brunswick, New Jersey, USA. White, D. W., and E. W. Stiles. 1990. Co-occurrences of 798 MARTIN L. CIPOLLINI AND DOUGLAS J. LEVEY foods in stomachs and feces of fruit-eating birds. Condor 92:291–293. Willson, M. F., A. K. Irvine, and N. G. Walsh. 1989. Vertebrate dispersal syndromes in some Australian and New Zealand plant communities, with geographic comparisons. Biotropica 21:133–147. Wrangham, R. W., and P. G. Waterman. 1983. Condensed Ecology, Vol. 78, No. 3 tannins in fruits eaten by chimpanzees. Biotropica 15:217– 222. Zitnak, A. 1979. Steroids and capsaicinoids of solanaceous food plants. Pages 41–91 in N. F. Childers and G. M. Russo, editors. Nightshades and health. Somerset Press, Somerville, New Jersey, USA.