Applied Animal Behaviour Science 100 (2006) 117–128 www.elsevier.com/locate/applanim How important is natural behaviour in animal farming systems?§ Marek Špinka * Ethology Group, Research Institute of Animal Production, CZ-104 01 Prague-Uhřı́něves, Czech Republic Available online 2 May 2006 Abstract It is often assumed and demanded that for a good welfare, farm animals should be given ‘‘the freedom to express their natural behaviour’’. This demand is problematic for at least two reasons. First, natural behaviour is difficult to delineate because of its variability and flexibility. Second, some behavioural patterns that are clearly natural are in fact detrimental to animal welfare. These include emergency behaviours such as flight reactions that bring the animal into a state of stress without achieving the goal for which the behaviour had evolved; and damaging behaviours such as rank-related or illness-related aggression during which animals inflict injuries or deprive their penmates of resources. Nevertheless, when these reservations are taken into account, opening possibilities for natural behaviour may be useful as guidance for improving the existing husbandry systems. Specifically, providing the farm environment with the key features towards which the behaviour was originally adapted brings three classes of benefits. First, it is often more efficient to allow animals to satisfy their own needs and achieve their goals than to address these needs and goals through technical means. Second, a large class of natural behaviours is associated with positive affective experience, and thus their performance directly enhances animal welfare. Third, the performance of natural behaviour in its richness and complexity often brings long-term benefits for the animal, such as improved proficiency in coping with social and physical challenges. Thus, while the freedom to perform the whole repertoire of natural behaviour is not per se crucial for farm animal welfare, the opportunity to perform natural behaviour may be an effective way improve their welfare in practice immediately, and a promising basis for the design of husbandry systems for the future. # 2006 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Welfare; Natural behaviour; Housing; Motivation; Emotions § This paper is part of the special issue entitled Sentience in Animals, Guest Edited by Dr. John Webster. * Tel.: +420 267 009 596; fax: +420 267 710 779. E-mail address: spinka@vuzv.cz. 0168-1591/$ – see front matter # 2006 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2006.04.006 118 M. Špinka / Applied Animal Behaviour Science 100 (2006) 117–128 1. Technical foreword—definitions In order for me to be able to build up arguments consistently, and for the reader to understand them, it is necessary to start with a working definition of three key concepts. In this paper, I will use the word(s) (animal) welfare as a label for how well an animal is doing in a farming system. By ‘‘doing well’’, I mean primarily how the animal is feeling (Duncan, 1996). However, the term welfare in my usage also includes how well the animal is functioning biologically as an organism, i.e., how well it is maintaining itself. The reason is that biological malfunctioning (ill health, injury, physiological failures to cope and pathological behaviour) ultimately almost always results in negative feelings of the animal like pain, nausea, psychologically perceived distress, tiredness, etc. On the other hand, the ‘‘wholeness’’ or ‘‘naturalness’’ of the animal’s state will not feature as a concept of welfare in this paper. Performance of natural behaviour often opens the possibility for good biological functioning and positive affective states, but this is not always the case. The main purpose of this paper is to clarify the relationship between natural behaviour and welfare, and positioning of ‘‘naturalness’’ within the concept of welfare, would just complicate the argumentation. The second term in need of clarification is the word ‘‘natural’’. By natural behaviour, I mean those behavioural elements and their sequences that are adaptive, i.e., that have evolved either during the evolution of the species or during its domestication ‘‘in order’’ to increase the fitness (see definition below) of the behaving animal. This means that behaviour which is natural in one situation or towards one object may be non-natural in other situation or towards another object. Thus, massaging the teats of a lactating sow is a natural behaviour for a suckling piglet, but directing this behaviour towards the bellies of its littermates (the behaviour called ‘‘bellynosing’’) is not natural. Natural behaviour is never fully hard-wired; it always includes an element of plasticity. Sometimes, the propensity to learn is even the core of the natural behaviour pattern. It is natural for young chicks to peck at small objects and to follow intently what the mother hen is pecking at. Through this trial and error, and the socially facilitated learning, they acquire the knowledge about the edibility of different kinds of food. Third, the term ‘‘fitness’’ is used here as evolutionary biologists understand it, i.e., as the capacity of the individual to survive and spread its genes through producing offspring or supporting relatives. Thus, fitness in this paper does not denote healthiness or ability to produce or capacity to withstand challenges, although these properties may contribute to the fitness. Fitness in this sense is neither identical with nor a part of the welfare of the animal. For example, a red deer stag A that succeeds to impregnate 10 hinds through fiercely fighting with his rivals but then is constantly harassed and dies slowly and painfully from a sustained debilitating injury has a higher fitness, but lower welfare than stag B that avoids conflicts and sires just five progenies during his 10-year long healthy life. 2. The odd freedom Somewhat similarly to Tinbergen’s Four Whys in ethology, the Five Freedoms developed by the British Farm Animal Welfare Council (Webster, 1994) have become a paradigm for organizing thoughts in animal welfare science. Among the five freedoms, four are freedoms from. It is relatively straightforward how to strive for their achievement. When an animal is well fed and watered, its nutritional needs are met, and so the ‘‘freedom from hunger, thirst and malnutrition’’ is provided. Similarly, the next three freedoms (from discomfort; from pain, injury M. Špinka / Applied Animal Behaviour Science 100 (2006) 117–128 119 and disease; from fear and distress) could be secured when adequate environment including a good resting place is provided; when painful procedures, risk of injury and occurrence of disease are kept at a minimum; when situations and conditions inducing fear or hindering effective coping are avoided. The fifth freedom is of a different kind: the freedom to express normal behaviour. This freedom is less clearly delimited and more controversial than the other four freedoms for two reasons. First, in contrast with the other four, it does not demand that specific undesirable physiological, health or psychological states are prevented; it rather requires a kind of general liberty for all kinds of behaviour of the animal. Thus, the boundaries of this freedom are loose and open to reinterpretation. Second, the yardstick for this liberty is ‘‘the normal’’ or, in some versions of this principle, ‘‘the natural’’ form of the behaviour. However, is it straightforward what behaviour is normal (or natural) and which is not? 3. The complexity of natural behaviour Natural behaviour does not exist in a form of one standard gauge. Some simple behavioural patterns are quite uniform across time and across individuals, such as ritualized signal movements (e.g., the play bow in canids; Bekoff, 1977). However, most behavioural elements are rather variable in form, duration, and intensity (even those that may seem rather uniform for the humans, e.g., barking in dogs; Yin and McCowan, 2004) and they occur at widely different rates and sequential combinations. Based on genetic predispositions, they are guided by interplay between internal motivational states of the animal and the time dynamic of the stimuli from the environment (Jensen and Toates, 1993). Nest building by sows provides a good example of this general point. Prepartum building of a nest from available vegetation or similar material is a natural behavioural pattern in prefarrowing sows. Nest building starts about 15 h before the start of parturition (Damm et al., 2003b), probably triggered by the rise in prostagladin F2a (Burne et al., 2001b). The behaviour can be roughly divided into two phases, namely the initial rooting phase, serving for the excavation of a hollow, and the second arranging phase, during which nesting material is gathered and arranged around the hollow. The duration and intensity of the nest building varies widely according to internal factors (e.g., sow experience; Thodberg et al., 2002) and external stimuli like availability of building material (Damm et al., 2000), space (Damm et al., 2002) and environmental temperature (Burne et al., 2001a). Jensen (1993) suggested that the termination of the second arranging phase may be under control of external stimuli. It is possible that if the sow perceives the nest as already adequate, taking into account the current environmental conditions, the location of the nest and available material (Arey, 1997), it will stop the nest building. Indeed, Jensen (1989) and Mayer et al. (2002) found that the nests of free-ranging domestic and wild boar sows were less elaborate when located in cover from trees or canopy that provided a better thermal environment and possible better security. Damm et al. (2000) demonstrated that a higher quality of the nest (due to inclusion of tree branches in it) induced an earlier termination of nest building activity, and Kurz and Marchington (1972) reported that in a hot climate, the nest may consist only of a bedded hollow. If what motivates sows to perform nest-building is the mismatch between the goal of a comfortable secure nest and the current state of the real nest, one would expect that the provision of a readymade nest in the form of soft, insulated and slightly hollow lying surface in a semi-hidden place would substantially reduce at least the second phase of the nest building in indoor housing systems (Baxter, 1983). On the other hand, if part of the motivation is a direct drive to execute nest building behaviour independently of the state 120 M. Špinka / Applied Animal Behaviour Science 100 (2006) 117–128 of the environment (as it seems to be the case in nest building in laying hens; Hughes et al., 1989), then the pre-parturient sow’s motivation will not be entirely quenched by the provision of a prefabricated comfortable and secure bed. Both these models focus on the duration of nest building and what stops it (Hughes and Duncan, 1988). Newer studies indicate that what matters is not only the quantity but also the quality of nest building. Damm et al. (2003a) showed that nest building was less varied, more fragmented by postural changes and accompanied by higher heart rate and more frequent oral/nasal stereotypies in crate-housed sows than in sows in enriched pens. These results indicate that nest building may be internally driven yet its form may be sensitive to the feedback from the environment. Given this complexity in the motivation of nest building, the current knowledge is insufficient to decide unequivocally whether nest building ‘‘in its natural form’’ is or is not allowed for in different husbandry systems. The complexity of the natural behavioural patterns could be illustrated on yet another class of examples, namely calves’ behavioural reactions to their separation from the mother. Beef calves, when suddenly separated from their mothers at the age of 2–6 months, produce a lot of vocalizations and show other signs of distress. However, when calves of the same age are first prevented from suckling while still in contact with the dam (e.g., through nose plates that do not allow to grasp the teat or through a net covering the udder), and physically separated a few days later, almost no vocalizations or other behavioural signs of distress occur (Haley et al., 2005). Since this two-stage method mimics the key feature of the natural weaning process, namely the presence of a stage when the calf still wants to suck milk but is prevented to do so by the mother, it is probable that both the strong and the very weak reaction of the calf to the separation from the mother are natural behaviours: if the mother disappears suddenly and the calf has been sucking until now, a strong reaction is appropriate since the mother is a very valuable source of milk. If weaning has been already fully accomplished, the mother is no longer provides the milk, the benefit from her vicinity for the calf drops to a fraction of the pre-weaning level, and it is thus adaptive not to invest much into attempts to reverse her loss. This example shows that the natural behaviour depends not only on the current internal motivation and the immediate state of the environment, but may also be quite different depending on how the behavioural interaction of the animal with the environment develops over time. A similar situation occurs in very early separation of dairy calves from their mothers. If the separation is accomplished during the first day, the behavioural (and physiological) reactions of the calf are much weaker than after a separation at 4, 7 or 14 days (Lidfors, 1996; Flower and Weary, 2001; Stěhulová et al., 2004). The weak reaction to mutual separation early after birth is once more a natural behaviour, since cattle are a ‘hiding’ species so it is natural for the newborn calf to see for only a couple of hours per day. It may also be that newborn calves assess whether mother is taking care of them by their satiation level, whereas older calves actually monitor the presence of the mother in their vicinity. Even the reaction of older calves to separation from mother is much dependent on the circumstances. When other lactating cows are available, the orphaned calf quickly redirects its efforts from attempts to reunite with the mother into endeavours to get milk from another source. If it is successful (e.g., through ‘‘stealing’’ milk between the hind legs of the stepmother), its behaviour in terms of activity, vocalizations and playing is unaffected by the removal or returns back to preseparation levels within 1 or 2 days (Špinka and Illmann, 1991, 1992). Thomas et al. (2001) showed that just feeding the calf with enough milk more frequently attenuates the reaction to a fraction of its usual level, thus proving that the post-separation reaction is mainly, although not exclusively, about the loss of regular milk supply. Overall, these results demonstrate that it is natural for a calf to suckle milk up to the age for at least 7 months (Reinhardt and Reinhardt, 1981; Reinhardt et al., 1986; Veissier et al., 1990); it is natural for it to strive for reunion with the M. Špinka / Applied Animal Behaviour Science 100 (2006) 117–128 121 mother when she disappears suddenly; but it is also natural for it not to seek the mother strongly when she disappears after it has been already weaned or when it has other reliable source of milk. Beside the fact that ‘‘natural’’ behavioural pattern ‘‘naturally’’ vary enormously in quantity, there is another even more radical source of variation in natural behaviour: the existence of behavioural strategies. The term behavioural strategies denotes the fact that different individual animals (or one individual on different occasions) can and do achieve one and the same goal through several behavioural routes. For example, young piglets get their nutrition through sucking milk from their mother. When several lactating sows are housed together in one area, individual piglets also take milk from alien sows in a certain proportion of cases. This behaviour, usually labelled cross-sucking or allosucking, is not a case of abnormal or mistaken behaviour. It does occur in free-roaming domestic sows (Newberry and Wood-Gush, 1985) as well as in free-living wild boar groups (Mauget, 1981). In the case of group-housed lactating domestic sows, there are in fact three sucking strategies among the piglets (Maletı́nská and Špinka, 2001; Illmann et al., 2005). The faithful piglets suckle exclusively on their own mother; the opportunistic allosucklers have the own mother as the main source of milk but occasionally suckle at other sows and finally permanent allosucklers are piglets that had switched completely to another lactating sows at some point during the suckling period of life. In a stable group of lactating sows and their litters, all three strategies are represented and more than one third of all sucklings may occur between a piglet and a non-biological mother. Yet, in spite of this high level of allosuckling, none of the piglets seem to suffer from lack of milk intake. In fact, the three strategies seem to be equally successful (Maletı́nská and Špinka, 2001). In sum, animals are usually very flexible and often have several alternative strategies available in their natural behaviour. Therefore, if animals of the same species perform a behavioural pattern differently (or do not perform it at all in one environment (e.g., a more artificial one) than in another (a more ‘‘natural’’ one), it does not necessary mean that they are behaving ‘‘unnaturally’’. 4. Is the performance of natural behaviour always good for animal welfare? The performance of natural behaviour does not necessarily make a positive contribution to the welfare of farm animals At least two classes of naturally occurring behaviours actually decrease the welfare of animals. The first class includes patterns that I will label as ‘‘Emergency behaviours’’, i.e., behavioural reactions to situations that the animal perceives as immediate and serious threats to its fitness. For instance, fowl kept in large groups have little experience with humans and a person entering the stable may cause a sudden increase in panic-like anti-predatory behaviour (Mills and Faure, 1990). A second example relates to sows of modern breeds, whose appetite has been strongly increased through artificial selection for high food intake. While they are pregnant, their ration is reduced to 60% of their ad libitum intake (Lawrence et al., 1988). Although this ration is perfectly adequate for their body maintenance and the pregnancy, the sows perceive it as a threat of starvation. Consequently, they engage in intensive foraging attempts and if these are not attenuated through the provision of fibrous material such as straw (Whittaker et al., 1998) they may develop repetitive abnormal behaviours like bar biting and sham chewing (Lawrence and Terlouw, 1993). Emergency behaviours are thus behaviours that cost a lot in terms of fitness but were adaptive in the natural environment of the ancestors because the high cost was outweighed by the prevention of an even higher fitness loss. However, in terms of welfare in the current husbandry situations, these behaviours do not bring any benefits to the animals, while their 122 M. Špinka / Applied Animal Behaviour Science 100 (2006) 117–128 welfare cost are high, both in terms of biological functioning and health (e.g., risk of injury; Knierim and Gocke, 2003) and in the subjective experience, as their performance is usually accompanied by aversive motivational affective states (Fraser and Duncan, 1998). The discrepancy between the naturalness and the welfare-compromising effect of emergency behaviours stems from the fact that natural selection was maximizing fitness, and not welfare, and these two parameters do not go hand in hand. Although emergency behaviours are perfectly natural responses to the outstanding situation, it would be a non-sense to argue that farm animals must be provided with the opportunity to perform these behaviours. On the contrary, the welfare of the animals requires that situations that are perceived as fitness-threatening by the animals should be avoided in captivity. The second class of natural behaviours that may compromise the welfare of the population are those that contribute to the fitness (and welfare status) of the actor, while, at the same time, compromise the welfare of another animal or animals. These are the damaging behaviours. The ancestors of our domestic animals were selected to behave in such a way that would enhance their own fitness and, in proportion to the degree of relatedness, the fitness of their relatives. When a limited resource such as space, food or mating partners are at stake, the natural adaptive behaviour is to push rivals out of the competition. Thus, pigs naturally live in small groups (Graves, 1984). They therefore perceive the intrusion (and non-retreat) of alien conspecifics as a threat to their resources and vigorously attack these ‘‘intruders’’ (Rushen and Pajor, 1987). When mutually unacquainted pigs or groups of pigs are mixed, aggression results. Young suckling piglets, too, fight fiercely for the possession of teats (de Passillé and Rushen, 1989). Newborn piglets are in fact equipped with effective weapons (sharp incisor teeth) that can inflict painful injuries. In large litters, the weakest ones (Fraser and Thompson, 1991) suffer from this competition in terms of milk intake and weight gain. Social animals such as pig also can behave aggressively towards lame or injured animals, possibly as a prevention against infectious diseases. Sometimes, not allowing these damaging behaviours through making the situation unnatural may be better for the welfare of the animals. For instance, keeping weaned and growing pigs in large groups reduces substantially the aggression level among the pigs (Turner and Edwards, 2004; Andersen et al., 2004). As with emergency behaviours, it would not be in the line of welfare-improving policy to guarantee the animals the freedom to perform such damaging behaviours, even though they are undoubtedly a part of their natural behaviour repertoire. The discrepancy between naturalness and welfare arises here because natural selection was maximizing individual fitness, while welfare-caring people are concerned with welfare of all animals in the group equally. 5. What use natural behaviour in farm animal welfare? Is the concept of natural behaviour of any use in farm animal welfare after all? Clearly natural behaviour can differ in accordance with rather subtle factors and hence it is a vague guide at best of what we should strive for in farming systems. Moreover some behavioural patterns that clearly match our concept of natural behaviour are obviously detrimental to the welfare of the animals. Should we therefore exclude the concept of natural behaviour from animal welfare science and from the search for practical strategies to improve animal welfare? Would not it be better to restrict ourselves to the first four freedoms? In my view, the answer is no. There are important areas of concern in animal welfare that are not covered by the first four freedoms. I identify three examples of the importance of natural behaviour to good welfare: M. Špinka / Applied Animal Behaviour Science 100 (2006) 117–128 123 (1) Allowing animals to behave freely in an environment with key natural features may be the most effective and prudent way of achieving the goals of the farmer. (2) Natural behaviour involves patterns that are associated with positive affective states. (3) Natural behaviour may bring long-term benefits to the animals that are difficult to achieve otherwise or even just to foresee. 6. Allowing for behavioural needs It has been long noted that animals do possess needs with regard to their behaviour. Thus, prevention of performance of certain behavioural patterns may cause distress or even suffering. To take an obvious example, if feeding is prevented, the animal’s welfare is reduced because of increasing hunger, and may be reduced even further if the individual sees neighbouring animals feeding (Desiré et al., 2002). Although a narrow definition of ‘‘behavioural needs’’ as internally driven behaviours has been proposed (Hughes and Duncan, 1988; Duncan, 2005), it seems to be more useful not to split the behavioural repertoire of a species into a catalogue of ‘‘needs’’ and ‘‘non-needs’’. The alternative broader approach proposed by Jensen and Toates (1993) maintains that to what extent certain behaviour is a need at the moment depends on the combination of species-specific motivational structure and the current environmental conditions. The ‘‘needyness’’ of the behaviour is a complex of obtaining a goal and performing the (usually feedback-sensitive) species-specific behaviour on the route to the goal (Jensen and Toates, 1993) and can be evaluated by considering what will happen if the behaviour was prevented. Non-executing of the behaviour may have negative consequences, and the frustration from the non-performance itself may be one of them. Considering all these consequences is a more inclusive approach to behavioural ‘‘needs’’ than that focused solely on the contrast between internally and externally driven behaviours. For instance, suckling maternal milk is a need for young piglets because if this behaviour is prevented (e.g., through early weaning), piglets suffer from (at least temporary) undernutrition (Pajor et al., 2002). These problems can be at least partially prevented if piglets are provided by a very specific diet. Coping with the switch from suckling milk to eating solid food can be further ameliorated if piglets are given the opportunity and indeed encouraged to take in solid food already before weaning. In very young piglets (certainly before the age of 3 weeks; Worobec et al., 1999), the need to suckle milk has also the component of performing the behaviour leading to milk intake, because piglets weaned at that age perform the behaviour called ‘‘belly-nosing’’ which is most probably a re-oriented teat-massaging behaviour. Although it is theoretically possible to secure the goals associated with a specific behaviour by adjustments of the environment without involvement of the behaviour (e.g., through specific diets for the piglets) and thus reduces the needyness of certain behaviours, it is in most cases more efficient to allow for the natural behaviour. The flexibility of natural behaviour is not an argument against using the concept of natural behaviour in our efforts to improve farm animal welfare—it is, in fact, one of its main advantages. The flexibility of the route towards achieving a goal is in most natural motivational systems secured through a number of feedback mechanisms and choice points. If we allow for natural behaviour, the animals will, in many cases, use this opportunity flexibly to achieve their goals and thus satisfy their needs. For instance, the goals of thermoregulation at high and low temperatures and the goal of keeping body clean from faeces can be all achieved by the pigs through their own behaviour if they are housed in group pens that contain both a dry bedded area, an unbedded part with cool flooring 124 M. Špinka / Applied Animal Behaviour Science 100 (2006) 117–128 and enough room so that the animals can designate their own dunging areas (Wechsler and Bachmann, 1998). 7. Promoting positive behaviours Besides behaviours that serve some immediate goal, the behavioural repertoire of farm animal species also contains patterns that may be seen as ‘‘luxuries’’, since nothing serious immediately happens if they are not performed. The examples of such behaviours are play exploration (WoodGush and Vestergaard, 1991), or, to take a more specific example, copious use of a rotating brush as a scratching post by loose housed cattle. The fact that non-performance of these behaviours does not pose an immediate threat to fitness is also reflected in their low motivational priority. If any behavioural need appears, they are immediately interrupted, and may not reappear until long after the behaviours satisfying needs have ceased. Fraser and Duncan (1998) characterized these behaviours as being incited by ‘‘opportunity’’, i.e., by a situation when the cost of performing them is especially low. The important aspect for our discussion is that these behaviours are associated with positive affective states (e.g., Widowski and Duncan, 2000, for dust-bathing; Špinka et al., 2001, for play). The reason why negative affective states are linked with needdriven behaviours and the positive ones with opportunity-driven behaviours is the above mentioned motivational priority: animals will first attempt to remove the source of negative feelings, and only thereafter strive for positive experience. Obviously, allowing or even encouraging the animals to perform behavioural patterns associated with positive emotions undoubtedly improves their welfare. 8. Supporting behaviours with long-term benefits Allowing for natural behaviours, besides being an effective way of satisfying needs and promoting affectively positive behaviours, may also contribute to farm animal welfare through bringing long-term benefits. Common sense wisdom suggests that rearing conditions that allow or even stimulate the richness of natural behaviour would lead to harmonious development of the various skills in the animals, especially the social ones. Substantial scientific evidence has accumulated in support of this claim. For instance, suckling piglets reared in environments that allow the sow to leave her litters and/or provide the opportunity of piglets from different litters to mingle together at early age are less affected by the stressful weaning procedure (Weary et al., 2002; see Section 9.1 for more details). Calves that have spent several days rather than just the first few hours with their cow mother are more stressed in the hours immediately following the separation, but when tested 3–6 weeks later for social competence, display higher level of socially positive behaviours (Stěhulová et al., 2004; Flower and Weary, 2001). Laying hens exposed to perches at 4 weeks of age later use them at least twice as much as birds with no experience with perches before week 8 (Appleby and Duncan, 1989). The performance of one type of natural behaviour can also positively affect other, seemingly unrelated behavioural patterns. For instance, caged mink farmed for fur value an access to a water pool very highly (Mason et al., 2001). The pool allows swimming, diving, head-dipping and exploration around water, but also increases substantially the amount of playing in the main cage, away from the water. Since natural selection probably shaped in the animals a choice between ‘‘keeping low profile’’ when conditions are unfavourable and expanding activity if life is good, it is probable M. Špinka / Applied Animal Behaviour Science 100 (2006) 117–128 125 that many instances of long-term or behaviourally remote positive consequences of behaving naturally will be found. 9. Allowing for natural behaviour improves welfare—examples from practice 9.1. Natural social environment for suckling piglets Suckling piglets of the ancestor of the domestic pig, the wild boar (Sus scrofa) naturally live in a complex social environment. Beside the mother who suckles them until the natural weaning at 17 weeks (Jensen and Recén, 1989), there are usually other lactating sows in the group and other litters of similarly aged suckling piglets (Graves, 1984). The most widely used commercial intensive housing system allow the piglets just contact with the mother, and that only for a limited period of 3–5 weeks and in a restricted way, since the sow herself is confined to a small crate. A more nature-like alternative is a system that has been designed and tested by the teams of Ed Pajor and Dan Weary in Canada. In its full version, it is ‘‘sow-controlled’’, i.e., it allows the sow to leave the piglets over a roller barrier (that the piglets cannot cross) into a space where she can socialize with other sows; and it also allows several litters of piglets to mingle a common area. In production terms, this alternative housing brings results comparable to the confinement systems (Weary et al., 2002), and yet it provides welfare advantages for both the sows and the piglets. First, the sows can regulate the amount time of time that they want to spend away from piglets and thus reduce the demand by the litter for a too-frequent nursing. Second, they can interact socially with each other and since the majority of interactions in stable groups of sows are positive, this is beneficial for their welfare. Third, although the piglets tend to grow a bit slower during the suckling period in this housing than in the restrictive one, this is fully compensated through their substantially better growth during the period after weaning, especially during the very first postweaning days. Because for piglets the weaning is a major stress associated with health and behavioural problems (diarrhoea, fighting and belly-nosing), the obviously better coping of litters from the alternative housing system enhances their welfare substantially. Other systems for farrowing and lactating sows that are based on the performance of natural behaviour have been developed and successfully put in practice in Scandinavia and in the United States (Lidfors et al., 2005). 9.2. Driving pigs in slaughterhouses Allowing animals to behave in a natural way can be effective and welfare-promoting also in more specific, down-to-earth situations. For instance, farm animals such as pigs need often to be brought somewhere on foot. There are two extreme options available: either to drive them by brute force, or to incite them to walk there by themselves. This problem is very acute on pig slaughterplants. Pigs must be moved fast and in a constant pace in order to achieve good economic results. Forcing the movement through shouting, hitting and electric proding of individual pigs in narrow aisles that guide the movement is possible. However, it needs a high input of very unpleasant labour, and the pigs are extremely stressed through this procedure. The alternative option is to use the natural tendency of groups of pigs to move gradually into an open space. Using slowly moving barriers that open new space in front of the animals and gradually close the space behind, groups of pigs can be moved much more peacefully towards the stunning point. If this system is connected with a well-functioning group stunning procedure using carbon dioxide, the pre-slaughter stress of pigs can be decreased. 126 M. Špinka / Applied Animal Behaviour Science 100 (2006) 117–128 10. Conclusion Farm animals do not need ‘‘natural behaviour’’ per se for good welfare. However, providing the opportunity for natural behaviour is often a very effective way to satisfy the needs and/or goals of the animals, to provide them with emotionally positive experience, and to stimulate their behavioural development in such a way that it brings long-term benefits. Therefore, natural behaviour of the species in question should be considered both when new housing systems are being developed (Lidfors et al., 2005) but also when solutions to particular problems in existing systems are being sought. Acknowledgements The preparation of this paper was supported by the grant IAA6307402 from the Grant Agency of the Czech Academy of Science and the grant MZE 0002701402 from the Czech Ministry of Agriculture. I am most grateful to the CIWF Fund for inviting me to write the paper. Bo Algers provided me with the information on alternative way of driving pigs in slaughterhouses. References Andersen, I.L., Naevdal, E., Bakken, M., Boe, K.E., 2004. Aggression and group size in domesticated pigs, Sus scrofa: ‘‘when the winner takes it all and the loser is standing small’’. Anim. Behav. 68, 965–975. Appleby, M.C., Duncan, I.J.H., 1989. Development of perching in hens. Biol. Behav. 14, 157–168. Arey, D.S., 1997. Behavioral observations of peri-parturient sows and the development of alternative farrowing accommodation—a review. Anim. Welf. 6, 217–229. Baxter, M.R., 1983. Ethology in environmental-design for animal production. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 9, 207–220. Bekoff, M., 1977. Social communication in canids: evidence for the evolution of a stereotyped mammalian display. Science 197, 1097–1099. Burne, T.H.J., Murfitt, P.J.E., Gilbert, C.L., 2001a. Influence of environmental temperature on pgf(2 alpha)-induced nest building in female pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 71, 293–304. Burne, T.H.J., Murfitt, P.J.E., Johnston, A.N.B., 2001b. Pgf(2 Alpha)-induced nest building and choice behaviour in female domestic pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 73, 267–279. Damm, B.I., Bildsoe, M., Gilbert, C., Ladewig, J., Vestergaard, K.S., 2002. the effects of confinement on periparturient behaviour and circulating prolactin, prostaglandin f-2 alpha and oxytocin in gilts with access to a variety of nest materials. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 76, 135–156. Damm, B.I., Lisborg, L., Vestergaard, K.S., Vanicek, J., 2003a. Nest-building, behavioural disturbances and heart rate in farrowing sows kept in crates and Schmid pens. Livest. Prod. Sci. 80, 175–187. Damm, B.I., Pedersen, L.J., Marchant-Forde, J.N., Gilbert, C.L., 2003b. Does feed-back from a nest affect periparturient behaviour, heart rate and circulatory cortisol and oxytocin in gilts? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 83, 55–76. Damm, B.I., Vestergaard, K.S., Schroder-Petersen, D.L., Ladewig, J., 2000. The effects of branches on prepartum nest building in gilts with access to straw. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 69, 113–124. de Passillé, A.M.B., Rushen, J., 1989. Suckling and teat disputes by neonatal piglets. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 22, 23–38. Desiré, L., Boissy, A., Veissier, I., 2002. Emotions in farm animals: a new approach to animal welfare in applied ethology. Behav. Process. 60, 165–180. Duncan, I.J.H., 1996. Animal welfare defined in terms of feelings. Acta Agric. Scand. Sect. A-Anim. Sci. 29–35. Duncan, I.J.H., 2005. Behavioral needs, stereotypies and animal welfare. SCAW Newslett. 27, 9–14. Flower, F.C., Weary, D.M., 2001. Effects of early separation on the dairy cow and calf. 2. Separation at 1 day and 2 weeks after birth. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 70, 275–284. Fraser, D., Duncan, I.J.H., 1998. ‘‘Pleasures’’, ‘‘pains’’ and animal welfare: toward a natural history of affect. Anim. Welf. 7, 383–396. Fraser, D., Thompson, B.K., 1991. Armed sibling rivalry among suckling piglets. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 29, 9–15. Graves, H.B., 1984. Behavior and ecology of wild and feral swine (Sus scrofa). J. Anim. Sci. 58, 482–492. M. Špinka / Applied Animal Behaviour Science 100 (2006) 117–128 127 Haley, D.B., Bailey, D.W., Stookey, J., 2005. The effects of weaning beef calves in two stages on their behaviour and growth rate. J. Anim. Sci. 83, 2205–2214. Hughes, B.O., Duncan, I.J.H., 1988. The notion of ethological ‘need’, models of motivation and animal welfare. Anim. Behav. 36, 1696–1707. Hughes, B.O., Duncan, I.J.H., Brown, M.F., 1989. The performance of nest building by domestic hens: is it more important than the construction of a nest? Anim. Behav. 37, 210–214. Illmann, G., Pokorná, Z., Špinka, M., 2005. Nursing synchronization and milk ejection failure as maternal strategies to reduce allosuckling in pair-housed sows (Sus scrofa domestica). Ethology 111, 652–668. Jensen, P., 1989. Nest site choice and nest building of free-ranging domestic pigs due to farrow. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 22, 13–21. Jensen, P., 1993. Nest building in domestic sows—the role of external stimuli. Anim. Behav. 45, 351–358. Jensen, P., Recén, B., 1989. When to wean—observations from free-ranging domestic pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 23, 49–60. Jensen, P., Toates, F.M., 1993. Who needs’ behavioural needs’—motivational aspects of the needs of animals. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 37, 161–181. Knierim, U., Gocke, A., 2003. Effect of catching broilers by hand or machine on rates of injuries and dead-on-arrivals. Anim. Welf. 12, 63–73. Kurz, J.C., Marchington, R.L., 1972. Radiotelemetry studies of feral hogs in South Carolina. J. Wildl. Manage. 36, 1240– 1248. Lawrence, A.B., Appleby, M.C., MacLeod, H.A., 1988. Measuring hunger in the pig using operant conditioning: the effect of food restriction. Anim. Prod. 47, 131–138. Lawrence, A.B., Terlouw, E.M.C., 1993. A review of behavioral factors involved in the development and continued performance of stereotypic behaviors in pigs. J. Anim. Sci. 71, 2815–2825. Lidfors, L., Berg, C., Algers, B., 2005. Integration of natural behavior in housing systems. Ambio 34, 325–330. Lidfors, L.M., 1996. Behavioural effects of separating the dairy calf immediately or 4 days post-partum. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 49, 269–283. Maletı́nská, J., Špinka, M., 2001. Cross-suckling and nursing synchronisation in group housed lactating sows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 75, 17–32. Mason, G.J., Cooper, J., Clarebrough, C., 2001. Frustrations of fur-farmed mink. Nature 410, 35–36. Mauget, R., 1981. Behavioural and reproductive strategies in wild forms of Sus scrofa (European wild boar and feral pigs). In: Sybesma, W. (Ed.), The Welfare of Pigs. Martinus Nijhoff, The Hague, pp. 3–13. Mayer, J.J., Martin, F.D., Brisbin, I.L., 2002. Characteristics of wild pig farrowing nests and beds in the upper coastal plain of South Carolina. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 78, 1–17. Mills, A.D., Faure, J.M., 1990. Panic and hysteria in domestic fowl: a review. In: Zayan, R., Dantzer, R. (Eds.), Social Stress in Domestic Animals. Kluwer Academic Press, Dordrecht, pp. 248–272. Newberry, R.C., Wood-Gush, G.M., 1985. The suckling behaviour of domestic pigs in a semi-natural environment. Behaviour 95, 11–25. Pajor, E.A., Weary, D.M., Caceres, C., Fraser, D., Kramer, D.L., 2002. Alternative housing for sows and litters. Part 3. Effects of piglet diet quality and sow-controlled housing on performance and behaviour. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 76, 267–277. Reinhardt, C., Reinhardt, A., Reinhardt, V., 1986. Social behaviors and reproductive performance in semi-wild Scottish Highland cattle. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 15, 125–136. Reinhardt, V., Reinhardt, A., 1981. Natural sucking performance and age of weaning in zebu cattle (Bos indicus). J. Agric. Sci. 96, 309–312. Rushen, J., Pajor, E., 1987. Offence and defence in fights between young pigs (Sus scrofa). Aggress. Behav. 13, 329–346. Špinka, M., Illmann, G., 1991. Development of group-housed dairy calves behaviour and the effect of separation of the mother 5 days post partum. In: Abstracts of the 25th International Congress of the Society of Veterinary Ethology, Edinburgh, July 3–6, p. 93. Špinka, M., Illmann, G., 1992. Suckling behaviour of young dairy calves with their own and alien mothers. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 33, 165–173. Špinka, M., Newberry, R.C., Bekoff, M., 2001. Mammalian play: training for the unexpected. Q. Rev. Biol. 76, 1–28. Stěhulová, I., Lidfors, L., Špinka, M., 2004. Response of dairy calves to the early separation from their mothers: effect of calves’ age and visual/auditory contact. In: Hänninen, L., Valros, A. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 38th International Congress of the ISAE, Helsinki, Finland, ISAE, p. 157. Thodberg, K., Jensen, K.H., Herskin, M.S., 2002. Nest building and farrowing in sows: relation to the reaction pattern during stress farrowing environment and experience. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 77, 21–42. 128 M. Špinka / Applied Animal Behaviour Science 100 (2006) 117–128 Thomas, T.J., Weary, D.M., Appleby, M.C., 2001. Newborn and 5-week-old calves vocalize in response to milk deprivation. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 74, 165–173. Turner, S.P., Edwards, S.A., 2004. Housing immature domestic pigs in large social groups: implications for social organisation in a hierarchical society. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 87, 239–253. Veissier, I., Lamy, D., Le Neindre, P., 1990. Social behaviour in domestic beef cattle when yearling calves are left with the cows for the next calving. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 27, 193–200. Weary, D.M., Pajor, E.A., Bonenfant, M., Fraser, D., Kramer, D.L., 2002. Alternative housing for sows and litters. Part 4. Effects of sow-controlled housing combined with a communal piglet area on pre- and post-weaning behaviour and performance. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 76, 279–290. Webster, J., 1994. Animal Welfare. A Cool Eye Towards Eden. Blackwell Science, Oxford. Wechsler, B., Bachmann, I., 1998. A sequential analysis of eliminative behaviour in domestic pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 56, 29–36. Whittaker, X., Spoolder, H.A.M., Edwards, S.A., Lawrence, A.B., Corning, S., 1998. The influence of dietary fibre and the provision of straw on the development of stereotypic behaviour in food restricted pregnant sows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 61, 89–102. Widowski, T.M., Duncan, I.J.H., 2000. Working for a dustbath: are hens increasing pleasure rather than reducing suffering. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 68, 39–53. Wood-Gush, D.G.M., Vestergaard, K., 1991. The seeking of novelty and its relation to play. Anim. Behav. 42, 599–606. Worobec, E.K., Duncan, I.J.H., Widowski, T.M., 1999. The effects of weaning at 7, 14 and 28 days on piglet behaviour. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 62, 173–182. Yin, S., McCowan, B., 2004. Barking in domestic dogs: context specificity and individual identification. Anim. Behav. 68, 343–355.