Perceptionof the voiced-voicelesscontrastin syllable-finalstops James Hillenbrand Department ofCommunicative Disorders, 2299Sheridan Road,Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois 60201 DennisR. Ingrisano Speech Research Laboratory, Department ofCommunicative Disorders andSciences, Wichita State University, Wichita;Kansas67208 Bruce k. Smith Department ofCommunicative Disorders, 2299Sheridan Road,Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois 60201 James E. Fiego Department ofBiocommunication, U•4BMedicalCenter,University Station, Birmingham, •41abama 35294 (Received 9 June1983;accepted for publication 6 February1984) A computer editingtechnique wasusedto removevaryingamounts of voicingfromthesyllablefinalclosureintervalsof naturallyproducedtokensof/pcb, pod,p•g, pag,pig,pug/. Vowelsfor all sixsyllables wereapproximately thesameduration,andthefinalrelease burstswereretained. Identificationresultsshowedthat voiceless responses tendedto occurin relativelylargenumbers whenall of theclosurevoicingand,in mostcases, a portionof thepreceding vowel-to-consonant (VC}transition hadbeenremoved. A second experiment demonstrated thatremoval offinal release burstshadverylittleeffectontheidentification functions. Acousticmeasurements were madeinanattempt togaininformation abouttheacoustic bases ofthelisteners' voiced-voiceless judgments. In general, stimulithatsubjects tended to identifyasvoiceless showed higherfirstformantoffsetfrequencies andshorterintensitydecaytimesthanstimulithatsubjects tendedto identifyasvoiced.However,for stopsfollowing/i/and/u/these acoustic differences were relativelysmall.We wereunableto finda singleacoustic measure, or anycombination of measures, that clearlyexplainedthelisteners'voiced-voiceless decisions. PACS numbers: 43.70.Dn, 43.70.Ve INTRODUCTION The voicingcontrastin Englishstopshasbeenstudied ratherextensively, but the majorityof thiswork hasfocused on stopsin initial position.The most well-developed approachto describing voicingcontrasts in initialstopsis the glottal/supraglottal timing view proposedby Lisker and Abramson (1964,1967,1970;seeMal6cot,1970,foranalternativepoint of view).Articulatorycontrolof initial stop voicingcontrasts isthoughtto involvevariations in thetimingofvoicingonsetrelativetoarticulatory release. Thevoießing opposition in finalstopsalsoinvolvesthe timingof a laryngealgesturerelativeto articulatoryeventsin theupper airway. For syllablesendingin voiceless stops,a laryngeal gesturetypicallyterminatesvoicingat aboutthe sametime that articulatoryclosureis achieved.For syllablesendingin voiced stops,glottal vibration generallycontinuesinto at leastsomeportionof theclosureinterval.In thissense, syllable-finalvoicingcontrasts mightbethoughtof in termsof a "voiceoffsettime" featuresomewhatanalogousto voiceonsettime in initial stops. In articulatoryterms,voiceoffsettime would refer to thetimingof a phonation-terminating gesturerelativeto the achievementofarticulatoryclosure.Becauseof theearlyterminationof glottal vibrationin final voicelessstops,these syllablesare generallycharacterized by (1) relativelyhigh firstformant(F 1)terminatingfrequencies (Wolf, 1978)and 18 J. Acoust.Sec.Am.76 (1), July1984 (2) silentrather thanßpartially or fully voicedclosureintervals (Smith, 1979;Hogan and Roszypal,1980;Flege and Brown, 1982). Further, measurementsthat were made as pilot work to the presentstudyshowthat vowelswhichprecedevoiceless stopsare generallyterminatedwith a more abruptdropin intensityascomparedto vowelsthat precede voicedstops(seeDerbock,1977,for similarfindingsonstops in FrenchandDutch).The kindof laryngealtimingdistinction discussedabove,however,doesnot accountfor the well- known influenceof consonantvoicingon the duration of preceding vowels.In phrase-final positionin English,vowels precedingvoicedconsonantsare generallyabout 50 to 100 ms longerthan vowelsprecedingvoiceless consonants(e.g., House and Fairbanks, 1953; House, 1961; Petersonand Le- histe,1960;Chen,1970)? On the basisof the acousticpropertiesassociatedwith the syllable-finalvoicing contrast, perceptual cues to this distinctionmight involvethreekindsof acousticfeatures:(1) thepresenceversusabsenceof a low-amplitudelow-frequen- cy "voicebar" duringtheclosureinterval,(2)somecombination of frequencyand intensitycharacteristicsassociated with gradual versusabrupt terminationof the preceding vowel(i.e.,F 1offsetfrequencyandintensitydecaytime),and (3)thedurationofthepreceding vowel.Althoughthevowel durationdifferenceis oftenconsideredto be of primary perceptualimportanceto finalstopcognateoppositions, experi- 0001-4966/84/070018-09500.80 ¸ 1984Acoustical Societyof America 18 mental evidence supporting thisconclusion consists primarIn thepresent studyvoicing cueswereexamined using ilyofa series ofsynthetic speech studies byRaphael andhis syllable-final voicedstopsvaryingin placeof articulation colleagues {Raphael, 1972; Raphael etal.,1975,1980). 2Ra- andvowelenvironment. Editingtechniques wereusedtodephael(1972) used thePattern Playback tosynthesize sylla- terminehowmuchof the finalvoicedsegment hadto be blesendingin voiced andvoiceless stopsandfriesrives. In general, finalconsonants wereheardasvoiceless whenpreceded byvowels ofshortduration andasvoiced whenprecededby vowelsof longduration.Raphaelconcluded that preceding voweldurationwasbotha necessary andsufficientcueto syllable-final voicing. Similarfindings werereported intwosubsequent synthesis studies byRaphael etal. (1975,1980). Theconclusions based onthese synthesis studies have notbeensupported by morerecentworkinvolving edited natural speech.For example,Wardrip-Fruin(1982a) showed thatvowels preceding finalvoicedstopscouldbe reduced in duration byone-third withoutelicitingvoiceless responses. Severalotherinvestigators usingeditednatural speech haveshownthatreducing vowelduration, by itself, doesnottypically makea naturally produced voicedstop sound voiceless (O'Kane,1978;HoganandRozsypal, 1980; Raphael, 1981;Revoile eta!., 1982).It hasalsobeenshown thatexpanding vowel durations ofnaturally produced sylla- removedbeforesubjects heardfinal voiceless stops.The studyinvolved twolistening experiments anda series ofpost hocacousticanalyses of the editedstimuli.The stimulifor experiment1 wereeditedin sucha wayasto retainthefinal releasebursts.Experiment2 examinedthe role of release burstsby askingsubjects to identifyeditedstimulibothwith andwithoutfinalbursts.The purposeof the acoustic measurements wasto providepreliminarytestsof severalhypotheses regarding acoustic cuesto finalvoicingcontrasts. I. EXPERIMENT 1 A. Stimuli 1.Record/ngand measurement Thestimuliconsisted ofa combination ofnaturallyproducedandeditedtokens of/p•b, p•d,p•g,pug,pig,pug,p•p, pet, p•k, pak, pik, puk/. A maletalkerproducedseveral repetitions ofeachsyllable. Audiorecordings weremadeina sound-treated boothwith a headsetmicrophone (Shure SMll) anda reel-to-reel tapedeck(AkaiGX --4(X}0DB). stimuliwerethenlow-pass filteredat 4 blesending invoiceless stops does notresultin voiced stop The tape-recorded kHz and digitized at a 10-kHz sample frequency. Oscillograjudgments(Hoganand Rozsypal,1980;Revoileet al., phicrepresentations of 100-mssegments of thestimuliwere 1982). 3 ona high-resolution graphics terminal(Tektronix Mostoftheevidence fromeditednaturalspeech studies displayed 4010). The oscillograms were used to segment eachstimulus suggests thatfinalstopvoicingcontrasts arecuedprimarily into (1) voice onset time (VOT) of initial/p/, (2)vowel,(3) by acoustic information in the vicinityof articulatory clofinal stop closure, and (4) final stop release burst. n For the sure.For example,Wolf (1978)madeeditingcutsat several stimuliendingin voicedstops,thesemeasurements were locations from naturallyproducedsyllables suchas/•eb/, used to select which of the multiple repetitions would be /•-cl/, and/a•g/.Removal oftheentireclosure intervalproused in the perception tasks. Tokens were required to meet duced16%voiceless responses, whileremovalof theclosure closure intervalmeasuring intervaland threepitchperiodsfrom the vowel-to-conso- twocriteria:(1)a fullyvoiced about 75 ms ( _ 5ms) and (2) an audible finalburstmeasurnantIVC) transitionproduced 70% voiceless responses (see ing 5-15 ms in duration. Measurements of the six stimuli Revoileetal., 1982,for similarfindings). Theseresultsseem ending in voiced stops are shown in Table I. For comparison tosuggest thatfinalstopvoicingcontrasts arecuedprimarily the table also gives duration measurements from thesixsylby differences in thewayin whichthepreceding vowelis lables ending in voiceless stops. These stimuli were included terminated {Parker,1974;WalshandParker,198I). as a reliability check on listener responses and were notreThepresentstudywasdesigned to examinecuesto the quired to meet any durational criteria. perception of finalstopvoicingin greaterdetailby usinga fine-grained editingtechnique on naturallyproduced sylla- /TABLEI. Timemeasurements (inms)for(1)VOToftheinitial stop, (2) duration ofthefinalstop,(4)burstduration, and ble-finalstopsin several vowelenvironments. Wolf (1978) vowelduration,(3)closure duration. Thevalues in parenth eses indicate thevoweland andRevoileet al. (1982)studiedcuesto finalstopvoicing {5}totalsyllable syllable durations afterapplication oftheeditingtechnique thatequali7ed usingstimuliin theenvironment of/•e/, a vowelthatmight voweldurations forstimuliendingin voicedstop•. tendto accentuate differences in bothF 1 offsetfrequency anddecaytime.Theopenvocaltractconfiguration of vowels VOT Vowel Closure Burst Syllable suchas/•e/results in relativelyhigh intensities and highfrequencyfirstformants.As the vocaltract constrictsfor the finalclosure,substantialdecreases are seenin overallintensi- p•b ped peg pag ty andin thefrequency of thefirstformant.Therefore,editingcutsthattruncatetheVC transitionwouldresultin rela- pig pug tivelyhighF 1 offsetfrequencies andshortdecaytimes.On pep theotherhand,moreconstricted vowelswouldbeexpected pet to showlessdramaticintensityandfrequency changes in the pœk VC transition.For thisreasonit is unclearwhetherthepat- pak tern of results obtained with/•e/would also be seen when editingcutsare made from stimuli in the contextof more constrictedvowels, such as/i/and/u/.. 19 J. Acoust. Soc.Am.,Vol.76, No.1, July1984 pik puk 37 47 67 61 122 (112) 131 (110) 106 (106) 174 (107) 77 71 76 73 7 4 8 16 243 (232) 253 (232) 257 (257) 324 (257) 49 65 149(113) 142(108) 73 75 13 5 284 (248) 288 (254) 30 58 82 ' 111 105 116 41 95 51 88 86 85 ...a 90 ..." 11 93 4 108 88 94 18 23 12 140 270 243 337 248 279 "Tiffs token was uareleased. Hillenbrand etal.: Voiced-voiceless contrast 19 2. Control of vowel duration TOhold anypotential influence ofvowel duration constantacrossthe six continua,a computereditingprocedure was usedto modify the vowel durationsof the six stimuli endingin voicedstops.Beginning withthethirdpitchperiod of thevowel,everyotherpitchperiodwasremoveduntil the vowelwaswithin one-halfpitchperiodof 110ms.To avoid introducing clicks,editingcutsweremadeat zerocrossings. By removingeveryotherpitchperiod,abruptdiscontinuities in fundamentalfrequencyand formant frequencieswere avoided.Sincethe editingprogramwasableto shortenbut notlengthenvowels,all of thestimuliwereadjusted'to accoroodatetheshortest vowel.The vowelin/peg/was theshortestat 106msand wasleft unmodified. Vowel and syllable durationsafter applicationof this editingtechniqueare shownin parenthese• in TableI. 3. Edit?}gof closurevoicing Varyingamountsofglottal pulsingwereremovedfrom the closureintervalsof the six syllablesendingin voiced stops.Figure I showsa continuumbasedon the natural to- FIG. 2. Oscillogram of the 20-msstimulusfrom the/peg/continuum shownwiththeassociated weighting function.Starting35 mspriorto the finalburst,theweighting function decays linearlyover15msfroma gainof onetoagainofzero,remains atzerofor20ms,thenreturnsinstantaneously to a gainof onefor the durationof the burst.Note that the nominalvalueof 20msrefersonlytotheamountoftimethattheweighting function remains at zero. kenof/peg/. Thecontinuum consisted of 13stimuliranging from 0-120 ms of signalremoved,in 10-mssteps.Because theclosure intervalfortheoriginal/peg/was76 msin duration(seeTableI}, thestimulifrom80to 120msin thecontinuuminvolvethe removalof all of the voicingfrom the closureintervalanda portionofthepreceding vowel.Editingof the stimuliwas accomplished by multiplyingthe original digitizedwaveforms by a seriesof weightingfunctionsof the typeshownin Fig. 2. This figuredisplaysa weightingfunctionsuperimposed onthestimulusfromthe/peg/seriesthat is labeled"20" in Fig. 1. Valuesin the weightingfunction rangedfrom zero(completeattenuationof the signal}to one {nosignalattenuation). The nominalvalueof 20 msrefersto theamountof timethat the weightingfunctionremainedat zero.The zero-amplitudeintervalwasprecededby a 15-ms linear decay function which was intendedto simulatethe gradualamplitudereductionthat generallyoccursin natural speechwhen voicingterminatesprior to release.The decay functionalso reducedthe possibilitythat the editing cuts would introduce transients into the stimuli. It should be em- phasizedthat the 20-msnominalvaluerefersonly to the zero-amplitude portionof the weightingfunctionand does notincludethatportionof thesignalattenuated bythe 15-ms decayfunction.Followingthe zero-amplitude interval,the weightingfunctionreturnedinstantaneously to a valueof onein orderto retain.thereleaseburst.A seriesof weighting functionswas calculatedindividually for each stimulusso that this final full-amplitudeportion of the weightingfunction exactlyfit the releaseburst. B. Subjects and procedures Subjectswere 23 NorthwesternUniversity students withnoreportedhistoryof hearingor speech problems. Presentationof stimuliand collectionof subjects'responses wereunderthecontrolof a laboratorycomputerequipped with a high-speeddisk drive and a 12-bit D/A converter.At theoutputof theD/A converter thestimuliwerelow-pass filteredat 4 kHz, amplified,attenuated,and deliveredbinaurallyovermatchedTDH-49 headphones. The outputattenuatorwasadjustedsothat signalspeakedat 78 dBA. Each stimulus continuum consisted of 14 tokens: one numbersto therightof eachoscillogram arenominalvaluesthatreflectthe exemplarof the naturalvoicedstop,12editedversions, and onenaturalproductionof thevoiceless cognate. The identificationtestsconsisted of tenrandomlyorderedpresentations ofthe 14stimuli.Oneachtrialsubjects wereaskedto pressa amountof signalthatwasremoved bytheeditingprocedure (seetextj. button labeledB,D,G or a button labeledP,T,K. Each sub- FIG. 1.Stimulus continuum constructed bymaking increasingly largeeditingcutsformthevoiced closure intervalofa naturallyproduced/peg/. The 20 d. Acoust.Soc.Am.,Vol.76, No.1, July1984 Hillenbrand etal.: Voiced-voiceless contrast 20 TABLE II. Mean voiced-voiceless categoryboundaries, standarddevia- tions,andranges foreachstimulus continuum. Thevalues (inms)represent the amountof signalremovedfrom the finalvoicedsegments of syllables endingin voicedstops. Standard 981 9(! • 881 89 >o7e 76 Continuum Mean deviation Range • /peb/ /peal/ /peg/ 68 77 77 iI 14 13 48- 88 58- 93 62-122 • 49 49 /pag/ /pig/ /pug/ 70 61 63 7 6 7 $5- 80 54- 80 5O- 78 c• 3el • •'e 39 28 88 18 ject wastestedon all sixcontinuaand eachreceiveda different orderingof the conditions. C. Results and discussion • 3el 4el 59 VOICING 88 76 89 • lei8 Ilei l:2(! N REI'K•VEO FIG. 4. Id•tification r•ul• shag ronm•t. •ch f•cfion repr•U • "N" • the a•s• repr•n• r• the eff•t of v•ng the vowel••1• r•u!• fr• 23 •e•. •e • the un•i• voi•l• sto•. Table II showsvoiced-voiceless categoryboundaries andmeasures Ofdispersion foreachcontinuum. Category boundaries were calculatedby linear interpolationof the 50% crossover fromvoicedto voiceless. In general,category boundaries tendedto occurin the 60- to 80-msrange.Figures3 and 4 showthe percentage of voicedresponses as a functionof the sizeof the editingcut. Figure 3 showsthat therewasa slightlyearliercrossover for the labialcontin- uumascompared to thealveolar andvelarcontinua. Figure 4 showsearliercrossovers for/pig/and/pug/, thetwovowelswith low-frequency first formants.Althoughthe place andvoweleffectsseemto befairly smallin termsof absolute magnitude,two separaterepeatedmeasuresANOVAs showed that both effects'were significant [place: F(2,44)= 4.9,p < 0.05;vowel:F(3,66)= 20.2,p < 0.01]. In general,the listeningtestsshowedthat voiceless responses werenot elicitedin largenumbersuntil the closure intervaland, in mostcases,a portionof the VC transition had beenremoved.This findingis consistentwith the idea Ilala= OUP DATA CN-23) leila that informationin the VC transitionsis importantto the perception of finalvoicingcontrast. A moredetaileddiscussionof thesefindingswill await the resultsof a seriesof acousticanalysesof the stimuli,describedin See.III. II. EXPERIMENT 2 The main findingof experimentI wasthat voiceless responses didnotpredominate untilrelatively largeamounts of voicinghadbeenremovedfromthestimuli.The possibility exists,however,thatthesyllable-final releaseburstscontainedcueswhichbiasedsubjectstowardvoicedresponses. Thispossibility motivateda second experiment with a new groupof 11 subjectswhich examinedthe role of release bursts. The stimuli for this experimentconsistedof the /peb/,/ped/, and/peg/continuafromexperiment1.Stimuli fromeachcontinuumwerepresented to thesubjects both with and without releasebursts. The releasebursts were eli- minated simplybyaligning a cursortoredefine theendofthe stimulus.Subject-selection criteria,instrumentation, and procedures wereidenticalto thosedescribed for experiment 1. A. Results and discussion • 78 7el • se se •• 40 •• •i •- PED PEa m- •3a • 20 4e Theresultsfromexperiment 2 areshownseparately for eachplaceof articulationin Fig. 5. Althoughtheburstconditionsshowearliercrossovers in eachcomparison, thedifferences in thecategoryboundaries areonly 3-4 ms.A twowayrepeated measures analysis of variancefelljustshortof significance for the burstversusno burstcomparison IF (1,10}= 4.6,p <0.07]. In contrastto the resultsof experi- 3e 29 ment 1, phoneticboundaries werenot significantly affected bydifferences in placeof articulationIF (2,20)= 3.0,p NS]. The place-by-burst interactiondid not approachsignifiThe relativelyminor role playedby burstsin this experi- •G. 3. Id•tion r•ul• •o•ng •e •t ofv•ing thep• ofpr• ducfi• • the• s•p. •h fun• r•r•n• the•1• •ul• 23•e•. •e "N" onthea•i• repr• r• to theunMi• voi•l• 21 sty. d.Acoust. Soc.Am.,Vol.76, NO.1, July1984 mentshouldbeinterpreted in lightof thefactthattherelease burstswere relativelybrief (15 ms or less}.Longerrelease burstsforvoicedstopssometimes showevidence ofperiodicity (e.g.,Wolf, 1978}andmight,therefore, influence voicing Hillenbrand eta/.: Voiced-voiceless contrast 21 -- not stronglyinfluencevoicingjudgmentsis supported by severalotherstudies(Mal6cot,1958;Wolf, 1978;Raphael, 1981;Wardrip-Fruin,1982a;Revoileet al., 1982).Thisresultisalsosensible in lightofproduction datashowing thata largeproportion of finalstopsareproduced withoutrelease bursts(Rositzke,1943;CrystalandHouse,1982). The minimalimportanceof finalburstswould'seemto GROUPDATA CN-I I) 9B 8B 6• implythattheduration of theclosure intervalwasnota S0 \\ 40 strongvoicingcue for thesestimuli.Sincethe final release burstmarkstheendof theclosureinterval,it seemslogicalto ß= PEB: BURST assume that removal of this marker would alter the listeners' 3• impression of the durationof the interval.Therefore,the failure to find large differences betweenthe burst and no burstconditions suggests that closuredurationdidnot playa significant role in the subjects' voicingjudgments.This is consistent with Raphael(1981}who showedthat extending final/g/closures to durationsappropriatefor/k/did not elicit/k/responsesfrom listeners. ;0 98 IlL ACOUSTIC Oneof thedi•cultiesin interpretingthelabelingresults isthatcategory boundaries occurred verycloseto thebeginningof theclosure interval.Locationof theboundary in this regionmakes it difficultto decide between twoverydifferent (andpossiblynonmutuallyexclusive) alternativesconcerningcuesto syllable-final voicing.Onepossibility isthatsubjeers'decisions werebasedonthepresence versus absence of audiblevoicingduringthe closureintervals.Anotherpossibility is that decisions werebasedon gradualversusabrupt 78 ß = PED= BURST • 38 IBB ANALYSIS termination of the precedingvowels.Our strategyin this sectionwas to try to make somespeculationsabout the acousticbasesof our listeners'voiced-voiceless judgments basedon acousticanalysisof the stimuli.In particular,we - triedto findsystematic acoustic differences betweenstimuli that straddledthe voiced-voiceless boundary.Threeparameterswereexamined:(1) presence versusabsence of closure voicing,(2)the offsetfrequencyof the firstformant,and (3) rate of intensitydecayat stimulusoffset.For all of these analyses, wecompared measurements fromthemostedited stimulus thatproduced 75%ormorevoiced responses with 80 $0 4B ß- theleasteditedstimulus thatproduced 75% or morevoicelessresponses. Since these represent themostphysically similar stimulithatproduced qualitatively different labeling re- PEG: BURST 3B. n•P•T sponses, wereasoned thatcomparing measurements from thesepairsmightrevealacoustic differences thatlisteners 20 usedto make voiced-voiceless judgments. 0 ß 0 10 29 30 40 $0 VOTCTNG 60 70 89 go I•N} 110 120 N REHOVED FIG.5. Identificationresultsshowingthe effectof removingthe release burst.Eachfunctionrepresents the pooledresultsfrom 11 listeners.The "N" on theabcissa represents responses to theuneditedvoiceless stops. Thefirstpossibility thatwasconsidered wasthatlisteners'judgments mightberelatedto thepresence versus absenceof closurevoicing.Closurevoicingcanbe measured eitherfromtime-or frequency-domain representations. Ona spectrogram, thedisappearance of formants aboveF 1 is typicallyusedto markthestartof theclosure inteval(e.g., Peterson and Lehiste,1960)?If a closureintervalis unvoiced, F 1andthehigherformants will generally terminate thesame time.Thisissimply another way judgments. There issis øevidence thattheremoval offinal atapproximately releaseburstsfrom voiceless stopshas a greatereffecton ofsaying thatthefirstformant or"voice bar"isnotusually voicingjudgments than the removalof burstsfrom final evidentduringtheclosure intervalforunvoiced stops.However,if a closureintervalis voiced,a low-intensity,low-frequencyfirstformantor "voicebar"will usuallyextendbe- voicedstops(Ma16cot,1958;Revoileet al., 1982).However, thegeneral conclusion thatfinalbursts fromvoiced stops do 22 J. Acoust.Soc. Am., Vol. 76, No. 1, July 1984 HJllenbrand etal.:Voiced-voiceless contrast 22 yondthetermination ofthehigher formants. Forthepresent studyclosure voicing wasdefined astheintervalbetween the offsetof the secondformant and the offsetof the first for- mant.This spectrum-based measurement technique was preferredto a time-domain methodbecause it allowedusto evaluate thepossibil/ty thatth• resolution oftemporalorder mightbeinvolvedin theperception offinalstopvoicingcontrasts.For example,it ispossible that a finalstopwill tendto soundvoiced//'theterminationoff I isdelayedby a significant amountrelativeto the offsetof the upperformants. Conversely, a finalstopmightsoundunvoiced ifF l andthe higherformantsterminateat aboutthe sametime. If th/s hypothesis iscorrect,thenstimulithat listenerstendto hear asvoicedshouldshowasynchronous offsets{i.e.,the terminationoff I isdelayedrelativeto theupperformants} while stimuli that listeners tend to hear as unvoiced should show synchronous offsets{i.e.,F l andthehigherformantsternflhateat roughlythe sametime}. Measurements were based on extraction of formant fre- quency,amplitude, andbandwidthbylinearpredictivecoding {LPC)analysis(Markeland Gray, 1976}.Theseparameters were extracted every 6.4 ms over 20-ms Hamming-windowed segments. The digital spectrograms producedwith thistechniquewereusedto measuretheoffset TABLE 111.Acousticmeasurements fromstimulionopposite sidesof the voiced-voiceless boundary forthesixcontinua. Thefirstcolumngivesthe percentage ofvoicedresponses thatweree/icitedbythestimulua. Thevalues /n parentheses areestimated difference limensfor decaytime(seetext). St/mulus labels indicate the continuum from which the token was drawn and thesizeof the editingcut. For all of the analyses, comparisons were made between the mo•t edited stimulus that was heard as voiced at least 75% of the time and the least edited sthnulus that was heard as voicelessat least 75% of the t/me. Percent Stimulus voiced responses F 1 Closure voicing offset frequency Decay time p•50 p•b80 93 25 26 0 226 395 48 (16} 20 l•d60 pr.d90 83 21 6 0 227 485 34 (13) 16 p•g60 p•g90 ' 89 14 0 0 3O5 486 29 (12} 16 pag60 87 0 334 49 (16) pag80 10 0 410 27 pig50 90 0 232 59 (18) pig70 20 o 249 4O pug50 ' pug70 82 23 13 0 159 182 73 (21} 54 time of the first formant relative to the offset time of the secondformant.This canbe thoughtof eitherasa measure, ment of the amountof voicingpresentduring the closure interval or as a measurement of relative offset time. The deci- sionregardingthe terminationof a formantwasbasedon a combinationof an increasein formantbandwidth,a decrease in formantlevel,andthepresence of abruptdiscontinuities withpreviously extractedformantfrequencies. (}rosserrors in formantextractionwereavoidedby checkingthe LPC derivedmeasurements againstconventionalspectrograms (Kay Sonagraph, model6061B}. F 1offsetfrequency wouldbeexpected regardless ofthelocationof theeditingcuts.Althoughtheappropriate psychophysicaldata are not available,it seemsunlikelythat F 1 offset differences of 17and23Hz couldberesponsible forthe largedifferences in labeling responses. ? The last parameterconsideredwas intensitydecay time.A computer programwasusedto measure overallrms intensityevery6.4 msover20-mstimewindows.Following the procedurerecommended by van den Broeckeand van The results of these measurements are shown in Table Heuven{1983},amplitudedecaytime wasexpressed as the III. 6Theresults donotsupport the'idea thatsubjects' voic- time neededfor signalintensity,expressedin decibels,to ingdecisions werebasedprimarilyon relativeoffsettimeor, drop from 90% of its peakvalueto 10% of its peakvalue. stateddifferently,on the presence versusabsence of closure TableIII showsamplitudedecaytimesfor stimulion oppovoicing.In mostcases F 1andF 2 terminhted simultaneously sitesidesof the voicingboundaryfor the sixcontinua.In all for stimuli on both sidesof the voiced-voicelessboundary. casesthe stimulusthat subjects tendedto hearasvoiceless Pastore(1983)hasshownthat thedifference limenfor offset showed a moreabruptdropin intensity. On theaverage, the asynchronies isabout7 msfor nonspeech stimuli300msin decaytimesfor stimulion the voicedsideof the boundary duration. ff thisisa reasonable estimate forspeech sounds, wereabout 1.7timeslongerthan thoseon the voiceless side. comparisons from only two of the sixcontinuawouldshow audible differences in relative offset time. The secondparameterthat wasmeasured wasthe terminatingfrequency of thefirstformant.Measurements off 1 offsetfrequencywere made usingthe LPC analysesdescribed above.TableIII givesF 1offsetfrequencies for stimuli on opposite sidesof thevoicingboundaryfor eachof the sixcontinua. In eachcomparisonF 1offsetfrequency ishigher for the stimuluson the voiceless sideof the boundary. However,theeffectismuchlargerforthevowelswithhighfrequencyfirst formants.F I offsetfrequencydifferences rangedfrom 76 to 258 Hz for stimuliin the environmentof /e/ and/a/ but wereonly 17Hz for/i/and 23 Hz for/u/. In fact, the first-formantcontoursfor the/pig/and/pug/ stimuliwereverynearlyflat;consequently, little variationin 23 J. Acoc•t. Soc. Am., Vol. ?6, No. 1, July 1984 Althoughth/seffectwasquiteconsistent, it is not immediatelyclearwhetherthesedecaytime differences areaudible. Van denBroeckeand van Heuven{1983)haveshownthat the difference !/men{DL) for decaytime canbe predicted quiteaccurately(r ----0.99}fromreference decayt/meby DL ----0.2t -I- 6.07, wheret is reference decayt/me.The decaytimedifferences measured from our stimuli exceeddifference limens e•timat- edfromthisformulaforall comparisons exceptforthestimuli drawn from the/pug/series.The differencein decaytime for thispair was 19 ms,comparedto an estimateddifference limenof 21 ms.For the/pig/continuum, however,the measureddecaytime differenceexceededthe estimatedDL by only .1ms. Hillenbrandoral.: Voiced-voicelesscontrast 23 IV. GENERAL DISCUSSION abruptdrop in amplitudethan the corresponding stimulus on the voiced side of the boundary. However, in some vowel To summarizebriefly,subjectswereaskedto identify environments the differences were quite small in relation to computer editedCVCsendingin stops. In general, subjects estimated DLs for decay time. Again, the/i/and/u/envitendedtochange fromhearingvoicedstopstohearingvoicelessstopswhentheclosure intervalanda portionofthevow- ronmentsshowedrelativelysmalldifferences. el-to-consonant transition had been removed. Removal of Taken as a whole, the acousticanalyseswere not as definitive as we had hoped: no singleacousticdimension the finalburstsdid not significantly alter the identification separated stimulion opposite sidesof the voicingboundary functions. Experiment1 showeda smalleffectfor placeof for all six continua. By itself this kindof outcomeshouldnot articulation.However,sincethiseffectwasnot replicatedin experiment 2, it isnotclearwhether theplaceofarticulation of thefinalstopisan importantfactor.Theeffectfor vowel environment wasstatistically significant, but it is not clear howthisfindingshouldbe interpreted. The two continua comeasa surprise. Manyinvestigators in thephonetic per- Basedon the measurements of closurevoicing,it doesnot sincetherewasno differencein voiceoffsettime for the/pig/ seriesandonly a 13-msdifferenceof the/pug/series. ceptionareaareaccustomed to thinkinglessin termsof unitary cuesto a contrastandmorein termsof tradingrelations amonga numberof cues(seeRepp,1982for a review).The that werebasedon vowelswith relativelylow first-formant resultsof thepresentstudy,however,do notappearto lend wellto thiskindof description. For example,it is frequencies --/i/ and/u/-- changed fromvoiced tovoice- themselves less 10-15 ms earlier than stimuli in the environment of the possibleto view abruptvowelterminationas a perceptual thatcanbecuedby a highF 1offsetfrequency, or twohighF 1vowels. Giventhepossibility thatF 1offset fre- dimension by a short decay time, or by some appropriate combination quencymightbea voicingcue,wehadanticipated a vowel of thesetwo properties.Recallthat the/pig/and/pug/concontexteffectin theopposite direction. Wereasoned thatif a in F 1 offsetfrequency. lowF 1 offsetfrequency wasa cueto voicing,an inherently tinua showedvery smalldifferences On the assumption that F 1 offset frequency anddecaytime low firstformantmightfavorvoicedresponses. The results trade against one another, one would expect to findthatthe didnotconfirmthisprediction: stimuliin thecontextof low lack of contrast in F 1 offset frequency was "compensated" F 1vowels showed slightly earliercrossovers fromvoiced to voiceless. It shouldbekeptin mind,however, thateachcon- by a relativelylargecontrastin decaytime.Thiswasclearly not the casesincethe differences in decaytime for the/pig/ tinuumwasbasedona singlenaturallyproduced token.For thatreason,whatappears to bea systematic vowelcontext and/pug/serieswereat bestbarelyaudible.The trade-off approach could,of course, beextended onestepfurtherby effectmightsimplyreflectidiosyncratic characteristics of that gradualversusabruptvoweltermination theparticular tokens. Wearecurrently designing a synthetic suggesting tradesagainstanotherdimension,suchas relativeoffset speech studyto examine voweleffects in moredetail. Acoustic analysisof stimuli straddlingthe voiced- time. A tradingrelationapproachwould suggestthat the voiceless boundaryprovidedfewdefinitiveanswers regard- relativelysmallcontrastin F 1 offsetfrequencyand decay by relativelylargedifferences in ingcuesto finalstopvoicingcontrasts. However,theresults timewouldbecompensated of these measurements do allow some tentative conclusions. relativeoffsettime. The datado not supportthishypothesis appearasthougha voicedclosureintervalis requiredfor subjects to reporthearinga voicedstop.This findingmay provideat leasta partialexplanation for the tendency of speakers to devoice relativelylargeamounts ofclosure intervals of syllablesending in phonologicallyvoiced stops (Smith,1979;HoganandRozsypal,1980;FlegeandBrown, 1982).It appears asthoughthetendency of speakers to devoiceis supported by perceptual strategies on the part of listenersthat do not rely on the presence versusabsence of closurevoicing. Acousticanalyses werealsousedto investigate thepossibilitythat subjects' voicingdecisions werebasedon the mannerof terminationof the precedingvowel. The two manifestations of gradualversusabruptvoweltermination that we investigated'were the offsetfrequencyof the first formantand amplitudedecaytime.Whenstimulion oppositesidesof thevoicingboundarywerecompared,largedifferences in F 1offsetfrequency wereseenonlyfor thestimuli with relativelyhigh-frequency firstformants.This suggests that a relativelyhighF 1 offsetfrequencyis not a necessary cueto theperception of a voiceless stop.It ispossible, however,that F l offsetfrequencyplaysa role in somevowel environments but not others.A similarpatternwasseenin thedecaytimemeasurements. Forallsixcontinua, thestimulus on the voicelessside of the boundaryshoweda more 24 J.Acoust. Soc.Am.,Vol.76,No.1,July1984 One additional issue that deserves attention is the rela- tionbetweenvoicingcuesfor initialstopsandthosefor final stops.It hasbeensuggested that the perception of voicing contrasts in initial stopsisbasedon the resolutionof relative onsettime (Pisoni,1977;seealsoMiller et al., 1976;Summerfield,1982;Hillenbrand,1984).Basedon the presentresuits, it does not seem likely that relative offset time is stronglyinvolvedin the perceptionof finalstopvoicingcontrasts.There were many instancesin which stimuli werelabeleddifferentlywithoutshowingdifferences in relativeoffset time. It is clear, however, that relative onset time is not the only cueto initial stopvoicingcontrasts.Differencesin the onsetfrequencyof the first formantalsoexerta strong influenceon initial stop voicingjudgments.In general,low F 1 onsetfrequenciestend to favor the perceptionof voiced stopsand highF 1 onsetfrequenciestendto favor the perception of voiceless stops(Lisker, 1975;Summerfieldand Haggard, 1977).The obviousfinal positionanalogof F 1 onset frequencywouldbeF 1offsetfrequency.Wolf hasspeculated that, "the amountof low-frequencyenergyin the vicinity of onsetor offsetmay serveasa commonbasisfor the perception of the voicingdistinctionin initial and final position" (1978,p. 299).Wolf's conclusions, however,werebasedlargelyon experimentsusingstopsfollowing/ae/, a vowelwith Hillenbrand etal.:Voiced-voiceless contrast 24 a high-frequency firstformant. Thepresent results suggest thatF 1oilsetfrequency differences mayplaylittleornorole whena stopfollowsa vowelwitha low-frequency firstformant. ACKNOWLEDGMENT Derbock,M. (1977)."An acousticcorrelateof the forceof articulation,"J. Phon. 5, 61-80. Derr, M. A., andMasaaro,D. M. (1980)."The contribution of vowelduration,F0 contour,andfricativedurationascuestothe/juz/-jus/distinction,"Percept.Psychophys. 27, 51-59. Fant,C. O. M. (1962)."Descriptive analysis of the acoustics of speech," LOGOS S, 3-17. Flanagan,J. L. (1955)."A difference limenforvowelformantfrequency," $. Thisworkwassupported by research fundsfrom NIH grantsT32NS07100-04and 5S05RR07028. Acoust. Soc. Am. 27, 613-617. Flege,J.,andBrown,W. S.(1982)."Thevoicingcontrastbetween English /p/and fo/as a functionof stressandposition-in-utterance," J. Phon. 10, 335-345. •Although voicing-dependent vowelduration differences havereceived a gooddealof attentionin theliterature,it is importantto emphasize that largeeffectsare seenprimarilyin phrase-final positionand in isolated worda.Thesedifferences are muchsmallerin nonphrase-final position (Umeda,1975;Klatt, 1973,1975,1976).Undercertainconditions these durational differences disappear altogether in connected speech (Crystal andHouse,1982). Fujimura,O., andLindqvist,J. (1971)."Sweep-tone measurements of vocaltract characteristics,"J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 49, 541-558. Hillenbrand,J. (1984}."Perceptionof sine-wave analogsof voiceonsettime stimuli," J. Acoust. SOc.Am. 75, 231-240. Hogan,$., and Rozsypal,A. (1980)."Evaluationof voweldurationasa cue for the voicingdistinctionin the followingword-finalconsonant," J. Acoust, SOc.Am. 67, 1764-1771. A. (1961)."On voweldurationin English,"J. Acoust.SOc.Am. 2Tosimplify thisreview, wearerestricting attention to voicing contrasts House, 1174-1178. involvingpostvocalic stopconsonants. Otherstudieshaveexaminedthe of cousonant enviroleofvoweldurationin finalfricativevoicing contrasts (e.g.,Denes,1955; House,A. S.,andFairbanks,G. (1953)."The influence ronmentupon the secondaryacousticalcharacteristics of vowels,"J. Derr andMassaro,1980,HoganandRozsypal,1980;PortandDalby, 1982;Massaro andCohen,1983)andofstops preceded bynonvocalic segments(Raphael etal., 1975). •It isverylikelythatvowel duration plays a roleintheperception offinal stopvoicingwhenothervoicingcuesareambiguous. A recentstudyby Wardrip-Fruin (1982b} showed thatvoweldurationbecomes animportant voicingcuewhenstimuliarepresented in noise. 4Noattempt wasmadetoseparate thesteady-state vowelfromthevowel-toconsonant transitions. The most'difficultsegmentation decision wasthe boundary between thevoweloffsetandthestartofthestopclosureinterval. Thisdecision wasbased ona dropinsignalintensity anda smoothing ofthe waveform, indicating thatmostoftheradiatedenergy isatornearthevoice •Attenuation isperhaps abetter wordthandisappearance. Sweep-tone measurements by Fujimuraand Lindqvist(1971}haveshownthat a second formantpeakisclearlypresent in thevocal-tract transfer functionduring stopocclusion (seealsoFant, 1962,p. 13).However,thelevelofF2 issome 10dBlowerthanF 1.Thisfact,incombination withthelow-frequency bias of theglottalsourcespectrum, meansthatonlythefirstformantis visible on a spectrogram duringthe periodof time corresponding to articulatory occlusion. Srhemeasurements ofclosure voicing giveninTableIII willnotagree with valuesobtained simplyby subtracting thenominalamountof "voicingremoved"fromtheuneditedclosuredurationsreportedin TableI. Thereare tworeasons forthisdiscrepancy. First,asindicated in Sec.I A thenominal stimulus values referonlyto thezero-amplitude portionof theweighting functions thatwereusedintheeditingprocedure. Sincethenominalstimulusvaluesdo not takeintoaccountthe 15-mslineardecayfunction,these values willunderestimate theamount ofclosure voicing thatwasanenuat-' ed.Second,the closurevoicingmeasurements givenin TableI werebased onosciHograms whilethosein TableIII werebasedon LPC deriveddigital spectrograms. (Reasons for switchingto the frequency-domain technique are explainedin Sec.IlI.) Thesetwo methodsshouldproducesimilarbut Acoust.So:. Am. 25, 105-113. Klatt, D. H. (1973)."Interactionbetweentwo factorsthatinfluence vowel duration,"J. Acoust.So:. Am. 54, 1102-1104. Klatt, D. H. (1975)."Vowel lengtheningis syntacticallydeterminedin a connecteddiscourse,"J. Phon. 3, 129-140. Klatt, D. H. (1976)."Linguisticusesof segmental durationin English: Acousticandperceptual evidence," J. Acoust.Soc.Am. 59, 1208-1221. Lisker, L. (1975I. "Is it VOT or a first-formanttransitiondetector?,"$. Acoust. Soc. Am. 57, 1547-1561. Lisker,L., andAbramson, A. S.(1964}."A cross-language studyof voicing in initial stops:Acousticalmeasurements," Word 20, 384-422. Lisker, L., and Abramson,A. S. (1967)."Someeffectsof contexton voice onsettimein Englishstops,"Lang.Speech10, 1-28. Lisker,L., andAbramson,A. S.(1970)."The voicingdimension: Someexperimentsin comparativephonetics,"in Proceedings of the6th InternationalCongress of Phonetic Sciences, Prague,1967(Academia,Prague), pp. 563-567. Mal6cot,A. (1958)."Theroleof releases in theidentification of released final stops,"Lang.34, 370-880. Mallcot, A. (1970)."The lenis-fortis oppostion: Its physiological parameters,"J. Acoust. SOc.Am. 47, 1588-1592. Markel,J.,andGray,A. (1976}.LinearPrediction ofSpeech{Springer-Verlag, New York). Massam,D. M., and Cogen,M. M. (1983)."Consonant-vowel ratio:An improbable cuein speech," Percept.Psychophys. 33, 501-505. Mermelstein,P. (1978)."Differencelimensfor formant frequencies of steady-state andconsonant-bound vowels,"J. Acoust.Soc.Am. 63, 572580. Miller, $. D., Wier, C. C., Pastore,R. E., Kelly, W. J., andDooling,R. J. (1976)."Discrimination andlabelingof noise-buzz sequences with varyingnoise-lead times:An exampleof categorical perception," J. Acoust. SOc.Am. 60, 410-417. O'Kane,D. (1978I. "Mannerof vowelterminationasa perceptual cueto the voicingstatusof post-vocalic stopconsonants," J. Phon.6, 311-318. ?Since dataarenotavilableon thediscrimination of F 1 offsetfrequency Parker,F. (1974)."The coarticulation of vowelsandstopconsonants," J. differences, speculations about audibilitymust rely on differencelimen Phon. 2, 211-221. data usingstimuliwith stationaryformants.The smallestestimateof the R.E.(i983). "'remporal order judgment ofauditory stimulus offdifference limenfor first-formant frequency is 15Hz, basedon a reference Pastore, set," Percept. Psychophys. 33, 54-62. sigualwith a 300-Hz F 1 (Flanagnn,1955).Mermelstein(1978),however, Peterson, G., andLehiste,I. (1960)."Durationof syllablenucleiin Engusedsimilarstimuliandfounda 50*Hz difference limenfor F 1.Evenusing lish," J. Acoust.SOc.Am. 32, 693-703. thesmallerestimate provided by Flanagan, the 17-and23-Hzdifferences Pisoni,D. B. (1977)."Identificationanddiscrimination of therelativeonset measuredfrom our stimuli would bejust detectablein a same/different not necessarilyidentical results. timeof two-component tones:Implications for voicingperception in task. stops,"J. Acoust.SOc.Am. 61, 1352-1361. Port,R. F., andDalby,F. (1982)."Consonant/vowel ratioasa cuefor voic- Chen,M. (1970)."Vowellengthvariationasa functionof thevoicingof the consonant environment," Phonefica 22, 129-159. Crystal,T. H., and House,A. S. (1982)."Segmentdurationsin connected speech signals: Preliminaryresults,"J. Aconst.Soc..•m. 72, 705-716. Denes,P. (195S)."Effectof durationon the perceptionof voicing,"J. Acoust. SOc.Am. 27, 761-764. 25 J. Acoust.Soc. Am., Vol. 76, No. 1, July 1984 ingin English,"Percept.Psychophys. 32, 141-152. Raphael,L. J. (1972)."Preceding voweldurationasa cueto theperception ofthevoicing characteristic of word-final consonants in AmedcanEnglish," J. Acoust. So:. Am. 51, 1296-1303. Raphael,L. J.(1981)."Durations andcontexts ascuestoword-final cognate oppositionin English,"Phouetica•1, 126-147. Raphael,L. J.,Dorman,M., Freeman,F., andTobin,C. (1975)."Voweland Hillonbrandeta/.: Voiced-voicelesscontrast 25 nasaldurationascuesto voicingin word-finalstopconsonants: Spectrographicand perceptualstudies,"J. SpeechHear. Res.18, 389-400. Raphael,L. J., Dotman,M, F., andLiberman,A.M. {1980)."On denning thevoweldurationthat cuesvoicingin finalposition,"Lang.Speech23, sants,"J. Acoust. Soc.Am. 62, 435-448. Umeda,N. (1•F/5)."Voweldurationin AmericanEnglish,"I. Acoust. Am. S8, 4?. 297-307. Repp,B. H. (1982)."Phonetictradingrelationsandcontexteffects: New evidencefor a phoneticmodeof perception,"Psychol.Bull. 92, 81-110. Revoile,S., Pickett,J. M., Holden, L. D., and Talkin, D. (1982)."Acoustic cuesto finalstopvoicingfor impaired-andnormal-hearing listeners," •. Acoust. Soc. Am. 72, 1145-1154. Rositzke,H. A. (1943)."Thearticulationof finalstopsin generalAmerican speech,"Am. Speech18, 32-42. Smith,B. L. (1•/9). "A phoneticanalysisof consonant devoicingin children'sspeech,"I. Child Lang.6, 19-28. Summerfield, Q. (1982}."Differences between spectral dependencies in auditoryandphonetic temporal processing: Relevance totheperception of voicing in initialstops," •. Acoust.Soc.Am. 72,51-61. 26 Summerfield, Q., andHazard, M.P. (19•7)."On thedissocintion of spectral and temporalcuesto the voicingdistinctionin initial stopconson- J. ^coust. Soc. Am., Vol. 76, No. 1, July 1984 van denBroecke,M.P. R., and van Heuven,V. J. (19•3). "Effectand artifactin theauditorydiscrimination of riseanddecaytime," Percept.Psychophys.33, 305-313. Walsh,T., and Parker,F. (1981}."Vowelterrain*lionasa erieto voicingin post-vocalic stops,"J. Phon.9, 105-108. Wardrip-Fruin,C. (1982s)."On thestatusof temporalcuesto phoneticca- tegories: Preceding voweldurationasa cueto voicingin finalstopconsonants,"J. Acoust. Soc.Am. 71, 187-195. Wardrip-Fruin,C. (1982b)."Final stopvoicing:Vowel durationthe primarycuei•noise•"paperpresented at theAmericanSpeech andHearing Association Convention, Toronto, November, 1982. Wolf,C. (1•/81."Voicingcuesin Englishfinalstops,"•. Phon.6, 299-309. Hillonbrandetal.: Voiced-voicelesscontrast 26