0306-4522/83/040791-07$33.00/O Pergamon Press Ltd 0 1983IBRO NeuroscienceVol. 8, No. 4, pp. 791 to 797, 1983 Printed in Great Britain TEMPORAL CONTIGUITY REQUIREMENTS FOR LONG-TERM ASSOCIATIVE POTENTIATION/DEPRESSION IN THE HIPPOCAMPUS W. B. LEVY and 0. STEWARD Department of Neurosurgery, University of Virginia School of Medicine, Charlottesville, VA 22908, U.S.A. Abstract-A previous study utilizing the powerful ipsilateral and weak crossed projection from the entorhinal cortex to the dentate gyrus in the rat revealed that long-term potentiation is an associative process in these systems. If the weak crossed projection alone receives potentiating stimulation consisting of 8 high-frequency trains 17.5 ms in duration, it does not exhibit long-term potentiation. However, long-term potentiation can be induced in the crossed projection if it is activated concurrently with the converging ipsilateral system. The present study is designed to determine the degree of synchrony required for the associative interactions by varying the timing and order of the potentiating trains delivered to the two converging systems. The associative induction of long-term potentiation does not require perfectly synchronous activation of the converging systems. The order of the trains is crucial, however. Long-term potentiation of the crossed projection can be induced if activity in the ipsilateral system is concurrent with or follows activity in the crossed projection. Indeed, there can be as much as 20 ms between the 17.5 ms trains in the two systems, and long-term potentiation of the crossed projection is still induced. Activation of the ipsilateral system that precedes activation of the crossed system depresses the responses evoked by the crossed system. If potentiating stimulation of the ipsilateral system follows activation of the crossed projection by too long an interval (200 ms, for example), then the crossed projection is depressed rather than potentiated. These results are discussed with regard to the nature of the associative process permissive for the induction of long-term potentiation and lead us to the conclusion that perfect temporal contiguity is not a requirement of this prototypical elemental memory unit. It is widely held that some persistent synaptic modification is the elemental basis of memory storage. The most popular hypothesis is Hebb’s suggestion’ which forms the central postulate of a variety of associative memory models. Hebb proposed that correlated pre- and postsynaptic activity leads to alteration of synaptic strength. More specifically, he proposed that an increase in synaptic efficacy occurs with repeated, concurrent activation of a relatively ineffective presynaptic element and the postsynaptic cell upon which this element terminates. Since most cells presumably have their discharge controlled by a population of presynaptic inputs, this postulate can be transformed into a requirement for co-activity between the relatively ineffective synapse and a number of other presynaptic elements whose convergent and synchronous activity exerts a major control upon postsynaptic cell firing. Thus, the hallmark of a ‘Hebb synapse’ is modifiability as a consequence of co-activity among converging presynaptic elements. Address all correspondence to: Dr. W. B. Levy, Department of Neurosurgery, Box 420, University of Virginia School of Medicine, Charlottesville, VA 22908, U.S.A. Abbreviations: DG, dentate gyrus; EC, entorhinal cortex; EPSP, excitatory postsynaptic potential; LTP, longterm potentiation of synaptic transmission. Until very recently, the Hebb synapse was a theoretical construct with no experimental verification. However, the synapses of entorhinal cortex (EC) upon the granule cells of the dentate gyrus (DG) possess features of a Hebb synapse and serve as a prototype for the class of synapses that could efficiently subserve associative memory storage.6 Three properties of EC-DG synapses distinguish them as prototypical elemental synaptic memory units: (1) a capacity for long-term increases in synaptic efficacy (LTP) which can last for days;‘** (2) an associative requirement for induction of LTP, that is, potentiation is a function of the number of active, converging presynaptic elements$* and (3) a capacity for long-term decreases of synaptic strength.6*1 3 These three properties are particularly well revealed using the sparse crossed EC projection to the DG as an investigative model6 In the normal rat, activation of the crossed EC to DG projection results in small responses.“*r3 Conditioning stimulation of the crossed EC-DG pathway alone induces no measurable potentiation (an insufficient number of synapses are concurrently active6). However, when this pathway is conditioned ‘concurrently’ with conditioning of a convergent and powerful excitatory pathway (the ipsilateral EC projection), the crossed response poten791 79’ W. B. Levy and 0. Steward tiates.6 On the other hand, this same conditioning of the ipsilateral system depresses the crossed pathway unless that crossed pathway is conditioned concurrently. The present study quantitatively defines the temporal limits of the co-activity requirement for potentiation. Specifically, we were interested in the question: how synchronous need be the conditioning coactivity in the converging ipsilateral and contralateral pathways to induce associative potentiation in the contralateral pathway? A priori, since conditioning the ipsilateral EC-DG pathway both potentiates and depresses the contralateral input as a function of convergent contralateral activity during ipsilateral conditioning, we viewed conditioning of the ipsilateral pathway as permissive for synaptic changes. The expectation was that a ‘permissive’ period following ipsilateral conditioning would persist for several milliseconds. Thus, we anticipated LTP of the contralateral pathway when the ipsilateral pathway was conditioned immediately preceding conditioning of the contralateral pathway, but expected no potentiation when conditioning of the contralateral pathway preceded that of the ipsilateral. This report shows that, in fact, exactly the opposite results were obtained. EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES Experimental animals were 250-300 g male rats of either albino or hooded variety obtained from Flow or Blue Spruce Labs. Although both albino and hooded rats were analyzed, the data which make up the illustrations come from hooded animals, since we have found that the \pars~ crossed ECDG projection is somewhat larger in these :rnmals than in albinos, producing larger and more c;rs~l!analyzed evoked responses. The general electrophysiological methods have been described previously.6,‘2 The animals were acutely anesthrtized with chloralose-urethane (55 mg/kg and 0.245 g/kg. respectively). Stimulating and recording electrodes were positioned as diagrammatically illustrated in Fig. I. The bipolar stimulating electrodes were positioned bilaterally in the angular bundle (the fiber tract carrying EC projections to the dentate gyrus). The final positions of the electrodes were based on their electrophysiological effectiveness (site of the optimal stimulation at minimal stimulus intensity). Current source density analysis performed on animals not used in this study showed that stimulating electrodes positioned by this method and/or at this depth are activating the proximal synapses of the entorhinal projection in the dentate gyrus. In other words, this position in the angular bundle is similar to positioning stimulating electrodes in the medial entorhinal cortex. Recording electrodes were positioned in the ventral blade of the dentate gyrus to record optimally the negative extracellular population excitatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP). in the molecular layer evoked by the crossed projection, or the positive reflection of the population EPSP at the somatic level. Following the experiment, the recording electrodes were re-adjusted to ensure that they had not shifted from the optimal recording site during the course of the experiment. Experimental procedure. To monitor synaptic efficacies, a single ‘test’ stimulus was delivered once every 30s to the contralateral stimulating electrode. Stimulus intensity was adjusted so that a near maximal extracellular EPSP was evoked with a minimum of polysynaptic activity (which occurs at a longer latency than the monosynaptic wave). rf?ec(L) Test Cond. I:+++++&--- SR-FiectL 0 - L- EC EC Fig. 1. The stimulation and recording set-up is illustrated diagrammatically. A single recording electrode (RecL) samples evoked responses generated by the crossed projection from the mtorhinal cortex (EC) on the right hand side (indicated by the heavy line). Stimulating ekctrodes are positioned on both sides. The conditioning contingencies which we have previously described6 are indicated on the right. Conditioning stimulation delivered to the right EC alone induces no (0) potentiation of the crossed projection from that side. Conditioning stimulation of the left EC induces dcpreasion of the responses evoked over the crossed projection from the right hand side. Activation of both sides simultaneously induces associative potentiation of the crossed projections. 1 Temporal contiguity requirements for potentiation Once a stable baseline had been established, the conditioning stimulation was begun. In all cases, stimulus intensity during conditioning stimulation was the same as that utilized for testing. The ordering of the stimulus trains delivered to the two stimulating electrodes varied in different experiments and within the same experiment. A special advantage provided by our system is the ability to ‘erase’ potentiation induced in a crossed pathway, thus permitting repetitive testing of specific conditioning configurations. After delivery of the conditioning stimulation, the synaptic efficacy was again monitored by resuming the test stimulation. A minimum of seven minutes passed between different conditioning configurations. In this study, conditioning stimuli were trains of eight pulses, delivered at 400 Hz and repeated 8 times at 10s intervals. The experimental variable investigated was the temporal relationship of individual trains delivered to the two stimulating electrodes. (For explanatory purposes, we will consider a single train of the ipsilateral pathway to be fixed in time, and we will vary the time of the contralateral conditioning stimulus.) In our previous study, the contralateral conditioning stimulation was delivered nearly simultaneously (kO.l-1.0 ms) with the ipsilateral conditioning stimulation. In the present study, contralateral conditioning stimulation sometimes precedes and sometimes follows the ipsilateral conditioning stimulation. When the contralateral conditioning stimulation precedes the ipsilateral conditioning stimulation, the time between trains will be the time between the last pulse of the contralateral train and the first pulse of the ipsilateral train. When the ipsilateral conditioning stimulation precedes the contralateral conditioning stimulation, the time between conditioning trains will be measured as the time between the last ipsilateral stimulation pulse and the first contralateral stimulation pulse. Each ‘bout’ of conditioning stimulation at the appropriate contingency and the following series of test pulses is termed a conditioning trial. The test periods after each conditioning bout lasted at least 5 min. For data analysis, responses were recorded on a 4 channel FM tape recorder and were subsequently analyzed offline with a PDP 11-04 computer. The digitized evoked responses were quantified by measuring either the amplitude of the negative extracellular EPSP or by measuring the initial slope of the negative wave. These two measures are highly correlated and reveal quite comparable changes induced by potentiation,” although the amplitude measure exhibits somewhat less variability than slope measures when very small responses are analyzed. RESULTS The responses evoked by the ipsilateral and sparse crossed EC-DG projection systems have been extensively described in previous studies.6*7~10~11 At the re- *While the normal crossed projection of albino rats does not exhibit LTP, the somewhat larger crossed projection of hooded animals is occasionally sufficiently powerful to generate limited LTP even when stimulated alone (unpublished observations). This potentiation does not complicate the present analysis since it is slight in comparison to the potentiation elicited by co-conditioning with the ipsilateral system. 793 sponse maximum, the population EPSPs generated by the crossed system range between 0.5 and 1.2 mV, with hooded rats exhibiting larger maximal crossed response amplitudes than albino rats. These responses are not accompanied by detectable population spikes. The converging ipsilateral projection system is much more powerful; the maximal population EPSPs are typically greater than 8 mV and, at the response maximum, the population EPSPs always evoke large (several mV) population spikes. The normal crossed projection of albino rats does not exhibit LTP when conditioned alone (see footnote*).6*l2 Our normal procedure for inducing potentiation of the crossed projection uses a paired conditioning paradigm in which the stimulus trains to the powerful ipsilateral and weak crossed projections overlap, beginning and ending within 1 ms of each other. Thus, the initial depolarization evoked by each pathway, as well as each subsequent depolarization, would occur roughly concurrently (within 1 ms). To delimit how close in time the stimulus trains must come, five animals were analyzed. In two animals, initial conditioning trials had the train to the contralateral afferents follow the train to the ipsilateral afferents. On subsequent trials, the ordering was reversed. In three other animals, initial trials had the contralaterally-delivered train preceding that of the ipsilateral side, and again, the ordering was reversed on later trials. The second of these paradigms is illustrated in Fig. 2. In each contingency, the train delivered to the crossed pathway (upper trace) is illustrated as being held constant, while the order and timing of the train to the ipsilateral system (lower trace) is varied. (Note that all figures present, to the right side, the relative increase of decrease in response amplitude, not the actual response amplitude. Because each successive conditioning procedure induces long-term potentiation or depression, changes are calculated relative to the most immediate sequence of preceding responses. Thus, values less than 100% indicate that the conditioning train, shown to the left, depressed the response while values greater than 100% indicate the conditioning train potentiated the response.) In this paradigm, the first three conditioning trials had the ipsilateral train follow the contralateral. As is evident, the contingency produces potentiation at the 20ms inter-train interval. Successive conditioning using progressively shorter inter-train intervals produces further increases. The lower traces illustrate an unanticipated phenomenon. When the ipsilateral conditioning precedes the contralateral, depression results. For inducing this depression, the inter-train interval did not appear critical. Long-term depression invariably occurred so long as the contralateral train began after the end of the ipsilateral train. Repetition of depression-inducing contingencies resulted in progressive decreases toward some apparently non-zero asymptotic level (traces 4-6 of Fig. 2). When this asymptotic level was reached, further depressioninducing conditioning resulted in no further relative 794 W. B. Levy and 0. Steward change with respect to preceding responses (see next to last trace in Fig. 2). In the 5 animals analyzed, the effects of the order of the trains were consistent. In the case of the optimal ordering for inducing potentiation (contra followed by ipsi) the maximal inter-train interval at which associative potentiation was observed varied from animal to animal. One animal exhibited slight and statistically insignificant potentiation with an intertrain interval of 40ms. Two others (one of which is illustrated in Fig. 2) exhibited potentiation with an inter-train interval of 20ms. The remaining two animals exhibited potentiation only when the inter-train interval was decreased to 10 ms and 5 ms, respect- corldii~ ively. In general, repeating a conditionmg contingency which had just previously been delivered resulted in only slight increases in response amplitude. C‘ompared to repetitive conditioning with long inter-train intervals, decreasing inter-train intervals more effectively induced further potentiation. Long-term depression was consistently observed both before and after the crossed response had been potentiated. confirming our previous observations that the converging ipsilateral system can decrease the response amplitude of a ‘control’ crossed projection.6 As noted above, the depression resulting from ipsilateral preceding contralateral stimulation was surprising, and was exactly opposite to our expectations. cotltingemcies Relative Fbspome Anrplskrde 100% 125’. 150% 1 I I Righl -Lett ‘17 ,* R-L 1ms R _tttttttt____ - L*: L-R ame L-R 1 ms L+R St imulete Right. Rmrd Left Fig. 2. The proper temporal ordering for long-term potentiation. This Figure shows that the contralateral conditioning stimulation must precede (R --+ L) or be synchronized (L + R) with the ipsilateral conditioning stimulation in order to potentiate the contralateral response. When the order of conditioning trains is reversed (L -+ R), depression results. On the left side of the figure are two traces associated with each data bar of the right side of the figure. These traces indicate the conditioning protocol used two minutes prior to the response indicated by the associated bar on the right. Each vertical line indicates the delivery of a single conditioning pulse. In the case of this experiment the contralateral stimulating electrode is on the right side of the animal (R = right; L = left). The recording electrode is on the left side of the animal. The responses plotted on the right half of the figure are from averages taken over a 5 min time period and are expressed as a percentage of the 5 min average response amplitude immediately preceding the indicated conditioning protocol. Thus, a response of lOOoJ,indicates no change after the indicated conditioning procedure, while a value lower than lao”/, indicates depression and a value greater than 100% indicates the associated procedure produces potmtiation. Test stimuli are delivered l/30 s; conditioning consists of 8 of the indicated train pairs at 1 train per 10 s. The data are from a single animal. The net changes following R -+ L 8 and 1 ms are significantly larger relative to the initial baseline. The net changes aher L + R 20, 8 and 1 ms are signi!icantly smaller compared to the baseline established just prior to the L -+ R 20 ms conditioning procedure. The last potentiation is significant. All significance values are at P < 0.05, two-tailed t-tests. 795 Temporal contiguity requirements for potentiation Relative Response Amplitude 100% 200% 3ftO% Condiiioning Contingencies t-20mr Riiht -wHHH_r I t 112’ 450 ms S 450 ms I Left R-luHw L R-L 5ms R-L 200ms R-L 5ms R-L 5ms R+L Stimulate Right, Record Left Fig. 3. An upper time limit exists for the potentiating effects of a properly ordered contralateral plus ipsilateral paired conditioning stimulation. When the contralateral conditioning train precedes the ipsilateral train by as little as 200 ms (R -+ L, 200 ms), depression results. As before, shorter intervals (in one case here, 5 ms) provide an adequately small interval of separation for the paired conditioning stimulation to potentiate the contralateral response. Details of this figure are the same as in Fig. 2 except that each response average, taken 2 min after the associated conditioning train, was collected for a period of 10 min. The data are from a single animal. The net depression after the first two conditioning stimulations is significant compared to the initial baseline. The changes following the next three conditioning stimulations are significant relative to the immediately preceding baseline period. The responses following the last two conditioning stimulations are significantly different relative to the baseline period preceding R -+ L 5 ms. Significance accepted if P < 0.05 on a two-tailed t-test. These results were reminiscent, however, of the situation found in classical conditioning paradigms. By analogy with classical conditioning’s conditioned stimulus and unconditioned stimulus, the weak contralateral stimulation is a very poor excitatory stimulus (an analog of the conditioned stimulus) for the postsynaptic cells, while the ipsilateral stimulus is very effective (an analog of the unconditioned stimulus). With pairing in which the contralateral stimulus precedes the ipsilateral stimulus (conditioned stimulus-unconditioned stimulus), the contralateral gains some of the properties of the ipsilateral stimulus, i.e. it becomes more powerful. The question to answer then was how far this interval could be extended. From the preceding experiments, it seemed that 20ms was about the upper limit for effective pairing. However, to be sure that we were absolutely outside the effective interval, it should be possible to increase the interval so that the contralateral and ipsilateral conditioning stimulations inter- act not at all. It would be as if there were no contralateral stimulation. Recall that when just the ipsilateral system is conditioned, long-term depression results. Thus, if no interaction occurred, one would anticipate depression with long inter-train intervals. In these experiments four new animals were used, and a variety of pairing intervals between 180 and 5OOms were tried. In all cases, profound depression occurred with these long intervals. Figure 3 gives an example of a typical experiment. First, this experiment shows that a 50ms inter-train interval causes depression of the contralateral response. When the delay is shortened to 5 ms, potentiation is obtained. Next a long interval is again used (200ms) and depression occurs. Following this second conditioned depression, potentiation is again demonstrated with short inter-train intervals. These results were significant for changes observed within each individual experiment (P < 0.05; two-tailed t-test using each test response as a sampling). 796 W. B. Levv and 0. Steward DISCUSSION The results have implications about the mechanisms of long-term potentiation and long-term depression and also suggest how potentiationjdepression could contribute to some information storage capabilities. Considering the mechanisms first, the results provide additional support for the proposition that some process is set into motion by conditioning stimulation of the powerfully depolarizing ipsilateral system that leads to modification of the synaptic efficacy of the crossed pathway, depending on the state of the crossed pathway. If the crossed pathway is active during a critical temporal window, potentiation occurs. If the crossed pathway is inactive, or is active at a time outside the critical temporal window, depression occurs. Based on the results here, the critical temporal window exists simultaneous with and slightly before the ‘enabling’ conditioning stimulation. The critical feature here is that both potentiation and depression of the crossed system require the same event (conditioning of the converging ipsilateral system), suggesting that some single process is set into motion by this stimulation which makes the synapses of the crossed pathway modifiable. The direction of modification, i.e. potentiation or depression, of the crossed projection (or any other weak input) thus is determined by its own activity. We have argued previously that the most likely site for associative interactions such as those described above is the point at which the two pathways converge (the postsynaptic cell or some portion thereof). Regardless of whether the critical signal is cell discharge, as Hebb reasoned, or simply a massive local dendritic depolarization, the present results place several restrictions upon any postulated chemical reaction which may regulate synaptic modifiability. First, the reaction triggered by strong activation must differ from the reaction triggered by weak activation, since the converging ipsilateral system must be strongly activated to induce either associative potentiation or depression of the crossed system.6 This implies that activation of individual spine synapses is not sufficient, and suggests that perhaps there is some ‘retrograde’ interaction between a process initiated within the main dendritic shaft and individual spines. This dendritic process would begin when a sufficient number of spines were depolarized concurrently. That these processes eventually ‘feed back’ to regulate individual synapses is suggested by both our previous neurophysiological work and by ultrastructural experiments (W. B. Levy & N. L. Desmond, unpublished)3 which suggest rather dramatic alterations of the synaptic interface (i.e. a larger postsynaptic receptive surface). The present results, by defining the temporal constraints of the postulated dendritic process, suggest that there is not enough time for a chemical product formed in the main dendrite to diffuse or be trans- ported to the spine head (e.g. 1 pm in 20 ms) \I here the ultimate reaction occurs. Perhaps a polymerization reaction of some supersaturated substance could move fast enough, but more appealing is ;L retrograde electrical invasion of the spine structure (e.g. a Ca2+ spike). However, in line with the preceding logic, such an electrical event would have to be distinctive from the electrical event associated with activation of the spine synapse by itself. Recall that a very weak, high-frequency conditioning stimulation does not potentiate itself. *.12 Yet considering the high resistance of a spine stem, this stimulation should drive the spine head of an active synapse nearly to the reversal potential of the synapse. Again referring to the principles discussed in the preceding paragraph, the retrograde signal from the dendrite and the signal generated at each specific synapse should be different to ensure the synapse by synapse specificity of associative potentiation/depression. Distinctive critical events could be accumulation of ions (Na+ vs Ca2+) or, in place of one of these ions, a molecule or moiety released by the associated presynaptic terminal of each synapse (e.g. a derivative of glutamate, ATP, or a polypeptide, etc.). Our results also suggest how the processes of potentiation/depression might be used for information storage. As pointed out previously,6 the associative potentiation/depression properties of the synapses currently being studied are not sufficient for mediating classical conditioning by themselves. Additional circuitry is required because the depressioninducing paradigm is not analogous to the extinction paradigm of classical conditioning. The present results indicate that even more circuitry is required if this synapse stores classically conditioned memories. Specifically, in trace conditioning the optimal interval between the conditioned and unconditioned stimuli is a few hundred ms4,9 and even longer intervals are sufficient. Yet paired conditioning at this optimal interval produced depression at the synaptic level. Therefore, some type of delay circuitry is necessary (such as a ‘reverberating’ loop that allows a response to circulate around the limbic system several times). This loop would maintain the activity of a conditioned stimulus during trace conditioning so that, at the cellular level, the conditioned and unconditioned stimuli could temporally converge upon synapses and cells mediating associative potentiation. Acknowledgements-This work was supported in part by NIH Grant No. ROl NS15488 to W.B.L. and NSF Grant No. BNS80-21865 to OS. O.S. is a recipient of Research Career Development Award No. NSO0325. The data analysis program utilized in the present study was developed by R. C. Wilson (Wilson, Levy & Steward, 1981). We would like to extend special thanks to Dr. L. Gray for his help in developing this program and for other suggestions whieh contributed substantially to our off-line data analysis. Temporal contiguity requirements for potentiation 797 REFERENCES 1. BLISST. V. P. & GARDNER-MEDWIN A. R. (1973) Long-lasting potentiation of transmission in the dentate area of the unanaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. J. Physiol., Land. 232, 357-374. 2. BLISST. V. P. & LIMO T. (1973) Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. J. Physiol., Lond. 232, 334-356. 3. DESMOND N. L. & LEVYW. B. (1981) Ultrastructural and numerical alterations in dendritic spines as a consequence of long-term potentiation. Anat. Rec. 199, 68A-69A. 4. FREY P. W. & Ross L. E. (1968) Classical conditioning of the rabbit eyelid response as a function of interstimulus interval. J. camp. Physiol. Psychol. 65, 246250. 5. HEBBD. 0. (1949) Organization of Behavior. John Wiley & Sons, New York. 6. LEVYW. B. & STEWARD0. (1979) Synapses as associative memory elements in the hippocampal formation. Brain Res. 175, 233-245. 7. LQMOT. (1971) Patterns of activation in a monosynaptic cortical pathway: the perforant path input to the dentate area of the hippocampal formation. Expl Brain Res. 12, 1845. 8. MCNAUGHTONB. L., DOUGLASR. M. & GODDARDG. V. (1978) Synaptic enhancement in fascia dentata: cooperativity among coactive afferents. Brain Res. 157, 277-293. 9. SCHNEIDERMAN N. (1966) Interstimulus interval function of the nictitating membrane response of the rabbit under delay us trace conditioning. .I. camp. Physiol. Psychol. 62, 397-402. 10. STEWARDO., COTMANC. W. & LYNCHG. (1976) A quantitative autoradiographic and electrophysiological study of the reinnervation of the dentate gyrus by the contralateral entorhinal cortex following ipsilateral entorhinal lesions. Brain Res. 114, 181-200. Il. WHITEW. F., GOLD~WITZD., LYNCHG. & COTMANC. W. (1976) Electrophysiological analysis of the projection from the contralateral entorhinal cortex to the dentate gyrus in normal rats. Brain Res. 114, 201-209. 12. WILKIN R. C., LEVYW. B. & STEWARD0. (1979) Functional effects of lesion-induced plasticity: long-term potentiation in normal and lesion-induced temporodentate connections. Brain Res. 176, 65-73. 13. WILSON R. C., LEVY W. B. & STEWARD 0. (1981) Changes in the translation of synaptic excitation to dentate granule cell discharge accompanying long-term potentiation-II. An evaluation of mechanisms utilizing the dentate gyrus dually innervated by surviving ipsilateral and sprouted crossed temporodentate inputs. J. Neurophysiol. 46, 339-355. (Accepted 15 September 1982)