Neumpsychology 1997,\iol.11,No.3,428-436 C-opyright1997by theAmericanPsychologicalAssociation,Inc. 089214105D7/$3.00 Examinationof Age-Related Deficitson theWisconsinCardSortingTest NathanaelM. Fristoe and Timothy A. Salthouse John L. Woodard EmoryUniversitySchoolof Medicine Georgia Institute of Technoiogy Adult age differences in wisconsin card Sorting Test (wcST) measures were examined before and after statistical confiol of age-related differences in measures of feedback usage, working memory, and perceptual-comparison speed. The proportion of age-related variance associated with a summary measure of wCST performance was greatly reduced after controlling for measures of feedback usage, working memory and perceptual-comparison speed.Furthermore, the age-related variance associated with the feedback-usage measure was reduced after controlling for working memory and perceptual-comparison speed measures. These results are consistent with the idea that age-related performance differences in the WCST are partially mediated by adult age differences in feedback usage and that age differences in feedback usage are mediated by age differences in working memory which are in turn mediated by age-related reductions in processing speed, indexed by measures of perceptual-comparison speed. This study was designed to analyze which cognitive processesmediate age-related differences in performance on a widely used neuropsychologicaltest, the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST; Grant & Berg, 1948; Heaton, Chelune, Talley, Kay, & Curtiss, 1993). Correlational analyseswere used, so that performance differences on a given task could be interpreted in terms of the relations among potential mediating variables and a specific criterion measure. When applied to the analysis of age-related performance differences on neuropsychological tasks, conelational analyses can provide valuable information regarding the contribution an assessmentmeasuremakes toward describing potentially clinically significant age-related performance differences (see Salthouse& Fristoe, 1995). This approach focuses on determining which aspectsof task performance are mediated by age-related differences in presumably simpler tasks. The WCST is a concept-identification task designed to measrue abstraction and planning abilities, as well as the tendency to perseverate to a given response pattern. Originally developed by Grant and Berg (1948), the current WCST consists of four stimulus cards and 128 response cards. The task is to sort the response cards according to color, form, and number. The examiner is allowed to give NathanaelM. Fristoe and Timothy A. Salthouse,School of Psychology,GeorgiaInstitute of Technology;John L. Woodard, Deparunentof Neurology,Emory University Schoolof Medicine. Portionsof this researchwere presentedat the 1996 Cognitive Aging Conference,Atlanta, Georgi4 April 1996; and the l04th Annual Conventionof the American PsychologicalAssociation, Toronto, Ontario, Canada,August 1996. This researchwas supportedby NationalInstituteon Aging TrainingGrantAG00I 75 and Grant6826.We wouldlike to acknowledge thevaluableassistance of S. H. Rheein datamanagementandJ. Thomasin recruiting and testingparticipants. Correspondence concerningthis article shouldbe addressedto NathanaelM. Fristoe,School of Psychology,GeorgiaInstitute of Technology,Atlanta,Georgia30332-0170.Electronicmail may be sentvia Internetto gt4360b@acme.gatech.edu. only positive or negative feedback after the examinee's placementof eachresponsecard beneathone of the stimulus cards. On the basis of this information, the participant must derive the correct sorting principle. After 10 consecutive correctly sorted cards,the sorting category changeswithout warning from color, to form, to number, and then to each category again in the same order. Measuresof performance on the WCST have been shown to be sensitive both to brain damage (Drewe, 1974; Milner, 1963; Robinson, Heaton, Lehman, & Stilson, 1980) and to effects associated with increased age (Anderson, Damasio, Jones, & Tranel, 1991; Axelrod & Henry, 1992; Crockett, Blisker, Hururitz, & Kozak, 1986; Daigneault, Braun, & Whitaker, 1992. Heaton et al., 1993; Libon et aI., 1994; Nelson, 1976; Parkin & Walter, 1991). The brain-damaged groups tested in several of these studies showed difEculty shifting from one category to another, and the same perseverative tendency has also been observed in older adults. For example, Axelrod and Henry (1992) tested 20 adults in each offour age groups (SGyearolds, 60-year-olds, 70-year-olds, and 80-year-olds) on the WCST and found significant age-related increases in the number ofperseverative errors and the number ofperseverative responses, as well as sigaificant age-related decreasesin the number of categories achieved. However, they found no significant age differences in total correct responsesor in the number ofnonperseverative errors. Daigneault et al. (1992) replicated the results of Axelrod and Henry (1992) in a comparison of the performance of adults age 20-35 years (n :70) with those age 4545 years (n : 58), finding significant age-related increases in the number ofperserverative errors and perseverative responsesand decreasesin the number of categories achieved. Boone, Ghaffarian, lpsser, Hill-Gutierrez, and Berman (1993) observed a somewhat different pattern of age-related deficits in WCST performance. These researchers tested a sample of 91 men and women ranging in age from 45 years to 83 years and examined the WCST number of categories, total errors, 428 429 AGE-RELATED DEFICITS ON THE WCST perseverative responses,trials to first category, percentageof perseverative errors, percentage of conceptual-level responses,and failure-to-maintain-set measures.They found age-related deficits only in the number of errors and in the percentage of conceptual-level response measures. With a sample of 259 adults between 18 and 94 years of age, Salthouse,Fristoe, and Rhee (1996) found significant age effects for every WCST measlue, with the exception of number of trials to first category. An exploratory factor analysis on the measures from the WCST revealed that despite initial variations in the relations of specific measures to age, two factors accounted for a sizable proportion of variance (R2: .661 and .114 for Factors 1 and 2, respectively) and were moderately correlated with age (r : .36 and .29 for Factors I and 2, respectively). This high degree of intercorrelation among the WCST measuressuggeststhat all WCST measures may be assessingsimilar aspects of performance, at least with respect to age-related influences. However, the question of which cognitive processeszue most important for successful WCST performance and which processes might be differentially sensitive to agerelated effects remains unanswered. One possible source of the age-related deficits on the WCST is the failure of older adults to induce tre correct sorting category on the basis of the available feedback. That is, the "right-wrong" feedback provided by the administrator of the WCST may not be remembered or may not be interpreted by the older adult in a way that facilitates the selection of the appropriate response. Effective use of feedback information should result in a win-stay and "win" l.ose-shifi response pattern. That is, after positive feedback, one should retain or sray with a hypothesis, and after negative "lose" feedback, one should change or shifi hypotheses.If older adults are not using feedback information effectively, then they should display significantly less win-stay and lose-shift behavior than young adults. The WCST failure-to-maintain-set performance measure can be viewed as a gross measureof feedback usage in that it is a record of nonoptimal win-shift behavior, but because an ocqurence is noted only after a break from five or more conect sorts, it provides no information about win-shift behavior for each item. Apotentially more sensitive measure of effective usage of feedback could be derived from the number of times a participant stayed with a hypothesis after positive feedback (win-stay) and shifts from a hypothesis after negative feedback (lose-shift). Using this approach, Offenbach (1974) examined the win-stay and lose-shift behavior of older adults on a concept-identification task similar to the WCST. Inspired by the work of Levine (1963) on the use of feedback information in a discrimination learning paradigm, the task developed by Offenbach required individuals to choose between two altematives differing along several dimensions and then to select from eight possible hypotheses the hypothesis on which they had based their previous decision. After these two choices, the experimenter provided the participant with feedback indicating whether his or her response had been correct or incorrect based on the response criterion in effect. Offenbach found that after positive feedback for a hypoth- esis, young adults were very likcly !o rE?eatthat h1'pothesis on subsequent choices (proportion of rcspoascs: -8&a). whereas older adults were as likely o usc a diffcreot hypothesis as they were to use the previous hypotbcsis (proportion of responses: .495). $imil4'ly, aftcr ocgmvc feedback, the young adults were unlikely to use thc prevkrrs hypothesis (proportion of responses: .W4), whercas tbc older adults were significantly more likely to do so (pr,oportionof responses: .2(X)). Offenbach (1974) interpreted these findings as indicating that older adults had less reliance on memory for previous outcomes than young adults. In fact, using a similar pandigm, Kellogg (1983) found that young adults were morc accurate than older adults in recalling their previousl;- urcd hypotheses. Because effective use of feedback dcpcnds on working memory ability, in that previously prescotcd information must be retained to correctly process neu i-nfornration, age-relateddeclines in effectivenessof fecdbrck usage might be related to age-related declines in working mcmory capacity. To summarize, the results of Offenbrh (1974) and Kellogg (1983) suggestthat older aduls oftco farl ro use the feedback information provided in conccpr-i&otification tasks such as the WCST, and it is sut€Estcd thar these failures may be related to working mdtor) limiarions. Furthermore, on the basis of the resuls of Saltbou-sc( I 99l , 1996) and others, it is possible thar maal- age-rclated working memory limitations may bc mcdi.tcd b1 dccreases in speedof processing.Salthouse(l996) prryccd that even on untimed tasks such as the WCST. slo*s processing speed may adversely affect perforrnancc. For rnsunce, slower processing speed may result in a dcrreasc rn the amount of information that is simultancously rtivc. *'hich may correspondto less working memory capacir). This study was designed to investigarc which frtors might be responsiblefor the age-relateddifucaccs in !*'CST performance. The primary hypothesis was that many of the age-relatedperformancedeficits obaervedon ttr WCST cqrld be accountedfor by failures of two rclated factqs: (a) working memory deficits and O) reduced speed of procassing. If these factors are important, then controlling for measuresof these hypothesized mediators with statistical procedures will reduce the magnitude of the age-related effects on thc measures of WCST performance. We also predicted that age-related variation in measures of effectiveness of fecdback usage would be substantially attenuated after control of working-memory measures and ttrat the age-related vuiation in measures of working memory would be significantly attenuated after control of measuresof speed of processing. Method Participants Young and older adults were recruitedthroughadvefiiscrmts postedin a largesoutheastern city. All participans were compcosatedwith $20 for their participationin a single sessionlasting between 1.5 and 2 hr. The young adult group consistedof 48 persons,ranglng from 18 to 38 yearsof age,and dre older adult groupconsistedof 49 persons,rangingfrom 6Oto 86 yearsof age. 430 FRISTOE, SAIJTI{OUSE,AND WOODARD stimulus "cards" were continuously present in the upper portion of a color monitor. For each trial, a single response card was presented in the lower half ofthe screen. To sort the response cards below the stimulus cards, the participant presseda number key between I and 4 (these numbers corresponded to the four stimulus cards in left-to-right order). ff the pairing matched the sorting category in effect, a brief, high-pitched tone was presented, and the word nrcrrr appeared in the lower left corner of the screen. If the pairing was Design and Procedure incorrect, a low-pitched tone was presented, and the word wnoNc The WCST was administeredon computers,with a program appeared in the lower left corner of the screen. The previous developed by Woodard (1994). In addition to this computerresponse remained in view while the participant made a new administeredversion of the WCST, a special version was develresponse.A total of 128 responsecards were presented, with the opedandadministered. Thishypothesis-generation examinee controlling the pace of presentation. WCST version required participants to indicate on what basis they intended to The computer-administered WCST with hypothesis generation (i.e.,accordingto number,form,or maketheirsubsequent response was similar to the standard computer-administered WCSI with color).This hypothesisinformationwasusedto assess theprobabilone important modification. Before each response, the participant ity of a changein hypothesisafter negativeor positive feedback was required to indicate on what basis he or she intended to make (win-stayandlose-shiftbehavior). his or her subsequentresponse, according to color, form, or number The computer-administered versionof the WCSTuseddisplays (by pressing either C, R or N on the keyboard). This information of stimuluscards identical to those in the traditional version. We was used in the analyses to determine the proportion of trials for administeredit as describedby Heatonet al. (1993).The four which participants changed their hypothesis after either negative or positive feedback. For both WCST versions, the number of perseverative errors, the Table I percentage of perseverative errors, the number of conceptual-level Demographic Characteristics of Research Participants responses,the percentage of conceptual-level responses, and the number of categories achieved were selected as measures of Young Older Group performance. These particular measru€swere chosen because they Variable adults adults difference are widely used (e.9., Axelrod & Henry, 1992; Ragland, Gur, N 48 49 Deutsch, Censits, & Gur, 1995) and are considered to be most Gender X 2 ( l , , l V : 9 7 ) = 1 . O 9 sensitiveto cognitive flexibility (e.g.,Ozonoff, 1995). Vo'ttO'tr'€n 75 65 Computer-administered numerical and verbal working memory Vomen 25 35 tasks were included to assessthe relationship between failure to use Age feedback information and working memory limitations. ComputerM 26.7 70.1 ized digit-digit and digit-symbol comparison tasks and several SD 5.7 7.2 Education (e5) = -1.68 paper-and-pencil speed measures were also included, to test M 13.3 r3.9 mediational hypotheses involving the role of processing speed in SD 1.3 2.0 determining age-related differences in WCST performance. Mea= Health satisfaction t(93) 2.62* sures of sensorimotor speed involved primarily a motor rcsponse M 2.5 2.la output component (e.g., rapidly drawing lines in designated SD 0.8 0.7 locations), whereas perceptual-comparison speed measures in(9s) = 1.43 Health rating volved an additional comparison of two or more stimuli (e.g., M 2.1 1.9 comparison of two strings of letters with a judgment of similarity). SD 0.8 0.7 Health-related activity All participants performed the same batt€ry of tasks in the same limitations t(93)= -.29 order. Everyone began by filling out a health questionnaire, M 1.7 1.7" followed by several paper-and-pencil tests, the computer-adminisSD 1.0 1.0 tered WCST, the computer-administered digitdgit and digitCardiovascular surgery t(93) = -237 symbol reaction time tasks, the computer-administered WCST with M 0.0 0.1" hypothesis generation, and the verbal and numerical working SD 0.0 0.3 memory tests. Only the working memory tasls were administered Hypertension medications t(93): -4.44* M in counterbalanced order: verbal, numerical, numerical again, then 0.04 0.4" SD o.2 0.5 verbal. The WCST conditions were not counterbalanced becauseof Head injury t(93) = r.67 the concern that administering the standard WCST after the M 0.1 0.02" hypothesis-generation WCST would bias performance in the SD 0.3 0.r standard condition. Neurological treatrnent t(92): 1.42 The paper-and-pencil tests consisted of the boxes, letterM 0.1 0.04" comparison, pattern-comparison, digit-copying, and synonym and SD 0.3 0.2 antonym vocabulary tests used by Salthouse (1993). With the Note. Age is in years,educationis self-reportednumberofyears exception of the vocabulary tests, these tasls each contained an of formal education completed,and health satisfactionand health instruction page with several examples, followed by two pages of rating are self-ratings on a scale ranglng fuom | (excellcnt) to 5 test items. (poor). Participants indicated yes (l) or zo (0) for reports of The stimuli in the boxes task consisted of three sides of an health-relatedactivity limitations,cardiovascularsurgeryhypertenincomplete square. For each three-sided box, the participant was to sion medications,headinjury, and neurologicalteafrnent. ",Oneor more missing observations. draw a line forming a fourth side, to make a squzre. The boxes test *P = .ol. spannedtwo pages, with lO0 boxes to a page. There was a 30-s Very few participants in either group were enrolled as full-time students at the time of testing. Demographic characteristics of the research participants are reported in Table 1. Note that significant group differences were found only for self-reported indications of health satisfaction and the reported use of hypertension medications. AGE-RELATEDDEFICITS ON TTIE WCST tirne limit per page, and the boxes score was the average number of items completed acrossthe two pages. lrtter-comparison items consisted of 2l pain of three, six, or nine letters. Participants were to write an S (for same) if both members of the pair were the same or a D (for different) if they were different. One half of the letter pairs were different becauseof a difference in the identity of one letter in the pair member. The letter-comparison score was the number of correct pairs minus the number of incorrect pairs completed in 30 s. Tlvo separately timed pages of the letter-comparison items were administered. Items in the pattern-comparison task were 30 pairs of line segments composed of either three, six, or nine segments. The research participant was to write an S (for same) between the two pattems if they were identical or aD (for different) if they were not. One half of the pairs differed because of a shift in the position of one line segment in a pair member. The score was the number of patterns correctly completed minus the number incorrectly completed in 30 s. T\vo separately timed pages of the patterncomparison task were administercd. The digit-copying task consisted of 10O pairs of boxes, one on top of the other, with a digit in the upper box and a blank lower box. The participant was to copy the upper digit in the blank lower box. T\vo pages of the digit-copyrng task were adminisEred The digitcqymg scorewas tre number of items completed in a 3Gs period. Vocabulary questions were taken from those used by Salthouse (1993). Both antonym and synonym vocabulary tests contained l0 five-alternative multiple-choice questions. Tlvo minutes were allowed for each test, and the score was the number of correct answerson each test. The boxes and digifcopying tests have been hypothesized to represent the construct of sensorimotor speed because they both involve perception of the stimuli followed by a simple motor response. The letter-comparison and pattern-comparison tests are thought to represent a perceptual-comparison speed construct (Salthouse,1994) becauseboth tasks require not only perceptionof the stimuli and a simple motor response but also an additional judgment of physical identity. The digit-symbol reaction time test was a modification of the Wechsler Digit Symbol Substitution Test (see Salthouse, 1992), designed to assessperceptual comparison speed. Eighteen practice trials were followed by a block of 90 test trials. The digitdgit test was also used by Salthouse (1992) and was designed to assess sensorimotor speed. Eighteen practice trials were followed by a block of 90 test trids. For both the digit-digit and digit-symbol tests, individual trial latencies and accuracy percentages were recorded and summarized with measures of median reaction time and percentage correct. Because accuracy rates for both the young adults and older adults were greater than95%, only reaction times are reported. Both the numerical and verbal working memory tasks were computer administered, as described by Salthouse and Coon (1994). Briefly, the numerical working memory task presented a simple two-term arithmetic problem (e.g., "5 * 2 = ?") in the upper portion of the computer screen. The participant indicated the correct answer by using the up and down arrow keys to position a large arrow adjacentto one of three alternative answers(e.g., ..5," "7," or "9") presented beneath the problem. Consecutive sequences of two or more problems were presented, followed by a request for the participant to recall the last digit from the previously presentedarithrneticproblems.For the nurnerical worrkingrnemorytask to continue, paticipants were required to be oonect on both the arithrnetic and memmy taskson two of trc ftrce tials at a given length. For the verbal working memory task, a sentence was presented in the upper portion of the computer screen (e.g., "Tom and Mary went to the party last night"), and a brief question was asked about 431 the sentence(e.g., "Where did they go?"). Again, the participant indicated the correct answer by using the up and down arrow keys to position a large arrow beside one of three answers (e.g., "school," "shopping," or "party") presentedbeneaththe question. After two or more sentences,the participant was asked to recall the last word from each sentence. The test was discontinued when tlre participant could not recall the words from two sentence sequences of the same length. Ttvo blocks of each working memory task were administered, each with different items. For both the verbal and numerical working memory tasks, a participant's span was determined by the longest sequenceof correctly recalled stimuli, with the requirement of correct processing of the arithmetic or comprehension questions on at least two of the three trials. Results Because of the large number of comparisons made in theseanalyses,a significance criterion of ct : .01 was used. Given the numerous variables not relevant for specific sets of comparisons (i.e., demographic variables), adoption of this more conservative alpha level seemed preferable to more drastic familywise error rate corrections. Table 2 contains means, standard deviations, and results of independent t tests for age group differences on the paper-and-pencil measures,the computer-administercd digitdigit and digit-symbol tests, and the computer-administered verbal and numerical working memory measures.Missing values on the vocabulary measures are attributable to the first 10 participants not receiving these tests, due to a procedural error. It can be seen that the mean differences between the two groups were significantly different from zero for all measures. Age trends were as expected, with older adults scoring lower than young adults on the speed and working memory measures but strowing sipificantly beUer performancethan young adults on the vocabultry measures. Composite variables were formed by averaging the sample z scores of different sets of measures to represent the constructs of working memory (verbal and numerical working memory span scores; r : .52), perceptual-comparison speed (pattern- and letter-comparison meursures;r: .65), and sensorimotorspeed(digit-copying and boxes measures; r : .69). For each composite, reliabilities were estimated by means of the Spearman-Brown formula, and the split-half correlations between composites were formed from the first and second administrations of each measure. These reliability estimateswere .89 for working memory, .89 for perceptual-comparisonspeed, and .97 for sensorimotorspeed. Measuresof WCST perfornance consistedof the number of categories achieved, the number of conceptual level responses,the percentageof conceptual-levelresponses,the number of perseverative erors, and the percentage of perseverative errors. Means and standard deviations for these five WCST performance measures are reported in Table 3. To evaluate a possible Age Group X WCST Version interaction, we conducted an Age Group X WCST Version multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) on the data summarized in Table 3. The number of conceptual-level responsesand number of perseverative errors measures werle 432 FRISTOE.SAIJITIOUSE. AND WOODARD Table2 Meansand Snndard Deviationsfor Speed,Vocabulary,and WorkingMemory Measures Youngadults(z : 48) M Box completion Digit copying Patterncomparison Lettercomparison Antonymvocabulary Synonymvocabulary Digit-digit Digit-symbol Workingmemoryspan Verbal Numerical Older adults (n : 49) SD 54.7 55.6 17.6 10.4 2.1 3.2 712 1,330 11.4 9.1 3.0 2.6 1.6 2.2 182.6 2N.7 46.9 46.4 12.6 6.8 3.7 5.4 881 1,806 I1.5 9.0 3.4 3.0 3.2 3.2 137.6 429.8 2.5 3.8 1.3 2.1 1.8 2.O 1.3 2.2 t(95)= t(95) : t(95) = t(95)= t(85) = 3.39 5.s5 7.66 6.21 -2.97 (8s;= -r.ut (es;= -t.tt t(95)= -6.72 t(95) = 2.75 t(95) = 4.96 Note. For the vocabulary measures, n : 42 young adults and 45 older adults. The digit-digit and digit-symbol testswere measuredin milliseconds. All t values were significant atp < .01. not included in this MANOVA because of their redundancy with the correspondingpercent measures.The main effects for both age group, F(3,93) : 9.97,p < .001, and WCST version, F(3, 93) : 16.14,p < .001, were significant,but the Age Group X WCSTVersion interaction was not significant, F(3,93) : 2.5O,p = .M.Univariate F tests,reportedin Table 3, revealed significant age differences for all measures. Furthermore, the proportion of age-related variance associated with each WCST measure was still significantly greater than zero after statistical confrol of variables representing years of education, self-rated health satisfaction, reported cardiovascular problems, reported neurological treatrnent, reported use of hypertension medications, and reported loss of consciousness.The only exception to this trend was the number-of-conceptual-level-responses measure from the standard WCST. For this measure, the age-related proportion of variance was no longer significant after control of either the reported neurological treafnent or hypertension medicationsvariables. We then conducted a principal-components analysis on 6 of the 10 measures listed in Table 3. The number of conceptual-level responsesand the number of perseverative errors were excluded becauseoftheir redundancy with the corresponding percent measwes. The first principal compo- nent explained 74.9Vo of the variance in the relevant variables and was significantly correlated with age (r : -.52). The factor loadings on this component were negative for the WCST perseverative errors mqrsure (a measure of poor WCST performance) and positive for the WCST measures related to good performance (i.e., number of categories and percentage conceptual-level responses). The first principal component was therefore used as the index of card-sorting performance in subsequent analyses, with a high score representing better performance. Note that this measure is an aggregate index of several aspects of performance in the two versions of the WCST administered in this study and does not dircctly correspond to any single measure derived from the WCST. Variables representing effectiveness of feedback usage were derived from the participant's hypothesis choices made before each response in the hypothesis-generation WCST. For optimum performance, a hypothesis would be retained after positive feedback (win-stay) and discarded after negative feedback (lose-shift). For example, if a participant chose the colorhypothesis and then matched a response card with two red stars to the stimulus card with one red triangle, the match might have been followed by a brief, high-pitched presented. The positive feedback tone with the word RJGITT Table 3 Means and Standard Deviations for WCST Measures Youngadults(n = 48) Older adults(n = 49) Measure StandardWCST No. categories No. conceptual-levelresponses 7oconcepfual-level responses No. perseverative errors % perseverative errors HypothesisWCST No. categories No. conceptual-levelresponses 7oconceptualJevel responses No. perseverative errors 70perseverative erors 4.8 65.5 61.3 16.4 t4.l 1.9 17.5 18.7 10.0 7.1 3.2 55.2 47.2 252 20.2 1.0 ll.0 12.6 5.4 3.7 3.7 60.1 55.2 20.3 16.6 SD F(1,es) 2.0 20.3 20.7 t2.l 8.8 16.04 7.14 12.22 t5.37 13.95 2.1 18.8 21.7 13.0 9.4 Note. WCST : WisconsinCardSortingTest.All F valueswere significant atp < .Ol. 5.6 70.9 77.1 9.1 9.2 31.39 I1.78 36.71 30.01 25.49 433 AGE-RELATED DEFICITS ON TIIE WCST produced by this response would encourage the pa*icipant to retain the color hypothesis (i.e., win-stay) rather than switching to eifher form or numben On the other hand, if in the preceding example, the participant had received negative feedback for the response, lose-shift behavior would dictate that a new hypothesis (e.g.,form or number) be selectedon the next sort. Individuals not using feedback information appropriately would display nonoptimal win-stay and loseshift behavior. An analysis of win-stay and lose-shift behavior is only meaningful if both young and older adult participants show consistency between reported hypotheses and card sorts (e.g., after reporting a hypothesis of form, the participant matches the card on the basis of form). Young adults showed a mean percentage consistency between reported hypothesis and actualresponseof94%o(SD : 6.5Vo),whereasthe older adults were consistent,on average, for 85Vo(SD: l5.7Vo) of their responses. A t test indicated that the age group difference in hypothesis-responseconsistency was significant, t(95) : 3.65, but because the mean consistency between reported hypothesis and acnral response was high for both goups, the probability of repeating a hypothesis after positive or negative feedback was calculated. The probability of repeating a hypothesis after positive feedback (mean percentage win-stay) was significantly higher for the young adults (M : .9'13,SD : .055) than for the older adults (M : .914, SD : .ll7), r(95) : 3.18. After negative feedback, the probability of staying with a hypothesis (perccntage lose-stay) was significantly lower for young adults (M: .268, SD: .157) than for older adults (M : .378, SD: .248), t(95) : -2.60. The correlation between percentage win-stay and percentage lose-shift was low and not significantly different from zero (r = .13), perhaps because of the small variance in the percentage win-stay measure relative to that in the percentage loseshift measure. Because both the percentage lose-stay and percentrge win-shift measures are indicative of poor use of feedback information, we formed a composite feedbackusagevariable by averaging the z scoresofthese measures. We conducted a series of hierarchical regression analyses using the first principal component as the criterion variable. Table 4 contains the results. Initially, only the proportion of variance accounted for by age was calculated, then the age-related variance was examined after controlling for the feedback-usage index, working memory and speed of processing. Controlling for the feedback-usage index resulted in a 77.4Vo attenuation of the age-related variance (i.e., from .266 to .060), and controlling for measures of working memory resulted in a 45.5Vo attenuation of the age-related variance (i.e., from .266 to .145). Simultaneously controlling for both the working memory index and the feedback-usage index resulted in a greater attenuation (85.OVo)of the age-related variance than when confiolling for either measure in isolation. Additional analyses revealed that the overall pattem of results was similar when individual measures of WCST performance were used as the criterion variables. From the results presented in Table 4, it can be seen that large attenuations in age-related variance were found, with Table 4 Results of Hierarchical Regression Arnlyses With First Principal Component From the WCST Measures as the CriterionVariable Predictor Age Age-feedback Age-WM Age-Dig-Dig Age-Dig-Sym Age-MSPD Age-PSPD Age-MSPD,PSPD Age-feedback,WM Age-feedback,WM, PSPD Age R2 9*" .266 .060 .145 .t6l .058 .238 .045 .057 .040 .022 -.516 -.267 *.427 -.461 -.314 -.558 -.294 -.337 -.235 -.2t3 I Note. WCST = Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; Feedback= feedbact-usageindex; WM : working memoryindex;Dig-Dig = digit-digit; Dig-Sym = digit-symbol; MSPD : sensorimotor speedindex; PSPD = perceptual-comparison speedindex. All valuesexceptthosefor age-feedback,WM, PSPDweresignificant atp=.01. control ofthe variablesrepresentingthe constructofpercep tual-comparison speed(the digit-symbol and the perceptualspeed index). However, large attenuations in age-related variance were also found with individual and combined statistical contol of the working memory index, the feedbackusage index, and the perceptual-comparison speed index (e.g., controlling for all three resulted in a 9l.7%oattenuation). This result is consistent with the hypothesis that age-related variation in WCST performance is partially mediated by feedback-usage and working memory components. The results reported in Table 4 also show that controlling for the sensorimotor speed indexes resultcd in little additional attenuation of age-related variance in the first principal component, and if anything, the sensorimotor speedindex actedas a suppressorvariable. We also conductedhierarchical regressionanalyses,using the feedback-usage index as the criterion variable. Table 5 contains the results of these analyses, focusing on the attenuations in the age-related variance after controlling for working memory and speed measures. Control of working memory perceptual-comparisonspeed,or digit-symbol speed resulted in large attenuations of the age-related proporTable 5 Results of Hierarchical Regression Arwlyses Wth Feedback Composite Measure as the Criterion Variable Predictor Age R2 P.r. Age .156* .395* Age-WM .o77* .311* Age-MSPD .096* .354* Age-PSPD .006 .103 Age-Dig-Dig .o52 .261 Age-Dig-Sym .019 .180 Age-MSPD,PSPD .006 .108 Age-WM, PSPD .003 .o75 Note. WM = working memory index; Dig-Dig : digit-digit; Dig-Sym : diglt-symbol; MSPD : sensorimotorspeedindex; PSPD : perceptual-comparison speedindex. *p' .ol. I I 434 FRISTOE, SALT'HOUSE,AND WOODARD tion of variance. These results are consistent with the proposed hypothesis that age-related variations in feedback_ usage_measuresare partially mediated by age_relatedvaria_ tions in working memory, which are d turn mediated by processing-speedmeasures. A similar seriesofhierarchical regressionanalyses,using the working_memory index as the criterion variable, ii reported in Thble 6. This set of analyses shows that the age-related variance in the working memory index can be substantially attenuated by controlling ftr perceptual_ comparison speed measures (controlling for the perceptual_ comparison speed index resulted in i g+Eo attenuition), which is consistentwith many other reports (e.g., Salthousi, 19.91,.1996).Support is therefore p.ouia"d ioi tt" hypoth_ esis that much of the age-related variation i., -o.ting memory is mediated through age reductions in speed Figure 1. Path analysis model of the relations among age, speed oT . -cari-*rtiog processing. of p^rocessing,working memory feedback usage, and performance. To summarize the relations among the variables that are suggestedby the hierarchical regressionanalysesreported in Tables 4 tlrough 6, a path analysis was conducted using the Discussion perceptual-comparison speed index, the working ."iory index, the feedback-usageindex, the first principh "o-po_ Variousresearchershave suggestedthat successfulWCST nent from the WCST measlues, and age. These variables performance is dependent on stimulus discrimination pro_ represented,in order, the constructsof speedofprocessing, cesses @errine, 1993), monitoring of feedback information working_memory feedback usage, and card_sorting perfoi_ (Cicerone, Lazar, & Shapiro, 1983), and working memory mimce. This path analysis was conducted with the I,iS-nff- S and reactive flexibility (parkin, Walter, & Hunkin, 1995j. (Jtireskog & Siirbom, 1993) program, beginning The main contribution of this study is the finding that mucir with the fully saturated model and then successivJy deleting paths of the age-related variance in the WCST [.fo.man"e with coefficients less than2 standard"rror, fro- zero. The measures could be accounted for through statistical control path diagram is illustrated in Figure l, with the numbers of measuresof feedback usage,working memory span, and next to the arrows corresponding to standardized path coeffi_ perceptual-comparison speed. The pattern of corrilational cients. results reported in Tables 4 through 6 implies that although Age-related effects on the use of feedback were entirelv use of feedback information and working memory were important factors in determining age-relatJd differences in lediated through the speed variable. Speed was indepen'_ dentlyrelated only to fe-edbackusageand working -"-ory; WCST performance, these age-related differences were the independent path of speed to Card-sorting p".for-un"" largely predictable from more basic speed-of-processing was not significant. Like the results summarizedin Tables 4 factors. One concern that should be addressedbefore discussing {rorg! 6, the path coefficients reported in Figure I indicate that although independent age-relited effects Jn card_sorting the primary findings is the question of whether the resulti performance exist, a large proportion of the age_related from this computer-administered WCST version are compa_ rable to what might occur with the standard WCST versibn. effects are mediated by the feed6ack-usage variable, which In fact, effect sizes for the computer-administered WCST in turn is mediatedby the speed-of-processingvariable. measures in this study were similar to those found bv Salthouse et al. (1996), who used the standard pup", "-i version of the WCST. For the same age groups used in the Table 6 present study, Salthouse et d. (1996) found effect sizes Resultsof Hierarchical Regression Analyses Wth the (n : 161)of .76, .37, .79, .Bl, and .g0for the agedifferences Working Memary Index as thc Criterion Variable in number of categories, number of concep-tual_level re_ sponses,percentageof conceptual_levelresponses,number Predictor Age R2 9o" of perseverative errors, and percentage oi perseverative Age -.451* .203* errors, respectively. The present study (N : 97) found effect Age-MSPD -.429* .141* sizesfor theseage differencesof .t6, .53, .67, .74, and .71. Age-PSPD -.249 .032 respectively. Age-Dig-Dig -.367* .102* Age-Dig-Sym An additional similarity between Salthouseet al.,s (1996) -.201 .024 Age-MSPD, PSPD -.256 .033 s{dy. an! the present study was that a single factor (the first principal component) accounted for moJt of the variance Note. Dig-Dig = digit-digit; Dig-Sym = digit_symbol; MSpD = . sensonmotor speed index; pSpD = perceptual_comparison t:, the present srudy and 66.lVo for Salthouseet al., speed 9^Y 1996) in the selectedWCST performance measures.Like the *p = .01. results of the exploratory factor analysis performed by AGE.RELATED DEFICITS ON TI{E WCST Salthouseet al. (1996), this result suggeststhat the WCST performance measures are assessing similar aspects of age-related performance differences and justifies the use of the first principal component as a composite measure of WCST performance. Another important result of this study is the discovery that older adults fail to shift hypotheses to the same extent as young adults after negative or positive feedback. This representsa replication of Offenbach's (1974) findings, on a concept-identification task with known clinical value, and suggeststhat one factor contributing to poorer performance of older adults on the WCST is less effective processingof feedback information. Most important, the results from the hierarchical regression analyses and path analysis are consistent with the proposed hypotheses regarding the relation between effective use of feedback and WCST performance. That is, statistically controlling for age-related differences in the use of feedbackinformation resulted in a large (77 .47o)attenuation in the age-related variation in card-sorting performance. Similarly, the hypotheses regarding the dependency of effective feedback usage on working memory capacity and speed-of-processing factors were supported by the finding that age-related variation in effectiveness of feedback usage was largely accounted for by measures of working memory (a 50.67o attenuation, controlling only for the working memory index) and speed of processing (a 96.2Voattenuation, controlling only for the perceptual-comparison speed index). Furthermore, age-related variations in working memory were largely accounted for by measuresof speed of processing (an 84.2Voattenuation, conrolling for the percep tual-comparisonspeedindex). Thesehierarchical regressionanalysessuggestthat across all criterion variables, speed-of-processing measwes may be functioning as the primary mediators of performance. Speedof-processing measures accounted for most of the agerelated variance in card-sorting performance, the feedbackusage index, and working memory measures. When taking into consideration performance on speeded tasks, agerelated performance differences on an untimed task (i.e., the WCST) were substantially reduced. Salthouse (1996) proposed that age-related declines in speedof processing can lead to impaired cognitive function because of both limited-time and simultaneitv mechanisms. Slower processing speed results in fewer operations being successively executed (limited-time mechanism), and the results from earlier processing may not be available when later processing is complete (simultaneity mechanism). Becausethe present study shows that WCST performance is related to both working memory and feedback usage, and both of these components may be mediated by speed-ofprocessing measures, both limited-time and simultaneity mechanisms could be involved in degrading WCST performance. For older adults, deficits in limited-time mechanisms may result in incomplete encoding of the WCST stimuli, whereas deficits in simultaneity mechanisms may make feedback results of earlier WCST card sorts unavailable by the time encoding of currentWCST stimuli is complete. The results ofthe present study are consistent with this interpre- 435 tation, in that controlling for the slower perceptualcomparison speed of older adults substantially attenuated age-related differences in a card-sorting performance measure. If speed-of-processingmeasuresdo not play a role in untimed tasks such as the WCST, then one would certainly not expect speed measures to be predictive of level of performance. References Anderson,S. W., Damasio,H., Jones,R. D., & Tranel,D. (1991). WisconsinCardSortingTestperformanceasa measureof frontal damage.Journal of Clinical and ExperimentalNeurcpsychology,13,9W-922. Axelrod,B. N., & Henry,R. R. (1992).Age-related performance on the WisconsinCard Sorting Test, Similarities, and Controlled Oral Word AssociationTests.Zfte Clinical Neurcpsychologist, 6(r),rG26. Boone,K.8., Ghatrarian,S., lrsser, I. M., Hill-Gutienez,E., & Berman,N. G. (1993).WisconsinCardSortingTestperformance in healthy,older adults:Relationshipto age,sex,education,and IQ. Joumal of Clinical Psyclrology,49(l), 54-{i0. Cicerone,K. D., Lazar,R. M., & Shapiro,W. R. (1983).Effectsof frontal lobe lesions on hypothesis sampling during concept formation.Neuropsychologia2I, 513-524. Crockett,D., Blisker,D., Hurwitz,T., & Kozak,J. (1986).Clinical utility of threemeasuresof frontal lobedysfunctionin neuropsychiatic samples.Intenational Journal of Newoscience, 30, 241-248. Daigneault,S., Braun,C. M. J., & Whitaker,H. A. (1992).Early effectsof normal aging on perseverativeand nonperseverative prefrontal measures.DevelopmentalNeurcpsychologl, 8, 9tl4. Drewe,E. A. (1974).The effectof typeandareaof brainlesionon WisconsinCardSortingTestperformance. Cortex,10, 159-170. Grant,D. A., & Berg,E. A. (1948).A behavioralanalysisof degree of reinforcementand easeof shifting to new responsesin a Weigl-type card sorting problem. Joumal of Expertmental P sychology, 38, 40441 1. Heaton,R. K., Chelune,G. J., Talley,J. L., Kay, G. G., & Curtiss, G. (1993).WisconsinCard Sorting Tbstmantml:Revisedand expanded. OdessqFL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Jcireskog, K., & Sdrbom,D. (1993).USRELS.Chicago:Scientific SoftwareInternational. Kellogg,R. T. (1983).Age differencesin hypothesistesting and frequencyprocessingin conceptlearnrng.Bulletin ofthc PsyclronomicSociety,2I, l0l-l04. Levine,M. (1963).Mediatingprocesses in humansat the outsetof discriminationlearning.PsychologicalReview 70, 254-276. Libon,D. J.,Glosser,G.,Malamut,B. L., Kaplan,E., Goldberg,E., Swanson,R., & Sands,L. P. (1994).Age, executivefunctions, and visuospatialfunctioningin healthyolder adults.Neurcpsychology,8,3843. Milner,B. (1963).Effectsof differentbrainlesionson cardsorting. Archivesof Neurology,9,90-100. Nelson,H. E. (1976).A modifiedcard sorting test sensitiveto frontallobedefects.Conen 12,313-324. Offenbach,S. I. (1974). A developmentalstudy of hypothesis testingand cue selectionstrategies.DevelopmentalPsychology, 10.48+490. Ozonoff, S. (1995).Reliability and validity of the WisconsinCard SortingTestin studiesof autism.Neurcpsychology,9, 491-5n. Parkin,A. J., & Walt€r,B. M. (1991).Aging, short-termmemory andfrontal dysfunction.Psychobiology, I 9, 175-179. 436 FRISTOE. SAUIJ{OUSE.AND WOODARD Parkin, A. J., Walter, B. M., & Hunkin, N. M. (1995). Relationships between normal aging, frontal lobe function, and memory for temporal and spatial inforrnation. Neuropsychology, 9, 3M-312. Perrine, K. (1993). Differential aspectsof conceptualprocessingin the Category Test and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Joumal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 461473. Ragland, J. D., Gur, R. C., Deutsch, G. K., Censits, D. M., & Gur, R. E. (1995). Reliability and construct validity of the pairedassociaterecognition test: A test of declarative memory using Wisconsin Card Sorting stimuli. Psychological Assessment,7, 25-32. Robinson, A. L., Heaton, R. K., Lehman, R. A., & Stilson, D. (1980). The utility of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in detectingand localizing frontal brain lesions.Journal ofConsulting and Clinical Psychalogy,48,605-614. Salthouse, T. A. (1991). Mediation of adult age differences in cognition by reductions in working memory and speed of processing.Psychological Science,2, 179-183. Salthouse,T. A. (1992). What do adult age differences in the Digit Symbol Substitution Test reflect? Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 47, Pl2l-P 128. Salthouse,T. A. (1993). Speedand knowledge as determinants of adult age differences in verbal tasks. Journal of Gerontology: P sycholo gical Sciences, 4 I, P29-P36. Salthouse, T. A. (1994). The nature of the influence of speed on adult age differences in cognition. Developmental Psychology, 30.240-250. Salthouse, T. A. (1996).Theprocessing-speed theoryof adultage differences in cognition. Psychalogical Review, I 03, 403428. Salthouse,T. A., & Coon, V. E. (1994). Interpretation of differential deficits: The case of aging and mental arithmetic. Joumal oJ Experimental Psychology: lzaming, Memory and Cognition, 20, tt72-1182. Salthouse,T. A., & Fristoe, N. M. (1995). Process analysisof adult age effects on a computer-administered Trail Making Test. N europsycholo gy, 9, 5 18-528. Salthouse,T. A., Fristoe, N. M., & Rhee, S. H. (1996). How localized are age-related effects on neuropsychological measures?lfeurop sychologl, I 0, 272-285. Woodard, J. (1994). Personal program-Computerized version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Received February 23, 1996 RevisionreceivedNovember7.1996 Accepted November 12, 1996 t