AMERICAN ANTHROPOLOGIST Limitations of Language for Conveying Navigational Knowledge: Way-Finding in the Southeastern Solomon Islands Richard Feinberg and Joseph Genz ABSTRACT Investigators have tended to view navigation either through the lens of cognition or of experience and embodiment. The cognitive approach assumes that perceptually salient aspects of the environment are mapped and retrieved in the mind (so-called cognitive mapping). The alternative is that navigators “feel” their way by ongoing sensations of movement, assessing their position through the sequential, temporal order in which salient environmental information is perceived. Recently, others have challenged models of knowledge that analytically separate cognitive and experiential modalities of knowing, suggesting that the navigator combines cognitive with visual, auditory, and kinesthetic information into an integrated whole. Such a holistic approach to spatial orientation and way-finding raises an important methodological challenge to cognitive anthropology, as certain forms of knowledge are not easily expressed in words. This is particularly true of kinesthetic knowledge of a canoe’s motion, which provides navigators with an indirect assessment of wave patterns. Here, we explore one of the authors’ observations during a voyage with a demonstrably accomplished navigator from the Solomon Islands’ Temotu Province who, nonetheless, appeared to provide inconsistent and self-contradictory accounts of his surroundings and performance. [Polynesia, navigation, spatial orientation, cognition] A nthropological explorations of the mind–body connection have a venerable history, dating at least to Marcel Mauss (1973[1935], 1979[1950]) and Maurice Merleau-Ponty (1962[1945]). The early focus largely involved symbolic projections of human anatomy onto political and ritual relationships (e.g., Hertz 1973 [1909]; cf. Needham 1973). Oppositions such as self/other or nature/culture came to permeate the work of Claude Lévi-Strauss and many of his structuralist followers. Symbolic and psychological anthropologists have explored the body’s (sometimes un-) boundedness and the cultural construction of “personhood” or notions of “self” (e.g., Hallowell 1955; Schneider 1968; Geertz 1976; Strathern 1988; Csordas 1994); and medical anthropology, by its very nature, examines the connections involving culture, mind, and body (e.g., Lock 1993; Scheper-Hughes and Lock 1987). Recent years have witnessed deconstruction of the body–mind distinction, owing to a recognition that the mind shapes physiology as much as it grows out of it (e.g., Scheper-Hughes and Lock 1987; Csordas 1988). And nowhere more than in the realm of navigation do cognitive strategies combine with multiple perceptual modalities and embodied practice to produce human skilled performance. Thomas Gladwin (1970) and David Lewis (1972) brought embodiment as a component of Pacific way-finding to anthropological attention in their discussions of wave patterns detected through a vessel’s pitch and roll. The navigator relates those kinesthetically perceived movements to such cognitive constructs as star and wind compasses and the spatial distribution of (largely unseen) islands. Yet, most discussions of way-finding have emphasized cognitive schemas that are consciously understood by the navigator and transmitted to apprentice voyagers or curious anthropologists primarily through articulate speech. Here we examine a case in which a capable navigator could not effectively explain critical environmental cues to an eager researcher. PROBLEM As we move about we often ask ourselves, “Where am I?” People living in particularly challenging environments c 2012 by the American Anthropological AMERICAN ANTHROPOLOGIST, Vol. 114, No. 2, pp. 336–350, ISSN 0002-7294, online ISSN 1548-1433. Association. All rights reserved. DOI: 10.1111/j.1548-1433.2012.01429.x Feinberg and Genz have addressed this question by developing elaborate navigational systems, and nowhere are navigational challenges more acute than in Oceania. Traditional way-finders must guide their voyaging canoes across a seemingly undifferentiated surface toward an island that lies below the horizon, without the benefit of instruments or charts. The attempt to understand how they accomplish this has generated numerous ethnographic investigations that both have drawn from and informed theories of spatial orientation. Clearly, multiple modalities of sensory experience are involved in successful way-finding. Still, most investigators have approached navigation either from the viewpoint of cognition or of experience and embodiment rather than exploring points of intersection. The cognitive approach assumes that perceptually salient aspects of the environment are mapped and retrieved in the mind, a process commonly termed “cognitive mapping” (e.g., Tolman 1948; Ingold 2000; Montello 2005). Cognitive anthropologists have attempted to uncover culturally shared mental representations (typically termed “cultural” or “cognitive” models) that provide a framework for spatial orientation (D’Andrade 1995; Bennardo 2009). Ethnographic studies have detailed how navigators from several island groups in the Pacific employ wind directions and the movements of celestial bodies to form conceptual compasses that are used for orientation and course setting. The canoe’s movement through its maritime environment enables navigators to estimate their position and maintain their course through dead reckoning. Other environmental features, such as sea life and disrupted swell patterns, are used to sense land before it becomes visible (e.g., Ammarell 1999; Feinberg 1988, 1991; Gladwin 1970; Hutchins 1983; Lewis 1972; Thomas 1987). Roy D’Andrade (1995:152) cites Gladwin’s (1970) study of Polowatese navigation as one of the best systematic descriptions of a cognitive model. A contrasting approach posits that a person “feels his way” via ongoing perceptions of movement through the environment (Ingold 2000). As anthropology shifted from viewing cognition as the internal mental processing of information to processes of engagement in practical activity (Bourdieu 1977), socialization (Hutchins 1995), and embodiment (Lave 1988), psychologist James Gibson (1979) proposed an ecological theory of perception, in which a person’s interaction with and movement through the environment are sufficient to encode information. Gibson argued that people travel through the environment by experiencing a direct, continual flow of perceptual information with little cognitive processing. Of particular interest is Gladwin’s (1970) rich ethnographic description of how Polowatese navigators feel their way toward a destination island in relation to the motion and sound of waves and wind, and the visual flow and patterning of stars. This suggested to cognitive scientist Edwin Hutchins (1995) that estimating position at sea is based on the passage of time during a voyage rather than a careful calculation of the distance traveled.1 Incorporating these ethnographic insights and experimental data • Limitations of Language 337 (Heft 1996) into Gibson’s ecological theory of environmental perception, Tim Ingold (2000) suggested that the answer to the question “Where am I?” may lie in assessing one’s position through movement in the environment in relation to the sequential, temporal order in which salient data enter one’s perceptual field of view rather than by defining one’s position in a precise maplike location. For Ingold, navigation is really a way-finding process of enacting and remembering each voyage as a flow of perspective through time, a “skilled performance in which the traveler, whose power of perception and action have been fine-tuned through experience, ‘feels his way’ towards his goal, continuously adjusting his movements in response to an ongoing perceptual monitoring of his surroundings” (Ingold 2000:220). Matthew Lauer and Shankar Aswani (2009) recently challenged models of knowledge that analytically separate cognitive and experiential modalities of knowing. Their ethnographic research upon fishing and navigational knowledge in Roviana, western Solomon Islands, shows that spatial knowledge of the maritime environment is developed over a lifetime of engagement in and performance of practical and socially situated activities. Following Ingold (2000), Lauer and Aswani assert that the foundation of a navigator’s knowledge lies in his or her embeddedness in the voyage itself rather than the ability to recall cognitive maps. However, acknowledging multiple forms of knowledge, they and others (e.g., Levinson 2003:219) view the process of Oceanic seafaring as combining cognitive information (e.g., star relationships) with visual, auditory, and kinesthetic information about the waves and wind into an integrated whole that yields a current estimate of the spatial environment and translates practically into minor adjustments to the canoe’s course. Such a holistic approach to spatial orientation and wayfinding raises a methodological challenge. From its beginning, cognitive anthropology has depended on language as a means of entry into people’s modes of thought. This is particularly evident in the componential analyses of the 1950s and 60s, with their assumption of an isomorphic relationship between cognitive categories and monolexemic labels, and their attempt to discern logical structures characterizing the arrangement of lexemes in a cultural domain.2 Through the succeeding decades, cognitive anthropology has grown more nuanced. Still, we typically infer our interlocutors’ mental models from linguistic communication. Although cognitive anthropology relies heavily on linguistic representations, certain forms of knowledge are not easily expressed in words (Shore 1996:60). This is particularly true of kinesthetic knowledge of a canoe’s motion, which provides navigators with an indirect assessment of swell patterns. The navigator, in turn, depends upon those swells for orientation, course heading, and remotely sensing land by detecting their wind-propelled flow across the open sea and their reflection by (or sometimes refraction around) islands. Yet, serendipitously built into studies of Oceanic way-finding is the “stern test of landfall” (Lewis 1972), which validates the navigational model, even if 338 American Anthropologist • Vol. 114, No. 2 • June 2012 certain aspects of the system cannot be articulated or appear inconsistent. Here, we explore these inarticulate and seemingly incongruous dimensions of navigational knowledge by examining one of the authors’ practical experiences and observations during a voyage in the Santa Cruz Islands of the southeastern Solomons. SETTING In 2007 and 2008, Feinberg spent nine months in the Solomon Islands, studying an attempt to revive what islanders understand to be traditional canoe building, sailing, voyaging, and navigation in the Santa Cruz region of Temotu Province. This area is well known among students of Pacific voyaging, owing to the work of William Davenport (1962, 1964), David Lewis (1972), Marianne George (1998, 1999; Feinberg and George 2008), and the Vaka Taumako Project (http://www.pacifictraditions.org/vaka/). Until the second half of the 20th century, islanders from Taumako, the Reef Islands, and the larger outcroppings of Ndeni, Utupua, and Vanikoro were involved in a complex system of exchange. Craftsmen from the Polynesian island of Taumako produced large voyaging canoes on order from sailors in the closely related Outer Reefs (known locally as Vaeakau).3 Vaeakau sailors used those canoes to travel to the larger Melanesian islands, where they exchanged fish, turtles, nuts, and sometimes women for scrolls of “red feather money” (muahau). The feather money was then used in bridewealth payments back home and to compensate Taumako craftsmen for providing the canoes. Vaeakau-Taumako navigators rely on way-finding techniques roughly similar to those documented for other parts of the Pacific: they follow movements of celestial bodies, patterns of ocean swells and reflected waves, and the prevailing winds. They report using natural occurrences not cited in other navigation systems—most especially a phenomenon locally termed te lapa, typically described as “like underwater lightning” (see Lewis 1972:208–211; Feinberg 2011). However, many details of their system were unknown to anthropological scholarship, and Feinberg and his collaborators hoped to unravel them through in-depth conversations with respected navigators as well as participant-observation at sea.4 They planned to observe the decision-making process while on inter island voyages and discuss with navigators the reasoning behind their decisions. As it turned out, no voyaging canoe was in regular operation during Feinberg’s time in the Solomons.5 However, on his way back from Taumako to the national capital in November 2008, he spent a week with Clement Teniau (see Figure 1), a respected navigator from Nukapu, and they traveled to four of the five Vaeakau islands in a fiberglass motor canoe. When traveling with Teniau, Feinberg had the opportunity to ask him to identify stars and swells and explain how he found his way from island to island. Below, Feinberg describes that voyage and his conversations, comparing Teniau’s sense of what was going on with his own subjective impressions Clement Teniau at helm of his sailing canoe on Graciosa Bay, Ndeni Island. (August 2008) FIGURE 1. as well as the measurements of his magnetic compass. Feinberg was perplexed by many of Teniau’s assessments, which seemed to contradict what he had been told by other navigators and, in a number of cases, even appeared inconsistent with what Teniau, himself, had said on other occasions. Yet, Teniau always arrived precisely at his destination. ELEMENTS OF VAEAKAU-TAUMAKO NAVIGATION Vaeakau-Taumako sailors navigate by stars, swells, and prevailing wind patterns. Sailing between Taumako and Vaeakau is more or less along an east–west axis, making it relatively easy to follow stars. The voyage between Vaeakau and the high islands of Ndeni, Utupua, and Vanikoro is along a north– south axis, making celestial navigation more difficult; but the distance is modest, so the large islands are usually visible from the outset if one travels during daylight in reasonable weather (see Figure 2). Major navigational asterisms—a term encompassing both individual stars and constellations—include Talo/Salo, “Taro” (Antares); Te Kilikā, “The Shark” (Scorpius’s tail); Hetumdavō, “Bunch of Stars” (Pleiades); Takelo (Orion’s Belt); Manu, the name of a Taumako culture hero (Sirius); Nga Papakau (or Kapakau), Manu’s two arms (Canopus and Procyon); and several others.6 Vaeakau-Taumako navigators, like those in other parts of the Pacific, distinguish swells from seas and reflected waves. Swells are regular waves produced by constant winds that blow over thousands of miles of open ocean. Seas are irregular waves produced by local winds. Reflected waves are the remains of swells that hit an island and bounce back toward the canoe. Swells in the Vaeakau-Taumako language are hokohua loa, “long waves,” whereas reflected waves are variously termed hokohua potopoto (also pronounced poroporo), “short waves,” hokohua tuktuki (also pronounced ssuki), “banging waves,” or hokohua te kaenga, “waves of the place.” Navigators distinguish two swell patterns, te hokohua loa te ngatae, “the trade-wind swell,” and te hokohua loa te angeho, “the monsoon swell.” The hokohua loa te ngatae comes from the southeastern quadrant; the hokohua loa te angeho is said to come more or less from the north, although it may be Feinberg and Genz FIGURE 2. • Limitations of Language 339 Map of the Solomon Islands (Courtesty of Amanda Mullett) anywhere from the northeast to the northwest—or even west–northwest. The third major guide is te nohoanga te matangi, “the seat (or dwelling place) of the wind,” or, more idiomatically, “the wind compass.” This is an abstract system indicating important wind points. Feinberg encountered several variants of the wind compass; the version rendered by Teniau during a conversation in Lata, the provincial capital, in November 2007 (see Figure 3) is fairly typical.7 • te tonga: a little north of southeast or, perhaps east– southeast (about 130◦ ) • te alunga: slightly south of due east • te tokelau tū: approximately due east • te palapu: east–northeast (about 60◦ ) • te tokelau: approximately due north; “straight toward Tinakula” from the Lata air strip • te hakahiu: approximately north–northwest (about 330◦ ) • te laki: about 250◦ • te ulu: just east of south (about 170◦ )8 The space in between each of these points is indicated by a compound term so that between te tonga and te alunga is te alunga-tonga, between te tokelau tū and te alunga is te tokelau tū-alunga, and so on. Designations for these intermediate regions are as follows: • Between te tonga and te alunga: te alunga-tonga • Between te alunga and te tokelau tū: te tokelau tū-alunga • • • • • • Between te tokelau tū and te palapu: te tokelau tū-palapu Between te palapu and te tokelau: te tokelau-palapu Between te tokelau and te hakahiu: hakahiu-tokelau Between te hakahiu and te laki: te hakahiu-laki Between te laki and te ulu: te ulu-laki Between te ulu and te tonga: te ulu-tonga In the following section, Feinberg describes his voyage with Teniau from Otmongi Village on Ndeni to four of the five Vaeakau islands and back to Ndeni. The description is taken, with minor editing from his 2008 field notes, and accordingly is rendered in the first person. THE VOYAGE On the morning of November 19, Teniau’s friend, Gordon Otai, brought his fiberglass canoe (see Figure 4) to Otmongi from Poa, the next village to the west. We set off for Nukapu at 10:15 a.m., heading almost due north, with the wind from the northeast, at 30 to 40 degrees (see Figure 5).9 The swells were only one to three feet, with an occasional four-footer—an estimate I made by comparing the wave crests with the position of the canoe’s gunwales and the passengers’ heads. We left the small bay at Otmongi with Teniau’s son, Lionel, at the helm. Once on the open sea, the wind came from the NNE at about 20 degrees, which was also our heading. I could only detect one swell, coming from the NE at 30 to 40 degrees. I thought it must be te hokohua loa te angeho, but Teniau insisted that it was te hokohua loa te ngatae and pointed to something coming from about 340 American Anthropologist • Vol. 114, No. 2 • June 2012 Vaeakau Taumako “wind compass” and dominant swells. (According to Clement Teniau as related in Lata, November 2007; Courtesy of Ghassan Rafeedie) FIGURE 3. 340 degrees, which he said was te hokohua loa te angeho. At first I could see nothing that looked or felt remotely like a swell from that direction. When we were finally behind Te Akau Loa, a huge reef that blocked most of the alleged ngatae swell, I thought that the angeho swell might have appeared. But as soon as we got out of the reef’s shadow, it disappeared again. And, as hard a time as I was having with the swells, the hokohua potopoto “short [i.e., reflected] waves” were still more problematic. Teniau insisted that he could see and feel them, and that although small they should be obvious. But all I could decipher were ripples on the surface that seemed to be going in many different directions. I asked Teniau if he steers by looking at the 2,800-foot high Tinakula Volcano and Ndeni Island (both of which were clearly visible and, indeed, hard to miss). He firmly said, No! He steers exclusively by te hokohua even on a clear day. I asked Lionel which way the current was flowing, and he said, “Ki alunga,” pointing toward the east. A little later, I asked Teniau the same question, and he said without hesitation, “Ki lalo” [To the west]. Lionel overheard this and immediately said, “No. It’s flowing ki alunga ‘eastward.’” At that point Teniau reversed himself and said, “Ki alunga.” This was not a simple slip of the tongue, as both times he pointed to where he thought the current was running as well as telling me in words. Lionel held a steady course of about 20 degrees, directly into the wind, which Teniau said was coming from te tokelau tū-palapu. Soon, Teniau took over and changed our heading to about 40 degrees, then gradually moved back toward due north, or even a little west of north. The shifts were not dramatic; to me they were not obvious by looking at the wave patterns. And I think Teniau believed he was holding a steady course. He said that from Otmongi, Nukapu is i te palapu. When he had shifted to due north and a little west of north, I asked if he were now going toward te tokelau. He said no; we were still heading toward te palapu. He then pointed to what he said was te tokelau—which at that point he identified as being in the west(!). Was he disoriented? Possibly. But a few minutes later, he was again heading at about 20 degrees and, pointing off the port bow, accurately identified what I understood to be te tokelau. We continued heading toward the NE or NNE until, after 2.5 hours at sea, at about 12:45 p.m., we sighted Matema on the horizon. Then we turned intentionally to the north, or even NNW. This is not the most direct route from Otmongi to Nukapu but seems to be a version of the island-hopping strategy described by Lewis (1972). One heads NE until Matema comes into view, then heads back toward the NNW. If one’s objective is to get to Nukapu, rather than to prove a point, this makes perfect sense as a navigational strategy. At 1:55 p.m., when Nukapu appeared on the horizon, Teniau gave me a big hug and a slap on the back and said, “You see? The custom compass works!” On the morning of November 20, our second day out from Otmongi, another seeming inconsistency arose. Standing in Teniau’s Nukapu house, with a half dozen others around to “help,” he identified the wind compass points as follows: • Tonga: just south of east • Alunga: east • Tokelau tū: northwest • Palapu: southwest • Tokelau: toward Tinakula (which would be south from Nukapu) Feinberg and Genz FIGURE 4. Lionel with Gordon’s motor canoe on beach at Otmongi, preparing for voyage to Nukapu FIGURE 5. Map of the Santa Cruz Islands (Courtesy of Amanda Mullett) • Limitations of Language 341 342 American Anthropologist • Vol. 114, No. 2 • June 2012 • Hakahiu (Teniau pronounces this “te huahiu”): toward Temotu Neo [SSE] • Laki: southeast • Ulu: east–southeast To me this appeared utterly confused—as if Teniau thought he was still in Lata, which would place Tinakula i te tokelau, but te tokelau would be to the north (see Figure 6). A possible alternative is that te tokelau is always toward Tinakula regardless of one’s own position, making its volcanic cone the center of the system. If so, he would be using a consummately radial frame of reference (cf. Bennardo 2002, 2009, n.d.), with axes emanating from Tinakula. But that system would have to be different from one that identifies te alunga as the direction of sunrise, which is approximately east regardless of one’s position. This led to a daylong quest to pin down directional terms; but in the end, I could find no way to reconcile the discrepancies. As Teniau and I pursued this conversation, he made it clear that he does not view Tinakula as necessarily residing in the tokelau. From Honiara (the national capital on Guadalcanal island, over 400 miles to the west), for example, it is toward te alunga. However, from every place in the Santa Cruz region, he identified it as being toward te tokelau. Later that day, on Pileni, a respected sailor named Roy Voia gave me what I thought was a consistent, plausible list of direction names, placing Tinakula toward the hakahiu. Teniau did not argue with Voia, but when we were by ourselves in the canoe, he immediately went back to insisting that Tinakula was i te tokelau. Other directions were adjusted to accommodate this, and he even placed te alunga and sunrise just to the east of north (at about 20 degrees). When I pointed out that my compass did not show that position as the point of sunrise, he insisted that my compass must be wrong! There was a similar inconsistency in Teniau’s identification of swells. As we motored from Nukapu toward Pileni, we noticed small but clear waves coming from the ESE, at about 115 degrees. Teniau identified this as te hokohua loa te ngatae, and the swell he had identified the previous day as te ngatae he now said was te angeho. That evaluation was consistent with my sense; but when the new wave pattern faded and, as far as I could tell, the only swell was the one coming from the NE (at about 50 degrees), he went back to insisting that that was te ngatae—and te angeho was again the westerly swell pattern that I could not detect. I was about to conclude that Teniau was a charlatan— making up facts as he went along—and the only reason he does not get lost is that sailing in the Santa Cruz Islands presents few navigational challenges, as one usually can see one’s destination almost from the start. But three facts dissuaded me: 1. 2. He appeared to be totally convinced of the correctness of his assessments, showing not the slightest doubt or hesitation. He really did have the ability to spot islands before I saw them, and the islands eventually appeared where 3. he said they should. Thus, it seemed possible that he could also see or feel other oceanographic phenomena that I could not. And, He correctly pointed to the location of every island in his navigational universe. Around mid-afternoon, we left Pileni for Nifiloli. The following morning I met with another old sailor, Peter Taea; then, we set off for Matema, the last of our Vaeakau islands. Once on Matema, Teniau announced that the wind was blowing from te palapu, which struck me as about right: approximately 20 degrees. But he still insisted that Tinakula was i te tokelau, which means that from Matema te palapu and te tokelau are about 90 degrees apart—Tinakula being just north of due west. He said te tokelau and hakahiu, by contrast, are very close together—as they must be to fit all eight directions into the model he was now presenting. Our next destination was Kala Bay on Ndeni. We planned to make the voyage at night in hopes of seeing te lapa, a navigational phenomenon resembling underwater lightning (see above). I was up on schedule, at 2 a.m.; but Teniau was sound asleep. At 2:30 a.m., I nudged him and asked if he still wanted to sail at night. I had considered pushing for an earlier departure. Teniau, however, insisted that the trip from Matema to Kala Bay would take less than an hour. As was consistently the case, he underestimated the time by a factor of four, and we did not arrive until well past seven. More critically, dawn makes its first approach at about 4 a.m., so by departing at 3:30, we only gave ourselves about a half hour to see te lapa. It took that long to get far enough to sea for te lapa from Ndeni to be potentially visible. In a perfect world, we might have been able to view it coming behind us from Matema, but that was directly in the path of the moon, which was bright enough to obliterate any trace. Teniau blamed our inability to see te lapa on the moon. However, it did not rise until late at night; and up to 3 a.m., it was obscured by clouds. If we had left at 1 or 2 a.m., we would have had a reasonable window of opportunity. Teniau chose to leave at a time when, it seemed, a sophisticated navigator should have known the chances of viewing te lapa were minimal. The nighttime departure gave us a chance to view the stars. But again, the results were disconcerting. Teniau appeared to be familiar with more stars and constellations than most of my consultants. To my surprise, however, I was quicker and more confident in identifying stars than he. Several times, he had trouble finding Takelo (Orion’s belt) until I pointed it out to him. He initially looked in the wrong part of the sky for Sino (Sirius). He described Te Huangi (aka Te Longi) and Lōleī but could not locate them until I figured out that he was talking about Orion’s shoulders and knees, respectively. When I pointed them out and asked if those were the stars he meant, he immediately said yes. Yet, later that night, he again placed Lōleī in the wrong region of the sky—far to the south rather than slightly northwest of the Feinberg and Genz FIGURE 6. • Limitations of Language 343 Teniau’s “wind compass” from Nakapu as described on November 19, 2008 (Courtesy of Ghassan Rafeedie) zenith. When I pointed to Orion and said, “I thought you told me it was there,” he quickly replied, yes, I had it right. Of course, even an accomplished navigator occasionally makes mistakes and misidentifies stars or does not immediately find the one he is seeking. I have been disoriented by cloud cover, moonlight, or other factors that change the apparent “shape of the sky” (Lewis 1972). And Teniau does not practice a lot. Most of his sailing is along the Ndeni coast, and most of it is during the day. Still, for me to be more adept than Teniau at finding and identifying the major navigational asterisms was, to say the least, disappointing. The disappointment was offset somewhat by Teniau’s repeated declaration that the “real kaastam compass” is the swell and not the stars.10 As he put it, “The stars move around; the waves are always there.” And for that reason, he said, he pays more attention to the wave patterns than the stars, even when traveling at night. I pointed out that the stars may move, but they do so according to predictable rules. And wave patterns are not absolutely stable. He agreed to the first proposition but was reluctant to accept the second and was determined to demonstrate to me the efficacy of the kaastam kompas by showing me how he would follow the hokohua loa te ngatae from Matema to Kala Bay. But I also had problems with this. The designated waves consisted of a very large swell coming strongly from just south of east, at about 110 degrees. The swell was unmistakable, cresting at as much as six or seven feet; I was standing in the bow and frequently found myself looking up at the waves. The direction also was consistent with what others had described as te hokohua loa te ngatae. However, the swell we used in sailing from Otmongi to Nukapu three days earlier was coming from the northeast, and Teniau said at the time that that was te hokohua loa te ngatae. Either he was wrong in his identification then (which I think is the case) or the swell is much less consistent than he was now saying. We headed due south, with the swell hitting the canoe from port, just forward of amidships. Kala Bay is slightly west of due south from Matema. After approximately an hour, Teniau said something about the current to Lionel, who turned the canoe sharply toward the west so that we were heading approximately southwest at 230 to 240 degrees. Teniau said the dramatic change of heading was because the current was running strongly to the east. We had to go westward to offset it and make a direct landing at Kala Bay. I asked Teniau how he knew the current was running eastward, and he offered several pieces of evidence: • When the tide is low, he said, the current between Ndeni and the Reefs runs from west to east—and the tide at the time was low; • he could feel the current pulling on the canoe. It is commonly believed (e.g., Sharp 1957) that, lacking instruments, one cannot detect the current when far out at sea. Still, it seemed that when we turned to the southwest we were surfing quickly down the face of the waves; but when we hit the trough, the engine more or less stalled, and it felt like we were being held back by the force of the current; and • the wind was only about 10 to 15 knots; yet the waves were as high as I had seen them during my 2007–08 field trip. Teniau believed this was because the current was running counter to the wind and swell, causing the water to “pile up” and make the waves higher than the wind speed would lead one to expect. Other VaeakauTaumako sailors have told me the same thing, and 344 American Anthropologist • Vol. 114, No. 2 • June 2012 Anutans, whose navigational practices I studied in the 1980s, also cited this as one way to detect the current when out of sight of land. Teniau, based on his assessment of the waves, wind, and current, had Lionel go sharply to the southwest and then, over the next hour, gradually pivot counterclockwise, so that by about 5:45 a.m. he was again heading pretty much due south. He hit the mouth of Kala Bay dead on, in the middle of the passage, for a perfect landing. DISCUSSION Feinberg was taken aback at the apparent contradictions in Teniau’s account of their course. He had conducted fieldwork in the southwestern Pacific for close to 40 years, and some of his research has involved navigation in other Polynesian communities. In addition, he was familiar with the work of others who have studied indigenous Pacific navigation (e.g., Alkire 1965; Finney 1979, 1994, 2003; Frake 1995; Gladwin 1970; Goodenough 1953; Goodenough and Thomas 1987; Lewis 1972; Thomas 1987). Nothing in his reading and experience prepared him for the gulf that separated actions from their verbal explanations during his journey through Nga Vaeakau. Assuming Teniau to be the capable navigator he commonly is understood to be, how might one account for the discrepancy between his obviously effective performance and his less-than-stellar narrative? One possibility is that directional names are unimportant. Teniau could point to the locations of those islands he might wish to reach as well as others he had only heard of. He understood prevailing wind directions during the different seasons. And he seemed to be generally familiar with the currents (despite his evident lapse on the first morning). Arguably, the name for a particular direction within an abstract scheme is simply a semantic quibble. Were this hypothesis correct, however, it is hard to see why many of Feinberg’s interlocutors, including Teniau, stressed the importance of getting the “wind compass” right and criticized one another for alleged mistakes. Some consultants stated that the names for wind compass directions change from one place to another because wind patterns differ as one moves to different islands. It seems unlikely, however, that wind patterns vary so widely within Temotu province that te tokelau should be to the north from Lata and to the south from Nukapu. It is also difficult to see, if that were the case, how one could describe interisland routes in terms of such directional names. It appears that the order in which one encounters named wind points remains more or less stable despite the variation in absolute positions. Thus, if one were to face te tonga and turn counterclockwise, the next named point would be te alunga, followed by te tokelau tū, and so on. The absolute position of each of those points in terms of magnetic compass bearings, however, is variable, and the distance between points differs from commentator to commentator as well as for the same commentator from one occasion to the next. Feinberg (1988: 91–98) found the same kind of variability in Anutans’ characterizations of their wind compass. In the Anutan case, however, the most effective navigators were generally consistent. More likely, Teniau had only limited conscious awareness of the factors that affected his perception of where he was in relation to his starting point and possible destinations. Many nonhuman animals navigate for hundreds (and, in some cases thousands) of miles without self-conscious understanding of what they are doing or an ability to express their experience in anything resembling human language. Perhaps some people also are attuned to certain aspects of their natural environment in such a way that they can respond effectively but, because they are unconscious of the physiological processes involved, are unable to articulate them. Arguably, this is the case with Teniau and other unusually adept navigators. If so, it would be no more than an extreme version of a common human experience: we all are constantly bombarded with sensory stimulation, much of which never intrudes upon our conscious thought. Yet, we may process that information and respond to it as if guided by rational consciousness (e.g., Hirst 1995). Some well-documented physiological phenomena lend this hypothesis a degree of plausibility. One is synesthesia, “a condition in which stimulation of one sensory modality causes unusual experiences in a second, unstimulated modality” (Hubbard and Ramachandran 2005:509). The best-known form of synesthesia involves the experience of certain colors in association with such phenomena as notes on a musical scale, days of the week, or other non-colorrelated words. A less common but well-documented form is spatial synesthesia, in which phenomena with no intrinsic spatial relationship to one another are visualized as being arranged according to a certain spatial pattern. Perhaps Teniau was experiencing something akin to spatial synesthesia, in which sensory perceptions produced by wave action or the movements of stars were translated directly into a mental image of spatial relations among islands. If the “translation” was direct and not mediated by language, it is unsurprising that he had difficulty articulating the systemic connections. Wanting to be helpful, he attempted to answer Feinberg’s questions, but because he was trying to express his implicit understandings in an unaccustomed medium, the message often came out garbled. This line of argument, admittedly, is speculative. Still, cognitive scientist Edward Hubbard (personal communication) agrees that Teniau could have had “a direct experience akin to synesthesia,” adding that, “If this were the case . . . it would be an example of how synesthesia might interfere with certain abilities, and not others.” An anthropological colleague and experienced yachtsman, James Dow (personal communication, June 11, 2010), offered a concurring opinion: The first task of a navigator is to know where he (or she) is. This is almost impossible to put into words. Some people . . . know it instinctively, others are constantly lost. If you don’t know where Feinberg and Genz you are, directions are irrelevant. If you do know where you are, directions need not be precise, as long as you keep knowing where you are as you move along. Dow’s suggestion that navigational abilities are hard-wired and exist to differing degrees in different individuals finds support in recent work by cognitive neuroscientists with nonhuman animals (Wills, et al. 2010; Langston et al. 2010). Tom Wills et al. (2010:1573), for example, depict the hippocampal cognitive map as a sort of Kantian a priori synthetic truth and propose—at least among preweanling and weanling rats—that “the basic constituents of the cognitive map develop independently of spatial experience, or might even precede it.” If spatial cognition is grounded in prehuman neurophysiology, it makes sense that navigational processes would be largely unconscious and, therefore, difficult to articulate. In Teniau’s case we have the added factor that he had spent decades traversing the same path. Perhaps the route had become so automatic that he ceased to think about what he was doing or how he might explain it. A useful analogy may exist between someone possessing an intuitive ability to discern spatial relationships and a musician with perfect pitch: the phenomenon seems so obvious to the person with the proper neurological wiring that it defies explanation; for the person who is “tone deaf,” it cannot be taught. Similarly, one might compare navigation with learning to play a musical instrument. Quality performance requires training and practice, but the training is wasted on someone who lacks the innate talent. Navigation, like music, is highly respected in many communities; yet, only a few individuals ever become accomplished navigators.11 Whatever merit our physiological hypothesis may hold, it must be treated with a degree of caution. Some Pacific navigators have been able to explain their knowledge and procedures to the satisfaction of anthropological investigators.12 Moreover, some communities have “schools” in which navigational arts are passed from generation to generation (see, e.g., Gladwin 1970). If the relevant information cannot be expressed in articulate language, it is difficult to envision how such schools would operate and why there would be differences among navigational schools in the same community. There are also documented instances of navigational knowledge being transmitted, at least in part, through oral communication. Native Hawaiian navigator Nainoa Thompson, for example, learned his art largely through verbal instruction from Mau Piailug of Satawal and through conversations with astronomer Will Kyselka in the Bishop Museum’s planetarium (Kyselka 1987). Although some way-finders are able to explain their skill through articulate discourse, verbal communication clearly has limits. Most people will never become skilled navigators despite superior instruction; and it seems likely that some special sensory acuity comprises part of the way-finder’s repertoire. Moreover, in at least some communities, navigational expertise is developed primarily by watching and doing rather than through verbal instruction. This is true • Limitations of Language 345 on Anuta (Feinberg 1988), and it may also account for some of the frustration expressed to Feinberg in 2008 by aspiring Taumako navigators. Several Taumako men who wished to study traditional way-finding techniques spoke of their disappointment at master navigator Crusoe Kaveia’s failure to conduct regular “classes.” He occasionally held sessions in which he described the wind compass and important navigational asterisms, but his detractors complained that the gatherings were sporadic. If his knowledge were of a kind that could not be adequately conveyed orally in a land-based, language-dependent class, it follows that he would not be inclined to waste his time on useless sessions.13 CONCLUSION By the end of Feinberg’s Vaeakau excursion, he had more questions than before he started. Was Teniau dissembling about his knowledge of the stars and swells? The apparent inconsistencies in his account suggest he was. The results, however, suggest that he was not; that he really knows what he is doing but is not good at explaining it. Assuming Teniau is an accomplished navigator, worthy of his considerable reputation, what might this experience teach us about Pacific way-finding and spatial cognition? Below, we offer a few suggestions. The past half century has witnessed a good deal of research on Pacific navigation, the thrust of which has been to emphasize the acumen of traditional way-finders. That was an important corrective to the work of skeptics like Sharp (1957), who argued that humans, operating without instruments, are incapable of accurate navigation over long distances and that the isles of Oceania must, therefore, have been settled as a result of accidental drift voyages. Feinberg’s experience with Teniau suggests that the precision of Oceanic way-finding techniques, while clearly effective, may be exaggerated. They do not replicate the precise measurements of modern instruments, and there is no reason to expect them to do so. As Lewis emphasized four decades ago (see also Ammarell 1999:126), way-finding techniques need only be accurate enough to get the voyagers reliably and safely to their destination. Beyond that, attempts at precision are, in essence, wasted effort. Feinberg’s voyage with Teniau informs the conversation concerning the respective roles of cognitive mapping and embodied experience in Pacific way-finding. Despite a degree of imprecision, Teniau appeared to operate with a mental map of his navigational universe. From anywhere in the Santa Cruz Islands, he was able to point with considerable accuracy to each of the Vaeakau islands plus Taumako, Tinakula, Ndeni, Temotu Neo, Utupua, Vanikoro, Tikopia, Vanuatu, and Anuta. These include both islands he knew well and places he had never visited but whose locations he understood from the reports of others. He was inconsistent about naming the direction in which each of these islands lay, but he was able to identify the physical direction. His mental map is perhaps less elaborate than certain others, such as 346 American Anthropologist • Vol. 114, No. 2 • June 2012 that held by Anutans of their fishing grounds (Feinberg et al. 2003; Feinberg 2008), but it exists on a far larger scale. Although Teniau employs a mental map of sorts, he also appears to rely on embodied experience involving physical reactions to a sequence of environmental cues encountered while traversing a well-established path. On leaving Otmongi, he determined the correct direction by visual sighting. Once the direction was established, he felt the canoe’s motion in response to the existing swell, and he maintained that kinesthetic orientation until Matema came into view. Then he turned slightly to port and felt the new pattern of movement, given the canoe’s new heading in relation to the swells. He maintained that feel until Nukapu appeared in sight.14 This sequence of perceptions and actions supports Lauer and Aswani’s conclusion that navigation involves an interface between cognitive mapping and embodied experience. In contrast with Caroline Islanders (Goodenough 1953; Gladwin 1970; Frake 1995), Anutans (Feinberg 1988), Nukumanu (Feinberg 1995), the Bugis of Indonesia (Ammarell 1999), and others, Teniau appears not to pay a great deal of attention to star paths. And, despite professed reliance on wave patterns, he was also inconsistent in his naming of the swells and speaking about currents. His performance indicates that he perceived environmental cues enabling him, with total confidence and accuracy, to reach his destination. Although the sequence of actions is clear, however, precisely what Teniau felt was not convincingly articulated, and the investigator was unable to experience what Teniau reported sensing. It is conceivable that Teniau was actually consistent in his descriptions, and the problem lay with Feinberg’s inability to understand his system for discussing space. Einar Haugen (1969), Alexandre Francois (2003), Mary Chambers (2009), Giovanni Bennardo (2009), and others note that multiple systems of spatial representation may coexist, and directional labels in some instances are homophones, thereby giving the illusion of inconsistency.15 A sometimes-complex set of rules arguably determines which of the directional systems is invoked in a particular situation; the researcher’s challenge is to ascertain the rules.16 We cannot rule out this explanation of Teniau’s performance—oral and navigational—but we feel it unlikely. The purportedly comparable cases appear to involve what Bradd Shore (1996) called “multiple models.” VaeakauTaumako, likewise, employs multiple models of spatial orientation that intersect according to an intricate set of linguistic and cultural rules. That phenomenon is addressed by Feinberg in two recent conference papers (n.d.a, n.d.b). Teniau, however, was explicit that he considered himself to be employing a single model. Sometimes he gave apparently contradictory accounts just moments apart, and on several occasions he emphatically disagreed with other navigators who were participating in the same conversation. In many cases he was dealing either with a version of a wind compass that is fairly consistent throughout the southeastern Solomon Islands, at least with respect to certain critical parameters (Boerger n.d.) or with the names of swell patterns, which seem quite well defined when speaking of them in general terms. Once, he reversed his assessment of which way the current was running after a brief exchange with his son, and sometime after the voyage he confided to Feinberg that he might have misspoken about directional names on one or two occasions. Clearly, successful navigation requires multiple streams of sensory input. Gladwin, Lewis, and others have called attention to the integration of data from sighting of asterisms with the kinesthetic experience of waves, winds, and currents. To these, Gene Ammarell (1999) adds auditory data, such as Bugis navigators’ reliance on the Muslim call to prayer, emanating from shore to calculate the passage of time. In most of these instances, however, the navigators provided cogent explanations of their process. With Teniau, that was not the case. The closest parallel may be etak, a conceptual tool that Carolinian navigators use to estimate their position by calculating the angle between a canoe and a sometimes-imaginary reference island off to one side of the designated route. Despite attempts to explain etak to a number of investigators, a debate continues over what it is and how it works (see, e.g., Hutchins’s 1983, 1995; Pyrek 2011).17 Similarly, Teniau appears to rely on a system that is a challenge to express in words. This is not to say that oral communication is irrelevant. It was through spoken language that Teniau made clear the primacy of waves over stars and the visual sighting of islands. For conveying astronomical information, it probably is indispensible. On the other hand, the processing of kinesthetic information provided by the canoe’s movement over an uneven sea may be difficult to communicate effectively through spoken language. Limitations of this kind have been reported for a few other Pacific voyagers who remotely detect land by sensing disrupted swell and current patterns. For example, Marshall Islands navigators guide their canoes by sensing subtle changes in the ocean’s flow that are assessed indirectly through the vessel’s motion (Ascher 1995; Finney 1998; Genz 2008). As the Marshallese language has encoded only a few terms to describe these sensations, navigators supplement their descriptions with hand gestures and resort to other nonlinguistic means of communication—notably, they blindfold students and place them on canoes in shallow water surrounding coral islets to simulate the movement of a canoe at sea when experiencing disrupted swell patterns (Genz 2008, n.d.). If language is not always an effective means of entry into another person’s thought process, that leaves the problem of how to access another’s cognition. Watching what others do is often as informative as listening to what they say. Yet people may engage in similar behaviors for quite different reasons. How to get from superficial action to cognition has been a long-standing challenge for anthropology and will continue to command attention for the foreseeable future. Feinberg and Genz 6. Richard Feinberg and State University, Kent, Department of Anthropology, Kent OH 44244; rfeinber@kent.edu; http://www.kent.edu/cas/anthropology/facstaff/∼rfeinber/ Joseph Genz Department of Anthropology, University of Hawai`i, Mānoa, Honolulu, HI 96822; genz@hawaii.edu. NOTES Acknowledgments. This article is based on a study conducted un- der the auspices of the National Science Foundation, Grant #2010– 70. We are indebted to many Solomon Islanders in the VaeakauTaumako region of Temotu Province for their hospitality, assistance, and extraordinary patience in sharing their voyaging and navigational systems. Particularly critical was the support and guidance of Taumako’s Paramount Chief Michael Tauopi; former paramount chief and master navigator Crusoe Kaveia; Provincial Assembly Representative Honourable Stanley Tehiahua; Father Johnson Vaike; Dr. Simon Salopuka; voyagers Nathaniel Leiau, Jonas Holland, Janet Longomaha, Ambrose Meakey, Noel Hatu, and William Keizy. Good friends Inny Taupea, Basil Tavake, and Mostyn Vane, all from Taumako, also provided invaluable assistance. In addition, we are grateful to Vaeakau sailors Joseph Laki, Roy Voia, Peter Taea, Shadrack Tuinamo, and George Tavake for taking the time to share their thoughts and voyaging experiences. We owe a special note of thanks to Clement Teniau of Nukapu and Otmongi for orchestrating a tour of the Vaeakau islands, arranging accommodations, and providing many important insights. We are indebted to Amanda Mullett and Ghassan Rafeedie for assistance with the figures. A preliminary version of this article was presented at the 2008 meeting of the Central States Anthropological Society. We owe thanks to audience members and fellow panelists including Jon Wagner, Jim Dow, Alex Mawyer, and Kate Grim-Feinberg for helpful commentary. Others who read and made perceptive comments on earlier drafts of this article include Edward Hubbard, Mary Ann Raghanti, Cathleen Pyrek, Joseph Grim Feinberg, Tereza Smejkalova, and three anonymous reviewers. 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. In practice, of course, time and distance are inextricably interwoven, as distance traveled is a product of time combined with speed. These assumptions and procedures are well illustrated by Stephen Tyler’s influential 1969 collection. The Vaeakau islands are Nupani, Nukapu, Pileni, Nifiloli, and Matema. In addition to Feinberg, the team included co-Principle Investigators Marianne George and Ben Finney. George had been working with Crusoe Kaveia, Taumako’s paramount chief and master navigator, since the early 1990s. Finney had investigated traditional Pacific navigation since the 1960s. One canoe was taken out for two brief sails on the fringing reef during Feinberg’s 2007 visit to Taumako, but it never ventured onto the open sea. George was able to sail several times in inshore waters with a film crew from the British Broadcasting Corporation in early 2008 while Feinberg was not in residence (BBC 2009). 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. • Limitations of Language 347 On other Polynesian islands, Manu is identified as a bird or a spirit with bird-like properties, and manu is the usual word for “bird.” In Vaeakau-Taumako, most people identify Manu as a man. Sirius is also known in Vaeakau-Taumako as Tino or Sino (“Body”), indicating that this is Manu’s body, as opposed to the two arms (Nga Kapakau or Nga Papakau). Terminology in other Polynesian languages is parallel. On Anuta, for example, Sirius is Manu (“Bird”) or Te Tino a Manu (“Bird’s Body”), while Canopus and Procyon are Te Kapakau Pakatonga (“The Southeast Wing”) and Te Kapakau Pakatokerau (“The North Wing”). The Taumako call these stars Te Papakau Nga Ndeni (“The Ndeni Arm”) and Te Papakau a Tātou (“Our Arm”). The Vaeakau-Taumako wind compass is discussed in greater detail in Feinberg n.d. For a quite different rendering, see the Vaka Taumako Project website: http://www.pacifictraditions. org/vaka/NohoangaTeMatangi.html. Te is the singular form of the definite article, roughly equivalent to the English word the. Teniau possessed a GPS that Marianne George had provided on an earlier visit, but he rarely used it owing to the cost of batteries. He and Feinberg took the instrument on their trip, and it consistently provided readings that were at five to ten degrees variance with the magnetic compass. After the first day, Feinberg dispensed with the GPS and relied on his compass, which had the advantage of being smaller and more water resistant and did not need to be turned on and off. Kaastam (or kaastom) is a word in Pijin, the lingua franca of the Solomon Islands. It is derived from the English custom and means something like “customary” or “traditional.” We are indebted to an anonymous reviewer of this manuscript for suggesting these analogies. Gladwin’s (1970) and Lewis’s (1972) interlocutors are exemplary in this regard. This observation is consistent with documented Polynesian learning styles. Ritchie and Ritchie (1989) found that Polynesian education is typically accomplished by observation and practice rather than verbal instruction. This is reminiscent of what has been termed a “route” as opposed to a “survey” perspective on giving and processing directions (Tversky 2003; Bennardo n.d.). Bennardo (2009 and elsewhere), for example, calls attention to the difference between small-scale and large-scale space for determining the spatial frame of reference invoked by Tongan consultants. Thanks to an anonymous reviewer for calling this possibility to our attention. Significantly, Hawaiian navigator Nainoa Thompson was never able to master etak despite Carolinian Mau Piailug’s efforts to impart the system. REFERENCES CITED Alkire, William H. 1965 Lamotrek Atoll and Inter-Island Socioeconomic Ties. Illinois Studies in Anthropology No. 5. Urbana and London: University of Illinois Press. 348 American Anthropologist • Vol. 114, No. 2 • June 2012 Ammarell, Gene 1999 Bugis Navigation. New Haven, CT: Yale University Southeast Asia Studies. Ascher, Marcia 1995 Models and Maps from the Marshall Islands: A Case in Ethnomathematics. Historia Mathematica 22:347–370. Bennardo, Giovanni 2002 Mental Images of the Familiar: Cultural Strategies of Spatial Representation in Tonga. In Representing Space in Oceania: Culture in Language and Mind. G. Bennardo, ed. Pp. 159– 177. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics, Research School of Pacific and Asian Studies, Australian National University. 2009 Language, Space, and Social Relationships: A Foundational Model in Polynesia. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. n.d. Giving Directions in Tonga. Paper presented to the annual meeting of the American Anthropological Association. November 17–21. New Orleans, LA. Boerger, Brenda n.d. Space, the Final Frontier: Spatial Particles and Winds of the language Natügu. Paper presented to working session on “Spatial Representation in Oceania” at the annual meeting of the Association for Social Anthropology in Oceania. February 8–12. Honolulu, HI. Bourdieu, Pierre 1977 Outline of a Theory of Practice. Cambridge, NY: Cambridge University Press. British Broadcasting Corporation 2009 Planet Ocean. Chambers, Mary Ruth 2009 Which Way is Up? Motion Verbs and Paths of Motion in Kubokota, an Austronesian Language in the Solomon Islands. Ph.D. dissertation. Endangered languages Academic Programme, School of Oriental and African Studies. Csordas, Thomas J. 1988 Embodiment as a Paradigm for Anthropology. Ethos 18:5– 47. 1994 Embodiment and Experience: The Existential Ground of Culture and Self. London: Cambridge University Press. D’Andrade, Roy 1995 The Development of Cognitive Anthropology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Davenport, William 1962 Red-feather Money. Scientific American 206(3):94–104. 1964 Notes on Santa Cruz Voyaging. Journal of the Polynesian Society 73:134–42. Feinberg, Richard 1988 Polynesian Seafaring and Navigation: Ocean Travel in Anutan Culture and Society. Kent, OH: Kent State University Press. 1991 A Long-distance Voyage in Contemporary Polynesia. Journal of the Polynesian Society 100(1):25–44. 1995 Continuity and Change in Nukumanu Maritime Technology and Practice. In Seafaring in the Contemporary Pacific Islands. R. Feinberg, ed. Pp. 159–195. DeKalb, IL: Northern Illinois University Press. 2008 Polynesian Representations of Geographical and Cosmological Space: Anuta, Solomon Islands. In Canoes of the Grand Ocean. A. Di Piazza and E. Pearthree, eds. Pp. 69–84. Oxford: British Archaeological Research (BAR) Archaeopress. 2011 In Search of Te Lapa: A Navigational Enigma in VaeakauTaumako, Southeastern Solomon Islands. Journal of the Polynesian Society 120(1):57–70. n.d.a Radial Representations and Multiple Models in Polynesia: Spatial Orientation on Taumako, Solomon Islands. Paper presented to the annual meeting of the American Anthropological Association. November 17–21. New Orleans, LA. n.d.b Conceptualizing Front and Back: Frames of Reference and Taumako Representations of Space. Paper presented to working session on “Spatial Representation in Oceania” at the annual meeting of the Association for Social Anthropology in Oceania. February 8–12. Honolulu, HI. Feinberg, Richard, Ute J. Dymon, Pu Paiaki, Pu Rangituteki, Pu Nukuriaki, and Matthew Rollins 2003 ‘Drawing the coral heads’: Mental Mapping and its Physical Representation in a Polynesian Community. The Cartographic Journal 40(3):243–253. Feinberg, Richard and Marianne George 2008 Seafaring in the Polynesian Outliers (by R. Feinberg and M. George). In Encyclopedia of the History of Science, Technology, and Medicine in Non-Western Cultures (second edition). H. Selin, ed. Pp. 1983–1989. Berlin, Heidelberg, & New York: Springer-Verlag. Finney, Ben R. 1979 Hōkūle‘a, the Way to Tahiti. New York: Dodd, Mead and Company. 1994 Voyage of Rediscovery. Berkeley: University of California Press. 1998 Traditional Navigation and Nautical Cartography in Oceania. In The History of Cartography, Vol.3, part 2: Cartography in the Traditional African, American, Arctic, Australian, and Pacific Societies. D. Woodward and G. M. Kewis, eds. Pp. 443–492. Chicago and London: University of Chicago Press. 2003 Sailing in the Wake of the Ancestors: Reviving Polynesian Voyaging. Honolulu: Bishop Museum Press. Frake, Charles O. 1995 A Reinterpretation of the Micronesian Star Compass. Journal of the Polynesian Society 104:147–158. Francois, Alexandre 2003 Of Men, Hills, and Winds: Space Directionals in Mwotlap. Oceanic Linguistics 42:407–437. Geertz, Clifford 1976 From the Native’s Point of View: On the Nature of Anthropological Understanding. In Meaning in Anthropology. K. H. Basso and H. A. Selby, eds. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press. Genz, Joseph 2008 Marshallese Navigation and Voyaging: Re-learning and Reviving Indigenous Knowledge of the Ocean. PhD dissertation, Department of Anthropology, University of Hawai‘i. Feinberg and Genz George, Marianne 1998 The Return of Lata: Building An Authentic Polynesian Voyaging Canoe. Sea History (Spring Issue):40–42. 1999 The Return of Lata. Sailing New Zealand (38):48–53. n.d. Ways of Knowing the Ocean in Marshallese Navigation. Paper presented to the annual meeting of the American Anthropological Association. November 17–21, 2010. New Orleans, LA. Gibson, James 1979 The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Gladwin, Thomas 1970 East Is a Big Bird: Navigation and Logic on Puluwat Atoll. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Goodenough, Ward H. 1953 Native Astronomy in the Central Caroline Islands. Museum Monographs. Philadelphia: University Museum, University of Pennsylvania. Goodenough, Ward H., and Stephen D. Thomas 1987 Traditional Navigation in the Western Pacific. Expedition 19(3):2–14. Hallowell, A. Irving 1955 The Self in its Behavioral Environment. In Culture and Experience. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Pp. 75–110. Haugen, Einar 1969 The Semantics of Icelandic Orientation. In Cognitive Anthropology. S. Tyler, ed. Pp. 330–342.New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. (Original 1957) Heft, Harry 1996 The Ecological Approach to Wayfinding: A Gibsonian Perspective. In The Construction of Cognitive Maps. J. Portugali, ed. Pp. 105–132. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic. Hertz, Robert 1973 The Pre-eminence of the Right Hand: A Study in Religious Polarity. In Essays on Dual Symbolic Classification. R. Needhan, ed. Pp. 3–31.Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (Original: 1909) Hirst, William 1995 Cognitive Aspects of Consciousness. In The Cognitive Neurosciences. M. S. Gazzaniga, ed. Pp. 1307–1319. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Hubbard, Edward M., and V. S. Ramachandran 2005 Neurocognitive Mechanisms of Synesthesia. Neuron 48(3):509–520. Hutchins, Edwin 1983 Understanding Micronesian Navigation. In Mental Models. D. Gentner and A. Stevens, eds. Pp. 191–226. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 1995 Cognition in the Wild. Cambridge: MIT Press. Ingold, Tim 2000 The Perception of the Environment: Essays in Livelihood, Dwelling and Skill. London and New York: Routledge. Kyselka, Will 1987 An Ocean in Mind. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press. • Limitations of Language 349 Langston, Rosamund F., James A. Ainge, Jonathan J. Couey, Cathrin B. Canto, Tale L. Bjerknes, Menno P. Witter, Edvard I. Moser, and May-Britt Moser 2010 Development of the Spatial Representation System in the Rat. Science 328:1576–1580. Lauer, Matthew, and Shankar Aswani 2009 Indigenous Ecological Knowledge as Situated Practices: Understanding Fishers’ Knowledge in the Western Solomon Islands. American Anthropologist 111(3):317– 329. Lave, Jean 1988 Cognition in Practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Levinson, Stephen C. 2003 Space in Language and Cognition: Explorations in Cognitive Diversity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Lewis, David 1972 We, the Navigators: The Ancient Art of Landfinding in the Pacific. Honolulu: University of Hawai‘i Press. Lock, Margaret M. 1993 Cultivating the Body: Anthropology and Epistemologies of Bodily Practice and Knowledge. Annual Review of Anthropology 22:133–155. Mauss, Marcel 1973 [1935] The Techniques of the Body. Economy and Society 2:70–88. 1979 [1950] Sociology and Psychology: Essays. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. Merleau-Ponty, Maurice 1962 [1945] Phenomenology of Perception. Translated, Colin Smith. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. Montello, Daniel R. 2005 Navigation. In The Cambridge Handbook of Visuospatial Thinking. P. Shah and A. Miyake, eds. Pp. 257–294. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Needham, Rodney, ed. 1973 Essays on Dual Symbolic Classification. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Pyrek, Cathleen Conboy 2011 The Vaeakau-Taumako Wind Compass: A Cognitive construct for Navigation in the Pacific. MA thesis, Kent State University. Ritchie, Jane, and James Ritchie 1989 Socialization and Character Development. In Developments in Polynesian Ethnology. A. Howard and R. Borofsky, eds. Honolulu: University of Hawai‘i Press. Scheper-Hughes, Nancy, and Margaret M. Lock 1987 The Mindful Body: A Prolegomenon to Future Work in Medical Anthropology. Medical Anthropology Quarterly 1: 6–41. Schneider, David M. 1968 American Kinship: A Cultural Account. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Sharp, Andrew 1957 Ancient Voyagers in the Pacific. Harmondsworth, Middlesex: Penguin Books. 350 American Anthropologist • Vol. 114, No. 2 • June 2012 Shore, Bradd 1996 Culture in Mind: Cognition, Culture, and the Problem of Meaning. New York, Oxford: Oxford University Press. Strathern, Marilyn 1988 The Gender of the Gift: Problems with Women and Problems with Society in Melanesia. Berkeley: University of California Press. Thomas, Stephen D. 1987 The Last Navigator. New York: Henry Holt and Co. Tolman, Edward C. 1948 Cognitive Maps in Rats and Men. Psychological Review 55:189–208. Tversky, Barbara 2003 Navigating by Mind and by Body. In Spatial Cognition III: Routes and Navigation, Human Memory and Learning, Spatial Representation and Spatial Reasoning. C. Freksa, W. Brauer, C. Habel, K. F. Wender, eds. Pp. 1–10. Berlin: Springer Verlag. Tyler, Stephen, ed. 1969 Cognitive Anthropology. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Vaka Taumako Project n.d. Vaka Taumako Project web site <http://www.pacifictraditions.org/vaka/> Wills, Tom J., Francesca Cacucci, Neil Burgess, and John O’Keefe 2010 Development of the Hippocampal Cognitive Map in Preweanling Rats. Science 328:1573–1576. FOR FURTHER READING (These selections were made by the American Anthropologist editorial interns as examples of research related in some way to this article. They do not necessarily reflect the views of the author.) Garcı́a-Quijano, Carlos G. 2007 Fishers’ Knowledge of Marine Species Assemblages: Bridging between Scientific and Local Ecological Knowledge in Southeastern Puerto Rico. American Anthropologist 109(3):529–536. Hinojosa, Servando Z. 2002 “The Hands Know”: Bodily Engagement and Medical Impasse in Highland Maya Bonesetting. Medical Anthropology Quarterly 16(1):22–40. Samudra, Jaida Kim 2008 Memory in Our Body: Thick Participation and the Translation of Kinesthetic Experience. American Ethnologist 35(4):665–681. Watson-Gegeo, Karen Ann 2005 A Different World: Embodied Experience and Linguistic Relativity on the Epistemological Path to Somewhere. Anthropology of Consciousness 15(2): 1–23. Wilf, Eitan 2010 Modernity, Creativity, and Embodied Practice in American Postsecondary Jazz Education. American Ethnologist 37(3):563–582.