Validating a human behavioral economic choice paradigm for assessing food demand among post-weight loss surgery patients Shannon Clark¹, Summar Reslan¹, Karen K. Saules¹, and Mark K. Greenwald² Eastern Michigan University, Department of Psychology¹ Wayne State University, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurosciences² Introduction Most human behavioral economic research has focused on drug choice. For example, within this theoretical framework, the reinforcing value of drugs has been assessed by measuring the amount of work a participant is willing to do to gain access to a preferred drug. Recently, we validated this approach in a human laboratory model of food-appetitive behavior (Reslan, Saules, & Greenwald, in press). We now expand this method to post-weight loss surgery (WLS) patients, because food choice may impact weight regain after WLS. Method Participants (N = 11) were at least 24 months post-WLS and all were above the 50% Excess Weight Loss (and maintained) criteria. Most (n=10) had undergone Roux-en-Y WLS, and one had undergone the gastric sleeve procedure. Participants completed a screening survey to ensure that those recruited would be non-pregnant (to avoid pregnancy-related effects on food preference) and without carpal tunnel syndrome (because the laboratory task involved computer “mouse”-pressing). All participants liked and were non-allergic to all food items offered during experimental sessions. Participants completed two sessions that imposed exponentially increasing prices to earn the same unit amount of each food. All food was consumed post-session. Task 1: High-Fat/High-Carb (Original Pringles) vs. No-Fat/HighCarb (Fat-Free Pringles) Task 2: High-Fat/High-Carb (Original Pringles) vs. No-Fat/LowCarb (Baby Carrots) Results For Task 1, mean breakpoint for No-Fat/High-Carb chips was 7.7 times greater than for High-Fat/High-Carb chips (i.e. 693.0 vs. 93.3). For Task 2, mean breakpoint was 3.7 times greater for No-Fat/Low-Carb Carrots than for High-Fat/High-Carb chips (i.e. 571.5 vs. 154.4). As seen in the figures, demand curves fitted to the data for the group-percent choice means exhibit a parallel shift or a change in demand intensity, but no significant difference in demand elasticity (curvature). Least-squares regression was used to estimate the overall fit of the demand curves to the data. According to these analyses, demand curves were a good fit (r ² = .84 to .96). 2275 2400 2100 Response Requirement 1800 1660 1500 1220 1200 835 900 540 600 340 300 5 12 33 1 2 3 100 180 Figure 3. Successful WLS patients’ demand for High-Fat/HighCarb food versus No-Fat/Low-Carb food again indicates that the more healthful option is selected more often, regardless of price. Discussion As seen in the results, the healthful option was selected more, independent of price, by our sample of successful WLS patients. Thus, our findings provide support for the use of behavioral economic procedures to better understand outcomes following WLS. In future studies, we hope to use this method to predict WLS outcomes prospectively. Our understanding of the factors that influence outcomes following WLS is in its infancy, but our study begins to show how the parameters of food choice may relate to these outcomes. This investigation was limited by a small sample size and relatively homogeneous sample of successful WLS patients. Although we predict that unsuccessful patients will work harder for unhealthful food, we were unable to test this hypothesis. Future research should expand these findings to participants with more heterogeneous weight loss outcomes. 0 0 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 Trial Figure 1. Response requirement (i.e., number of mouse button presses) increased exponentially. Figure 2. Successful WLS patients’ demand for High-Fat/High-Carb food versus No-Fat/High-Carb food indicates that the more healthful option is selected more often, regardless of price. Contact: sclark42@emich.edu Presented at the 2012 Annual Meeting of The Obesity Society