Photographic cropping as a paradigm for experimental aesthetics Assessing the role of individual differences, colour, meaning and expertise Chris McManus, Anita Zhou, Sophie I'Anson & Lucy Waterfield UCL Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, Gower Street, London WC1E 6BT, UK Gustav Theodor Fechner’s Vorschule der Aesthetik of 1876 introduced the three main methods used in experimental aesthetics: ● The Method of Choice ● The Method of Use ● The Method of Production The least popular method, despite its theoretical attractions, has been the Method of Production, in which participants produce objects that they regard as aesthetically attractive. The problem is mainly practical, most subjects not having sufficient technical ability at drawing or other skills. Photography solves that problem. People are used to taking photographs, often making decisions on how to frame a subject in the viewfinder, or to crop a larger photograph to a smaller. People also agree that some photographs are aesthetically superior. The main practical problem in using real photography is that there are too many degrees of freedom – camera position, zoom, focus, aspect ratio, and shutter timing. These studies therefore are restricted to the cropping of photographs (and in some sense a photograph ‘crops’ the visual world). Cropping is part of the broader aesthetic problem of composition, arranging objects in relation to each other and a frame to produce an attractive outcome. Such questions were considered by Denman Ross’ Theory of Pure Design of 1907, and Rudolf Arnheim’s Art and Visual Perception of 1954, but not applied to photographs. Our computer task was designed to parallel the ways in which photographs are taken. Method Subjects firstly saw a 1024 x 768 pixel photograph on a computer screen (see figure 1). This Viewing window also contained a yellow Inclusion box, which was a part of the image that had to be included in the final crop (the ‘subject’ in some sense). A click on the mouse button opened a 512 x 384 Crop window (half the linear extent and a quarter the area) the original), through which the image could be seen. Instructions were purposely minimal, asking subjects to move the crop window until the cropped image picture looked “as good as possible”. Subjects found the task intuitive and natural (and indeed, they often also described it as fun and interesting). Fig 1: a) original image; b) Viewing Window; c & d) possible Crop Windows a) c) b) d) Results Figure 2 shows a typical set of croppings of an image by 41 non-expert subjects. The large yellow box is the bound for possible crops given the position of the inclusion box. Individual croppings are shown as pastel-coloured boxes, red squares show the centre of each subject’s crop window, and the red box is the ‘median cropping window’. Fig 2: Crop windows chosen by 41 nonexpert subjects. The five studies addressed key questions about the psychometrics of cropping. Study 1: Do subjects differ and are those differences reliable? 20 ‘croppers’ cropped 40 images, and then immediately cropped the same 40 images again. Although croppers showed large individual differences, the median crop location for images did not differ across occasions, and subjects were reliable across occasions (mean imagelevel correlations: .614 (H) and .562 (V)). Subjects find cropping an acceptable task that they carry out consistently. Studies 2 & 3: Are some people better croppers than others (i.e. their crops are generally preferred aesthetically)? 21 new subjects – ‘judges’ – rated sets of cropped images produced by 6 croppers from Study 1 who were chosen by cluster analysis to have maximally disparate sets of crops (A, B, C, D, E, F). On each trial, a judge scored each of 6 randomly arranged images (see figure 3b), rating one image as ‘Best’ (4), two as ‘Good’ (3), two as ‘Bad’ (2) and 1 as ‘Worst’ (1). Fig 3: a) croppings of image in Study 1; b) crops by six croppers seen by judges in Study 2 a) b) Crops made by some croppers (e.g. D & F) were significantly preferred to those of others (e.g. A & E) – see figure 4. We conclude that some people consistently produce more aesthetically satisfying crops than do other people. Fig 4: Preference of judges for crops by six different croppers. Five separate studies were carried out, with a total of 71 subjects, 61 of whom were nonexperts, and 10 were taking the MA in Photography at the Royal College of Art. a) b) i.mcmanus@ucl.ac.uk Study 3: This study replicated the important conclusion of Study 2 with a different set of 41 judges and a paired comparison design. Cropper F was significantly preferred to cropper B who in turn was preferred to cropper E. Study 4: What is the role of colour and content in determining cropping? Sixteen images were presented in colour, in monochrome, and as ‘content-filtered’ versions (low-pass Gaussian filtered and thresholded) which naïve viewers could no longer recognise but retain the original patterns of light and dark (see fig 5). Subjects cropped all three versions of each image, some thresholded versions being presented first and others presented last. Fig 5: Croppings of colour, monochrome and thresholded versions of an image There were few differences in median cropping position between colour and monochrome images, but large differences between monochrome and thresholded images. Within-image correlations were high for colour-monochrome pairs, but close to zero for monochrome-thresholded pairs. We conclude colour has only a minor influence on cropping position, whereas content (meaning) has a large effect. Study 5: Do expert photographers crop differently from non-experts? Fig 6: Cropping of image a) by 41 nonexperts and b) by 10 experts a) b) Median horizontal crop positions were similar for experts and non-experts, but differed significantly in the vertical direction. Experts also showed more variance in crop positions, vertically and horizontally, took longer to crop, and spent longer ‘dwelling’ on the image without moving the window. Conclusions. Photographic cropping is an ideal experimental paradigm for aesthetics. Subjects are familiar with the task, find it natural, straightforward and interesting, they differ in their choices of crop position, and are consistent in their choices. Some croppers are better than others, their crops consistently being preferred to those of other croppers. Cropping is influenced little by colour, but much more by image content. Experts crop somewhat differently, particularly in the vertical direction, and they show greater variability in their croppings. A final important advantage, not always the case in experimental aesthetics, is that experts find the task interesting, appropriate and natural, and are happy to carry it out.