An investigation of childrens perceptions of story content as elicited by three modes of presentation: the storyteller, the reader, the sound slide show by Margaret Kernan Rolando A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Education Montana State University © Copyright by Margaret Kernan Rolando (1984) Abstract: This study attempted to determine if three different modes of presenting a Story--Storyteller, book and reader, and sound slide show--represented different information potential by bringing different content to children's attention. Ninety third graders from three elementary schools in Butte, Montana, during spring 1984, were presented in small groups, with one of three presentations of the fairy tale, "The Wild Swans" and administered a paper and pencil instrument to determine their perceptions of three different types of content from the story—affective, inference and factual. This content had emerged from preliminary investigation with students and an adult panel analysis of the story text. Since there was no existing instrument to determine children's perception of the story content, a crucial preliminary was the instrument development, which (though ancillary to the original intent of the study) became a major portion of the research. As most similar cross-media research has utilized individual presentations with followup interviews for data collection, this paper and pencil instrument, more readily amenable to statistical analysis, proved a novel approach warranting further consideration, especially with different age groups and reading levels. Results indicated there was no statistically significant difference among the modes in affective content in the foreground of attention, number of and overall inferences and factual recall. There was, no difference in approximately eighty percent of the specific inferences. While no definite conclusions about media differences could be drawn, the research process yielded some relevant observations: there appeared more active student participation with book/ reader and sound slide presentations than with the storyteller. The format of the sound slide show illustrations may have influenced those specific questions evidencing response differences. Sound slide show and book/ reader presentations may not differ enough to elicit different perceptions, although this bears further investigation. The following cross-media research is also suggested: further research into the development of suitable paper-pencil instruments; continued investigation into the possible differences in information potential of different media, problem solving fostered, preferences related to learning style, long-term effects, and delineation of specific media attributes. AN INVESTIGATION OF CHILDREN'S PERCEPTIONS OF STORY CONTENT AS ELICITED BY THREE MODES OF PRESENTATION THE STORYTELLER, THE READER, THE SOUND SLIDE SHOW by Margaret Kernan Rolando A the sis submitted in p a r tia l f u l f i l l m e n t of the requirements f o r the degree of Doctor of Education MONTANA STATE UNIVERSITY Bozeman, Montana December 1984 APPROVAL o f a the sis submitted by Margaret Kernan Rolando This thesis has been read by each member of the the sis committee and has been found to be s a tis fa c to r y regarding content, English usage, format, c i t a t i o n s , b ib lio g ra p h ic s ty ld , and consistency, and is ready fo r submission to the College of Graduate Studies. Date Dr. Hen^y Worrest Chairperson, Graduate Committee Approved fo r the Major Department Date Dr. Henty Worrest Head, Major Department Approved fo r the College of Graduate Studies t-Z-tr Date Dr. Michael Malone, Graduate Dean iii STATEMENT OF' PERMISSION TO USE In presenting t h i s the sis in p a r tia l f u l f i l l m e n t o f the require­ ments fo r a doctoral degree at Montana State U n iv e rs ity , I agree that the Library shall make i t ava ila b le to borrowers under rules of the L ib ra ry . I fu r th e r agree th a t copying of th is thesis is allowable only f o r scholarly purposes, consistent w ith " f a i r use" as prescribed in the U.S. Copyright Law. Requests fo r extensive copying or reproduction of t h i s thesis should be referred to U n iv e rs ity Microfilms In te rn a tio n a l, 300 North Zeeb Road, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48106, to whom I have granted "the exclusive r ig h t to reproduce and d is t r ib u t e copies of the dis s e r­ t a t io n in and from m ic ro film and the r ig h t to reproduce and d is t r ib u t e by abstract in any form at." Si gnature Date V ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would l i k e to express my g ra te fu l appreciation to the fo llo w in g people, without whose continued support and help t h is d is s e rta tio n could not have been completed. Dr. Henry Worrest, my doctoral com­ m ittee chairperson, has been a tru s te d advisor and constant source of encouragement since the beginning of my doctoral studies. Dr. Larry Ellerbruch has motivated me to pursue c re a tiv e research and spent long hours helping w ith my research design and s t a t i s t i c a l analysis. Dr. Janis Bruwelheide has provided professional and personal support and encouragement in the p u rs u it of my goals. To these and to a ll the s t a f f of the Montana State U n iv e rs ity Department of Secondary Educa­ tio n and Foundations and to. a l l my committee, I am g r a t e f u lly indebted. My studies could not possibly have been undertaken and completed w ithout the u n fa ilin g support of my fam ily and frie nds to whom I w i l l always be g r a te f u l. I e s p e c ia lly want to thank my c h ild r e n , Peter and Anna, and my parents, Raphael and MaeKernan, fo r t h e i r tremendous s a c r ific e s on my behalf. I wish also to thank Butte School D i s t r i c t Number One, especially Curriculum D ire c to r Dr. Tim S ullivan and former superintendent Mr. Bi 11 M illig a n , f o r supporting me in pursuing my doctoral studies and in undertaking th is research, and a ll the students and s t a f f who p a r t i c i ­ pated in t h is study. F in a lly , I want to thank Lee Dolan, my t y p i s t , f o r her consis­ t e n t l y exc e lle n t work on t h is manuscript. vi ■TABLE OF CONTENTS ■ . Page APPROVAL............................ ................ ................................................ ............... ii STATEMENT OF PERMISSION TO USE............ ..................................................... iii VITA...................................................................................................................... iv ACKNOWLEDGMENTS..................................... ......................................................... v TABLE OF CONTENTS................... LIST OF TABLES...... .......................................................................................... vi ix ABSTRACT............................ ... x 1. I INTRODUCTION............................... Statement of the Problem......................................... Need f o r the Study......................................... .............. ............ Purpose of the Study....................... Questions to be Answered................................................. General Procedures....................... D e lim ita tio n s and L im ita tio n s ..................... D e fin itio n of T e r m s ..................................... Summa r y ................................................................ 2. REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE....................................... In tro d u c tio n ............................. H is to ric a l and Comprehensive Overview......................... Findings of Educational Media Research and Cross-Media S tudies................... S pe c ific A ttrib u te s and C h a ra c te ris tic s of Media . . . . . . . S to rie s , Language and S t o r y t e l l i n g . ............................... L ite ra r y Genre - The Fairy T a l e . . . . ........ .................... . Summary.......................................................... 3. PROCEDURES........................................... In t r o d u c tio n ................................ Review o f the L it e r a t u r e ............................. Selection of the S to ry ............. Production of the Sound S lide Show........................................ Approaches P ilo ted fo r Analyzing the Story and Developing the Instrument......................... "Brainstorming" A n a ly s is ..................... Interview Approach...................... ........................ ; ........... 6 7 9 IO 11 13 14 17 20 20 21 24 .29 37 42 44 45 45 46 47 49 51 51 55 v ii TABLE OF CONTENTS ( c o n t i n u e d ) Page 4. Analysis and Development o f Instrument from the Text.......................... .................................................... M ethodology................................................ Preparation of Story Presentation and Sound SIide Show.......................................... ................ .............. Story Analysis and Development of the In s tru m e n t.. Population and Sampling Procedures.............. ............. P i lo t of the I n s t r u m e n t . . . . . .......................................... Experimental Treatment and Control fo r Contaminating V aria bles.................................................... Data C o lle c tio n ................................................................... Research Questions and S t a t i s t i c a l Hypotheses.................. Description of Independent and Dependent V a r i a b l e s . . . . . Instrumentation and Analysis of the Data............................ Organization o f Data................. P ilo t Data from Third Grade Classes............................ P ilo t Data from Sixth Grade Class.............................. Experimental Treatment Data—Part One: A ffe c tiv e and N on a ffe ctive Content Choices.................................. Experimental Treatment Data--Part Two: Inferences........................................................................ Experimental Treatment Data--Part Three: Factual R e c a ll......................................'.............................. Permissions..................................................................................... Summary...................... Chapter 3 Endnotes....................................................................... 65 66 67 69 69 70 70 71 STATISTICAL ANALYSIS OF DATA................................................... 77 In tro d u c tio n ............... Organization of Chapter Four............... Overview o f P ilo t and Determination of R e l i a b i l i t y ........ Third Grade P ilo t Data................. Part One - A ffe c tiv e Content.......... ..................... Part Two - Inferences..................... Part Three - Factual R e c a ll............................. P ilo t Data - Sixth Grade Students................................ Part One - A ffe c tiv e Content................................ Part Two - Inferences............... Part Three - Factual R e c a ll.................................. Experimental Data......................................................................... Overview............... Part One - A ffe c tiv e Content.......................................... Part Two - Inferences........................................................ Part Three - Factual R e c a ll........................................... Summary................................................................................... Chapter 4 Endnotes............................................................. 77 78 80 81 81 83 84 87 88 88 89 92 92 93 95 100 102 105 56 59 59 59 61 63 72 72 73 73 73 76 viii TABLE OF CONTENTS ( c o n t i n u e d ) Page 5. DISCUSSION, CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS................. iok In tro d u c tio n .................................................................................. Organization.......... .............................. ........................................ Discussion of the P ilo tin g of the Instrument.................... Discussion of A ffe c tiv e Content in the Foreground o f A tte n tio n ......................................................................... .. Discussion o f Inference Questions.......................................... Discussion of Factual Content in the Foreground of A tte n tio n ........................................... Conclusions......................................................... Observations and In te r p r e ta tio n s ........................................*. Recommendations f o r Further Research............................. Educational Im p lic a tio n s ........................................................... Summary............................................................................................. 114 115 117 119 121 123 REFERENCES CITED..................................................................................... 125 APPENDICES................................................................................................. 130 Appendix A - P ilo t M a te ria l.................................................... Brainstormed Content........ ............. .................................. A ffe c tiv e Content................................................................ Content from Guided Brainstorm ing........................... Instrument Directoy from Text........................................ Instrument: Inferences about A ffe c tiv e C o n te n t... Appendix B - Final Instrument and Story T e s t.. . . . . . . . . . Final Instrument....... ........................................................ Story T ext...................................................................... ... Appendix C - In s tru c tio n s to Treatment Group and L o g ... Appendix D - Standardized Data on Comparable Student : Groups....................... .. ................... ................ . Appendix E - P e r m is s io n s .... ............................. ................... 106 107 108 110 112 131 132 135 137 140 144 ■ 147 148 154 160 164 166 ix ' LIST OF TABLES Table I Page Third Grade A ffe c tiv e Content Choices Pearson r C orrelation C o e ffic ie n t.......... , .......................................... 82 II Third Grade P ilo t Inference Questions - Chi Square........ 85 III • Third Grade P i lo t Inference Questions - Chi Square w ith Yates' C o rre c tio n .................................................................. IV V VI V II V III 86 Third Grade Factual Recall Pearson r C orrelation C o e f f i c i e n t . . ................................................................................. 87 Sixth Grade A ffe c tiv e Content Choices Pearson r ■ C orrelation C o e ffic ie n t......................... 88 Sixth Grade Inference Questions Pretest to Post Test - Choice "3" O m itted................................................ 90 Sixth Grade Inference Questions Pretest to Post Test - Includes Choice " 3 " ........................................................... 91 Sixth Grade Factual Recall Pearson r C orrelation C o e ffic ie n t...................................................................... 92 A ffe c tiv e Content Choices by Story Treatment D e s c rip tiv e s ................... 93 X A ffe c tiv e Content Choices by Story Treatment - ANOVA. . . 94 XI Students' t Test - Book and R eader/S toryteller Comparison x Number A ffe c tiv e Content Choices.................. 95 XII Inference Choice to No Inference Choice by Mode............. 96 X III Chi Square and Response Pattern - Inference' Question #9............................................................................ 97 Chi Square and Response Patterns - Inference Questions #9, #12, and #15............... 98 XV R e l i a b i l i t y o f Inference Questions #9, #12, and # 1 5 . . . . 99 XVI Factual Recall by Story Treatment - D e s c rip tiv e s ............ 101 XVII Factual Recall by Story Treatment - ANOVA........................... 101 XVIII Students' t Test - Comparison x Number Correct Factual Recall Items...................................................................... IX XIV 102 X ABSTRACT This study attempted to determine i f three d if f e r e n t modes of presenting a S to r y - - S to r y te ll e r , book and reader, and,sound s lid e show--represented d if f e r e n t inform ation p o te n tia l by bringing d i f f e r ­ ent content to c h ild re n 's a tte n tio n . Ninety t h ir d graders from three elementary schools in Butte, Montana, during spring 1984, were pre­ sented in small groups, w ith one of three presentations of the f a i r y t a l e , "The Wild Swans" and administered a paper and pencil instrument t o determine t h e i r perceptions of three d if f e r e n t types of content from the s to ry —a f f e c t iv e , inference and fa c t u a l. This content had emerged from p relim ina ry in v e s tig a tio n w ith students and an adult panel analysis of the story t e x t . Since there was no e x is tin g instrument to determine c h ild r e n 's perception of the story content, a c ru c ia l prelim inary was the i n ­ strument development, which (though a n c illa r y to the o r ig in a l in te n t of the study) became a major po rtion of the research. As most s im ila r cross-media research has u t i l i z e d in d iv id u a l presentations w ith fo llo w ­ up interview s fo r data c o lle c t io n , t h is paper and pencil instrument, more re a d ily amenable to s t a t i s t i c a l an alysis, proved a novel approach warranting fu r th e r con sideration , e s p e c ia lly with d if f e r e n t age groups and reading le v e ls . Results indicated there was no s t a t i s t i c a l l y s ig n if ic a n t d i f f e r ­ ence among the modes in a ffe c tiv e content in the foreground of atte n ­ t i o n , number of and o v e ra ll inferences and factual r e c a ll. There was, no d iffe re n c e in approximately eighty percent of the s p e c ific i n f e r ­ ences. While no d e f i n i t e conclusions about media d iffe ren ces could be drawn, the research process yielded some relevant observations: there appeared more active student p a r tic ip a tio n w ith book/ reader and sound s lid e presentations than w ith the s t o r y t e l l e r . The format of the sound s lid e show i l l u s t r a t i o n s may have influenced those s p e c ific questions evidencing response d iffe re n c e s . Sound s lid e show and book/ reader presentations may not d i f f e r enough to e l i c i t d if f e r e n t percep­ tio n s , although th is bears fu r th e r in v e s tig a tio n . The fo llo w in g cross-media research is also suggested: fu rth e r research in to the development of s u ita b le paper-pencil instruments; continued in v e s tig a tio n in to the possible differences in information p o te n tia l of d if f e r e n t media, problem solving fostere d, preferences related to learning s ty le , long-term e ff e c ts , and d e lin e a tio n of s p e c ific media a t t r ib u t e s . I CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION There are indeed few s itu a tio n s in contemporary education which do not incorporate some use of in s tr u c tio n a l media. The use of these media, whether 16 mm movies, f i l m s t r i p s , te le v is io n or audio record­ ings, is accepted as common pra ctice in most educational endeavors. The influence of e le c tro n ic media is even more pervasive apart from formal education. As our world has been re volutio nized by in c re a s in g ly common e le c tro n ic technologies, they have gradually come to replace many o f t h e i r nonelectronic counterparts which, s u p e rfi­ c i a l l y , may have f u l f i l l e d some of the same ends. For example, the dive rsion and entertainment of the comic book, through the medium of p r in t and i l l u s t r a t i o n , may be being replaced by the dive rsion and entertainment of the te le v is e d "Saturday morning cartoons." Children were once exposed to nursery rhymes and f a i r y ta le s which are part of our oral t r a d i t i o n through the medium of a " s t o r y t e l l e r , " or a reader w ith a p ic tu re book. parents. Often th a t in d iv id u a l was one of the c h ild 's Now i t is not uncommon to fin d a c h ild who has had only te le v is io n or movie exposure to f a i r y ta le s and f o l k l o r e . Smardo and Curry (1982) have noted the wide range of presentation media, including video and audio cassettes, cable t e le v is io n , film s and other formats, now a ffe c ts even t r a d it io n a l story hours in public l i b r a r i e s : "In view of the increasing use o f (these) m edia...system atic in v e s tig a tio n 2 o f the impact of these methods of story hour presentation upon child re n is needed" (1982:70). And in the classroom, lis te n in g w ith one's peers to the teacher read a chapter each day must now vie f o r space with "hands on" computer time and videotaped productions in the crowded school day. The effectiveness of in s tr u c tio n a l media in the educational s e ttin g has been examined through the la s t three decades by consider­ able research. In surveying a number of studies both Schramm (1977) and 01 son (1974A) have found the re s u lts to be inconclusive. Huston- Stein and Wright (1979), Salomon (1974A) and 01 son (1974) have pointed out th a t these studies may have compared the d i f f e r i n g technologies as technologies, ra th e r than comparing t h e i r c h a ra c te ris tic s or the way they represent info rm a tio n . This l a t t e r concept was popularized by Marshall McLuhan (1964) who suggested th a t j u s t as a re lia nce on p r in t may have affected people's co g n itiv e processing so also may people's basic modes of th in k in g and t h e i r c o g n itiv e processing be affected by the representational properties of e le c tro n ic technology. I f th is is the case, the d is t in c t io n between media as they are used f o r in s tr u c tio n a l purposes and as they are used in mass communi­ cation ( e . g . , commercial te le v is io n ) may be a r t i f i c i a l . Salomon (1979: 388) suggests th a t "only a ft e r one has I earned about the psychological e ffe c ts of unique, inherent a ttr ib u te s of media qua media can one ask about t h e i r u t i l i t y f o r i n s t r u c t io n . " He contends th a t i t is the sym­ b o lic , communicational diffe ren ces of media rather than t h e i r techno­ lo g ic a l differences which may make s ig n if ic a n t d iffe re n c e s .in le a rn in g . 3 Most media research has not addressed what aspects of the education process are more amenable to c e rta in kinds of media (1977). Salomon (1972A, 1979) and 01 son (1974) have hypothesized that d if f e r e n t media represent d if f e r e n t "symbol systems," i . e . , d if f e r e n t organizations of sets of symbols which s tru c tu re inform ation d i f f e r ­ e n tly and th e re fo re may represent d i f f e r e n t information p o te n tia l. Huston-Stein and Wright (1979) also suggest th a t some formal features o f a medium may be viewed as representing c e rta in mental s k i l l s or operations and th a t a media presentation may supplant a new s k i l l or c a ll upon an e x is tin g one, thereby a ffe c tin g the basic th in k in g pro­ cesses. Researchers Huston-Stein and Wright (1979), Salomon (1979) and Meringoff (1980) have attempted to address th is p o s s i b i l i t y . In com­ paring the kind of inform ation child re n derive from d i f f e r e n t media presentations of a s to r y , Meringoff (1980) found th a t those children who were presented w ith a te le v is io n version had greater re c a ll of actions and a greater re lia nce on visual content in drawing i n f e r ­ ences about the story than did those c h ild re n who were presented with a reader and p ic tu re book version. Conversely, the l a t t e r group had a greater re c a ll of story language th a t eludes visual de piction and a greater use of te x tu a l content and more outside story knowledge as a basis f o r t h e i r inferences. Gardner (1980) has c ite d research stemming from the same Project Zero research at Harvard which supported M e rin g o ffs study in which c h ild re n were s im ila r ly presented w ith book and reader presentations and f i l m presentations. The fin d in g s of these studies corroborated 4 M e r in g o f f s: the children exposed to the book version remembered much more of the story on t h e i r own and were b e tte r able to re c a ll informa­ tio n when they were cued. The book c h ild re n were also able to recall the precise wording and fig u re s of speech, whereas the te le v is io n children tended to paraphrase the verbal info rm a tio n . Children from both groups made inferences about the story arid reached the same conclusions, but the lin e of reasoning they used was d i f f e r e n t : the te le v is io n c h ild re n were l i k e l y to depend on what they saw, ra re ly going beyond the visual info rm a tio n , whereas the book child re n were l i k e l y to draw on t h e i r own or real world experience in making i n f e r ­ ences. There were also notable diffe ren ces in the c h ild r e n 's concep­ t io n of time and space. Most of the research has examined what Schramm (1977) has referred to as the "big media," e . g . , in s tr u c tio n a l te le v is io n , 16 mm movies, computer assisted in s tr u c tio n , those media which represent more sophis­ tic a te d technology, and concomitantly, a greater expense to the educa­ tio n a l consumer. There have been few studies dealing w ith what Schramm referred to as the " l i t t l e media," e . g . , f i l m s t r i p s , s lid e s , audio recordings. Yet these media are used extensively throughout education. While there have been studies comparing a ttitu d e changes e l i c i t e d by various media presentations, e . g . , C roft et a l . (1969), and w hile, as Huston-Stein and Wright (1979) have pointed out, there has been considerable research on the s o c ia liz in g aspects of mass media t e l e ­ v is io n , there have been few, i f any, studies th a t have attempted to d i r e c t l y determine the effectiveness of d if f e r e n t media in tra n s m ittin g emotional or a ff e c tiv e content. 5 The fo llo w in g , th e re fo re , is the background upon which t h is study is based: d if f e r e n t media may represent d if f e r e n t inform ation poten­ t i a l which may, w ith repeated, continual exposure, a ff e c t basic modes of th in k in g and c o g n itiv e processing. These media which transm it inform ation may be p r i n t , t e le v is io n , ra d io , movies, f i l m s t r i p s , or people. For example, both a s t o r y t e l l e r and a reader w ith a pictu re book are examples of media possessing p a r tic u la r a tt r ib u t e s through which inform ation may be tra n s m itte d . Audiovisual media are common­ place in many educational s e ttin g s and have, in some cases, replaced reliance on more t r a d it io n a l media such as p r in t or l i v e presentation. While research has attempted to determine whether or not audiovisual media is e ff e c tiv e in teaching, there has been l i t t l e research on determining what, i f any, p o te n tia l f o r psychological e f f e c t , whether on information processing, problem solving or a ffe c tiv e responses, d if f e r e n t media presentations might have. I t is possible, as Marshall McLuhan (1964) has s ta te d , th a t the medium is the message: th a t i s , the medium through which inform ation is conveyed may be more in flu e n ­ t i a l than the actual content th a t is tra n s m itte d . However, as Salomon ( I 974A:393-394) has noted, t h i s hypothesis has received l i t t l e in v e s tig a tio n : actual "When roughly the same idea is represented in two d i f ­ fe re n t symbol systems, is the same meaning obtained from the messages? There are no cle a r research fin d in g s on t h i s p o in t." Furthermore, most media research has focused on the "big media" such as movies and in s tr u c tio n a l t e le v is io n . There has been l i t t l e on the simpler media such as f i l m s t r ip s and s lid e s . Yet these media pre­ sentations are commonplace in schools and many commercially produced 6 versions of s to r ie s , f a i r y ta le s and fo lk ta le s are now a v a ila b le . For example, the Caldecott Medal books which are chosen annually as the best i l l u s t r a t e d c h ild r e n 's p ic tu re books published in the United States (Arbuthnot and Sutherland, 1972) have been made in to a sound f l i m s t r i p series by the Weston Woods Company of Weston, Connecticut. I f these various media do represent d i f f e r e n t inform ation p o te n tia l, then i t might be hypothesized th a t such d if f e r e n t presentations might e l i c i t q u a lit a t iv e ly d i f f e r e n t responses from the viewers, and th a t d if f e r e n t inform ation may be being brought to the foreground of the audience's a tte n tio n . For example, both Shannon (1979) and Ross (1980) suggest that s t o r y t e l l i n g is a medium characterized by i t s richness in immediate p a r tic ip a tio n and personal contact. I t might be hypothesized that personal contact is a s a lie n t a tt r ib u t e c h a ra c te ris tic of th a t medium, and as such, i t might be an e ff e c tiv e tra n s m itte r of a ff e c tiv e content. The determination of such a ttr ib u te s and the concomitant coding of the inform ation they tra n s m it could be valuable to educational theory and to the p ra c tic a l use of the various media. Statement of the Problem In consideration of the preceding discussion, t h is researcher has inve stigate d whether or not d if f e r e n t kinds of content, i . e . , a ffe c tiv e versus n o n a ffe c tive, number of and s p e c ific inferences, and factual content, are brought to the foreground of t h i r d grade c h ild r e n 's a tte n ­ tio n by three media used in pu blic school language arts and l i b r a r y programs—sound s lid e show, book and reader, and s t o r y t e l l e r . 7 The types of content were o p e ra tio n a lly defined and emerged from an adult analysis of the s to ry . Instrumentation to measure c h ild re n 's perceptions of s a lie n t content was developed by th is researcher and v e r if ie d by a regional language arts expert. The three media represented what Schramm (1 977) has characterized as the less elaborate or simpler forms of media which have received little in v e s tig a tiv e study. One of these mediums, the sound s lid e presentation, approximated the f i l m s t r i p version of a storybook presen­ t a t io n , a format now used in l i b r a r y and language arts programs. t i o n a l l y , the review of the l i t e r a t u r e indicated l i t t l e Addi­ or no research in to the transmission of a ffe c tiv e content by these media. Both prac­ t i c a l l y and th e o r e t ic a lly i t was considered valuable to determine i f these media forms of a story brought to the p a r tic ip a n ts ' a tte n tio n the same type of content. Need f o r the Study The need f o r th is study orginated in the in d ic a tio n s of media researchers such as Salomon (1974A,, I 979), Olson (1979), Huston-Stein and Wright. (1979), and Gardner (1980), who have suggested th a t media research has produced inconclusive re s u lts because i t has not focused on comparing d if f e r e n t a tt r ib u t e s which may code inform ation d i f f e r ­ e n tly and represent d if f e r e n t inform ation p o te n tia l. This study attempted to compare three media (sound s lid e show, book and reader, and s t o r y t e l l e r ) on the a t t r i b u t e of type of content each medium brings to the foreground of a tte n tio n . S pecific types o f content included a ffe c tiv e and n o naffective, inferences and factual content. 8 This study attempted to deal w ith ra ther narrow and s p e c ific aspects of a more general and th e o re tic a l problem described by Salomon (1979). He suggested th a t research in to the educational effectiveness o f media is inappropriate u n til the psychological e ffe c ts of the char­ a c t e r i s t i c a ttr ib u te s and representational q u a litie s of media can be I determined. This consideration would seem to be important since audio­ visual media presentations are used in education and now cover the gamut of the curriculum includ ing story and l i t e r a t u r e presentations which often transm it values and/or a ff e c tiv e content. Furthermore, as A llen (1980) has pointed out, in s p ite of the use of media in the schools, actual media education, i . e. , the development o f media l i t e r ­ acy s k i l l s , has a c tu a lly declined. While t h is study has not d e alt d i r e c t l y w ith the construct of symbol systems, i t has been t h e o r e t ic a lly based in part on the recog­ n it io n th a t researchers such as Olson (1974) have stated, i . e . , th a t educational and psychological theory are lim ite d by the lack of a theory o f the stru ctu re s pervading our environment and the cog nitive consequences of being exposed to those symbols and t h e i r tra n s m ittin g media. D ir e c tly related to t h i s study was M e rin g o ffs (1980) suggestion th a t i f a given medium brings s p e c ific kinds of story content to the foreground of a c h i l d 's a tte n tio n ( f o r example, a ff e c tiv e or a c tio n ), then w ith repeated exposure to th a t medium the c h ild may accumulate experience w ith th a t kind of inform ation and may come to use and prefer i t to other kinds. 9 A n c illa ry to these considerations was a problem th a t Schramm (1977) has pointed out: there has been l i t t l e research on the simpler media such as f i l m s t r ip s and s lid e s , which are nonetheless in prevalent use in education. This study has focused on three of the simpler media forms—sound s lid e show, book and reader, and s t o r y t e l l e r —which may be found in education, p a r t ic u la r ly in language arts and l i b r a r y s e ttin g s . A d d itio n a lly , th is study has used two approaches to media research not used c o n junctive ly in previous cross-media studies: the use of a paper and pencil instrument fo r data c o lle c tio n and the use of small groups of subjects f o r the presentations. M e rin g o ffs study (1 978), f o r example, used in d iv id u a liz e d presentations and an in te rv ie w tech­ nique fo r data c o lle c t io n . Smardo and C urry's in v e s tig a tio n (1982) in to the effectiveness of d if f e r e n t story hour presentations on pre­ school c h ild r e n 's receptive language has more nearly approached the kind of story s e ttin g th a t would be used in schools and l i b r a r i e s , s p e c if ic a lly presentations made to groups of c h ild re n . The f e a s i b i l i t y of a c tu a lly using groups of children in a normal school s e ttin g with a paper and p e n c il, re a d ily administered instrument, has not previously been in v e s tig a te d . Purpose of the Study This study attempted to in v e s tig a te whether or not there were d iffe ren ces in t h i r d grade c h ild r e n 's perceptions of story content th a t was brought to the foreground of a tte n tio n by three modes of presenta­ tio n : a sound s lid e presentation (comparable to a f i l m s t r i p ) , a reader and p ic tu re book presentation, and a s t o r y t e l l e r presentation. 10 The types of content included " a f fe c tiv e " and " n o n a ffe c tive" as o p e ra tio n a lly defined, emerging from an analysis of the t e x t of the s to ry . Instrumentation to determine which type of content was brought to the foreground of a tte n tio n was developed by the researcher and has been delineated in the methodology section of Chapter Three, which des­ cribes the prelim inary research th a t was done p r io r to t h is study. Questions to be Answered This study attempted to answer the fo llo w in g questions: 1. What are the fin d in g s of media research in the la s t three decades relevant to understanding the e ffe c t of media on inform ation p o te n tia l and c o g n itiv e processes? 2. Does each of these kinds of media, a sound s lid e presenta­ t i o n , a reader and p ic tu re book presentation, and a s to r y ­ t e l l e r presentation, bring to the foreground of c h ild re n 's a tte n tio n d if f e r e n t types of content? a) Do child re n perceive a ff e c tiv e or nonaffective content to be s a lie n t in each of these presentations? b) Do c h ild re n viewing these d if f e r e n t presentations make q u a n tit a tiv e ly and s p e c if ic a lly the same inferences about content in the story? c) Is c h ild r e n 's re c a ll of factual content the same fo r each type of presentation? 11 General Procedures The fo llo w in g general procedures were used in th is study: 1. The researcher conducted a review of the research relevant to understanding the e ffe c t of d if f e r e n t forms of media on inform ation p o te n tia l. I n t e r d is c ip lin a r y research, speci­ f i c a l l y on c h ild r e n 's language development and the s tru c tu ra l s ig n ific a n c e of s to r ie s , also was included. This background was used in developing the th e o ry , j u s t i f i c a t i o n and research design fo r t h is study. 2. The f a i r y t a le . The Wild Swans, i l l u s t r a t e d by Susan J e ffe rs and re to ld by Amy E hrlich (Andersen, 1981), was selected fo r t h i s study on the c r i t e r i a of genre, s u i t a b i l i t y fo r s to ry ­ t e l l i n g , com parability, q u a lity of the i l l u s t r a t i o n , size of the book, and a u th e n tic ity o f the m a te ria l. 3. A sound s lid e presentation of the book, narrated by the s t o r y t e l l e r and using one hundred nine 35 mm s lid e s , selected on t h e i r representation of the story lin e and a u th e n tic ity in portraying the i l l u s t r a t i o n s of the book, was developed by the researcher. 4. During the f a l l of 1982, the researcher p ilo te d three approaches to analyzing the story to delineate the content and develop an instrument which would provide a measure fo r the research questions. Chapter Three. These approaches are described in On the basis of t h i s p i l o t i n g , one approach, a content analysis and development of the instrument d i r e c t l y from the t e x t , was selected f o r use in t h is study. 12 5. The te x t of the story was submitted to a panel of three pub­ l i c school language a rts and/or English teachers fo r analysis o f a ffe c tiv e and nonaffective content in the story t e x t . 6. A th re e -p a rt instrument was developed by the w r i t e r . The f i r s t part consisted of questions about the type of content in the foreground of a tte n tio n . The second part consisted o f inference questions about the s to ry . tained factual re c a ll content. The t h i r d part con­ The instrument was submitted to a regional expert in the language a rts f i e l d to a u th e n ti­ cate the construct v a l i d i t y of the o p e ra tio n a lly defined content and the ove ra ll face v a l i d i t y o f the instrument. 7. The s t o r y t e l l e r selected for. the two l i v e presentations and f o r na rra ting the sound s lid e show was chosen because of her experience in drama and th e a te r groups, her experience in working w ith child re n and her in te r e s t in and enthusiasm f o r the study and s t o r y t e l l i n g experiences. 8. The instrument was p ilo te d w ith two t h i r d grade classes and one s ix th grade class from a school not used in the actual study to determine the instrum ent's r e l i a b i l i t y ( s t a b i l i t y ) . 9. Three schools from the nine elementary schools in the Butte School D i s t r i c t Number One were determined comparable on the basis of standardized achievement scores. From each of the three schools, a random sample of t h i r t y t h i r d graders was selected. The students were randomly assigned to one o f the three modes of presentation. There were ten in each 13 group w ith equal numbers of boys and g i r l s , except in one school in which there were only fourteen t h ir d grade boys. 10. The presentations were made to the groups in the spring of . 1984. Conditions fo r presentation were k e p t. as s im ila r as p ra c tic a b le given the actual school s e ttin g s . 11. The instrument was administered immediately fo llo w in g the presentations. 12. The re s u lts obtained were analyzed and reported in th is study. 13. Al I proper a u tho rization s and permissions were obtained before the study was a c tu a lly begun. D e lim ita tio n s and Lim ita tio n s I This study was r e s tr ic te d in the fo llo w in g ways: 1. A m a jo rity of the sources f o r the review o f the l i t e r a t u r e was obtained from the Renne L ib rary at Montana State Univer­ s ity . Additional sources were obtained through i n t e r l i b r a r y loan services and a personal l i b r a r y . 2. A b i l i t y to draw ge neralization s from the fin d in g s was res­ t r i c t e d by the population th a t was sampled f o r the study. Such r e s tr ic tio n s are appropriate and inherent to exploratory research.. Future research w ith other population groups would be recommended f o r increasing the g e n e ra lity of such findings 3. While an attempt was made to choose a s u ita b le story fo r presentation, the study was nonetheless lim ite d by th a t s e le c tio n . 14 4. The study was lim ite d by the s t o r y t e l l e r 's in te rp r e ta tio n of the story and the inherent v a r i a b i l i t y in communicating th a t in te r p r e ta t io n . 5. While a serious attempt was made to produce a s lid e show of comparable q u a lity to the other presentations, the d if f e r e n t natures of the media precluded exact comparisons. 6. The study was lim ite d to the types of content th a t were oper­ a tio n a lly defined and th a t were rated in the instrum entation. 7. While an attempt was made to control fo r contaminating v a ria ­ bles during the presentations, the use of the actual school s e ttin g and the nature of the d e liv e ry systems and the sub­ je c ts precluded complete control of a l l unforeseen v a ria bles. D e fin itio n of Terms Certain terms were considered in the fo llo w in g context: Action Content, (operational d e f .) A phrase, sentence or clause containing an action verb. A f f e c t iv e . P ertaining to or re s u ltin g from emotions or fe e lin g s (The American Heritage D ic tio n a ry , s .v . " a f f e c t i v e " ) . A ffe c tiv e Content, (operational d e f.) A phrase, sentence or clause, e x p l i c i t l y s ta tin g an emotional reaction ( e . g . , was h o r r if ie d ) or s ta tin g an action th a t is d i r e c t l y associated w ith emotional reaction ( e . g . , "she wept"). Audiovisual Media. Nonprint in s tr u c tio n a l m aterials designed to teach through visual and/or auditory means (Davies, 1974: 461). 15 Brainstorm ing. A method of shared problem solving in which a l l members o f a group spontaneously co n trib u te ideas (The American Heritage D ic tio n a ry , s .v . "b ra in s to rm in g "). C ognition. The act or process of knowing in the broadest sense (Webster's Third New In te rn a tio n a l D ic tio n a ry , Una­ bridged, s .v . "c o g n itio n " ) . C o g n itiv e . Of, r e la tin g to , being, or in v o lv in g cognition (Webster's Third New In te rn a tio n a l D ic tio n a ry , Unabridged, s .v . " c o g n itiv e " ) . D escriptive Content, (operational d e f.) A phrase, sentence or clause th a t d e p ic ts , describes, without emphasizing action or emotion; phrases th a t help draw a clea rer "mental p ic tu r e ," e . g . , metaphor, s im ile . Formal A t t r i b u t e s . A c h a r a c te r is tic p e rtain ing to the form as opposed to the content of a s p e c ific medium; also re la tin g to the form in which inform ation is coded in th a t s p e c ific medium ( Huston-Stein and W right, 1979:23). Genre. A l i t e r a r y type or cla s s . Works are sometimes clas­ s i f i e d by s u b je c t...b u t the more usual c l a s s if ic a t io n is by form and treatment ( L ite ra r y Terms: "genre"). In s tru c tio n a l Media. A D ic tio n a ry , s .v . P rin t and nonprint materials designed to teach and/or accomplish educational o b je c tiv e s . Mass Media. The instruments of communication th a t reach larg e numbers of people at once w ith a common message, such as magazines, te le v is io n , radio (Davies, 1974:464). 16 Medium, p i. media. A veh icle f o r conveying info rm a tion; an agent fo r communicating ideas (Davies, 1974:464). Nonaffective Content, (operational d e f.) A phrase, sentence or clause th a t does not e x p l i c i t l y state an emotional reaction or an action d i r e c t l y associated w ith emotional re action . In th is study, there were two subcategories of o p e ra tio n a lly defined, nonaffective content: action and ' d e s c rip tiv e . Operational D e f i n it io n . "The concepts used...should be defined through a s u f f i c i e n t l y accurate d e s c rip tio n of the procedures used by the in v e s tig a to r in esta b lis h in g and te s tin g them" ( Encyclopedia of Psychology, s .v . "operational d e fin itio n " ). R e lia b ility . The extent to which in d iv id u a l differences are measured c o n s is te n tly as determined by c o e ffic ie n ts of s ta ­ b i l i t y , th a t i s , the c o rre la tio n of a set of measurements w ith themselves over a sp e c ifie d time period (Sax, 1980:630). Symbol System. A set of symbols organized to form a system of in te rre la te d options th a t c o rre la te with a f i e l d of r e fe r ­ ence, such as language, music, numbers. The d iffe re n c e in the s tru c tu re of inform ation in these systems may be d is ­ cussed in terms of inform ation p o te n tia l which may be tra n s ­ mitted through various media (Olson, 1974:12, 13). Theme, (operational d e f ) . A phrase, sentence or clause th a t expresses what the story is about, i t s main p o in t, what i t is t r y in g to t e l l about l i f e . .1 7 Vali d i t y . The extent to which measurements or items corres­ pond with c r i t e r i a . In t h is case, the extent to which measurements support the existence of an hypothesized t r a i t or a b i l i t y (Sax, 1980:634). Summary Media research in the la s t three decades has yielded inconclusive I re s u lts , yet the use of audiovisual media in educational settin gs is extensive. Researchers Salomon (1974A, 1979), Olsori (1974), Huston- Stein and Wright (1979), Meringoff (1980), and Gardner (1980), have pointed to a need f o r studies which i d e n t i f y the a ttr ib u te s and spe­ c i f i c c h a ra c te ris tic s p e c u lia r to the various media. These a ttr ib u t e s , which code and transm it in fo rm a tio n , may a ffe c t the th in k in g processes and information coding of people c o n tin u a lly exposed to a p a r tic u la r medium. As Marshall McLuhan hypothesized (1964), the medium through which the inform ation is transm itted may be more i n f l u e n t i a l than the actual message or content i t s e l f . L i t t l e research has been conducted on what Schramm (1977) termed the " l i t t l e media," f o r example, s lid e s , f i l m s t r i p s , audio recordings. Yet, these smaller and less expensive media are in common use in educa­ tio n . A f i l m s t r i p version of a s to ry , f a i r y t a le or f o l k t a l e may replace or supplement the teacher or l i b r a r i a n t e l l i n g the story or reading i t aloud. Many of these s to rie s intended fo r younger children tra nsm it a t t it u d in a l and a ff e c tiv e content which, h y p o th e tic a lI y , might be a lte red by the medium through which i t is conveyed. The purpose of th is study was to in v e s tig a te whether or not there were differences in t h i r d grade c h ild r e n 's perceptions of story content 18 th a t was brought to the foreground of a tte n tio n by three modes or pre­ sen tatio n: a sound s lid e presentation, a reader and p ic tu re book presentation, and a s t o r y t e l l e r presentation. The l a t t e r two have been characterized by the personal contact and immediacy o f the s t o r y t e l ­ l e r ' s or reader's presence. I t was hypothesized th a t each medium could bring d if f e r e n t types of content to the foreground of the c h ild re n 's a tte n tio n and th a t th is could be re fle c te d in t h e i r perceptions of s a lie n t content. The subjects f o r the study were a sample of t h i r d grade students drawn from Butte School D i s t r i c t Number One during the 1983-84 school year. A sound s lid e version of the chosen story was produced using the i l l u s t r a t i o n s from the book and the voice of the s to r y te lle r /r e a d e r . The story was presented to small groups of the subjects and t h e i r perceptions o f s a lie n t content i d e n t if ie d with a researcher designed instrument. The fin d in g s of the research have been reported in t h is study. The lim it a t io n s inherent in the nature of the study i t s e l f i n ­ cluded the choice of the s to r y , the s to r y t e lle r / r e a d e r 's in te rp r e ta tio n o f th a t s to ry , the inherent v a r i a b i l i t y in conveying th a t in te r p r e ta ­ t i o n , and v a r i a b i l i t y re s u ltin g from using real school s e ttin g s . A d d itio n a lly , while a rigorous e f f o r t was made to produce a s lid e presentation of comparable q u a lity to the s t o r y t e l l e r and the reader presentations, the natures of the media precluded exact comparisons. The study was lim ite d fu r th e r by the types o f content th a t were rated in the instrum entation. 19 Generalizations from the study were r e s tr ic te d by the fa c t th a t the sample was drawn from t h i r d grade students from a s p e c ific school population. The fo llo w in g terms, relevant to t h is study, were d e fin e d : a c tio n , a ff e c t iv e , a ffe c tiv e content, audiovisual media, brainstorming, c o g n itio n , c o g n itiv e , d e s c rip tiv e content, formal a t t r ib u t e s , genre, in s tr u c tio n a l media, mass media, medium (p. media), n o n a ffe c tive con­ t e n t , operational d e f i n i t i o n , r e l i a b i l i t y , symbol system, theme, and v a lid ity . 20 CHAPTER 2 REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE In tro d u c ti on A review o f the l i t e r a t u r e relevant to th is study revealed the fo llo w in g : considerable research has been done on audiovisual media, focusing on both general and educational considerations, and these studies have generally yielded in c o n s is te n t and inconclusive re s u lts . Current media research has focused on s p e c ific a ttr ib u te s and charac­ t e r i s t i c s of media, and cross-media comparisons are now being under­ taken to delineate the s p e c ific dimensions along which media may d iffe r. Research s p e c if ic a lly on s t o r y t e l l i n g or p ic tu re book presenta­ tio n s and types of story hour experiences has only recently begun; w hile there is considerable research on c h ild re n 's conceptualizations of s to rie s and t h e i r use of language, l i t t l e of t h is research has d ir e c t bearing on t h is study. Consequently, t h i s review is organized as fo llo w s . media research is tre ate d f i r s t . three general categories: Audiovisual This to p ic has been subdivided in to an h is to r ic a l and comprehensive overview of the research and of media education; fin d in g s of educational media research and cross-media studies; and research on the s p e c ific a t t r i ­ butes and c h a ra c te ris tic s o f media, e s p e c ia lly as these a tt r ib u t e s may 21 re la te to cog n itiv e processes and mental s k i l l s necessary to e x tra c t inform ation from presentations. The reader w i l l fin d considerable overlap among these categories. However, they have been grouped together in an attempt to provide a cogent th e o re tic a l background upon which t h i s research has been based. Since there was a paucity of a r t ic le s th a t were genuine antecedents to t h is study, i t was necessary to b u ild t h is th e o re tic a l basis on re lated research and i n t e r d is c ip lin a r y concerns. The l a t t e r part of t h is review o f the l i t e r a t u r e addresses the research and th e o re tic a l concerns related to the use of s to r ie s , la n ­ guage and s t o r y t e llin g and the l i t e r a r y genre of the f a i r y t a l e . H is to ric a l and Comprehensive Overview Although a few studies of various media antedate the 1950's, most media research has been conducted in the la s t three decades. Huston- Stein and Wright (1979), in discussing te le v is io n , have noted th a t research has s h ifte d from the medium of te le v is io n to the content of t e le v is io n : during the 1950's the research focused on the medium i t s e l f ; during the 1960's research was concerned with te le v is io n as a s o c ia liz in g agent, e s p e c ia lly as i t related to violence; in the 1970's other s o c ia liz in g aspects, such as the negative e ffe c t of a d vertisin g and stereotypes, were examined. The authors suggest th a t new research examine the forms of the medium ra ther than i t s content, an idea f i r s t popularized in the 1960's by Marshall McLuhan (1964) who suggested th a t formal properties of audiovisual media might have profound e ffe c ts on the basic modes of th in k in g and co g n itiv e processing. 22 Gardner (1980), who has also noted th a t the studies which have been done on te le v is io n have fa ile d to t e l l us about the medium per se, has proposed the fo llo w in g reasons fo r t h is deficiency in the research. F i r s t , much i n i t i a l te le v is io n research made use of methodology a p p li­ cable to older forms of media. Second, te le v is io n research has been p r a c t ic a lIy rather than t h e o r e t ic a lly oriented because of s o c ie ty 's concern w ith violence, commercials, and so on. T h ird, since almost everyone in t h is country has a te le v is io n se t, no v a lid comparison can be made between people who have te le v is io n s and those who d o n 't. One way media research can be categorized is according to purpose, s p e c i f i c a l l y , media as a form of mass communication and media as an in s tr u c tio n a l tool used to achieve some educational o b je c tiv e . How­ ever, one current researcher indicated th a t th is may be an in a p p ro p ri­ ate d is t in c t io n : "Only a f t e r one has I earned about the psychological e ffe c ts of.unique, inherent a ttr ib u te s o f media qua media can one ask about t h e i r u t i l i t y fo r in s tr u c tio n " (Salomon 1974:388). Olson and Bruner (1974) have also theorized th a t such d is tin c tio n s may be ina ppropriate: they have noted th a t education has assumed th a t knowledge was central to the educational en terprise and independent of both the form of experience from which the knowledge was derived and the goals fo r which i t was used. They have theorized th a t d iffe r e n t forms of experience may y ie ld nonequivalences, not so much in the knowl­ edge acquired but in the s k i l l s involved in e x tra c tin g or u t i l i z i n g th a t knowledge: "In s tru c tio n a l means converge as to the knowledge conveyed but they diverge as to the s k i l l s they assume and develop" (01 son and Bruner, 1974:149). 23 Schramm (1977) has reviewed and evaluated research in media according to the type of d e liv e ry system, e .'g ., in s tr u c tio n a l t e l e ­ v is io n , programmed le a rn in g , s lid e s , ra d io . He has noted th a t the bulk of the research has been done on what he terms "big media," fo r example, te le v is io n and computer assisted in s tr u c tio n . studies have been conducted on in s tr u c tio n a l te le v is io n . P rim a rily , There have been fa r fewer on the " l i t t l e media," e . g . , s lid e s , f i l m s t r i p s , radio, records. Schramm has also noted a doctoral d is s e rta tio n by D. W. S tic k e ll at Pennsylvania State U n ive rsity in 1963 which pointed to the questionableness of the experimental design o f many media stud ies. Of 250 experimental comparison studies (classroom in s tr u c tio n compared w ith te le v is io n i n s t r u c t i o n ) , Stickwel I was able to fin d only ten studies which he termed f u l l y in te rp r e ta b le , meaning th a t they had met every requirement of a rather demanding standard. F in a lly , Allen (1980) has noted th a t media education, popular in the la te 1960's and early 1970's and a tt r ib u t a b le in part to the i n f l u ­ ence of Marshall McLuhan, has la rg e ly disappeared from pu b lic school c u r r ic u la . McLuhan's Center fo r Culture and Technology at the Univer­ s i t y of Toronto was closed in 1980. The return to basics in education movement reinforced the concept th a t mass media detracts from basic lit e r a c y s k i l l s . Media education, a f f i l i a t e d with the a rts and human­ i t i e s and i n t e r d is c ip lin a r y by i t s nature, has lo s t i t s c r e d i b i l i t y as pu blic concern about diminished basic s k i l l s has led to a more compart­ mentalized and structured curriculum . A llen (1980) has noted th a t a ro le of education has been to provide students with the s k i l l s required to process and u t i l i z e inform ation to t h e i r best advantage, and th a t 24 in an age of mass media and e le c tro n ic communication, media l i t e r a c y ought s t i l l to be considered an important s k i l l . As a counterpoint to t h is view o f media education. Morrow (1979) has indicated th a t media education lo s t i t s impetus because McLuhan's i theories were d e te rm in is tic and n e g a t iv is tic in t h e i r view of human p o te n tia l as being subject to the medium i t s e l f and because l i t t l e research existed to substantiate any of the claims about media. Findings o f Educational Media Research and Cross-Media Studies Authors who have reviewed research relevant to education have a rrived at d if f e r e n t conclusions. Moldstad (1974) stated th a t studies have la rg e ly compared r e la t iv e student learning outcomes when methods incorporating in s tr u c tio n a l technology have been evaluated against t r a d it io n a l methods. In reviewing some t h i r t y studies covering various forms of media, he concluded the fo llo w in g : s ig n if ic a n t ly greater learning re s u lts when media is inte gra ted w ith t r a d it io n a l in s tr u c ­ tio n a l methods; equal amounts of learning are accomplished in less time when in s tr u c tio n a l technology is incorporated; multimedia i n ­ s tru c tio n a l programs based on a systems approach often f a c i l i t a t e more e ff e c tiv e student learning than t r a d i t i o n a l in s tr u c tio n ; and students usually prefer multimedia and/or a u d io - tu to ria l in s tr u c tio n a l programs when compared to t r a d it io n a l in s tr u c tio n . The reader should be cau­ tio n e d , however, th a t the t i t l e , "S e le c tiv e Review of Research Studies Showing Media E ffective ness," indicates th a t a r t ic le s f o r inclu s io n in the review may have been chosen because they did show the effectiveness o f media. 25 Schramm (1977) has concluded th a t research shows media can teach e ffe c tiv e ly . Most of the research has been done on what he terms "big media," p rim a rily in s tr u c tio n a l t e le v is io n . w ith simple media. Few studies have dealt In s p ite of the fa c t th a t in s tr u c tio n a l te le v is io n can t e a c h 'e ffe c tiv e ly , Schramm stated th a t there is no basis in the research fo r saying th a t students do learn more or less from te le v is io n than from classroom teaching. Furthermore, review of the research shows th a t the b e tte r the design the less l i k e l y the chance of fin d in g s ig n if ic a n t or consistent diffe ren ces when comparing learning from audiovisual media to learning from another in s tr u c tio n a l method. These comparative studies have contributed l i t t l e to a theory of media. Thus, Schramm concluded th a t learning is more affected by what is delivered than by the d e liv e ry system i t s e l f . In the in tro d u c tio n to the Seventy Third Yearbook of the National Society fo r the Study o f Education e n t i t l e d . Media and Symbols: The Forms o f Expression, Communication and Education, e d ito r 01 son (1974) stated th a t surveys of the research which compare d if f e r e n t treatments such as f i l m versus p r in t versus l i v e teachers overwhelmingly showed no s ig n if ic a n t d iffe re n c e . He indicated the impact of educational tech­ nologies is e ith e r n e g lig ib le or perhaps unknown since we d o n 't know how to assess the psychological e ffe c ts of the technologies or how to adapt them to educational purposes. Olson (1974:9) a]so stated th a t research, both educational and psychological, is severely lim ite d by the absence of a theory of the s tru c tu re of the symbols th a t make up such an important part o f our environment, the media th a t propagate those symbols, and the c o g nitive consequences of exposing children to them. 26 Salomon (1978) has summarized the media research as in d ic a tin g the fo llo w in g : a l l media can i n s t r u c t . Media may be interchangeable un­ less c r i t i c a l a tt r ib u t e s are emphasized. And, media are composed of d i f f e r e n t , d is t i n c t i v e elements, each element being a source of pos­ sible- v a r ia tio n . Salomon has also emphasized th a t i t is essential to get a deeper understanding of the components o f media and how these operate under d if f e r e n t conditions to develop theories of media in education. In looking at d i f f e r e n t mental s k i l l s th a t are necessary to ex­ t r a c t inform ation from presentations, there e x is ts much research which compares c h ild r e n 's developmental language a b i l i t i e s , i . e . , reading as opposed to lis te n in g s k i l l s . For example, D urrelI (1969) found th a t lis te n in g vocabulary is greater than reading vocabulary at a ll lower grade le v e ls . By f i f t h grade reading comprehension reaches ninety percent of lis te n in g comprehension and by eighth grade the two a b i l i t i e s are equal. While studies such as these are not real cross­ media in v e s tig a tio n s , they do in d ic a te the need to d if f e r e n t i a t e the co g n itiv e s k i l l s required to process info rm a tion. Paris and Mahoney (1974) have inve stigate d c h ild r e n 's de riv a tio n and re tentio n of meaning from pictu res and from sentences. In t h e ir s tu d y , they presented the same re la tio n s h ip s e ith e r v e rb a lly or p ic ­ to r ia l ly . Their re s u lts indicated th a t fo u rth grade c h ild re n could c o r re c tly v e r if y more assertions about p i c t o r i a l Iy than o r a lly pre­ sented info rm a tion. More s p e c ific to cross-media comparisons, Jerome Bruner (1964) has theorized th a t in young c h ild r e n 's co g n itiv e development, learning and 27 problem solving s itu a tio n s re ly heavily on visual a tt r ib u t e s . by Hayes and Birnbaum (1980) supports t h is theory. A study Their studies, conducted to assess the degree to which preschool children and adults remember the audio portions versus the video portions of popular t e l e ­ v is io n shows, indicated th a t c h ild r e n 's re tentio n of v is u a lly portrayed events was c o n s is te n tly higher than t h e i r re tentio n of auditory events. Adults, however, showed comparable re tentio n of the two types of events. Results indicated th a t preschoolers tend to ignore large parts of the audio portions of c e rta in te le v is io n programs and pay greater a tte n tio n to the visual aspects. In another cross-media study by Rohwer and Harris (1 975), para­ graphs of prose were presented o r a l l y , in p r i n t , and in pictu res e ith e r alone or in combination. Results of the study indicated th a t the degree of success w ith which inform ation is conveyed to child re n w i l l vary according to the c h i l d 's background, the type of media presenta­ t i o n , the type of inform ation to be acquired, and the type of perform­ ance used to in d ic a te a c q u is itio n . Performance in the combined media c o n d itio n s , e s p e c ia lly oral and pictu res tog eth er, was superior to performance of a sing le media condition f o r low socioeconomic black c h ild re n . However, the combinations of media were o f l i t t l e ' be nefit " in the performance of high socioeconomic white subjects. There have been few studies s p e c i f i c a l l y r e la tin g the e ffe c t of media to a s u b je c t's emotional response. Some research, such as th a t by Franklin and Kemp (1980), has indicated th a t the use of audiovisuals in the teaching of poetry may produce a greater a ffe c tiv e involvement on the part of the student, which in tu rn may f a c i l i t a t e re tentio n of 28 c o g n itiv e m a te ria l. Simonson, Thies and Burch (1979) have reviewed experimental studies dealing w ith media and a ttitu d e s published in AV Communication Review from 1953 to 1977. been four trends in media a tt it u d e research: They stated th a t there have early studies examined preferences and l i k i n g of media presentations; a second phase examined changes in preferences; a t h i r d phase focused on a tt it u d e comparisons, p a r t i c u l a r l y when mediated in s tr u c tio n was compared with t r a d it io n a l in s tr u c tio n . The current phase was id e n t if ie d as th a t of m e d ia /a tti­ tude in te ra c tio n in which media c h a ra c te ris tic s have been isolated and compared to the variables of the I earner and the learning process. They concluded the fo llo w in g : a p o s itiv e l in k exis ts between learner a ttitu d e s toward content inform ation and achievement; a preference has been found fo r mediated in s tr u c tio n ; and the re la tio n s h ip between media, a tt it u d e and achievement, i f any, has not been c le a rly estab­ lis h e d . In a follow -up a r t i c l e , Simonson (1980) concluded th a t to date there were no s p e c ific guidelines f o r generating a tt it u d e changes through a use of media. C ro ft et a l . (1969) examined a tt it u d e changes e l i c i t e d by two id e n tic a l verbal presentations, one l i v e and one videotaped. They hypothesized th a t greater a t t it u d in a l changes would re s u lt from the l i v e presentation because more cues would be ava ila ble from which inform ation could be extracted. Two hundred twenty in tro d u c to ry sociology students from Brigham Young U n iv e rs ity were pretested to id e n t i f y a ttitu d e s toward in t e r c o lle g ia te a t h le t ic s . An attempt was made to change the a ttitu d e s in a negative d ire c tio n since generally p o s itiv e a ttitu d e s existe d. Three treatment conditions were applied: 29 taped presentation, and a control s itu a tio n in which no persuasive message was presented. Results indicated the l i v e presentation pro­ duced s i g n if ic a n t l y greater a t t it u d in a l changes (pc.Ol) than did the videotaped presentation, which in turn produced greater a t t it u d in a l changes than did the control s itu a tio n (p <.0 2). C o llin s (1970) examined the in te ra c tio n between media and age in the learning o f material judged to be essential or nonessential to com­ prehending a media presentation. His subjects, one hundred s ix ty nine children in grades th re e , s i x , seven, and nine, were shown a media presentation fo r which they were given no in s tr u c tio n to lea rn. were tested afterwards on essential and nonessential content. They The re s u lts showed th a t the learning of essential content increased as a lin e a r function of age while the learning of nonessential content was found to be a c u r v ilin e a r fun ction of age. S pe c ific A ttrib u te s and C ha ra c te ris tic s of Media As noted e a r l i e r , media studies have generally produced inconclu­ sive r e s u lts . Salomon (1974), 01 son (1974) and Huston-Stein and Wright (1979) have pointed out th a t perhaps the studies have compared d i f ­ fe rin g technologies and content ra ther than the c h a ra c te ris tic s and a tt r ib u t e s th a t accompany the various media. I t is to t h i s to p ic th a t t h is section of the review has been addressed. Huston-Stein and Wright (1979) suggested th a t some formal features of a medium may be viewed as representing c e rta in mental s k i l l s or mental operations: a media presentation may require a new s k i l l or c a ll upon an e x is tin g s k i l l , operation or code. These codes could % 30 conceivably a ffe c t basic modes of th in k in g and cog n itiv e processing: in d iv id u a ls exposed to a s p e c ific medium might adopt these codes in to t h e i r own th in k in g . They suggested research in the fo llo w in g areas: d e lin e a tin g the formal features th a t a t t r a c t and hold a tte n tio n ; ascertaining whether formal features are more or less important than content in gaining and holding a tte n tio n ; and id e n tify in g any develop­ mental changes in the e ffe c ts of formal fea tures. Huston-Stein and Wright (1979) c ite d a paper, "Discrepant Social Communication," presented by F. R. Volkmar, E. L. Hoder and A. E. Siegel at the March 1977 meeting of the Society fo r Research in Child Development, which supported the predominance of auditory cues in young c h ild re n : a series of studies of c h ild re n , one to three years o ld , showed th a t when visual and verbal cues from an adult dicta te d c o n tra d ic to ry behavior, the young c h ild re n followed auditory cues more often than the visual cues. However, Hayes, Chemeski and Birnbaum (1981) have found a visual s u p e rio rity e f f e c t , i . e . , a higher retention of visual than auditory info rm a tion, under conditions of both in te n ­ tio n a l and incid e n ta l le a rn in g . Huston-Stein and Wright (1979) noted th a t papers presented by Gavriel Salomon and his colleagues at the U niversity o f Jerusalem supported the hypothesis th a t the understanding of and a b i l i t y to use common media codes increases with age and, to some ex te n t, experience. Less experienced and younger viewers benefited more from those media formats which supplanted re lated in t e lle c t u a l s k i l l s . The work also indicated th a t content messages were b e tte r comprehended when the children understood the formats used to represent the contents. 31 In a paper noted by Huston-Stein and Wright (1979) presented to the March 1979 meeting of the Society f o r Research in C hild Develop­ ment, "Recall of Television Content as a Function of Content Type and Level of Production Feature Use," the authors C alvert, Watkins, HustonStein and Wright found th a t children benefited from s a lie n t formal features (a c tio n , sound e ff e c ts , special visual t r i c k s ) , i . e . , these features helped them remember c e n t r a l , theme-related content. Further­ more, these s a lie n t formal features benefited kindergarten children more than they did t h i r d and fo u rth graders. I Salomon (1974, 1979) has addressed the f a i l u r e o f research to i d e n t i f y s ig n if ic a n t differences in what is I earned via various modes o f p r e s e n ta tio n .. Schramm (1977:273) has said th a t while a l l media can teach e f f e c t i v e l y , "lea rnin g seems to be affected more by what is delivered than by the d e liv e ry system." But Salomon (1974, 1979) has contended the research has fa ile d to address media's most essential c h a ra c te ris tic s : i t s symbol systems. A symbol system as defined by Olson (1974:12, 13) is a set of symbols so organized as to form a system of in te rre la te d options which are correlated with a f i e l d of reference, e . g . , language, music, numbers. This d iffe re n c e in the s tru c tu re of inform ation in d if f e r e n t symbolic sys­ tems conveyed by d i f f e r e n t media may be discussed in terms of inform ation p o te n tia l. Salomon has indicated th a t the d i f f e r e n t symbol systems of media c a ll fo r d if f e r e n t mental s k i l l s , which may in some cases be c u ltiv a te d and tra n s fe ra b le and may also overlap. The s p e c ific content q u a litie s and the ways the inform ation is stru cture d and presented can be d i s t i n ­ guished. "The point is th a t media of communication and in s tr u c tio n are 32 most e s s e n tia lly d is tin g u is h a b le from each other in terms of the symbol systems one can best use w ith them" (Salomon, 1979:133). S pecific to th is issue, e a rly research by Salomon (1972) addressed the question of whether the a b i l i t y to v is u a liz e a c e rta in operation and apply i t c o v e rtly to new instances can be improved by exposing a person to a presentation which c o v e rtly supplants the vis u a liz e d opera­ tio n . The studies s p e c if ic a lly focused on the zoom technique used in film in g in which the focus is moved ra p id ly away from or toward the photographic sub ject. The re s u lts of t h e i r studies suggested visual media can be used in in s tr u c tio n to supplant images of operations useful to a tta in in g c e rta in goals, but not yet s a t i s f a c t o r i l y mastered by the learners. Another early study by Salomon (1974A), again using the zoom tech­ nique but also a technique of laying out or arranging s o lid o b je c ts , also led him to conclude th a t a media modeling, i . e . , f i l m i c modeling o f a schematic operation, can lead to i t s in te r n a liz a tio n and thereby improve the a b i l i t y to use the operations as a mental s k i l l . Salomon's research (1977) has also found th a t when content is held constant f o r messages, d i f f e r e n t formats of a medium, s p e c if i­ c a lly t e le v is io n , c a ll fo r d if f e r e n t mental s k i l l s . Their studies have also approached the question of c ro s s -c u ltu ra l d iffe ren ces and the c u lt iv a t io n o f media s k i l l s (1977-ERIC, 1976). They found that a I i t e r a t e exposure to te le v is io n did c o rre la te p o s itiv e ly w ith the mas­ te r y of relevant mental s k i l l s and th a t such c o rre la tio n s were found es p e c ia lly among younger c h ild re n . Their research used the Sesame Street te le v is io n program and Is r a e li and American child re n who had 33 been d i f f e r e n t i a l l y exposed to the medium of te le v is io n . Salomon's research led him to conclude th a t i t is the symbolic, communication^! diffe ren ces o f media, ra ther than t h e i r technological d iffe re n c e s , which make the s ig n if ic a n t d iffe re n c e in lea rning . Salomon (1974B) has also postulated th a t media can be conceived as composed of three major components: and the technologies of transm ission. the symbol systems, the messages, A d d itio n a lly , these in te ra c t w ith the learner and the s p e c ific educational goal or ta s k . He stated th a t one can postulate th a t d if f e r e n t modes of inform ation e x tra c tio n and processing are activated to the extent th a t d if f e r e n t media code inform ation in d if f e r e n t ways: one can ask whether or not the same meaning is extracted when roughly the same idea is presented in two d if f e r e n t symbol systems. In ordinary experience, fo r example, a film ed novel may convey a d if f e r e n t meaning from i t s w ritte n counter­ p a rt, and a play may convey a d if f e r e n t message when i t is read than when i t is performed. A study by Meringoff (1980) showed th a t there is a d iffe re n c e in the kind o f inform ation children derive from d if f e r e n t media presenta­ tio n s . The researcher compared c h ild r e n 's apprehension of an unfam il­ i a r story read to them from an i l l u s t r a t e d book or presented as a comparable te le v is e d f i l m . Forty e ig ht subjects, twenty fo u r w ith a mean age o f seven and s ix tenths years and twenty fo u r w ith a mean age o f nine and six tenths years, were randomly assigned to one presenta­ tio n or another and in d iv id u a lly presented the s to ry . I t was hypoth­ esized th a t because te le v is io n can depict story actions dynamically and concomitantly, behavioral features o f the story would be more 34 s a lie n t and the te le v is io n presentation would re s u lt in a greater rfecalI of actions and a greater re lia nce on visual content in drawing inferences 'about the s to r y . In c o n tra s t, the book presentation might allow fo r more auditory a tte n tio n to the t e x t , more re c a ll of story language th a t eludes visual d e p ic tio n , and a greater use of textual content and more outside story knowledge as a basis fo r inferences. The children were measured on: the re c a ll of o b je c tiv e informa­ t io n of the s to ry , a p ic tu re ordering task (which was an a lte r n a tiv e , nonverbal measure of an understanding of the story l i n e ) , the i n f e r ­ ences drawn from the story and sources used to substantiate those inferences, and the viewing behavior of the subjects. Results showed th a t child re n given the te le v is io n version exhib­ ite d a higher re c a ll of the story actions than those given the pictu re book presentation (p < .0 5 ). Conversely, the p ic tu re book presentation e l i c i t e d greater re c a ll of the s to r y 's f i g u r a t iv e language than did the te le v is io n presentation (p s.01). As hypothesized, the older child re n recalled more of the actions (p ^.0 1 ), f i g u r a t iv e language (p.1.05), and dialogue (p^.01) than did the younger c h ild re n . The d if f e r e n t presentations were also corre la te d with the c h ild r e n 's use o f d if f e r e n t kinds of inform ation as a basis fo r making inferences. The te le v is io n story was associated w ith a s ig n if ic a n t l y greater use o f visual inform ation as a basis fo r drawing inferences. The book presentation generally was associated w ith more use of other kinds of info rm a tion. The child re n exposed to the te le v is io n story u$ed more physical gestures to i l l u s t r a t e t h e i r verbal r e t e l l i n g than clid those presented the book. 35 Meringoff credited Salomon (1979) w ith a possible im p lic a tio n o f her study: I f a given medium brings s p e c ific story content to the fo re ­ ground of c h ild r e n 's a tte n tio n (th a t content emphasized being a function of i t s own material p r o p e rtie s ), then children repeatedly exposed to t h is medium may accumulate experience w ith some kinds of inform ation more than others. They may attend to i t , remember i t , in te r p r e t i t , use i t in problem s o lv in g , perhaps even p re fe r i t to other info rm a tion. More­ over, they may be c u lt iv a t in g the p a r tic u la r co g n itiv e s k i l l s required to e x tra c t t h i s inform ation from the symbol systems in which i t is represented (M ering off, 1980:248). Meringoff added a possible consequence: children exposed p r i ­ m a rily to te le v is io n s to rie s may develop a strong s e n s i t i v it y to and visual memory o f these s to r ie s ; child re n exposed more to less visual media may fo s te r greater lis te n in g s k i l l s and may be able to apply more self-generated knowledge in in te rp r e tin g t h e i r meaning. M e rin g o ffs study was supported by Harvard's Project Zero. Another Harvard researcher, Howard Gardner, has noted (1980) some a d dition al supporting research. Using f i l m materials developed by Weston Woods Company, researchers presented children w ith a book and reader or a f i l m version of a storybook. The studies showed that c h ild re n exposed to a book version remember much more of the story on t h e i r own and are also b e tte r able to re c a ll inform ation when they are cued. He noted a s ig n if ic a n t d iffe re n c e th a t book c h ild re n can recall precise wording and fig u re s of speech but th a t subjects exposed to the te le v is io n version tend to paraphrase the verbal info rm a tion. A d d itio n a lly , Gardner noted th a t both groups of c h ild re n make inferences about the story and reach the same conclusions, but t h e i r lin e of reasoning is d i f f e r e n t . Book child re n tend to draw on t h e i r 36 own or real world experiences, whereas. te le v is io n children are l i k e l y t o depend on what they see, seldom in f e r r in g beyond the visual informaI tio n . \ ' 1 There are also notable diffe ren ces in the c h ild r e n 's conception o f time and space. Adults do not show thesd diffe re n c e s : only modest diffe ren ces are evident in t h e i r inference making process. Gardner concluded: In a l l , te le v is io n emerges as a much more self-contained experience fo r c h ild re n , and w ith in i t s boundaries, the visual component emerges as paramount. The book experience, on the other hand, allows f o r greater access to the s to r y 's language and suggests greater expanses of time and space. Books may encourage readers to make connections w ith other realms of I i f e - - j u s t as some b ib lio p h ile s argue (1980:113). In summary, a review of the media l i t e r a t u r e points to the f o l ­ lowing conclusions. The research has branched in d if f e r e n t d ire c tio n s which include analyzing and assessing the fo llo w in g : the e ffe c ts of content, media as a s o c ia liz in g agent, the effectiveness of in s tr u c ­ tio n a l media, the effectiveness o f media in changing a tt it u d e s , and the developmental diffe ren ces in attending and responding to the various media. More re c e n tly , media research has focused on formal a tt r ib u t e s and symbol systems c h a r a c te r is tic of the s p e c ific media. These a ttr ib u te s and c h a ra c te ris tic s may c a ll upon d i f f e r e n t mental processes f o r inform ation processing. Some w rite rs have suggested th a t w ith repeated exposure to a s p e c ific medium an in d iv id u a l may gradually come to use the required mental processes demanded by th a t medium more and more in his or her own problem solving, and c o g n itio n . by Marshall McLuhan in 1964. This possible e ffe c t was suggested Furthermore, most of the research has been done on what might be c alled the "big media"—in s tr u c tio n a l 37 t e le v is io n , mass media t e le v is io n , computer aided in s tr u c tio n . L ittle research has been done on the " l i t t l e media" such as s lid e s , fIim s t r i p s , tapes, ra dio. While there have been studies on the e ff e c t iv e ­ ness of media presentations in changing a ttitu d e s , there have been few, i f any, on the effectiveness of d if f e r e n t media in bringing a ffe c tiv e content to the foreground of a tte n tio n . The l i t e r a t u r e indicates th a t we are media consumers without being media I it e r a t e , i . e . , we have little understanding of the psychological and emotional e ffe c ts of prolonged exposure to various media and we may be influenced by the representational c h a ra c te ris tic s of the media as much as by the content i t d e liv e rs . These are the concerns th a t th is current research has attempted to address.. S to rie s , Language and S to r y te llin g There are a lim ite d number of a r t ic le s on s t o r y t e l l i n g and l i t t l e , i f any, s p e c ific research on the form of presentation. However, there is a large body of research on s to rie s and c h ild re n 's language develop­ ment. Arthur N. Applebee (1978) has w ritte n a book. The C h ild 's Con­ cept of Story, Ages Two to Seventeen, in which he d e ta ils his and others' research on the language processes children use when they hear s to rie s and the l i n g u i s t i c and psychological factors organizing and constraining t h e i r responses to the s to r ie s . He has found th a t child re n as young as two and a h a lf years old use language in s to ry - ■ t e l l i n g p a tterns, often assuming the s to rie s are about real people and r actual events. As they mature, children gradually create s to rie s which are more complex s t r u c t u r a lly and which involve s e ttin g s , actions and themes outside t h e i r immediate experience. I t is through th is 38 distancing of s to rie s th a t child re n are able to explore the conse­ quences of anxiety producing s itu a tio n s . This theory is consonant w ith B ettleheim 1S (1976) view th a t child re n can deal w ith emotionally and e x i s t e n t i a l l y traum atic experiences through the vehicle of the f a i r y ta le which in h e re n tly allows f o r a c h i l d 's distance from tro u b lin g s itu a tio n s . Brown and Smiley (.1 977) have used story re c a ll as a vehicle fo r learning about the perceived organization and importance of story con­ te n t. They found th a t child re n favor the central theme when recounting s to rie s and disregard nonessential d e ta ils and th a t re c a ll e ffic ie n c y is affected by the s tru c tu ra l importance of the story u n its . Simi­ l a r l y , Johnson (1970), in a series of experiments, o b je c tiv e ly divided verbal passages in to l i n g u i s t i c subunits according to acceptable pausal locations and then had the l i n g u i s t i c u n its o b je c tiv e ly ordered according to t h e i r importance to the s tru c tu re of the la rg e r prose passage. Subjects attempted verbatim re c a ll of a sing le prose passage immediately a ft e r reading i t or a f t e r a s ix ty three day in te rim . Results showed th a t the s tru c tu ra l importance of the l i n g u i s t i c units was related to t h e i r r e c a ll. Handler and Johnson (1977) have presented a report analyzing the underlying s tru c tu re of simple s to rie s and examining the im plications o f such s tru c tu re f o r r e c a l l . They use the term “ story schema" to re fe r to an ide alized in te rn a l representation of the parts and t h e i r re la tio n s h ip s in the s to r y . schemata from two sources: They theorize th a t people construct story from lis t e n in g to many s to rie s they develop knowledge about the sequencing of events, and from experience, they 39 develop knowledge about causal re la tio n s and various sequences of actio ns. Schemata act as a general framework w ith in which d e tailed comprehension takes place. For example, i t d ire c ts a tte n tio n to cer­ t a in aspects of incoming info rm a tion. The authors also the orize that story schemata are used as a set of r e tr ie v a l cues; s p e c i f i c a l l y , when one ca n 't remember an exact aspect of a s to ry , he reconstructs what might have occurred. Blank and Frank (1971) have also examined story re c a ll and lan­ guage a c q u is itio n in young c h ild re n . S p e c ific a lly , they investigated kindergarten c h ild r e n 's re c a ll of various s y n ta c tic transformations under d if f e r e n t presentations and they looked at the re la tio n s h ip between in te lle c t u a l fu n c tio n in g and l i n g u i s t i c performance by kinder­ garten c h ild re n . They found th a t the more a c tiv e ly involved the sub­ j e c t was in the presentation, the b e tte r the r e c a ll. One of the studies dealing with c h ild r e n 's language development most relevant to the present research was an in v e s tig a tio n by Smardo and Curry (1982) of story hours and receptive language. Their research is c le a rly a cross-media study, but i t is discussed in t h i s section because o f i t s use of s to ry m a te ria l. Their study concerned the e ffe c ­ tiveness of three kinds of pu b lic l i b r a r y story hour presentations on the receptive language o f preschool c h ild re n from varying socioeconomic le v e ls . The three types of story hour presentations included l i v e , videotaped, and 16 mm f i l m . The authors defined receptive language as synonymous w ith auditory decoding and ac tiv e lis t e n in g , i . e . , the a b i l i t y to comprehend or understand the language which is heard. c h ild re n were divided in to fou r groups according to presentation: The 40 Group One received a l i v e story presentation by a professional l i b r a r ­ ia n ; Group Two received a videotaped story hour program of Group One; Group Three received a commercially produced 16 mm f i l m presentation based on the books used in Groups One and Two; and Group Four, the control group, received no experimental treatm ent. Each story hour session was approximately t h i r t y minutes in length and was conducted once a week fo r s ix months. Two instruments were used to measure the c h ild re n 's receptive language: a standardized te s t and a researcher contracted designed instrument of story comprehension. Results of the study indicated the fo llo w in g order of effectiveness of presentations on a c q u is itio n of receptive language s k i l l s as measured by the standardized te s t : f i l m , video, and c o n tro l. liv e , Thus, the story hour presentations were e ff e c tiv e in improving receptive language s k i l l s as measured by the standardized t e s t . However, the data obtained by the other instrument did not show any diffe re n c e s of story comprehension based on story hour treatment groups. S to r y t e llin g i t s e l f might be termed a fo lk a rt which.has ju s t re cently been revived. While s t o r y t e l l i n g w i l l never become an impor­ ta n t medium in a formal educational s e ttin g , i t s revival in society indicates th a t among some groups, perhaps those disenchanted w ith the technologies of mass media, i t may be a valued form of communication. / The study by Meringoff (1980) c ite d e a r l i e r , which compared c h i l ­ dren's apprehensions of an u n fa m ilia r story presented as a tele v is e d f i l m or as a reader and p ic tu re book presentation, indicated th a t there were s ig n if ic a n t d iffe ren ces in the c h ild r e n 's responses to comparable 41 story material presented in the two d if f e r e n t media. p ic tu re book is not the same format as a s t o r y t e l l e r . A reader and However, the observed diffe ren ces provided some support fo r the hypothesis that there are s tru c tu ra l differences in the media and th a t these d i f f e r ­ ences, p a r t ic u la r ly the r e la tiv e v is u a liz a tio n , influence which content is conveyed more e f f e c t i v e l y . George Shannon, w r itin g in English Journal (1979:50), referred to the s t o r y t e l l e r as the ric h e s t audiovisual device a v a ila b le . A f i l m , a f i l m ­ s t r i p or recording may share the same subject m atter, but i t is the s t o r y t e l l e r in person, sharing images d i r e c t l y , th a t puts him above the r e s t. S to r y t e llin g f o r both the t e l l e r and lis te n e r is one of the most personal of experiences. S to r y t e lle r Ramon Ross (1972) theorized th a t across the gamut of d a ily a c t i v i t i e s there is a re b irth of in te r e s t in keeping a liv e the "humanness" of immediate p a r tic ip a tio n : That sense of personal contact is lo s t somehow when we view the most tale n te d e n te rta in e r on te le v is io n or watch the grandly choreographed dance. Neither gives us the same experience as being in the presence of a r e a l - l i v e s t o r y t e l l e r or clasping hands w ith others in a simple country dance. Marshall McLuhan was r i g h t . The medium does a l t e r the message....Reassembling a performer out of a p ic tu re c on sisting of tens of thousands of t i n y points of l i g h t is not the same as s i t t i n g at the elbow of a performer who responds to us, catches our thoughts w ith him, warms to our laughter and holds our flag ging in te r e s t (1972:4). I f such diffe ren ces do e x is t among the coding prope rties of the media, i t is perhaps unfortunate th a t less research has been conducted on the simpler media, inc lu d in g the s t o r y t e l l e r and the reader presen­ ta t io n s . Continual exposure to c e rta in forms may influence an i n d iv i d ­ u a l's perceptions and thought processes. I f such is the case, the 42 determination o f what these e ffe c ts are could have considerable im p l i ­ cation fo r education. L ite ra r y Genre - The Fairy Tale L i t t l e or no experimental research e x is ts on l i t e r a r y genre, although a large body o f research is emerging on the s tru c tu re of s to rie s and the use of language in s to r ie s . The l i t e r a r y genre of the f a i r y ta le is treated t h e o r e tic a lly and p h ilo s o p h ic a lly and is included in th is discussion because th e o re tic a l considerations were used in th is w r i t e r 's choice of a story f o r the study. The f a i r y ta le emerged from oral t r a d i t i o n of f o lk lo r e (Yearsley, 1929) and as such is p a r t ic u la r ly s u ita b le fo r s t o r y t e l l i n g . Yearsley noted th a t the actual d e liv e ry of f a i r y ta le s in th is oral t r a d i t i o n was extremely conservative in th a t very l i t t l e v a ria tio n existed in words, gestures, a ctio ns, and fa c ia l expressions. Thus, w hile some v a r ia tio n would in e v ita b ly evolve through in d iv id u a l r e t e l l i n g over generations, the s to r y 's essential elements remained the same. Psychologist Bruno Bettleheim (1976) hypothesized th a t t r a d itio n a l f a i r y ta le s , handed down o r a l l y , may be superior to contemporary chi I -. dren's l i t e r a t u r e in t h e i r a b i l i t y to stim ulate and nuture the re­ sources needed fo r coping with d i f f i c u l t inner problems. He stated th a t contemporary l i t e r a t u r e provides info rm a tion, entertainment and teaches about s p e c ific conditions o f modern l i f e , , but f a i l s to allow a c h ild to c l a r i f y his emotions in a symbolic and s im p lifie d manner. The f a i r y t a le presents universal problems and e x is te n tia l dilemmas (lo v e , death, courage) sym bolically and allows the c h ild to fantasize 43 and ruminate on his own solu tions w ithout couching them in the concrete and sometimes threatening r e a l i t y o f his actual expedience. This concept is consonant w ith Applebee's (1978) fin d in g s th a t children experience distancing w ith t h e i r s to rie s as they mature, which allows c h ild re n to explore the consequences of otherwise taboo s itu a tio n s . Handler and Johnson (1977), in analyzing the underlying stru c tu re of simple s to r ie s , have theorized th a t the story schema is used as a set of r e trie v a l cues, th a t i s , when people c a n 't remember an exact aspect of a s to ry , they reconstruct what might have occurred. accounts f o r the re g u la riz a tio n of ir r e g u la r s to rie s over time. This They note th a t f o lk t a le s , fa b le s , and myths have s im ila r and unusually c lea r s tru c tu ra l c h a ra c te ris tic s . S t o r y t e lle r Ramon Ross (1980) indicated th a t legends and migratory ta le s , both part of the oral t r a d i t i o n from which fo lk ta le s spring, are the representation of archetypes, th a t i s , thematic r e a l i t i e s , part of the accumulated wisdom of humankind. Archetypes such as love, death and war are manifested sym bolically and are formed by centuries of experience in the f o lk wisdom of a people. Bettleheim (1976) has stated th a t a d is t in c t io n should be made between o r a l, t r a d it io n a l f a i r y ta le s and l i t e r a r y f a i r y ta le s which were popularized by Hans C h ris tia n Andersen. The l a t t e r resemble oral f a i r y ta le s in s e tt in g , c h a ra c te riz a tio n , social themes, and language but are w ritte n by a sin g le in d iv id u a l. C hildren's author Jane Yolen, however, has implied th is d is t in c t io n is a r t i f i c i a l : pure about a f o l k t a l e a f t e r a l l . already. "There is nothing The oral story is a bastardized one I t has on i t s body the thumbprints of h is to ry " (1978:702). 44 Bettle^eim (I 576), Yolen (I 978) and L1Engle ( I 978) have discussed several prevalent m otifs in f o lk lo r e and f a i r y ta le s which contain strong a ffe c tiv e content. in t h e i r discussions: The fo llo w in g are some th a t were included the struggle of good versus e v i l ; the true princess; the younger son; separation from loved ones; courage; the th re a t and fea r of death; the quest; the transform ational ta le where man becomes beast; and the happy ending. These m o tifs , with t h e i r p o te n tia l fo r carrying a ffe c tiv e content, have existed fo r centuries in many d i f f e r e n t f a i r y ta le s and, as Yolen has pointed out (1977), continue to e x is t today in popular te le v is io n cartoon characters and space f i c t i o n heroes. These aspects of the f a i r y ta le were considered by th is researcher in the s e le ction of the story book used fo r th is s tu d y . Summary . Chapter 2 presented a review of the l i t e r a t u r e relevant to the background and th e o re tic a l considerations of media studies and theore­ t i c a l considerations of s to rie s and s t o r y t e l l i n g . Because of the exploratory nature of t h is research, t h i s review was somewhat i n t e r ­ d is c ip lin a r y in i t s approach. The review included an overview ( h is ­ t o r i c a l and comprehensive), the fin d in g s of educational media research and cross-media stud ies, a discussion of the l i t e r a t u r e th a t has dealt w ith the s p e c ific a tt r ib u t e s and c h a ra c te ris tic s of media and a d is ­ cussion of s to r ie s , language and s t o r y t e l l i n g . The chapter was con­ cluded w ith a discussion of the th e o re tic a l considerations of l i t e r a r y genre and the f a i r y t a l e . 45 CHAPTER 3 PROCEDURES In tro d u c ti on Audiovisual media presentations are prevalent in most c h ild re n 's experiences today, even, as noted by Smardo and Curry (1982), i n f l u ­ encing the tr a d it io n a l pu b lic l i b r a r y story hour. Olson (1974) and Salomon (1974, 1979) have hypothesized th a t d if f e r e n t media represent unique systems which s tru c tu re inform ation d i f f e r e n t l y and may, th e re ­ fo r e , have various inform ation p o te n tia l. Meringoff (1980) has sug­ gested th a t d if f e r e n t media bring d if f e r e n t content to c h ild re n 's * a tte n tio n and with repeated exposure they gain f a m i l i a r i t y w ith th a t content and could come to p re fe r i t and use i t in problem so lv in g . Schramm (1977) has stated th a t more research in the audiovisual f i e l d has been done on what he c a lls the "big media" such as in tr u c tio n a l te le v is io n than on the " l i t t l e media" such as f i l m s t r ip s and s lid e s . Yet, the l a t t e r type is also used throughout education. In considering these problems, th is study was designed to inves­ t i g a t e whether or not there are diffe ren ces in t h ir d graders' percep­ tio n s of story content th a t is brought to the foreground o f a tte n tio n by three modes of presentation representative of the less elaborate audiovisual media and tr a d it io n a l presentations: a sound s lid e presen­ t a t io n , a reader and p ic tu re book presentation, and a s t o r y t e l l e r p re s e n ta tion. 46 The procedures th a t were used are described in t h is chapter. The f i r s t section describes the review of the l i t e r a t u r e , the selection of the s to ry , and the production of the sound s lid e show. The second section describes the p i l o t in g which was done to analyze the story and develop the instrument. Three d if f e r e n t approaches f o r the analysis and instrument are discussed: an aly s is . brainstorm ing, in te rv ie w , and textual The ra tio n a le f o r the f in a l choice of the story analysis and development of the instrument are given. The t h i r d section e x p li­ cates the methodology used in completing t h is study. The chapter concludes w ith a summary. Review o f the L ite ra tu re An i n i t i a l review of the l i t e r a t u r e was conducted during spring 1982. The f i r s t phase of the review obtained a general overview o f the f i e l d o f media research. Studies relevant to the use of audiovisual media in education, general trends th a t have developed in the la s t three decades o f research and media education in the schools were noted. Sources used p r im a rily included Education Index, Psychological' Abstracts and ERIC Documents. The second phase of the review focused on research conducted p r i ­ m a rily during the la s t ten years and dealing w ith the c h a ra c te ris tic s and a ttr ib u te s of media and i t s possible coding of inform ation as symbol systems. The doctoral d is s e r ta tio n , A Story, A Story: The Influence of the Medium on C hildren's Apprehension of S to r ie s , (M ering off, 1978) comparing apprehension of content of a te le v is e d presentation as opposed to a book and reader presentation was used to 47 provide th e o re tic a l background and methodological design f o r t h is stud y. The l i t e r a t u r e review included th e o re tic a l and philosophical a r t ic le s dealing with the o r ig in s , a t t r ib u t e s , thematic content, and possible s ig n ific a n c e of f a i r y ta le s as a l i t e r a r y genre. I t also d e alt w ith the s t o r y t e l l i n g t r a d i t i o n as a f o lk a r t which has received revived in te r e s t and which may possess c h a ra c te ris tic s valuable to the transmission o f c u ltu ra l h e ritag e. Selection o f the Story The sele ction of a s to ry was based on considerations which include a r tic u la te d c h a ra c te ris tic s th a t would make a story s u ita b le fo r such a study and are s im ila r to those used by Meringoff in her study (1978): genre, c o m p a ra b ility , a u th e n tic ity o f the m a te ria l, and f a m i l i a r i t y . The f i r s t consideration was the type o f story to be used. A fa iry t a l e was preferred over other genre because of i t s possible potential fo r conveying a ffe c tiv e content and because i t o rig inated from the oral t r a d i t i o n which is p a r t ic u la r ly s u ita b le f o r s t o r y t e l l i n g . Bettleheim (1976) hypothesized th a t f a i r y ta le s may be superior to contemporary l i t e r a t u r e in s tim u la tin g the inner resources which enable a c h ild to c l a r i f y his emotions. Through t h e i r symbolic representation of univer­ sal problems and e x is te n tia l m o tifs , they allow a c h ild to deal with d i f f i c u l t and a f f e c t iv e ly threatening problems in an ab stract and safe manner. The f a i r y ta le "The Wild Swans" (Andersen, 1980) was chosen because i t embodied some of the m otifs mentioned by Bettleheim (1976), Yolen 48 (1978) and L 1Engle (1978) as containing strong a ffe c tiv e content. These included: the struggle of good versus e v i l ; the tru e princess and the younger son; separation from loved ones; courage; the fe a r and th re a t of death; the quest; the transform ational ta le where man becomes beast; and the happy ending. The story is an adaptation by E rlic h , i l l u s t r a t e d by J e ffe rs , o f Hans C h ris tia n Andersen's version of the Brothers Grimm "The Six Swans" (Grimm, 1963). In the adapted version, eleven brothers are turned in to swans by a cruel stepmother and t h e ir s i s t e r E lise is banished from the palace. A fte r a number o f years, E lis e fin d s her brothers, who recount how they are transformed in to w ild swans at daybreak and back in to human form each n i g h t f a l l . Elise is t o ld by a f a i r y th a t she can break the spell by k n it t in g eleven s h ir t s f o r the swans from the f la x of s tin g in g n e ttle s and vowing complete silence u n til the task is completed. A handsome king finds E lise and marries her but is eventualIy persuaded she is a witch and should be put to death. At the moment she is taken to the stake, she completes her ta s k , throws the s h ir ts over the w ild swans and breaks the s p e ll. At the happy ending, she is reunited w ith her brothers and w ith the king who now understands her p e c u lia r behavior. Besides genre, the c r i t e r i o n of com parability had to be considered in se le c tin g a s to ry . The study required th a t s i m i l a r i t y e x is t between the presentation of the s to r y 's content w hile employing c h a ra c te ris tic s of each mode. A p i l o t sound s lid e show was developed in May 1982 to e s ta b lis h the successful ness with which the book could be transformed in to th a t medium. A d d itio n a lly , the rather large format of the book (nine and th re e -fo u rth s inches by twelve and one-fourth inches) 49 enhanced i t s s u i t a b i l i t y fo r the reader and book presentation as the subjects were able to see the i l l u s t r a t i o n s e a s ily . , A u th e n tic ity <?f the material was considered in choosing a s to ry . Published story m aterials were considered preferable to nonpublished because of the possible a p p l i c a b il i t y of the study's fin d in g s . This was the same reason M eringoff (1978) chose e x is tin g m aterials fo r her study. The question of f a m i l i a r i t y was addressed in s ele cting the s to ry . While Meringoff (1978) thought u n fa m ilia r ity was im portant, i . e . , th a t the children should have no p r io r exposure to the story in e ith e r f o r ­ mat, the preference of a f a i r y t a le f o r i t s a ffe c tiv e and archetypal content obviated the use of a t o t a l l y u n fa m ilia r s t o r y . However, the story of the swans has not been as popularized as have some other f a i r y ta le s , e . g . , C inderella and Snow White. A d d itio n a lly , the chosen ver­ sion d i f f e r s in story lin e from the Brothers Grimm version (1963) and is a recent p u b lic a tio n . F in a lly , the choice of a f a i r y t a le is in part posited on the f a m i l i a r i t y of m otifs which may sym bolically code a ff e c tiv e content. The researcher judged th a t the m otifs of the true princess, the quest, the tra n sfo rm a tio n , and the happy ending would be f a m i li a r to most child re n whether th a t exposure had come from the story of the swans or another t a l e . Production o f the Sound Slide Show A p i l o t show was produced in May 1982 using approximately ninety 35 mm s lid e s selected f o r t h e i r representation of the story lin e and t h e i r a u th e n tic ity in protraying the i l l u s t r a t i o n s in the book. The ~1 50 e n tire te x t was read and taped and t h is na rra tion synchronized with the s lid e s . A b r i e f piano s ele ction from Debussy's "C la ire de Lune" was added before and a ft e r the n a rra tio n . Inaudible advance and dissolve technique, whereby one p ic tu re fades in to another so there is no d is ­ c o n tin u ity on the screen, were used. The duration of t h is version was over twenty minutes and the researcher judged th a t not only was i t too long fo r holding t h ir d graders' a tte n tio n , but i t also would make the exact memorization of the story unduly d i f f i c u l t f o r the n a rra to r. The text.was shortened during the f a l l of 1982 by the researcher and an experienced s t o r y t e l l e r and elementary language a rts teacherJ Two sections judged nonessential to the s to ry lin e and transmission of a ff e c tiv e m o tifs and unnecessarily complicating were eliminated en­ tir e ly .- The rest of the t e x t was condensed, making i t more amenable to s t o r y t e l l i n g . The researcher wanted the sound s lid e show to approximate a sound f i l m s t r i p presentation as much as possible since the l a t t e r is preva­ le n t in audiovisual renderings of c h ild r e n 's l i t e r a t u r e . Five recently released f i l m s t r i p versions of c h ild r e n 's s to rie s were viewed. sure time per frame and photographic techniques were noted. time ranged from as l i t t l e Expo­ Exposure as f iv e seconds to as long as t h i r t y three, w ith an average being approximately ten to twelve seconds. The photo­ graphic techniques included f u l l page shots, closeups, flashbacks to e a r l i e r p ic tu re s , and changes in v e r tic a l and horizontal o r ie n ta tio n . The p i l o t s lid e show was revised during spring 1983. Additional s lid e s using close-ups and other techniques were taken, the dissolve feature eliminated since f i l m s t r ip s do not have th a t a t t r ib u t e and the 51 shortened te x t used fo r n a rra tio n . A new sele ction was made of one hundred nine s lid e s representing the revised t e x t . Exposure per s lid e averaged about seven seconds and the to t a l length of time was twelve and a h a lf minutes. The music was l e f t only at the very beginning and end of the na rra tion since i t could influ ence the students' preceptions o f a ffe c tiv e content. However, since most f i l m s t r i p versions of s to rie s have music during at le a s t part of the t e x t , i t was judged de s ira b le , both f o r i t s aesthetic q u a lity and a u th e n tic ity , to leave in those b r ie f segments. Approaches P iloted f o r Analyzing the Story and Developing the Instrument The fo llo w in g three techniques were explored during a p i l o t period to determine the most appropriate approach fo r story analysis and instrument development: brainstorming, in te rv ie w and tex tu a l analysis. "Brainstorming" Analysis Purpose. The purpose of the brainstorming technique was to deter­ mine the story content from the t e x t using children themselves to analyze "what is the content?" and "what content is important?" R ationale. The child re n themselves should be able to t e l l what the story is about. Their perceptions of content and t h e i r actual language could be used as a basis fo r developing the instrument. This technique could avoid an a r t i f i c a l Iy academic or u n r e a lis t ic approach to developing instrum entation. Procedures. Three sets of child re n w ith accompanying d if f e r e n t procedures were used. 52 1) The f i r s t set were the researcher's own two children (ages s ix and ten at the tim e ) . many times. They had seen and heard the story They were read the story and to ld to " t e l l everything they could remember th a t was important about i t . " Their responses would have to be considered contaminated; however, the researcher thought they were important to i n ­ clude because w ith t h e i r repeated exposure to the story they might be more attuned to the content th a t was being empha­ sized by the t e x t i t s e l f . 2) The second group, a fo u rth grade class from Webster G arfield School, Butte, was read the story during t h e i r l i b r a r y period and encouraged to brainstorm f r e e l y . and w ritte n down. Responses were taped Questions were nonguided although "what . happened next?" was asked. 3) The t h i r d group, also a Webster G arfie ld fo u rth grade class, was encouraged to brainstorm but more directed questioning was used. Responses from t h i s group more c lose ly followed the story lin e and seemed to be more d e ta ile d , but not sub­ s t a n t i a l l y d i f f e r e n t from those of the other group. Analysis o f the Content. The c h ild r e n 's responses were typed on s lip s of paper to f a c i l i t a t e a n alysis. The fo llo w in g types of content, which would d is tin g u is h much of the story content, were. o p e ra tio n a lly defined: A f f e c t iv e . A phrase, sentence or clause e x p l i c i t l y s ta tin g an emotional reaction ( e . g . , "was h o r r i f i e d " ) or s ta tin g an 53 action th a t is d i r e c t l y associated w ith emotional reaction ( e . g . , "she w ept"). A c tio n . A phrase, sentence or clause containing an action verb. D e s c rip tiv e . A phrase, sentence or clause th a t depicts or describes, without emphasizing action or emotion; phrases th a t help draw a c le a re r "mental p ic tu r e ," e . g . , metaphors. Theme. A phrase, sentence or clause th a t expresses "what the story was about," " i t s main p o in t," "what i t was try in g to t e l l about I i f e . " x The content was sorted by the researcher and two other adults in to categories and an 89.7% agreement on placement in the categories obtained. See Appendix D f o r the actual items. C ritic is m of This Approach. The researcher did not th in k th a t these brainstormed content items were p a r tic u la r ly amenable to i n ­ strumentation fo r th is study fo r the fo llo w in g reasons: "Story content" as a construct can be viewed two ways: f i r s t , as fragmentary b i t s , or second, as parts th a t carry meaning or accomplish a purpose. Using the f i r s t con struct, sentence fragments or even c lu s te rs of words (such as "about a princess," "a wicked queen," "witches") would q u a lify as o p e ra tio n a lly defined content. Meringoff (1978) approached content in th is manner. For ex­ ample, " f ig u r a t iv e language" was defined by the use o f words fo r t h e i r form ally expressive p ro p e rtie s , aside from possible referen­ t i a l meanings. "Dialogue" was defined as discre te clauses of 54 speech directed between characters, often preceded by "said" or "answered." Content thus defined can be quite fragmentary. However, when content is viewed as accomplishing a purpose in the story or carrying meaning, i t s length can vary but i t is no longer fragmentary nor removed from context. Most of the brainstormed content items were c la s s ifie d under the f i r s t con struct: they were fragmentary b its of information th a t did not necessarily transm it meaning out of context. To determine, through the development and ad m inistration of an instrument, which of these kinds of content are in the foreground of a tte n tio n would be more the determination of fragmentary units in the foreground of a tte n tio n than meaningful content. Further­ more, the development of such an instrument would have required the researcher to couch these brainstormed items in some sentence context. To do so would have departed even fu r th e r from the story l i n e and added an addition al r is k of experimenter bias. This study was p r im a rily concerned with " a f fe c tiv e " content posited against " n o n a ffe c tiv e ." The researcher wanted to deter­ mine whether a ffe c tiv e "messages" or aspects of the story are brought more to the foreground of a tte n tio n with one mode of presentation or another. The im p lic a tio n s of t h is study could have been severely lim ite d i f content were reduced to fragmentary u n its , whereas the fin d in g s could be more generalizable and a p p li­ cable i f content were viewed as having meaning or accomplishing a purpose. The researcher judged th a t the student analysis of the 55 s to r y 's content as derived from the brainstorming was not amenable to t h is more a l I -encompassing approach. Interview Approach Following M e rin g o ff1s approach (1978) to analyzing the story and determining type of content in the foreground of the c h ild re n 's a tte n ­ t i o n , the researcher developed an in te rv ie w format, which ( t h e o r e t i­ c a lly at le a s t) could be used by s p e c ia lly tra in e d interview ers to e l i c i t responses immediately fo llo w in g the presentations. The story would have f i r s t had to be analyzed fo r i t s content, and then the c h ild interviewed to re la te back the content. The number of content references would be scored. There were a considerable number of experimental variables re­ q u irin g control in th is approach: the rigorous use of standardized inte rview format, cues and probes; the exacting t r a in in g of in te rv ie w ­ e rs ; the number of interview ers required ( f iv e to te n ) ; and the r e l i a ­ b i l i t y in determining whether the c h ild 's response q u a lifie d fo r g e ttin g the central idea of the content. Nevertheless, one inte rview was conducted a ft e r a presentation of the story to a second grade, class at the Butte Greely School. The youngster was most cooperative and her comments yielded some i n t e r ­ esting in s ig h ts in to the s to ry . However, since the researcher had determined th a t i t would be desirable to work with small groups of students, since th a t r e fle c ts a real school s it u a tio n , the interview technique w ith groups seemed un w ie ldly, and a paper and pencil approach with c le a r ly defined answers p referable . 56 Analysis and Development of Instrument from the Text Purpose. The purpose of t h is analysis was to determine a ll r e fe r ­ ences to a ffe c tiv e content in the te x t o f the story and to posit these against references to nonaffective content. R ationale. The ra tio n a le fo r th is analysis was th a t the story t e x t i t s e l f contains a ffe c tiv e content which might be brought to the foreground of a tte n tio n through one medium more than through another. Procedure. The te x t of the story as i t was read to the students was given to two adults who were t o ld to underline any word or phrase th a t e x p l i c i t l y stated an emotional re action . This would determine the construct v a l i d i t y of a ffe c tiv e content delineated f o r instrum entation. While there was only a 60% o v e ra ll concur­ rence of choices between the two a d u lts , one adult simply under­ lin e d more content references than did the other. Of the r e fe r ­ ences chosen by the f i r s t person, there was nearly 100% agreement by the second person. The te x t was then divided in to ten general sections such as " s e t t in g , " "in tro d u c tio n of c o n f l i c t , " "beginning of the quest." From each of these sections, an a ff e c tiv e reference was taken from the te x t as a complete thought and posited against another com­ p le te thought by which i t was juxtaposed in the t e x t . Most of these a lte rn a tiv e s could be categorized as "action c on ten t." (See Appendix D - Instrument D ir e c tly from Text.) questions were developed th is way. Nine such 57 In a second part of the instrum ent, seven questions were developed to probe possible inferences about a ffe c tiv e content. Most of these inferences were taken from the responses to the "guided brainstorming" and were posited against a ff e c t iv e ly neu­ t r a l or a ff e c t iv e ly d i f f e r e n t ( e . g . , opposite) content. F in a lly , f iv e factual re c a ll questions were included fo r a to t a l o f 21 questions. P r e p ilo t . I n i t i a l p ilo t in g w ith a group of students indicated th a t using f u l l length sentences d i r e c t l y from the t e x t placed undue s tra in on the c h ild r e n 's a tte n tio n span. fin is h e d lis te n in g to the e n tire s to ry : They had ju s t being administered the instrument was almost l i k e lis te n in g again to the s to r y . For th a t reason, the te x tu a l language in the instrument was compressed (condensed) as much as possible w hile tr y in g to re ta in the authen­ t i c i t y of the language. (See Appendix D - Instrument D ire c tly from Text.) S p e cific Purposes. The researcher was p rim a rily inte reste d in determining the fo llo w in g , which i f i t proved f r u i t f u l would be refined in a f in a l instrument: 1) The f e a s i b i l i t y o f administering t h is type of instrument to t h i r d grade c h ild re n . 2) Some cursory determination of the instrum ent's r e l i a ­ b ility . 3) Some determination of whether the instrument could y ie ld meaningful data. 58 Results o f the P r e p ilo t. A t h i r d grade class of twenty f iv e Webster G a rfie ld students was read the story and administered the instrument immediately afterwards. Response choices ( e . g . , a ffe c ­ t i v e versus action) were t a l l i e d f o r a l l questions. Although the instrument required reading a b i l i t y , the questions were read aloud to the students. There was no apparent problem w ith keeping the c h ild r e n 's in te re s t throughout the a d m in is tra tio n . The researcher had intended to readminister the instrument, using a t e s t / r e t e s t design, in three weeks to determine r e l i a b i l ­ it y , i . e . , s ta b ility . However, an intervening illn e s s resulted in a lapse of nearly three months from te s t to re te s t. students were used f o r the re te s t. below. Only ten R e l i a b i l i t y re s u lts are shown They were recorded as simple percentages of id e n tic a l choices fo llo w in g a precedent established by Meringoff (1978) in which percentage of same choices between independent ra ters was used to determine r e l i a b i l i t y o f observations. For example, in Part Three a l l of the students responded to each question iden­ t i c a l l y to the way they responded on the f i r s t ad m in is tra tio n . Total Precentage Same Responses Overall = 95.49% Total Percentage Same Responses Part I (A ffe c tiv e /O th e r) = 89. 88% Total Percentage Same Responses Part 11 (Inferences) = 94% Total Percentage Same Responses Part I I I (Factual Recall) = 100% Results of p r e p ilo tin g t h is prototype indicated the fo llo w in g to the researcher: 59 Such an instrument had construct v a l i d i t y as types of content were o p e ra tio n a lly defined and analyzed by a d u lts . I t would be fe a s ib le to administer t h is type instrument w ith th ir d grade children without placing undue stress or demands on t h e i r a tte n tio n . F in a lly , such an instrument would have r e l i a b i l i t y ( s t a b i l i t y over time) and po te n tia l fo r y ie ld in g meaningful data when comparing scores of three d if f e r e n t modes o f presentation. Methodology Preparation of Story Presentation and Sound Slide Show A s t o r y t e l l e r was selected fo r the two l i v e presentation modes and f o r n a rra ting the sound s lid e show. 2 The in d iv id u a l was experienced in the atre productions and presentations with c h ild re n . In preparing the presentation, the s t o r y t e l l e r was coached and monitored by the researcher. The in d iv id u a l memorized the te x t of the story fo r the presentations. She had access to the te x t to insure as id e n tic a l presentations as possible. The s lid e show was prepared in f in a l format using a taped nar­ ra tio n by the s t o r y t e l l e r congruent w ith the other presentations and fo llo w in g the procedure used in i t s development. Story Analysis and Development of the Instrument . The analysis of the story fo r o p e ra tio n a lly defined content was based on M e rin g o ffs precedence of story analysis (1978). However, a 60 three person panel and a regional language arts expert were used instead of a single i n d iv i d u a l . 3 Three kinds of content—a f f e c t iv e , d e s c rip tiv e and action--were id e n t i f i e d . Panel members were in s tru c te d to read the story through once f o r fa m ilia r iz a t io n and then mark a l l phrases or clauses th a t c le a rly f i t the a ffe c tiv e content c r i t e r i o n . Then they were to iden-j t i f y an action or d e s c rip tiv e phrase or clause juxtaposed near the a ffe c tiv e content. A unanimously agreed upon phrase would autom atically be included in the instrument. Content items on which there was not unamimous agreement and other items selected by the researcher were resubmitted a second and t h i r d time as fo llo w s . The t e x t was positioned alongside the phrase to be i d e n t if ie d so the context was a v a ila b le . Panel mem­ bers were in s tru c te d to label the s p e c ific content as i t was operation­ a l l y defined. A two out of three agreement between panel members q u a lifie d the item f o r in c lu s io n . The instrument was then developed as fo llo w s . Part I - Sixteen questions were included to determine c h ild re n 's perceptions of s a lie n t content, a ff e c tiv e as opposed to nonaffectiv e . A ffe c tiv e references as o p e ra tio n a lly defined and de ter­ mined by the panel were posited against nonaffective references. As w ith the p r e p ilo t instrument, these textual references were condensed. The child re n were asked to id e n t if y which "seemed most im po rta nt." Part I I - This section determined c h ild r e n 's inferences about the story content and included sixteen items which were based mostly 61 on the brainstormed responses completed during the p r e p ilo t phase. Students were able to choose one of two inferences or the t h ir d option of 111 d o n 't know." Part I I I - Sixteen tru e and fa ls e items were taken from each part of the story to determine factual re c a ll of content. The content items f o r Part I and the e n tire instrument were sub­ m itted to a regional expert in the f i e l d o f language a rts fo r v e r i f i ­ cation of the construct v a l i d i t y o f the items in the instrument. Population and Sampling Procedures The population from which the sample was drawn was th a t group id e n t if ie d as t h i r d grade students attending regular classes in three o f the nine elementary schools in the Butte Public School System, School D i s t r i c t Number One, during the school year 1983-1984. At the end of the 1982-1983 school year, three elementary schools were closed and much of the c i t y r e d is t r ic t e d . Of the three schools used in th is study—Margaret Leary, Longfellow and Kennedy--Kennedy was more affected than any other by the r e d i s t r i c t i n g since i t absorbed nearly the e n tire Blaine population when i t was closed. Margaret Leary was affected in a d if f e r e n t manner as i t became the s ite of one of the three cen tra lize d kindergartens. Standardized achievement scores from School D i s t r i c t Number One's te s tin g program— Stanford Achievement Test, Form E— (since th a t data was av a ila b le f a l l 1983) were compared to determine i f these students were comparable. scores were used. assessed tog eth er. Total reading, lis t e n in g and lis te n in g comprehension Data from Kennedy and the closed Blaine School were The m a jo rity of the scores were in the same or .62 adjacent stanine bands and were v e r if ie d by the D i s t r i c t Curriculum D ire c to r4 as representing s im ila r populations. Scores from the other grade leve ls were also looked at to consider overall s i m i l a r i t y of school populations (see Appendix D). Third grade was preferred to other grade levels because of the length and complexity of the s to ry , because the instrument required some reading a b i l i t y and because i t was judged th a t above the t h ir d grade there would be a d dition al r is k of the students' s o p h is tic a tio n w ith story m aterials and media in flu e n c in g t h e i r responses to the ! s to ry . From each of the three schools, a random sample of f i f t e e n t h i r d grade boys and f i f t e e n t h i r d grade g i r l s was drawn. In one case, the e n tir e population of fourteen t h i r d grade boys was used. The random­ ized sele ction was accomplished through the use. of a computer program w ritte n esp ecially fo r t h is purpose by the computer coordinator fo r Butte School D i s t r i c t Number One and v e r if ie d by a regional expert in the f i e l d of computer e d u c a tio n . 5 Substitutes were also randomly chosen to replace absentees. Total number of subjects were n in e ty , t h i r t y per treatment group. This sample size was based on Spatz and Johnson's (1981) recommendation th a t t h i r t y is a s u f f i c i e n t l y large number fo r the sampling d i s t r ib u ­ tio n of means to approach a normal curve, although they state th a t " i f the population i t s e l f is symmetrical the sampling d i s t r ib u t i o n of the mean w i l l be normal with a sample size much smaller than t h i r t y " (1981: 149). 63 Each school was given each kind of treatment group-sound s lid e show, book and reader, and s to r y te lle r - - a n d each treatment was composed of f i v e boys and f i v e g i r l s each, except one in which there were six g i r l s and one boy. Spache and Spache (1977) have c ite d research both in reading readiness and reading achievement which in d ic a te the super­ i o r i t y o f g i r l s over boys in th is language a rts s k i l l . While these differences may be due to c u ltu ra l and teacher expectations, i t was decided th is possible v a r ia tio n should be minimized in the present study. Therefore, in as much as possible, treatment groups contained an equal number of boys and g i r l s . Whereas Meringoff (1 978) presented the reader version and t e l e ­ vised version in d iv i d u a l l y , i t was judged th a t part of the condition de sirable fo r the s t o r y t e l l i n g and the book and reader s e ttin g was the shared experience. For t h is reason, groups of ten were used fo r the presentations. P ilo t of the Instrument A p i l o t of the instrument was conducted on two Webster G arfield School t h ir d grade classes (a to t a l of fo rty -tw o students), which were id e n t if ie d by the same c r i t e r i o n of standard achievement scores as used fo r the groups from the three schools in the study. ' The book and reader version only was used in the p i l o t . During the p i l o t , the instrum ent's r e l i a b i l i t y , i . e. , s t a b i l i t y over tim e, as defined by Sax (1980), was determined. Procedures f o r the experimental treatment and f o r administering the data gathering instrument were also established. A t e s t - r e t e s t design suggested by Ferguson (1981) to determine the instrum ent's r e l i a b i l i t y was applied using a Pearson product-moment 64 c o rre la tio n c o e ffic ie n t as suggested by G uilfo rd (1973) and approxi­ mately a three-week intervening i n t e r v a l. I n i t i a l re s u lts during the p i l o t phase indicated the necessity o f determining whether or not a higher r e l i a b i l i t y could be obtained i f the experimenter were the s t o r y t e l l e r . A s im ila r pre and post te s t design was used w ith three addition al t h i r d grade classes from two other schools. An examination of the data obtained from these three a d dition al groups indicated not enough v a ria tio n in consistency of response to warrant a s t a t i s t i c a l an aly s is . I n i t i a l re s u lts of the p ilo t in g also indicated student responses on the post te s t were influenced by t h e i r f i r s t exposure to the story presentation. Since i t was necessary to determine a r e l i a b i l i t y level on the instrument i t s e l f , as opposed to student responses, two addi­ tio n a l steps were incorporated in to the design. F i r s t , the t h i r d grade pi-lot students had the instrument adminis­ tered again, s i l e n t l y , w ith no story treatment at a l l . Second, since reading a b i l i t y could also contaminate student responses in th is retest design, a class of seventeen s ix th grade students from the same school was administered the story to read s i l e n t l y . An in te rv a l of one week was used in the re te s t design to minimize intervening contaminating v a ria b le s . Following t h is i n t e r v a l , the students were administered the instrument to read and complete s i l e n t l y , with no story treatment. Results from t h is design were used to obtain r e l i a b i l i t y c o e ffic ie n ts f o r Parts One and Three. Results from the various c on figuration s were reported and discussed f o r Part Two. Altogether, s ix d if f e r e n t groups 65 of students were used during the p i l o t phase, which was completed from January through March of 1984. Experimental Treatment and Control fo r Contaminating Variables The subjects were randomly assigned to one of the three treatment groups in which there were equal numbers of g i r l s and boys except fo r one group, where an a d dition al g i r l was required. E s s e n tia lly , the treatments consisted of each group being presented w ith one of the three versions under as s im ila r conditions as possible. In s tru c tio n s were provided at the beginning explaining th a t t h is was a p ro je c t to I earn what the child re n th in k about s to rie s (see Appendix C). A fter the presentation was completed, students were allowed to stand up and stre tc h to diminish fa tig u e , and questions about what the students thought of the story follow ed. P ilo tin g indicated the necessity of creating as relaxed and spontaneous an atmosphere as possible fo r the effectiveness of the presentations. This was also necessary because both the experimenter and the r e a d e r /s to r y te lle r were u n fa m ilia r to the c h ild re n . Contaminating variables were tre ate d in the fo llo w in g manner: 1) Two l e t t e r s of in s tr u c tio n were sent to the teachers, one general explanatory l e t t e r a few weeks ahead of time and one l e t t e r of s p e c ific in s tr u c tio n s several days ahead of the presentations. Building p rin c ip a ls had discussed the p ro je c t ahead o f time with the involved personnel. 2) Teachers were requested to mention the story p ro je c t ahead o f time to a le r t students to the schedule change and to request students not to ta lk about the story experience 66 u n t il the end of the p r o je c t. Because of a longer time fo r two of the schools from re ceipt of the in s tru c tio n s to the actual p r o je c t, teachers in those two bu ild ings were asked again to remind students not to discuss the p ro je c t. 3) The same room was used in each b u ild in g fo r a l l three presen­ ta tio n s except fo r one session which had to be moved to avoid an unexpected in te r r u p tio n . 4) Students were asked to be seated comfortably on the f l o o r . 5) The presentations f o r each s p e c ific school were given on the same day in the morning. The order of treatment groups was the same. ' 6) The same in s tr u c tio n s were given each group. P ilo tin g estab­ lished the necessity of creating comfortable rapport with the students. For th a t reason, in s tr u c tio n s were explained rather than read verbatim, and as much eye contact as pos­ s ib le was maintained throughout. Any deviations in the in s tru c tio n s were noted in a log (see Appendix C). 7) The s t o r y t e l l e r and the book and reader presentations were standardized as much as possible by the s t o r y t e l l e r . Data C ollectio n A fte r completing the presentations, the subjects were administered the instrument. t a t io n s . The s t o r y t e l l e r was not v is ib le during these presen­ The students were allowed to stand and s tre tc h to diminish fa tig u e and to allow f o r passing out m a te ria ls . The groups were given the same in s tru c tio n s in a s im ila r manner to the i n i t i a l in s tr u c tio n s . Any v a ria tio n s were noted in the log (see Appendix C). 67 The experimenter read aloud the questions and choices twice as the students read s i l e n t l y . Extreme care was taken to insure a stan­ dardized, nonbiased reading of the questions. The subjects were asked to check t h e i r papers to make sure they had answered.all questions. Research Questions and S t a t i s t i c a l Hypotheses 1. What are the find ings of media research in the la s t three decades relevant to understanding the e ffe c t of media on inform ation coding and inform ation po ten tial? 2. Are there in d ic a tio n s th a t a sound s lid e presentation, a reader and p ic tu re book presentation, and a s t o r y t e l l e r pre­ sentation bring d if f e r e n t content to the foreground of atten tio n ? This question is discussed in the review o f the l i t e r a t u r e , the discussion of the find ings of t h is study and the conclusion and recommendations. 3. Are there in d ic a tio n s th a t a ff e c tiv e content as opposed to nonaffective content is more e f f e c t iv e ly brought to the fo re ­ ground of a tte n tio n by any one of the three named modes of presentation? This question is discussed in the review of the l i t e r a t u r e , the discussion of the find ings of th is study and the conclusion and recommendations. 4. Are there in d ic a tio n s th a t c h ild re n make more or fewer i n f e r ­ ences about story content based on, the mode of presentation? This question is discussed in the review of the l i t e r a t u r e , the discussion of the fin d in g s of t h is study and the conclu■ sion and recommendations. 68 5. Are there in d ic a tio n s th a t c h ild re n make d if f e r e n t inferences about story content based on the mode of presentation? This . question is discussed in the review o f the l i t e r a t u r e , the discussion of the fin d in g s of th is study and the conclusion ■ and recommendations. 6. Are there in d ic a tio n s th a t c h ild r e n 's re c a ll of factual mate­ r i a l is based on the mode of presentation they have e xp eri­ enced? This question is discussed in the review o f the l i t e r a t u r e , the discussion of the fin d in g s of t h is study and the conclusion and recommendations. The fo llo w in g n u ll hypotheses were tre ate d s t a t i s t i c a l l y : 1. H0 : There is no d iffe re n c e in t h i r d grade c h ild r e n 's per­ ception of the type of content, s p e c i f i c a l l y , a f f e c t iv e , I content as opposed to n o n a ffe c tive, th a t is brought to the foreground of t h e i r a tte n tio n by three d if f e r e n t modes of presentation: a s t o r y t e l l e r , a book and reader, and a sound s i ide show. 2. H0 : There is no d iffe re n c e in the number of inferences about story content made by t h i r d grade c h ild re n exposed to three d if f e r e n t modes of story presentation: a s to ry te lle r, a book and reader, and a sound s lid e show. 3. H0: There is no d iffe re n c e in in d iv id u a l inferences mqde about story content by t h i r d grade children exposed to three d if f e r e n t modes of story presentation: book and reader, and a sound s lid e show. a s to ry te lle r, a 6 9 4. H0: There is no d iffe re n c e in t h i r d grade c h ild r e n 's re c a ll of factual material based on mode of p resen tatio n-a s t o r y t e l l e r , a book and reader, and a sound s lid e show. Description of Independent and Dependent Variables The design of t h is study incorporated one independent v a ria b le , mode of presentation, w ith three le v e ls : and sound s lid e show. s t o r y t e l l e r , book and reader, There were fou r dependent va ria b le s : I ) per­ ception of s a lie n t content, a ff e c tiv e as opposed to n o naffective; 2) number of inferences about content; 3) choice of inferences about content; and 4) re c a ll of factual content. Instrumentation and Analysis of the Data The instrument was composed of three subsections which were sta­ t i s t i c a l l y analyzed i n d iv id u a lly . The f i r s t subsection yielded a number f o r each subject of a ffe c ­ t i v e content choices in the foreground of a tte n tio n . These were to ta le d and a mean fo r each treatment group c alcu la ted . A one-way analysis o f variance was applied and s t a t i s t i c a l s ig n ific a n c e obtained and reported. No post hoc comparison te s ts were warranted by the obtained s t a t i s t i c a l s ig n ific a n c e , but an in d iv id u a l t te s t was applied f o r two group means. The second subsection, inferences about story content, was tre ate d two ways: response choices one and two were to ta le d together and compared to response choice thre e , i . e . , 111 d o n 't know." A to t a l o f response choices per in d iv id u a l were calculated and a mean per treatment group obtained. A one-way analysis of variance was applied 70 and s t a t i s t i c a l s ig n ific a n c e obtained and reported. A d d itio n a lly , a chi square te s t of independence was applied to each in d iv id u a l question in the second subsection. The t h i r d subsection yielded f o r each subject a number of correct factual re c a ll choices. These were to ta le d and a mean fo r each t r e a t ­ ment group c a lc u la te d . A one-way analysis of variance was applied and s t a t i s t i c a l s ig n ific a n c e obtained and reported. No post hoc comparison te s ts were warranted by the obtained s t a t i s t i c a l s ig n ific a n c e , but in d iv id u a l t te s ts were applied to two sets of the group means. Appropriate d e s c rip tiv e s t a t i s t i c s were also calculated fo r the subsections and reported. S t a tis tic s gathered during the p i l o t phase, in clud ing Pearson product-moment c o rre la tio n c o e ffic ie n ts as a measure of the instrum ent's r e l i a b i l i t y , have been reported along w ith appro­ p r ia te d e s c rip tiv e s t a t i s t i c s . C alculations were made using a computer program, SPSS, S t a t i s t i c a l Package f o r Social Sciences ( Nie, 1975), a v a ila b le through Montana State U n iv e rs ity . Al I research, p r e p ilo t to completion, was done under the supervision of both the research super­ v is o r and the major advisor from t h i s researcher's doctoral committee. Organization of Data The data gathered during the p i l o t and the experimental phases was organized and presented in the fo llo w in g manner. P ilo t Data from Third Grade Classes Parts one, two and three of the instrument were s t a t i s t i c a l l y treated and presented separately. 71 D escriptive s t a t i s t i c s fo r Part O ne --a ffective content choices to n o n a ffe c tive content choices—were recorded fo r p re te s t, post te s t and post-post t e s t . Pearson r , c o r re la tio n c o e f f ic ie n t , fo r pretest to post t e s t , post-post te s t to. p re te s t, and post-post te s t to post t e s t , as well as a ll p r o b a b ility le v e ls and degrees of freedom were reported. An in d iv id u a l t t e s t was also reported. D escriptive s t a t i s t i c s fo r Part Two--number of inferences--were recorded. Inference to no-inference choice was compared overall using a one-way ANOVA and in d iv id u a lly w ith chi squares. Chi square te s t of independence on each question was also recorded pretest to post t e s t , post-post t e s t to p re te s t, and post-post t e s t to post t e s t . Probabil­ i t y lev e ls and degrees o f freedom were reported. D escriptive s t a t i s t i c s f o r Part Three--factual re c a ll--w e re recorded fo r p re te s t, post te s t and post-post t e s t . Pearson r , corre­ la t io n c o e f f ic ie n t , f o r pretest to post t e s t , post-post te s t to pre­ t e s t , and post-post te s t to post t e s t , as well as a ll p r o b a b ility le v e ls and degrees of freedom were reported. Two in d iv id u a l t tests were also reported. P ilo t Data from Sixth Grade Class Parts One, Two and Three of the instrument were s t a t i s t i c a l l y tre ate d and presented separately. > D escriptive s t a t i s t i c s fo r Part O ne --a ffective content choices to nonaffective content choices—were recorded f o r pretest and post t e s t . Pearson r, c o r re la tio n c o e f f ic ie n t , f o r pretest to post t e s t as well as p r o b a b ility leve ls and degrees of freedom were reported. 72 D escriptive s t a t i s t i c s f o r Part Two--IUimber of i nferences--were recorded fo r pretest and post t e s t . Inference to no-inference choice was compared using a one-way ANOVA. Fisher exact te s ts and chi squares were used on in d iv id u a l inference questions. Phi and contingency, c o e ffic ie n t were also reported. D escriptive s t a t i s t i c s fo r Part Thre e --fa c tu a l r e c a l l —were recorded fo r pretest and post t e s t . Pearson r , c o rre la tio n c o e f f i ­ c ie n t, fo r pretest to post te s t as well as p r o b a b ility le v e ls and degrees of freedom were reported. Experimental Treatment Data— Part One: A ffe c tiv e and Nonaffective Content Choices D escriptive s t a t i s t i c s , in c lud ing mean number of a ffe c tiv e choices per treatment group—s t o r y t e l l e r , book and reader, and sound s lid e show—were recorded. A one-way analysis of variance was applied and p r o b a b ility level and degrees of freedom reported. An in d iv id u a l t t e s t between one set of group means was reported. Experimental Treatment Data— Part Two: Inferences D escriptive s t a t i s t i c s were reported. These included a mean num­ ber of responses per treatment group-^number of inferences compared to choice of no inference. A one-way analysis of variance was applied and p r o b a b ility level and degrees of freedom reported. An in d iv id u a l chi square was reported as warranted f o r one question. Chi square te s ts of independence and Fisher exact t e s t s , comparing inference choices by treatment g r o u p - - s to r y te lle r , book and reader, and sound s lid e show— were calculated and reported. 73 Experimental Treatment Da,ta—Part Three: Factual Recall D escriptive s t a t i s t i c s , in c lu d in g mean number of correct choices per treatment g r o u p - - s to r y te lle r , book and reader, and sound s lid e show--were recorded. A one-way analysis of variance was applied and p r o b a b ility level and degrees of freedom reported. Individual t te s ts between two sets of group means were reported. Permissions Al I required permissions, in c lu d in g permission from Dial Press to use the book. The Wild Swans, and permission from School D i s t r i c t Number One personnel, were obtained p r io r to conducting the study. Summary The purposes of t h i s study were I ) to discuss the fin d in g s of media research relevant to understanding the e ffe c t of media on i n f o r ­ mation p o te n tia l and co g n itiv e processes; 2) to determine whether or not there are in d ic a tio n s th a t a s t o r y t e l l e r presentation, a p ic tu re book and reader presentation', and a sound s lid e show bring d if f e r e n t content to the foreground of a tte n tio n ; 3) to determine whether or not there a r e 'in d ic a tio n s th a t a ffe c tiv e content as opposed to nonaffective content is more e f f e c t iv e ly brought to the foreground of a tte n tio n by any one of the three named modes of presentation; 4) to determine whether children make q u a n tita tiv e ly d if f e r e n t inferences based on the mode of story presentation; 5) to determine whether child re n make d if f e r e n t inferences based on the mode o f presentation; and 6) to determine i f c h ild r e n 's re c a ll of factual material is d i f f e r e n t based on the mode of presentation. 74 A f a i r y t a l e . The Wild Swans, by Hans C hristia n Andersen, adapted by Amy E rlic h and Susan J e ffe rs (Andersen, 1981), was selected f o r th is study on the c r i t e r i a of genre, s u i t a b i l i t y fo r s t o r y t e l l i n g , co m p a ra b ility , q u a lity o f the i l l u s t r a t i o n s , size of the book, and a u th e n tic ity of the m a te ria l. The t e x t of the story was shortened to make i t more s u ita b le f o r s t o r y t e llin g w ith t h i r d grade c h ild re n . Preliminary research indicated to the researcher the fo llo w in g methodology fo r accomplishing the purposes of the study: an analysis of the t e x t of the story y ie ld in g types of content as o p e ra tio n a lly defined was completed using a three-stage procedure and a panel of three judges. The judges were asked to determine o p e ra tio n a lly defined content, and those content items fo r which there was not unanimous agreement were re-evaluated by the panel. A th re e -p a rt instrument was developed based on the prototype i n ­ strument from the p relim ina ry research, the story analysis and the student brainstormed analysis from the prelim inary research. Part One of the instrument determined whether a ffe c tiv e or n o n a ffe c tive content was in the foreground of a tte n tio n . Part Two determined an overall number of inferences and differences on in d iv id u a l inferences. Part Three determined re c a ll of factual m a te ria l. A sound s lid e show con sisting of one hundred nine 35 mm slides o f i l l u s t r a t i o n s from the book was developed by the w r it e r a f t e r viewing and comparing techniques fo r commercially produced f i l m s t r ip s of c h ild r e n 's l i t e r a t u r e . The s t o r y t e l l e r , who was also the reader and the na rra to r f o r the s lid e show, was coached by the researcher. 75 A random sample of ninety t h i r d grade students from three schools in the Butte School D i s t r i c t Number One, 1983-1984, was selected fo r the study. ment groups. Students were randomly assigned to one of the three t r e a t ­ A p i l o t of the experimental treatment and the instrumen­ ta t io n was administered to two t h i r d grade classes from a d if f e r e n t , but s im ila r , school. The story t e x t was also read s i l e n t l y and the questions answered by a group of s ix th grade students. A te s t-re te s t design incorporating Pearson product-moment c o rre la tio n c o e ffic ie n t on the instrument was used to determine r e l i a b i l i t y . For the actual study, the three experimental story treatments were presented in a manner to minimize the e ffe c t of contaminating v a ria b le s . The instrument was administered and the data c o lle c te d , tabulated and analyzed. A one-way analysis of variance was used to analyze the three subsections of the instrument and chi square te s t o f independence were applied to in d iv id u a l questions in the second subsection. 76 C h a p t e r 3 Endnotes William Ni kola, former second grade teacher at Irv in g School, Bozeman, Montana, and current teaching a s s is ta n t. Department of Elementary Education, Montana State U n iv e rs ity , Bozeman, Montana. Bonnie S te fa n ic, Community Coordinated Child Care, B utte, Montana. The panel was composed of language a rts and/or English teachers from the Butte School D i s t r i c t Number One, 1983-1984: B i ll M a t t io li , elementary reading teacher; Shelia Youngblood, Acceler­ ated Learners Program language a rts teacher; and Annette Giop, ju n io r high school English teacher. Dr. Gerald S u lliv a n , Professor, Reading Education, Department o f Elementary Education, Montana State U n iv e rs ity , Bozeman, Montana, was the regional language arts expert. Dr. Tim S u lliv a n , Curriculum D ire c to r f o r Butte School D i s t r i c t Number One, Butte, Montana. The program was w ritte n fo r an Apple I Ie computer by Don Plessas, computer coordinator fo r the Butte School D i s t r i c t Number One, and v e r if ie d by Dr. Larry E lIerbruch, Associate Professor, Math Educa­ t i o n , Department o f Elementary Education, Montana State U n iv e rs ity . 77 CHAPTER 4 STATISTICAL ANALYSIS OF DATA In tro d u c ti on This study was designed to determine i f three comparable presen­ ta tio n s o f,a s to r y —a s t o r y t e l l e r , a book and reader, and a sound s lid e show—brought the same c o n te n t-ra ffe c tiv e , inference and f a c t u a l- - t o the foreground of t h i r d grade c h ild re n 's a tte n tio n . The study u t i l i z e d a researcher designed instrument and small groups of students in an actual school s e ttin g . Third grade students, determined as comparable, from three schools in the Butte School D i s t r i c t Number One, were randomly assigned to one. o f three treatment groups and given the story presentations. Presenta­ tio n s were made to groups of te n , equally divided by sex as much as possible. The instrument was administered by the researcher immedi­ a te ly a f t e r the presentation. Prelim inary in v e s tig a tio n required as background fo r th is research, analysis of the story and development of the instrument, determination of comparable groups, determination of instrument r e l i a ­ b i l i t y and experimental procedures and data c o lle c tio n were described in the preceding chapter of t h is study. The s t a t i s t i c a l data collected from th is study is reported and analyzed in th is chapter. 78 Organization of Chapter Four Chapter 4 is organized around the presentation and discussion of the fo llo w in g : I. P ilo t Data 1. An overview o f the p i l o t as i t related to the concept of and determination of instrument r e l i a b i l i t y . 2. Third Grade Data. These data focused on determining the r e l i a b i l i t y of each subpart of the instrument. 1) Part One - A ffe c tiv e Content. i A Pearson r c o rre la tio n c o e ffic ie n t was used to obtain a r e l i a b i l i t y c o e f f i ­ c ie n t: p re te s t to post t e s t ; pretest to post-post; post t e s t to post-post. 2) Part Two - Inferences. A chi square te s t of indepen­ dence was used as a measure of r e l i a b i l i t y on each in d iv id u a l question: p re te s t to post t e s t ; pretest to po st-post; post te s t to post-post. 3) Part,Three - Factual R ecall. A Pearson r c o rre la tio n c o e ffic ie n t was used to obtain a r e l i a b i l i t y c o e f f i ­ c ie n t: p re te s t to post t e s t ; pretest to post-post; post te s t to post-post. 3. Sixth Grade Data. These data also focused on determining the r e l i a b i l i t y of each subpart of the instrum ent. I) Part One - A ffe c tiv e Content. A Pearson r c o rre la tio n c o e ffic ie n t was used to obtain a r e l i a b i l i t y c o e f f ic ien t:- p re te s t to post t e s t . / 79 2) Part Two - Inferences. As a measure of r e l i a b i l i t y , a chi square te s t of independence fo r a 3 x 3 contingency ta b le was used. A F ish er's exact t e s t , p h i, and corre­ la tio n c o e ffic ie n t were used fo r the 2 x 2 ta b le s . 3) Part Three - Factual R ecall. A Pearson r correlatio n, c o e ffic ie n t was used to obtain a r e l i a b i l i t y c o e f f i ­ c ie n t: p re te s t to post t e s t . II. Experimental Data 1. Part One - A ffe c tiv e Content. D escriptive s t a t i s t i c s were reported and a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to compare a ff e c tiv e content choices among the treatment groups—book and reader, s t o r y t e l l e r and sound s lid e show. A t t e s t was also used to compare mean number of a ffe c tiv e content choices between the book and reader and the s to ry ­ t e l l e r modes. 2. Part Two - Inferences. D escriptive s t a t i s t i c s were reported and a one-way ANOVA was used comparing the three treatment groups on inference or no-inference choice. A chi square te s t of independence was used to compare each s p e c ific i n f e r ­ ence question by mode--book and reader, s t o r y t e l l e r and sound s lid e show. 3. Part Three - Factual R ecall. D escriptive s t a t i s t i c s were reported and a one-way ANOVA was used to compare correct responses among the treatment groups--book and reader, s to ry ­ t e l l e r and sound s lid e show. A t te s t was also used to com­ pare mean number of correct responses between the book and 80 reader and the s t o r y t e l l e r modes, and the s t o r y t e l l e r and the sound s lid e show modes. Chapter 4 concludes with a summary. Overview of the P ilo t and Determination of R e l i a b i l i t y Data were gathered on two groups of t h i r d graders ( f o r t y students) to determine the instrum ent's r e l i a b i l i t y , i . e . , s t a b i l i t y over time, as defined by Sax (1980). A t e s t - r e t e s t design was applied as sug­ gested by Ferguson (1981). In the f i r s t a p p lic a tio n of the te s t - r e t e s t design, the story treatment (book and reader) was given both times. three to four week intervening in te rv a l separated the sessions. A An a d dition al re te s t using no story treatment was applied a f t e r examining the re s u lts of the f i r s t sessions since the re s u lts indicated th a t the story treatment i t s e l f might be a contaminating fa c to r in the re te st s it u a tio n . Approximately a month separated these l a t t e r sessions. Each subsection of the instrument was s t a t i s t i c a l l y treated separately. Results of the three sets of data gathered from t h i r d graders did in d ic a te th a t the story treatment in the re te s t s itu a tio n was a contaminating v a ria b le . A d d itio n a lly , students' reading l e v e l, since they were required to read the instrument, could mask r e l i a b i l i t y . The age of the t h i r d grade children made i t too d i f f i c u l t to minimize these fa c to rs . Therefore, data had also been gathered on a class of seventeen s ix th grade students. Since the purpose of the p i l o t was to determine the r e l i a b i l i t y of the instrument i t s e l f , i t seemed appro­ p ria te to create a s itu a tio n in which inte rven ing variables th a t could mask r e l i a b i l i t y were minimized. Since both memory and fo r g e ttin g are 81 forms of in te rv e n tio n , an attempt was made to balance these two, m in i­ mize the e ffe c t of reading level and elim in ate the variables of s to ry ­ t e l l e r e ffe c t and a d dition al treatm ent. The s ix th graders were asked to read the story s i l e n t l y and then respond s i l e n t l y to the instrument. A week l a t e r , the students were again administered the instrument s ile n tly . I t was hoped th a t t h is form of the t e s t - r e t e s t design would balance the period of time and te s tin g s itu a tio n s th a t were discussed by Barr et al (1953) and G u ilfo rd (1965) as c o n trib u tin g to flu c tu a ­ tio n s in measures of r e l i a b i l i t y . Third Grade P ilo t Data Part One - A ffe c tiv e Content Part One of the instrument yielded f o r each subject a number of a ffe c tiv e content choices in the foreground of a tte n tio n . Data were colle c te d on f o r t y t h i r d graders to determine r e l i a b i l i t y of t h is sub­ sectio n, i . e . , s t a b i l i t y as defined by Sax (1980). A Pearson product moment c o rre la tio n c o e ffic ie n t (Pearson r) as suggested by G uilfo rd (1973) was used in the tre a tm e n t/te s t-tre a tm e n t/re te s t design. A three to four week in te rv a l separated the sessions which were desig­ nated as pretest and post t e s t . An a d d itio n a l te s tin g (but no t r e a t ­ ment) session was administered approximately a month l a t e r . designated as post-post t e s t . This was A Pearson r was calculated pretest to post t e s t , pretest to post-post and post te s t to post-post. Results, pretest to post t e s t , w ith an n of 40, yielde d a Pearson r c o rre la tio n c o e ffic ie n t = .4826 w ith an associated p r o b a b ility of p^.OOl. R esults, pretest to post-post, w ith an n of 35, yielde d a 82 Pearson r c o rre la tio n c o e f f ic ie n t = .3342 w ith an associated proba­ b i l i t y o f pj^.025. Results, post te s t to post-post, w ith an n of 33, yielde d a Pearson r c o rre la tio n c o e f f ic ie n t = .5165 with an associated p r o b a b ility o f pjc.001. See Table I . Williams (1979) has c ite d G u ilfo rd (1956) as suggesting a corre­ la tio n of .4826- to represent a moderate c o rre la tio n and G uilfo rd (1965) has emphasized th a t the c o rre la tio n is always re la tiv e to the s itu a tio n under which i t is obtained and must be in te rp re te d in l i g h t of those circumstances. However, in p ra c tic e , r e l i a b i l i t y c o e ffic ie n ts are usu ally expected to be in the upper brackets of r , i . e. , .70 to .98, a c r i t e r i o n not reached by these c o rre la tio n c o e ffic ie n t s . The no tic e ­ able drop in the c o rre la tio n c o e f f ic ie n t , pretest to post-post ( r = .3342), as contrasted w ith the higher c o rre la tio n c o e f f ic ie n t , post t e s t to post-post ( r = .5165), in which no treatment was administered, post-post, indicated a contamination e ff e c t of the addition al treatment which l i k e l y affected the c o rre la tio n c o e f f ic ie n t . An attempt was made to control fo r t h is and other fa c to rs in the design using s ix th grade students, the re s u lts of which are reported fu r th e r in t h is chapter. Table I . Third Grade A ffe c tiv e Content Choices Pearson r C orrelatio n C o e ffic ie n t Pretest to Post Test Pretest to Post-Post Post Test to Post-Post 40 35 33 r .4826 .3342 .5165 P< .001 .025 Terms: n = number of students Pearson r = c o r re la tio n c o e ffic ie n t p = associated p r o b a b ility O O n 83 Part Two - Inferences Part Two of the instrument yielded a s p e c ific inference choice on sixteen separate items. Since the data in th is subsection were'nominal data, a chi square t e s t of independence, as suggested by Spatz and Johnston (1981) was used as a measure of r e l i a b i l i t y , i . e . , s t a b i l i t y , on each in d iv id u a l question. This data yielded a set of two by three . contingency tables with an expected frequency of less than f iv e in at le a s t f i f t y percent of the c e lls of each question. Spatz and Johnston (1981) and G u ilfo rd (1965) have suggested th a t when expected frequen­ cies are small, frequency c e lls may be combined, provided the combina­ tio n s are lo g ic a l. A d if f e r e n t op tio n , when combinations are not l o g ic a l , could be discarding one of the a lte rn a tiv e s provided few chose the a lte r n a tiv e . Since inference choice th re e , i . e . , " I do n't know," was a consistent option and seldom chosen, i t seemed prudent to discard th a t a lte rn a tiv e fo r these data. This reduced the sets to two by two contingency ta b le s . Results of the chi square have been reported two ways: with the Yates' correctio n when expected frequency in one of the c e lls is fiv e or less, as suggested by Spatz and Johnston (1981), and w ithout the Yates' c o rre c tio n , as suggested by Ferguson (1981). Ferguson has reported studies th a t in d ic a te the Yates' correctio n is unduly conser­ v a tiv e fo r data which conform to random and mixed models. He suggests th a t use of the Yates' co rre c tio n w i l l lead to too few s t a t i s t i c a l l y s ig n if ic a n t re s u lts , and th a t the chi square te s t without the correc­ t io n w i l l provide a reasonably accurate protectio n against Type I 84 errors when the number of subjects is greater than or equal to eight (1981). Results of p retest to post te s t chi square analysis indicated four inference questions s ig n if ic a n t at p_<.05 when the Yates' corecti on was applied, and six inference questions s ig n if ic a n t at p_<.05 w ithout the Yates c o rre c tio n . Results of ^pretest to post-post chi square analysis indicated no inference questions s ig n if ic a n t at p_<.05 when the Yates' c orrectio n was applied, and no inference questions s ig n if ic a n t at p<_.05 w ithout the Yates' c o rre c tio n . Results of post t e s t to post-post chi square analysis indicated two inference questions s ig n if ic a n t at p£.05 when the Yates' correction was applied, and f iv e inference questions s ig n if ic a n t at p<.05 without the Yates' c o rre c tio n . These re s u lts were used as part of the measures o f r e l i a b i l i t y on the inference questions and were necessary to i n t e r ­ pret re s u lts of the f in a l data. See Tables 11 and I I I . Part Three - Factual Recall Part Three of the instrument yielded a number of c o rre c t factual re c a ll items. Data to determine r e l i a b i l i t y , i . e . , s t a b i l i t y , of th is subsection were colle c te d pretest to post t e s t , pretest to post-post, and post te s t to post-post, using a Pearson r c o rre la tio n c o e ffic ie n t as in subsection one and the same intervening time in te r v a ls . Pearson r c o rre la tio n c o e f f ic ie n t , pretest to post t e s t , with an n of 40, was r = .4963 w ith an associated p r o b a b ility o f of p_<.001. Pearson r c o rre la tio n c o e f f ic ie n t , pretest to post-post, w ith an n of 35, was r = .3671 w ith an associated p r o b a b ility of p<_.015. Pearson r Table I I . Third Grade P ilo t Inference Questions - Chi Square Pretest to Post Test Inference Question I 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Il 12 13 14 15 16 Terms: Raw Chi Square 32 2.74286 39 12.31579 33 6.30317 .03733 28 .39583 38 29 2.70813 34 14.48804 36 6.18132 39 .11729 29 1.74127 .11395 39 37 8.15716 .57963 37 34 I .44947 34 4.85917 n Pretest to Post-Post Post Test to Post-Post P< df n Raw Chi Square P< df .098 .000 .012 .847 .529 .010 .000 .013 .732 .187 .736 .004 .447 .229 .028 I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I 31 27 32 29 22 34 30 27 33 33 28 34 33 28 31 32 .11071 3.30750 I .91210 I .83044 .64706 .42929 I .15385 .12706 .34921 3.09770 .53904 .06439 .70875 2.54545 .22610 I .01349 .739 .069 .167 .176 .421 .512 .283 .722 .555 .078 .463 .800 .400 .111 .634 .314 I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I n Raw Chi Square 32 32 5.71320 31 5.37333 26 7.46173 25 .44643 30 .37037 25 I .8828 25 .00113 30 3.80952 31 .24800 28 14.93333 32 31 I .23358 27 .02096 26 4.47262 27 .03444 . — P< df __ .017 .020 .006 .504 .543 .170 .973 .051 .619 .000 — .267 .885 .034 .853 n = number of students Inference question = s p e c ific inference question from Part I I of instrument Raw chi Square = chi square t e s t of independence without Yates' c orrectio n p.= associated p r o b a b ility d f = degrees of freedom - - = SPSS does not compute s t a t i s t i c s when the number of non-empty rows or columns is one. N onsignificant at p£.05 as determined by hand c a lc u l a t i on. I I I I I I I I I I — I I I I Table I I I . Third Grade P ilo t Inference Questions - Chi Square with Yates' Correction Pretest to Post-Post Pretest to Post Test Inference Question I 2. 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 ■ 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 Terms: n __ Raw Chi Square P< df __ .30476 32 39 2.58717 33 4.61062 .000 28 .000 38 1 .18966 29 34 11.98633 36 4.15632 .0000 39 .64391 29 .0000 39 37 4.85656 .04823 37 34 .68050 34 3.31955 .581 .108 .031 1.000 I .000 .275 .001 .042 I .000 .422 I .000 .028 .826 .409 .069 I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I n Raw Chi Square 31 .00001 27 .42188 32 .14222 29 .95194 22 .0000 34 .0000 30 .03205 27 .0000 33 .03410 33 I .14188 28 .09358 34 .0000 33 .00061 28 I .13131 31 .00022 32 .32648 P< I .000 .516 .706 .329 I .000 1.000 .858 1.000 .854 .285 .760 I .000 .980 .288 .988 .568 Post Test to Post-Post df I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I n Raw Chi Square 27 .97470 31 .87635 26 5.46181 25 .01240 30 .0000 25 .50760 25 .0000 30 2.38757 31 .0000 28 11.84531 — 31 27 26 27 — .04186 .0000 2.92597 .0000 P< .324 .349 .019 .911 I .000 .476 I .000 .122 I .000 .001 — .838 I .000 .087 I .000 df I I I I I I I I I I — I I I I n = number o f students Inference question = s p e c ific inference question from subpart I I o f instrument Corrected chi square = chi square t e s t of independence using Yates' correctio n p = associated p r o b a b ility d f = degrees of freedom - - = SPSS does not compute s t a t i s t i c s when the number of non-empty rows or columns is one. N onsignificant at p_<.05 as determined by hand c a lc u la tio n . 87 c o rre la tio n c o e f f ic ie n t , post te s t to po st-post, with an n of 33, was r = .5492 w ith an associated p r o b a b ility o f p£.000, as reported by SPSS, which c a rrie s re s u lts to fou r decimal places. These r e l i a b i l i t y c o e ffic ie n ts were not considered acceptable and an attempt was made to control fo r possible contaminating variables in the design reported in the next section. Table IV. See Table IV. Third Grade Factual Recall Pearson r C orrelation C o e ffic ie n t Pretest to Post Test Pretest to Post-Post 33 n 40 r .4963 .3671 .5492 P< .001 .015 .000 Terms: 35 Post Test to Post-Post n = number of students Pearson r = c o r re la tio n c o e ffic ie n t p = associated p r o b a b ility P ilo t Data - Sixth Grade Students A tre a tm e n t/te s t-r e te s t design was applied to a class of seventeen s ix th grade students attending the same school as the p i l o t t h i r d graders. To minimize the e ffe c ts of reading level and nature of the d e liv e ry , the story treatment consisted of the students reading the t e x t s i l e n t l y and responding s i l e n t l y to the questions. An in te rv a l of a week separated the te s t from re te s t, a period of time determined to. be a'balance between the masking e ffe c ts o f memory and of fo r g e t tin g . These sessions were designated as pretest and post t e s t . 88 P a r t One - A f f e c t i v e Co n te n t Part One of the instrument yielded fo r each subject a number of a ffe c tiv e content choices in the foreground of a tte n tio n . A Pearson r c o rre la tio n c o e f f ic ie n t , calculated p re te s t to post t e s t , was used as a measure of r e l i a b i l i t y . Results calculated with an n of 17 yielded a Pearson r = .6569 w ith an associated p r o b a b ility of p_<.002. Table V. See Though modest, t h is r e l i a b i l i t y c o e ffic ie n t ind ica tes im­ proved control of the contaminating variables which seemed in f lu e n t ia l in the design w ith t h i r d graders and is in d ic a tiv e of a moderate c o r r e l a tio n . Table V. Sixth Grade A ffe c tiv e Content Choices Pearson r C orrelation C o e ffic ie n t Pretest to Post Test Terms: n 17 r .6569 P< .002 n = number of students Pearson r = c o r re la tio n c o e ffic ie n t p = associated p r o b a b ility Part Two - Inferences Part Two of the instrument, y ie ld in g a s p e c ific inference choice on sixteen items, was tre ate d both as a 3 x 3 contingency ta b le , in clud ing choice "3 - I d o n 't know," and a 2 x 2 contingency ta b le , o m ittin g choice "3 ." number of cases. ta b le . Data' were reported both ways because of the small Chi square was reported f o r the 3 x 3 contingency SPSS applies a F ish er's tw o -ta ile d exact te s t fo r fewer than 89 twenty-one cases. I t is in te rp re te d d i r e c t l y , i . e . , the p r o b a b ility o f g e ttin g th a t d i s t r ib u t io n of frequencies under the n u ll hypothesis of no re la tio n s h ip is th a t s p e c ific fig u re (Schutte, 1977). Chi square and Fisher exact t e s t fig u re s were reported as measures of r e l i a b i l i t y . Some scores may be spurious and must be c au tiously in te rp re te d because o f small c e ll frequency. Phi and a contingency c o e f f ic ie n t were also reported f o r the 2 x 2 ta b le s . Both are measures of the strength of a re la tio n s h ip and are based on chi square. Phi corrects fo r the fa c t th a t the chi square value is d i r e c t l y proportional to the number o f cases. I t assumes a value of zero when no re la tio n s h ip e x is ts and a value of +1 when v a ria ­ bles are p e rfe c tly re la te d . Contingency c o e ffic ie n t assumes a minimum value of zero and a maximum value of .707 f o r a 2 x 2 ta b le ( Nie et a l . , 1980). See Table VI. Results of the chi square analysis indicated four inference ques­ tio n s w ith an associated p r o b a b ility of p<_.05. See Table V I I . Results o f the F is h e r's exact t e s t indicated one inference question with an associated p r o b a b ility of p_<.05. See Table VI. These re s u lts , used as measures of r e l i a b i l i t y , in conjunction w ith the t h ir d grade data, must be cau tiously in te rp re te d because of small c e ll frequencies. Part Three - Factual Recall Part Three of the instrument yielde d f o r each subject a number of correct factual re c a ll items. A Pearson r c o rre la tio n c o e f f ic ie n t , calculated p re te s t to post t e s t , was used as a measure of r e l i a b i l i t y . Results calculated w ith an n of 17 yielded a Pearson r = .6570 with an associated p r o b a b ility o f p_<.002. See Table V I I I . Though modest. 90 t h is r e l i a b i l i t y c o e ffic ie n t ind ica tes improved control of the contami­ nating variables which seemed i n f l u e n t i a l in the design w ith t h ir d graders and is in d ic a tiv e of a moderate c o rre la tio n . Table VI. Sixth Grade Inference Questions Pretest to Post Test Choice "3" Omitted (based on 2 x 2 Contingency Table) Inference Question! n Fisher's Exact Test 3 4 5 8 9 11 12 14 15 16 15 7 10 12 12 13 15 11 13 16 .13333 .28571 .2000 .54545 . .18182 .15385 .13333 .15152 .00466 I .0000 Phi .68139 .64550 .6667 .35355 .57735 .67700 .68139 .54167 .85391 .14907 > Contingency C o e ffic ie n t .56309 .54233 .55470 .33333 .5000 .56061 .56309 .47628 .64937 .14744 I Inference questions I , 2, 6, 7, 10, and 13 are omitted as SPSS does not compute s t a t i s t i c s when the number of non-empty, rows or columns is one. Terms: n = number of cases F is h e r's Exact Test = tw o -ta ile d t e s t . Figure in te rp re te d d i r e c t l y as associated p r o b a b ilit y . (SPSS applies w ith 2 x 2 ta b le , n<21.) Phi = measure of strength of re la tio n s h ip based on chi square. Values range from no re la tio n s h ip , "O," to " + I." Contingency c o e f f ic ie n t = measure of strength of re la tio n s h ip based on chi square. Values range from "0" to .707 f o r a 2 x 2 ta b le . I 91 Table V II. Inference Question I 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 Sixth Grade Inference Questions Pretest to Post Test - Includes Choice "3" n 17 17 17 17 17 17 17 17 17 17 17 17 17 17 17 Chi Square P< df .003 .264 .082 4 4 4 Fish er's Exact Test, 2-Tailed! .21891 15.87245 5.23983 8.27513 ' .33088 .12059 4.41558 7.55556 .353 .109 4 4 7.96875 7.96875 .14167 6.54815 10.83750 .9444 .019 .019 .932 .162 .029 .624 2 4 2 4 4 2 .94118 ! f i s h e r 's exact t e s t , 2 - t a ile d , is reported instead of chi square, when contingency ta b le is reduced to 2 x 2 because of student response p a tte rn . P ro b a b ility level is in te rp re te d d i r e c t l y . Terms: n = number of cases Inference question = s p e c ific inference question from subpart I I of instrument Chi square = chi square te s t of independence p = associated p r o b a b ility df = degrees of freedom - - = SPSS does not compute s t a t i s t i c s when number of non­ empty rows or columns is one. Nonsignificant at p j\0 5 as determined by hand c a lc u la to r. 92 Table V I I I . Sixth Grade Factual Recall Pearson r C orrelatio n C o e ffic ie n t Pretest to Post Test Terms: n 17 r .6570 P< .002 n = number of students Pearson r = c o r re la tio n c o e ffic ie n t p = associated p r o b a b ility Experimental Data Overview A sample of ninety t h i r d grade students from three schools in Butte School D i s t r i c t Number One were randomly assigned to one of three experimental story treatment groups--book and reader, s t o r y t e l l e r , and sound s lid e show. Treatment groups consisted of ten students each, equally divided by sex as much as possible. Students were administered the story treatment and the instrument under as s im ila r conditions as possible, given the actual school s e tt in g . Total number of students receiving each treatment was t h i r t y . . The instrument used to c o lle c t the data was designed to determine whether each of the three modes of presentation brought d if f e r e n t c o n te n t--a ffe c tiv e or n o naffective, inference, and f a c t u a l—to the foreground o f a tte n tio n . Results o f the data obtained from the e xp eri­ mental treatment are presented in the fo llo w in g section. .93 Part One - A ffe c tiv e Content This subsection yielde d f o r each subject a number of a ffe c tiv e content choices in the foreground of a tte n tio n . These were to ta le d fo r each treatment group--book and reader, s t o r y t e l l e r , and sound s lid e show--and a mean f o r each treatment group calcu la ted . The book and reader mode yielded a mean number of a ff e c tiv e responses, x = 10.20, with a standard deviation o f 2.4969. The s t o r y t e l l e r mode yielded a mean number of a ffe c tiv e responses, x = 9.2333, w ith a standard devia­ tio n of 2.1764. The sound s lid e mode yielde d a mean number of a ffe c ­ t i v e responses, x = 9.3667, w ith a standard deviation of 2.6325. See Table IX. Table IX. A ffe c tiv e Content Choices by Story Treatment - Descriptives Mode Terms: n X Standard Deviation Book and Reader 30 10.2000 2.4969 S torytel Ie r 30 9.2333 2.1764 Sound S lide Show 30 9.3667 2.6325 n = number of students X = mean number of a ff e c tiv e responses Standard d e viation = standard de viation fo r the mean Mode = story treatment A one-way ANOVA was applied comparing these means to t e s t the n u ll hypothesis th a t there is no d iffe re n c e in t h ir d grade c h ild re n 's perceptions of a ffe c tiv e content as opposed to nonaffective content brought to the foreground of t h e i r a tte n tio n by the three modes of presentation (book and reader, s t o r y t e l l e r and sound s lid e show). 94 Results of the ANOVA yielded an F r a t io = 1.380 with an associated p r o b a b ility leve l of p_<.2571 w ith two degrees of freedom. In view o f these re s u lts , the nu ll hypothesis was accepted and post hoc comparison te s ts were not warranted. Table X. See Table X. A ffe c tiv e Content Choices by Story Treatment - ANOVA F Ratio Pl 1.380 .257 df 2 One-way ANOVA comparing mean number of a ff e c tiv e content choices by mode. Terms: F r a t i o = s t a t i s t i c calculated to determine s t a t i s t i c a l s ig n ific a n c e f o r ANOVA p = associated p r o b a b ility d f = degrees of freedom An in d iv id u a l t t e s t was applied comparing the mean of the book and reader treatment to the mean of the s t o r y t e l l e r treatment since the book and reader mean was outside the 95 percent confidence in te rv a l of the s t o r y t e l l e r mean. Results of the t te s t yielded a t value = 1.60 w ith a tw o -ta ile d associated p r o b a b ility o f p<J1.5 w ith 58 degrees of freedom. These re s u lts could not be considered to in d ic a te there was a s t a t i s t i c a l l y s ig n if ic a n t d iffe re n c e between these two means at a p r o b a b ility le v e l, p£.05. See Table XI. 95 Table XI. Students' t Test - Book and R ea der/S toryteller Comparison x Number A ffe c tiv e Content Choices X X Book & Storyt Reader t e l I er Value 10.2000 9.2333 Terms: Pl I .60 .115 df 58 x = mean number of a ffe c tiv e content choices t value = students' t s t a t i s t i c calculated to determine s t a t i s t i c a l s ig n ific a n c e of x differences p = associated p r o b a b ility d f = degrees of freedom Part Two - Inferences Data from the second subsection of the instrument were used to t e s t two nu ll hypotheses: I ) th a t there is no d iffe re n c e in the number o f inferences about story content made by t h i r d grade child re n exposed to the three d if f e r e n t modes of story presentation and 2) th a t there is no d iffe re n c e in in d iv id u a l inferences about story content made by t h i r d grade children exposed to the three modes of story presentation. To te s t the f i r s t of these n u ll hypotheses--that of no diffe ren ce in the number of inferences about story con ten t--th e sets o f 3 x 3 con­ tingency tables yielded by the data were collapsed in to 2 x 3 tab le s . This was accomplished by combining response choices one and two together and comparing them to response choice three, i . e . , " I don't know," which was considered as the "no inference" choice. Total res­ ponse choices per in d iv id u a l were calculated and a mean per treatment group obtained. Analysis of the book and reader data yielded a mean, X = 13.533, w ith a standard deviation of 2.2397. Analysis of the s t o r y t e l l e r data yielded a mean, X = 13.9333, with a standard devia­ t io n of 2.4766. Analysis of the sound s lid e data yielded a mean. 9 6 x = 13.7778, with a standard de viation o f 2.5297. A one-way ANOVA was applied to determine whether there was a s t a t i s t i c a l l y s ig n if ic a n t d iffe re n c e among these means. Results of the ANOVA yielde d an F r a tio = .211 w ith an associated p r o b a b ility o f p_<.810 w ith two degrees of freedom. In view of these re s u lts , the nu ll hypothesis was accepted and post hoc comparison te s ts were not warranted. Table X II. Inference Choice to No Inference Choice by Mode Descripi:ives Mode Terms: See Table X II. X AMOVA Standard Deviation F Ratio P< Book and Reader 13.5333 2.2397 S torytel Ie r 13.9333 2.4766 Sound Slide 13.8667 2.8975 .211 .810 df 2 Inference = Response choices I and 2 combined No inference = "3 - I d o n 't know" response Mode = Story treatment x = Mean F Ratio = S t a t i s t i c calculated to determine associated p r o b a b ility of ANOVA p = associated p r o b a b ility d f = degrees of freedom The in d iv id u a l questions were analyzed by a chi square t e s t of independence comparing inference choice ( i . e. , response choice one and two combined) to no inference choice ( i . e . , response choice thre e , " I d o n 't know"). From t h is a n a ly s is , only one of the sixteen questions, i . e . . Question #9, yielded re s u lts in d ic a tiv e of a s ig n if ic a n t d i f f e r ­ ence in response p a tte r n J The analysis of Question #9 yie ld e d a chi square value = 8.8889, with an associated p r o b a b ility o f p_<.012 w ith two degree^ of freedom. The response pattern fo r the book and reader 97 and the s t o r y t e l l e r group were id e n tic a l and d if f e r e n t from the response pattern of the sound s lid e group. Chi Square and Response Pattern - Inference Question #9 Mode n Students Choosing Inference Students Choosing No Inference Chi Square Book, and Reader 30 29 I S to r y t e lle r 30 29 I Sound Slide 30 23 7 Terms: 8.88889 P< df O ro Table X I I I . See Table X I I I . 2 Inference = An inference, e ith e r response choices I or 2, was chosen. No inference = Response choice "3 - I d o n 't know" Mode = Story treatment p = associated p r o b a b ility d f = degrees of freedom To t e s t the n u ll hypothesis th a t there is no d iffe re n c e in i n d i ­ vidual inferences about story content made by t h i r d grade children exposed to the three d if f e r e n t modes of story presentation, a chi square te s t of independence was applied to each inference question comparing the three response choices by mode. This analysis yielded re s u lts in d ic a tin g three of the questions demonstrated s ig n if ic a n t d iffe ren ces in response pattern by mode;! analysis of Question #9 yielde d a chi square value = 13.01316 with an associated p r o b a b ility pjC.OlI w ith fou r degrees of freedom. Analysis of Question #12 yielded a chi square value = 10.28270 with an associated p r o b a b ility of p£.036 w ith four degrees of freedom. Analysis of Question #15 yielde d a chi square value = 11 .77697 w ith an associated p r o b a b ility p£.019 with fo u r degrees of freedom. Al I three of these questions were considered 98 r e li a b l e , with acceptable associated p r o b a b ility levels by one or more o f the p i l o t measures, although one must be in te rp re te d q u ite cau­ t i o u s l y , as discussed below. Table XIV. See Table XIV. Chi Square and Response Patterns - Inference Questions #9, #12, and #151 Question #9 Story Treat­ ment n Question #12 Question #15 Choice Choice Choice Choice Choice Choice Choice Choice Choice I 2 3 I 2 3 I 2 3 Book & Reader 30 5 24 I 4 21 5 20 2 8 Storyte l I er 30 9 20 I I 28 I 12 12 6 Sound SI ide 30 10 13 7 7 22 I 18 4 8 Chi Square 13.01316 10.28270 11.77697 Pl .011 .036 .019 df 4 4 4 Terms: n = Total number of students per treatment group Choice I = Inference choice " I " Choice 2 = Inference choice "2" Choice 3 = Inference choice "3 = I d o n 't know" p = Associated p r o b a b ility df = degrees of freedom For Question #9, p re te s t to post t e s t , the associated p r o b a b ility of the chi square was p_<.013. Results o f the Yates' corrected chi square, pretest to post t e s t , yielded an associated p r o b a b ility of p_<.042. These values, as measures of r e l i a b i l i t y , f a l l w ith in the acceptable range of a p r o b a b ility l e v e l, p_<.05. See Table XV. 99 Table XV. R e l i a b i l i t y of Inference Questions #9, #12, and #15 Type o f Test Grade Level Question #9 Question #12 Question #15 P< P< P< Chi Square - Pre­ te s t to Post Test 3 .013 NS . NS Chi Square - Post to Post-Post 3 .051 NS .034 3 .042 NS NS 6 ■ NS NS .005 3 NA .0000* NA 6 NA .019 NA Chi Square with Yates1-P re te st to Post Fisher Exact Test Pretest to Post Test Chi Square - 3 x 3 Tables - Pretest to Post-Post Chi Square - 3 x 3 Tables - Pretest to Post Terms: • Grade Level = Grade level of students from which data was taken. Fisher Exact Test = SPSS applies f o r 2 x 2 ta b le w ith fewer than 21 cases p = associated p r o b a b ility NS = Associated p r o b a b ility _<.05 NA - Not ap plicab le . Other data was used fo r r e l i a b i l i t y fo r these questions. *SPSS c a rrie s out to fou r decimal places. For Question #12, an acceptable r e l i a b i l i t y measure was obtained with two sets of data in which inference choice "3" was retained. These re s u lts must be in te rp re te d in view o f the fa c t th a t these data sets were not reported as measures of r e l i a b i l i t y fo r the other two questions because of small c e ll frequency in those sets. An acceptable r e l i a b i l i t y measure was obtained pretest to post-post from the chi square value w ith an associated p r o b a b ility p<.0000, which SPSS c a rrie s out to four decimal places. The s ix th grade data also yielde d a chi 100 square value with an acceptable associated p r o b a b ility p<.019. While these chi square values in d ic a te acceptable r e l i a b i l i t y fo r Question #12, t h is r e l i a b i l i t y must be viewed very cau tiously since these data sets were not used fo r the other two questions. data must be in te rp re te d in t h is context. Results of the fin a l See Table XV. An acceptable chi square value as a measure of r e l i a b i l i t y was obtained f o r Question #15, post te s t to post-post, w ith an associated p r o b a b ility p<_.034 and w ith the s ix th grade data, pretest to post t e s t , Fisher exact value = .00466. See Table XV. In view o f the re s u lts of the chi square analyses of the fin a l data, the n u ll hypotheses of no d iffe re n c e in in d iv id u a l inferences about story content made by t h i r d grade child re n exposed to the d i f f e r ­ ent presentation modes was accepted except fo r three s p e c ific inference questions. The n u ll hypothesis f o r these three questions was rejected at a p r o b a b ility level p£.05. Al I three of these questions were con­ sidered r e lia b le , although in one case in te rp re te d c a u tio u s ly , by re s u lts obtained by one or more of the p i l o t measures. Part Three - Factual Recall *1 This subsection yielded a number of correct factual responses fo r each sub ject. These were to ta le d f o r each treatment group—book and reader, s t o r y t e l l e r and sound s lid e show—and a mean f o r each t r e a t ­ ment group calcu la ted . The book and reader mode yielded a mean number o f correct responses x = 12.500 w ith a standard deviatipn = 1 .8336. The s t o r y t e l l e r mode yielded a mean number of correct responses x = 1 1 .7333 w ith a standard deviation = 2.0331 . The sound s lid e show mode 101 yielde d a mean number of correct responses x = 12.1667 w ith a standard d e v ia tio n = 1.9667. Table XVI. Terms: See Table XVI. Factual Recall by Story Treatment - Descriptives Mode n X Book and Reader 30 12.5000 1 .8336 S torytel I er 30 11.7333 2.0331 Sound Slide 30 12.1667 I .9667 Sta. Dev. n = Number of students x = Mean number of correct factual re c a ll responses Sta. Dev. = Standard de viation fo r the mean Mode = Story treatment A one-way ANOVA was applied comparing these means to t e s t the null hypothesis th a t there is no d iffe re n c e in t h i r d graders' re c a ll of fac tu a l material based on mode of presentation--book and reader, s to ry ­ t e l l e r and sound s lid e show. Results of the ANOVA yielde d an F r a tio = 1.170 w ith an associated p r o b a b ility le v e l of p O l S l w ith two degrees of freedom. In view of these re s u lts , the n u ll hypothesis was accepted and post hoc comparison te s ts were not warranted. Table XVII. See Table XVII. Factual Recall by Story Treatment - ANOVA F Ratio Pl 1.170 .315 df 2 One-way ANOVA comparing mean number correct factual re c a ll items by mode. Terms: F r a t io = S t a t i s t i c used to determine s ig n ific a n c e o f ANOVA p = Associated p r o b a b ility d f = Degrees of freedom 102 Individual t te s ts were applied comparing means of. the book and reader and the sound s lid e treatment to th a t of the s t o r y t e l l e r since those means were e ith e r outside or close to the outer l i m i t of the 95 i percent confidence in te rv a l f o r the s t o r y t e l l e r mean. Results of the t te s t comparing book and reader mode to s t o r y t e l l e r mode yielded a t value = 1.53 w ith an associated p r o b a b ilit y , tw o - ta ile d , o f p£.131 w ith 58 degrees of freedom. Results of the t te s t comparing sound s lid e mode to s t o r y t e l l e r mode yielded a t value = -.8 4 w ith an asso­ ciated p r o b a b ilit y , tw o - ta ile d , of p£.405 with 58 degrees of freedom. See Table X V III. These re s u lts could not be considered to in d ic a te there was a s t a t i s t i c a l l y s ig n if ic a n t d iffe re n c e between these means at a p r o b a b ility le v e l, p_<.05. Table X V III. Students' t Test - Comparison x Number Correct Factual Recall Items Book and Reader/S to ry tel I er X t Val ue Storydf Tel I e r X Sound SI ide I 1.53 .131 58 11.7333 12.1667 t Val ue «3OO 12.5000 11.7333 Terms: X X Book & StoryReader Tel Ie r Sound S lid e /S to r y te lle r P< df .405 58 x = Mean number correct fac tu a l re call items t value = Student's t s t a t i s t i c p = Associated p r o b a b ility d f = Degrees of freedom Summary The inform ation compiled in Chapter Four has considered the re s u lts of the p i l o t data co lle c te d on t h i r d grade and s ix th grade students comparing three modes of story treatment--book and reader. 103 s t o r y t e l l e r and sound s lid e show—on a ff e c tiv e content choices, i n f e r ­ ences and factual r e c a ll. 1. A Pearson r c o rre la tio n c o e f f ic ie n t r = .6569 w ith an asso^ ciated p r o b a b ility of p£.002 was obtained from the. s ix th grade pretest to post te s t data. Part One of the instrument, in d ic a tin g moderate c o rre la tio n and an acceptable though modest measure of r e l i a b i l i t y fo r th is study. 2. Reported chi square and Fisher exact te s ts as measures of r e l i a b i l i t y fo r Part Two yielde d s ig n if ic a n t r e s u lts , some needing cautious in te r p r e ta t io n , on ten of sixteen questions. 3. A Pearson r c o rre la tio n c o e f f ic ie n t r = .6570 w ith an asso­ ciated p r o b a b ility of p£.002 was obtained from the s ix th grade pretest to post te s t data. Part Three of the in s tr u ­ ment, in d ic a tin g moderate c o rre la tio n and an acceptable though modest measure of r e l i a b i l i t y fo r t h is study. 4. Results from a one-way ANOVA on the experimental data yielded no s ig n if ic a n t d iffe re n c e among the mean number of a ffe c tiv e content choices brought to the foreground of a tte n tio n by each of the three modes of story presentation. In view of these r e s u lts , the n u ll hypothesis of no d iffe re n c e in t h i r d grade c h ild r e n 's perception of a ffe c tiv e content as opposed to n o n a ffe c tive content brought to the foreground of t h e ir a tte n tio n by each of three modes of story presentation was i accepted. 5. Results from a one-way ANOVA on the experimental data yielded no s ig n if ic a n t d iffe re n c e —inference choice to no-inference 104 choice--among the three modes of story presentation. In view of these re s u lts , the nu ll hypothesis of no d iffe re n c e in the number of inferences about story content made by t h i r d grade children exposed to the three d if f e r e n t modes of story pre­ sentation was accepted w ith the fo llo w in g q u a lif ic a t io n : re s u lts of chi square te s ts of independence on the in d iv id u a l inference questions indicated th a t on one of the sixteen questions there was a s ig n if ic a n t d iffe re n c e in the number o f inferences made by the c h ild re n . Results from the experimental data chi square te s ts of inde­ pendence on the in d iv id u a l questions yielded no s ig n if ic a n t d if f e r e n t response patterns on th ir te e n of the questions and s ig n if ic a n t l y d i f f e r e n t response patterns on three o f the questions. In view of these re s u lts , the n u ll hypothesis of no d iffe re n c e in in d iv id u a l inferences about story content made by t h i r d grade child re n exposed to the d i f f e r e n t modes o f presentation was accepted except fo r three s p e c ific ques­ tio n s . Results from a one-way ANOVA on the experimental data yielded no s ig n if ic a n t d iffe re n c e among mean number of correct fa c ­ tu a l re c a ll choices by each of the three modes of story presentation. In view of these re s u lts , the n u ll hypothesis o f no d iffe re n c e in t h i r d grade c h ild re n 's re c a ll of factual material a ft e r being exposed to the three d if f e r e n t modes o f presentation was accepted. 105 Chapter 4 Endnotes. The fo llo w in g questions are referred to in the te x t : #9 - Do you th in k when E lise met the old woman in the fo re s t th a t E lise a. b. c. was a fra id of her? was not a fra id of her? I d o n 't know. #12 - When E lis e was f l y i n g in the net over the sea, do you think she was a. b. c. frightened to be so high? happy to be w ith her brothers? I d o n 't know. #15 - Which do you th in k best describes the bishop? a. b. c. He wanted to know a l l th a t was going on. He was an e v i l , mean person. I d o n 't know. 106 CHAPTER 5 DISCUSSION, CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS Introd uction The procedures discussed in the preceding chapters of th is study have d e alt with whether or not three d if f e r e n t modes of story presen­ t a t io n representing, t h e o r e t i c a l l y , d i f f e r e n t media experiences—a book and reader, a s t o r y t e l l e r , and a sound s lid e show—bring d if f e r e n t story content to the foreground of a tte n tio n . The kinds of story con­ te n t were, s p e c i f i c a l l y , a ffe c tiv e as opposed to n o n a ffe c tive, i n f e r ­ ences and fa c tu a l. A f a i r y t a le . The Wild Swans, was analyzed by a group of three adults to id e n t if y a ffe c tiv e and nonaffective content as o p e ra tio n a lly defined. A researcher designed instrument was developed consisting of three subparts of sixteen questions each. In the f i r s t subsection, a ffe c tiv e content was posited against nonaffective content. In the second subsection, s p e c ific inference choices were given, most of which had emerged from brainstorming sessions w ith students during a pre­ p i l o t phase. tio n s . The t h i r d subsection consisted of factual re c a ll ques­ The instrument was submitted to a regional language arts expert fo r v e r if ic a t io n of construct v a l i d i t y and face v a l i d i t y . P ilo tin g to determine instrument r e l i a b i l i t y , i . e . , s t a b i l i t y , and e sta blish procedures was conducted w ith two classes of t h i r d grade students from one of the elementary schools in Butte School D i s t r i c t 107 Number One using a tre a tm e n t/te s t-tre a tm e n t/re te s t design. A number o f intervening variables were suggested by the re s u lts and an attempt was made to elim in ate t h e i r e ffe c t w ith an a d dition al p i l o t using s ix th grade students and a tr e a tm e n t/te s t-r e te s t design. An acceptable but modest r e l i a b i l i t y c o e ffic ie n t was obtained using th is procedure. Third grade students determined to be comparable from three schools in Butte School D i s t r i c t Number One were randomly assigned to one of three treatment groups--book and reader, s t o r y t e l l e r , and sound s lid e show. Groups consisted of ten students equally divided by sex as much as possible f o r a to ta l of ninety students, t h i r t y per each treatment mode. Presentations were made in a real school s e ttin g under s im ila r c on dition s. The same in d iv id u a l gave a l l presentations and narrated the sound s lid e show which had been developed by the researcher during the prelim inary phase. The instrument was adminis­ tered by the researcher immediately fo llo w in g the presentations. The f in a l data were compiled and analyzed using a one-way ANOVA on subsec­ tio n s one and th re e , a one-way ANOVA on collapsed reponses from sub­ section two, and chi square te s ts of independence on the in d iv id u a l questions in subsection two. Organization Chapter 5 is organized around the fo llo w in g discussions: the development and p i l o t in g of the instrument have been reviewed and the inherent problems and usefulness have been considered. f i n a l data have been discussed. Results of the These are comprised of the cross-media comparisons of content in the foreground of a tte n tio n —a ffe c tiv e as 108 opposed to n o n a ffe c tive, inferences and fa c t u a l. Conclusions derived from the study, includ ing a consideration of the o rig in a l hypotheses and research questions, have been discussed. The conclusions have been followed by a section in which the researcher's subjective observations and in te rp re ta tio n s have been shared fo r the reader's consideration. Recommendations fo r fu r th e r research, based upon the study, have been discussed. lend i t s e l f to concrete educational im p lic a tio n s . The data did not However, throughout the process of t h is study, the researcher observed possible educational im p lic a tio n s which have been offered f o r the reader's consideration in t h is sectio n. The chapter concludes w ith .a summary. Discussion o f the P ilo tin g o f the Instrument A c ru cia l prelim ina ry to the study was the development of the instrument. Most previous research on the kinds of content d iffe r e n ­ t i a l l y brought to c h ild r e n 's a tte n tio n by various modes of presentation has not u t i l i z e d a paper and pencil t e s t nor group presentations and data gathering. Rather, most research has. re lie d on in d iv id u a l pre­ sentations and inte rview techniques. A paper and pencil instrument has a number of advantages over an in te rv ie w technique. Ind ividual presentations, which do not r e fle c t "real world" s e ttin g s , are almost always necessary w ith the interview technique. Besides making group presentations possible, a paper and pencil instrument elim inates the s u b je c t iv it y of in te rp r e tin g interview responses and allows fo r much more data to be gathered in a fa r shorter amount of time. On the other hand, the inte rview technique has the 109 a u th e n tic ity of the sub jects' actual responses which is h ig h ly de s ira b le , es p e c ia lly w ith a ff e c tiv e and inference kinds of content. There were a number of problems th a t became apparent in developing t h i s instrument which bear serious consideration fo r fu tu re research. These problems p rim a rily have to do w ith obtaining a r e l i a b i l i t y coef­ f i c i e n t acceptable fo r a researcher designed instrument. A major problem was creating an adequate number of te s t items f o r each section w ithout making the e n tire t e s t too long f o r the students' a tte n tio n span. Since three d i s t i n c t kinds of s a lie n t content were measured, these categories could not be combined, and w hile incorpor­ ating more questions in to each section would have lengthened the t e s t , possibly increasing r e l i a b i l i t y , the students' a tte n tio n span was a d e f in it e c o n s tra in t. A d d itio n a lly , w hile the story was i n t r i n s i c a l l y content ric h enough to y i e l d enough questions per category fo r an in te rv ie w , i t was d i f f i c u l t to obtain an adequate number of questions s u ita b le f o r a paper and pencil t e s t . This researcher would expect s im ila r problems in developing other instruments of t h is type. A dditional problems regarding r e l i a b i l i t y were related to the students' reading le v e l. The t h ir d grade students needed the in s t r u ­ ment read aloud, w hile they responded s i l e n t l y , pre and post t e s t , to ensure t h e i r understanding of the questions. r e lia b ility . This procedure could mask From examining the t h i r d grade p i l o t data, the story treatment i t s e l f appeared to be a contaminating fa c to r . But, because the story was too d i f f i c u l t fo r the students to read by themselves, e lim in a tin g these variables with t h is age group was impossible. Con­ sequently, a pre and post t e s t design w ith s ix th graders was u t i l i z e d HO to elim in ate some of these contaminating v a ria b le s . These s ix th graders read the story and responded to the instrument s i l e n t l y and were readministered the instrument s i l e n t l y a ft e r a period o f time determined to be a balance between memory and fo r g e t tin g . This s ile n t reading design seemed preferable fo r minimizing the fa c to rs th a t might p o te n tia l Iy mask r e l i a b i l i t y . S im ila r instruments could be valuable in related research, but they should be used with an age group capable o f independent reading and responding to elim in ate these lim it a t io n s . R e l i a b i l i t y c o e ffic ie n ts fo r subsections one and three of the instrument, determined by Pearson c o rre la tio n c o e ffic ie n ts on the s ix th grade pretest to post te s t data, f e l l at the upper level of "moderate c o r r e la t io n ," as suggested by Williams (1979) who quotes G u ilfo rd (1956). Results of reported chi square and Fisher exact te s t s , re­ q u irin g cautious in te r p r e ta tio n and used to determine r e l i a b i l i t y of in d iv id u a l questions in subsection two, yielde d a maximum of ten ques­ tio n s with an associated p r o b a b ility o f p£.05. require higher r e l i a b i l i t y c o e f f ic ie n t s . Most te s ts usually However, due to the explora­ to ry nature of t h is study and G u ilfo rd 's (1965) consideration th a t r e l i a b i l i t y must be viewed in the context of population and te s t con­ d itio n s , i t was determined th a t th is r e l i a b i l i t y , although modest, was s u f f ic i e n t to proceed with the study and a ll re s u lts would have to be in te rp re te d in t h is context. Discussion of A ffe c tiv e Content in the Foreground of A tte n tio n Data were gathered comparing three modes of story presentation— book and reader, s t o r y t e l l e r and sound s lid e show--on a ff e c tiv e Ill content, o p e ra tio n a lly defined and contrasted with n o n a ffe c tive—in the foreground of a tte n tio n . A one-way ANOVA was applied comparing the mean number of a ffe c tiv e content choices calculated per treatment group and no s ig n if ic a n t d iffe re n c e among these means was found. Based on these re s u lts , the null hypothesis of no d iffe re n c e in t h i r d grade c h ild r e n 's perception of a ffe c tiv e content brought to the foreground of t h e i r a tte n tio n by the three modes o f presentation was accepted. Also based on these re s u lts , there were no data to substantiate the research question th a t a ff e c tiv e as opposed to nonaffective content might be more e f f e c t iv e ly brought to the foreground of a tte n tio n by any of the three modes of presentation, The lack o f s ig n if ic a n t fin d in g must be in te rp re te d in l i g h t of the experimental c o n dition s: the in te ra c tio n of the s t o r y t e l l e r / re ade r/na rra tor w ith the subjects, the inherent v a r i a b i l i t y in the s t o r y t e l l i n g and reading, the ad m inistra tion of the instrument and the instrument i t s e l f may have a l l contributed to "no d iffe re n c e " among treatment groups. Perhaps more im p o rta n tly , there may not have been a dramatic enough d iffe re n c e between the modes of presentation to create real diffe re n c e s in the communication p o te n tia l of the media. The s to ry ­ t e l l i n g experience, because of the necessity of presenting a memorized t e x t and consistent d e liv e ry , was f a r more s t i l t e d than a spontaneous s t o r y t e l l i n g experience. And the sound s lid e show, which lacked the a tt r ib u t e s of movement and animation c h a ra c te ris tic of te le v is e d pre­ sen ta tio n s, such as Meringoff (1978) used, may simply heive been too close to the book and reader experience to produce a d iffe re n c e . 112 Modes possessing more c le a r ly delineated and v a ria n t a tt r ib u t e s ( e . g . , te le v is io n versus book and reader) might produce more dramatic re s u lts . Furthermore, paper and pencil instrumentation may not be sen s itiv e enough to capture d iffe ren ces in the in d iv id u a l subjects' perceptions of a ffe c tiv e content, and the story analysis i t s e l f , upon which the instrument was based, may not have been s e n s itiv e enough to detect a ffe c tiv e nuances. I f th a t were the case, an in te rv ie w technique might r e c t i f y most of these inadequacies. Discussion of Inference Questions Data from the second subsection of the instrument were used to t e s t two n u ll hypotheses: I ) th a t there is no d iffe re n c e in the number of inferences about story content made by t h i r d grade child re n exposed to the three d if f e r e n t modes of story presentation and 2) th a t there is no d iffe re n c e in in d iv id u a l inferences about story content made by t h i r d grade children exposed to the three modes of story presentation. Results from these data were also used to assess two research ques­ tio n s : I ) whether or not there were in d ic a tio n s th a t c h ild re n make more or fewer inferences about story content based on the mode of pre­ sentation and 2) whether or not there were in d ic a tio n s child re n make d if f e r e n t inferences based on the mode o f presentation. To t e s t the f i r s t of these nu ll hypotheses, the f i r s t two i n f e r ­ ence choices fo r each question were combined as an "inference choice" and compared w ith response choice three, i . e . , " I d o n 't know," which was considered as a "no inference" choice. A mean per treatment group was calculated and a one-way ANOVA applied comparing mean d iffe re n c e s . 113 Results of the ANOVA yielde d no s t a t i s t i c a l l y s ig n if ic a n t d iffe re n c e between means. \ In view o f these re s u lts , the null hypothesis was accepted and post hoc comparison te s ts were not warranted. Chi square te s ts of independence were applied to each ind ivid ual qu estion, inference choice to no inference choice, and yielded one out o f sixteen questions in which there were s ig n if ic a n t ly more no i n f e r ­ ence responses, i . e . , 111 d o n 't know" responses, among the sound s lid e respondents. In view o f these re s u lts and re s u lts of the ANOVA, the research question of whether or not there are in d ic a tio n s th a t children make more or fewer inferences based on mode of presentation, seemed unsubstantiated except in one out of the sixteen s p e c ific cases. To te s t the nu ll hypothesis th a t th e r e , is no d iffe re n c e in i n d i ­ vidual inferences about s to ry content made by t h ir d grade children exposed to the three d if f e r e n t modes of story presentation, a chi square t e s t of independence was applied to each inference question comparing the three response choices by mode. This analysis yielded re s u lts in d ic a tin g three of the questions demonstrated s ig n if ic a n t diffe ren ces in response pattern by mode. These questions had been determined to be acceptably r e lia b le by at least one of the analyses from the p i l o t i n g . In the case of one of the three questions, however, in te rp r e ta tio n of the r e l i a b i l i t y , was considerably more questionable because the s p e c ific data sets used were not used fo r the other two questions because of small c e ll frequency. In view o f these re s u lts , the n u ll hypothesis of no d iffe re n c e in in d iv id u a l inferences about story content made by t h i r d grade children exposed to the d if f e r e n t presentation modes was accepted except fo r 114 three s p e c ific inference questions. Based on these re s u lts , the re ­ search question of whether or not there are in d ic a tio n s th a t children made d if f e r e n t inferences about s to ry content according to mode of presentation could not be answered d e f i n i t e l y and had to be considered in the context of the re s u lts from a l l the inference questions. These re s u lts do not seem to be completely consistent with M e rin g o ffs (1978) fin d in g s in which the inferences across the media were the same but the lin e of reason f o r a r r iv in g at the inferences was d i f f e r e n t . However, in general, these re s u lts are consistent with the analysis of the re s u lts of Part One of the instrument from th is study in which there was no d iffe re n c e in a ffe c tiv e as opposed to nona ff e c tiv e content brought to the foreground of a tte n tio n by each of the three modes of presentation. Besides the p o s s i b i l i t y th a t the factors o f experimental conditions and instrumentation may have affected these fin d in g s , there also may not have been a dramatic enough d iffe re n c e between the modes of presentation to create more d iffe re n c e in the communication p o te n tia l of the media. Discussion o f Factual Content in the Foreground o f A tten tion Data were gathered comparing three modes of story p resen tatio n-book and reader, s t o r y t e l l e r and sound s lid e show--on factual content in the foreground of a tte n tio n . A one-way ANOVA was applied comparing the mean number of correct responses per treatment group and no s i g n i f ­ ican t d iffe re n c e among these means was found. Thus, the n u ll hypothe­ s is of no d iffe re n c e in t h i r d grade c h ild r e n 's re c a ll o f factual mater­ ia l based on the three modes of presentation was accepted. Also, 115 based on these re s u lts , there were no data to substantiate the research question th a t factual content might be d i f f e r e n t i a l Iy brought to the foreground of a tte n tio n by any of the three modes of presentation. The lack o f s ig n if ic a n t fin d in g s is consistent w ith the lack of s ig n if ic a n t fin d in g s from the other two subsections. There simply was no d iffe re n c e , at le a s t on these questions, in factual re c a ll discrim ­ in a tio n by mode. I t is possible th a t using an in te rv ie w technique fo r data gathering might produce more diffe ren ces in factual r e c a l l , and th a t diffe ren ces might be augmented i f the modes of presentation were representative of more v a ria b le a t t r ib u t e s , and the questions more c lo s e ly correlated w ith those a t t r ib u t e s . Conclusions The fo llo w in g conclusions were reached as a re s u lt of t h is e x p e ri­ mental study. I. There was no d iffe re n c e in a ff e c tiv e content as opposed to n o n a ffe c tive content brought d i f f e r e n t i a l l y to the foreground o f a tte n tio n by the three modes of story presentation as determined in th is experimental s e ttin g . Based on th is con­ c lu s io n , the n u ll hypothesis of no d iffe ren ce in t h i r d grade c h ild r e n 's perception of a ff e c tiv e content brought to the foreground o f t h e i r a tte n tio n by the three presentation modes was accepted. There was no evidence to substantiate the research question th a t a ff e c tiv e as opposed to nonaffective content might be more e f f e c t i v e l y brought to the foreground o f a tte n tio n by any o f the i d e n t if ie d modes of presentation. 116 These conclusions must be in te rp re te d in the context of the modest r e l i a b i l i t y c o e ffic ie n t obtained fo r th is subsection o f the instrument. 2. There was no d iffe re n c e , o v e r a ll, between inference choice as opposed to no inference choice made by students presented the three modes of story treatm ent-book and reader, s to ry ­ t e l l e r and sound s lid e show—as determined in t h is experimen­ t a l s e ttin g . Based on t h is conclusion, the n u ll hypothesis of no d iffe re n c e in the number of inferences about story content made by t h i r d grade child re n exposed to the three d if f e r e n t presentation modes was accepted. O v e ra ll, the research question of whether or not there are in d ic a tio n s th a t children make more or fewer inferences based on mode of presentation seemed unsubstantiated except in one out of the sixteen s p e c ific cases. These conclusions must also be viewed in the context of the modest r e l i a b i l i t y indices obtained in t h is subsection of the instrument. 3. There was no d iffe re n c e in in d iv id u a l inferences about story content made by t h i r d grade child re n exposed to the three d i f f e r e n t modes of presentation f o r approximately eighty percent of the in d iv id u a l inference questions. Based on th is conclusion, the nu ll hypothesis of no d iffe re n c e in i n d iv id ­ ual inferences about story content made by t h i r d grade c h i l ­ dren exposed to the d if f e r e n t presentation modes was accepted except f o r three s p e c ific questions. The research question addressing whether or not there are in d ic a tio n s th a t children 117 make d if f e r e n t inferences about story content according to the presentation mode could not be answered d e f i n i t e l y . These conclusions must be considered in the context of the modest r e l i a b i l i t y indices obtained on these questions. 4. There was no d iffe re n c e in t h i r d grade c h ild r e n 's correct factual re c a ll based on the three modes of presentation— book and reader, s t o r y t e l l e r and sound s lid e show—as de ter­ mined in th is experimental s e tt in g . Based on t h is conclu­ s ion , the n u ll hypothesis of no d iffe re n c e in t h i r d grade c h ild r e n 's re c a ll of factual m aterial based on the three modes of presentation was accepted. There was no evidence to substantiate the research question th a t fa c tu a l content might be d i f f e r e n t i a l Iy brought to the foreground of atten­ tio n by any of the three presentation modes. While these conclusions are warranted by the re s u lts of the study, they must be viewed in the context o f the modest r e l i a b i l i t y c o e f f ic ie n t obtained f o r th is subsection of the instrument. Observations and In te rp re ta tio n s The fo llo w in g s ub jective observations and in te rp r e ta tio n s have been based on the researcher's experiences during the e n tir e process involved in the con ceptualization, execution and completion of th is study. Because of the exploratory nature of t h is research, these sub­ je c t i v e observations have been offered to the reader f o r consideration. They are not, however, conclusions based on re s u lts of the experimental s tu d y . % 118 1. A fe a s ib le technique f o r story analysis is the determination of o p e ra tio n a lly defined content by panel consensus. How­ ever, t h is technique may not y ie ld a s u f f ic i e n t qu a n tity of content items to render i t u s e fu l. 2. I t is possible to design a paper and pencil instrument fo r determining sub jects' perceptions of s a lie n t c o n te n t; how­ ever, i t is very d i f f i c u l t to obtain a s u f f i c i e n t l y high r e l i a b i l i t y c o e f f i c i e n t , e s p e c ia lly when the instrument is used w ith elementary school aged child re n and when i t is determining more than one type of s a lie n t content. Further in v e s tig a tio n of the development of th is kind of instrument with d if f e r e n t age groups may be warranted since a large q u a n tity o f data can be c ollected in a short time with group presentations and since such an instrument could y ie ld v a l­ uable inform ation about the e ffe c ts of d if f e r e n t media. 3. Although demonstrated by the data fo r only three s p e c ific questions, there are in d ic a tio n s th a t some inference content may be d i f f e r e n t i a l l y brought to the foreground of a tte n tio n by the d if f e r e n t modes of presentation. The researcher's in te rp r e ta tio n of the students' response patterns on these three questions would seem to in d ic a te th a t the inference choices were influenced by a tt r ib u t e s of the p ic tu re stimu­ lu s , e. g. , the presence or absence of c e rta in p ic tu re s , and the use of close-up shots of characters' fa c ia l expressions. 4. Although not substantiated by the study, the modes of book and reader, s t o r y t e l l e r and sound s lid e show may possess 119 a t t r ib u t e s th a t are not c h a r a c te r is tic a lly distin ctive .e n o u g h to d i f f e r e n t i a l l y influence perception of content. Recommendations f o r Further Research The fo llo w in g recommendations fo r fu r th e r research were derived ) from the conduct of t h is study. 1. In lie u of a r e p lic a tio n of t h i s study, i t is recommended th a t a book and reader, s t o r y t e l l e r and f i l m s t r i p or sound s lid e show comparison be made on an in d iv id u a l basis using an in te rv ie w technique, s im ila r to th a t used by Meringoff (1978). This would provide an a lte rn a tiv e method to deter­ mining whether or not these media possess a tt r ib u t e s charac­ t e r i s t i c a l l y d i f f e r e n t enough to create a d iffe re n c e in inform ation p o te n tia l. 2. I t would seem productive f o r fu tu re research to more c le a rly delineate s p e c ific a ttr ib u te s of various media and to t r y to re la te id e n t i f i e d , s p e c ific a tt r ib u t e s to a ffe c tiv e and in fe r e n tia l in te rp r e ta tio n s of content. 3. Any s im ila r research should possibly minimize the environ­ mental fa c to rs which might contaminate the r e s u lts . For example, i t is recommended th a t the same lo c a tio n be used fo r a ll the presentations. A d d itio n a lly , i t might be de sirab le i f the presenter were f a m ilia r to the students. I f students from only one school were used, cross grade level comparisons could be made. However, accommodations would have to be made fo r the students' d i f f e r i n g a tte n tio n 120 spans and reading le ve ls i f those were fa c to rs in the research design. 4. The development of paper and pencil instruments f o r deter­ mining inform ation p o te n tia l in cross-media studies warrants f u r th e r in v e s tig a tio n . Since more data can be c o llected in a shorter amount of time and since group s e ttin g s are more r e f le c t iv e of real school s itu a tio n s , the refinement of a technique f o r such instrument development would be valuable. Such instrument development should be appropriate to the independent reading level of the subjects and t h e i r a tte n tio n span. I t might be necessary to l i m i t the instrument to c o l­ le c tin g only one kind of data ( f o r example, only one kind of s a lie n t c on ten t). Knowing the f e a s i b i l i t y of re g u la rly using t h i s kind of instrument would give d ire c tio n to fu r th e r research. 5. A lte rn a tiv e approaches both to paper and pencil instrument and in te rv ie w technique seem warranted at t h is p o in t. Some of these might include nonverbal conceptualization of the story such as arranging, s o rtin g or ranking pictu re s or observation techniques using video equipment. Further re­ search could draw from other d is c ip lin e s , such as psychology, which may have nonverbal measures of a ffe c tiv e responses which could be correlated w ith d if f e r e n t presentations of a s to ry . 6. In ad dition to perceptions of a ffe c tiv e content and i n f e r ­ ences' about content, other areas, such as problem solving . 121 could be in v e s tig a te d . An in te re s tin g approach might be to in v e s tig a te whether or not there are differences in problem solving techniques on a problem s p e c if ic a lly related to a story a ft e r having i t presented by d if f e r e n t modes. 7. An in v e s tig a tio n of preferences of mode might be undertaken, r e la tin g these preferences to the in d iv id u a liz e d varia ble of learning s ty le . 8. Future research should focus on comparing modes of presen­ ta t io n th a t possess, at lea st apparently, d ram a tically d i f f e r e n t a tt r ib u t e s . However, the medium of the f i l m s t r i p , e s p e c ia lly used fo r presentations of c h ild r e n 's storybooks, warrants more in v e s tig a tio n . Since there are so many excel­ le n t commercially produced f i l m s t r i p s , they could perhaps be used in a series of comparisons on some i d e n t i f i a b l e t r a i t s . 9. The long term and overall e ffe c t of d if f e r e n t media presen­ t a t io n s , such as the work done by Smardo and Carry (1982), warrants fu r th e r in v e s tig a tio n . In th is kind of study, a type of media presentation is given re g u la rly over a long period o f tim e. Pre and post te s tin g is used to determine whether or not there are changes on some generalized a b i l i t y th a t are related to these d i f f e r e n t i a l exposures. Educational Im plications The actual data derived from t h is study did not lend i t s e l f to concrete educational recommendations. a- However, throughout the process o f conducting t h is study, the researcher has perceived a number of 122 possible educational im p lic a tio n s which are suggestions fo r the reader's consideration. 1. I t appears th a t l i v e presentations are enjoyed by students who seem to appreciate the personal contact with the reader or s t o r y t e l l e r . Memorized s t o r y t e l l i n g presentations do not seem as e ff e c tiv e in capturing and holding a tte n tio n as other modes. Spontaneous s t o r y t e l l i n g might be preferred fo r some purposes to memorized s t o r y t e l l i n g . Although not demonstrated by the study, teachers, lib r a r ia n s and parents who value reading aloud and using l i v e presentations might be reassured th a t there seems to be value in these experi­ ences even though at times such value might be in ta n g ib le and not subject to q u a n tita tiv e analysis. 2. Although t h is suggestion goes beyond the data, teachers and li b r a r i a n s , when using media such as f i lm s t r ip s or te le vised productions to present storybooks, might give consideration to the e ffe c t on the students th a t they wish the presentation to have. Although not determined in th is study, there may be subtle diffe ren ces in inform ation po ten tial th a t are tra n s m itte d . Other research has indicated t h is p o s s i b i l i t y . I t might be worthwhile to u t i l i z e a lte rn a tiv e media forms when a v a ila b le . 3. I t appears to t h is researcher th a t f a i r y ta le s and fo lk lore s t i l l seem to be popularly received by c h ild re n , and teachers and lib r a r ia n s might be encouraged not to omit these and other t r a d it io n a l l i t e r a r y forms from the c h ild r e n 's exposure. 123 4. From t h is researcher's observation, a relaxed, comfortable atmosphere is important to a successful s t o r y t e l l i n g or book and reader experience. I t also is important f o r the s to ry ­ t e l l e r or reader to be personally f a m ilia r with the students. 5. Although not substantiated by the data, the presence or absence of i l l u s t r a t i o n s and the kind of expression the i l l u s t r a t e d characters display may make a d iffe re n c e in some inferences about story content. In view o f t h i s p o s s i b i l i t y , teachers and lib r a r ia n s might be wise to c a r e fu lly select i l l u s t r a t e d versions of s to rie s when a lte rn a tiv e s e x is t. 6. Although t h is suggestion goes beyond th is study, w ith so many forms of media accessible to students today, i t would seem prudent to expose students to as many of these forms as possible, giv in g them a wide range of experience in i n t e r ­ p reting inform ation transm itted through d if f e r e n t forms. Summary Chapter 5 of t h is study has d e alt w ith the fo llo w in g : a discus­ sion of the p i l o t and the instrum entation, a discussion of the results of the study regarding a ff e c tiv e and nonaffective content, inferences and factual r e c a ll, conclusions (in c lu d in g a consideration of the o r ig ­ ina l hypotheses), sub jective observations and in te r p r e ta tio n s , observed educational im p lic a tio n s , and recommendations fo r fu r th e r research. Discussion of the p i l o t focused on problems inherent in the devel­ opment of a researcher designed instrument. Since a s ig n if ic a n t pre­ lim in a ry to t h is study was the development of the instrument and since 124 use of a paper and pencil instrument is very uncommon in t h is research, t h i s discussion could bear consideration f o r fu tu re research of a s im ila r nature. Discussion of the experimental fin d in g s explicated the re sults obtained when students were exposed to the three d if f e r e n t modes of presenting the same s to ry —book and reader, s t o r y t e l l e r and sound s lid e show. The instrument compared t h e i r responses on a ffe c tiv e as opposed to n o n a ffe c tive content, inferences and factual r e c a ll. Conclusions were derived from the o r ig in a l hypotheses upon which t h i s study was based. A d d itio n a lly , s ub jective observations and in te rp r e ta tio n s made by the researcher during the e n tire experimental process were discussed. Because of the exploratory nature of t h is research and paucity of precedent s tu d ie s , i t may be th a t the study's primary value to educational research has been the d e lin e a tio n of the experimental procedures used throughout t h is process. There were no concrete educational im p lic a tio n s derived from the data. However, throughout the study, the researcher perceived a number o f possible educational im p lic a tio n s , which (as suggestions f o r the reader's consideration) have been discussed in t h is s e c tio n . Perhaps research in new areas leaves more questions than i t does answers. The study described in these chapters is somewhat consonant w ith th is observation. Throughout the experimental process, however, the study has f r u i t f u l l y provided new questions related to cross-media considerations, possible new approaches to the questions th a t motivated th is study, and hopefully background inform ation fo r in d iv id u a ls i n t e r ­ ested in the theory and research re la tin g to media stud ies. REFERENCES CITED 126 A lle n , Don, "And Everyone Quoted McLuhan a Lot: an Autobiographical Retrospective on the Media Studies Movement." Media and Methods 17, 1980: 22-25, 28, 56, 58, 59. American Heritage D ictio n a ry of the English Language. American H eri­ tage Publishing Co., In c ., New York: 1982, s .v . " a f f e c t iv e , " "brainsto rm in g." Andersen, Hans C h ris tia n . The Wild Swans. Adapted by Amy E h rlic h . U lu s , by Susan J e ffe r s . New York: Dial Press, 1981. Applebee, Arthur N. The C h ild 's Concept o f Story, Ages TwO to Seven­ tee n. Chicago: U n iv e rs ity of Chicago Press, 1978. Arbuthnot, May H i l l and Sutherland, Zena. Children and Books. Glenvil i e , IL: S c o tt, Foresman and Co., 1972. 4th ed. Barr, A v ril S ., Dairs, Robert A. and Johnson, Palmer 0. Educational Research and A pp ra is a l. P hiladelphia: J. B. L ip p in c o tt Co., 1953. Beckson, Karl and Ganz, A rthur. L ite ra r y Terms: A D ic tio n a ry . York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux, 1972, s .v . "genre." ~~* New B e t t i eheim, Bruno. The Uses of Enchantment: The Meaning and Impor­ tance of Fairy Tales. New York: A lfre d A. Knopf, 1976. Blank, Marion and Frank, Sheldon M. "Story Recall in Kindergarten C hildren: E ffect of Method of Presentation on P sycholin guistic Performance." Child Development 42 (1971) 299-312. Brown, Ann L. and Smiley, Sandra S. "Rating the Importance of Struc­ tu ra l Units of Prose Passages: A Problem of Metacognitive Devel­ opment." Child Development 48 (1977) 1-8. Bruner, Jerome S. "The Course of Cognitive Growth." o g is t 14 (1964) 1-15. American Psychol- C o llin s , Andrew W. "Learning of Media Content: A Developmental Study." Child Development 4 (1970): 11 33-1142. C ro ft, Roger G. et a l . "Comparison o f A ttitu d e Changes E lic it e d by Live and Videotape Classroom P resentation." Audio Visual Communi­ cation Review 17, No. 3 (1969): 315-321 . Davies, Ruth Ann. The School L ib ra ry Media Center: A Force f o r Educa­ tio n a l Excellence. New York: R. R. Bowker C o., 1974. D a r r e ll, Donald. "L is te n in g Comprehension Versus Reading Comprehen­ s io n ." Journal of Reading 12 (1969) 455-560. 127 Encyclopedia of Psychology. H. J. Eysenck, W. Arnold, and R. M e i l i , eds. , Seabury Press, New York: 1979, s„v . “ operational d e f i n i ­ tio n ." Ferguson, George A. S t a t is t ic a l Analysis in Psychology and Education. 5th ed. New York: NcGraw-Hi11, 1981. F ra n k lin , Sandra I . and Mabry, N. Kemp. "A ffe c tiv e Response with Multi-Media Teaching o f Poetry." Reading Improvement 17 (1980) 311-312. Gardner, Howard. "Reprogramming the Media Researchers." Today 13, No. 8 (1980) 6, 12, 14. Psychology Grimm, Brothers. Household S to rie s . Dover P u blica tions, 1963. New York: Trans. Lucy Crane. G u ilfo rd , J . P. Fundamental S t a t is t ic s in Psychology and Education. 4th ed. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1965. G u ilfo rd , J . P. and Fruchter, Benjamin. Psychology and Education. 5th ed. Fundamental S t a t is t ic s in New York: McGraw-Hill, 1973. Hayes, Donald S. and Birnbaum, Dana W. "Preschooler's Retention of Televised Events: Is a Picture Worth a Thousand Words." Develop­ mental Psychology 16, No. 5 (1980) 410-416. Hayes, Donald S., Chemels k i, Bruce E. and B irnbaum, Dana W. "Young C hildren's Retention of Televised Events." Developmental Psychol­ ogy 17, No. 2 (1981 ) 230-232. Huston-Stein, Aletha and John C. Wright. "Children and T e levision: Effects of the Medium, Its Content and Its Form." Journal of Research and Development 13, No. I (1979): 20-31. Johnson, Ronald E. "Recall of Prose as a Function of the S tructural Importance of the L in g u is tic U n its ." Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 9 (1970) 12-20. L'Engle, Madeleine. "What is Real?" Language Arts 55 (1978): 447-451. Mandler, Jean M. and Johnson, Nancy S. , "Remembrance of Things Passed: Story S tructure and R e c a ll." Cognitive Psychology 9 (1977) 111-151. McLuhan, Marshall. Understanding Media: York: McGraw-Hill, 1964. The Extensions o f Man. New 128 M e rin g o ff, Laurene Krasny. "A Story A Story: The Influence of the Medium on C hildren's Apprehension of S to rie s ." Harvard Univer­ s i t y , Ed.D., 1978. Ann Arbor, MI: U niversity M icro film s, c . 1978. M e rin g o ff, Laurence Krasny. "In fluen ce of the Medium on C hildren's Story Apprehension." Journal o f Educational Psychology 72, No. 2 (1980): 240-249. Moldstad, John A. "S elective Review o f Research Studies Showing Media Effectiveness: A Primer f o r Media D ire c to rs ." Audio Visual Communication Review 22, No. 4 (1974): 387-407. Morrow, James. "Werewolves and P r in te r 's D evils: A H istory of Media in the Schools." Curriculum Review 18, No. 4 (1979): 266-269. Nie, Norman H., H u ll, C. H ad la i, Jenkins, Jean G., S te inbrenner, Karin, and Bent, Dale H. SPSS, S t a t is t ic a l Package fo r Social Science. 2nd E d itio n . New York: McGraw-Hill, 1980. Olson, David R., ed. . National Society fo r the Study o f Education. Media and Symbols: The Forms of Expression, Communcation and Education. Seventy-third Yearbook, Par I . Chicago: U niversity of Chicago Press, 1974. Olson, David R. and Bruner, Jerome S. "Learning Through Experience and Learning Through Media." National Society fo r the Study of Education. Media and Symbols: The Forms o f Expression, Communi­ cation and Education. Seventy-third Yearbook, Part I . Chicago: U n iv e rs ity o f Chicago Press, 1974. P aris, Scott G. and Mahoney, Gerald J. "Cognitive In te g ra tio n in C hildren's Memory fo r Sentences and P ic tu re s ." Child Development 45 (1974) 633-642. Rohwer, William D., J r . and H a rris , Wendy J . "Media E ffe c ts on Prose Learning in Two Populations of C h ild re n ." Journal o f Educational Psychology 67, No. 5 (1975) 651-657. Ross, Ramon. S to r y t e lTer. Columbus, Ohio: Charles E. M e r r i l l , 1980. Salomon, G abriel. "Can We A ffe c t Cognitive S k ills Through Visual Media? An Hypothesis and I n i t i a l Findings." AVCR 20, No. 4 (1 972) 401-422. Salomon, G abriel. "Cognitive S k i l l Learning Across C u ltu re s ." of Communication 26, No. 2 (1976) 138-144. Journal Salomon, G abriel. " In te r n a liz a tio n of F ilm ic Schematic Operations in In te ra c tio n with Learner's A p titu d e s ." Journal of Educational Psychology 66, No. 4 (1974) 499-511 . 129 Webster's Third New In te rn a tio n a l D ictiona ry o f the English Language, Unabridged. G. and C. Merriam Co., S p rin g fie ld , MA: 1971, s .v . " c o g n itio n ," " c o g n itiv e ." W illiam s, F rede rick. Reasoning w ith S t a t i s t i c s . H o lt, Rinehart and Winston, 1979. 2nd ed. New York: Yearsley, Macleod. The F o lklore o f the Fairy Tale. London: Watts and Co., 1924. Reissued by Singing Tree Press, D e tr o it, MI: 1968. Yolen, Jane. "How Basic is SHAZAM." Language Arts 54 (1977): 645-650. Yolen, Jane. Tales." "Shape S h ifte rs : Every C h ild 's Adventures in Fairy Language Arts 55 (1978): 699-703. 130 APPENDICES APPENDIX A PILOT MATERIAL 132 BRAINSTORMED CONTENT 133 a messenger from the c astle E lise had a fo o ts to o l made of gl ass ...( c lo u d s ) th a t looked l i k e fo re s ts , mountains and shining pal aces golden s h ir t s w ith stars and swords she was glad—excited when she got to the seashore they thought she was a witch because she went to the graveyards at night the f a i r y was the old lady the king thought she was a witch the old lady was good (old woman in the woods was) a witch ...s h ip s th a t looked l i k e sea g u lls sparkling in the water witches i t was tr y in g to t e l l about l i f e mi n is te r about her brothers bony fing ers i t was tr y in g to t e l l about I i f e - the e x c itin g things i t was tr y in g to t e l l about the ups and down of l i f e eleven princes and one princess eleven swans and a princess' l i f e somebody t o ld her the y'd been turned in to swans they understood what she was doing was fo r them i t was tr y in g to t e l l about love ( i t was tr y in g to t e l l you) some­ times you run in to bad things in life she cried and laughed they wept she loved her brothers she was sad when she was growing up when you run in to bad things in l i f e you have to work your way out princess seen a witch in the fo r e s t when she was looking fo r her brothers the king wanted to marry her at the beginning they are happy from the time they wake up u n til they go to bed at night princes are happy at the beginning they made a net so she could go w ith them f a i r y to ld her to gather a l l the n e ttle s she could get he d i d n 't l i k e her (m in is te r) he was going to burn her she was dreaming about going up to castles 134 she saw the swans land princess was sent away to a farm wicked queen turns them in to eleven w ild swans princes were turned, in to swans she found eleven swans' feathers on the beach she found the n e ttle s in the grave­ yard ■ she f e l l in to t h e i r arms (she saw) feathers they f e l l in to each oth ers' arms (witches) clawed the ground she la id down and the swans grasped the net and flew over the sea lady t o ld her (brothers turned in to swans) she saw clouds they made a net they made a net so she could go w ith them she saw ships f a i r y t o ld her to take as many n e ttle s as she can to make some sweaters she dreamed of a f a i r y she saw a f a ir y they found E lise s ile n t he (m in is te r) went back and to ld the king (when she went to get the n e ttle s ) he followed her the king came--he.picked her up and took her away a f t e r a w hile the brothers came fa th e r married a wicked woman princess k n it some sweaters (m in is te r) nice i f the sun went down and they were s t i l l up in the sky the y'd come crashing down she needed the weeds to turn her brothers back in to boys AFFECTIVE CONTENT 136 1 - these chi I den were happy 2 - queen who hated the child re n 3 - tears streamed down her face 4 - once more they were child re n playing together carefree and loved 5 - E lise threw hereself down and began to weep 6 - she looked up, to her jo y 7 - they laughed and cried 8 - the brothers came back and were alarmed 9 - the king saw her smile fo r the f i r s t time 10 - joyously clasped her to him1 11 - king, who loved her and wanted so much to please her 12 - she s to le away f e a r f u ll y 13 - to her horror 14 - E lis e saw the k in g 's sorrow 15 - i t troubled her g r e a tly , adding to the pain 16 - she was so happy 17 - the crowd moved back f e a r f u l l y 18 - she awoke and smiled at him 19 - she had loved him 137 CONTENT FROM GUIDED BRAINSTORMING 138 What was the story about? eleven princes and one princess princes were turned into, swans princess seen a witch in the fo re s t when she was looking f o r her brothers princess sent away to a farm princess k n it some sweaters she found the n e ttle s in the graveyard f a i r y to ld her to gather a ll the n e ttle s she could get fa th e r married a wicked woman What else was in the graveyard? witches clawed the ground (what did they do?) (what kind of fin g e rs did they have?) bony When she went- looking f o r her brother? (how did she fin d out her brothers had been turned in to swans?) lady to ld her saw feathers she was glad - excited when she got to the seashore a ft e r a while the brothers came sun came down and swans came and they turned back in to boys (what would happen i f the sun went down and they were s t i l l up there?) . they would come crashing down (how did she decide to look fo r her brothers?) because somebody to ld her the y'd been turned in to swans What was the old woman in the woods lik e ? a witch - nice - (because she to ld her about the swans) What did they do? made a net (why did they want to make a net?) so she could go with them w hile she was in the net what did she do? she was dreaming about going up - to castles 139 I Then they got to the land and she dreamed—what did she dream about? about her brothers she saw a f a i r y f a i r y to ld her to take as many n e ttle s as she can to make some sweaters What did the n e ttle s do when she touched them? made her fin g e rs b l i s t e r Next day as she was k n it t in g the s h ir t s what happened? they turned to sweaters the king came - he picked her up and took her away Then they got married— did they l i v e happily ever afte r? no Who was there a t the palace—who married them—what kind o f person was he? m in is te r - p r ie s t - nice - churchman Was he nice to E l i se? he d i d n 't l ik e her - he to ld the king she was a witch (when she went to get the n e ttle s , what did he do?) he followed her and ( then he saw the witches and what did he do?) he went back and to ld the king 140 INSTRUMENT DIRECTLY FROM TEXT 141 INTRODUCTION At the beginning E lise and her brothers are described. most important as the story was t o ld to you? I) 2) Which seemed these child re n were happy (from the time they woke in the morning u n t il they went to bed at nig ht) [ A ffe c t iv e ] these children liv e d in a warm and pleasant land (the princes wore stars on t h e i r s h irts and t h e i r s is t e r had a fo o ts to o l made of glass) [D e s c rip tiv e ] CONFLICT AND TRAGIC EVENTS The c h ild r e n 's l i f e is changed when the king gets married . seemed most important as the story was to ld to you? WhiCh I D the queen sent E lis e away (to be looked a ft e r by farmers) [A c tio n ] 2) the queen hated the children (and wanted only to be r id of them) [A f f e c t iv e ] BEGINNING OF THE QUEST The k in g 's messenger t e l l s E lis e th a t her brothers were turned in to swans. She resolves to fin d them. Which seemed more important as the was t o ld to you? D 2) (At n i g h t f a l l ) she came in to a great fo re s t where the a i r was s t i l l and mild (and every­ where glowworms shown w ith a green f i r e ) [D e s c rip tiv e ] (A lI n ig h t) E lise dreamed of her brothers and (once more) they were (c h ild re n ) playing together carefree and loved. [ A ffe c ti ve] 142 REUNION E lis e and her brothers are reunited on the seashore, important as the story was to ld to you? I) 2) Which seemed most They laughed and crie d and held each other (in the growing darkness) [ A ffe c t iv e ] They spent the n ig h t making a net ( from w illow bark and rushes) [A c tio n ] IMPLEMENTATION OF THE SOLUTION The f a i r y (comes to E lise in her dream) shows E lis e the n e ttle s and t e l l s her how she can free her brothers. Which seemed more important? I) (Then) E lise awoke and began her task. [A c tio n ] 2) The brothers were alarmed (by her silence) but they understood her work was fo r them ( f o r t h e ir sake) [A f f e c t iv e ] ARRIVAL OF THE KING The king discovers E lis e and takes her to his magnificent c i t y where he marries her. Which seemed most important? 1) 2) (A fte r E lis e became queen) the king led her to a chamber where her bundle o f s h ir t s and n e ttle s were. [A c tio n ] E lise smiled fo r the f i r s t time (and the king joyously held her to him) [A ffe c t iv e ] DESCRIPTION OF THEIR RELATIONSHIP*1 2 Which seemed most important? 1) 2) The king (loved her) and wanted (so much) to please her. [A ffe c t iv e ] (Each nig ht) when the king was sleeping E lise k n itte d the s h ir t s . [A c tio n ] 143 FINAL CONFLICT INTRODUCED E lis e runs out of n e ttle s and has to gather more from the church yard (a t n i g h t ) . Which seemed most important? 1) 2) * The bishop (follow ed E lis e and) t o ld the king about her (n ighttim e) v i s i t and (about) the witches. [A c tio n ] E lise saw the k in g 's sorrow; and i t troubled her (g re a tly ) and added to (her) pain (she already f e l t ) . [ A ffe c t iv e ] BEGINNING OF THE CLIMAX SCENE While E lis e was in the dungeon one of the swans finds her. seemed most important? 1) 2) Which She was happy because ( though she might soon be dead) her task was almost done and her brothers were nearby. [ A ffe c t iv e ] The brothers t r i e d to (see the king and) save E lis e but were turned in to swans as the sun came up. [A c tio n ] FINAL SCENE The swans are turned in to E l is e 1s brothers and she is freed. seemed most important? Which I) She (awoke and) smiled a t the king fo r she had loved him from the beginning. [ A ffe c t iv e ] 2) They returned to the palace (g re a t flocks of birds appeared and) churchbelIs rang out throughout the land. [A c tio n ] 144 INSTRUMENT: INFERENCES ABOUT AFFECTIVE CONTENT 145 INTRODUCTION CONFLICT AND TRAGIC EVENTS (From the! way the story was to ld to you) do you thin k th a t D E lis e 's fa th e r was sad about what happened ito her and her brothers. 2) E lis e 's fa th e r d id n 't care what happened to her and her brothers. [ A ffe c t iv e opposites] THE QUEST (From the way the story was to ld to you) do you thin k th a t the old woman in the fo re s t D d id n 't want to be bothered by E lis e . [A ffe c t iv e - 2) was f r ie n d ly to E lis e . opposites] (From the way the story was t o ld to you) when E lise FIRST reached the sea she was D exhausted and sad. [ A ffe c t iv e ] 2) excited and happy. opposites] (From the way the story was t o ld to you) when E lise was f l y i n g in the net over the seashore she was D 2) ' peaceful and calm. frightened to be so high above the water. [A ffe c t iv e opposites] SOLUTION Which best describes E lis e when the huntsmen fin d her D She is pleased (a t) the k in g 's (c a lls her ,"beautiful maiden") a tte n tio n . 2) She is worried th a t she w i l l be taken from her work. [ A ffe c t iv e opposites] 1 4 6 CONFLICT Which best describes the bishop. I) He was a bad person. [ A ffe c t iv e - 2) He (wanted to know,) what went, on (a t the palace). n e u tra l] When the bishop sees the witches (on the graves which do you think best describes how he f e l t ? ) how did he feel? I) (He was) frig hten ed (watching them claw the e a rth ). 2) (He was) glad (they were there) because he could t e l l the king. [A f f e c t iv e ] opposites] As the story was to ld to you how do you thin k the king f e l t when the people judged E lise to be a witch? I) Relieved he d i d n 't have to decide. 2) Sad they said she was a w itch. Which of these is most true abou the story? 1) I t was a story about hardship in l i f e and love. 2) I t was a story o f long ago when there was adventure, magic, kings, and queens. 147 APPENDIX B FINAL INSTRUMENT AND STORY TEXT 148 FINAL INSTRUMENT NAME________ SCHOOL THE WILD SWANS PART I 1. At the beginning, which seemed most important? a. b. 2. A fte r the king got married, which seemed most important? a. b. 3. The queen sent E lis e away. The queen hated the c h ild re n . As the brothers flew from the palace, which seemed most important? a. b. 4. These children were happy. These child re n liv e d in a warm and pleasant land. They c ir c le d the countryside. Their c rie s were strange and m ournful. Which seemed most important? a. b. The years th a t passed f o r E lis e were bleak and empty. E lise was h o r r if ie d at the sad fa te of her brothers. i 5. Which seemed most important as E lis e wandered about? a. b. 6. When Elise reached the seashore, which seemed most important? a. b. 7. Tears streamed down her face. The a i r in the fo r e s t was s t i l l and m ild. She began to weep. She saw eleven swans' feathers on the sand. Which seemed most important when she saw the swans? a. b. The swans had golden crowns. E lise was joyous. 149 8. Which seemed most important? a. b. 9. A fte r the King found E lis e , which seemed most important? a. b. 10. The king saw the witches s i t t i n g on the graves. The king could watch no more and c rie d , "Let the people judge h e r!" A fte r E lise was taken to the dungeon which seemed most important? a. b. 16. The k in g 's sorrow troubled her and added to her pain. The king and the bishop both followed E lise the next time. Then which seemed most important? a. b. 15. E lise saw the k in g 's sorrow. She had only one more s hirt, to k n i t . Which seemed most important? a. b. 14. The bishop had followed E lis e to the churchyard. The kin g 's heart was broken when he heard about E lis e . Which seemed most important? a. b. 13. E lise walked to the churchyard alone. She was h o r r if ie d when she saw the witches. Which seemed most important? a. b. 12. The king loved E lise and wanted to please her. The bishop whispered the fo re s t maiden must be a w itc h . When she went to get more n e ttle s , which seemed most important? a. b. 11. They laughed and cried and held each oth er. They spent the night making a net. A ll night long she worked. She was happy though she might soon be dead. Which of these seemed most important? a. b. She cried " I am innocent." The oldest to ld them a ll th a t had happened. 150 pArt i i From the way the story was to ld to you: I» Do you th in k when they were l i t t l e a. b. c. 2. mean to her? kind to her? I d o n 't know. Do you th in k th a t E lis e 's fa th e r a. b. c. 7. was pretty? was not pretty? I d o n 't know. Do you th in k the farmers who took care of E lise were a. b. c. 6. had been nice? had not been nice? I d o n 't know. Do you th in k the queen the king married a. b. c. 5. She had moved away. She had died. I d o n 't know. Do you th in k th a t t h e i r real mother a. b. c. 4. did not get along.w ith each other? did get along w ith each other? I d o n 't know. Why do you th in k the c h ild r e n 's real mother was not l i v i n g w ith them? a. b. c. 3. E lise and her brothers d id n 't care about what happened? was sad about what happened? I d o n 't know. Why do you th in k the king did not t r y to help E lis e and her brothers? a. b. c. He d id n 't want to . The queen had power over him. I d o n 't know. 151 From the way the story was t o ld to you: 8. Do you th in k the fo re s t E lise went in to a. b. c. 9. Do you th in k when E lis e met the old woman in the fo re s t th a t Elise a. b. c. 10. being shot at by hunters? not fin d in g enough food? I d o n 't know. When E lis e was f l y i n g in the net over the sea, do you th in k she was a. b. c. 13. was fr ie n d ly to Elise? d id n 't want to be bothered by Elise? I d o n 't know. When the brothers liv e d as swans, do you thin k they were most a fra id of a. b. c. 12. was a fra id of her? was not a fra id o f her? I d o n 't know. Do you th in k th a t the old woman in the fo re s t a. b. c. 11. was an ordinary forest? had a magical power? I d o n 't know. frightened to be so high? happy to be w ith her brothers? I d o n 't know. Do you th in k E lise a. b. c. lik e d her task because i t would free her brothers? d is lik e d her task because i t would cause her a l o t a pain? I d o n 't know. 152 14. Which do you th in k best describes E lis e when the huntsmen fin d her? a. b. c. She is worried she w ill be taken from her work. She is pleased at the k in g 's a tte n tio n . I d o n 't know. From the way the s to ry was to ld to you: 15. Which do you th in k best describes the bishop? a. b. c. 16. He wanted to know a ll th a t was going on. He was an e v i l , mean person. I d o n 't know. Which of these do you th in k is most tru e about the story? a. b. c. I t is a sto ry about love and l i f e ' s bad times and good tim es. I t is a sto ry o f long ago when there was adventure and magic. I d o n 't know. 153 PART I I I I. TRUE FALSE E lise had a fo o ts to o l made of glass. 2. TRUE FALSE E lise liv e d w ith the farmers only a few months. 3. TRUE FALSE E lis e had th irte e n brothers who were turned in to swans. 4. TRUE FALSE A messenger to ld E lis e what happened to her bro th e rs. 5. TRUE FALSE While she s le p t in the fo re s t, E lis e dreamed o f her b rothe rs. 6. TRUE FALSE The swans wore royal purple capes on t h e ir backs. 7. TRUE FALSE At sunset the swans were changed in to men. 8. TRUE FALSE The swans c a rrie d E lis e on a net made o f rushes and bark. 9. TRUE FALSE The f a ir y in E lis e 's dream and the old woman in the fo re s t did not look lik e each o th e r. 10. TRUE FALSE I f E lis e ta lke d w h ile she k n it, her hands would be too b lis te re d to work. 11. TRUE FALSE The n e ttle s E lis e needed grew in graveyards. 12. TRUE FALSE The people in the k in g 's co u rt did not th in k E lise was b e a u tifu l. 13. TRUE FALSE Elise- was frig h te n e d by the witches in the graveyard. 14. TRUE 15. TRUE FALSE The s tic k s fo r the f i r e grew leaves and roses. 16. TRUE FALSE The o ld e st brother was l e f t w ith a swan's wing instead of an arm. FALSE While E lis e was in the dungeon, her brothers went to the palace to t r y to see the kin g . 1 5 4 STORY TEXT "THE WILD SWANS" Far, fa r away, in a warm and pleasant land, there once liv e d a king who had eleven sons and one daughter. The princes wore stars on t h e ir s h irts and swords at th e ir sides, and th e ir s is te r E lis e sat on a fo o ts to o l made o f gla ss. These c h ild re n were happy from the time they woke in the morning u n til they went to t h e ir beds at n ig h t. But a fte r some years had passed, the king married a wicked queen who hated the ch ild re n and wanted only to be rid of them. I t was not long before the queen sent E lise away to be looked a fte r by farmers. But the fa te of the princes was even worse. "You sh a ll become great voiceless birds and f l y out in to the w o rld ," she commanded. the princes turned in to eleven w ild swans. At once With strange and mournful c rie s , they flew from the palace windows and c irc le d the countryside u n til they came to the place where ETise liv e d . They hovered over the ro o f, beating th e ir wings but by the tim e E lise came o u tside, they had gone. The years th a t passed were bleek and empty. Then one b rig h t morning a messenger from her fa th e r a rriv e d to t e l l E lise the sad fa te o f her bro th e rs. took a ll her l i f e . fie ld s and h i l l s . H o rrifie d * E lise resolved to fin d them, even i f i t That n ig h t she s to le away and wandered over the Tears streamed down her face. At n ig h tfa ll she came in to a great fo r e s t. The a ir was s t i l l and m ild , and everywhere glowworms shone w ith a green f i r e . lay down they cascaded upon her lik e shooting s ta rs . As she Al I n ig h t long 155 E lis e dreamed of her bro th e rs. Once more they were c h ild re n playing to g e th e r, carefree and loved. But when she awoke she was alone in the fo re s t and she set out again. She had walked only a few steps when she met an old woman. E lis e asked i f she had seen eleven p rin ce s. "No," said the old woman, "but e a r lie r I saw eleven swans w ith golden crowns swimming down a stream nearby." She pointed the way, and E lis e follow ed the stream u n til i t came out upon the open shore where before her lay the whole immense sea. E lis e threw h e rs e lf down and began to weep. Suddenly on the sand she saw eleven swans' feathers and as she looked up, to her jo y , E lise saw eleven swans w ith golden crowns fly in g toward the shore. As the red disk o f the sun sank beneath the waves, they a lig h te d and one by one they turned in to her bro th e rs. She ran in to th e ir arms and they laughed and crie d and held each other in the growing darkness. The e ld e st to ld her t h e ir s to ry . "As long as the sun is in the sky we f l y about as w ild swans, but when nig ht comes, we re tu rn to human shape. That is why we must always search fo r s o lid ground at sunset, fo r we would f a l l from the clouds to our deaths. must f l y away to a land across the sea. Tomorrow we Have you the courage to come w ith us?" "Yes!" c rie d E lis e . So they spent th a t n ig h t making a net from w illo w bark and rushes. E lis e lay down upon i t and f e l l asleep. At daybreak as the brothers were changed in to swans they seized the net w ith t h e ir beaks and flew up to the clouds. over the sea when E lise awoke. They were fa r out 1 5 6 As the sun rose higher she began to see p ictu re s in the clou ds-mountains and fo re sts and shining palaces--and at la s t she looked down and saw land. The swans flew over cedar woods th a t smelled fra g ra n t in the sunshine and set her before a cavern hung w ith d e lic a te green p la n ts . There she f e l l asleep, and dreamed th a t she flew up to the palaces she had seen in the clouds. A b e a u tifu l f a ir y came to her who looked ju s t lik e the old woman she'd seen in the fo r e s t. "Your brothers can be fre e d ," she s a id , "but the pain you must bear is g re a t. Look c lo se ly a t the s tin g in g n e ttle in my hand. graves in churchyards. Gather only those th a t grow on the They w ill b lis t e r your s k in , but you must crush them, spin the fla x and k n it i t in to eleven s h ir ts . When you throw these over the w ild swans, the sp e ll w ill be broken. But remember--from the time you begin the work u n til i t is ended, you must be s ile n t . Your f i r s t word w ill pierce your b ro th e r's hearts lik e a sword." Then E lis e awoke and began her ta s k . At sundown the brothers came back and were alarmed by her silence and her b lis te re d hands, but they understood th a t her work was fo r t h e ir sake. The next day, as she k n itte d suddenly hunting horns rang out and huntsmen appeared at the cavern. The most handsone among them was the king. "Why are you hiding here, b e a u tifu l maiden?" he asked. shook her head, not daring to speak. E lise only "Come w ith me," he said and the king took her on his horse and galloped o f f among the mountains, to h is m agnificent c it y . He led her in to a b e a u tifu l palace where she 157 was brought before the c o u rt. So g lo rio u s was her beauty th a t everyone bowed down before her and the king took her fo r his b rid e . Only the bishop was unmoved. He.whispered th a t the fo re s t maiden must be a w itch and th a t she had beguiled the king . Though he d id n 't want to , he was forced to marry them. A fte r E lis e became queen the king led her to a l i t t l e looked e xa ctly lik e the cavern in the fo r e s t. and the s h irts were th e re . chamber th a t Her bundle of n e ttle s As E lis e was shown a ll these thin gs the king saw her smile fo r the f i r s t tim e and joyo usly embraced her. As the days passed E lis e yearned to confide in the k in g , who loved her and wanted so much to please her. Each nig ht when he was sleeping E lis e k n itte d , one s h ir t a fte r another. she found there was no fla x l e f t . But as she began the seventh So one m oonlit n ig h t she s to le away fe a r fu lly and walked to the churchyard alone where she found more n e ttle s . To her h o rro r though on the newly dug graves she saw a group o f witches clawing at the earth w ith long bony fin g e rs . She was not the only one who saw them. The bishop who had f o l ­ lowed her saw them too and now was c e rta in E lis e was a w itc h . Quickly he to ld the king whose heart was broken. Elisfe saw the k in g 's sorrow, and i t tro uble d her g re a tly , adding to the pain she already f e l t fo r her b ro th e rs . fin is h e d and had only one more s h ir t to k n it . churchyard. But now she was almost She went again to the This time the bishop and the king follow ed her. When the king saw the witches s it t in g on the graves he could watch no more and c rie d , "Let the people judge h e r!" and they d id . she was a w itch and must d ie by f i r e . They declared 158 E lis e was led away to a dark dungeon where she continued k it t in g . Toward evening a swan a lig h te d at her c e ll. b ro th e r. He had found E lis e at la s t! I t was her youngest She was so happy because though she might soon be dead, her task was almost done and her brothers were nearby. Al I n ig h t long she worked. S hortly before daybreak the eleven brothers came to the palace and demanded to be taken before the king . They begged and threatened u n til f i n a l ly the king appeared. But at th a t very moment the sun came up, and only eleven w ild swans could be seen, fly in g away. As the execution drew near a ll the people of the c it y streamed from the gates to see the w itch d ie . E lis e was taken to the f i r e in- an open c a rt k n ittin g the la s t s h ir t . The crowd je e re d , "Look at the w itc h !" and rushed at he r, but eleven swans flew down upon the c a rt and beat t h e ir wide wings f u r i ­ o u sly. The crowd moved back fe a r fu lly and some whispered, "Perhaps th is is a sign from heaven and she is in n o ce n t!" As the executioner took E lise by the hand she q u ic k ly threw the eleven s h irts over the w ild swans and suddenly, eleven handsome princes stood th e re , the youngest o f which had a swan's wing in place of an arm, fo r E lis e had not time enough to fin is h his sleeve. "Now I can speak!" she c rie d . in to her b ro th e rs' arms, exhausted. " I am inn ocen t!" Then she f e ll "Yes, she is in n o ce n t," said the e ld e st and he to ld them a ll th a t had happened. As he spoke the a ir was f i l l e d w ith perfume, fo r every stake in the f i r e had grown leaves and red roses and at the very top shone a ' 159 s in g le w hite rose. When the king picked i t and held i t to her face she awoke and smiled at him, fo r she had loved him from the beginning. As they returned to the palace great flo c k s o f bird s appeared and churchbells rang out throughout a ll the land. 160 APPENDIX C INSTRUCTIONS TO TREATMENT GROUPS AND LOG 161 In s tru c tio n s to Treatment Groups In s tru c tio n s were standardized except fo r deviation s as noted below in the lo g . The researcher introduced h e rs e lf as a lib r a r ia n working w ith the elementary lib r a r ie s . Students were to ld they were part o f a p ro je ct to fin d out what "kid s th in k about s to r ie s ." I t was explained they would be presented a s to ry , th a t afterw ards they could stand up to "get the kinks out" and then they would be given some questions to fin d out what they thought about the s to ry . The s t o r y te lle r was introduced. A fte r the presentations, she l e f t and was not present during the a d m in is tra tio n o f the questions, except during the la s t a d m in istra tio n on the la s t day. The students helped pass out the instrum ents were given to the student. In the la t t e r case, the and spoke his or her name to gain m aterial (p e n cils and cardboard), and students by the experimenter or by a experimenter went to each student f a m ilia r it y and e s ta b lis h rapport. I t was explained th a t the instrum ent was not a regula r kind of te s t but one used to determine what "k id s th in k about s to rie s " and th a t there were three parts w ith no rig h t or wrong answers in the f i r s t two and tru e -fa ls e questions in the t h ir d . The researcher attempted to make the students fee l as relaxed and com fortable as possible and to e s ta b lis h a nonthreatening atmosphere. The d ire c tio n s were explained in fo rm a lly to the students, ra th e r than fo rm a lly , to e s ta b lis h th is atmosphere. The students were to ld th a t two other schools were in v o lv e d , th a t each student was in d iv id u a lly id e n tifie d by the number at the top of the page, and th a t th e ir answers would subsequently be fed in to a com­ p u te r. Log - Data C o lle c tio n , May 4-11 The fo llo w in g de via tio n s from com pletely standardized procedures occurred during data c o lle c tio n . 1. Group I (May 4, Margaret Leary) a d m in is tra tio n o f questions was in te rru p te d by an unexpected chorus rehearsal. Questions had to be repeated lo u d ly and in some cases gone over again. 2. Group 2 (May 4, Margaret Leary) was moved to a d iffe r e n t lo c a tio n (a q u ie t corner of the lib r a r y ) to avoid having in te rfe re n c e from a chorus rehearsal next door. Since the school is an open space school, the students were probably f a i r l y com fortable in the new s e ttin g . 162 3. Students from one class reported t h e ir teacher had not in ­ formed them ahead of time of the p ro je c t. 4. I f the students seemed to grow re s tle s s during adm inistra­ tio n of the questions, they were asked to stand up and s tre tc h an a d d itio n a l tim e. 5. Margaret Leary (May 4 ), a fifte e n minute recess separated the f i r s t two presentations from the t h ir d ; Kennedy (May 8 ), a t h i r t y minute recess separated the f i r s t two presentations from the t h ir d ; Longfellow (May 11), no recess separated the presen tatio ns. 6. Group 7 (book and reader, Longfellow , May 11) in s tru c tio n to the students to "pay close a tte n tio n " was in a d v e rte n tly o m itte d . However, observation ind ica ted the students paid as good, i f not b e tte r , a tte n tio n than other groups. 7. Kennedy Groups 4, 5 and 6 were presented on Tuesday, May 8, instead o f Monday, May 7, as scheduled because o f an unan­ tic ip a te d change of plans. 8. Seating fo r Kennedy groups was not as com fortable because the lib r a r y flo o r is not carpeted. Also, the room was not as dark as in the other schools fo r the s lid e presentation because of the lig h tin g c o n d itio n s . 9. Groups which included a lte rn a te students because o f absences were to ld th e ir names had been selected by a. computer. This explanation proved valuable as one of the teachers stated a c e rta in parent might ob ject to the s e le c tiv e grouping. In th a t school, the s lid e show presentation was brought back fo r the whole class the fo llo w in g week. 10. With Groups 7 and 8, Longfellow , May 11, the experimenter was introduced by an aide. Group 9, Longfellow , May 11, was sent e a rly by the teacher, and the re was, consequently, a delay in s e ttin g up the s lid e show. 11. Two groups spontaneously applauded (Group I , Margaret Leary, May 4, and Group 3, Longfellow , May 11). 12. D elivery of the s to ry was very standardized fo r Groups I , 2, 4, 5, 7, and 8, although responsiveness of the students var­ ie d , Groups 4 and 5 being le a s t responsive, as observed by the researcher. 13. One o f the students (512) used a red p e n c il. 1 6 3 14. Other deviations included: one loudspeaker in te rru p tio n (May 4, Group I ) and students re tu rn in g lib r a r y books (May 11, Group 7 .and Group 8 ). 15. Other d e via tio n s included: one loudspeaker in te rru p tio n (May 4, Group I ) and students re tu rn in g lib r a r y books (May 11, Group 7 and Group 8 ). I APPENDIX D STANDARDIZED DATA ON COMPARABLE STUDENT GROUPS 1982-1983 STANDARDIZED DATA ON COMPARABLE STUDENT GROUPS IN STUDY School Stanford Stanford O tis Achieve­ O tis * Achieve­ Stanford Achievement A b ilit y ment A b ilit y ment* Grade 6Grade 3 Grade 4 Grade 5 Stanine Grade 2 — Stanine Total Total Total Liste ning x O tis Language x O tis Language Total Sample. Score Reading L iste n in g Comprehension Score Sample 4 4 6 104 5 97 5 Kennedy 4 5 5 101 6 100 6 Blaine 7 7 6 94 7 95 4 Margaret Leary 5 6 8 105 7 97 5 W ebster-Garfield 6 5 7 102 6 99 5 Terms: Grade 2 = Grade 2 students were the actual th ir d graders in th is study, x = Mean O tis A b ilit y Score Stanine = Stanine band *Accelerated Learner Program students not included in th is data. 165 Longfellow APPENDIX E PERMISSIONS ■ 167 tieptombor 7, 1983 Dr. Tim Sullivan Curriculum Director Butte School District No. I 111 N. Montana Butte, MT 59701 Dear Dr. Sullivan: As you know I am working on my doctoral diss.urtation at Montana Statu University. I am hoping you will give your approval for me to do a study this school year with third grade students from the Butte School District. If you give your permission, I am hoping you will submit- this letter to Superintendent Mr. Milligan for his approval. The study I am proposing is a comparison of throe modes of story presentation: a sound slide show, a book and reader and a story teller. The study ties in closely with library-media concerns since all three modes of story presentation are common in school libraries. The purpose of the study is to determine whether each mode brings different content to the foreground of t he"children *s attention. Altogether approximately 125 students would be involved. I would like to use four different schools - one for the piloting to determine instrument reliability, and three for the actual study. I would like to be able to use schools designated by Mr. Milligan and yourself as ’’average" Butte schools. For the actual study, each student would only be involved for about 40 minutes. The presentations would tJe in closely with a library and/or literature presentation, since a fairy tale is being used for the story, and hopefully the presentations would be enriching and enjoyable for the students. I appreciate your consideration of my request and will be happy to furnish any additional information you or Mr. Milligan would like to have. very much for your time! Sincerely, Thank you ! 168 B u tte P u b lic S c h o o ls O fllce of Ihe Curriculum Director Phone 782-6315 ext. 12 or 46 111 North Montana Street" Butte, Montana 59701 Or. Tlm Sullivan Curriculum Director August 3, 1984 Margaret Rolando 2725 St. Anns Butte, Montana 59701 Dear Mrs. Rolando: This is to v e rify that you received permission from both Superintendent W illiam M illig a n and myself during the Fall of 1983 to conduct your doctoral research in Butte School D is tric t No. I . In a d d itio n , permission was granted by a ll the princip als of the schools involved p rio r to conducting the research. Dr. Tim Sullivan Curriculum Director TES:ib AN EQUAL OPPORTUNITY EMPLOYER 169 May 31, 1983 Ms. Margaret Rolando 2725 St. Ann's Butte, MT 59701 Dear Ms. Rolando: We hereby grant you permission to use the name Weston Woods in accordance with the request made in your letter of May 22, 1983. I would be interested in seeing a copy of your dissertation upon completion, if it would be practical for you to share it with me. With best wishes for the success of your project, Sincerely, Morton Schindel M S :br western woods studios weston, ct. 06883 Telephone 800 2<i3-5020/ConnechcuM?03)?26-33r>5 DIAL BOOKS FOR YOUNG READERS A D ivision o f E .R D u tlo n 1 Inc July 5, 1983 3 P A R K AV EN U E • N E W V O R K 1 N Y. 10016 ( 212) 725-1818 Cable YA flD fAfl M n . Margaret Rolando 2725 St. Ann's Butte, MT 59701 Telex: 12-5636 Dear Mn. Rolando: Thie will confirm our conversation of this m o m IMt that we will grant you permission without charge to utilize THE WILD SWANS by Hana Christian Andersen ♦ retold by Aray Ehrlich» and illustrated by Susan Jeffers in your dissertation, now tentatively titled THE STORY TELLER, THE READER, AND THE MEDIA: An Investigation of Children's Perceptions of Story Context as Illustrated by Three.Modes of Presentation, as partial fulfilment of the degree requirements at Montana State University in Bozeman, Montana. It is agreed that you will fully credit author, illustrator and publisher of the title. It is further understood, that this oermission covers use for your dissertation in unpublished form only. In the event that your dissertation should be published, in whole or in part, you will need to reanply to us. With all best wd Sincerely yours Christopher M. I Subsidiary Rights manager . work. MONTANA STATF iikm /rD cr-rv __________ D 378 R6U2 R o l a n d o , M. K. An investigation of children's perceptions, cop. 2 IS S U E D T O D A T E :1 D 378 R61#2 con. 2