Perceptrral andMofor Skills, 2000,91, 123-130. O Perceptual and Motor Skills 2000 STUTTERERS' VOCAL REACTION TIMES' TO UNILATERALLY PRESENTED HIGH AND LOW FREQUENCY VERBS ' MANISH K. RAM1 University of Norfh Dakota RICHARD E. SHINE AND MICHAEL P. RASTATTER Tachistoscopic Viewing Laboratory Department of Cornmunica~iotzSciences and Disorders East Carolina Utzjversity Szrmmary.-The current study investigated vocal reaction times of 11 stuttering participants to verbs with high and low frequencies of occurrence when these verbs were unilaterally visually presented. N o significant main effects or interactions between stimuli and visual-half field were found. Reaction times were 16 msec. faster after right visual-field presentations and the Pearson correlation coefficient between visual fields .71 (p=.02). These results were interpreted as suggesting h a t the Iefc hemisphere was dominant for processing the current lexical items, findings which parallel those for normal speakers. The use of tachistoscopic viewing procedures with people who stutter has yielded data from which we may infer that both the right and left hemispheres are involved in responses to visually presented information. Specifically, while it has been shown that the left hemisphere5 of stuttering subjects are involved in response to language, evidence suggcscs that the right hemispheres contain the dominant neuronal structures underlying many lquistic events, includmg object and letter naming, performing verbal processing strategies, and evokmg lexical decisions (Hand & Haynes, 1983; Johannsen & Victor, 1986; Rastatter & Dell, 1987a, 1987b, 1988; Rastatter, Loren, & Colcord, 1987; Rastatter & Loren, 1988; Rastatter, McGuire, & Loren, 1988; Rastatter & Stuart, 1995). While studes indicate that the right hemisphere is dominant primarily for visual language processing in those who stutter, issues pertaining to the nature of hemispheric organization for a larger set of hguistic elements in stuttering subjects remain equivocal. Specifically, it is not known whether the left and right hemispheres of stutterers are involved in processing other forms of language as they are for normal speakers. Research has shown that the intact, normal right hemisphere shows limited linguistic processing char- 'Please direct correspondence to hfichael P. Rastatter, Ph.D., Chair, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, East Carolina University, Greenville, NC 27858-4353 or e-mail (rastatterm@mail.ecu.edu). 124 M.K. RAMI, ET AL. acteristics. Various studies have indexed the semantic capacity of the right hemisphere using various tachistoscopic viewing techniques and stimuli (Day, 1977, 1979; Rastatter, Dell, McGuire, & Loren, 1987; Rastatter & McGuire, 1990). Rastatter and Loren (1988) provided information pertaining to the involvement of the left and right hemispheres in processing verbs by normal subjects. In that investigation vocal reaction times of normal subjects were measured to unilaterally presented verbs of high and low frequency. There was a significant interaction between the stimulus items a n d visual fields. Post hoc tests showed that vocal reaction times to verbs of high frequency were significantly faster following presentations to the right visual field. No significant differences in reaction times were found between the two visual fields for the verbs of low frequency. Also, significant differences were obtained benveen the two types of verbs following presentations to the left visual field but not the right. These results were interpreted as suggesting that right hemispheric analysis was restricted to the verbs of high frequency in the presence of a dominant left hemisphere. Under experimental conditions in which both the left and right hemispheres of normal subjects are involved in processing a certain lexicon, the verbal competence of the minor (right) hemisphere is said to be inhibited by the superior (left) hemisphere via an interhemispheric process referred to as reciprocal inhibition ( b s b o u r n e , 1975; Moscovitch, 1976; Rastatter, et al., 1987). This phenomenon, based on general biological principles that govern the relationships among many laterally symmetrical structures, is related to the magnitude of cortical substrate subserving a given function. Because the neurological structures mediating language processing are larger in the left hemisphere in normal, right-handed speakers, the corresponding interhemispheric inhibitory effect reflects this asymmetry and is expressed as a suppression of right-hemisphere linguistic abhties. From their study, Rastatter and Loren (1988) inferred that, compared with unfamhar, action-oriented hguistic processes, left hemispheric-processing mechanisms responsible for analyzing familiar verbs impose an interhemispheric inhibition on parallel right hemispheric hguistic functions. Unhke the high frequency items, the latency findmgs suggested that the right hemisphere was more restricted in its processing functions, serving possibly as an afferent channel shuttling the verbs of low frequency to the left hemisphere for signal analysis. The curve obtained for the verbs of low frequency was nearly horizontal, with the intercept slightly higher (14 msec.) for the left visual-field. Based on the principles of interhemispheric inhibition, active right hemispheric processing of the verbs of low frequency should have produced a more robust intervisualfield response time due to the reciprocal inhibiting effects of the left hemisphere. Since this was not the case, right hemisphere signal analysis was not predicted for the low frequency items. VOCAL REACTION TIMES FOR VERBS IN STUTTERERS 125 While information pertaining to neurolinguistic organization needs further exploration in stuttering individuals, the inferred nature of interhemispheric interaction governing the events associated with processing verbs of high and low frequency remains uncertain. This is important since functional differences in reciprocal, interhemispheric inhibition for other language processing tasks have been observed for the stuttering population (Rastatter & Dell, 1987b). The purpose of the current experiment was to measure vocal reaction times of people who stutter to unilateral, tachistoscopically presented verbs of high and low frequency to study the response configuration and suggest a processing model. Zaidel (1983) argued that callosal relay mode of processing is manifested as significant positive correlation while the direct access mode of processing is evidenced by a low positive correlation or a high negative correlation. So a correlation analysis between the two lateralized conditions should top the mode of processing for action-oriented lexical information. The concern here was primarily with examination of response configurations and applicable models. Eleven people who stuttered (8 males, 3 females; age range=14-56 years) and were native English-only speakers served as participants. It should be noted that although there might be some change in reaction time over the age range, the configuration of the responses, which is of primary inrerest, should remain constant. No effects for sex have been reported in the literature for this type of task with stutterers so none were examined. AU participants had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. Case history information was used to validate the inclusion criteria of no other speech or language disorder than stuttering. Severity of stuttering ranged from mild to severe. AU participants indcated a right-hand preference (Annett, 1970). Ten high frequency and 10 low frequency four-letter verbs from Rastatter and Loren (1988), and 20 four-letter, pronounceable nonwords were used as stimuli. The frequency of occurrence of the high-frequency and lowfrequency verbs was at least 100 and 9.5 per mdhon, respectively, in general reading material (Thorndlke & Lorge, 1944). The nonword stimuli were randomly arranged letters constructed from the experimental items. All nonword stimuh were developed in accordance with the rules of English phonetic sequencing. Each stimulus word was presented vertically in capital letters (Font: Geneva; Size: 14; Style: Bold) using a PICT f i e format created with a commercially available software (Claris Corporation Model MacDraw I1 Ver. 1.1). All stimuli were presented unilaterally to both the left and the 126 M.K. RAMI, ET AL right visual fields at a subtended angle of approximately 3.5" (83 pixels) from the central fixation to the lateral periphery. Apparatus The visual stimuh were presented to both visual fields (right and left) through the use of SuperLab Pro (Cedrus Corp., 1990), an experimental software program that presents visual stimuli on a 12-in. monitor (Applevision Model 1710 AV Display) with the screen resolution set at 640. 480 pixels, which served as a tachistoscope. Each stimulus item was presented for a duration of 100 msec. Simultaneously with the onset of illumination, SuperLab Pro's presentation timer activated a second dtgital timer, accurate to 1 msec. A signal from the voice-operated relay stopped the timer, which provided a vocal reaction time (rnsec.) for each stimulus item. The timer was stopped by a signal from a voice-operated relay (SuperLab Pro) activated by a microphone (Apple Model Plaintalk) placed approximately 65 cm from the lips with an orientation of 0" azimuth and 20" altitude. This timed activation yielded a lexical decision response time for each stimulus item. Procedure A participant was seated in front of a computer screen a t a distance of 64 cm so that a constant visual angle was maintained throughout the experiment. The head position was secured on a chin rest, and each person was asked to focus on a central fixation point present on the computer screen. The experimenter explained that a series of vertically typed words would appear to- the left or the right of the fixation point. The participant's task was to phonate the vowel /a/ into the microphone when the stimulus was an actual word. Participants were asked not to phonate when a nonword was presented. All stimulus words were presented for an exposure duration of a 100 msec. An interstimulus interval of 2000 msec. was employed. The order of stimulus presentation was randomized, with the restriction that an identical four-letter stimulus would not appear in direct sequence. The order of presentation was identical for all participants. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION The means and standard deviations summarized in Table 1 were subjected to a repeated two-factor analysis of variance to test the significance of main effects and interactions. Main effects for visual field (F1,,,=0.12, ns, o2= 0), verb frequency ( F , , , =3.36, , ns, oZ= 0), and the interaction of visual field by verb fluency (F,,8=1.62, ns, w2=0) were not significant. The Pearson correlation between the reaction times for the right and the left visual fields, collapsed across verb frequency, was statistically sign&cant (r = .71, p = .02). These results showed that the main effects for stimuli and visual-half VOCAL REACTION TIMES FOR VERBS IN STUTTERERS 127 TABLE 1 A N D STANDARD DEVIATIONS O F REACTION TIMES(MSEC.)FOR GRANDMEANS ~ G H T A N DLEFTVISUAL FIELDS FORALLVERBSCOMBINED (n= 11) Right Visual Field Verbs Left Visual Field M SD M SD 873 172 889 205 field were nonsignificant, with vocal reaction times favoring the right sensory inputs by 16 msec. Such a finding best fits an interhemispheric transfer interpretation. This indicates that, although each hemisphere may be capable of performing a component of a given processing task, the stages of processing required to complete the operation are functionally localized to one hemisphere (Moscovitch, 1976). When stimuli are delivered to the hemisphere capable of partial processing only, the information eventually must be sent to the specialized hemisphere via a transcallosal route. Left Right Visual Field FIG. 1. Mean reaction times (in msec.) for verbs of high ( 0 ) and low ( A ) frequency by visual field The correlations between the two visual-half fields support an inference of interhemispheric transfer as well. Correlational analyses between lateralized conhtions have been used to prehct the presence of left and right hemispheric processing capacities. Zaidel (1983) argued that the presence of a high positive correlation predicts an interhenlispheric transfer interpretation since the processing strategy of a single hemisphere is responsible for completing the task demands. Under these conditions the dominant hemi- 128 M. K. M I . ET AL. sphere must be accessed for the task to be completed, regardless of the hemisphere stimulated. The correlation between visual fields was high and significant in this experiment, hence the inference of an interhemispheric transfer interpretation is supported for these stuttering subjects' data. Such findings parallel the normal subjects of Rastatter and Loren (1988) in certain respects but diverge in others. Specifically, the finding that reaction times were faster following- right visual-field stimulation suggests that the left hemispheres of the stuttering participants were dominant for the current lexical decision tasks, which findings are consistent with the data derived from the normal speakers. Zaidel (1983) suggested that a consistent laterality effect of modest magnitude would be predicted in the intact brain based on conditions of exclusive specialization of hemispheric processing. Under these processing condtions, wherein perceptual laterality effects are based on "callosal connectivity," information delivered initially to the hemisphere not containing the resources required for stimulus analysis must be shuttled via the corpus callosum to the hemisphere specialized for performing the task demands required for analysis. In addition to the finding that the left hemisphere was responsible for analyzing the current verb s t i m d , the main effect for verb type was nonsignificant. Such a fiidmg is consistent with the normal speaker's data, suggesting that there is no processing hierarchy for analyzing verbs of high versus low frequency. Ln other words, the time involved in generating a lexical decision for the two types of verbs was not related to frequency of occurrence of the hguistic set. Such a finding is intriguing and suggests that neurolinguistic organization for certain lexical classes are parallel for stuttering and nonstuttering speakers. These findmgs are not consistent with hemispheric organization for other lexical classes in the stuttering population. While it has been inferred from empirical work that the left hemispheres of stuttering subjects can process visual language, evidence suggests that the right hemisphere is dominant for performing many hguistic events, including object and letter naming, performing verbal processing strategies, and evoking lexical decisions (Hand & Haynes, 1983; Johannsen & Victor, 1986; Rastatter & Dell, 1987a, 1987b, 1988; Rastatter, Loren, & Colcord, 1987; Rastatter & Loren, 1988; Rastatter, McGuire, & Loren, 1988; Rastatter & Stuart, 1995). While the results obtained for the stuttering subjects seem parallel to those observed for the nonstuttering participants of Rastatter and Loren (1988) with respect to laterality characteristics, the vocal reaction times for the stuttering participants suggest that interhemispheric inhibiting patterns underlying the processing operations for completing the current task demands are likely not the same as those of normal speakers. As noted above, the stuttering participants' latencies were considered consistent with an inference of interhemispheric transfer wherein the left hemisphere is responsi- VOCAL REACTION TIMES FOR VERBS IN STUTTERERS 129 ble for verb recognition. Such a finding may indicate that inhibitory interactions between the two hemispheres governing verb assimilation among stutterers may not be related to frequency of occurrence. As noted earlier, Rastatter and Loren (1988) suggested the right hemispheres of the normal participants were involved in completing lexical decisions for the verbs of high frequency. On such findings were based the inference that the right-hernispheric, linguistic substrates are capable of processing such information in nonstuttering samples. The vocal-reaction findings obtained for the stuttering participants, however, suggest that the right hemisphere was not involved in the performance of lexical decisions on the verb stirnd. Given that the inference the right hemisphere is organized and based on associations, emotions, and structural correspondence, and responsible for various hguistic decoding and encoding events in stutterers, limitations in neural resources may necessitate bilateral linguistic processing related to neurohguistics by stuttering people. Finally, the current results, taken in concert with past neurolinguistic evidence, suggest that a diversity of sensorilinguistic models may be operational for stuttering samples. While the relation between hemispheric-hguistic processing and stuttering behavior is not clear, the present findmgs increase our understanding of the psychophysical dimensions subserving language-related events in stuttering subjects. Such avenues of experimentation may broaden our empirical base and foster theoretical efforts pertaining to the complex neuronal dynamics that underlie or accompany stuttering behavior. REFERENCES A classification of hand preference by association analysis. Britirh Journal ANNETT.M. (1970) ofPsychology, 61, 301-321. CEDRUS COUP.(1990) Szrper Lnb Pro. [Compurer software] Phoenix, AZ: Author. DAY,J. (1977) %ght-hemisphere language processin in normal right-handers. Jotrrrzal of Experimental Psychology: Htl~nanPerceplion and Pe&nnance, 3, 518-528. DAY,J. (1979) Visual half-field word recognition as a function of syntactic class and imageability. Nezrropsychologia, 17, 515-519. HAND, C. R., & HAYNES.W. 0. (1983) Linguistic processing and reaction time differences in stutterers and nonsrutterers. Journal ofSpeech and Hearing Research, 26, 181-185. JOHANNSEN, H. S., &VICTOR,C. (1986) Visual information processing in the left and right hemis heres during unilateral tachistoscopic stimulation of stutterers. Jotlrrtal of Fluency ~i.rorLrs,61, 285-291. KINSBOURNE, M. (1975) Lateral interactions in the brain. In M. Kinsbourne & W. L. Smith (Eds.), Hemispheric disconnec/io~iand cerebral fz~~zctto~z. Springfield, IL: Thomas. Pp. 239259. Moscov~rc~ M.. (1976) O n interpreting data regardip the h uistic competence and performance of the right hemisphere: a reply to Selnes. Bruin an%langzrage, 3, 590-599. R A S T A ~ EM. R , l?, &DELL,C. (1987a) Simple visual versus lexical decision vocal reaction times of stuttering and normal subjects. Jozrrnal of Fltiency Disorders, 12, 63-69. R A S T A ~ E RM. . I?. &DELL.C. (1987b) Vocal reaction times of stuttering subjects to tachistosco ically presented concrete and abstract nouns: a closer look at cerebral dominance an!language processing. Jor,rnal o/Speech and Hearing Re.~earch,30, 306-310. 130 M . K. RAMI, ET AL. RASTATTER, M. I?, & DELL,C. (1988) Reading reaction times of stuttering and nonstuttering subjecrs to unilaterally presented concrete and abstract words. ]orrmal of Fluency Disorders, 13, 319-329. RASTAITER,M. P., DELL,C. W., MCGUIRE,R., &LOREN,C. (1987) Vocal reaction rimes to unilaterally presented concrete and abstract nouns: some evidence for differential right hemisphere semantic processing. Cortex, 23, 135-145. ~ T A T ~ E R M. . I?, & LOREN,C. (1988) Vocal reaction rimes to tachistoscopically icesenred high- and low-Ere uency verbs: some evidence for selective minor hemisphere ngulsclcs analysis. ~erceptrr>and Motor Skillr, 66. SO)-810. &TATTER, M. I?, LOREN,C., & COLCORD,R (1987) Visual coding strategies and hemisphere dominance characreristics of stutterers. Jozrrnai of Fluency Disorders, 12, 305-31.5. RASTATTER, M. I?, &MCGUIRE,R. A. (1990) Some effects of advanced aging on the visual-language processing capacity of the left and right hemispheres: evidence from unilateral tachistoscopic viewing. lorrrnal of Speech and Hearrng Research, 33, 134-140. RASTATTER, M. I?, MCGUIRE,R. A., &LOREN.C. (1988) Linguistic encoding dominance in stutt e r i n 5 some evidence for temporal and cqualitative processing differences. ]ournal of Fizrency rsorders, 13, 215-224. RASTAITER,M. I?, &STUART,A. (1995) Hemispheric picrure-naming hierarchies in stuttering subjects. Perceptual and Mofor Skills, 81, 899-908. THORNDIKE, E. L., & LORCE,1. (1944) The teachers' zuord book of 30,000 words. New Yock: Columbia Univer. ZAIDEL,E. (1983) Disconnection s ndrome as a model for laterality effects in the normal brain. Ln J. B. HeUige (Ed.), Lrebrol hemirphere asymmetry: method, theory and application. New York: Praeger. Pp. 95-151. Accepted ]tine 13, 2000.