ISSN 2312-5705 Volume 1 Issue 2, December 2014 MJHS MALTA JOURNAL OF HEALTH SCIENCES FACULTY OF HEALTH SCIENCES www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Malta Journal of Health Sciences - Journal of the Faculty of Health Sciences https://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs/ mjhs@um.edu.mt Editorial Board Chairperson Professor Angela Xuereb Dean, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Malta, MSD 2080, Malta. angela.a.xuereb@um.edu.mt Editor-in-Chief Dr Daniela Gatt Department of Communication Therapy, University of Malta, MSD 2080, Malta. mjhs@um.edu.mt Associate Editors Dr Stephen Lungaro-Mifsud Department of Physiotherapy stephen.lungaro-mifsud@um.edu.mt Dr Josianne Scerri Department of Nursing josianne.scerri@um.edu.mt Dr Victoria Sultana Department of Nursing victoria.sultana@um.edu.mt Dr Vasilis Valdramidis Department of Food Studies & Env. Health vasilis.valdramidis@um.edu.mt Dr Francis Zarb Department of Radiography francis.zarb@um.edu.mt Advisors Dr Rita Borg Xuereb, Department of Midwifery Dr Sandra Buttigieg, Department of Health Services Management Dr Cynthia Formosa, Department of Podiatry Prof. Carmel J. Caruana, Department of Medical Physics Mr René Mifsud, Department of Occupational Therapy Copy Editors Web Administrators Dr Stephen Lungaro-Mifsud Dr Vasilis Valdramidis Dr Francis Zarb Ms Juanita Briffa juanita.briffa@um.edu.mt Mr Nicolai Schembri nicolai.schembri@um.edu.mt Aim and scope The Malta Journal of Health Sciences is a peer-reviewed, open access publication that promotes the sharing and exchange of knowledge in Health Sciences. It provides a platform for novice and established researchers to share their findings, insights and views within an inter-professional context. The Journal originates within the Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Malta. The Malta Journal of Health Sciences disseminates research on a broad range of allied health disciplines. It publishes original research papers, review articles, short communications, commentaries, letters to the editor and book reviews. The readership of the journal consists of academics, practitioners and trainee health professionals across the disciplines of Applied Biomedical Science, Audiology, Communication Therapy, Community Nursing, Environmental Health, Food Science, Health Services Management, Medical Physics, Mental Health Nursing, Midwifery, Nursing, Occupational Therapy, Physiotherapy, Podiatry and Radiography. Submitted manuscripts undergo independent blind peer review, typically by two reviewers with relevant expertise. All manuscripts are reviewed as rapidly as possible and an editorial decision is generally reached within approximately two months of submission. Authors of manuscripts that require revisions will have two weeks to submit their revised manuscripts. No manuscript that has already been published or is under consideration for publication elsewhere will be considered. Types of manuscripts Review articles will present a substantial survey with an adequate historical perspective of the literature in an area of Health Sciences. The manuscript should not exceed the limit of 5,000 words. Research papers should be manuscripts comprising complete reports of original, scientifically sound research. They must contribute new knowledge, be prepared for a wide readership and should not exceed 4,000 words. Short communications should be brief reports of scientifically sound research, but of limited scope e.g. limited amount of data, that contribute new knowledge. They should not exceed 2,000 words. Commentaries should be focused and opinionated articles on any subject within the scope of the Journal. These articles should be related to a contemporary issue, such as recent research findings. They should not exceed 2,000 words. Letters to the editor should provide further debate on a topic addressed by a paper published in the Journal. Authors of the manuscript will be sent an edited copy of the letter and they will have the right to reply. They should not exceed 1,000 words. Book reviews should be timely and objective. They should discuss and assess the main ideas and major objectives of the book and how effectively these are accomplished. Finally, constructive comments about the strengths and weaknesses of the book should be presented. They should not exceed 1,000 words. The manuscript lengths cited above do not include the Abstract and References. Instructions for authors Prior to manuscript submission, it should be ensured that the following guidelines are adhered to. Manuscripts not conforming to these guidelines may be returned. Submission of Manuscripts Manuscripts are to be emailed to the Editor-in-Chief on mjhs@um.edu.mt General guidelines All papers must be written in correct and consistent English (UK). The manuscript, including references, tables and figure legends, should be typed in double line spacing, with margins at least 25 mm on each side. Text should be standard 10 or 12 pt. Author names and/or affiliations should not appear anywhere in the manuscript pages or images in order to ensure blind peer review. Continuous line numbers should be added to the whole document (in Microsoft Word: View menu → Print Layout → Layout → Text Layout → Line Numbers). Title page: The full title page should also be submitted as a separate document and should include the following: Title of paper Abbreviations should not be used in the title. Submission category This should be specified as full research paper, book review etc. Authors Full names of all authors are to be provided, followed by qualifications, affiliations and addresses. The corresponding author should be indicated and full details, including email and mailing addresses and telephone numbers, should be provided. Running Head A running headline of not more than 40 characters should be included at the top of the page. Blind title page: A blind title page giving only the title without author details should also be submitted. Abstract: The abstract should be an accurate summary of the paper presented in one paragraph (not exceeding 300 words). The abstract should: (i) indicate the specific purpose of the article; (ii) describe the methodology used to achieve the purpose; (iii) present the findings including key statistics (this section may be omitted for articles that are not data-based); (iv) present the conclusions based on the data provided; (v) highlight the novelty of the work. The abstract should not include references. Keywords: Immediately after the abstract, a list of up to 6 key words is to be included. Selected words and phrases should relate directly to the content of your work. N.B. The title, abstract and keywords should be chosen carefully as they determine the ease with which readers access the article through online search engines. Main text: The article text should consist of the sections outlined below, in the same order. Results or Findings and Discussion could also be combined. Repetitions between sections should be avoided. Abbreviations and acronyms may be used where appropriate but must always be defined at their first mention. Introduction Briefly describe the purpose of your work, explain its importance and provide a relevant theoretical background. Methods Describe the research design, the materials and/or participants and the methods used for data collection. Sufficient detail should be provided to allow reproduction of the study design. Results or Findings Present results in a clear and logical sequence. If tables are used, do not duplicate the data in the text but describe trends and main points. Discussion This should provide an interpretation of the results without repeating them. Conclusions The main conclusions of the paper are to be presented in this section. Acknowledgements (if relevant) Collate acknowledgements in a separate section at the end of the article before the references. N.B. Please refer to the section on Ethical Guidelines for more information. References Authors are responsible for the accuracy of references. All references must appear both in the text and the reference list. References should follow the Harvard referencing system. In the text, the information required is: the surname of the authors; the year of publication; the page number (direct quotes only). Commas are used to separate information within parentheses e.g. (author’s name, year of publication). Full stops are always used after the abbreviation p. for page number e.g. p.34 (or pp. for multiple pages). If the citation is at the end of the sentence, it is considered to be part of the sentence, so the full stop is placed after the closing parenthesis. Examples Book (1 author) Neville, C. (2010) The Complete Guide to Referencing and Avoiding Plagiarism. 2nd Ed. Maidenhead: Open University Press. In text: Neville (2010) argues that... Book (2 to 3 authors) Bradbury, I., Boyle, J. & Morse, A. (2002) Scientific Principles for Physical Geographers. Harlow: Prentice Hall. In text: Bradbury, Boyle and Morse (2002)... Book (4 or more authors) Reece, J.B., Urry, L.A., Cain, N.L., Wasserman, S.A., Minorsky, P.V. & Jackson, R.B. (2012) Campbell Biology. 9th Ed. London: Benjamin Cummings. In text: (Reece et al., 2012).... Journal article (printed) Trefts, K. & Blacksee, S. (2000) Did you hear the one about Boolean Operators? Incorporating comedy into the library induction. Reference Services Review, 28(4), pp.369-378. In text: Trefts and Blacksee (2000) argue that.... Journal article (electronic/online) Wilson, J. (1995) Enter the Cyberpunk librarian: future directions in cyberspace. Library Review. [Online] Emerald Database 44(8), pp.63-72. Available from: http://www.emeraldinsight.com. [Accessed: 30th January 2012]. In text Wilson (1995) argues that..... Website BBC News.(2008)Factory gloom worst since 1980. [Online] Available from:http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/business/7681569.stm. [Accessed: 19th June 2012]. References should be arranged first alphabetically and then further sorted chronologically if necessary. More than one reference from the same author(s) in the same year must be identified by the letters “a”, “b”, “c” etc., placed after the year of publication. Tables should be referred to specifically in the text of the article and provided in the same document. They should be in editable MS Word format. Number tables consecutively with Arabic numerals (1, 2, 3 etc.), in the order in which they appear in the text. Each table should have a short descriptive title, be self-explanatory and not duplicate data given in the text or figures. Ensure that all columns and rows are properly aligned. Include horizontal rules at the top and bottom of a table and one below the column headings. If a column heading encompasses two or more subheadings, then the main headings and subheadings should be separated by a single short rule. No other rules should be included, neither horizontal nor vertical. Appropriate space should be used to separate columns. Rows should be double-spaced. A table may have footnotes if necessary. Each footnote should begin on a new line. Abbreviations in tables should be defined in footnotes even if defined in the text or in a previous table. Figures should be referred to specifically in the text of the paper. Number figures consecutively using Arabic numerals and avoid repeating material in the text. Abbreviations used in figures should be defined in the caption. Image files should be supplied in EPS, TIFF, PDF, PPT or JPEG format, with each figure uploaded separately and numbered. Unless essential to the content of the article, all illustrations should be supplied in black and white with no colour contained within them. Files should be saved at the appropriate dpi (dots per inch) for the type of graphic - definitely not less than the typical screen value of 72 dpi for both the vertical and horizontal directions. Lower resolutions will not be usable. Save line drawings at 800 dpi or 1200 dpi for fine line work and 300 dpi for halftone and colour work. The image should be cropped to show just the relevant area, while the amount of white space around the illustration should be kept to a minimum. Supply illustrations at the size they are to be printed, usually 76 mm wide (single column of text) or, for especially large figures, 161 mm wide (two columns of text). All annotations should be included within the images supplied. Participant identity must be obscured. Additional points to note: (i) do not put a box around graphs, diagrams or other artwork; (ii) avoid background gridlines unless these are essential (e.g. confidence limits); (iii) ensure that lettering is appropriately sized and corresponds to 8 or 9 pt when printed; (iv) include all units of measurement on axes; (v) all lines (e.g. graph axes) should have a minimum width of 1/4 pt (0.1 mm), otherwise they will not print; 1 pt weight is preferable; (vi) avoid using tints (solid black and white or variations of crosshatching are preferred). Any tints that are used must be at a minimum 5% level to print (but do not use too high a tint as it may print too dark); (vii) do not use three-dimensional histograms when the addition of a third dimension gives no further information. Appendices are discouraged if the material can be included in the main text. If an appendix is necessary e.g. mathematical calculations that would disrupt the text, it should be supplied as a separate file. If more than one appendix are included, these should be identified using different letters. An appendix may contain references. Appendices must be referred to in the main text. Footnotes should be used sparingly. If used, they should be numbered consecutively throughout the paper using superscript Arabic numbers. Do not include footnotes in the reference list. Peer review process All manuscripts are pre-screened by the Editor-in-Chief (or Editorial Board member) upon submission. Priority is given to articles that will help allied health professionals to make better decisions, increase pertinent knowledge base and support high quality research. Immediate manuscript rejection by the Malta Journal of Health Sciences is expected in the following instances: (i) study does not have a theoretical or evidence-based argument for the relevance of the work to allied health practice; (ii) manuscript does not contain sufficiently new or important information and therefore does not offer a significant contribution to the literature; (iii) reliability study does not include a discussion of the influence that the findings will have in the field; (iv) study evaluates a new test without a sound comparison to current tests; (v) study has serious flaws in the Methods section; (vi) authors did not obtain ethics approval from a properly constituted ethics committee, where appropriate. Manuscripts that qualify for review are evaluated by at least two experts as appropriate. A double blind review process is adopted. After the reviewers receive a paper from the editor, they read it closely and provide individual critiques in which they: (i) comment on the validity of the science, identifying scientific errors and evaluating the design and methodology used; (ii) judge its significance by evaluating the importance of the findings; (iii) determine the originality of the work based on how much it advances the field; (iv) recommend that the manuscript is rejected or accepted with minor revision; major revision will entail resubmission and further review. The Editor-in-Chief/Editorial Board member reads the manuscript and the reviewers’ comments, makes his/her own comments, and provides guidance to the Editorial Board on the suitability of the manuscript for publication. The Editor-in-Chief does not need to heed this recommendation. The Editorial Board will then decide on the content of the feedback letter to the author/s. Ethical guidelines The Malta Journal of Health Sciences adheres to the following ethical guidelines for research and publication. Authorship The definition of authorship is based on the following criteria: (i) significant contribution of authors to the conception and design of the research, data collection or analysis and interpretation of data; (ii) drafting and/or critically reviewing the manuscript for its academic content; (iii) providing final approval of the version to be submitted (or resubmitted) for publishing. Authors are expected to meet all three criteria. Contributors who do not qualify under the authorship criteria should be included in the Acknowledgments section. Acknowledgments Any acknowledgements should be included at the end of the article, prior to the Declaration of Conflicting Interests (if applicable) and your References. Any sources of funding must also be acknowledged under a separate heading entitled Funding, directly after the Acknowledgments section. Funding acknowledgments should be in the form of a sentence as shown below, with the funding agency written in full, followed by the grant number in square brackets: This work was supported by XXX [grant number xxx]. Declaration of conflicting interests Authors are expected to disclose any associations that might pose a conflict of interest in connection with the submitted manuscript. All sources of funding or products must be included in the Acknowledgements section of the manuscript. Originality of work Submission of a manuscript implies that the work described: (i) has not been published before (except in the form of an abstract or academic thesis), and (ii) is not under consideration for publication elsewhere. If the manuscript is accepted for publication, it cannot be published in the same form elsewhere, in print or electronically, in English or in any other language, without the written consent of the copyright-holder. Permissions When previously published material (e.g. tables, photographs or illustrations) are included in submitted manuscripts, the authors are required to obtain a written statement from the copyright holder giving permission for reproduction. A copy of the permission obtained must be submitted to the Editor-in-Chief of this journal. Author misconduct and article withdrawal As a policy, submitted articles are checked for plagiarism using duplication detecting software. The Editor-in-Chief reserves the right to reject any manuscript where it is suspected there is unethical conduct during a research project, or in the manuscript authoring or content. Studies involving human and animal subjects (details) Research undertaken with human and animal subjects must have been carried out in accordance with relevant internationally accepted guidelines such as the Declaration of Helsinki for clinical research involving humans (http://www.wma.net/en/30publications/10policies/b3/ index.html), recommendations by the International committee of Medical Journal Editors (http://www.icmje.org), the British Educational Research Association ethical guidelines (http://www.bera.ac.uk/publications/Ethical%20Guidelines) and the EU Directive 2010/63/EU for research on animals (http://ec.europa.eu/environment/chemicals/lab_animals/ legislation_en.htm). Ethical approval Ethical approval from a research ethics committee and/or institutional review board is usually required and details regarding the board providing approval should be listed in the Methods section of the manuscript. Publishing policy Copyright on the published article will be retained by the author/s. Authors grant the Malta Journal of Health Scienceslicense to publish the article and identify itself as the original publisher. In addition, authors may allow any third party the right to use the article as long as its original authors, citation details and publisher are identified, save for commercial purposes. Individual users may access, download, copy, display and redistribute to colleagues open access Malta Journal of Health Sciences articles solely for non-commercial and non-promotional purposes. Table of Contents Malta Journal of Health Sciences Volume 1 - Issue 2 (December 2014) Editorial 28 Editorial Daniela Gatt Regular Articles 29 Key environmental stress biomarker candidates for the optimisation of chemotherapy treatment of leukaemia Review Article Eirini G. Velliou, Susana Brito Dos Santos, Maria Fuentes-Garí, Ruth Misener, Eleni Pefani, Nicki Panoskaltsis, Athanasios Mantalaris and Efstratios N. Pistikopoulos 35 Degree of burnout among Maltese podiatrists working in two different settings Research Article Trista Agius and Cynthia Formosa 40 The effectiveness of social communication groups in mainstream schools Research Article Tiziana Baldacchino and Kristina Agius 54 The lived experience of persons with an amputation Research Article Chanelle Grech and Roberta Farrugia Debono 60 Speech and language skills of Maltese children with bilateral cochlear implants: three case studies Research Article Martina Galdes and Daniela Gatt Malta Journal of Health Sciences http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs DOI: http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/EDITLANG.2.28 Editorial Daniela Gatt Editor-in-Chief, Malta Journal of Health Sciences As we launch Issue 2 of the Malta Journal of Health Sciences (MJHS), I would like to share with you some thoughts on one particular topic which I hold very much at heart, namely children’s language development. In particular, I would like to highlight the importance of social interaction in helping children to develop into effective communicators. The complex process of language learning is grounded in the child’s social experiences. In the months preceding the emergence of language, babies thrive on the attention, smiles, language input and physical contact of their caregivers. Early communicative exchanges drive them to channel their reflexive actions and vocalisations into purposeful ones. In early language learning, the child depends on social interactions with significant others to be able to forge links between the speech sounds and meanings they receive in their input. Language needs to be experienced naturally, as it is used to identify and describe entities, persons, objects and experiences in daily settings. Interactive experiences help the child to crack the language code and realise the value of language in enabling a wide range of communicative functions. As children’s language capacity expands, communicative exchanges allow them to practise available language skills and develop new ones. Experiencing the communicative potential of language is priceless for language-learning children. It is not merely a question of exposing the child to the ambient language. While technology may assist the child’s language learning, it is no substitute for oneto-one interactions in encouraging the uptake of language. Strikingly, children are extremely adept at using technological devices and applications, to the extent that they often become engrossed in them and ignore other forms of input. Although such devices can be useful and educational in their own right, they surely cannot make up for the rich language input that may be derived from meaningful one-to-one or group interactions. These not only support language development but also assist the learning of social rules for communication. The sheer beauty and effectiveness of simple, face-to-face exchanges tends to be under-estimated. Communicative exchanges in everyday contexts provide children with the necessary tools for learning language and developing social skills. Through social interactions, infants and toddlers learn the basic components of language and test their newly-acquired skills in real-life settings. Involvement in social exchanges enables older children to learn the rules for social language use and to practise organising multiple thoughts in a coherent sequence prior to verbalising them. Participation in conversations equips them with skills for understanding and using shades of humour and sarcasm, among others. Importantly, they also develop an appreciation of others’ perspectives and emotions, while developing their own. These are but a few of the advantages that may be derived from encouraging children’s involvement in social exchanges. In today’s day and age, isolation from face-to-face communication has become acceptable. However, we cannot afford to have our children detach themselves from social exchanges. To reap the full benefits of the technological facilities available today, they can be focused on and explored in interactional exchanges. Children can be encouraged to share, describe and discuss their favourite gadgets, programmes and applications. This should go a long way in encouraging them to appreciate the social dimension of language and become truly effective communicators. Having aired my views on the importance of social engagement for children’s language and communication development, I now invite you to explore the articles in this issue. The diversity of topics should make for interesting and intriguing reading. I trust that these papers will spur us on to engage in further health science research. Malta Journal of Health Sciences Online http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs DOI: http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/CHEMOLEUK.2.29 Review paper KEY ENVIRONMENTAL STRESS BIOMARKER CANDIDATES FOR THE OPTIMISATION OF CHEMOTHERAPY TREATMENT OF LEUKAEMIA Eirini G. Velliou1 , Susana Brito Dos Santos2 , Maria Fuentes-Garí2,3 , Ruth Misener4 , Eleni Pefani3 , Nicki Panoskaltsis5 , Athanasios Mantalaris2 , Efstratios N. Pistikopoulos3 1 Department of Chemical and Process Engineering, Faculty of Engineering and Physical Sciences, University of Surrey, Guildford, Surrey, GU2 7XH, U.K. 2 Biological Systems Engineering Laboratory (BSEL), Department of Chemical Engineering, Imperial College London, South Kensington Campus, London, U.K. 3 Centre for Process Systems Engineering (CPSE), Department of Chemical Engineering, Imperial College London, South Kensington Campus, London, U.K. 4 Department of Computing, Imperial College London, South Kensington Campus, London, U.K. 5 Department of Haematology, Imperial College London, Northwick Park and St. Mark’s Campus, London, U.K. Abstract. The impact of fluctuations of environmental parameters such as oxygen and starvation on the evolution of leukaemia is analysed in the current review. These fluctuations may occur within a specific patient (in different organs) or across patients (individual cases of hypoglycaemia and hyperglycaemia). They can be experienced as stress stimuli by the cancerous population, leading to an alteration of cellular growth kinetics, metabolism and further resistance to chemotherapy. Therefore, it is of high importance to elucidate key mechanisms that affect the evolution of leukaemia under stress. Potential stress response mechanisms are discussed in this review. Moreover, appropriate cell biomarker candidates related to the environmental stress response and/or further resistance to chemotherapy are proposed. Quantification of these biomarkers can enable the combination of macroscopic kinetics with microscopic information, which is specific to individual patients and leads to the construction of detailed mathematical models for the optimisation of chemotherapy. Due to their nature, these models will be more accurate and precise (in comparison to available macroscopic/black box models) in the prediction of responses of individual patients to treatment, as they will incorporate microscopic genetic and/or metabolic information which is patient-specific. Keywords: leukaemia, environmental stress, hypoxia, autophagy, chemotherapy, cellular biomarkers Correspondence to: E. G. Velliou (e.velliou@surrey.ac.uk) Received: 24.07.2014 - Revised: 25.09.2014 - Accepted: 14.10.2014 - Published: 17.12.2014 c 2014 The authors 1 Introduction Leukaemia is a severe cancer of the haematopoietic system characterised by the incapability of blood progenitors to mature normally, leading to the accumulation of immature white blood cells, the so-called blasts, in the bone marrow (BM) (Beutler et al., 2001). Alternatively, this disease can be viewed as the formation of an abnormal haematopoietic tissue, the initiation of which is the result of the function of a small amount of Leukaemic Stem Cells (LSCs) (Passegué et al., 2003). Depending on the decrease of the normal blood cell populations, the disease symptoms can consist of fatigue, haemorrhage, infections and fever. The health condition of the patient depends on the amount of normal blood cells compared to the amount of those that are leukaemic. According to Cancer Research U.K. (2014), 8,616 people in the U.K. were diagnosed with leukaemia in 2011, with 4,503 deaths from this disease occurring in 2010. Moreover, 82,300 new cases of leukaemia were diagnosed in 2012 within the European Union. Leukaemia can be divided into different types, depending on the haematopoietic lineage in which the proliferation disorder occurs. More specifically, myeloid leukaemia occurs in the myeloid lineage and lymphocytic leukaemia occurs in the lymphoid lineage. Depending on the speed of evolution of the disease, leukaemia can be divided into acute, whereby the number of blasts increases rapidly, leading to a faster disease evolution, and chronic i.e. the progress of the disease is slower as there is production of partly mature but not fully functional white blood cells. Based on this categorisation, the following four general types of leukaemia can be found: Acute Myeloid Leukaemia (AML), Chronic Myeloid Leukaemia (CML), Acute Lymphocytic Leukaemia (ALL) and Chronic Lymphocytic Leukaemia (CLL). Acute Myeloid Leukaemia (AML) is one of the most aggressive types of leukaemia. According to Cancer Research U.K. (2014), approximately 2,921 cases of AML occurred in 2011 in the U.K. Key environmental stress biomarker candidates for the optimisation of chemotherapy treatment of leukaemia AML is a type of leukaemia which is characterised by an accumulation of immature blasts in the myeloid lineage and, as a consequence, insufficient blood cell production. These blasts have low proliferation capability. However, a small population of cells with higher proliferation capacity and self-renewal potential, the LSCs, are the key component for maintenance of the disease (Bonnet & Dick, 1997). AML is usually the result of somatic mutations in either a pluripotent haematopoietic stem cell or in a slightly differentiated progenitor cell, leading to a deregulation and/or inhibition of normal haematopoiesis due to space restrictions and inhibitory and clonal factors specific to the disease (Lowenberg, Downing & Burnett, 1999; Lichtman, 2001; Panoskaltsis, Reid & Knight, 2003). Chronic Myeloid Leukaemia (CML), alternatively known as Chronic Granulocytic Leukaemia (CGL), is a stem cell disease of the myeloid lineage - characterised by an exaggerated granulocytosis, anaemia, granulocytic immaturity, basophilia and splenomegaly. There is an extreme cellular accumulation in the BM. From a genetic point of view, 90% of cases show a reciprocal translocation between chromosomes 9 and 22, the so-called Philadelphia (Ph) chromosome translocation. In many cases, the disease can progress into a very high speed phase, resembling AML (Lichtman, 2001). CML is a rather rare cancer of the blood. According to Cancer Research U.K. (2014), 675 patients were diagnosed with CML in 2011 in the U.K. Acute Lymphocytic Leukaemia (ALL) (alternatively known as Acute Lymphoblastic Leukaemia) is a type of leukaemia which mostly affects children. Approximately 4,000 cases of ALL are reported on an annual basis in the U.S.A., of which two thirds are children (Pui & Evans, 2006). According to Cancer Research U.K. (2014), 654 cases of ALL were diagnosed in 2011 in the U.K. ALL is a leukaemia in which the cellular deregulation occurs in the lymphoid lineage of the haematopoietic system. More specifically, an abnormality in the lymphocytes takes place, leading to an accumulation of non-functional white blood cells (blasts) in the BM as a result of abnormal cellular proliferation, blocking of cellular differentiation and increased resistance to apoptosis i.e. cell death (Pui, 2009). Chronic Lymphocytic Leukaemia (CLL) is a type of leukaemia which is characterised by an accumulation of immature lymphocytes (of the B-cell lineage) in the human BM, peripheral blood and the lymphoid tissues (Kipps, 2001). The progress of CLL is much slower than that of ALL and it affects older adults i.e. people over 51 years of age. According to the National Health Service (NHS) U.K., about 2,400 people in the U.K. are diagnosed with CLL on an annual basis and the disease affects 2.7 persons per 100,000 in the U.S.A. (Kipps et al., 2001). According to Cancer Research U.K. (2014), 3,233 people were diagnosed with CLL in the U.K. in 2011. The most common treatment for all types of leukaemia is chemotherapy (Cancer Research U.K., 2014). A variety of chemotherapy drugs are generally used, depending on the type of leukaemia. The most commonly used drugs for treatment of leukaemia are cytarabine (cytosine arabinose or ara-C), which is an antimetabolite targeting to block the DNA/RNA replication by attacking the cells that are in the S-phase of the cell cycle, and the anthracycline drugs (such as fludarabine) which attack cells that are in the G1-phase of the cell cycle (American Cancer Society, 2013). Current chemotherapy treatment protocols are designed on the basis of pre-clinical animal experiments and empirical clinical trials, as well as the acquired experience of specialist physicians. The design parameters for these protocols consist of the patient BM aspirate examination (blasts percentage, immunophenotype, cytogenetic and molecular analysis) and physiological patient characteristics (height, weight) for the normalisation of the dose applied on the body surface area (BSA). http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/CHEMOLEUK.2.29 2 30 Towards a Personalised Chemotherapy Treatment: integrating in vivo, in vitro and in silico knowledge As mentioned in the previous section, traditional clinical diagnosis and further treatment of leukaemia focuses on each patient’s clinical symptoms and signs, as well as characteristics (such as sex and family history) and laboratory imaging evaluation. This process is a reactive approach to the disease, initiating after the disease symptoms appear. Moreover, in the past, drug development by pharmaceutical industries was based on empirical observations. However, nowadays, with significant progress constantly taking place in the areas of genomics, proteomics and metabolomics, it is believed that specific information related to the genetic characteristics and proteomic and metabolomic profile of an individual patient could be used for tailored medical care, or personalised medicine. Some of the challenges in the delivery of personalised medicine lie in (a) the fidelity and validity of current experimental, i.e. in vitro, systems used to investigate human disease, (b) the integration of patient-specific and disease-specific datasets i.e. in silico and (c) the application of these models in clinical practice to identify simple targets and more efficient, yet less toxic, therapies for a specific condition, i.e. in vivo. Therefore, ’closing the loop’ from in vivo to in vitro and in silico is a first step towards optimisation and consequently, personalisation of chemotherapy treatment . Moving in that direction, an appropriate platform has been developed at the Centre for Process Systems Engineering at Imperial College London, consisting of three main blocks i.e. in vivo-in vitro-in silico which, upon completion, will allow the prediction of optimal drug dosages for each individual, based on specific (personalised) characteristics (Velliou et al., 2014a). The main advantage of this platform is the dynamic interaction of its three robust building blocks. As opposed to existing computational or experimental studies, our platform consists of the interaction of both: data from our in vitro platform are used as an input for the development of mathematical tools. These mathematical tools are further validated with patient data. Figure 1 describes the different parts of the integrated platform that is currently being designed at the Centre for Process Systems Engineering at Imperial College London (Velliou et al., 2014a). Development and optimisation of the in vitro and in silico blocks will eventually bridge the gap between laboratory experimentation, mathematical modelling and in vivo optimal chemotherapy treatment for a specific individual. A very innovative in vitro tool has been developed and is being used for the ex vivo long-term patient sample cultivation at the Centre for Process Systems Engineering (Mortera-Blanco et al., 2010). This tool is a three-dimensional highly porous polyurethane-based matrix coated with collagen type I, therefore mimicking the porosity as well as the extracellular matrix present in the human BM. This three-dimensional system allows the longterm (up to 6 weeks) cultivation of patient cells in an environment that mimics the in vivo one. An in silico tool, or mathematical model, which enables the estimation/design of chemotherapy protocols for a specific individual based on the personalisation of the drug schedule has been developed by Pefani et al. (2013). This model consists of a Pharmacokinetic (PK) and a Pharmacodynamic (PD) part. The PK part consists of mass balances of the drug distribution in different body organs such as the heart, the liver and the BM. The PD part calculates the effect of the drug on both normal and abnormal cells in the BM, which is the location of the tumour. The model input is the treatment inflow, which is calculated based on the drug administration route and the injection rate. The latter is a function of body characteristics such as height and weight. http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Key environmental stress biomarker candidates for the optimisation of chemotherapy treatment of leukaemia Figure 1: Towards optimisation/personalisation of chemotherapy for leukaemia treatment (adapted from Velliou et al., 2014a). For cases in which the initial drug dose proposed clinically failed to eliminate the cancerous population, an optimisation process was followed in order to re-determine the optimal drug dose on the basis of clinically relevant constraints. Remarkably, the model output treatments suggest drug dosages similar to those used clinically but, for example, the scheduling is different. The optimised treatment would have much better outcomes with respect to the elimination of the tumour (Pefani et al., 2014). Furthermore, a more detailed cell cycle model based on the work of GarcíaMünzer et al. (2013, 2014) has been developed, consisting of a multi-stage population balance model (MS-PBM) (Fuentes-Garí et al., 2014). This model is distributed on cell cycle progressrelated events i.e. cyclin or DNA expression (different cyclins are produced at different phases of the cell cycle and DNA is produced at the S-phase of the cell cycle). Cell cycle kinetics are tracked not only across different cell cycle phases but also within each phase, thus allowing a more efficient monitoring of the evolution of the leukaemic population throughout the cell cycle. The latter is of high importance for chemotherapy optimisation since, as mentioned previously, most chemotherapeutic drugs are cell cyclespecific, targeting cells that are present in specific phases of the cell cycle. This PBM has been successfully validated with experimental data (Fuentes-Garí, 2014). Moving towards the delivery of optimal chemotherapy protocols for each individual, it is important to understand and further incorporate in the mathematical models the impact of environmental stress. More specifically, the effect of alterations of environmental parameters (oxygen, temperature, nutrients) on the evolution and further response of leukaemia to chemotherapy have to be incorporated (Velliou et al., 2014a, b). Fluctuations of those parameters can take place either across different patients or within the same patient. For example, glucose levels in the blood can differ across a diabetic, a hypoglycaemic and a normal patient. During chemotherapy treatment, it is possible that a patient may have fever; high body temperature can be experienced as a temperature shock by the cancerous population. Oxygen levels can fluctuate in different body compartments e.g. the BM and peripheral blood. The impact of those environmental factors on the leukaemic evolution is further discussed in the following section. 2.1 Environmental Stress and Leukaemia Fluctuations in the micro-environmental conditions of the BM i.e. oxygen concentration, composition and concentration of nu- http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/CHEMOLEUK.2.29 31 trients such as glucose, cytokines or other growth factors, may be experienced as an environmental stress. As a consequence, these fluctuations can highly affect the normal and abnormal haematopoietic proliferation, metabolic activity as well as drug resistance and further evolution. For example, several researchers have shown that oxidative stress i.e. the increase in the concentration of Reactive Oxygen Species (ROS), leads to activation of survival pathways and is a key factor that promotes progression of cancerous stem cells as well as resistance to chemotherapy (see as examples Adbal Dayem et al., 2010; Fruehauf & Mayskens, 2013; Liu et al., 2009; Lyu et al., 2008). Especially in the case of the abnormal haematopoietic situation of leukaemia, alterations of the oxygen and glucose concentration in the different body compartments e.g. in the BM and the peripheral blood or the liver, and, on the other hand, between patients, i.e. individual cases of hypoglycaemia or hyperglycaemia, may lead to a different stress adaptation of the leukaemic population. The latter will most likely affect the cancer growth and inactivation kinetics, as well as the response to a chemotherapeutic drug in vivo. A variety of research studies have revealed the strong relation between resistance i.e. longer survival and increased proliferation of haematopoietic and/or leukaemic cells and/or resistance to chemotherapy, with (1) oxygen or (2) starvation stress in vitro and/or in vivo. For example, Fecteau et al. (2013) observed an increased in vitro survival of cells from BM aspirates of patients with CLL in 5% O2 compared to 20% O2 . This increased survival under hypoxic conditions was a result of the Mesenchymal Stromal Cells (MSCs’) increased proliferation and the production of soluble pro-survival factors i.e. CXCL12. Interactions between CLL and (increased) MSCs lead to enhanced CLL resistance. Wilkinson, Tome and Briehl (2012) state that chronic oxidative stress may contribute to increased resistance of lymphoma patients to chemotherapy. This is due to the fact that oxidative stress leads to alteration of the mitochondria i.e. release of intermembrane proteins, which leads to an increased permeabilisation of the outer mitochondrial membrane and further resistance to apoptosis. Zhou et al. (2010) pointed out the possible relation between the relapse of AML and increased oxidative stress in vivo. Specifically, parameters related to oxidative stress e.g. activities of adenosine deaminase and xanthine oxidase, antioxidant capacity (T-AOC), levels of human thioredoxin (TRX) and indoleamine 2,3-dioxygenase, as well as expression of specific genes related to oxidative stress, were monitored in patients with AML for a time period between a primary and a relapsed status. Low T-AOC and up-regulated TRX expression led to a relapse of the disease, indicating a strong correlation between oxidative stress and AML development and relapse. Lodi et al. (2011) observed that hypoxia is a key factor that affects metabolic activity i.e. adaptation of phospholytic and glycolytic metabolism, and evolution of KG1a and K562 leukaemic cell lines. Mitochondrial respiration remained unaltered for both cell lines, indicating the ability of these leukaemic cell lines to increase their resistance under oxidative stress. Giuntoli et al. (2011) studied the effect of the level of glucose on the growth and proliferation of K562 cell lines, U937 cell lines or primary CML cells under hypoxic conditions i.e. 0.1% O2 , as well as under normoxia i.e. 21% O2 . Although in general, slower growth was observed for lower glucose concentrations in hypoxia as well as in normoxia, glucose shortage in hypoxia led to increased size of the leukaemic population compared to the normal haematopoietic one. Herst et al. (2011) have pointed out a possible relation between the level of glycolytic metabolism of AML blasts and resistance to chemotherapy. Analysis of 26 BM aspirates showed that AML cells with higher glucose consumption were more tolerant to in vitro apoptosis caused by ATRA and/or ATO drug. http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Key environmental stress biomarker candidates for the optimisation of chemotherapy treatment of leukaemia All previously mentioned studies point out that there is a very strong relation between oxidative or starvation stress and the leukaemic evolution i.e. growth, kinetics or resistance to chemotherapy. Therefore, the possible oxidative and starvation cellular stress should be systematically followed and/or quantified experimentally (in vitro) and eventually further incorporated in mathematical models (in silico). Efficient monitoring of the level of oxidative and metabolic stress and further adaptation can take place via the selection and further quantification of specific biomarker(s) i.e. intracellular molecules, the expression and/or concentration of which may alter depending on the fluctuation of oxygen and glucose in the system. In order to select ‘stress’ biomarker(s), an in-depth investigation of possible mechanisms of oxidative and starvation stress cellular responses is needed (Velliou et al., 2014a). Hereafter, autophagy, a crucial mechanism which is activated in the absence of nutrients and low oxygen levels, is described and potential biomarker molecules related to the cell stress response are summarised. 2.2 (Macro-)Autophagy: the cellular response to metabolic stress and hypoxia Autophagy is a cellular mechanism which aims at the maintenance of homeostasis of a normal cell, via degradation of organelles and cellular components by the lysosomes (Banerji & Gibson, 2012; Choi, Ryter & Levine, 2013; Kongara & Karantza, 2012, Levine & Kroemer, 2008; Lozy & Karantza, 2012). Autophagy is activated as a result of exposure to a stress factor and most probably in the absence of nutrients i.e. glucose starvation, as well under hypoxic conditions (Lum et al., 2005; Scherz-Shouval et al., 2007). Degradation of damaged mitochondria as well as aggregation of proteins and other cellular organelles via the autophagic mechanism protect the cells from apoptotic signalling (Jin & White, 2007; Moore, Allen & Sommerfield, 2006). Autophagy may be important in the regulation of cancer development and in the determination of the response of cancer cells to chemotherapy (Degenhardt et al., 2006; Hippest, O’Toole & Thorburn, 2006; Wilkinson, Tome & Briehl, 2012). Several researchers have shown that autophagy plays a crucial role in maintenance of normal haematopoiesis and function of Haematopoietic Stem Cells (HSCs) (Kundu et al., 2008; Warr et al., 2013). Others have shown that autophagy may lead to an increased resistance to chemotherapy and survival of several cancers, including haematological malignancies. For example, Mortensen, Watchon & Simon (2011) observed that loss of autophagy resulted in loss of normal function of murine HSCs, leading to the expansion of a progenitor cell population in the BM which has as a consequence a severe myeloproliferation. This myeloproliferation strongly resembled human AML, indicating a possible link between maintenance of autophagy and avoidance of malignancies such as AML. Wallington-Beddoe et al. (2011) showed that activation of autophagy (induced by the FTY720 drug) leads to increased survival of ALL cells. 2.3 2.3.1 Biomarker Candidates for the Prediction of the Disease Evolution under Stress (Autophagic) Biomarker candidates For ‘switching on’ and maintenance of the autophagic response, a variety of genes are over-expressed and proteins, mainly kinases, are activated and/or de-activated, depending on whether they have a positive or negative regulatory role in autophagy. Therefore, in order to monitor autophagy in an in vitro system, many different biomarkers of genomic and/or protein level can be considered (see Table 1 for an overview). http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/CHEMOLEUK.2.29 Table 1. leukaemia Biomarker LKBI-AMPK AMPK ULKI P53 PTEN Atg7 HIF mTOR kinase FOXO3A FUMH 32 Stress biomarker candidates for Genomic Protein + + + + + + + + + + A possible candidate-biomarker is the serine threonine kinase ULK1, which is a key initiator of autophagy. Its activation is essential for clearance of cellular mitochondria and ribosomes (Kundu et al., 2008). This kinase is activated under growth factor deprivation and leads to activation of the glycogen synthesis kinase 3 (GSK-3). The latter phosphorylates the acetyltransferase TIP60 which in turn aetylates and activates ULK1 (Lin et al., 2012). Another crucial kinase which is directly related to autophagy is the mTOR kinase which, under normal nutrient concentrations, binds and phosphorylates the ULK1, therefore suppressing autophagy. Under nutrient deprivation, the activation of the P53 gene enables activation of the ULK1 and autophagy (Feng et al., 2005). Another possible candidate biomarker is the Atg7 gene, which is an essential gene for activation of autophagy and further regulation of HSC maintenance (Kundu et al., 2008; Mortensen et al., 2011; Mortensen & Simon, 2010). It has been shown that deleting this gene in murine HSC leads to death as a result of an accumulation of mitochondria and ROS, increased proliferation and DNA damage (Mortensen et al., 2011; Mortensen & Simon, 2010). FOXO3A can also be a possible biomarker candidate. It has been found to have a critical role in autophagy induced in mice in a cytokine-free environment (Warr et al., 2013a). Several researchers have reported a tumour suppressor role of autophagy. More specifically, it has been shown that activation of the AMPK pathway has a suppressor role in AML. AMPK is a protein kinase which regulates protein and energy homeostasis at an intracellular level via autophagic recycling of intracellular components. Practically, AMPK acts as a metabolic sensor of alteration of the intracellular lipid composition and restores energy by maintaining the balance ATP vs AMP, through the LKBI-AMPK activation. LKBI-AMPK is a tumour suppressor in AML (Green et al., 2010). Activation of the P53/PTEN genes has been shown to have a tumour suppressive role as these allow initiation of autophagy via inhibition of the activity of the mTOR kinase (Feng et al., 2005). 2.3.2 (Non-autophagic) Stress biomarker candidates A biomarker related to oxidative stress is the Hypoxia Induced Factor, HIF, which is the central regulator of oxygen homeostasis. More specifically, HIF1-a regulator is overexpressed in many cancer types and HIF proteins mediate cell adaptation to hypoxia (Birner et al., 2000; Talks et al., 2000, Warr & Passagué, 2013b; Zhong et al., 1999). A possible cell biomarker related to starvation stress is the metabolic enzyme fumarate hydratase (FUMH) which converts fumarate to malate. This enzyme highly controls the intracellular levels of fumarate, with silencing of the expression of FUMH leading to fumarate intracellular accumulation (Ratcliffe, 2007). http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Key environmental stress biomarker candidates for the optimisation of chemotherapy treatment of leukaemia 3 Conclusion The impact of environmental stresses such as oxidative and starvation stress on the leukaemic cell evolution i.e. growth and/or resistance to drugs, has been discussed in depth. It is clear that the effect of these factors on the cancer evolution should be taken into consideration for the accurate prediction of optimal chemotherapy protocols for specific individuals. Furthermore, several cellular components that are related to the cell response to the stress have been analysed. Quantitative information on these key biomarkers could serve as an appropriate input for the construction of more detailed predictive models for the in silico description of the leukaemic evolution. More specifically, from a detection/quantification point of view and as a first step, application of techniques such as next generation sequencing (NGS) or deep sequencing will enable the selection of biomarker candidates that could be linked to in vitro drug resistance. These data will enable an accurate model development. As a second step, screening of patients’ genetic profile through NGS in BM biopsies, will enable a cross-link of in vivo with in vitro biomarkers. Consequently, the developed in silico tool will indicate appropriate and different chemotherapy treatment options in a closely personalised manner. Overall, quantification of appropriate (intra-) cellular biomarkers that are related to the leukaemic in vitro kinetics can enable the combination of macroscopic kinetics with microscopic information, leading to the construction of more detailed models. The (micro) cellular information that these models account for will allow a much more accurate prediction of the cancer response, eventually leading to delivery of personalised chemotherapy protocols for the treatment of patients with leukaemia. This review article was compiled as part of the invited seminar by Dr E.G. Velliou titled ‘A framework for the personalisation/optimisation of the treatment of Acute Myeloid Leukaemia (AML)’, which was held at the Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Malta on 4 April 2014. 4 Funding This work is supported by ERC-BioBlood (no. 340719), ERCMobile Project (no. 226462), the EU 7th Framework Programme [MULTIMOD Project FP7/2007-2013, no. 238013] and the Richard Thomas Leukaemia Research Fund. Ruth Misener is further thankful for a Royal Academy of Engineering Research Fellowship. 5 Conflicts of Interest The authors report no conflicts of interest. References Adbal Dayem, A., Choi, H.-Y., Kim, J.-H. & Cho, S.-G. (2010) Role of oxidative stress in stem, cancer, and cancer stem cells. Cancers, 2, pp. 859-884. American Cancer Society (2013) Available from: www.cancer.org [Accessed 24th July 2014]. Banerji, V. & Gibson, S.B. (2012) Targeting metabolism and autophagy in the context of haematologic malignancies. International Journal of Cell Biology, Volume 2012, pp. 1-9. Beutler, E., Coller, B.S., Lichtman, M.A., Kipps, T.J. & Seligsohn, U. (Eds) (2001) Williams Hematology. New York: McGraw-Hill. Birner, P., Schind, M., Obermair, A., Plank, C., Breitenecker, G. & Ober-huber, G. (2000) Overexpression of hypoxiainducible factor 1a is a marker for an unfavorable prognosis in early-stage invasive cervical cancer. Cancer Research, 60, http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/CHEMOLEUK.2.29 33 pp. 4693-4696. Bonnet, D. & Dick, J. (1997) Human acute myeloid leukemia is organized as a hierarchy that originates from a primitive hematopoietic cell. Nature Medicine, 3(7), pp. 730-737. Cancer Research U.K. (2014) Available from: http://www.cancerresearchuk.org. [Accessed 11th June 2014]. Choi, A.M.K., Ryter, S.W. & Levine, B. (2013) Autophagy in human health and disease. The New England Journal of Medicine, 368, pp. 651-552. Degenhardt, K., Mathew, R., Beaudoin, B., Bray, K., Anderson, D., Chen, G., Mukherjee, C., Shi, Y., Gelinas, C., Fan, Y., Nelson, D.A. & White, E. (2006) Autophagy promotes tumour cell survival and restricts necrosis, inflammation and tumori-genesis. Cancer Cell, 10(1), pp. 51-54. Fecteau, J.-F., Messmer, D., Zhang, S., Cui, B., Chen, L. & Kipps, T.J. (2013) Impact of oxygen concentration on growth of mesenchymal stromal cells from the marrow of patients with chronic lymphocytic leukemia. Blood, 121, pp. 971-974. Feng, Z., Zhang, H., Levine, A.J. & Jin, S. (2005) The coordinate regulation of the p53 and mTOR pathways in cells. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science (PNAS), 102(23), pp. 8204-8209. Fruehauf, J.-P. & Meyskens, F.-L. Jr. (2007) Reactive oxygen species: a breath of life or death? Clinical Cancer Research, 13, pp. 89-794. Fuentes-Garí, M., Misener, R., García-Münzer, D.G., Velliou, E., Panoskaltsis, N., Pistikopoulos, E.N. & Mantalaris, A. (2014) Development and experimental validation of cyclin-based population balance model of the cell cycle in leukaemia cell lines. Journal of Tissue Engineering and Regenerative Medicine, 8, pp. 489. García-Münzer, D.G., Kostoglou, M., Georgiadis, M., Pistikopoulos, E.N. & Mantalaris A. (2013) Developing a cyclin blueprint as a tool for mapping the cell cycle in GS-NS0. Biochemical Engineering Journal, 81, pp. 97-107. García-Münzer, D.G., Kostoglou, M., Georgiadis, M.C., Pistikopoulos, E.N. & Mantalaris, A. (2014) A cyclin distributed cell cycle model in GS-NS0. Computer Aided Chemical Engineering, 33, pp. 19-24. Giuntoli, S., Tanturli, M., Gesualdo, F.D., Barbetti, V., Rovida, E. & Sbarba, P.D. (2011) Glucose availability in hypoxia regulates the selection of chronic myeloid leukemia progenitor subsets with different resistance to imatinib-mesylate. Haematologica, 96(2), pp. 204-212. Green, A.S., Chapuis, N., Maciel, T.T., Willems, L., Lambert, M., Arnoult, C., Boyer, O., Bardet, V., Park, S., Foretz, M., Viollet, B., Ifrah, N., Dreyfus, F., Hermine, O., Moura, I.C., Lacombe, C., Mayeux, P., Bouscary, D. & Tamburini, J. (2010) The LKB1/AMPK signaling pathway has tumour suppressor activity in acute myeloid leukemia through the repression of mTOR-dependent oncogenic mRNA translation. Blood, 116, pp. 4262-4273. Herst, P.M., Howman, R.A., Neeson, P.J., Berridge, M.V.& Ritchie D.S. (2011) The level of glycolytic metabolism in acute myeloid leukemia blasts at diagnosis is prognostic for clinical outcome. Journal of Leukocyte Biology, 89, pp. 5155. Hippest, M.M., O’Toole, P.S. & Thorburn, A. (2006) Autophagy in cancer: good, bad or both? Cancer Research, 66, pp. 9349-9351. Jin, S. & White, E. (2007) Role of autophagy in cancer: management of metabolic stress. Autophagy, 3(1), pp. 28-31. Kipps, T.J. (2001) Classification of malignant lymphoid disorders. In E. Beutler, B.S. Coller, M.A. Lichtman, T.J. Kipps & U. Seligsohn (Eds) Williams Hematology. New York: McGrawHill. Kongara, S. & Karantza, V. (2012) The interplay between au- http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Key environmental stress biomarker candidates for the optimisation of chemotherapy treatment of leukaemia tophagy and ROS in tumorigenesis. Frontiers in Oncology, 2(171), pp. 1-13. Kundu, M., Lindsten, T., Yang, C.-Y., Wu, J., Zhao, F., Zhang, J., Selak, M.A., Ney, P.A. & Thompson, C.B. (2008) Ulk1 plays a critical role in the autophagic clearance of mitochondria and ribosomes during reticulocyte maturation. Blood, 112, pp. 1493-1502. Levine, B. & Kroemer, G. (2008) Autophagy in the pathogenesis of disease. Cell, 132(1), pp. 27-42. Lichtman, M.A. (2001) Classification and clinical manifestations of the clonal myeloid disorders. In E. Beutler, B.S. Coller, M.A. Lichtman, T.J. Kipps & Seligsohn, U. (Eds) Williams Hematology. New York: McGraw-Hill. Lin, S-Y., Li, T.Y., Liu, Q., Zhang, C., Li, X., Chen, Y., Zhang, S-M., Lian, G., Liu, Q., Ruan, K., Wang, Z., Zhang, C.S., Chien, K-Y., Wu, J., Li, Q., Han, J. & Lin, S.C. (2012) GSK3-TIP60-ULK1 Signaling pathway links growth factor deprivation to autophagy. Science, pp. 336-477. Liu, R., Li, Z., Bai, S., Zhang, H., Tang, M., Lei, Y., Chen, L., Liang, S., Zhao, Y.-I., Wei, Y. & Huang, C. (2009) Mechanism of cancer cell adaptation to metabolic stress: proteomics identification of a novel thyroid hormone-mediated gastric carcinogenic signaling pathway. Molecular and Cellular Proteomics, 8, pp. 70-85. Lodi, A., Tiziani, S., Khanim, F.L., Drayson, M.T., Gunther, U.L., Bunce, C.M. & Viant, M.R. (2011) Hypoxia triggers major metabolic changes in AML cells without altering indomethacin-induced TCA cycle deregulation. American Chemical Society (ACS ) Chemical Biology, 6, pp. 169-175. Lowenberg, B., Downing, J. & Burnett, A. (1999) Acute Myeloid Leukemia. New England Journal of Medicine, 341(14), pp. 1051-1062. Lozy, F. & Karantza, V. (2012) Autophagy and cancer cell metabolism. Seminars in Cell and Developmental Biology, 23, pp. 395-401. Lum, J., Bauer, D.E., Kong, M., Harris, M.H., Li, C., Lindsten, T. & Thompson, C.B. (2005) Growth factor regulation of autophagy and cell survival in the absence of apoptosis. Cell, 120, pp. 237-248. Lyu, B.N., Ismailov, S.B., Ismailov, B. & Lyu, M.B. (2008) Mitochondrial concept of leukemogenesis: key role of oxygenperoxide effects. Theoretical Biology and Medical Modelling, 5(23), pp. 1-11. Moore, M.N., Allen, J.I. & Somerfield, P.J. (2006) Autophagy: role in surviving environmental stress. Marine Environmental Research, 62, S420-S425. Mortensen, M. & Simon, A.K. (2010) Nonredundant role of Atg7 in mitochondrial clearance during erythroid development. Autophagy, 6, pp. 423-425. Mortensen, M., Soilleux, E.J., Djordjevic, G., Tripp, R., Lutteropp, M., Sadighi-Akha, E., Stranks, A.J., Glanville, J., Knight, S., Jacobsen, S.-E.W., Kamil, R., Kranc, K.R. & Simon, A.K. (2011) The autophagy protein Atg7 is essential for hematopoietic stem cell maintenance. The Journal of Experimental Medicine, 208(3), pp. 455-467. Mortensen, M., Watchon A.S. & Simon, A.K. (2011) Lack of autophagy in the hematopoietic system leads to loss of hematopoietic stem cell function and dysregulated myeloid proliferation. Autophagy, 7(9), pp. 1069-1070. Mortera-Blanco T., Mantalaris, A., Bismarck, A. & Panoskaltsis, N. (2010) Development of a three-dimensional biomimicry of human acute myeloid leukemia ex vivo. Biomaterials, 31, pp. 2243-2251. Panoskaltsis, N., Reid, C.D.L. & Knight, S.C. (2003) Quantification and cytokine production of circulating lymphoid and myeloid cells in acute myelogenous leukemia (AML). Leukemia, 17, pp. 716-730. Passegué, E., Jamieson, C.H.M., Ailles, L.E. & Weissman, I.L. http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/CHEMOLEUK.2.29 34 (2003) Normal and leukemic hematopoiesis: are leukemias a stem cell disorder or a reacquisition of stem cell characteristics? Proceedings of the National Academy of Science (PNAS), 100, 11842-11849. Pefani, E., Panoskaltsis, N., Mantalaris, A., Georgiadis, M.C. & Pistikopoulos, E.N. (2013) Design of optimal patientspecific chemotherapy protocols for the treatment of acute myeloid leukemia (AML). Computers and Chemical Engineering, 57, pp. 187-195. Pefani, E., Panoskaltsis, N., Mantalaris, A., Georgiadis, M.C. & Pistikopoulos, E.N. (2014) Chemotherapy drug scheduling for the induction treatment of patients diagnosed with Acute Myeloid Leukemia. Transactions of Biomedical Engineering Journal, 61(7), pp. 2059-2056. Pui, C.H. (2009) Acute Lymphoblastic Leukaemia: introduction. Seminars in Hematology, 46(1), pp. 1-2. Pui, C.H. & Evans, W.E. (2006) Treatment of acute lymphoblastic leukemia. The New England Journal of Medicine, 354, pp. 166-178. Ratcliffe, P. J. (2007) Fumarate hydratase deficiency and cancer: activation of hypoxia signaling? Cancer Cell, 11, pp. 303305. Scherz-Shouval, E., Shvets, E., Fass, E., Shorer, H., Gil, L. & Elazar, Z. (2008) Reactive oxygen species are essential for autophagy and specifically regulate the activity of Atg4. The EMBO Journal, 26, pp. 1749-2760. Talks, K. L., Turley, H., Gatter, K. C., Maxwell, P. H., Pugh, C. W., Ratcliffe, P. J. & Harris, A. L. (2000) The expression and distribution of the hypoxia-inducible factors HIF-1a and HIF-2a in normal human tissues, cancers, and tumourassociated macrophages. American Journal of Pathology, 157, pp. 411- 421. Velliou, E., Fuentes-Gar’, M., Misener, R., Pefani, E., Rende, M., Panoskaltsis, N., Pistikopoulos, E. N. & Mantalaris A. (2014a) A framework for the design, modeling and optimisation of biomedical systems. Computer Aided Chemical Engineering, 34, pp. 225-236. Velliou, E., Brito Dos Santos, S., Papathanasiou, M., FuentesGar’ M., Misener R., Panoskaltsis, N., Mantalaris, A. & Pistikopoulos, E.N. (2014b) Evolution of an AML model system under oxidative and starvation stress: a comparison between two and three dimensional cultures. Journal of Tissue Engineering and Regenerative Medicine, 8, pp. 383. Wallington-Beddoe, C.T., Hewson, J., Bradstock, K.F. & Bendall, L.J. (2011) FTY720 produces caspase-independent cell death of acute lymphoblastic leukaemia cells. Autophagy, 7(7), pp. 707-715. Warr, M.R., Binnewies, M., Flach, J., Reynaud, D., Garg, T., Malhorta, R., Debnath, J. & Passegu’, E. (2013) FOXO3A directs a protective autophagy program in hematopoietic stem cells. Nature, 494, pp. 323-327. Warr, M.R. & Passegu’, E. (2013) Metabolic makeover for HSCs. Cell Stem Cell, 12, pp. 1-3. Wilkinson, S.T., Tome, M.E. & Briehl, M.M. (2012) Mitochondrial adaptations to oxidative stress confer resistance to apoptosis in lymphoma cells. International Journal of Molecular Sciences, 13, pp. 10212-10228. Zhong, H., Marzo, A. M., Laughner, E., Lim, M., Hilton, D. A., Zagzag, D., Buechler, P., Isaacs, W. B., Semenza, G. L. & Simons, J. W. (1999) Overexpression of hypoxia-inducible factor 1 alpha in common human cancers and their metastases. Cancer Research, 59, pp. 5830’5835. Zhou, F.-L., Zhang, W.-G., Wei, Y.-C., Meng, S., Bai, G.-G., BaiWang, B.-Y., Yang, H.-Y. , Tian, W., Meng, X., Zhang, H. & Chen, S.-P. (2010) Involvement of oxidative stress in the relapse of Acute Myeloid Leukemia. The Journal of Biological Chemistry, 285, pp. 15010-15015. http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Malta Journal of Health Sciences http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs DOI: http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/BURNPOD.2.38 Research paper DEGREE OF BURNOUT AMONG MALTESE WORKING IN TWO DIFFERENT SETTINGS PODIATRISTS Trista Agius1 , Cynthia Formosa2 1 Podiatry Clinic (Primary Health Care), Mosta Health Centre, Mosta, Malta 2 Department of Podiatry, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Malta, Msida, Malta Abstract. The issue of burnout is receiving increasing research attention. However, most of the literature on burnout has focused on the nursing, physiotherapy and occupational therapy professions, with very few studies within the podiatric profession exploring this concept. The purpose of this paper is to compare the degree of burnout among Maltese podiatrists working in two different settings, namely the public health service and private practice. A quantitative non-experimental study, involving a convenience sample, was conducted. The Copenhagen Burnout Inventory (CBI) was used to evaluate levels of burnout in the two participant groups. No statistical difference was found between the overall mean burnout scores for Maltese podiatrists employed in the public health service and those working in private practice (p = 0.067, mean = 48.89 vs 31.84). Although the level of burnout did not differ between podiatrists working within the two different settings, podiatrists working in the public sector in general reported higher mean scores for each subscale of the CBI than podiatrists working in private practice. More research is necessary to build a better understanding of burnout among podiatrists in parallel with other health care professionals, in order to help prevent or alleviate this phenomenon. Keywords: occupational burnout, podiatrists, public health service, private practice, performance, job satisfaction 1 Introduction The literature suggests that burnout is a serious problem which needs to be addressed appropriately (Akroyd, Caison & Adams, 2002; Akgun, Al-Assaf & Bakan, 2008). Burnout emerged as an important concept in the 1970s and continues to do so today (Schaufeli, Leiter & Maslach, 2008). Burnout is regarded as a multidimensional syndrome which comprises three components, namely emotional exhaustion, depersonalisation and reduced personal accomplishment (Brenninkmeijer & Van Yperen, 2003). Burnout syndrome can have deliberate implications for health care professionals, their patients and the organisation (Lloyd & King, 2004). For this reason, research on burnout syndrome is Correspondence to: Cynthia Formosa (cynthia.formosa@um.edu.mt) Received: 08.02.14 - Revised: 16.10.14 - Accepted: 16.10.14 Published: 17.12.14 c 2014 The authors increasing, particularly in the health service professions. There has been a continuous search among researchers to develop improved burnout measures to study this concept. These include the Maslach Burnout Inventory (Schaufeli et al., 1996), the Oldenburg Burnout Inventory (Demerouti et al., 2001) and the more recent Copenhagen Burnout Inventory (CBI) (Kristensen et al., 2005). All these instruments embrace the assumption that the burnout concept comprises all the three components discussed above. Burnout is a distinct kind of occupational stress which generally occurs among health care professionals, mainly due to their continual exposure to patients’ problems, although the point at which burnout begins is unclear (Mandy & Tinley, 2004). This in turn results in emotionally drained and burnt out professionals (Akroyd et al., 2002). Burnout is known to result in social and medical problems in the professional, such as dissatisfaction, frustration, anxiety, detachment, psychological fatigue, headaches, sleep disorders, intention to quit the job, depression and substance abuse (Akgun et al., 2008; Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998). Angerer (2003) reported that this concept, which can lead to cynicism in the individual’s work performance, is correlated with absenteeism, job turnover and low productivity, reduced job satisfaction and decreased commitment to the job. Failing to identify burnout among workers will inevitably lead to considerable financial, social and psychological costs (Maslach & Leiter, 1997; Glasberg, Norber & Soderberg, 2007). In Malta, the podiatric profession is still relatively new when compared to other local health care professions. Podiatry exists as a clinically independent profession involving the diagnosis and treatment of foot problems. The profession has grown considerably since the patient population is also growing, with 52% to 90% of older people complaining of a foot problem. In Malta, podiatry is providing its services to over 60,000 people annually through the public health service only (The Association of Podiatrists of Malta, 2007). A podiatrist is a qualified health professional who specialises in the prevention, diagnosis, treatment and rehabilitation of disorders of the foot and ankle. Underlying lower limb structures, such as the knee and the upper third of the lower limb, may also be examined by a podiatrist since these structures might be related to a foot or ankle problem. The complex mechanics of the feet (52 bones, a network of muscles, joints and ligaments) and a wide range of foot problems (over 200 identified conditions) demand a separate and autonomous profession which provides high levels of training and professional expertise in the field of podiatry (The Association of Podiatrists of Malta, 2007). As a result, the podiatrist’s scope of practice is diverse and has expanded to include curative and preventive strategies in paediatrics, diabetes, sports injuries, structural and biomechanical problems, geriatrics, vascular and podiatric surgery (The Associ- Degree of burnout among Maltese podiatrists working in two different settings ation of Podiatrists of Malta, 2012). There are currently approximately 66 state-registered podiatrists within the Council for the Professions Complementary to Medicine of Malta. These professionals work in a variety of settings including the public health service, private practice, education and research, or as a part of a multi-disciplinary team operating in a specific specialised clinic. Most of the literature on burnout has addressed the nursing, physiotherapy and occupational therapy professions (Burke, Koyuncu & Fiksenbaum, 2010). A small number of studies has been conducted within the podiatric profession. It has been argued that the relationship between podiatrists and their patients differs from that in other professions due to the fact that podiatrists are often required to spend extensive time with their patients on a one-to-one basis rather than working in teams with other health care professionals, possibly resulting in higher levels of work stress and burnout (Mandy, Lucas & McInnes, 2003). A comparative research study on burnout among Australian and British podiatrists was carried out by Mandy and Tinley in 2004. Results suggest that levels of burnout among podiatrists were higher than those indicated by published normative data for all health care professionals. These results are of great concern to the podiatric profession and have prompted the present investigation that addresses this concept in the Maltese culture. The latter identifies the Maltese population as unique, distinguishing it from its European neighbours. The Maltese culture is broadly Mediterranean, but it is at the same time very distinctive: it has its own unique blend of historical and economic traditions which in turn have influenced the values, motivations, expectations and practices which characterise the Maltese people (Baldacchino, 2000). The aim of this study was to compare levels of burnout in Maltese podiatrists working in two different settings, namely the private or the public sector only. The nature of work undertaken by each group was fundamentally the same; it was the environment that the profession was practised in which differed between the two groups. The null hypothesis which was formulated for this study suggested ‘no difference in the levels of burnout between Maltese podiatrists working in the public health service and those working in private practice only’. 2 Methods A quantitative non-experimental study design was employed. Ethical approval was sought and later granted by the University of Malta Research Ethics Committee. Convenience sampling was adopted. The Register of the Council for the Professions Complementary to Medicine was used to identify potential participants. An information letter was sent to all Maltese podiatrists working either in private practice or within the public health service only. A copy of the Copenhagen Burnout Inventory (CBI), together with a data sheet which requested details on the nature of employment, the length of time since qualification, age, gender, marital status and the number of patients seen each week, was also sent, together with a stamped, self-addressed envelope. Anonymity and confidentiality of information provided was ensured. All 18 podiatrists working in Malta on a full-time basis either in private practice or within the public health service participated in the study (response rate = 100%). 2.1 The Copenhagen Burnout Inventory (CBI) The CBI is a questionnaire that measures the degree of psychological fatigue experienced in three sub-dimensions of burnout: personal, work-related and patient-related. The personal burnout scale is generic, in that it can be answered by any individual. The work-related burnout scale assumes that the person undertaking the questionnaire has some kind of paid employment, while the patient-related burnout scale assumes that the individual’s http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/BURNPOD.2.38 36 work involves patients (Kristensen et al., 2005). This inventory is unique since its scales were developed in such a way that they should be able to indicate whether individual feelings of burnout are due to personal or work-related factors (Kristensen et al., 2005). Translations of the CBI into eight languages are available, allowing its use in many studies worldwide. Results support the reliability and validity of the tool, with high internal consistencies ranging from 0.85 to 0.87 (Borritz et al., 2006). The CBI questionnaire consists of 19 items with five response categories for each question: Always, Often, Sometimes, Seldom, Never/Almost, Never. For some of the questions, the response categories are adjusted as follows: To a very high degree, To a high degree, Somewhat, To a low degree, To a very low degree. Participants are required to choose only one answer for every question. The response categories are re-scaled to a 0-100 metric (0 = Never/Almost Never/To a very low degree, 25 = Seldom/To a low degree, 50 = Sometimes/Somewhat, 75 = Often/To a high degree, 100 = Always/To a very high degree. If less than three questions are answered from scales 1 and 3, or less than four questions are answered from scale 2, the respondent is classified as a nonresponder. A mean rating score greater than 50 indicates a high degree of burnout, whilst a mean rating score smaller than 50 implies a lower degree of burnout. The CBI is relatively easy to complete, requiring approximately 15 minutes to be filled in. It is a public domain questionnaire, unlike other measures of burnout which are protected by copyright and distributed by a commercial publisher, thus making it more accessible to researchers. Permission was obtained from the authors of the CBI (Kristensen et al., 2005) to employ the original English version of the CBI in the present study. Evidence confirming the reliability and validity of the instrument enhanced the study’s methodological rigour. 3 Results Twelve male and six female podiatrists working on a full-time basis either in private practice or in the public health service participated in this study. Participants were aged between 25 and 45 years, with 10 participants aged between 25 and 35 years and eight participants aged between 36 and 45 years. Sixty-one percent of the sample was married. Seventy-two percent (N = 13) of the participants worked within the public health service and 27% (N = 5) worked in private practice. Ninety-five percent of the respondents reported seeing approximately 31 to 40 patients per week. 3.1 CBI Scores The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test showed collected data to be normally distributed. The one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) test was used to confirm or reject the null hypothesis which was formulated for this study, namely that there was no difference in burnout levels between podiatrists working in the two different settings. One-way ANOVA is a parametric test which compares the means of several independent samples to make assumptions on the population means (Bowling, 2002). When the p-value was found to be less than the 0.05 level of significance, it was deduced that the mean rating scores for the CBI differed significantly, thus leading to rejection of the null hypothesis. No statistical difference was found between the overall mean burnout scores of podiatrists working in the public health service and private practice (p = 0.067). Moreover, the overall mean burnout score was less than 50 for each group, implying a low degree of burnout being present among Maltese state-registered podiatrists (mean score for public sector podiatrists = 48.87; mean score for private practice podiatrists = 31.84). Individual mean burnout scores were higher for podiatrists working within the public sector when compared to podiatrists working in the private sector for nearly all items of the three subscales of the CBI, as illustrated in Tables 1, 2 and 3. http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Degree of burnout among Maltese podiatrists working in two different settings 37 Table 1. CBI Scale 1 - Personal Burnout: means and standard deviations for the participants (N = 18). Mean Std. Deviation p-value How often do you feel tired? Public sector 61.54 24.185 0.769 Private sector 65.00 13.693 How often are you physically exhausted? Public sector 55.77 27.298 0.687 Private sector 50.00 25.000 How often are you emotionally exhausted? Public sector 57.69 25.789 0.104 Private sector 35.00 22.361 How often do you think ‘I can’t take it any more’ ? Public sector 48.08 29.689 0.039 Private sector 15.00 22.361 How often do you feel worn out? Public sector 57.69 23.683 0.083 Private sector 35.00 22.361 How often do you feel weak and susceptible to illness? Public sector 50.00 22.822 0.014 Private sector 20.00 11.180 Table 2. CBI Scale 2 -Work Burnout: means and standard deviations for the participants (N = 18). Mean Std. Deviation p-value Is your work emotionally exhausting? Public sector 51.92 31.394 0.190 Private sector 30.00 27.386 Do you feel burnt out because of your work? Public sector 57.69 23.683 0.104 Private sector 35.00 28.504 Does your work frustrate you? Public sector 32.69 27.735 0.862 Private sector 30.00 32.596 Do you feel worn out at the end of the working day? Public sector 59.62 26.098 0.250 Private sector 45.00 11.180 Are you exhausted in the morning at the thought of another day at work? Public sector 40.38 29.823 0.057 Private sector 10.00 22.361 Do you feel that every working hour is tiring for you? Public sector 40.38 21.743 0.074 Private sector 15.00 33.541 Do you have enough energy for family and friends during leisure? Public sector 67.31 27.735 0.877 Private sector 65.00 28.504 4 Discussion The aim of this study was to compare the degree of burnout among Maltese podiatrists working in two different settings (private versus public sector). It is important to note that this study captured the views of all Maltese podiatrists who were working in either the private or public sector only at the time of the investigation (100% response rate). Identification of burnout using the CBI requires participants to obtain high scores on all three subscales, namely personal burnout, work-related burnout and patient-related burnout (Borritz et al., 2006). No statistical difference between mean burnout scores of podiatrists working in the public health service and private prac- http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/BURNPOD.2.38 tice was identified. Moreover, the mean burnout scores for each subscale of the CBI were less than 50 for each group, implying a low degree of burnout among Maltese state-registered podiatrists. Previous study results on burnout among podiatrists in other populations are not congruent with the results of this study. Podiatrists in the United Kingdom and Australia were reported to exhibit high levels of emotional exhaustion and depersonalisation, as well as low levels of personal accomplishment when using the Maslach Burnout Inventory and the Work Stress Inventory to measure burnout levels (Mandy, 2000; Mandy & Tinley, 2002, 2004). However, this is the first study to have explored burnout within the Maltese context. Furthermore, the tool used to measure burnout in this specific population of podiatrists is differ- http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Degree of burnout among Maltese podiatrists working in two different settings ent from the ones employed in the studies mentioned above. It could be argued that the limitations of this study include that all data were collected using a self-report questionnaire, raising the possibility of response set tendencies. Furthermore, the data were collected at one point in time, making it difficult to determine causality. Participants’ personality and past or recent traumas/losses were not taken into consideration. When comparing the overall mean burnout scores between the two groups, podiatrists working in the public sector demonstrated a higher score than those working in the private sector. Research carried out among various health care professionals has led to the conclusion that health care workers within the public sector exhibit a higher degree of burnout when compared to those in private practice (Mrayyan, 2005; Pillay, 2009; Pinto, 2009). Various reasons have been given for this outcome, including that the private sector is less afflicted by rules, regulations and restrictive work, and that motivation in this sector is highly based on profits (Macklin, Smith & Dollard, 2006; Cherniss, 1995). Consideration of the Personal Burnout CBI subscale demonstrates that podiatrists working within the public sector reported feeling more physically and emotionally exhausted and more worn out than podiatrists working privately. On the Work Burnout subscale, podiatrists employed within the public sector reported being more emotionally exhausted at work, feeling more burnt out as a result of their work, feeling more frustrated with their work, feeling more worn out at the end of a working day and also feeling more exhausted at the thought of another day at work when compared to podiatrists working in the private sector. However, it is interesting to note that despite these negative responses, podiatrists working in the public sector reported more energy for family and friends after work than those working in the private sector. As for the Patient Burnout scale, podiatrists working within the public sector reported that, when working with patients, they found it harder to work, were more frustrated, felt more drained and felt that they gave more than they got back when compared to podiatrists working within the private sector. The patient-clinician relationship can differ across the public and private sectors. A number of common themes have been identified across studies, explaining why podiatrists working within the public sector tend to be more burnt out than podiatrists working in the private sector. These include lack of professional status among colleagues and patients, as well as labeling by patients as 38 being unproductive, inefficient, distant and poorly motivated (Ferrinho et al., 2004). Long waiting lists are attributed to health care professionals working inside the public sector, leading them to being viewed as incompetent and disrespectful (Forrest et al., 2002). Over-demanding patients, shortage of staff and professional isolation have also been reported to result in professional burnout (Mandy & Tinley, 2004). Moreover, patients attending private services considered their practitioners to be more understanding than practitioners working in the public sector and reported that clinicians had more time available to spend with them (Bjorngaard et al., 2008). Feedback from patients is important for any health care professional to feel appreciated (Mrayyan, 2005). Maslach and Leiter (1997) argue that feedback from patients attending public health care services is either non-existent or almost always negative. Patients attending the public sector do not pay directly for services rendered and thus are more likely to neglect and ignore advice given to them by their clinicians. This in turn leaves health care professionals feeling frustrated, tired and exhausted. Further research investigating differences in burnout between different podiatric clinical environments in this specific population is warranted if a better service is to be offered to patients attending podiatric services. A better understanding of the burnout concept could be developed through the use of longitudinal research designs that also address off-job experiences in the context of burnout research, particularly addressing indicators of psychological well-being as predictors of work outcomes, in addition to using the three CBI subscales (Sonnentag, 2005). Such studies should enable a better understanding of burnout and why it happens. Results could also provide information on how to cope with it, prevent it or combat it (Schaufeli et al., 2008). Prevention and management of burnout is of utmost importance since it is not just a personal problem but it could also have significant implications for organisations’ performance. More motivated podiatrists have a better quality of life, which in turn translates into improved patient care (Mandy & Tinley, 2004). 5 Conclusion The results of this study have shown no difference in burnout between Maltese podiatrists working in the public and private sectors. However, the podiatrists working in the public sector Table 3. CBI Scale 3 - Patient Burnout: means and standard deviations for the participants (N = 18). Mean Std. Deviation p-value Do you find it hard to work with patients? Public sector 34.62 26.098 0.134 Private sector 15.00 13.693 Do you find it frustrating to work with patients? Public sector 32.69 21.371 0.044 Private sector 10.00 13.693 Does it drain your energy to work with patients? Public sector 48.08 29.689 0.256 Private sector 30.00 27.386 Do you feel that you give more than you get back when you work with patients? Public sector 61.54 24.185 0.082 Private sector 40.00 13.693 Are you tired of working with patients? Public sector 30.77 25.318 0.241 Private sector 15.00 22.361 Do you sometimes wonder how long you will be able to work with patients? Public sector 40.38 33.132 0.794 Private sector 45.00 32.596 obtained higher mean burnout scores for most items of the CBI http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/BURNPOD.2.38 http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Degree of burnout among Maltese podiatrists working in two different settings subscales when compared to the practitioners working privately. More research is warranted in this area of study in Malta to contribute towards a better understanding of burnout among podiatrists in parallel with other health care professionals, in order to help prevent or alleviate this phenomenon. Furthermore, results of this study encourage further research with the purpose of establishing the CBI’s validity and reliability in the local context. 6 Acknowledgements The authors would like to thank all podiatrists who agreed to participate in this study. The authors are also grateful to Marianne Borritz who gave them permission to use the English version of the CBI. 7 Funding This research has received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or non-profit sectors. 8 Conflicts of Interest The authors report no conflicts of interest. References Akgun, S., Al-Assaf A.F. & Bakan, C. (2008) Reducing burnout among hospital professionals: EAPs can help identify and alleviate the factors that cause burnout and improve the health and performance of hospital staff. Available from: http://www.avitadestek.com/media/img_basin/ joea.pdf. [Accessed 29th July 2013]. Akroyd, D., Caison, A. & Adams, R. (2002) Patterns of burnout among U.S. radiographers. Radiologic Technology, 73(33), pp. 215-223. Angerer, J.M. (2003) Job burnout. Journal of Employment Counseling, 40, pp. 98-107. Baldacchino, G. (2000) Introducing Social Studies: A Maltese reader. Malta: PEG Ltd. Bjorngaard, J.H., Garratt, A., Grave, R.W., Bjertnes, O.A. & Ruud, T. (2008) Patient experiences with treatment in private practice compared with public mental health services. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 49, pp. 385-392. Borritz, M., Rugulies, R., Bjorner, J. B., Villadsen, E., Mikkelsen, O. A. & Kristensen, T. S. (2006) Burnout among employees in human service work: design and baseline findings of the PUMA study. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health, 34 (1), pp. 49-58. Bowling, A. (2002) Research Methods in Health: Investigating health and health services (2nd ed.). Buckingham: Open University Press. Brenninkmeijer, V. & Van Yperen, N. (2003) How to conduct research on burnout: advantages and disadvantages of a unidimensional approach in burnout research. Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 60 (Suppl 1): i16-i20. Burke, R.J., Koyuncu, M. & Fiksenbaum, L. (2010) Burnout, work satisfaction and psychological well-being among nurses in Turkish hospitals. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 1, pp. 63-81. Cherniss, C. (1995) Beyond Burnout: Helping teachers, nurses, therapists and lawyers recover from stress and disillusionment. London: Routledge. Demerouti, E., Bakker, A.B., Nachreiner, F. & Schaufeli, W.B. (2001) Job demands resources of burnout. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, pp. 499-512. Ferrinho, P., Lerberghe, W.V., Fronteira, I., Hipólito, F. & Biscaia, A. (2004) Dual practice in the health sector: review of the evidence. Human Resources for Health, 2(14), pp. http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/BURNPOD.2.38 39 1-17. Forrest, C.B., Shi, L., Schrader, S.V. & Ng, J. (2002) Managed care, primary care, and the patient-practitioner relationship. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 17, pp. 270277. Glasberg, A.L., Norber, A. & Soderberg, A. (2007) Sources of burnout among health care employees as perceived by managers. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 60(1), pp. 10-19. Kristensen, T.S., Borritz, M., Villadsen, E. & Christensen, K.B. (2005) The Copenhagen Burnout Inventory: A new tool for the assessment of burnout. Work and Stress, 19(3), pp. 192-207. Lloyd, C. & King, R. (2004) A survey of burnout among Australian mental health occupational therapists and social workers. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 39, pp. 752-757. Macklin, D.S., Smith, L.A. & Dollard, M.F. (2006) Public and private sector work stress: workers’ compensation, levels of distress and job satisfaction, and the demand-controlsupport model. Australian Journal of Psychology, 58(3), pp. 130-143. Mandy, A. (2000) Burnout and work stress in newly qualified podiatrists in the NHS. British Journal of Podiatry, 3(2), pp. 31-34. Mandy, A., Lucas, K. & McInnes, J. (2003) Psychosocial Approaches to Podiatry; A companion for practice. United Kingdom: Churchill Livingstone. Mandy, A. & Tinley, P. (2002) Burnout and occupational stress in Australian podiatrists. Australasian Journal of Podiatric Medicine, 36, pp. 101-108. Mandy, A. & Tinley, P. (2004) Burnout and occupational stress: comparison between United Kingdom and Australian podiatrists. Journal of the American Podiatric Medical Association, 94(3), pp. 282-291. Maslach, C. & Leiter, M.P. (1997) The Truth About Burnout, How Organisations Cause Personal Stress and What To Do About It. USA: John Wiley & Sons. Mrayyan, M.T. (2005) Nurse job satisfaction and retention: comparing public to private hospitals in Jordan. Journal of Nursing Management, 13, pp. 40-50. Pillay, R. (2009) Work satisfaction of professional nurses in South Africa: a comparative analysis of the public and private sectors. Human Resources for Health, 7(15), pp. 1-10. Pinto, C. (2009) Work stress, satisfaction and burnout in New Zealand radiologists: comparison of public hospital and private practice in New Zealand. Journal of Medical and Radiation Oncology, 53(2), pp. 194-199. Schaufeli, W.B. & Enzmann, D. (1998) The Burnout Companion to Study and Practice: A critical analysis. London: Taylor & Francis. Schaufeli, W.B., Leiter M.P. & Maslach C. (2008) Burnout: 35 years of research and practice. Career Development International, 14(3), pp. 204-220. Schaufeli, W.B., Leiter M.P., Maslach C. & Jackson, S.E. (1996) Maslach Burnout Inventory General Survey (MBI GS). In C. Maslach, S.E. Jackson and M.P. Leiter (Eds) Maslach Burnout Inventory (3rd ed.). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Sonnentag, S. (2005) Burnout research: adding an off-work and day-level perspective. Work and Stress, 19(3), pp. 271-275. The Association of Podiatrists of Malta (2007) History of Podiatry. Available from http://www.owenwebhosting.com/ apm/HistoryPodiatry.htm 2007. [Accessed 29th July 2012]. The Association of Podiatrists of Malta (2012) The Podiatrist. Available from podiatry.org.mt/en/home.htm. [Accessed 22nd September 2013]. http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Malta Journal of Health Sciences Online http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Doi: http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COMMAUT.2.40 Research paper THE EFFECTIVENESS OF SOCIAL COMMUNICATION GROUPS FOR CHILDREN WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER IN MAINSTREAM SCHOOLS Tiziana Baldacchino, Kristina Agius Department of Communication Therapy, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Malta, Msida, Malta Abstract. The purpose of this study was to evaluate the effectiveness of a 10-week Social Communication Group (SCG) programme involving six children diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). The sessions were carried out at the mainstream school attended by the children. This study evaluated the effectiveness of the programme in enhancing specific social communication skills in each participant, as opposed to previous research on social skills groups where results were more general. The children’s social communication skills were rated pre- and post-therapy by the first author, parents and Learning Support Assistants (LSAs) to allow comparison. Feedback forms filled in by the LSAs and the first author’s own observations allowed ongoing evaluation of each child’s performance throughout sessions. Questionnaires given pre- and post-therapy to the parents and LSAs were expected to provide evidence of generalisation of social communication skills at home and at school. Qualitative and quantitative data obtained from this small group study indicated that this method of intervention was effective in enhancing the social communication skills of children with ASD. Although all the participants showed an improvement in their overall performance, specific objectives targeted through structured activities that were motivating and repetitive elicited a higher percentage improvement. Factors such as the children’s primary language, pre-therapy status and their understanding at conversational level seemed to have an impact on their individual improvement. Generalisation of skills acquired during therapy was limited since it depended on the commitment of parents and LSAs in implementing the goals of the programme at home and at school. Keywords Autism Spectrum Disorder, social communication, group therapy, mainstream school, case study 1 Introduction The development of social communication is necessary for the adequate use of verbal and non-verbal communication for social purposes, across various contexts and with different communication partners (Prizant & Wetherby, 2005). According to the fifth Correspondence to: Tiziana Baldacchino (tiziana6@gmail.com) Received: 04.08.14 - Revised: 07.11.14 - Accepted: 08.11.14 Published: 17.12.14 c 2014 The authors edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-V) (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), one of the criteria necessary for diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is an impairment in social communication and social interaction, including deficits in social-emotional reciprocity, deficits in non-verbal communicative behaviours used for social interaction and deficits in developing and maintaining relationships. There are two main theories which attempt to explain the social deficits in ASD, namely Theory of Mind and social motivation theory. Previous studies have supported the hypothesis that Theory of Mind is impaired in children with ASD (Baron-Cohen, 2000). Theory of Mind is required for the understanding of the mental states of others, including their beliefs, desires, intentions and emotions (Baron-Cohen, 2001). In other words, children with ASD find it challenging to infer the thoughts of others. An impairment in Theory of Mind would result in difficulties during social communication (Cummings, 2009). In contrast, some theories have suggested that the social communication deficit arises from lack of social motivation rather than an impairment in social cognition. Social motivation models indicate that due to their impairment in early social attention, the social learning experience of children with ASD would be limited during development. Therefore, as a result of their preference for non-social stimuli, the impairment in social skills and social cognitive development would be more significant (Brodkin et al., 2012). Social skills intervention must be part of the therapeutic programme of children with ASD since it is unlikely that these skills will improve spontaneously (Flood et al., 2010). Furthermore, the impairment in interacting with others limits these children’s development of additional social communication abilities. Intervention targeting social communication should focus on encouraging the acquisition of skills, enhancing performance of skills, eliminating competing negative behaviours and facilitating generalisation (Gresham, Horner & Sugai, 2001). According to Forness et al. (1999), intervention is more effective when it targets specific skills rather than when it is general. Moreover, frequent therapy sessions would produce more positive results (Gresham et al., 2001). Social communication skills are generally targeted using a behavioural approach which involves modelling, prompting and reinforcement (Baker, 2010). These strategies could be implemented in a Social Communication Group (SCG) programme. Intervention within a small group of individuals having social communication difficulties would give them an opportunity to interact during conversations and games while enabling them to form friendships (Baker, 2010). Generalisation in children with ASD does not occur spontaneously and must be incorporated as part of the therapy programme (Flood et al., 2010). The effectiveness of social communication groups in mainstream schools Table 1. Age and language l Child Age Primary l (years) Language 1 8;00 English 2 8;01 Maltese 3 4 8;05 9;01 English Maltese 5 9;02 English 6 9;03 Maltese abilities of each child. Summary of Language Abilities Adequate receptive and expressive language Receptive Language: requires modelling to understand instructions and is unable to follow a conversation Expressive Language: does not use complex sentences when speaking Adequate receptive and expressive language Receptive Language: difficulty understanding at conversational level Expressive Language: difficulty expressing himself when answering questions Receptive Language: difficulty understanding at conversational level Expressive Language: adequate Receptive Language: difficulty understanding when asked a question, especially at conversational level Expressive Language: difficulty expressing himself, especially in statements requiring reasoning Research on SCGs for children with ASD has shown inconsistent results due to the diverse presentation of participants as well as the different methods and measurements used across studies (e.g. Barry et al., 2003; Chung et al., 2007; Dittner et al., 2006; Fombonne et al., 2007; Jones et al., 2004). Evidence regarding the effectiveness of SCGs is necessary to prove that resources are being put to good use (Cicchetti et al., 2009). Research should aim to establish an intervention method suitable for individuals with ASD to avoid treatments with additional risks (Offit, 2008). The purpose of the current study was to investigate the effectiveness of SCGs for children with ASD who attend mainstream schools. A case study approach was adopted, thus enabling a detailed group evaluation with reference to individual characteristics of each child. Apart from identifying the specific communication skills that were enhanced in children after participating in the SCG programme, the study evaluates the factors which might have affected improvement on an individual basis. It also seeks evidence of generalisation of the skills acquired during the sessions to everyday settings. 2 2.1 Methods Participants The subjects were recruited from the caseload of speechlanguage pathologists (SLPs) working in a local Health Centre. The sample consisted of six students who had been diagnosed with ASD and attended a mainstream school, where they had a Learning Support Assistant (LSA). The participants were aged between 8;00 and 9;03 years, with a mean age of 8;06 years. This age range was selected since the chosen SCG programme, designed by Agius (2007), was formulated for children of this age. The six participants are referred to in the present text using a number from 1 to 6, with Child 1 being the youngest child. Although the participant selection criteria did not include gender specifications, the children whose parents consented to their participation in the study were all males. 2.2 41 Procedure The main goals of the programme were to enhance basic skills such as attention, listening and greeting, to improve the participants’ conversation skills, to enable them to be aware of different emotions, to enhance their friendship skills and to encourage them to take part in activities requiring teamwork. The programme consisted of i) an initial meeting with the parents during which they were provided with detailed information on the programme http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COMMAUT.2.40 ii) 10 group intervention sessions with the six participants and iii) a final meeting with the parents and LSAs during which they were given reports summarising each child’s progress. The intervention sessions were carried out at the participants’ school, which was considered to be a clinical setting for the purposes of this study. Parents and LSAs were advised to attend the sessions, which took place once a week and lasted one hour. Each session targeted a specific set of social skills. Since the children were bilingual, both Maltese and English were used when targeting the SCG goals. Across sessions, the participants were provided with a series of worksheets, which formed part of the SCG programme. Parents and LSAs were advised to use these at home and at school to encourage generalisation of skills targeted during therapy. A detailed overview of the programme construction is found in Agius (2007), who describes how the activities for each session were planned following a review of relevant literature and research into different social skills programmes (e.g. Rinaldi, 1992; Aarons and Gittens, 1998). Available resources such as handouts were included among the session materials (e.g. Schroeder, 1996; Barratt et al., 2000; Mortimer et al., 2005). Emotions were addressed during every session using The Transporters, a series of short animated films produced by the Autism Research Centre at Cambridge University (2006). Finally, the guidelines of the programme were adapted from Rinaldi’s (1992) Social Use of Language Programme. 2.3 Measures The effectiveness of the intervention programme was measured using pre- and post- treatment comparison. The programme’s Observation Schedule and Checklist (refer to Appendix 1) was used to measure the children’s performance in various social communication skills. The Checklist content was based on research into the various areas typically targeted during therapy with children with ASD, as well as other social skills programmes and observation schedules (see Agius, 2007). In completing the Checklist, the children’s social communication skills were assigned a ‘pre-rating’ and ‘post-rating’ score before and after therapy. Table 2 exemplifies use of the Checklist rating scale with the conversational skill of ‘Looking (Eye Contact)’ (Section 1). Baseline and outcome measures were supplemented by field notes on individual participants’ performance on tasks, based on ongoing observation and evaluation. The pre- and post-treatment measures together with the field notes were collected by the first author. In addition, LSAs were requested to complete a feedback form during each session, thus providing another ongoing measure http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs The effectiveness of social communication groups in mainstream schools 42 Table 2. Rating of ‘Looking (Eye Contact)’ on the Observation Schedule and Checklist. Rating Level 1 2 3 Description Not evident Emerging, but need improvement Appropriate level Since children with ASD find it difficult to generalise skills acquired during therapy to other settings such as home and classroom (Keen & Rodger, 2012), the effectiveness of the programme at home and at school was also considered. The parents and LSAs of the participants were requested to fill in a Questionnaire on their opinion of the child’s communication skills before the therapy programme (refer to Appendix 2). An identical Questionnaire was completed upon completion of intervention. Both the Checklist and the Questionnaire were divided into five main sections evaluating conversation skills, emotions, games, teamwork and friendship skills. Reliability and validity of measures were not addressed directly in the methodological design due to time restrictions. However, use of the Checklist in previous research (Agius, 2007) showed that it effectively evaluated skills targeted during programme sessions. Moreover, it was explicitly designed to accompany this particular therapy programme. The specificity of measures to the participants reduced the generalisability of results, limiting their external validity (Jewell, 2014). Measurement error in preand post-treatment scores was minimised since the same observer assigned all ratings. The Questionnaire items targeted the same skills addressed in the Observation Schedule and Checklist, enhancing the validity of results. However, completion of the Questionnaire by different individuals may have introduced observer bias in the responses. Example No eye contact Eye contact inconsistent Eye contact present even during conversations children was calculated by obtaining a percentage of the total number of enhanced skills in relation to the skills that were not at an appropriate level pre-therapy. This is represented in Figure 1. Figure 1: Overall percentage improvement of all children compared to the mean. Data collected were analysed using both quantitative and qualitative methods. Data obtained from the Checklist and Questionnaire was analysed quantitatively using descriptive statistics. The ongoing observations of children’s performance during tasks and the feedback forms completed by the LSAs for every session were analysed qualitatively. 2.4 Ethical considerations Ethical approval from the University of Malta’s Research Ethics Committee was obtained prior to data collection. The Committee’s ethical guidelines were followed throughout the research study. 3 3.1 Results Analysis of Performance within the Clinical Setting Of the 10 sessions making up the SCG programme, each child attended at least eight. Average attendance was 92% of the total number of sessions. The average attendance of relatives (mothers and grandmothers) and LSAs was 66% and 90% respectively. Pre- and post-rating Checklist scores were compared to calculate the percentage improvement in social communication skills for each child and the mean percentage improvement for the group. Skills not requiring further intervention at the pre-therapy stage were identified through the Checklist. Skills which were not evident or were still emerging at the onset of therapy were grouped according to whether they improved or not by the end of the programme. Improvement was accounted for when skill ratings moved up by one or two ranking points. The overall improvement of each child and the mean improvement of all six http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COMMAUT.2.40 Figure 2: Mean percentage improvement of the main areas of social communication skills. The mean percentage improvement of the six participants in conversation skills, emotions, games, teamwork and friendships skills was calculated and represented in Figure 2. A qualitative description of relevant results is provided in the next sections. 3.1.1 Conversation Skills The main conversation skills targeted during the SCG included greetings, eye contact, turn taking, listening, topic maintenance, http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs The effectiveness of social communication groups in mainstream schools initiation and voice volume. - Greetings: The six children all showed improvement in greeting by saying “hello” and “goodbye” more spontaneously. - Eye contact: None of the children showed adequate eye contact for conversational purposes pre-therapy, but 50% of them improved this skill. Child 4, 5 and 6 improved by maintaining more eye contact. Child 1 was able to maintain eye contact when listening but not when speaking. Child 2 avoided looking at the listener’s face when speaking so that eye contact was not evident whereas Child 3 made eye contact when greeting but not during conversation. - Turn taking: Five of the children did not show appropriate turn taking skills pre-therapy and only Child 4 did not show improvement since he did not wait for his turn to speak during conversation. - Listening: Five of the children presented with difficulties in listening attentively during conversation since they interrupted the speaker or appeared to be distracted. Child 4 was the only child who did not show improvement in active listening due to his tendency to vocalise when someone else was speaking. - Topic maintenance: Improvement in topic maintenance was observed in all the children since they were able to speak for a longer time about a particular subject. - Initiation: The six children presented with difficulties in initiating a conversation. Five of them showed improvement by introducing a topic or by asking questions. - Volume: Of the four children who presented with an inappropriate volume level, three showed improvement. Child 2 increased the volume level of his voice while Child 4 and 6 decreased their loudness. Child 5 did not use adequate volume since it was either too high or too low. 3.1.2 Emotions The emotions targeted were happy, sad, angry, afraid, excited, worried, surprised, disappointed, tired, unfriendly, sorry, kind and helpful. Five of the participants had presented with difficulties in explaining and/or recognising emotions in themselves pretherapy. They all showed improvement in this area. In addition, all six children presented with difficulties in explaining and/or recognising emotions in others. Only Child 2 did not show any evident improvement in the latter. By the end of the programme, 50% of the children, including Child 1, 3 and 5, also exhibited an understanding of complex emotions. 3.1.3 Games Games included in the SCG focused on enhancing participation, following and accepting rules, attending as well as showing appropriate behaviour when winning or losing. Five of the children presented with difficulties in participation during games and an improvement was observed in that they became more attentive. This was evidenced by an increase in their listening and/or looking behaviours. All of the children showed an improvement in collaboration during games, which was observed through turn taking, understanding and/or following instructions and accepting and/or following rules. Although all the children behaved appropriately when winning a game, four of them had difficulties when losing. Three of the latter showed improvement in behaving appropriately in this area. 3.1.4 Teamwork Teamwork activities included in the SCG focused on encouraging collaboration, sharing, commenting and giving assistance. Although the children were encouraged to work together, two of them preferred working alone throughout all the sessions. Improvement was noted in the skills listed below. - Collaboration: The children who tended to take a leadership role improved by allowing others to contribute, while those who preferred working alone improved by joining the rest of the group. http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COMMAUT.2.40 43 Child 2 showed progress by simply participating, given that in earlier sessions he refused to take part in teamwork activities. Child 6 was the only participant who did not demonstrate any difference in collaboration pre- and post-therapy since he preferred working individually. - Sharing materials: Four of the children were not observed to share materials pre-therapy. Three of them improved by sharing the materials used during the crafts activities with the members of the team. This was not observed in Child 2. - Commenting: Discussion of ideas was not evident in the initial session. Four of the children improved since they started discussing ideas with the group while the others just followed the rest of the team. - Giving assistance: Only one of the children was observed helping others during the initial session. However, by the final session, three other children started giving assistance by helping others find pictures, helping them to finish their puzzle or preparing for the activities. - Leadership: Although not targeted directly, three out of the six children improved in leadership skills by accepting leadership, speaking for the team and accepting ideas of others. 3.1.5 Friendship skills Friendships skills targeted through role play and brainstorming included asking another child to play, asking a question, introducing oneself, controlling anger, practising independence and solving problems. Improvement observed in friendship skills is described below. - Welcoming others: The participants became more welcoming towards their peers as the sessions progressed, except for Child 5. This trend was evident as participants accepted peers joining them when asked to play with them. Willingness to work with others was also shown during teamwork activities. Participants also started showing a preference for particular peers when choosing teams. -Introducing self : All children improved in introducing themselves by using greetings, saying their name and their age, and mentioning their hobbies. - Reciprocity: Four of the participants took part in more reciprocal conversations. However, this improvement was not observed in Child 1, who continued talking in a monologic style most of the time, and in Child 2, who did not take part in conversations. - Controlling Anger: Child 4, 5 and 6 became more able to control their anger when waiting for their turn during games and when disagreeing with a peer. Child 1 and 2, who also presented with difficulties in controlling their anger, did not show any improvement in this regard. - Independence: Child 1, 3 and 5 became more independent when making decisions while the other participants did not show any improvement in this area. - Solving problems: Although all of the children presented with difficulties in solving problems during social situations, only Child 1 and 3 showed progress in this skill. - Making friends: All the children improved in making friends except for Child 2 who, even though frequently encouraged, was rarely seen interacting with the others. 3.2 Evaluation of Performance at Home and at School The pre-therapy and post-therapy Questionnaires given to parents and LSAs were compared. Figures 3 and 4 show percentage change in rating of skills according to whether parents and LSAs reported improvement, no change or regression. The mean percentage improvement of skills at home and at school was 20% and 19% respectively. Most of the skills remained the same after therapy, both at home and at school. According to the Ques- http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs The effectiveness of social communication groups in mainstream schools tionnaire data, regression was evident in some of the skills. This outcome is discussed further in the next section. The percentage improvement at home of Child 2 is not included since the parents’ post-therapy Questionnaire was not returned. Figure 3: Percentage improvement at home relative to the mean percentage improvement for the whole group. losing. These variables might therefore account, at least in part, for lack of improvement in the skills targeted by the programme. However, it could not be established whether limited progress was specific to children with ASD since a control group of typicallydeveloping children was not included in the study. Attendance did not seem to affect the children’s individual improvement. However, it might have had an impact if they missed more than 80% of the sessions. More improvement was shown in children who a) were English-speaking, b) had more age-appropriate skills before therapy and c) had adequate understanding at conversational level. English-speaking children possibly showed most improvement since they tended to participate and lead more in activities, resulting in more use of English during sessions which in turn encouraged their participation further. Nevertheless, this improvement could be a result of their increased willingness to participate, which led to more opportunities for practising their skills. Another factor which might have had an impact on this result is the fact that the DVDs targeting emotions were in English. Although Maltese was also used during the sessions, the use of English in the previously mentioned task might have been a limitation for the Maltese-speaking children. Those children having a more positive pre-therapy status showed increased improvement since they presented with fewer skills requiring intervention. Another possible reason is that they had more skills which were already emerging pre-therapy, rather than skills which were not evident. Therefore, they required intervention on improvement of skills as opposed to others who necessitated intervention on acquisition. Finally, the comprehension difficulties exhibited by Child 2, 4, 5 and 6 seemed to have affected performance during tasks requiring conversational understanding, namely free conversation with peers, show-and-tell and teamwork activities. Improvement in each of the main skills addressed by the SCG programme is discussed in the following sections. 4.1 Figure 4: Percentage improvement at school relative to the mean percentage improvement for the whole group. 4 Discussion The purpose of this study was to evaluate improvement in the social communication skills of six children with ASD after attending a 10-week SCG programme in a mainstream school. Based on observations within the clinical setting and parents’ and LSAs’ ratings, improvement was evident in the social communication of all six children taking part in the study. However, a 100% improvement was not shown by any of the children, implying that further therapy on social communication was required. Social interaction and pragmatic skills are influenced by cultural factors (Bratanić, 2007; Harzing & Neyer, 2008). It should therefore be taken into consideration that difficulties in social communication might also be observed in typically-developing school-aged children, as in the inability to wait for a turn. In addition, personality has an impact on reaction to social situations even in typically-developing children (Mischel & Shoda, 2008), as in acceptance or otherwise of http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COMMAUT.2.40 44 Conversation skills The results obtained for conversation skills correspond to those reported in the study by Barry et al. (2003), where improvement was mostly shown in greetings rather than in other conversation skills. However, Barry et al. (2003) did not evaluate improvement in specific conversation skills such as eye contact, turn taking, listening, topic maintenance, initiation and voice volume. In Cragar and Horvath’s (2003) case study of a 10-year-old boy attending group therapy, improvement in conversations was evident in that the child interrupted the speaker less. Yet, lack of progress in the child’s understanding of non-literal language and humour was reported, similar to findings emerging from the Checklist and Questionnaire data in the current investigation. Several studies have reported an improvement in the initiation of social interactions as a result of group therapy (e.g. Bauminger, 2002; Crooke, Hendrix & Rachman, 2008; Kroeger, Newsome & Schultz, 2007). In this study, only Child 2 did not improve in initiating conversations. At the start of the programme, this child did not participate in interactions, so that for him, progress entailed maintaining a conversation rather than initiating it. In the four case studies evaluated by Chung et al. (2007), all children progressed when making initiating comments. However, in contrast to the current study, Chung et al. (2007) also involved typically-developing peers who modelled adequate use of social skills to the participant group. 4.1.1 Emotions The children showed more improvement in recognising and explaining emotions as experienced by themselves rather than by others. This ties in with the proposed difficulties in Theory of Mind (Baron-Cohen, 2000), which prevents children with ASD from understanding the perspective of others, including their feelings. Improvement in the understanding of complex emotions was only observed in three of the children. This was expected since studies such as Balconi and Carrera’s (2007) reported that chil- http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs The effectiveness of social communication groups in mainstream schools dren with ASD had the most difficulty in recognising complex emotions. Baron-Cohen et al. (2007) found that difficulties in recognising complex emotions might persist through adulthood. However, there is evidence that intervention may encourage the development of these emotions. In fact, Bauminger’s (2002) study on the effectiveness of cognitive behavioural intervention for children with ASD in a group setting reported improvement in the production of complex emotions. 4.2 Games The highest percentage improvement in skills was observed in games. This was probably due to the fact that games were more structured when compared to the tasks used to target the other objectives. In addition, the prospect of winning acted as a motivator, which reinforced the children’s participation and collaboration during games. This aspect of reinforcement forms part of the behavioural approach which encourages learning in children with ASD (Baker, 2010). There are very few studies that have evaluated the effectiveness of games as part of a SCG programme. Barry et al. (2003) found that children showed more improvement in play skills compared to conversation skills, while Dittner et al. (2006) reported that children improved in their ability to play with others. Targeting social skills through games, Jones et al. (2004) observed improvement in all the skills addressed, although progress in the children’s actual performance during games was not evaluated. 4.3 Teamwork There was a general improvement in collaboration, sharing materials, commenting, giving assistance and leadership. This corroborates the findings of Jones et al.’s (2004) study, in which participants were reported to start offering help and encouragement to others. An improvement in the sharing of ideas was observed in four of the children in the current study. Improvement in this area was similarly reported by Jones et al. (2004). Leadership skills were observed in the children who were more willing to collaborate in the earlier sessions of the programme. Dittner et al. (2006) found no significant improvement in leadership skills in their investigation. However, the scales they used to assess performance might not have been sufficiently valid since they included general social skills and did not evaluate the abilities targeted during sessions. 4.4 Friendship skills Friendship skills exhibited the least improvement, possibly because the relevant intervention was less structured and more unpredictable, excluding the friendship skills targeted during brainstorming activities and role play. The lack of structure in intervention was due to the fact that it depended on situations presenting themselves during the sessions, which would differ from one group to another. Five of the participants improved in making friends, but Child 2 failed to interact with others through to the final session. In Fombonne et al.’s (2007) study, 12 out of the 13 adolescents with Asperger’s Syndrome and High-Functioning Autism who gave feedback on their ability to make friends reported an improvement. However, this outcome was based on self-report rather than the researchers’ observations, suggesting that the participants’ subjectivity in reporting may have influenced results. In Barnhill et al.’s (2002) study, seven male adolescents were able to form a friendship with one other group member, even though they did not improve in the actual goals targeted during therapy. Cragar and Horvath (2003) reported that the child observed in their case study managed to make friends at school after attending a social skills group. Moreover, in a parent-assisted social skills group investigated by Dillon et al. (2012), 28 teenagers were successful in maintaining most of the friendship skills acquired during therapy. The current study was unable to determine whether friendships http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COMMAUT.2.40 45 were maintained since a follow-up assessment was not carried out due to a limited time frame. 4.5 Generalisation The limited evidence of generalisation could have resulted from the lack of implementation of worksheet activities at home or at school. Although parents and LSAs were encouraged to use these worksheets, it was reported that they did not have time to implement them. Most children with ASD are not able to generalise skills outside the clinical setting without additional support, suggesting that this aspect must form an integral part of any SCG programme (Flood et al., 2010). Previous studies have also reported limited generalisation skills. For example, Barry et al. (2003) found greetings to be generalised as opposed to conversation and play skills. Generalisation was reported by the parents taking part in Fombonne et al.’s (2007) study, but no evidence of generalisation was present in the investigation carried out by Jones et al. (2004). A decrease in aggression, increased selfcontrol and improved interaction were reported by Dittner at al. (2006). In the latter study, however, parents participated directly during sessions and the measurement scales were based on their reports. Cragar and Horvath (2003) reported an increase in generalisation after a nine-month follow up. When comparing pre- and post-therapy Questionnaires completed by the parents and LSAs, regression was observed in specific skills which differed across participants. This outcome could have been due to a change in the parents’ and LSAs’ perceptions of the children’s skills. In turn, the latter could have resulted from a recent incident involving the child, such as difficulties with conflict resolution emerging during an argument with a peer. Regression might have also been an outcome of parents’ and LSAs’ increased awareness of social communication deficits: upon completion of the programme, they were likely to be more knowledgeable in the requirements of each skill. Another possibility could be that parents and LSAs compared their child with other participants in the group and subsequently rated his skills as less appropriate. In addition, the pre-therapy Questionnaire was completed at the beginning of the scholastic year, when LSAs might have not yet had sufficient experience with the children to judge whether specific skills were at an appropriate level pre-therapy. Furthermore, reporting bias in parents and LSAs may have resulted in lack of objectivity in the generalisation data. The latter was manifested in discrepancies in the ratings of individual children’s behaviours by the two observers. 4.6 Limitations The findings of this study cannot be generalised to the broader population of children with ASD due to the small sample size. The data were obtained from a case study of a single group, so that the results are not necessarily applicable to other Maltese children attending a different mainstream school. Regretfully, the study lacked a control group consisting of children not receiving group therapy. This would have allowed comparison with the experimental group, providing more objective results regarding the effectiveness of the SCG. However, the ethical issue of limiting therapy to the experimental group led to exclusion of a control group from the study’s methodological design. Nonetheless, improvements in social communication do not usually occur spontaneously (Flood et al., 2010), which implies that unless the participants were attending additional therapy, improvement shown could be attributed to the SCG. Indeed, at the beginning of the programme, none of the parents reported that their children were receiving intervention targeting their social skills. It is also acknowledged that the Checklist and Questionnaire measures depended on the perspective of the individuals completing these instruments. Yet, subjectivity should have been minimised since forms were completed by the same individuals pre- and post- http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs The effectiveness of social communication groups in mainstream schools therapy. Ensuring the validity and reliability of the Checklist and Questionnaire data would have increased the objectivity and accuracy of results. In addition, skills emerging during the group sessions might have been restricted by the structured nature of the clinical setting, limiting the representativeness of the data. 5 Conclusion The results of this study support the claims in the literature that SCGs are effective in enhancing the social communication skills of verbal children with ASD. The percentage improvement resulting in the main skills targeted highlights the importance of structured activities, motivation through rewards and repetition of tasks for effectiveness in the intervention of children with ASD. Although differences in children’s primary language, pre-therapy status and understanding at conversational level had an impact on their individual improvement, further investigation is required regarding the effects of these specific factors. Results showed limited evidence of generalisation, but the parents and LSAs still reported an improvement in each child’s specific deficits. These positive results should encourage the implementation of SCGs in mainstream schools, although further research on their effectiveness with a more representative sample is recommended. 6 Acknowledgements We would like to thank the participants, their families and their LSAs for taking part in the study. 7 Funding This research has received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or non-profit sector. 8 Conflicts of interest The authors report no conflicts of interest. References Agius, K. (2007) Social Communication Groups in a Mainstream School with Children with Autistic Spectrum Disorders. Unpublished M.Ed. Dissertation. United Kingdom: University of Birmingham. American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing. Baker J. (2010) Social skills training for children on the autism spectrum: current research and integration. Autism Advocate, 58(1), pp. 8-13. Balconi, M. & Carrera, A. (2007) Emotional representation in facial expression and script: a comparison between normal and autistic children. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 28, pp. 409-422. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2006.05.001 Barnhill, G. P., Cook, K. T., Myles, B. S. & Tebbenkamp, K. (2002) The effectiveness of social skills intervention targeting nonverbal communication for adolescents with Asperger syndrome and related pervasive developmental delays. Focus on Autism and other Developmental Disabilities, 17(2), pp. 112-118. Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Theory of Mind and autism: a fifteen year review. In D. J. Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg & S. Baron-Cohen (Eds) Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 3-20). Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University Press. Baron-Cohen, S. (2001) Theory of Mind in normal development and autism. Prisme, 34, pp. 174-183. Baron-Cohen, S., Golan, O., Hill, J. J. & Rutherford, M. D. http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COMMAUT.2.40 46 (2007) The ’Reading the mind in the voice’ test-revised: a study of complex emotion recognition in adults with and without autism spectrum conditions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, pp. 1096-1106. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0252-5. Barry, T. D., Bodin, S. D., Gilmore, T., Klinger, L. G., Lee, J. M. & Palardy, N. (2003) Examining the effectiveness of an outpatient clinic-based social skills group for highfunctioning children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(6), pp. 1096-1106. doi: 01623257/03/1200-0685/0. Bauminger, N. (2002) The facilitation of social-emotional understanding and social interaction in high-functioning children with autism: intervention outcomes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(4), pp. 283-298. Bratanić, M. (2007) Nonverbal communication as a factor in linguistic and cultural miscommunication. In A. Esposito, E. Keller, M. Bratanić & M. Marinaro (Eds) Fundamentals of Verbal and Nonverbal Communication and the Biometric Issue (pp. 82-91). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: IOS Press. Brodkin, E. S., Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Schultz, R. T. & Troiani, V. (2012) The social motivation theory of autism. Trends of Cognitive Sciences, 16(4), pp. 231-239. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.02.007. Chung, K., Drewry, J., Matthews, T., Mosconi, M., Reavis, S. & Tasse’, M. J. (2007) Peer-mediated social skills training program for young children with high-functioning autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 28, pp. 423-436. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2006.05.002. Cicchetti, D., De Los Reyes, A., Klin, A., Koenig, K. & Scahill, L. (2009) Group intervention to promote social skills in schoolage children with pervasive developmental disorders: reconsidering efficacy. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, pp. 1163-1172. doi: 10.1007/s10803-0090728-1. Cragar, D. E. & Horvath, L. S. (2003) The application of social skills training in the treatment of a child with Asperger’s disorder. Clinical Case Studies, 2(1), pp. 34-39. doi: 10.1177/1534650102239087. Crooke, P., Hendrix, R. & Rachman, J. (2008) Brief reports: measuring the effectiveness of teaching social thinking to children with asperger syndrome (AS) and high-functioning autism (HFA). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, pp. 581-591. Cummings, L. (2009) Clinical Pragmatics. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. Dillon, A. R., Frankel, F., Gantman, A., Laugeson, E. A. & Mogil, C. (2012) Parent-assisted social skills training to improve friendships in teens with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, pp. 10251036. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1339-1. Dittner, C. A., Mellon, M., Sim, L. & Whiteside, S. P. (2006) Effectiveness of a social skills training program with school age children: transition to the clinical setting. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 15, pp. 409-418. doi: 10.1007/s10826006-9049-6. Flood, M. F., Hansen, D. J., Jordan, J. A. & Smith, A. J. (2010) Social skills interventions. In C. A. Erdley, D. J. Hansen, D. W. Nagle & P. J. Norton (Eds) Practitioner’s Guide to Empirically Based Measures of Social Skills (pp. 99-115). New York: Springer. Fombonne, E., Meng, L., Tagalakis, V., Tse, J. & Strulovitch, J. (2007) Social skills training for adolescents with asperger syndrome and high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, pp. 1960-1968. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0343-3. Forness, S. R., Kavale, K. A., Mathur, S. R., Quinn, M. M. & http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs The effectiveness of social communication groups in mainstream schools 47 Rutherford, R. B. (1999) A meta-analysis of social skill interventions for students with emotional or behavioral disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 7(1), pp. 54-64. Gresham, F. M., Horner, R. H. & Sugai, G. (2001) Interpreting outcomes of social skills training for students with highincidence disabilities. The Council for Exceptional Children, 67(3), pp. 331-334. Harzing, A. W. & Neyer, A. K. (2008) The impact of culture on interactions: five lessons learned from the European Commission. European Management Journal, 26(5), pp. 325-334. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.emj.2008.05.005 Jewell, D. V. (2014) Guide to Evidence-based Physical Therapist Practice. Sudbury, MA: Jones & Bartlett. Jones, W. P., Miller, S. P., Pierce, T. B., Strawser, S. & Webb, B. J. (2004) Effects of social skill instruction for highfunctioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Focus on Autism and other Developmental Disabilities, 19, pp. 53-62. doi: 10.1177/10883576040190010701. Keen, D. & Rodger, S. (2012) Working with Parents of a Newly Diagnosed Child with an Autism Spectrum Disorder: A guide for professionals. London, U.K.: Jessica Kingsley. Kroeger, K. A., Newsome, C. & Schultz, J. R. (2007) A comparison of two group-delivered social skills programs for young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, pp. 808-817. Mischel, W. & Shoda, Y. (2008) Towards a unified theory of personality: integrating dispositions and processing dynamics within the cognitive-affective processing system. In L. A. Pervin, O. P. John & R. W. Robins (Eds) Handbook of Personality: Theory and research (pp. 208-241). New York: The Guilford Press. Offit, P. (2008) Autism’s False Prophets: Bad science, risky medicine, and the search for a cure. New York: Columbia University Press. Prizant, B. M. & Wetherby, A. M. (2005) Critical issues in enhancing communication abilities for persons with autism spectrum disorders. In A. Klin, D. Cohen, F. R. Volkmar & R. Paul (Eds) Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders - Volume Two: Assessment, interventions, and policy (3rd ed.). Hoboken, N.J.: John Wiley & Sons. http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COMMAUT.2.40 http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs The effectiveness of social communication groups in mainstream schools 48 Appendix 1 http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COMMAUT.2.40 http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs The effectiveness of social communication groups in mainstream schools http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COMMAUT.2.40 49 http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs The effectiveness of social communication groups in mainstream schools http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COMMAUT.2.40 50 http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs The effectiveness of social communication groups in mainstream schools 51 Appendix 2 http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COMMAUT.2.40 http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs The effectiveness of social communication groups in mainstream schools http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COMMAUT.2.40 52 http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs The effectiveness of social communication groups in mainstream schools http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COMMAUT.2.40 53 http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Malta Journal of Health Sciences http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Doi: http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/EXPERAMP.2.54 Research paper THE LIVED EXPERIENCE OF PERSONS WITH AN AMPUTATION Chanelle Grech, Roberta Farrugia Debono Department of Psychology, Faculty for Social Wellbeing, University of Malta, Msida, Malta Abstract. This paper presents an exploratory study of Maltese persons living with an amputation. Semi-structured interviews were conducted with four participants. Three of the participants had a lower limb amputation, while the fourth had an upper limb amputation. Interpretative phenomenological analysis (IPA) was used in the analysis phase. The three core superordinate themes that emerged from the data revolved around the emotional reactions towards the amputation, the numerous changes it brought to the participants’ lives, and the support which enabled amputees to cope with their situation. The study highlights the range of emotions experienced by the participants on having an amputation and the changes in their lifestyle. Support from family, friends, health professionals and other amputees was identified as a factor that helped participants cope and adapt to their new condition. in hospital and medical facilities (Government of Malta, 2011). Various studies on the experiences of amputees (e.g. Desmond & MacLachlan, 2006; Liu et al., 2010; Senra et al., 2012) focus on three broad aspects: the psychological impact, the processes of adjustment linked to the change in physical image and pain, as well as the impact of social factors such as social support and education. Furthermore, most of the available research (Horgan & MacLachlan, 2004; Senra et al., 2012) focuses on the lived experience of persons having an amputation of the lower limb. This study aims to contribute to this literature by exploring the lived experience of Maltese individuals with a lower or upper limb amputation. 2 2.1 Keywords: amputation, lived experience, prosthesis, social support, qualitative research, interpretative phenomenological analysis 1 Introduction Limb amputation is considered to be one of the oldest surgical procedures with a history of more than 2,500 years (Van der Meij, 1995). It is defined as a medical procedure in which there is “partial or complete removal of a limb for a variety of reasons, including tumours, gangrene, intractable pain, crushing injury or uncontrollable infection” (Fremgen & Frucht, 2009, p.10). Amputation is usually considered as a last resort in the case of life endangerment (Paudel, Shrestha & Banskota, 2005), since it brings about drastic social, economical and psychological changes to both the amputees’ and their families’ lives (Sinha, van den Heuvel & Arokiasamy, 2011). Local statistics indicate that there were approximately 88 Maltese patients who had undergone one or multiple major amputations over a one-year time frame, between 1st June 2011 and 31st May 2012 (Cassar, 2012). Fortunately, the number of major limb amputations above or below the knee in Maltese patients has decreased by around 33% between 2003 and 2010, as a result of early detection, timely intervention and improvement Correspondence to: Chanelle Grech (chanellegrech@gmail.com) Received: 22.07.14 - Revised: 22.11.14 - Accepted: 28.11.14 Published: 17.12.14 c 2014 The authors Methods Participants Four Maltese participants were interviewed regarding their lived experience of being an amputee. Interpretative phenomenological research is usually conducted using small samples (three to six participants) since a detailed analysis of each case is necessary (Smith, Flowers & Larkin, 2009). The purposive sample consisted of two male and two female amputees whose ages ranged between 30 and 80 years. Participants were recruited from the Amputees4Amputees Association, which is a support group for Maltese amputees. They were informed about the study by the Association through an information letter which contained details on the nature, aims and requirements of the study. Those persons who were willing to participate informed the designated Association member, who then forwarded their contact details to the first author. Criteria for inclusion in the study included being over 18 years of age and having had an amputation for at least 4 years prior to the study, to ensure that emotional distress levels would have returned within the norm (Horgan & MacLachlan, 2004). A profile of the participants is presented in Table 1. 2.2 Data Collection The interview schedule consisted of ten semi-structured and open-ended questions. Questions were open-ended to provide participants with an opportunity to elaborate on their responses. Their semi-structured nature allowed for probing and a more conversation-like interview, rather than a rigid question-andanswer approach (Langdrige & Hagger-Johnson, 2009). The participants could opt to be interviewed in Maltese or English. In fact, all opted for the Maltese language. The duration of the tape-recorded interviews was 40-60 minutes and took place in a 55 The lived experience of persons with an amputation Table 1. Participant characteristics. Pseudonym Gender Status Type of amputation Jessica Female Single Below the knee Maria Joseph Mario Female Male Male Married Married Married Above the knee Upper Limb Below the knee Reason Birth defect limb malformation Gangrene Accident Accident Note: Participants’ ages are not included in order to preserve their anonymity. Table 2. Super-ordinate themes. Super-ordinate themes Sub-themes An emotional reaction Impact on self Feelings of significant others A different life Changes in lifestyle Prosthesis and discomfort Searching for meaning Social connections and support Family and friends Professionals as a source of information and care setting suggested by the interviewee. Three core areas were explored, namely participant pre-amputation and post-amputation experiences, as well as external factors such as social support and social integration. Interviews were conducted by the first author between December 2012 and January 2013. 3.1 An Emotional Reaction Participants gave details of their own personal reactions to the amputation, as well as the reactions of family members and significant others. 3.1.1 2.3 Data Analysis Interpretative phenomenological analysis (IPA) was used, following procedures outlined by Smith et al. (2009). This approach was found to be the most suitable to examine personal experiences as it involves a detailed analysis of each case (Smith et al., 2009), and therefore the researcher must take on the role of an interpreter (Smith & Osborn, 2003). IPA aims to engage with people’s reflections on their subjective experiences, paying particular attention to what the experience was like for every participant and how s/he made sense of it (Smith et al., 2009). Studies using IPA are often concerned with existential issues in relation to major life-changing or threatening events, as in the case of the loss of a limb (Smith, 2004). 2.4 Ethics Ethical approval to conduct this study was obtained from the University of Malta Research Ethics Committee (UREC). All participants were asked to sign a consent form prior to the study. They were informed that they could terminate the interview at any point, or decline to answer any question without the need to provide a reason. Pseudonyms were assigned to each participant, and transcripts and audio- recordings were stored in a locked cabinet to safeguard participant confidentiality. 3 Results and Discussion Three super-ordinate themes emerged from participant accounts. These were: a) the Initial Emotional Reaction, b) a Different Life and c) Social Connections and Support. In the following sections, each super-ordinate theme is divided into sub-themes, which are discussed with the use of excerpts from participant accounts and compared to existing literature. http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/EXPERAMP.2.54 Impact on self Most of the research available on the experience of amputation is related to one’s reaction to his or her own amputation, which is dependent on a number of factors such as age, level (below or above the knee/elbow) and time since amputation (Horgan & MacLachlan, 2004). The participants in this study described their initial reaction to their own amputation, occurring as a consequence of an accident or gangrene, in a negative manner. Mario explained: “Initially it was a huge shock. I was crushed!” 1 Maria stated: “I really cried!” 2 Similarly, participants in a study conducted by Senra et al. (2012) expressed feelings of sadness, shock and surprise. However, Jessica, who had her limb amputated due to a malformation in her leg (birth defect), perceived it in a positive light, as exemplified in the following excerpt. “The iron rods around my [malformed] leg were so ugly and so uncomfortable. In fact, the first thing I asked her [my mother] as a child, was whether the amputation would allow me to wear heels and any kinds of shoes. In that aspect I can say it was more of an improvement for me.” 3 This demonstrates that the cause of an amputation may influence one’s reaction towards it (Horgan & MacLachlan, 2004). Participants in this study denied experiencing any long-lasting emo1 “G~all-ewwel kien xokk kbir. Kont imfarrak! ” kemm bkejt” 3 “Il-~adid li kelli ma’ saqajja [l-isfurmata] kien vera ikrah u skomdu. Infatti, l- ewwel ~aġa li staqsejtha [lil ommi], ta’ tifla li kont, kienet jekk inkunx nista’ nilbes takkuna u żraben li rrid. Jigifieri minn dak il-lat jiena kelli improvement.” 2 “Tg~idx http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs 56 The lived experience of persons with an amputation tional distress such as depression and anxiety, even though psychopathological symptoms following amputation are commonly cited in the literature (see Hamill, Carson & Dorahy, 2010). This could be due to “heroic cheerfulness” and denial of any problems (Thomson & Haran, 1984, cited in Horgan & MacLachlan, 2004). Another possible reason could be the presence of sufficient support, a possible mediator between amputation and the psychological outcome (Hamill et al., 2010). All the amputees in this study voiced their preoccupation that the loss of a limb would affect their physical appearance. A research study by Miller and Deathe (2004) highlighted the effect of an amputation on aesthetics. The ability to sustain femininity and wear heels and skirts, was also of particular importance to the females interviewed in this study. In contrast, male participants seemed to be more concerned over the loss of their breadwinner status. “Even aesthetically, as a woman, I want to make sure that it [the prosthesis] looks good, especially if I’m wearing a dress.” 4 (Jessica) Joseph also described how the amputated limb affected his selfpride. “. . . it made me feel inferior, because I could no longer be my family’s breadwinner.” 5 This concern finds support in the literature. Men place more importance on the functional aspect of the limb and their ability to financially provide for the family (Murray & Fox, 2002). During each of their interviews, participants expressed similar opinions that certain character traits such as courage, confidence and being positive helped them to cope, as exemplified by the following excerpt. “. . . but every human mind is different, not everyone thinks alike. Some people get disheartened . . . There were some who gave up and passed away within a fortnight.” 6 (Maria) In a study about positive adjustment to lower limb amputation, Unwin, Kacperek & Clarke (2009) suggested that positive mood was a strong contributor towards a good early adjustment. 3.1.2 Feelings of significant others Participants also described the emotional responses of significant others in their lives. Maria reported how her granddaughter, with whom she had a close relationship, reacted to the amputation. She explained how initially her granddaughter appeared to be afraid of her, but soon realised that her grandmother was still the same person. Jessica, who had undergone her amputation during childhood, mentioned how preoccupied her mother was about the comments and judgements of others, especially children’s. Mario described his experience in the following way: “. . . they [my parents] took it really badly. Especially my father, who had never wanted me to ride a motorcycle [for fear of an accident].” 7 The emotional difficulties that parents endure as a result of their child’s amputation were also highlighted in a case study on child amputation (Judd, 2001). In this study, the participant describes the distress experienced by his mother in the following way: “Mum. . . every time I cry she cries with me.” (p.50) 4 “Anke estetikament b~al speċi mara, nara li nkun irridha [il-prosthesis] pulita, li jekk jien ~a nilbes libsa, tidher pulita.” 5 “ . . . bdejt in~ossni inferjuri jien, g~ax speċi I’m not the breadwinner.” 6 “. . . imma l-mo~~ tal-bniedem, mhux kul~add ja~seb l-istess. Min jaqta’ qalbu. . . kien hemm min qata’ qalbu u fi ~mistax miet.” 7 “. . . ~aduha [il-ġenituri] bi kbira ~afna. Speċjalment missieri, li ma riedni qatt bil-mutur [g~ax jibża’ li jkolli xi inċident].” http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/EXPERAMP.2.54 3.2 A Different Life Findings from the present study showed that living with an amputation may bring about several changes in one’s life. The following section provides details on the participants’ perceived changes in their life, as a result of their amputation. 3.2.1 Changes in lifestyle All participants had similar views on the fact that the amputation had an effect on their independence, posing certain limitations and at times making them dependent on others. “. . . It’s very difficult! You have to depend on many people. I hate it when I can’t do something myself.” 8 (Joseph) Changes in lifestyle were specifically cited by participants who previously enjoyed sports and physical activities. “. . . certain things like running, sports and football. . . The worst thing of them all is not being able to run and play with my children.” 9 (Mario) According to Asano et al. (2008), mobility was the second best predictor of quality of life among individuals who had a lower limb amputation. In this study, participants highlighted the changes in their everyday life brought about by the amputation. Similar to what was reported by Liu et al. (2010), three of the participants described feelings of frustration as a result of their loss of independance and increased dependence on caregivers. Another participant, Maria, described the change in her intimate relationship with her husband following her amputation. She explained that her husband had a fear of illness and was greatly upset by her amputation. This in turn affected their sexual relationship. “It took a very long time until we were able to be sexually intimate again, as a married couple. When he started to initiate intimacy again, I used to just cover it [the stump].” 10 The research literature on sexuality in amputees is limited. However, the present study corroborates the findings of a prominent investigation on the topic by Williamson and Walters (1996), which suggested that sexual activity in a couple was influenced by the perceptions of the spouse regarding the stump or prosthesis. Two of the participants explained that while reactions from people whom they had dated were mostly positive, the parents of such individuals seemed to think otherwise. “Some mothers didn’t want their daughters to date me. One of them even paid for her daughter to move to Australia.” 11 Mario, however, also described how he managed to take advantage of having an amputation. “. . . they [females] felt more comfortable approaching me themselves, because they felt sorry for me. Rather than having to approach them myself, they would come to me . . . I would then choose the best one!” 12 8 “Diffiċli ~afna! Tiddependi minn ~afna nies. Jiena jdejquni affarijiet li ma nistax nag~milhom jien.” 9 “. . . ċertu affarijiet b~al ġiri, sport u futbol. . . L-aktar li t~oss illi ma tistax tiġri u tilg~ab mat-tfal.” 10 “Dam voldieri ma kellniex x’naqsmu flimkien b~ala miż żewġin. Imbag~ad meta beda jerġa’ jersaq lejja, din [is- sieq maqtug~a] kont ng~attiha.” 11 “Ftit ommijiet ma ridux lil binthom to~roġ mieg~i. Wa~da minnhom ~allsitilha [lit-tifla] biex titlaq l-Awstralja.” 12 “. . . bdew i~ossuhom komdi iktar it-tfajliet jiġu fuqi huma, u kienu jaqbdu mieg~i huma milli kont immur jiena fuq tfajla. . . u nag~żel lill-a~jar wa~da! ” http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs 57 The lived experience of persons with an amputation 3.2.2 All participants described how the use of a prosthesis involved a process of acceptance and adjustment, as demonstrated in the following two excerpts. “. . . you start adjusting to the prosthesis by wearing it for half an hour. Then you start going for very short walks.” 13 (Mario) “. . . You must trust your body, you must trust the leg so that you can stand on it and walk. You can’t remain dependent on crutches or a wheelchair.” 14 (Jessica) These comments highlight the importance of the adjustment process of the amputee to his/her prosthetic limb. Saradjian, Thomson and Datta (2008) showed that adjustment to a prosthesis is a personal and individual process. All the participants had different ways of adjusting, with some even customising their prosthesis to suit their own personal needs or wearing it initially for short periods of time. Participants also complained about the discomfort and increased sweating induced by wearing the prosthesis in the hot Maltese summer, as well as the burden of its weight. The former factor was also cited in Saradjian et al.’s (2008) study, in which amputees spoke about the sweating and skin sores caused by wearing a prosthesis in warm temperatures. Although the amputees in the current study did not mention any phantom limb pain, they complained of residual pain and pain in the lower back. Ehde et al. (2000) state that this could be due to altered gait patterns to accomodate the prosthesis. 3.2.3 they are obsessed and blind.” 16 Prosthesis and discomfort Searching for meaning Participants were consistent in reporting that the amputation had resulted in a change in how they viewed their own lives. They explained how they had begun to appreciate life and had learned to live on a day-to-day basis, rather than focusing on the future. This was even more evident in those participants who had lost their limb in an accident, as exemplified by the following statement. “Nowadays, I don’t take anything for granted . . . because I know I could leave here in five minutes, a minute or a second, and my life could end, and so your priorities do change.” 15 (Joseph) A common question asked by all the participants was “Why me?”, representing an effort to understand why the amputation had happened to them. Similarly, Dossey and Dossey (1998) had identified the need for participants in their study to arrive at an understanding to this question. This was reported to often result in spiritual discomfort, which could in turn lead to the complete negation of God in some amputees (Dossey & Dossey, 1998). This was clearly evident in Mario’s statement. “I don’t believe in anything anymore. When I look at people going to church nowadays, they remind me of when we used to watch Red Indian movies, in which people adored wood or stones. . . People go there because 13 “. . . sakemm tibda tidraha [il-prosthesis] just kemm tilbisha toqg~od nofs sieg~a biha. Imbag~ad mixja minn hawn s’hemm u lura . . . ” 14 “Inti trid tafda lil ġismek, trid tafda lis-sieq, li inti trid tirfes fuqha u timxi. Ma tistax tibqa’ tiddependi fuq il-crutches, fuq il-wheelchair.” 15 “Illum il-ġurnata ma sirt nie~u xejn for granted. . . G~ax naf li jiena nista’ no~roġ minn hawn ~ames minuti, minuta jew sekonda o~ra u ~ajti tispiċċa, u allura qisek il-prijoritajiet tieg~ek se jinbidlulek.” http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/EXPERAMP.2.54 Conversely, Maria described a very different experience. “It [the experience of an amputation] brought me closer to God. It made me realise God was by my side all along.” 17 Maria goes on to explain how her experience encouraged her to join her local parish group. A relationship with God/higher power has been demonstrated to offer security and strength (Baldacchino et al., 2013) and to serve as a means of positive coping. Joseph also described how his initial reaction was that of anger towards God. However, over time he realised that God had nothing to do with the incident that befell him. 3.3 Social Connections and Support Participants all highlighted the importance of social support, be it from family and friends, from professionals and staff at hospital, or from other amputees. Furthermore, the importance of support from family and friends was highlighted as one of the most important aspects in their experience. 3.3.1 Family and friends Participants did not report any change in the way they interacted with others. However, they highlighted the importance of re-integration and remaining socially active by joining organisations, or simply by spending time with friends and family. Williams et al. (2004) reported that this is an important aspect of adjusting to life after the loss of a limb. As explained by Mario: “The way you re-integrate in the first few months. . . if you take long to re-integrate, yes, I think you’ll suffer. I was meeting my friends and going to the disco when I was still in a wheelchair. Maintaining your social life is very important!” 18 Furthermore, family members also served to encourage the amputee during the rehabilitation process. For example: “You need to let go. You need to let go of your walking aids. In fact, the more time passed, the more I felt pressured by those around me [to trust the prosthesis].” 19 (Jessica) Other family members improvised ways of enabling the amputee to cope in his/her situation, as described by Joseph: “I was really lucky that my son was able to modify my prosthesis. . . he attached an electric motor which helps me a lot.” 20 Although support from family and friends is considered to be of importance following an amputation (Liu et al., 2010), at certain times, participants felt that they were being over-protected by their families. For instance, Jessica recounted the following episode. 16 “Anqas g~adni nemmen f ’xejn. Meta n~ares lejn in-nies imorru l-knisja llum il-ġurnata jien nist~ajjilhom meta konna naraw il-films tal-Indjani, jaduraw xi injama jew ġebla. . . In-nies imorru hemmhekk g~ax ossessjonati u g~omja.” 17 “ Ġabitni [l-esperjenza tal-amputazzjoni] aktar viċin talMulej. Ġeg~litni nirrealizza li Alla kien mieg~i l-~in kollu.” 18 “Kif tintegra l-ewwel ftit xhur. . . jekk inti ddum biex terġa’ tintegra, iva, na~seb li tbati. Jiena kont niltaqa’ ma’ s~abi u mmur id-diskow, kont g~adni bil-wheelchair. Importanti ~afna li ż żomm social life! ” 19 “Inti trid you let go. Li inti dawn l-aids you let go of them. Aktar ma beda jg~addi ż-żmien iktar ~assejtha l-pressure minn dawk ta’ madwari [biex nafda l-prosthesis].” 20 “Jiena kont lucky ~afna wkoll illi kelli t-tifel kien g~enni biex irranġajna l-prosthesis. . . g~amilhieli bl-electric motor u tg~inni ~afna jiġifieri.” http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs 58 The lived experience of persons with an amputation “Last time my mum saw me climbing up the ladder, to change a light bulb. She started panicking and asked me to get down [due to fear of a fall].” 21 “They all give their 100% to see you satisfied. . . they provide the support that you need. They see you as a person and not a number.” 26 (Maria) The participants in Hamill et al.’s (2010) investigation similarly identified this over-protective behaviour as frustrating for amputees. The amputees in the current study spoke about the need to form relationships with others in the same situation as themselves. They also highlighted the importance of being able to share and encourage each other throughout the experience. One of the participants had been introduced to another amputee her age before the amputation. In their review on rehabilitation following major traumatic amputations, Eldar and Jelić (2003) mentioned the importance of early involvement of rehabilitation health professionals who can provide useful advice on rehabilitaion, prosthetic options and ways on maintaining muscle strength, as well as self-care skills. “When they introduce you to an amputee who is already walking, you get to see the final results.” 22 (Jessica) Similarly, Joseph explained: “That [meeting another amputee] helped me so much, because automatically, when you see someone who has been in the same situation, you can’t help listening to them.” 23 Other participants managed to meet up with other amputees through information meetings and sessions held by local associations for amputees or when visiting the Orthotic and Prosthetics Unit or Physiotherapy Outpatients Department following their amputation. These experiences tie in with Liu et al.’s (2010) finding that peer support is helpful in providing relief from emotional distress, and reducing isolation. 3.3.2 Professionals as a source of information and care Health professionals, such as doctors, nurses and physiotherapists, were also perceived as an important source of support. Participants seemed to mention different aspects of support when describing their experience in hospital. Joseph described the support that he received from hospital staff as follows: “I cannot complain about anything. When I was in hospital, I had good support because I had doctors, nurses. . . they were all supportive. They were always there to help change my dressings [following the amputation] and provided for my needs.” 24 In contrast, Maria held a very different viewpoint. “Nothing! While in hospital I found no emotional support at all. They give you no information on the procedure, the way it will affect your life and how to cope.” 25 Despite the mixed reactions regarding the support given during their stay in hospital, all participants agreed on the quality of support given at the Physiotherapy Outpatients Department and the Orthotic and Prosthetics Unit, which they had been making use of following their amputation. 4 Limitations The present study addresses the lacuna in local literature on the lived experiences of persons with lower and upper limb amputations. There is, however, a need for further research which is longitudinal in nature and explores the lived experiences of study participants over a period time. The limited time frame for this study posed limitations on the number of participants with whom interviews were conducted, and on the possibility of second interviews. Also, the fact that only one of the four participants had an upper limb amputation limits the ability of this study to delve into the experience of amputees of the upper limb. A more diverse sample that included more upper limb amputees would have allowed further understanding of the experiences of persons having different types of amputation. 5 Conclusions This study explored the lived experience of four Maltese amputees. It identified the various reactions and changes which occur following the loss of a limb. Being specific to the Maltese population, it contributes to a better understanding of the experiences of persons with an amputation in the local context. It also explores the needs of amputees in Malta as well as the type of services provided. Furthermore, it highlights the importance of family members’ and friends’ psychological and physical support as an essential part of individuals’ adjustment to their new life as amputees and the use of their prostheses. 6 Acknowledgments The authors would like to thank the amputees who gave their time to participate in this study and who shared their experience with them. The authors would also like to express their sincere gratitude towards Dr Josianne Scerri for offering her invaluable advice on an earlier version of this paper. 7 Funding This research study has received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or non-profit sectors. 8 Conflicts of Interest The authors report no conflicts of interest. 21 “L-a~~ar darba ommi ratni tiela’ fuq sellum, biex inbiddel bozza. Bdiet tippanikja u tg~idli biex ninżel [ma mmurx naqa’].” 22 “Inti meta jlaqqg~uk ma’ xi ~add li diġà qed jimxi, inti qieg~da tara l-final results.” 23 “Dik g~enitni ~afna, g~ax inti qisu awtomatikament meta tara lil xi ~add fl-istess sitwazzjoni, tismag~hom bla ma trid.” 24 “Ma nistax ingerger. Mindu kont l-isptar kelli sapport tajjeb g~ax kelli t-tobba, kelli n-nersis. . . kollha kienu ta’ sapport. Dejjem kienu hemm biex ibiddluli l-faxex [wara l-amputazzjoni] u jipprovduli li kelli bżonn.” 25 “Xejn! L-isptar ma sibt l-ebda sapport emozzjonali. Ma jag~tuk l-ebda informazzjoni dwar l-operazzjoni, kif se taffettwalek ~ajtek u kif tista’ tikkowpja.” http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/EXPERAMP.2.54 References Asano, M., Rushton, P., Miller, W. C. & Deathe, B. A. (2008) Predictors of quality of life among individuals who have a lower limb amputation. Prosthetics and Orthotics International, 32(2), pp. 231-243. Baldacchino, D., Torskenaes, K., Kalfoss, M., Borg, J., Tonna, A., Debattista, C., Decelis, N. & Mifsud, R. (2013) Spiri26 “Kollha jag~tuk il-100% biex jarawk sodisfatta. . . jag~tuk kull sapport li hemm bżonn. Jarawk b~ala persuna u mhux b~ala numru.” http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs The lived experience of persons with an amputation 59 tual coping in rehabilitation - a comparative study: part 2. British Journal of Nursing, 22(7), pp. 402-408. Cassar, M. (2012) National Amputee Database (June 2011 - end of May 2012). Pieta’, Malta: Amputee Rehabilitation Unit. Desmond, D. M. & MacLachlan, M. (2006) Affective distress and amputation-related pain among older men with long-term, traumatic limb amputations. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, 31(4), pp. 362-368. Dossey, B. M. & Dossey, L. (1998). Body-mind-spirit: attending to holistic care. The American Journal of Nursing, 98(8), pp. 35-38. Ehde M, D., Czerniecki M, J., Smith G, D., Campbell M, K., Edwards T, W., Jensen P, M. & Robinson R, L. (2000) Chronic phantom sensations, phantom pain, residual limb pain, and other regional pain after lower limb amputation. Archives of Physical Medicine, 81, pp. 1039-1044. Eldar, R. & Jelić, M. (2003) The association of rehabilitation and war. Disability and Rehabilitation, 25(18), pp. 1019-1023. Fremgen, B. F. & Frucht, S. S. (2009) Medical Terminology: A living language (4th ed.) NJ: Prentice Hall. Government of Malta (2011) Significant Reduction in Major Lower Limb Amputations in Malta. [Online] Available from: https://www.gov.mt/en/Government/Press% 20Releases/Pages/2011/November/30/pr2328.aspx. [Accessed 22nd October 2014]. Hamill, R., Carson, S. & Dorahy, M. (2010) Experiences of psychosocial adjustment within 18 months of amputation: an interpretative phenomenological analysis. Disability and Rehabilitation, 32(9), pp. 729-740. Horgan, O. & MacLachlan, M. (2004) Psychosocial adjustment to lower limb amputation: a review. Disability and Rehabilitation, 26(14/15), pp. 837-850. Judd, D. (2001) ‘To walk the last bit on my own’ - narcissistic independence or identification with good objects: issues of loss for a 13-year-old who had an amputation. Journal of Child Psychotherapy, 27(1), pp. 47-67. doi: 10.1080/00754170010023947. Langdrige, D. & Hagger-Johnson, G. (2009) Introduction to Research Methods and Data Analysis in Psychology. London: Pearson Education Ltd. Liu, F., Williams, R. M., Liu, H. E. & Chien, N.-H. (2010) The lived experience of persons with lower extremity amputation. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 19, pp. 2152-2161. Miller, W. C. & Deathe, A. B. (2004) A prospective study examining balance confidence among individuals with lower limb amputation. Disability and Rehabilitation, 26(14-15), pp. 875-881. Murray, C. D. & Fox, J. (2002) Body image and prosthesis satisfaction in the lower limb amputee. Disability and Rehabilitation, 24(17), pp. 925-931. Paudel, B., Shrestha, B. K. & Banskota, A. K. (2005) Two faces of major lower limb amputations. Kathmandu University Medical Journal, 3(11), pp. 212-216. Saradjian, A., Thomson R, A. & Datta, D. (2008) The experience of men using an upper limb prosthesis following amputation: positive coping and minimizing feeling different. Disability and Rehabilitation, 30(11), pp. 871-883. Senra, H., Oliveira, R. A., Leal, I. & Vieira, C. (2012) Beyond the body image: a qualitative study on how adults experience lower limb amputation. Clinical Rehabilitation, 26(2), pp. 180-191. Sinha R, van den Heuvel, W.J. & Arokiasamy, P. (2011) Factors affecting quality of life in lower limb amputees. Prosthetics and Orthotics International, 35(1), pp. 90-96. Smith, J. A. (2004) Reflecting on the development of interpretative phenomenological analysis and its contribution to qualitative research in psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 1(1), pp. 39-54. Smith, J. A., Flowers, P. & Larkin, M. (2009) Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis: Theory, method and research. London: Sage Publications. Smith, J. A. & Osborn, M. (2003) Interpretative phenomenological analysis. In J.A. Smith (Ed.) Qualitative Psychology: A practical guide to research methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Unwin, J., Kacperek, L. & Clarke, C. (2009) A prospective study of positive adjustment to lower limb amputation. Clinical Rehabilitation, 23(11), pp. 1044-1050. Van der Meij, W. K. N. (1995) No Leg To Stand On: Historical relation between amputations surgery and prostheseology. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. The Netherlands: University of Groningen. Williams, M. R., Ehde, M. D., Smith, G. D., Czerniecki, M. J., Hoffman, J. A. & Robinson, R. L. (2004) A two-year longitudinal study of social support following amputation. Disability and Rehabilitation, 26(14/15), pp. 862-874. Williamson, G. M. & Walters, A. S. (1996) Percieved impact of limb amputation on sexual activity: a study of adult amputees. The Journal of Sex Research , 33(3), pp. 221- 230. http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/EXPERAMP.2.54 http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Malta Journal of Health Sciences Online http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Doi: http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COCHIMP.2.60 Research paper SPEECH AND LANGUAGE SKILLS OF MALTESE CHILDREN WITH BILATERAL COCHLEAR IMPLANTS: THREE CASE STUDIES Martina Galdes, Daniela Gatt Department of Communication Therapy, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Malta, Msida, Malta Abstract. The purpose of this study was to document the speech and language skills of three Maltese children with bilateral cochlear implants. One child was simultaneously implanted and had a hearing age (HA) of 2;10 years at the time of testing, another was sequentially implanted and had a HA of 3;06 years, while the third child was sequentially implanted and had a HA of 5;03 years. Maltese standardised speech and language assessments were used to gather information on the children’s speech and language skills, with data being collected during their speech and language therapy sessions. Following data transcription and analysis, the participants’ speech and language abilities were compared to those of their HA- and chronological age (CA)-matched peers using available norms for Maltese children. Additional information regarding the children’s speech and language history prior to and post-implantation was also collected, providing a holistic overview of the participants’ speech and language development. Results indicated that the children presented with speech and language delay when compared to their CA-matched peers. Variations across the participants were found in specific language skills. Similarities in language patterns were also noted, including expressive abilities in advance of receptive skills. These findings extend the limited data on the speech and language skills of Maltese children with cochlear implants, with comparison to norms for typically-developing children being a novel approach to research in this area. Keywords speech, language, hearing loss, dren, cochlear implants, bilingualism 1 Maltese chil- Introduction In order to receive language input, children mainly rely on their hearing abilities. If the latter are impaired, a temporary or permanent hearing difficulty may result. Sensorineural hearing loss (SNHL) is a permanent type of hearing impairment that has often led to fitting of hearing aids. However, depending on the severity of the hearing loss, the latter may not be ideal because Correspondence to: Martina Galdes (martinagaldes@gmail.com) Received: 02.09.2014 - Revised: 23.11.2014 - Accepted: 24.11.2014 - Published: 17.12.2014 c 2014 The authors they amplify but do not clarify incoming sound for the hearingimpaired individual (Welling, 2010). Thus, in cases where the individual is unable to comprehend spoken language through conventional hearing aids, cochlear implants (CIs) might be an alternative (Roland, 2000). Indeed, CIs are a major technological attainment to treat deafness (Niparko et al., 2010). They allow the development of auditory skills in naturalistic, albeit supported environments and are able to support the acquisition of speech, language and communication (Archbold, 2002). Children with a unilateral CI are reported to have difficulty understanding speech in noise and localising sound (Scherf et al., 2009). Bilateral implantation can overcome these limitations. Various researchers (e.g. Battista & Highhouse, 2011; Dunn et al., 2008; Litovsky et al., 2006; Sammeth, 2007; Therres, 2012) assigned three effects to binaural listening as a result of bilateral implantation, namely the head shadow effect, the binaural summation effect and the binaural squelch effect. In the head shadow effect, both ears are active. The ear with the more favourable signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) is immediately accessible and the person can selectively attend to this ear (Therres, 2012). Binaural summation enables the individual to perceive the sounds presented to both ears louder. Specifically, an improvement of 3 dB is known to occur for binaural listeners, leading to better speech perception (Therres, 2012). Furthermore, when input is received from both ears, the brain and the auditory system will be able to combine inputted information from both ears and produce a better central representation, providing a clear separation of the speech and noise signals (Therres, 2012). This is referred to as the binaural squelch effect. With these reported benefits, the cochlear implant enhances the individual’s hearing experience which, as a result, would help the acquisition of speech and language. Individuals with bilateral implants are implanted either sequentially or simultaneously. Sharma et al. (2007) compared the P1 latency, an evoked potential which analyses the auditory cortical response to auditory stimuli, in 20 children who received a bilateral implantation during the first 3;06 years of life. Ten children had a simultaneous implantation while the rest received sequential bilateral implants. In both groups, the average P1 latency was measured on implant activation and at two-month intervals postimplantation up to 15 months. The P1 latency for both groups was within normal limits by 3.5 months post-implantation. This suggests that any bilateral implantation that occurs within the first 3;06 years takes place within a highly plastic central auditory nervous system. Despite the sensitive time proposed by Sharma et al. (2007), several studies have shown that there should be no age limit for the second implantation. Scherf et al. (2009) observed the auditory abilities and speech performance of 35 children with sequen- Speech and language skills of Maltese children with bilateral cochlear implants: three case studies tial bilateral CIs following three years of bilateral implant use. CIs offered improved auditory skills to all children, even those sequentially implanted after the age of 6;00 years, although it emerged that the latter took longer than those implanted before the age of 6;00 years to demonstrate bilateral benefit. The older children, that is, those implanted after the age of 6;00 years, obtained better speech recognition levels. Additionally, at two to three years post-bilateral implantation, almost all children benefited in noisy situations. In another study, Kim, Kim and Jeong (2013) investigated the functional benefits of sequential bilateral cochlear implantation in 42 children having a mean inter-stage interval of 5;06 years between the two implants. Average ages were 4;02 years at the first implant and 9;07 years at the second implant. The participants were grouped according to the inter-stage intervals. The second and third group, those with an inter-stage interval of 5;00 and 6;09 years, and 7;00 and 9;09 years respectively, obtained comparable results in both quiet and noise tests to the first group, who had a shorter interval of 3;00 and 4;09 years, two years following bilateral use. Additionally, speech performance in noise improved even in the older children following prolonged use of bilateral CIs. These results show that the binaural hearing experience plays an important role in the speech outcome. On the other hand, simultaneous implantation enhances children’s speech and language acquisition and decreases their ability difference in comparison to their chronological age (CA)-matched peers. Wie (2010) investigated a group of 21 children with cochlear implants two years post-simultaneous implantation. The majority of these acquired receptive and expressive language skills at a faster pace than expected in comparison to their hearing age (HA)-matched peers. Apart from the type of implant, language skills depend on the severity of the SNHL, the age of identification and implantation, as well as the child’s cognitive abilities (Welling, 2010). For example, Niparko et al. (2010), who studied 188 children who underwent cochlear implantation before the age of five, reported greater rates of improvement in children with better residual hearing and higher socioeconomic status. Additionally, Geers, Nicholas and Sedey (2003), who studied 181 8;00- and 9;00-year-old children who also received a cochlear implant by the age of five, found that the child’s learning ability contributed greatly to the linguistic outcome. Participants with average learning ability were reported to show language levels similar to those of their HA-matched peers. Similar findings were reported for two Maltese children with a cochlear implant (Tabone, 2004). However, Geers et al. (2003) further stated that children with average or above average intelligence performed similarly to their CA-matched peers. Together, these results show that the linguistic outcome is multifactorial. Speech production is influenced by the child’s age at onset of deafness, time of implantation as well as the mode of communication and rehabilitation (Robbins, 2000). CIs improve speech perception and intelligibility (Robbins, 2006), both of which eventually improve speech production. In the two Maltese children with CI studied by Tabone (2004), speech development was identified as delayed, implying typical developmental stages emerging with a time lag, rather than as deviant. Furthermore, the Maltese-speaking child observed by Xuereb (2004) at 4;06 years post-implantation experienced speech and language delay when compared to her CA-matched peers on tests of speech and language. However, the tests used in both case studies were not standardised on the Maltese-speaking population, so interpretations of results need to be considered as tentative. Importantly, speech and language assessments standardised on the Maltese hearing population have recently become available, enabling comparative evaluation of the speech and language skills of children with CIs in relation to those of Maltese hearing children. Such a comparison was the purpose of the current study. The following research questions were addressed: http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COCHIMP.2.60 61 1. What speech and language skills are manifested by three Maltese children with bilateral CIs? 2. Are the speech and language skills of these three Maltese children comparable to those shown by HA- or CA-matched typicallydeveloping children? More specifically, the study documented the participants’ phonetic inventory, phonological error patterns, inconsistency in word production, verbal comprehension and expression, aiming to identify their strengths and weaknesses. It also sought to determine whether their skills were the same as, similar to or different from the speech and language abilities documented in a normative sample of Maltese children. This information was needed to indicate whether the speech and language development of the three Maltese participants with CIs was age-appropriate, delayed or deviant in relation to hearing children, while acknowledging the limited generalisability of results to other Maltese children with CIs. 2 2.1 Methods Participants All speech-language pathologists working in the public health service were asked to distribute an information sheet among potential participants in their current or previous caseloads. Children considered as potential participants had bilateral cochlear implants, a HA ranging between 2;00 and 6;00 years and were Maltese-English bilinguals. Children reported to have other developmental disabilities were excluded. Three children having a HA in the range of 2;10-5;03 years were identified as participants. Table 1 lists background information on each child. 2.2 Research design A multiple case study design was implemented. Since this design is known to enable researchers to investigate real-life events holistically (Yin, 2009), its implementation in this study allowed the chosen participants to be studied in depth. The status of their speech and language development was assessed through the use of standardised assessments and results were analysed quantitatively and qualitatively. Furthermore, previous audiology and speech and language progress reports were accessed for background information that could help in the interpretation of results. 2.3 Method All data were collected by the first author. Background information was obtained by reviewing the participants’ medical and audiology file. Details on their hearing assessment results and implantation were noted. Their HA was subsequently calculated. Speech and language data were then collected through the use of two standardised assessments, outlined below, which enabled each participant’s scores to be compared with population norms. The Maltese-English Speech Assessment (MESA) (Grech, Dodd & Franklin, 2011) assesses speech and taps into articulation, phonology, consistency, diadochokinesis, consonantal clusters and multisyllabic words, as well as single oral movements and sequenced movements. In the present study, only the articulation, phonology and consistency subtests were administered. The articulation and phonology subtests involved naming 42 coloured pictures eliciting all the English and Maltese vowels and consonants (Grech & Dodd, 2008). This enabled compilation of their phonetic inventory and the tabulation of phonemes. Percentage Consonants Correct (PCC) and Percentage Vowels Correct (PVC) scores were calculated and error patterns were described and analysed. In the consistency subtest, the participants were to name 17 pictures, three times on separate trials. An inconsistency score was then computed. http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Speech and language skills of Maltese children with bilateral cochlear implants: three case studies Table 1. Background information on the participants chosen for the study Child 1 Child 2 Chronological age (CA) during time of assessment (years) 4;08 6;10 Hearing age (HA) during time of assessment (years) 2;10 3;06 Type of implantation Simultaneous Sequential bilateral bilateral Chronological age (CA) at first implant (years) 1;10 3;04 Chronological age (CA) at second implant (years) 1;10 5;10 Interval between both implants (years) -2;06 Language acquisition* Sequential Sequential bilingual bilingual Primary language Maltese Maltese Gender Male Male 62 Child 3 7;06 5;03 Sequential bilateral 2;03 3;01 0;10 Simultaneous bilingual English Female *Child 1 and Child 2 were referred to as sequential bilinguals since, in line with Iglesias and Rojas’s description (2012), they were predominantly exposed to Maltese (L1) from an early age and then to English (L2) at school. Child 3 was referred to as simultaneous bilingual following Genesee’s (1989, cited in Iglesias and Rojas, 2012) definition of concurrent acquisition of more than one language during the time of primary language development. In fact, she was consistently exposed to both Maltese and English at home, although English input was reported to be more predominant. The Language Assessment for Maltese Children (LAMC) (Grech, Franklin & Dodd, 2011) examines language skills, tapping into verbal comprehension, narrative vocabulary and grammar, sentence imitation, phonological awareness and pragmatics. It also includes a voice and fluency checklist. Participants were administered the verbal comprehension, narrative vocabulary and grammar subtests. They were first asked questions related to the story previously narrated by the assessor using a picture stimulus book. They were subsequently encouraged to retell the story with the help of the stimulus book. Child 1 and Child 2 were assessed at their Speech and Language Clinic. Child 3, who no longer needed speech and language therapy, was assessed at home. Child 1 and 3 completed both assessments in one session, which took approximately 1.5 hours. Since Child 2 was noted to be distracted following the first part of assessment, the test battery was spread over two sessions lasting an hour each. Assessment duration was typical as during the standardisation of MESA and LAMC, administration of each test lasted up to 60 minutes. Assessment sessions were audio-recorded. Information from the children’s speech and language file about performance during previous speech and language therapy sessions was also recorded. These details provided a holistic background to the quantitative speech and language data collected. The participants were free to speak in their preferred language during assessment. The primary language was taken to be the language used spontaneously and predominantly by the child. The speech data were transcribed phonetically while the language data were transcribed orthographically. 2.4 Ethical considerations Prior to commencing data collection, the study was approved by the University of Malta Research Ethics Committee, ensuring that the participants’ rights were safeguarded. The legal guardians of the potential participants were given an information sheet through their speech-language pathologist, explaining the children’s potential involvement in the study. For Child 3, the information sheet was passed on through the speech-language pathologist previously working with the child. Upon understanding the children’s involvement in the study and agreeing to their participation, parents completed a consent form. http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COCHIMP.2.60 3 Results Descriptive statistics, including raw scores, means, standard deviations, z-scores and percentiles for each participant on every speech and language subtest, were calculated. The children’s individual performance was compared to that of typically-developing HA- and CA- matched peers involved in assessment standardisation. However, the available norms for both assessments cover the 3;00-6;00-year age group, with the exception of the verbal comprehension subtest, for which norms begin at 2;06 years. Grech, Franklin and Dodd (2011) explain that children aged 2;00-2;06 years carried out only a small part of the language assessment. The resulting data did not warrant further analysis. Consequently, given Child 1’s actual HA of 2;10 years, he was compared to HAmatched peers aged 3;00 years. Analogously, Child 2 and Child 3, whose actual CAs were 6;10 and 7;05 years respectively, were both compared to CA-matched peers aged 6;00 years, as this was the ceiling age of both assessments. When the MESA was standardised, children were divided into monolingual and bilingual groups. As defined by Grech, Dodd and Franklin (2011), the monolingual group consisted of children exposed to Maltese at home and to English at school, whilst children in the bilingual group had simultaneous exposure to both languages at home. Hence, the results of Child 1 and 2 were compared to the monolingual group norming data, whilst Child 3’s data were compared to the bilingual group. Each participant’s phonetic inventory was determined first. According to Ingram (1981, cited in Grech, 2006), a criterion for a sound to become part of the child’s inventory is its frequency, in that it has to occur at least once in a mixture of 25 words selected at random. The phonetic inventory was noted from the articulation subtest of the MESA by listing the sounds that the child produced. These sounds are hereafter referred to as phones. The number of phonemes was subsequently counted from performance on the phonology subtest. For a sound to be considered a phoneme it should be used regularly and contrastively and it should occur in minimal or near minimal pairs (Grech, 2006). The phonology subtest also enabled calculation of the PCC and PVC scores. Finally, the inconsistency score was derived from the participants’ inconsistent production of words on the consistency subtest. http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Speech and language skills of Maltese children with bilateral cochlear implants: three case studies 63 Table 2. Child 1’s PCC, PVC and inconsistency scores (means and standard deviations (SD) quoted from the MESA manual) with the equivalent z-scores and percentiles derived for HA- and CA-matched peers Raw Score Quoted Quoted Derived Derived Mean SD z-score Percentile PCC HA: 2;10 years 91.45 89.6 9.20 0.201 58 CA: 4;08 years 91.45 97.23 2.68 -2.157 2 PVC HA: 2;10 years 98.99 99.18 3.29 -0.058 48 CA: 4;08 years 98.99 99.89 0.35 -2.609 0.5 PVC without outlier HA: 2;10 years 100 99.18 3.29 0.250 60 CA: 4;08 years 100 99.89 0.345 0.320 63 Inconsistency Score HA: 2;10 years 29.41 15.05 18.59 0.748 77 CA: 4;08 years 29.41 4.03 7.79 3.258 100 Table 3. Child 1’s phonological error processes, frequency of occurrence and age expected, based on data by Grech, Dodd and Franklin (2011) Phonological Error Processes Error Processes Frequency Systemic Error Processes Voicing 2 Backing 2 Fronting 2 Lateralisation of /r/ 2 Nasalisation 1 Structural Error Processes Weak syllable deletion 1 Final consonant deletion 1 Initial consonant deletion 1 Compensatory vowel lengthening 1 Consonant harmony 1 Epenthesis 1 Gemination reduction 1 Cluster formation 1 at which they are Typically expected 3;00 - 3;05 years 3;00 - 3;05 years 3;00 - 6;00 years 3;00 - 4;11 years MEP* 3;00 - 3;05 years 3;00 - 4;11 years 3;00 - 3;11 years MEP* MEP* MEP* MEP* MEP* *Minimally used error pattern (MEP): an error pattern used by less than 10% of children between 3;00 and 6;00 years 3.1 Child 1 Child 1 produced a complete phone repertoire, 22 phones, which included all the Maltese consonants documented by Azzopardi (1981) (cited in Borg & Azzopardi-Alexander, 1997). Given the child’s CA (4;08 years) and Grech and Dodd’s (2008) MESA results indicating that both sequential and simultaneous MalteseEnglish bilingual children produce all phones by 4;00 years, this was an expected finding. Therefore, Table 2 shows Child 1’s raw scores on the MESA speech subtests in comparison to his HA- and CA-matched peers and Table 3 lists his phonological error processes. In the light of his CA, Child 1 should have mastered the majority of the adult phonemes (Grech, Dodd & Franklin, 2011). The child did show a complete phonemic inventory by producing 22 phonemes. Yet, his phonological system was still developing, with his performance similar to that of his HA-matched peers. On PCC, he scored close to the average of his HA-matched peers yet well below average in comparison to his CA-matched peers. A similar performance was noted on PVC. Although Child 1 performed below average in PVC when compared to both groups, there was a slight difference between his raw score and the mean score for http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COCHIMP.2.60 both age-matched groups. This was due to an omitted vowel. Without this outlier, the child would have obtained a full score. Also, the possibility of an inconsistent phonological disorder was eliminated as the child obtained an inconsistency score lower than 40% (see Grech, Dodd & Franklin, 2011). However, the discrepancy between the child’s score and the mean score for both age groups seems to suggest that he was still learning the Maltese and English phonological systems. As discussed by Grech and Dodd (2008), inconsistent production increases with initial exposure to two languages. Child 1 produced five systemic and eight structural error processes. None were considered frequently-used error patterns. Yet, they are potential error patterns. When compared to his HAmatched group, seven error processes used by the child were all typically used at 3;00 years. Alternatively, when compared to his CA peers, voicing, backing, weak syllable deletion and initial consonant deletion should have been eradicated by the age of 4;08 years. Furthermore, half the error processes used by Child 1 (see minimally used error patterns http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Speech and language skills of Maltese children with bilateral cochlear implants: three case studies 64 Table 4. Child 1’s scores on verbal comprehension and narrative ideas (means and standard deviations (SD) quoted from the LAMC manual) with the equivalent z-scores and percentiles derived for HA- and CA-matched peers Raw Score Quoted Mean HA: 2;10 years CA: 4;08 years 11.50 11.50 10.61 16.80 HA: 2;10 years CA: 4;08 years 14 14 0.92 9 Quoted SD Derived z-score Derived Percentile 3.38 1.87 0.263 -2.834 60 0.2 1.44 6.14 9.083 0.814 100 79 Verbal Comprehension Score Narrative Ideas Score Table 5. Child 2’s PCC, PVC and inconsistency scores (means and standard deviations quoted from the MESA manual) with the equivalent z-scores and percentiles derived for HA- and CA-matched peers Raw Score Quoted Mean HA: 3;06 years CA: 6;10 years 96.32 96.32 93.35 95.53 HA: 3;06 years CA: 6;10 years 98.85 98.85 99.99 100 HA: 3;06 years CA: 6;10 years 100 100 99.99 100 HA: 3;06 years CA: 6;10 years 35.29 35.29 10.60 3.19 Quoted SD Derived z-score Derived Percentile 6.47 5.58 0.456 0.135 68 55 0.02 0∗ -57 - 0 - 0.02 0∗ 0.5 - 69 - 14.96 6.65 1.65 4.827 95 100 PCC PVC PVC without outlier Inconsistency Score *A standard deviation of 0 indicates no variation around the mean score, so that the z-score and percentile could not be computed. (MEP) in Table 3) were typical between 2;00 and 3;00 years, as documented in Grech’s (1998) study. This further suggested delayed phonological development in comparison to the child’s CA. Child 1’s verbal comprehension and narrative ideas scores are shown in Table 4. Child 1 understood simple commands and ‘why’ questions. However, comprehension of ‘who’ and ‘what’ was difficult. Therefore, his understanding of wh-questions was still emerging. As documented by Sax and Weston (2007), typically-developing children between 2;00 and 3;00 years comprehend and produce simple wh-questions. However, Friedmann and Szterman (2011) reported that the understanding and production of wh-questions of 11 Hebrew-speaking children, some of whom were fitted with binaural hearing aids and others with a CI, was weaker compared to typically-developing children. Child 1 also understood basic colour terms, but knowledge of numbers and counting was still emerging. Child 1’s expression was mainly in Maltese. He used proper names and target lexical nouns appropriately with correct singular and plural forms. Various lexical target verbs and the definite and assimilated article were also employed appropriately. There was evidence of the use of the Maltese negative form mhux (not), the English lexeme ‘no’ and spatial and pronominal use of deictic expressions. On 90% of occasions, he produced correct person and number agreement and adequate sentence structures. On the other hand, Child 1 made limited use of http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COCHIMP.2.60 adjectives and adverbs, conjunctions, prepositions, direct and indirect pronouns. He did not use possessives and showed gender agreement difficulties. The masculine form was used for 90% of the time. Infrequent and incorrect use of feminine forms possibly indicated their emergence at the time. A similar trend was documented by Coppini (2002), who investigated gender agreement between nouns and adjectives in Maltese children. 3.2 Child 2 Child 2 produced 22 phones, which made up a complete phone repertoire in view of his HA (3;06 years) and CA (7;10 years). Table 5 shows his raw scores on the MESA speech subtests in comparison to his HA- and CA-matched peers. Table 6 lists the phonological error processes. Child 2 produced 22 phonemes, thus he had also developed a complete phonemic inventory. However, his phonological development was still emerging. On PCC, Child 2 performed moderately and slightly above average compared to his HA- and CA-matched peers respectively. Furthermore, on PVC, the slight difference between the child’s score and that of the normative population was mainly due to the incorrect production of /mEr5/ (mirror), for which he said /m5r5/ (woman). Without this outlier, Child 2 obtained a full score, further indicating no vowel difficulty. His inconsistency score indicated a higher possibility of an inconsistent http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Speech and language skills of Maltese children with bilateral cochlear implants: three case studies disorder. Child 2 exhibited a chronological mismatch in the phonological processes observed. He produced five systemic error processes and no structural error processes. When compared to his HAmatched group, Child 2 produced developmental phonological processes typical of children younger than his HA. Alternatively, when compared to his CA-matched peers, backing, voicing and stopping should have been eliminated by 3;11 years. Therefore, a phonological delay was observed. Conversely, as expected by his CA, Child 2 did not exhibit structural error processes. Thus, he followed some patterns expected at 6;10 years. Child 2’s verbal comprehension and narrative ideas scores are shown in Table 7. Child 2 understood simple commands and ‘why’ questions. Comprehension of ‘where’ and ‘what’ questions, as well as knowledge of numbers and counting was emerging. He made 65 appropriate regular lexical choices of nouns and produced the Maltese negative form ma (verb)x. He made regular and adequate use of articles and conjunctions. On 90% of occasions, he also made correct verb choices and used adequate pronouns. The direct object was used appropriately but infrequently, on less than 30% of occasions. Child 2 showed correct gender, number and person agreement. On the other hand, he replaced proper names with nouns and made adequate use of prepositions while omitting others. His use of adjectives and adverbs amounted to less than 30% of their required usage. He rarely employed spatial deictic expressions and sentence structure was occasionally disrupted. The indirect object was incorrectly used due to person and number disagreement and possessive terms did not emerge. Table 6. Child 2’s phonological error processes, frequency of occurrence and age at which they are expected based on data by Grech, Dodd and Franklin (2011) Phonological Error Processes Error Processes Frequency Typically expected Systemic Error Processes Backing 3 3;00 - 3;05 years Devoicing 1 3;00 - 3;05 years Stopping 1 3;00 - 3;11 years Delteralisation of /l/ 1 MEP* Frictation 1 MEP* *Minimally used error pattern (MEP): an error pattern used by less than 10% of children between 3;00 and 6;00 years Table 7. Child 2’s scores on verbal comprehension and narrative ideas (means and standard deviations (SD) quoted from the LAMC manual) with the equivalent z-scores and percentiles derived for HA- and CA-matched peers Raw Score Quoted Quoted Derived Derived Mean SD z-score Percentile Verbal Comprehension Score HA: 3;06 years 10.5 14.87 2.50 -1.748 4 CA: 6;10 years 10.5 17.21 2.43 -2.761 0.2 Narrative Ideas Score HA: 3;06 years 16 2.88 3.19 4.113 100 CA: 6;10 years 16 16.86 6.30 -0.137 45 Table 8.Child 3’s PCC, PVC and inconsistency scores (means and standard deviations quoted from the MESA manual) with the equivalent z-scores and percentiles derived for HA- and CA-matched peers Raw Score Quoted Quoted Derived Derived Mean SD z-score Percentile PCC HA: 5;03 years 100 99.34 2.21 0.299 62 CA: 7;06 years 100 99.18 1.24 0.661 75 PVC HA: 5;03 years 100 99.94 0.21 0.280 61 CA: 7;06 years 100 99.74 0.83 0.313 62 Inconsistency Score HA: 5;03 years 5.88 0.45 1.63 3.331 100 CA: 7;06 years 5.88 1.32 2.62 1.740 96 http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COCHIMP.2.60 http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Speech and language skills of Maltese children with bilateral cochlear implants: three case studies 66 Table 9. Child 3’s verbal comprehension and narrative ideas scores (means and standard deviations (SD) quoted from the LAMC manual) with the equivalent z-scores and percentiles derived for HA- and CA-matched peers Raw Score Quoted Quoted Derived Derived Mean SD z-score Percentile Verbal Comprehension Score HA: 5;03 years 18.5 19 1 -0.500 31 CA: 7;06 years 18.5 18 1.41 0.355 64 Narrative Ideas Score HA: 5;03 years 35 29.33 4.04 1.403 92 CA: 7;06 years 35 23 7.07 1.697 96 3.3 Child 3 Child 3 produced 23 phones. As expected for her HA (5;03 years) and CA (7;05 years), Child 3 produced all phones reported by Azzopardi (1981) (cited in Borg & Azzopardi-Alexander, 1997) including the voiced velar nasal [ŋ] which pertains to English phonology. Table 8 shows her raw scores on the MESA speech subtests in comparison to her HA- and CA-matched peers. Child 3 produced 23 phonemes, thus showing availability of a complete phonemic inventory. She obtained full score in PCC and PVC. When compared to her age-matched groups, she performed above average. Additionally, Child 3 exhibited no phonological error patterns. Table 9 shows Child 3’s verbal comprehension and narrative scores. Just like typically-developing children, Child 3 was able to attend to the story while replying correctly to the questions asked. She understood simple commands, basic colour terms and why-questions. She had mastered the knowledge of numbers and counting. Her responses to questions during the comprehension subtest showed evidence of verbal reasoning. Yet, she did not understand the spatial term ‘on’. Child 3 made good lexical choices of nouns, verbs, adjectives and adverbs. She produced the negative ‘not’, together with lengthy and informative sentences. Conjunctions, prepositions, pronouns, articles, possessive determiners, as well as gender, person and number agreement, were adequately used. Conversely, no deictic terms were employed by the child. 4 Discussion The aim of this study was to describe and evaluate the speech and language skills of three children with bilateral CIs, to derive information on their strengths and weaknesses in relation to their HA- and CA- matched peers. The participants were assessed using three speech and two language subtests of the MESA and LAMC respectively, all of which were standardised on the Maltese population. Discussion of results follows and acknowledges the potential influence of each child’s internal characteristics, learning environment and speech and language therapy on their speech and language performance. Child 1 followed typical speech development. Yet, his scores indicated that he was on a par with his HA group whilst delayed when compared to his CA-matched peers. This was consistent with Tabone’s (2004) and Xuereb’s (2004) conclusions on the speech development of Maltese children with CI. On the verbal comprehension subtest, Child 1 performed within the average range compared to his HA-matched peers. Caselli et al. (2012) reported similar trends in lexical comprehension for 17 Italian children who received a CI at 2;00 years of age. The child’s generation of expressive ideas on narrative was excellent when compared to his HA-matched peers. Performance was above average when compared to his CA-matched peers. Therefore, at 2;10 years post-simultaneous bilateral implantation, the child’s expressive skills were progressing well. This concurred with http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COCHIMP.2.60 Wie’s (2010) findings that the majority of children simultaneously implanted acquire both receptive and expressive language skills at a faster pace than expected in comparison to their HA. As observed from the reviewed files, the family members provided good support to the child. This was well reflected in his expressive language, which was developing at a steady pace. Additionally, it was reported by his speech-language pathologist that co-operation, motivation and carryover of therapy in the natural environment contributed to the child’s positive prognosis. Furthermore, the fact that the child was given a shared learning support assistant (LSA) from kindergarten years enhanced and supported his educational needs. The speech-language pathologist liaised with the child’s LSA to consolidate the child’s learning in both the school and clinical contexts. Child 2’s speech development followed the pattern of typicallydeveloping children. Nevertheless, his scores were similar to his HA-matched peers and sometimes even slightly delayed in comparison. He exhibited typical phonological error processes with a chronological mismatch, since he used error processes from different age groups with inconsistent production of words. The latter might be indicative of incomplete knowledge of two phonological systems. It seems reasonable to associate high variability in speech production with children having the least hearing experience. However, this was still evident at the HA of 3;06 years, reflecting instability in adult-like word production. Child 2’s performance on verbal comprehension was considerably poor when compared to both age groups. His difficulty understanding wh-questions was possibly due to the delayed auditory stimulation of 3;04 years. In fact, his audiology file reported that his left ear, being the worse one, was implanted at 3;04 years while he was wearing the right hearing aid on a full-time basis. Yet, as documented in his audiology file, Child 2 seldom made use of this hearing aid. He received his second implant at 5;10 years. Scherf et al. (2009) observed improved auditory skills even in children receiving their second CI after the age of 6;00 years. Yet, older children obtained comparable results two years following bilateral use. In the case of Child 2, bilateral use had been in effect for one year. This suggests that further bilateral experience was required. In contrast, Child 2’s narrative ideas were excellent when compared to his HA-matched group and slightly below average when compared to his CA-matched peers. This discrepancy between his receptive and expressive skills is not what is normally experienced by a typically- developing child. The wh-questions were likely to have impeded his performance on the comprehension subtest. From the assessments carried out by his speech-language pathologist, the child showed understanding of commands with two to three information carrying words. Therefore, rewording of the comprehension assessment questions and substitution of words like ‘bniet’ (girls) with simpler ones might have possibly enhanced his performance. However, since both assessments used are standardised, this was not possible. Additionally, Child 2 had few instances of unintelligibility which http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Speech and language skills of Maltese children with bilateral cochlear implants: three case studies might have been due to the manifestation of systemic error processes. Allen, Nikolopoulos and O’Donoghue (1998) concluded that children with CI continue to develop intelligibility even after five years of implantation. Also, during the speech and language therapy sessions following implantation, emphasis was R placed on the production of Ling sounds (/5/, /0/, /i/, /m/, / /, /s/) and thus, not all the speech sounds were targeted. This might have also possibly contributed to unintelligibility in the production of words. As in the case of Child 3, speech and language therapy targeted all the speech sounds in all the different word positions and also within sentences. Child 3 showed typical speech skills. Her performance was similar to her CA-matched peers. Yet, unexpectedly, Child 3 obtained a higher percentile when compared to her CA than when compared to her HA-matched group. However, Grech and Dodd (2007) noted a dip in the phonological developmental profile of 5;00-5;05-year-old typically-developing bilingual children (Child 3’s HA-matched group). Thus, the child’s performance was typical for her HA. Moreover, she showed adequate phonological proficiency in both Maltese and English. This could have been due to her early exposure to both languages. The language subtests also showed Child 3 to perform better in comparison to her CA- than to her HA-matched group. This led to the reliability of the derived equivalent z-scores and percentiles being queried. As discussed in the LAMC manual, z-score accuracy may be low when used for smaller samples (Grech, Franklin & Dodd, 2011). The English norming group to whom Child 3 was compared was small (N = 30) compared to the Maltese group (N = 205). Child 3 performed well above average compared to both groups of age-matched peers. She had outgrown her HA peers and performed closely to her CA-matched peers. Her hearing experience of 5;03 years might have influenced her skills, given that she had the longest exposure to language when compared to the other participants in this study. In fact, Schramm, Bohnert and Keilmann (2010) concluded that children with CIs increase their linguistic abilities with their HA, that is, the longer the child is fitted with cochlear implants, the better the linguistic outcome. Furthermore, Child 3 was the only female participant in the study. Schramm et al. (2010) observed that better language competence was developed by females, possibly indicating that this factor might have played a role in her linguistic performance. Interestingly, and unlike the other two participants, it was revealed by Child 3’s file that following the diagnosis of hearing loss, she attended sessions with a teacher of the hearing-impaired, during which she was taught baby signs. It was reported that the child immediately picked up the signs and used them communicatively. Having the means to communicate from an early age might have therefore limited the gap in relation to her CA-matched peers. Also, following the implantations, Child 3 attended speech and language therapy sessions on a weekly basis. At the time of testing, the child did not attend therapy as speech and language difficulties were no longer reported. Furthermore, although Child 3 performed similarly to her CA-matched peers (6;00 years), her actual CA was 7;06 years. Thus, one needs to be cautious when interpreting her results, as a fair and accurate comparison would have ideally involved her actual CA. It is acknowledged that this study presented with shortcomings which could not be controlled. These called for cautious interpretation of results. Data on Maltese children with bilateral CIs were not available. Thus, reference was made to sparse data on Maltese children with unilateral CI and comparison was then made to results of foreign research studies. However, exact comparisons could not be carried out due to the different languages and methodologies used. Data on Maltese typically-developing children were also limited. Moreover, generalisation of the results was not possible due to the case study design and the individual variability across the participants, which might have contributed http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COCHIMP.2.60 67 to the differences in scores. Therefore, each participant was analysed and discussed separately. 5 Conclusion The results of the current study shed light on the speech and language performance of three Maltese children with bilateral CIs. These findings are preliminary and cannot be generalised. However, with respect to the three participants, it appears that cochlear implantation provides relevant auditory information that helps develop oral communication. It was concluded that two of the participants presented with delayed but typical speech development, whilst the third child performed on a par with her CAmatched peers. With increased CI experience, phonetic accuracy improved. Additionally, the expressive language score was higher than verbal comprehension for all participants. It emerged that the earlier the cochlear implantation, the shorter was the gap in sequential implants. Moreover, better speech and language outcomes were observed with increased experience with CIs. Results showed that early cochlear implantation enabled the participants to engage in a typical school environment with their HA-matched peers. 6 Acknowledgements The authors are grateful to Professor Helen Grech for granting them permission to use the standardised assessments and their respective norms, and to the audiologist and speech-language pathologists who helped in identifying the participants. Warmest thanks go to the parents of the participants for consenting to their children’s participation and to the children themselves, who patiently and cheerfully co-operated during assessment. 7 Funding This research has received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or non-profit sectors. 8 Conflicts of Interest The authors report no conflicts of interest. References Allen, M.C., Nikolopoulos, T.P. & O’Donoghue, G.M. (1998) Speech intelligibility in children after cochlear implantation. The American Journal of Otology, 19(6), pp. 742-746. Archbold, S. (2002) Cochlear implants. In W. McCracken & S. Laoide-Kemp (Eds) Audiology in Education (pp. 239-266). London: Whurr Publishers. Battista, R. A. & Highhouse, K. (2011) Bilateral cochlear implants. Audiologyonline, Article #840. [Online] Available from: www.audiologyonline.com. [Accessed 5th September 2013]. Borg, A. & Azzopardi-Alexander, M. (1997) Maltese. London: Routledge. Caselli, M. C., Rinaldi, P., Varuzza, C., Giuliani, A. & Burdo, S. (2012) Cochlear implant in the second year of life: lexical and grammatical outcomes. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55, pp. 382-394. doi:10.1044/10924388(2011/10-0248). Coppini, A. (2002) The acquisition of number and gender agreement between nouns and adjectives in 3 to 5 year old Maltese-speaking children. Unpublished B.Sc. (Hons) dissertation. Malta: University of Malta. Dunn, C.C., Tyler, R.S., Oakley, S. & Gantz, B.J. (2008) Comparison of speech recognition and localization performance in bilateral and unilateral cochlear implant users matched http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Speech and language skills of Maltese children with bilateral cochlear implants: three case studies on duration of deafness and age at implantations. Ear and Hearing, 29(3), pp. 352-359. Friedmann, N. & Szterman, R. (2011) The comprehension and production of WH-questions in deaf and hard-of-hearing children. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 16(2), pp. 212-235. Geers, A. E., Nicholas, J. G. & Sedey, A. L. (2003) Language skills of children with early cochlear implantation. Ear and Hearing, 24(1), pp. 46S-58S. Grech, H. (1998) Phonological development of normal Maltese speaking children. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation. United Kingdom: University of Manchester. Grech, H. (2006) Phonological development of Maltese-speaking children. In Z. Hua & B. Dodd (Eds) Phonological Development and Disorders in Children: A multilingual perspective (pp. 64-73). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. Grech, H. & Dodd, B. (2007) Effects of language learning context on phonological acquisition. Paper presented at the First International Conference of Maltese Linguistics. Germany: Bremen University. Grech, H. & Dodd, B. (2008) Phonological acquisition in Malta: a bilingual language learning context. International Journal of Bilingualism, 12(3), pp. 155-171. Grech, H., Dodd, B. & Franklin, S. (2011) Maltese-English Speech Assessment (MESA). Malta: University of Malta. Grech, H., Franklin, S. & Dodd, B. (2011) Language Assessment for Maltese Children (LAMC). Malta: University of Malta. Iglesias, A. & Rojas, R. (2012) Bilingual language development of English language learners: modelling the growth of two languages. In B. A. Goldstein (Ed) Bilingual Language Development and Disorders in Spanish-English Speakers (2nd ed.) (pp. 3-30). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Kim, J-S., Kim, L-S. & Jeong, S-W. (2013) Functional benefits of sequential bilateral cochlear implantation in children with long inter-stage interval between two implants. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 77, pp. 162-169. Litovsky, R., Parkinson, A., Arcaroli, J. & Sammeth, C. (2006) Simultaneous bilateral cochlear implantation in adults: a multicenter clinical study. Ear and Hearing, 27(6), pp. 714731. Niparko, J. K., Tobey, E. A., Thal, D. J., Eisenberg, L. S., Wang, N. Y., Quittner, A. L. & Fink, N. E. (2010) Spoken language development in children following cochlear implantation. The Journal of the American Medical Association (JAMA), 303(15), pp. 1498-1506. Robbins, A. M. (2000) Rehabilitation after cochlear implantation. In J. K. Niparko (Ed.) Cochlear Implants: Principles and practices (pp. 323-363). Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Robbins, A. M. (2006) Language development in children with cochlear implants. In S. B. Waltzman & J. T. Roland (Eds) Cochlear Implants (2nd ed.) (pp. 153-166). New York: Thieme Medical Publishers. Roland, J. T. (2000). Complications of cochlear implant surgery. In S. B. Waltzman & N. L. Cohen (Eds) Cochlear Implants (pp. 171-175). New York: Thieme Medical Publishers. Sammeth, C. A. (2007) Why two is better than one - the benefits of bilateral cochlear implants. Implantable Hearing Solutions, 1(3), pp. 14 - 16. Sax, N. & Weston, E. (2007) Language development milestones. [Online] Available from: (http://www.rehabmed.ualberta. ca/spa/phonology/milestones.pdf [Accessed 20th August 2014]. Scherf, F., van Deun, L., van Wieringen, A., Wouters, J., Desloovere, C., Dhooge, I., Offeciers, E., Deggouj, N., De Raeve, L., Wuyts, F. & van de Heyning, P. (2009) Three-year postimplantation auditory outcomes in children with se- http://dx.medra.org/10.14614/COCHIMP.2.60 68 quential bilateral cochlear implantation. Annals of Otology, Rhinology & Laryngology, 118(5), pp. 336-344. Schramm, B., Bohnert, A. & Keilmann, A. (2010) Auditory, speech and language development in young children with cochlear implants compared with children with normal hearing. International Journal of Otorhinolaryngology, 74(7), pp. 812-819. doi:10.1016/j.ijporl.2010.04.008. Sharma, A., Gilley, P. M., Martin, K., Roland, P., Bauer, P. & Dorman, M. (2007) Simultaneous versus sequential bilateral implantation in young children: effects on central auditory system development and plasticity. Audiological Medicine, 5, pp. 218-223. Tabone, N. (2004) Four case studies on the language skills of hearing-impaired children with hearing aids and cochlear implants. Unpublished B.Sc. (Hons) dissertation. Malta: University of Malta. Therres, M. (2012) Therapy: same or different for a child with two cochlear implants? Audiologyonline, Article #7027. [Online] Available from: www.audiologyonline.com. [Accessed 20th August 2014]. Welling, D. R. (2010) Hearing and language development. In B. B. Shulman & N. C. Capone (Eds) Language Development: Foundations, processes, and clinical applications (pp. 95134). U.K.: Jones and Bartlett Publishers. Wie, O. B. (2010) Language development in children after receiving bilateral cochlear implants between 5 and 18 months. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 74(11), pp. 1258-1266. Xuereb, J. (2004) Language skills in a cochlear-implanted child: a case study. Unpublished B.Sc (Hons) dissertation. Malta: University of Malta. Yin, R. K. (2009) Case Study Research: Design and methods (4th ed.). London: U.K.: Sage Publications. http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Malta Journal of Health Sciences http://www.um.edu.mt/healthsciences/mjhs Table of Contents Malta Journal of Health Sciences Volume 1 - Issue 2 (December 2014) Editorial 28 Editorial Daniela Gatt Regular Articles 29 Key environmental stress biomarker candidates for the optimisation of chemotherapy treatment of leukaemia Review Article Eirini G. Velliou, Susana Brito Dos Santos, Maria Fuentes-Garí, Ruth Misener, Eleni Pefani, Nicki Panoskaltsis, Athanasios Mantalaris and Efstratios N. Pistikopoulos 35 Degree of burnout among Maltese podiatrists working in two different settings Research Article Trista Agius and Cynthia Formosa 40 The effectiveness of social communication groups in mainstream schools Research Article Tiziana Baldacchino and Kristina Agius 54 The lived experience of persons with an amputation Research Article Chanelle Grech and Roberta Farrugia Debono 60 Speech and language skills of Maltese children with bilateral cochlear implants: three case studies Research Article Martina Galdes and Daniela Gatt