Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Dannika Osei Case Study 1 – An Evidence-Based Practice Review Report Theme: Interventions Involving Parents Does the ‘Tuning into Kids’ program enable parents to reduce emotional and behavioural difficulties in their children? Summary This systematic literature review aims to discover how effective the Tuning into Kids programme is in enabling parents to reduce emotional and behavioural difficulties in their children. Tuning into Kids and its variants (Tuning into Toddlers and Tuning into Teens) is an emotion-focused parenting programme aimed at developing emotionally responsive parenting with the aim of increasing emotion knowledge in children as well as reductions in child behaviour problems. Six studies met the inclusion criteria and were reviewed using the Weight of Evidence Framework (Gough, 2007) and the APA Task Force Coding Protocol by Kratochwill (2003). The programme was effective in reducing emotional and behavioural difficulties in children as indicated by small-medium effect sizes across the majority of studies however was most effective for a clinical population as shown by large effect sizes. Potential use and implications for future directions are discussed. Introduction What is Tuning into Kids? The Tuning into Kids (TIK) program and its age-specific programs (Tuning into Toddlers; Tuning into Teens) is a parenting intervention developed by Havighurst, Wilson, Harley and Prior (2009) and is aimed at improving parent’s emotion coaching skills. According to Gottman, Katz and Hooven (1996), emotion coaching parents are aware of low-intensity emotions in themselves and their children, see negative emotions in their child as an 1 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Dannika Osei opportunity for intimacy or teaching, validate their child’s emotions and help the child to verbally label their emotions. Emotion coaching also involves problem solving with the child, identifying goals for dealing with the problem situation and setting behavioural limits. This contrasts with emotion dismissing parents who aim to quickly change a child’s negative emotions, deny their feelings and convey to their children that negative emotions are unimportant. The latter parenting style is believed to be related to poor child outcomes (Havighurst et al, 2009). The components of the program are outlined in Havighurst et al (2009) and have been summarised in Table 1. TIK is a six-session, two-hour-per-week parenting program. TIK hinges on teaching parents the five steps of emotion coaching outlined by Gottman (1997) cited in Havighurst et al (2009). These five steps are similar to the above however also emphasises communicating understanding and acceptance of the child’s emotion. The steps are broken down into a series of exercises to be carried out throughout each session. As the first four steps are thought to be most difficult to learn, they are prioritised, therefore first three weeks of the program emphasise attending to the lower intensity emotions exhibited by children, being able to reflect, label and empathise with the child’s negative emotion. The fourth session focuses on anxiety and problem solving skills. The last two weeks examine emotions such as anger and teach emotion regulation techniques. As the importance of parents being aware of their own emotions was noted by Gottman et al (1996), parents are taught how to understand and regulate their own emotions and reflect on their experiences with emotion beliefs and responses deriving from their family of origin. Sizes of parenting groups ranges from 7-15 and are carried out by two trained facilitators. As the review focuses on Tuning into Kids, Toddlers and Teens, the program will be referred to as “Tuning into” (TI). 2 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Dannika Osei Table 1 – Summary of steps taught in ‘Tuning into Kids’ Steps taught in ‘Tuning into Kids’ 1 Emphasis To become aware of the child’s Becoming aware of emotions and emotion, especially if this emotion is at how a low intensity. 2 may present at a physiological level. To view the child’s emotion as an opportunity for intimacy and teaching. 3 they Attending to the child’s To communicate to the child an intensity behaviours. lower Reflecting, understanding and acceptance of their labelling and empathising with the emotion. 4 emotion. To help the child to be able to use Focuses on anxiety and problem words to describe how they feel. solving. Also focuses on more intense emotions such as anger. 5 To assist the child with problem solving Teaching while setting limits. emotion regulation strategies involving slow, breathing, relaxation, expressing anger in a safe way and the turtle technique which teaches self-control (PATHS; Greenberg et al, 1995). Throughout Parents are taught to regulate and understand their own emotions program 3 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Dannika Osei Psychological Basis The TI program draw on a number of psychological theories. A parent’s ability to respond to their child’s emotions hinges on the parent’s emotional well-being. Therefore emotion expression and regulation (Havighurst et al., 2009), meta-emotion (Gottman et al., 1996) and Mindfulness is integrated into the program. This teaches parents to reflect on their experiences of emotion socialisation as well as being able to “sit with” their emotions when they need to respond to their child’s emotions. However, Social learning Theory (Bandura, 1977) strongly underpins this program. According to Bandura, humans learn through observing the attitudes, behaviour and the consequences of behaviour that others in their environment display. Thus, most human behaviour is learned through others such as parents, teachers, peers who model the behaviour. Through this, people are able to form ideas on how to execute new actions which later serves as a guide for how they should perform the behaviour. According to Denham (1998), the emotional expression and regulation demonstrated by parents is an important model for the child on how to go about managing and showing their emotions. The TI program suggests children experience emotion socialisation through interacting with caregivers, siblings and teachers and the emotional experiences they are exposed to through these people. This plays a crucial role in how a child develops emotional competence (Havighurst et al., 2009). Attachment theory is another major concept in the development of the program. Bowlby (1958) states attachment is an innate primary drive in an infant and results in the infant maintaining proximity to their caregiver and looks at how infant’s emotions and behaviours such as crying, smiling and clinging were received and responded to by parents. Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters and Wall (1978) identified three categories of attachment: securely attached, insecure-avoidant attachment and insecure resistant. Secure attachment is 4 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Dannika Osei associated with positive interactions between parent and child whereas the others were associated with negative interactions. Relating attachment to emotion socialisation, Ainsworth, Bell and Stayton (1971, 1974) in Meins (2003) found mothers who responded sensitively to their infants’ cues had securely attached infants while mothers who rejected their infants’ cues had insecure-avoidant children and inconsistency in parenting was linked to insecure-resistant attachment styles. According to Green and Goldwyn (2002), ambivalent attachment styles are linked to anxiety and social withdrawal and avoidant styles are linked to antisocial development. They also note attachment disorganisation could lead to vulnerability in a child’s self-concept and emotion-regulation. Given the research on and importance of early attachments, TI aims to develop supportive and emotionally responsive parenting. Rationale In the new Special Educational Needs and Disabilities Code of Practice (SEND, 2014) emotional and behavioural difficulties is referred to as Social, Emotional and Mental Health (SEMH) Difficulties. This area of need is characterised by displaying withdrawn or isolated behaviour, challenging, disruptive behaviour as well as disturbing behaviour. According to the Department for Education (2014), in state-funded primary schools, 18.4% of pupils with statements and those at school action plus had a primary need of Behaviour, Emotional and Social difficulties (BESD) and in state-funded secondary schools this figure was 26.7%. As well as supporting the inclusion of pupils with academic learning difficulties, Educational Psychologists (EPs) must also work to support the inclusion of pupils whose SEMH pose barriers to their learning. These pupils are overrepresented in UK exclusion figures 5 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Dannika Osei (Sellman, 2009). Farrell et al. (2006) highlight the role EPs have in providing support and intervention for children and young people who are experiencing BESD. However, early intervention and prevention is another valuable aspect of EP practice (Farrell et al., 2006) and resonates with frameworks such as Positive Educational Practices (PEPs; Noble and McGrath, 2008) which states EPs must focus on promoting wellbeing such as through teaching social and emotional competency. As emotion socialisation begins with the primary caregivers of a child, promoting positive emotional learning opportunities from early childhood could be particularly advantageous to pupils at-risk of SEMH, as well as to Educational Psychology Services (EPSs) in the long run. Therefore the TI intervention could potentially be important to EPs in empowering parents in being able to prevent the development of behavioural difficulties in their children. Therefore this review aims to answer the following question: Does the ‘Tuning into’ program enable parents to reduce emotional and behavioural difficulties in their children? Critical Review of the Evidence Base Literature Search Initial searches were carried out during December 2014 using electronic databases PsychINFO, Medline and ERIC. Using a multi-field search, the following search terms were entered into ‘all fields’ or ‘title’ to retrieve studies (see Table 2). As the research base for this intervention is relatively new, the Principal researcher for the TI programme was emailed to retrieve unpublished or ‘in press’ studies on the intervention. 6 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Dannika Osei Table 2: Search terms used in PsychINFO, Medline and ERIC Search number 1 1 2 3 Tuning into (T) Tuning into (T) Emotion social* Emotion Behavio* Emotion social* Behavio* 4 Emotion focused parenting Tuning Behavio* 5 Parenting Emotion coaching Emotion coaching 2 3 Behavio* ‘conduct’ or ‘behavio’ (T) 4 child OR toddler OR Teen OR adolescent child OR toddler OR Teen OR adolescent kids children T = title only * = wildcard search term Inclusion and exclusion criteria Studies retrieved were included in the review if they met the criteria detailed in Table 3. As shown in Figure I, 86 papers were found through the electronic databases: 65 from PsychINFO, 14 from ERIC and 7 from Medline. Furthermore, 15 studies were received from the principal researcher of the intervention, bringing the total amount of retrieved papers to 101. 39 papers were excluded as duplicates and the remaining studies were screened through their titles and abstracts and excluded based on the below criteria. 9 papers were eligible for a full review and a further 2 were excluded using the criteria in Table 3 (see Appendix A). As shown in Appendix B, a total of 7 papers were suitable for inclusion in the review. 7 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Dannika Osei Table 3: Inclusion and exclusion criteria Inclusion criteria Exclusion criteria 1. Type of publication a) Must be in a peer reviewed-journal or have been submitted for peer-review a) Is not or has not been submitted for peer review i.e. Books chapters. 2. Language and setting a) Must be written in English. No restrictions on country in which research has taken place. a) The study must solely implement the Tuning into Kids/Teens/Toddlers intervention. a) Study is not written in English 3. Intervention b) Must include core components of teaching 5 steps of emotion coaching skills (Gottman, 1997) and must be implemented for standard duration of 6 weeks. 4. Type design of 5. Outcomes 6. Participants a) Must be a group design that reports between group outcomes (e.g. an RCT) or one that reports pre and post measures. a) The study reports on outcomes of child behaviour postintervention a) Must involve parents of children and young people. b) Sample is unique to this paper Rationale Peer reviewers assess the quality of a study and therefore the study in these journals is likely to meet the required standards. Reviewer does not have the resources to access other languages. a) Study does not implement the ‘Tuning into’ intervention or, study implements ‘Tuning into…’ intervention alongside another intervention. b) Study implements TIK program for longer than 6 sessions. a) The study is a single-case design. The review is based on the ‘Tuning into’ intervention. Reviewer will be unable to discern what effects are due to the ‘Tuning into’ intervention. If interventions last longer in some studies, effect sizes may differ according to this variable. a) Child behaviour outcomes are not reported The review aims to find out whether the parenting intervention improves child behaviour. The review aims to find out how a parenting programme can improve behavioural outcome for children. If sample has been used for more than one study, findings may not be entirely representative therefore the study that most closely fits inclusion criteria will be used. a) The study does not involve parents b) Sample has been used for multiple articles 8 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Dannika Osei Figure 1: Flowchart: Application of inclusion and exclusion criteria Papers for review of title and abstract Articles identified from electronic databases Psychinfo n= 65 ERIC n= 14 Medline n= 7 Total n= 86 Articles sent by author N= 15 N=101 Electronic database: Papers excluded on basis of inclusion criteria 3a N= 54 A Papers excluded as duplicates N=24 Total – 78 Sent by author: Papers excluded on basis of inclusion criteria 1a, 3a, 3b, 4a, 3b, 5a N=6 Papers excluded as duplicates N= 9 Total - 15 Papers for review of full text N =9 Papers excluded Inclusion criteria 6b – n=2 Papers included N=7 9 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Dannika Osei Critical appraisal for quality and relevance The seven papers were summarised to capture the main aspects as well as the information relevant to the review question (See Appendix C). The quality and relevance of each study was appraised using the Weight of Evidence (WoE) framework (Gough, 2007). WoE comprises four judgements. WoE A examines methodological quality in terms of the quality of how well the study was executed. WoE A was judged using the APA Task Force Coding Protocol by Kratochwill (2003) which was adapted according to the purpose of the review question and to determine the methodological quality of the studies (See Appendix D). Each study was examined using this protocol in order to systematically analyse each study based on the same criteria. WoE B addresses the methodological relevance of the study the appropriateness of the study in relation to the review question. Finally WoE C appraises the relevance of the focus of the study to the review question. Outcomes from the aforementioned WoEs are averaged to calculate an overall WoE – termed WoE D (See Table 4). For further information on how each study was appraised, see Appendix E. Table 4- Overall Weight of Evidence (WoE D) Studies WoE A Quality of methodology WoE B Relevance of Methodology WoE D Overall weight of evidence Medium 2 WoE C Relevance of evidence to the review question Medium 2 Havighurst et al (2009) and Havighurst et al (2010) Havighurst et al (2011) Kehoe et al (2014) Lauw et al (2014) Wilson et al (2012) Havighurst et al (2004) High 2.5 High 3 High 2.5 Medium 1.25 High 2.75 Medium 2 High 3 Medium 2 Low 1 Medium 2 Low 1 High 3 Medium 2 Low 1 Low 1 Medium 2 High 3 Medium 2.17 Low 1.11 Medium 1.92 Medium 1.27 Medium 2.17 19 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Dannika Osei Participants The number of participants in the studies ranged from 34-225 and were recruited from cities such as Melbourne and Knox, Australia. The socioeconomic status (SES) of parents across the sample varied as indicated by household incomes, however three studies (Kehoe et al.; 2014; Lauw et al.; 2014; Wilson et al.; 2012) had a middle-upper class majority. All included studies included only the primary caregiving parent of which the majority were female (93.8%). Inclusion criteria for involvement in the study involved parents being able to speak enough English to understand the intervention, having a child within the target age for the study and not being committed to other research projects or other parenting programmes. However, Havighurst et al. (2010) and Havighurst et al. (2013) excluded parents if they had a child with a diagnosis of any communication disorders or pervasive developmental disorders. Finally, Havighurst et al. (2013) only included parents who had children who were above the clinical cut-off on the Eyberg Child Behaviour Inventory (ECBI) to ascertain whether TIK could be used as an intervention for children with behavioural difficulties therefore was rated highly on WoE C. Samples were acquired using convenience sampling methods whereby parents in schools, preschools, a kindergarten, a Maternal and Child Health centre, or behaviour clinics in hospitals were asked to participate through distribution of information flyers and letters. In Psychology research, this method of sampling is common however, non-random sampling methods like convenience sampling are biased as not every member of the target population has an equal chance of being selected (Barker, Pistrang and Elliot, 2002). As the majority of participants opted into the study, particular characteristics in these parents such as a motivation to improve parenting, an interest in socioemotional functioning or the time available, may have influenced the decision to participate which may not be present 20 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Dannika Osei in parents that did not opt-in. Therefore this may affect generalizability of results to the rest of their target population. Design Randomised Controlled trials (RCTs) and prettest-posttest designs were included in this review. Four of the studies were RCTs: Havighurst et al. (2009; 1a) and (2010; 1b), Havighurst et al. (2013), Kehoe et al. (2014) and Wilson et al. (2012). Randomisation methods varied from randomising schools into intervention/control groups to randomising participants into groups (Havighurst et al., 2011) and was done using a random-number generator. Havighurst et al. (2013) was given a high rating because it used an ‘active’ comparison group over a waitlist control group who received treatment as usual (Paediatric treatment). This was rated highly as using an active comparison may show the intervention is more effective than currently applied interventions for children presenting with behavioural difficulties. Furthermore active comparisons are more ethical than a no intervention group which withholds a potentially beneficial intervention from a group, however this poses less risk if participants are not clinically distressed (Barker, Pistrang and Elliot, 2002). The remaining studies using a waitlist intervention are deemed more ethical than a no intervention group however, on WoE B, received a medium rating. This is because one cannot conclude from these studies that the TI intervention is better than alternative interventions in reducing emotional and behavioural difficulties in children. All RCTs used objective methods to randomise participants into intervention and control groups thus increasing the chances of equivalence between groups and reducing the risk of error of bias in results (Evans, 2003). Prettest-posttest designs used were in Lauw et al. (2014) and Havighurst et al. (2004) and were both pilot studies. These studies were rated low on WOE B as it is problematic 21 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Dannika Osei attributing changes in behaviour to the intervention alone without the presence of a control group and poses threats to validity and reliability. Cook and Campbell (1979) cited in Barker, Pistrang and Elliot (2002) note there are possible threats to internal validity when using a prettest-posttest design including maturational trends whereby participants grow out of their problem, a case especially relevant to children. Studies varied on when measurements were taken. Some measured behaviour at preintervention and post-intervention only (Lauw et al., 2014) pre-intervention, postintervention and follow-up (Havighurst et al., 2009;1a; 2010;1b; Havighurst et al., 2013; and Havighurst et al., 2004) whereas others did pre-intervention and follow-up only (Kehoe et al., 2014) and Wilson et al., 2012). Taking follow-up measures as well as pre-intervention and post-intervention measures provides information on not only how effective an intervention is but how long-lasting effects are. However if measures are only taken at two time points pre-intervention and post-intervention then the study only provides information on the immediate effect of the intervention. Conversely, it can be argued that, only taking measures of behaviour at follow-up provides some evidence about the sustainability of change after the intervention. However without immediate measures of change to compare it to, it is possible that other factors may have contributed to improvement in behaviour over the 3/6 month follow up period. Therefore studies measuring behaviour change preintervention, post-intervention and follow-up were rated high on WoE B whereas those with control groups that only took measures at two time-points were rated ‘medium’ and low ratings were given to those without a control group that only took measures at two-time points. 22 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Dannika Osei Measures The Eyberg Child Behaviour Inventory 6 (ECBI; Eyberg and Pincus, 1999) was used across all studies but Kehoe et al. (2014) and Lauw et al. (2014). The ECBI has Cronbach’s alpha reliability scores ranging from .90 to .94 across studies therefore has a high reliability. These studies were also multi-source and multi-method thus receiving high ratings on WoE A and C. The Sutter-Eyberg Student Behaviour Inventory – a teacher’s version of the ECBI was used by Havighurst et al. (2009;1a and 2010;1b) and Havighurst et al. (2013) which had Cronbach’s alpha’s of .97. As this was an additional measure of behaviour, in addition to the ECBI as completed by parents, this moved data toward triangulation which led to high weightings on WoE A and C. Other measures include the Social Competence and Behaviour Evaluation (SCBE-30; LaFreniere and Dumas, 1996), a teacher report. In this study (Wilson et al., 2012), they examined social competence and anger and aggression scales thus was deemed a suitable measure of behaviour. Reliability coefficients ranged from .88 - .92. Kehoe et al. (2014) sought to measure internalising behaviour difficulties (anxiety and depression) in youth. Anxiety was measured using the Spence Children’s anxiety scale (SCAS; Spence, 1998) and the parent-report version (SCAS-P, Nauta et al., 2004) both of which had Cronbach’s alphas of .90 - .93. Depressive symptoms were measured using the Child Depression Inventory (CDI; Beck, 1977) and the parent version, CDI:P (Garber, 1984) and had reliability coefficients ranging from .84-.87. Consequently, this study was rated highly on WOE A and C. In Havighurst et al. (2004), the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ: Goodman, 1997) was used which had reliability coefficients ranging from .57 to .77. Cronbach’s alphas were not reported for the ECBI or the ERC, however studies reporting reliability were cited, therefore on WOE A this received a medium rating. Lauw et al. (2014) also received a medium rating for measures but only included the Brief Infant-Toddler Social 23 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Dannika Osei and Emotional Assessment (Briggs-Gowan and Carter,2007) with no reliability coefficients reported, this was given a medium weighting on WoE A and low on WoE C. All child behavioural outcomes were measured using questionnaire data however using self-report may be subjective which poses validity issues. Respondents may be less truthful on questionnaires due to social desirability bias in that they may want to impress researchers or may report outcomes suggesting improvements due to expectancy effects (Barker, Pistrang and Elliot, 2002). Therefore validity could be improved if observational measures of child behaviour were taken pre and post intervention in addition to self-report measures. However, where parent self-report was supplemented by self-reports from other perspectives (as specified on WoE A and C), this increases validity. Findings Parent-reported behaviour Effect size data is briefly summarised in Table 6. A more in depth summary can be found in Appendix F. The majority of effect size outcomes in studies using the ECBI were smallmedium however some large effect sizes were found. This suggests the TI programme has moderate efficacy in reducing emotional and behavioural difficulties in children. Havighurst et al. (2009; 2010) had a small-medium effect size for behaviour intensity at posttest which was maintained at follow-up as well as a medium WoE D suggesting the effect size is an accurate depiction of the study’s effectiveness. Wilson et al. (2012) however had a small effect size as well as non-significant results for their time-condition interaction effect. This may be due to the fact that post-intervention measures were only taken 6 months after the intervention ceased. Therefore, immediate effects of the intervention, which are valuable indicators of effectiveness, were not captured. Another potential reason is that the programme facilitators were professionals from the local 24 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Dannika Osei community, many of whom did not have Psychology degrees, experience in group facilitation or parenting education. Although professionals were trained for two days and provided with a manual, arguably as it is a specialised psychological intervention with a range of components, the facilitator may need to have a background in Psychology to be able to deliver this to full effect. This is reflected in a low WOE C rating. Havighurst et al. (2013), was the only study with a large effect size on the ECBI and rated high on WoE D making this a particularly impressive study. However effects for follow-up measures were small for behaviour intensity and small-medium on behaviour problems. This may suggest using the TI programme as an intervention with children at the clinical level on the ECBI is where the intervention has the strongest effect, however long-term effects are not as strong as immediate effects. The only drawback is that this study did not have a sufficient sample size therefore may have been underpowered. If a sample does not have enough power, the chances of the study detecting a significant effect is reduced, therefore although a statistically significant effect was found for reduction in behaviour intensity, a larger sample size may have found a smaller p-value. Despite having a medium WoE D, Havighurst et al. (2004) also used the ECBI along with the ERC which both had small effect sizes. This could be because in addition to having a small sample size, this study was the pilot of TI programme and although the essential ‘emotion coaching’ components were present, the added benefit of helping parents to help children to regulate anger and worry, and enhancing parental emotion awareness and regulation was only added when the intervention formally became TIK. Kehoe et al. (2014) had medium effect sizes when anxiety and depression was rated by parents (SCAS-P, CDI-P) however small effect sizes when this was rated by youth (SCAS, CDI). Potential reasons for this could be similar to the above in that there was an expectancy bias present in parents. A small effect size was found on the Brief-ITSEA in 25 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Dannika Osei Lauw et al. (2014). As this too was a pilot study without a control group, it was also rated low on WOE D. Additionally, the study had approximately half the required sample size and so may have lacked enough power to detect a more significant improvement in toddler behaviour problems. Teacher-reported behaviour The SESBI was used in Havighurst et al. (2009; 2010) and Havighurst et al. (2013) as a follow-up measure and showed small and medium effects respectively. However, Havighurst et al. (2004) used an alternative teacher-report at posttest rather than follow up. These outcomes showed a medium effect size. This may be as teacher-reported measures were only taken at follow up thus immediate effects observed by parents may have diminished with time as also shown by smaller parent-reported effect sizes at follow-up. Similarly parent-reported behaviour change may reflect an expectancy bias as they were the main change agents for the children and were aware of the purposes of the intervention, therefore they may have anticipated a positive change in their child’s behaviour which may be reflected in their ECBI scores. Conversely, teachers may be more objective as they were not directly involved in the intervention. However as some effect was shown, it supports the idea that moderate improvements were observed. 26 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Outcome Effect size Study Behaviour Intensity (ECBI) PPC SMD = -0.44 (Small-medium) Havighurst et al (2009) PPC SMD= -0.76 (Large) Behaviour Problem (ECBI/SESBI/BITSEA) Lability/Negativity (ERC) Dannika Osei Number of Participants 218 Overall Weight of Evidence Medium Havighurst et al (2013) 54 High PP SMD = -0.26 (Small) Havighurst et al (2004) 47 Medium PPC SMD= -0.83 (Large) PP SMD = -0.26 (Small) PP SMD = -0.31 (Small) Havighurst et al (2013) Lauw et al (2014) 54 High 34 Low 47 Medium PP SMD= 0.49 (Medium) PP SMD = -0.31 (Small) Havighurst et al (2004) Havighurst et al (2004) 47 Medium Havighurst et al (2004) Table 6 – Summary of effect sizes and overall quality ratings a) Pre-post 33 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Outcome Effect size Study Behaviour Intensity (ECBI/SESBI) PPC SMD = - 0.41 (Small-medium) PPC SMD = -0.2 (Small) Havighurst et al (2010) Number of Participants 216 Overall Weight of Evidence Medium Havighurst et al (2010) 216 Medium PPC SMD = -0.13 (Small) Havighurst et al (2013) 54 High PPC SMD= -0.58 (Medium) Havighurst et al (2013) 54 High PPC SMD= -0.22 (Small) Wilson et al (2012) 128 Medium Havighurst et al (2004) 47 Medium Havighurst et al (2013) 54 High PPC SMD= -0.5 (Medium) Havighurst et al (2013) 54 High PPC SMD= -0.26 (Small) Wilson et al (2012) 128 Medium PP SMD = -0.42 (Small-medium) Havighurst et al (2004) 47 Medium PPC SMD = -0.19 (Small) Kehoe et al (2014) 225 Medium PPC SMD = -0.48 (Small-medium) Kehoe et al (2014) 225 Medium PPC SMD= -0.13 (Small) Kehoe et al (2014) 225 Medium PPC SMD =-0.46 (Small-medium) Kehoe et al (2014) 225 Medium PPC SMD= 0.02 (Small) PP SMD = -0.41 (Small-medium) Wilson et al (2012) 128 Medium Havighurst et al (2004) 47 Medium PP SMD = -0.4 (Small-medium) Behaviour Problem (ECBI/SESBI) Anxiety (SCAS/SCAS-P) Depression CDI:S/CDI:P) Anger/Aggression Lability/Negativity Dannika Osei PPC SMD = -0.42 (Small) b) Pre-follow-up Note: Effect sizes were calculated from data given in the studies. For RCTs, Morris (2007) PrettestPosttest Control (PPC) design Standardised Mean Difference (SMD) was used. This SMD was 34 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Dannika Osei used as opposed to Cohen’s d as it allows a computation of the difference in prettest-posttest scores between the intervention and control groups whereas a Cohen’s d calculation would not factor in pre-intervention scores. This gives a more accurate measure of the effects of the intervention and takes advantage of the strengths of an RCT design (Morris, 2007). Effect sizes using this calculation will be referred to as PPC SMD. For studies without a control group, Becker (1988) SMD was used as this calculation also examines prettest and posttest scores (referred to as PP SMD). Due to the absence of a control group, these effect sizes must be interpreted with caution and cannot be directly compared to PPC SMDs. Cohen’s d (1988) effect size descriptors (high, medium, low) have been used to describe effect sizes at .02, .05 and .08 respectively. Conclusions This systematic literature review aimed to discover to what extent the TI intervention enabled parents to reduce emotional and behavioural difficulties in their children. This programme is efficacious in achieving this with small-medium effect both at posttest and prettest. However, largest effects are clearly observed when implementing the programme with a clinical population. This may be because children were displaying above average behavioural difficulties thus had greater scope for improvement than mainstream populations. According to Forgatch and DeGarno (1999), interventions have been shown to have greatest effect on participants when they are in the clinical range. This is because the clinical sample are usually close to homogeneity in terms of diagnosed problems whereas prevention studies use samples that are heterogeneous in type and intensity of their difficulties thus need a greater amount of power than clinical trials. 35 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Dannika Osei Limitations All of the studies were carried out by the same research group and consistently involved Havighurst, Wilson and/or Harley. This could potentially be a source of bias across studies as researchers may have a particular interest in and investment in promoting how effective the programme is. This may lead to results being analysed and reported in similar ways, as well as a tendency to only report the most significant results and downplay the significance of any non-significant results. For example only commenting on the “main effect” significance instead of the interaction effect i.e. in Wilson et al. (2012). However, the use of standardised measures of child outcomes could be deemed to control for some of the bias. Recommendations In light of the above, although TI shows promising outcomes as a prevention programme in terms of WoE, effect sizes and strong outcomes as an intervention programme for a clinical sample, a more extensive, varied body of research is needed before this is adopted as an intervention programme within EPSs. More research on its effect on children at the clinical level of behavioural difficulties is needed to build on the idea that this programme is best as an intervention. To reduce potential bias in the studies, the programme may benefit from different research groups researching its effectiveness. Subsequent studies may benefit from including an observational measure of behavioural into the measures to triangulate questionnaire data obtained. Most studies incorporated a follow-up measure into the design which shows how long the effects of the intervention last therefore to further develop this, a longitudinal study could be conducted whereby child behavioural outcomes are measured over the course of a few years. 36 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Dannika Osei The outcomes of this review show that the TI programme has potential benefits on the emotional wellbeing of children and young people as both an intervention and prevention programme. As studies where the programme was delivered by psychologists produced better outcomes than the one that was delivered by other professionals who had received training, it is recommended that this programme be delivered by EPs or other psychologists of a similar background. After training, the programme could be delivered by EPs to groups of parents identified as having children/young people with/at risk of developing SEMH. The effectiveness of the programme should be evaluated by measuring behaviour pre and post intervention and at follow up using the EBCI, SESBI, SCAS, or CDI. 37 References Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. New Jersey: Prentice Hall. Barker, C., Pistrang, N., & Elliott, R. (2002). Research methods in clinical psychology: An introduction for students and practitioners. (2nd ed) Chichester: John Wiley &Sons LTD. Bowlby, J. (1958). The nature of the child’s tie to his mother. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 39, 350-371. Briggs-Gowan, M. J., & Carter, A. S. (2007). Applying the Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (ITSEA) and Brief-ITSEA in early intervention. Infant Mental Health Journal, 28, 564– 583. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Cohen, J. (1992). Quantitative Methods in Psychology. A Power Primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155–159. Dawson, J., & Singh-Desi, D. (2010). Educational psychology working to improve psychological well-being: an example. Emotional and Behavioural Difficulties, 15, 295–310. Denham, S.A. (1998). Emotional development in young children. New York: Guilford Press. Department for Education. (2013). Statistical First Release: Special Educational Needs in England, 1–13. Evans, D. (2003). Hierarchy of evidence: A framework for ranking evidence evaluating healthcare interventions. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 12, 77–84. 34 Eyberg, S., & Robinson, E. A. (1983). Conduct problem behavior: Standardization of a behavioural rating scale with adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 12, 347-354. Eyberg, S., & Pincus, D. (1999). Eyberg Child Behavior Inventory and Sutter-Eyberg Student Behavior Inventory_Revised: Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Farrell, P., Woods, K., Lewis, S., Rooney, S., Squires, G., & Connor, M. O. (2006). A Review of the Functions and Contribution of Educational Psychologists in England and Wales in light of “ Every Child Matters : Change for Children .” Forgatch M. & S, DeGarmo D. S. (1999). Parenting through change: An effective prevention program for single mothers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 67, 711–724. Garber, J. (1984). The developmental progression of depression in female children. New Directions for Child Development, 26, 29–58. Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1996). Parental meta-emotion philosophy and the emotional life of families: Theoretical models and preliminary data. Journal of Family Psychology, 10(3), 243–268. Gough, D. (2007). Weight of evidence: a framework for the appraisal of the quality and relevance of evidence, Research Papers in Education, 22(2), 213-228. Green, J., & Goldwyn, R. (2002). Annotation: Attachment disorganization and psychopathology: New findings in attachment research and their potential implications for developmental psychopathology in childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 835–846. Greenberg, M.T., Kusche, C.A., Cook, E.T., & Quamma, J.P. (1995). Promoting emotional competence in school-aged children: The effects of the PATHS curriculum. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 117–136. 35 Havighurst, S. S., Harley, A., & Prior, M. (2004). Building Preschool Children’s Emotional Competence: A Parenting Program. Early Education & Development, 15(4), 423–448. Havighurst, S. S., Wilson, K. R., Harley, A. E., Kehoe, C., Efron, D., & Prior, M. R. (2013). “Tuning into Kids”: reducing young children’s behavior problems using an emotion coaching parenting program. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 44(2), 247–64. Havighurst, S. S., Wilson, K. R., Harley, A. E., & Prior, M. R. (2009). Tuning in to kids: an emotionfocused parenting program-initial findings from a community trial. Journal of Community Psychology, 37(8), 1008–1023. Havighurst, S. S., Wilson, K. R., Harley, A. E., Prior, M. R., & Kehoe, C. (2010). Tuning in to Kids: improving emotion socialization practices in parents of preschool children--findings from a community trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 51(12), 1342– 50. Kehoe, C. E., Havighurst, S. S., & Harley, A. E. (2014). Tuning in to Teens: Improving Parent Emotion Socialization to Reduce Youth Internalizing Difficulties. Social Development, 23(2), 413– 431. Kovács, M. (1981). Rating scales to assess depression in school-aged children. Acta Paedopsychiatrica, 46 (5–6), 305–315. Kovács, M., & Beck, A. (1977). An empirical clinical approach toward a definition of childhood depression. In J. G. Schulterbrandt & A. Raskin (Eds.), Depression in children: Diagnosis, treatment and conceptual models (pp. 1–25). New York: Raven Press. Kratochwill, T. R. (2003). Evidence-Based Practice: Promoting Evidence-Based Interventions in School Psychology. School Psychology Quarterly, 18(4), 389-408. 36 LaFreniere, P. J., & Dumas, J. E. (1995). Social competence and behaviour evaluation (preschool edition). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. LaFreniere, P. J., & Dumas, J. E. (1996). Social Competence and Behavior Evaluation in children ages 3 to 6 years: The Short Form (SCBE-30). Psychological Assessment, 8, 369–377. Laible, D. J., & Thompson, R. A. (1998). Attachment and emotional understanding in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1038–1045. Lauw, M. S. M., Havighurst, S. S., Wilson, K. R., Harley, A. E., & Northam, E. A. (2014). Improving Parenting of Toddlers’ Emotions Using an Emotion Coaching Parenting Program: a Pilot Study of Tuning in To Toddlers. Journal of Community Psychology, 42(2), 169–175. Meins, E. (2003). Emotional Development and Early attachment relationships. In Slater,A., and Bremner, G. Developmental psychology: An introduction. Oxford: Blackwell. Morris, S. B. (2003). Estimating Effect Sizes From Pretest-Posttest-Control Group Designs. Organizational Research Methods, 11, 364–386. Nauta, M. H., Scholing, A., Rapee, R. M., Abbott, M., Spence, S. H., & Waters, A. (2004). A parent report measure of children’s anxiety: Psychometric properties and comparison with child-report in a clinic and normal sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 813–839. Noble, T., & McGrath, H. (2008). The positive educational practices framework: A tool for facilitating the work of educational psychologists in promoting pupil wellbeing. Educational and Child Psychology, 25, 119–134. Sellman, E. (2009). Lessons learned: student voice at a school for pupils experiencing social, emotional and behavioural difficulties. Emotional and Behavioural Difficulties, 14(January 2015), 33–48. 37 Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1999). Emotion Regulation Checklist. Unpublished questionnaire, Mt. Hope Family Center, University of Rochester, NY. Spence, S. H. (1998). A measure of anxiety symptoms among children. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 545–566. Wilson, K. R., Havighurst, S. S., & Harley, A. E. (2012). Tuning in to Kids: an effectiveness trial of a parenting program targeting emotion socialization of preschoolers. Journal of Family Psychology : JFP : Journal of the Division of Family Psychology of the American Psychological Association (Division 43), 26(1), 56–65. 38 Appendices Appendix A – Excluded studies Excluded Studies – excluded at full text Studies Havighurst, S. S., Kehoe, C. E., & Harley, A. E. (Under review). Tuning in to Teens: Improving Parental Responses to Anger and Reducing Youth Externalizing Behavior Problems. Development and Psychopathology. Kehoe, C. E., Havighurst, S. S., & Harley, A. E. (Early view). Somatic complaints in early adolescence: The role of parents’ emotion socialisation. Journal of Early Adolescence. Reason for exclusion 6b – This study used the same sample as the Tuning into Teens study by Kehoe et al (2014) which looked at internalizing behaviour. 6b – This study also used the same sample as the Tuning into teems study by Kehoe et al (2014). 39 Appendix B – Included studies Included studies Havighurst, S. S., Harley, A., & Prior, M. (2004). Building Preschool Children’s Emotional Competence: A Parenting Program. Early Education & Development, 15(4), 423–448. doi:10.1207/s15566935eed1504_5 Havighurst, S. S., Wilson, K. R., Harley, A. E., Kehoe, C., Efron, D., & Prior, M. R. (2013). “Tuning into Kids”: reducing young children’s behavior problems using an emotion coaching parenting program. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 44(2), 247–64. Havighurst, S. S., Wilson, K. R., Harley, A. E., & Prior, M. R. (2009). Tuning in to kids: an emotionfocused parenting program-initial findings from a community trial. Journal of Community Psychology, 37(8), 1008–1023. doi:10.1002/jcop.20345 Havighurst, S. S., Wilson, K. R., Harley, A. E., Prior, M. R., & Kehoe, C. (2010). Tuning in to Kids: improving emotion socialization practices in parents of preschool children--findings from a community trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 51(12), 1342–50. Kehoe, C. E., Havighurst, S. S., & Harley, A. E. (2014). Tuning in to Teens: Improving Parent Emotion Socialization to Reduce Youth Internalizing Difficulties. Social Development, 23(2), 413–431. Lauw, M. S. M., Havighurst, S. S., Wilson, K. R., Harley, A. E., & Northam, E. a. (2014). Improving Parenting of Toddlers’ Emotions Using an Emotion Coaching Parenting Program: a Pilot Study of Tuning in To Toddlers. Journal of Community Psychology, 42(2), 169–175. Wilson, K. R., Havighurst, S. S., & Harley, A. E. (2012). Tuning in to Kids: an effectiveness trial of a parenting program targeting emotion socialization of preschoolers. Journal of Family Psychology : JFP : Journal of the Division of Family Psychology of the American Psychological Association (Division 43), 26(1), 56–65. 40 Appendix C – Mapping the field A - Information Author and Aim (relevant to review) 1) A – Havighurst, Wilson, Harley and Prior (2009) – To report on an initial evaluation of a communitybased parenting program teaching skills to parents that impact on children’s emotional competence and behaviour Sample size a) 218 Intervention 106 Control - 111 – Sample Characteristics (age, gender, any presenting difficulties) a) Primary caregivers - Mothers (n=209) and Fathers (n=9) (Mean age=36.52, SD=4.98) and their children (age = 4-5.11 years) Country Intervention A+B) Australia A + B) Tuning into Kids intervention. Six two-hours a week session delivered to parents (average – 10 parents per group) by trained facilitators. Control group – waitlist control who were offered a 10-month delayed start for the intervention B – Havighurst, Wilson, Harley, Prior and Kehoe (2010) (Follow up study of Havighurst et al, 2009) To consider whether the Tuning into Kids (TIK) intervention resulted in children’s emotion competence and behaviour b) 216 Intervention 106 Control - 110 – b) Primary caregivers - Mothers (n=207) and Fathers (n=9) (Mean age=36.57, SD=4.97) and their children (mean age = 56.28 months, SD= 4.59 months) Community sample obtained from preschools. 2) Havighurst, Wilson, Harley, Kehoe, Efron and Prior (2013) Would emotion socialisation factors related to children’s emotion competence and behaviour improve after parents participated in the TIK program? Would the TIK program then improve children’s emotion knowledge and reduce their behaviour problems? 54 Intervention – 31 Control - 23 Primary caregivers (all mothers, Mean age= 35.66, SD =6.73) of children (mean age = 59.31 months, SD= 7.38) attending a behaviour clinic as they presented with externalising behaviour difficulties. Australia Tuning into Kids intervention. Six twohours a week session delivered to parents (8-14 parents per group) by trained facilitators. Intervention groups consisted of part of the clinical sample alongside another community sample. Control group – treatment as usual group who received paediatric treatment involving guidance on behavioural strategies, speech and language, psychology and occupational therapy where needed. 41 3) Kehoe, Havighurst and Harley (2014) Does the Tuning into Teens program reduce youth internalizing difficulties? 225 121 (intervention) Primary caregivers (200 mothers, 25 fathers, mean age=44.1, SD=5.13) of adolescents (mean age= 12.01, SD= .42) Australia Community sample obtained from schools 4) 5) 6) Lauw, Havighurst, Wilson and Harley (2014) To determine whether an emotion-coaching intervention, with adaptations to address the toddler developmental stage, would be worth investigating further Wilson, Havighurst and Harley (2012) To evaluate the effectiveness of the Tuning into Kids program under real-world conditions. Havighurst, Harley and Prior (2004) A program teaching parenting skills in emotional awareness, acceptance and coaching would lead to an improvement in emotion competence and reduction in child behaviour problems 104 (control) 34 Pre and post measures design 128 62 (intervention) 66 (control) 47 Pre and post measures design Primary caregivers (mothers, mean age=35.91, SD=3.36) and their toddlers (mean age= 25.37 months, SD=6.15) Tuning into Teens (TINT) – an adapted version of TIK aimed at parents of adolescents. Control group – no intervention Australia Tuning into Toddlers (TOTS) – an adapted version of TIK aimed at parents of toddlers Sample obtained from Maternal and Child Health centres Primary caregivers (118 mothers, 10 fathers, mean age= 36.3, SD=4.3) and their children (mean age=4.19, SD=.41) Australia Tuning into Kids program Community sample obtained from preschools. Primary caregivers (43 mothers and 4 fathers) and their children (aged between 4 and 5 years). Australia Control group – waitlist control group A parenting program delivered in six, twohour sessions per week. Sample obtained from kindergarten classes. 42 B - Methods Author and Aim (relevant to review) 1) A – Havighurst, Wilson, Harley and Prior (2009) – To report on an initial evaluation of a community-based parenting program teaching skills to parents that impact on children’s emotional competence and behaviour B - Havighurst, Wilson, Harley, Prior and Kehoe (2010) (Follow up study of Havighurst et al, 2009) To consider whether the Tuning into Kids (TIK) intervention resulted in children’s emotion competence and behaviour 2) Havighurst, Wilson, Harley, Kehoe, Efron and Prior (2013) Would emotion socialisation factors related to children’s emotion competence and behaviour improve after parents participated in the TIK program? Would the TIK program then improve children’s emotion knowledge and reduce their behaviour problems? Measures (Child measures only) A) The Eyberg Child Behaviour Inventory 6 (ECBI; Eyberg and Pincus, 1999). This is a 36-item parent report scale that measures perceptions of children’s problem behaviours. b) ECBI and Sutter-Eyberg Student Behvaiour Inventory – a teacher’s version of the ECBI. The Eyberg Child Inventory 6 (ECBI) Behaviour Sutter-Eyberg Student Behaviour Inventory n.b. -Post-intervention measures taken immediately after program ended. Follow-up measures taken 6-month post-intervention Analyses a) A one way ANOVA was used to compare post-intervention differences between the control group and intervention group. b) General Linear Modelling (GLM) repeated measures was used to analyse condition i.e. intervention vs. control across pre, post and follow-up. Data were analysed using Growth Curve Modelling (GCM) and an ANCOVA. Outcomes (child outcomes only) a) Behaviour problems (intensity) – researchers found a significant improvement in the intervention group F(1, 181) = 18.39, p<.001 Follow up Reported in 1b b) Behaviour problems (intensity) – Researchers found a significant interaction between condition and time on the EBCI, F(1, 169) = 11.14. p<.001 On the SESBI, there was also a significant interaction between condition and time F(1,150)=6.87, p=.02 Behaviour Outcomes - No significant difference found between intervention and control group for rate of improvement in behaviour intensity, F (1,34) = -2.031, p=.208 and behaviour problems, F(1,34) = 1.002, p=.098 Statistical data provided for 6-month follow-up is in the form of means and standard deviations and effect size. ANCOVA at time 2 showed that parents in the intervention condition reported significantly lower child behaviour intensity than the control group, F(1,34) = 6.32, p =.009 For the intervention group, ECBI Behaviour intensity Cohen’s d = .74 and for behaviour problem Cohen’s d= 1.00. For the control group Cohen’s d=.58 and .55 respectively. ANCOVAs of teacher reports on child behaviour indicated that at follow-up, children in the intervention condition were perceived to have lower behaviour intensity, F(1,26) = 4.87, p=.036, partial eta squared =.16, and fewer behaviour problems F(1,26) = 4.87, p=.036, partial eta squared = .16 For the intervention group, SESBI behaviour intensity, Cohen’s d = .56 and for behaviour problem Cohen’s d= .46 43 3) Kehoe, Havighurst and Harley (2014) Does the Tuning into Teens program reduce youth internalizing difficulties? Spence children’s anxiety scale (SCAS; Spence, 1998). This is a self-report measure of youth anxiety symptoms. Multi-level mixed effect models were used to analyse data. Youth with parents in the intervention condition reported significantly lower anxiety, F(1,217.36) = -2.17, p=0.31. n.b. – post intervention measures taken at 6month follow up only Parents in the intervention condition reported significantly lower youth anxiety, F(1,215) = -4.92, p<.001. Spence child anxiety scale for parents (SCAS-P; Nauta et al, 2004). This a parent-report of youth anxiety symptoms. No significant difference was found for youth reported depressive symptoms, F(1,206.97) = -1.17, p=.244 Child Depression Inventory shortform child self-report (CDI:S; Kovacs, 1981; Kovacs and Beck, 1977). This measured self-reported youth depressive symptoms. Parent reported youth depression was also significantly lower for the intervention group, F(1,215.46) = -4.06, p<.001 Child depression Inventory parent report (CDI:P; Garber,1984). A parent report of youth depressive symptoms. 4) Lauw, Havighurst, Wilson and Harley (2014) To determine whether an emotion-coaching intervention, with adaptations to address the toddler developmental stage, would be worth investigating further 5) Wilson, Havighurst and Harley (2012) To evaluate the effectiveness of the Tuning into Kids program under real-world conditions. Brief Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (BriggsGowan and Carter,2007) – an 11item subscale measuring behaviour problems. n.b. pre-post intervention measures only The Eyberg Child Behaviour Inventory 6 (ECBI) The short form of the Social Competence and Behaviour Evaluation (SCBE-30; LaFreniere and Dumas, 1995,1996) is a teacher-report measure of social competence, affective expression and adjustment on 2.5-6 year old children. Paired samples t-tests were used to analyse data. Behaviour problems Parents reported significantly lower toddler externalising behaviour difficulties t(1,33)=-2.14, p<.05 None Mixed effects multilevel modelling used to analyse data. For interaction between time and condition, parent reported behaviour intensity were non-significant, F(1,123.46) = 2.80, p = .97, as were parent reports of behaviour problem frequency F(1,123.78) = 2.68, p=.104 n.b – postintervention measures taken at 6month follow-up only No significant effects were found for teacher reports of anger and aggression, F(1,117.87) = 0.04, p=.843 44 6) Havighurst, Harley and Prior (2004) A program teaching parenting skills in emotional awareness, acceptance and coaching would lead to an improvement in emotion competence and reduction in child behaviour problems The Emotion Regulation Checklist (ERC; Shields and Cicchetti, 1999). This is a parent-report measure of Lability-Negativity and Emotion regulation. The Eyberg Child Behaviour Inventory (ECBI: Eyberg and Robinson,1983). This is a 36-item parent repoprt measure of perceptions of conduct problem behaviours in children. A MANOVA was used to analyse data. ECBI A significant effect was found for Behaviour intensity, F(2,44) = 10.62, p <.001 and behaviour problems, F(2,44) = 9.94, p<.001 ERC A significant effect was found for emotion lability/negativity F(2,45)=8.06, p<.001. Statistical data provided for 6-month follow-up is in the form of means and standard deviations only No significant effects found on emotional difficulties and emotion regulation. Univariate Repeated measures ANOVA showed that for the group with lower scores on the ECBI, there were no significant changes in ECBI intensity post intervention, F(2,22) = 2.79, n.s. For the group with higher scores on the ECBI, significant improvements were found post-intervention, F(2,22) = 2.79 =9.66,p<.001 45 Appendix D Coding protocols and items removed from APA Task Force Coding Protocol by Kratochwill (2003) Coding Protocol: Group-Based Design Domain: School- and community-based intervention programs for social and behavioral problems Academic intervention programs Family and parent intervention programs School-wide and classroom-based programs Comprehensive and coordinated school health services Name of Coder(s): Date: 27/12/14 M /D/Y Full Study Reference in APA format: Full Study Reference in proper format:__ Havighurst, S. S., Wilson, K. R., Harley, A. E., Kehoe, C., Efron, D., & Prior, M. R. (2013). “Tuning into Kids”: reducing young children’s behavior problems using an emotion coaching parenting program. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 44(2), 247–64 Intervention Name (description from study): Tuning into Kids Study ID Number (Unique Identifier): 03 Type of Publication: (Check one) Book/Monograph Journal article Book chapter Other (specify) 46 I. General Characteristics A. General Design Characteristics A1. Random assignment designs (if random assignment design, select one of the following) A1.1 A1.2 A1.3 A1.4 Completely randomized design Randomized block design (between-subjects variation) Randomized block design (within-subjects variation) Randomized hierarchical design A2. Nonrandomized designs (if nonrandom assignment design, select one of the following) A2.1 A2.2 A2.3 A2.4 A2.5 Nonrandomized design Nonrandomized block design (between-participants variation) Nonrandomized block design (within-participants variation) Nonrandomized hierarchical design Optional coding of Quasi-experimental designs (see Appendix C) A3. Overall confidence of judgment on how participants were assigned (select one of the following) A3.1 A3.2 A3.3 A3.4 A3.5 A3.6 A3.7 Very low (little basis) Low (guess) Moderate (weak inference) High (strong inference) Very high (explicitly stated) N/A Unknown/unable to code B. Statistical Treatment/Data Analysis (answer B1 through B6) B1. Appropriate unit of analysis B2. Familywise error rate controlled B3. Sufficiently large N Statistical Test:Growth Curve Modelling and ANCOVA _ level: ES: .5 N required: 64 yes no yes no yes no N/A B4. Total size of sample (start of the study): 54 N B5. Intervention group sample size: 31 N B6. Control group sample size: 23 N 47 C. Type of Program (select one) C1. C2. C3. C4. C5. Universal prevention program Selective prevention program Targeted prevention program Intervention/Treatment Unknown D. Stage of the Program (select one) D1. D2. D3. D4. Model/demonstration programs Early stage programs Established/institutionalized programs Unknown E. Concurrent or Historical Intervention Exposure (select one) E1. E2. E3. II. Current exposure Prior exposure Unknown Key Features for Coding Studies and Rating Level of Evidence/ Support (3=Strong Evidence 2=Promising Evidence 1=Weak Evidence 0=No Evidence) A. Measurement (answer A1 through A4) A1. Use of outcome measures that produce reliable scores for the majority of primary outcomes. The table for Primary/Secondary Outcomes Statistically Significant allows for listing separate outcomes and will facilitate decision making regarding measurement (select one of the following) 1.1 Yes 1.2 A1.2 No A1.3 Unknown/unable to code A2. Multi-method (select one of the following) A2.1 A2.2 A2.3 A2.4 Yes No N/A Unknown/unable to code A3. Multi-source (select one of the following) A3.1 A3.2 A3.3 A3.4 Yes No N/A Unknown/unable to code 48 A4. Validity of measures reported (select one of the following) A5.1 A5.2 A5.3 A5.4 Yes validated with specific target group In part, validated for general population only No Unknown/unable to code Rating for Measurement (select 0, 1, 2, or 3): 3 2 1 0 B. Comparison Group B1. Type of Comparison Group (select one of the following) B1.1 B1.2 B1.3 B1.4 B1.5 B1.6 B1.7 B1.8 B1.9 B1.10 Typical contact Typical contact (other) specify: Attention placebo Intervention elements placebo Alternative intervention PharmacotherapyB1.1 No intervention Wait list/delayed intervention Minimal contact Unable to identify comparison group B2. Overall confidence rating in judgment of type of comparison group (select one of the following) B2.1 B2.2 B2.3 B2.4 B2.5 B2.6 Very low (little basis) Low (guess) Moderate (weak inference) High (strong inference) Very high (explicitly stated) Unknown/Unable to code B3. Counterbalancing of Change Agents (answer B3.1 to B3.3) B3.1 By change agent B3.2 Statistical B3.3. Other B4. Group Equivalence Established (select one of the following) B4.1 B4.2 B4.3 B4.4 Random assignment Posthoc matched set Statistical matching Post hoc test for group equivalence B5. Equivalent Mortality (answer B5.1 through B5.3) B5.1 Low Attrition (less than 20% for Post) B5.2 Low Attrition (less than 30% for follow-up) 24% attrition B5.3 Intent to intervene analysis carried out Findings Rating for Comparison Group (select 0, 1, 2, or 3): 3 2 1 0 49 B2. Overall confidence rating in judgment of type of comparison group (select one of the following) B2.1 B2.2 B2.3 B2.4 B2.5 B2.6 Very low (little basis) Low (guess) Moderate (weak inference) High (strong inference) Very high (explicitly stated) Unknown/Unable to code B3. Counterbalancing of Change Agents (answer B3.1 to B3.3) B3.1 By change agent B3.2 Statistical B3.3. Other B4. Group Equivalence Established (select one of the following) B4.1 B4.2 B4.3 B4.4 Random assignment Posthoc matched set Statistical matching Post hoc test for group equivalence B5. Equivalent Mortality (answer B5.1 through B5.3) B5.1 Low Attrition (less than 20% for Post) B5.2 Low Attrition (less than 30% for follow-up) – 24% attrition B5.3 Intent to intervene analysis carried out Findings 50 D. Educational/Clinical Significance Pretest Outcome Variables: D1. Categorical Diagnosis Data Diagnostic information regarding inclusion into the study presented: Yes No Posttest Follow Up Positive change in diagnostic criteria from pre to posttest: Positive change in diagnostic criteria from posttest to follow up: Unknown Yes D2. Outcome Assessed via continuous Variables No Unknown Yes Positive change in percentage of participants showing clinical improvement from pre to posttest: Yes No Unknown Importance of behavior change is evaluated: Yes No Unknown Importance of behavior change from pre to posttest is evaluated positively by individuals in direct contact with the participant: D4. Social Comparison: Behavior of participant at pre, post, and follow up is compared to normative data (e.g., a typical peer). Participant’s behavior is compared to normative data Yes No No No Unknown Importance of behavior change from posttest to follow up is evaluated positively by individuals in direct contact with the participant: Yes Yes Unknown Positive change in percentage of participants showing clinical improvement from posttest to follow up: Yes D3. Subjective Evaluation: The importance of behavior change is evaluated by individuals in direct contact with the participant. No No Unknown Unknown Participant’s behavior has improved from pre to posttest when compared to normative data: Participant’s behavior has improved from posttest to follow up when compared to normative data: Unknown Yes Rating for Educational/Clinical Significance (select 0, 1, 2, or 3): No 3 Unknown 2 Yes 1 No Unknown 0 F. Implementation Fidelity F1. Evidence of Acceptable Adherence (answer F1.1 throughF1.3) F1.1 Ongoing supervision/consultation F1.2 Coding intervention sessions/lessons or procedures F1.3 Audio/video tape implementation (select F1.3.1 or F1.3.2): F1.3.1 Entire intervention F1.3.2 Part of intervention F2. Manualization (select all that apply) F2.1 Written material involving a detailed account of the exact procedures and the sequence in which they are to be used F2.2 Formal training session that includes a detailed account of the exact procedures and the sequence in which they are to be used F2.3 Written material involving an overview of broad principles and a description of the intervention phases 51 F2.4 Formal or informal training session involving an overview of broad principles and a description of the intervention phases F3. Adaptation procedures are specified (select one) Rating for Implementation Fidelity (select 0, 1, 2, or 3): yes no 3 2 unknown 1 0 H. Site of Implementation H1. School (if school is the site, select one of the followingoptions) H1.1 H1.2 H1.3 H1.4 H1.5 H1.6 Public Private Charter University Affiliated Alternative Not specified/unknown H2. Non School Site (if it is a non school site, select one of the following options) H2.1 H2.2 H2.3 H2.4 H2.5 H2.6 H2.7 H2.8 H2.9 H2.10 H2.11 Home University Clinic Summer Program Outpatient Hospital Partial inpatient/day Intervention Program Inpatient Hospital Private Practice Mental Health Center Residential Treatment Facility Other (specify): community settings Unknown/insufficient information provided I. Follow Up Assessment Timing of follow up assessment: specify 6 months Number of participants included in the follow up assessment: specify 41____ Consistency of assessment method used: specify Same method used Rating for Follow Up Assessment (select 0, 1, 2, or 3): 3 2 1 0 III. Other Descriptive or Supplemental Criteria to Consider A. External Validity Indicators A1. Sampling procedures described in detail yes no Specify rationale for selection: Participants chosen if children had elevated scores on ECBI Specify rationale for sample size: A1.1Inclusion/exclusion criteria specified yes no 52 A1.2 Inclusion/exclusion criteria similar to school practice A1.3 Specified criteria related to concern yes yes no no A2. Participant Characteristics Specified for Treatment and Control Group 53 Participants from Treatment Group Child/Student Parent/caregiver Teacher School Other Child/Student Parent/caregiver Teacher School Other Grade/age 35.66 Gender Ethnicity or Multiethnic Ethnic Identity Female Australia n Race(s) Acculturation White Pri mary Language SES Family Structure Locale Disability Functional Descriptors Disability Functional Descriptors English Worki unknow Australi ng/mid n a dle class Child/Student Parent/caregiver Teacher School Other Child/Student Parent/caregiver Teacher School Other Participants from Control Group Child/Student Parent/caregiver Teacher School Other Child/Student Parent/caregiver Teacher School Other Grade/age 35.66 Gender Ethnicity or Multiethnic Ethnic Identity Female Australia n Race(s) Acculturation White Pri mary Language SES Family Structure Locale English Worki unknow Australi 35.66 ng/mid n a dle class Child/Student Parent/caregiver Teacher School Other Child/Student Parent/caregiver Teacher School Other A3. Details are provided regarding variables that: A3.1 Have differential relevance for intended outcomes yes no Specify: 54 A3.2 Have relevance to inclusion criteria yes no Specify: Participants excluded if they did not have sufficient English language skills A5. Generalization of Effects: A5.1 Generalization over time A5.1.1 Evidence is provided regarding the sustainability of outcomes after intervention is terminated yes no Specify: 6 month follow up measures reported A5.1.2 Procedures for maintaining outcomes are specified yes no Specify: A5.2 Generalization across settings A5.2.1 Evidence is provided regarding the extent to which outcomes are manifested in contexts that are different from the intervention context yes no Specify: A5.2.2 Documentation of efforts to ensure application of intervention to other settings yes no 55 Specify: A5.2.3 Impact on implementers or context is sustained yes no Specify: A5.3 Generalization across persons Evidence is provided regarding the degree to which outcomes are manifested with participants who are different than the original group of participants for with the intervention was evaluated yes no Specify: B. Length of Intervention (select B1 or B2) B1. Unknown/insufficient information provided B2. Information provided (if information is provided, specify one of the following:) B2.1 weeks 6 N B2.2 months N B2.3 years N B2.4 other N C. Intensity/dosage of Intervention (select C1 or C2) C1. Unknown/insufficient information provided C2. Information provided (if information is provided, specify both of the following:) C2.1 length of intervention session 2 hours N C2.2 frequency of intervention session weekly N D. Dosage Response (select D1 or D2) D1. D2. Unknown/insufficient information provided Information provided (if information is provided, answer D2.1) 56 D2.1 Describe positive outcomes associated with higher dosage: 57 E. Program Implementer (select all that apply) E1. E2. E3. E4. E5. E6. E7. E8. E9. Research Staff School Specialty Staff Teachers Educational Assistants Parents College Students Peers Other Unknown/insufficient information provided F. Characteristics of the Intervener F1. F2. F3. Highly similar to target participants on key variables (e.g., race, gender, SES) Somewhat similar to target participants on key variables Different from target participants on key variables G. Intervention Style or Orientation (select all that apply) G1. G2. G3. G4. G5. G6. G7. Behavioral Cognitive-behavioral Experiential Humanistic/interpersonal Psychodynamic/insight oriented other (specify): Unknown/insufficient information provided H. Cost Analysis Data (select G1 or G2) H1. H2. Unknown/insufficient information provided Information provided (if information is provided, answer H2.1) H2.1 Estimated Cost of Implementation: I. Training and Support Resources (select all that apply) I1. I2. Simple orientation given to change agents Training workshops conducted # of Workshops provided Average length of training Who conducted training (select all that apply) I2.1 I2.2 Project Director Graduate/project assistants 58 I2.3 I2.3 I3. I4. I5. I6. Other (please specify): Unknown Ongoing technical support Program materials obtained Special Facilities Other (specify): J. Feasibility J1. Level of difficulty in training intervention agents (select one of the following) J1.1 J1.2 J1.3 J1.4 High Moderate Low Unknown J2. Cost to train intervention agents (specify if known): J3. Rating of cost to train intervention agents (select one of the following) J3.1 J3.2 J3.3 J3.4 High Moderate Low Unknown 59 Summary of Evidence for Group-Based Design Studies Overall Evidence Rating Indicator Description of Evidence Strong Promising Weak No/limited evidence NNR = No numerical rating or or 0-3 Descriptive ratings General Characteristics General Design Characteristics NNR Statistical Treatment NNR Type of Program NNR Stage of Program NNR Concurrent/Historical Intervention Exposure NNR Key Features Measurement 3 Strong Comparison Group 3 Strong Primary/Secondary Outcomes are Statistically Significant Educational/clinical significance N/A Identifiable Components N/A Implementation Fidelity 3 Replication N/A Site of Implementation N/A Follow Up Assessment Conducted 3 3 Strong Strong Strong 59 Appendix E Weighting of studies The APA Task Force Coding Protocol by Kratochwill (2003) coding protocol was used to code each of the studies in order to generate a ‘Weight of Evidence A’ rating for included studies. The table below shows the adaptations made to the protocol along with a rationale for these amendments. Items removed Rationale Sections I.B.7 - B.8 Studies did not use qualitative research methods. Section II.C The protocol was used to rate the methodological quality of the included studies. Outcomes are examined separately in the review Section II.D removed (with the exception of Havighurst et al 2013) All studies being reviewed (apart from one) examined the programme as a prevention/universal program therefore this section was not relevant for the type of studies in the review. Section II.E removed The intervention components are not separated. Section II.G removed There was no within study replication. Rating scale for section II.H removed As the review question was concerned with whether the program would enable parents to improve behavioural outcomes for their children, rating the site of implementation was irrelevant in ascertaining the methodological quality of each study. However for information purposes, details on site of implementation was left in. Table in section III.A4 Receptivity of intervention by target group was not removed deemed necessary in determining methodological quality nor was it reported in the studies. 60 Weight of Evidence A: Methodological Quality This was assessed based on the guidance for methodological quality detailed in the Kratochwill (2003) coding protocol. Studies were weighted on ‘measures’, ‘comparison group’, ‘fidelity’, and ‘follow-up’. 1. Measures Weighting High Medium Low Descriptors - Studies used measures that produce reliable scores of at least .70, for the majority of primary outcomes - Data was collected using multiple methods, and collected from multiple sources - Validity is reported - Studies used measures that produce reliable scores of at least .70 - Data was collected using multiple methods, and/or collected from multiple sources - A case for validity does not need to be presented. - Studies used measures that produce reliable scores of at least .50 - Data may have been collected either using multiple methods and/or from multiple sources however, this is not required. - A case for validity does not need to be presented. 2. Comparison group Weighting High Medium Descriptors - Uses at least one type of "active" comparison group - Initial group equivalency must be established - Evidence that change agents were counterbalanced - Equivalent mortality and low attrition at post, and if applicable, at follow-up - Uses at least a "no intervention group" type of comparison - There is evidence for at least two of the following: counterbalancing of change agents, group equivalence established, or 61 - Low - - equivalent mortality with low attrition. If equivalent mortality is not demonstrated, an intent-to intervene analysis is conducted. The study uses a comparison group At least one of the following is present: counterbalancing of change agents, group equivalence established, or equivalent mortality with low attrition. If equivalent mortality is not demonstrated, an intent-tointervene analysis is conducted. 3. Fidelity Weighting High Medium Measures - The study demonstrates strong evidence of acceptable adherence. - Evidence of fidelity is measured through at least two of the following: ongoing supervision/consultation, coding sessions, or audio/video tapes, and use of a manual. - The “manual” is either written materials involving a detailed account of the exact procedures and the sequence in which they are to be used or formal training session detailing exact procedures and sequence. - The study demonstrates evidence of acceptable adherence. - Evidence of fidelity is measured through at least one of the following: ongoing supervision/consultation, coding sessions, or audio/video tapes, and use of a manual. - The “manual” is either written materials involving an overview of broad principles and a 62 Low - description of the intervention phases, or formal/informal training session involving an overview of broad principles and a description of the intervention phases. Demonstrates evidence of acceptable adherence measured through at least one of the above criteria or use of a manual 4. Follow-up Weighting High Medium Low Descriptors - The study conducted follow up assessments over multiple intervals with all participants that were included in the original sample - Uses similar measures used to analyse data from primary or secondary outcomes - The study conducted follow up assessments at least once with the majority of participants that were included in the original sample - Similar measures used to analyse data from primary or secondary outcomes - The study conducted follow up assessments at least once with some participants from the original sample. Overall methodological quality The following ratings were assigned to each weighting to calculate the overall methodological quality of the studies: High weightings = 3 Medium weightings = 2 Low weightings = 1 No rating = 0 63 The scores were averaged: Overall Methodological Quality High Medium Low Average scores >2.5 1.5 - 2.4 <1.4 Study Measures Comparison Fidelity Group Follow-up Havighurst et al (2009) and Havighurst et al (2010) Havighurst et al (2011) Kehoe et al (2014) Lauw et al (2014) Wilson et al (2012) Havighurst et al (2004) 3 2 3 2 Overall Quality of Methodology 2.5 3 3 3 3 3 3 2 3 2 2.5 2 0 3 0 1.25 3 2 3 3 2.75 2 0 2 3 1.75 n.b. Havighurst et al (2009) and Havighurst et al (2010) have been weighted as one study (1a and 1b) as the former reports preliminary post-intervention findings and the latter reports full post-intervention findings as well as follow-up outcomes of the same sample. 64 Weight of Evidence B: Methodological relevance This weighting is a review-specific judgement about the suitability of the evidence for answering the review question. Weighting High Medium Low Descriptors An ‘active’ comparison group is used Participants are randomly assigned to groups Pre, post and follow up measures are taken for both groups. A waitlist/no intervention comparison group is used Participants are randomly assigned to groups Pre and post or pre and follow-up measures are taken for both groups Pre and post measures are taken As the TI programme is a relatively new intervention, the review question sought to review the efficacy of the studies. Therefore in WoE B, a classic evidence hierarchy (Evans, 2003) was used to influence descriptors for the weighting of studies. Therefore, randomised controlled trials (RCTs) are deemed as high quality evidence as results are at lower risk of error or bias that a study that has no control group or a sample that was not randomised to conditions. The addition of an ‘active comparison group’ to the ‘high’ category is due to the fact that the use of an active comparison group can show that the intervention being researched is better than the usual/alternative form of intervention suggested for a presenting problem whereas a “no intervention/waitlist control” group just shows that the intervention is better than not intervening at all. Finally, pre, post and follow up measures are desirable as it shows not only the baseline measures or participants before intervention versus immediately post-intervention it also shows the durability of the intervention effects which adds to the evaluation of the intervention’s efficacy. 65 Weight of Evidence C: Topic Relevance This weighting is a review specific judgement about the relevance of the focus of the evidence for review question Weighting High Descriptors The Tuning into Kids/Teens/Toddlers programme is delivered by trained psychologists. Includes only children that are at the clinical cut-off on the EBCI/other behavioural measures Medium Behavioural measures are gathered from parents and teachers or parents and child/young person The Tuning into Kids/Teens/Toddlers programme is delivered by trained psychologists. Includes all children regardless of baseline scores on ECBI/other behavioural measures Low Behavioural measures are gathered from parents and teachers or parents and child/young person. The Tuning into Kids/Teens/Toddlers programme is delivered by trained professionals Includes all children regardless of baseline scores on ECBI/other behavioural measures Behavioural measures are gathered from parents It is argued that TI is a specialised intervention that involves teaching parents how to emotion coach their children using highly specialised psychological techniques as well as the ability to teach parents about their own emotions. Therefore it is argued that the person delivering the parent training would need to be a psychologist to be able to deliver this to the maximum effect, whereas, although a manual is used, training professionals outside of the field to deliver this intervention may not be as effective. Studies including children that are at the clinical cut off on the ECBI were weighted as highest as this demonstrates that the intervention’s effectiveness as treatment for children at the clinical level of behavioural difficulties. Finally, the if measures were multi-source e.g. derived from parents and teachers or parents and the child/young 66 person, then this shows evidence of triangulating data which provides a richer, more valid report of the outcomes of the intervention. Weight of Evidence D: Overall Weight of Evidence This is an overall assessment of the extent to which the evidence contributes to answering the review question which is assessed by giving studies the following scores: High weightings = 3 Medium weightings = 2 Low weightings = 1 The scores obtained were averaged to give an overall weight of evidence score Overall Weight of Evidence High Medium Low Average scores >2.5 1.5 - 2.4 <1.4 Studies WoE A WoE B Quality of Relevance methodology of Methodology WoE D Overall weight of evidence Havighurst et al (2009) and Havighurst et al (2010) Havighurst et al (2011) Kehoe et al (2014) Lauw et al (2014) Wilson et al (2012) Havighurst et al (2004) High 2.5 Medium 2 WoE C Relevance of evidence to the review question Medium 2 High 3 High 3 High 3 High 3 High 2.5 Medium 1.25 High 2.75 Medium 1.75 Medium 2 Low 1 Medium 2 Low 1 Medium 2 Low 1 Low 1 Medium 2 Medium 2.17 Low 1.08 Medium 1.92 Medium 1.58 Medium 2.17 67 Appendix F Full effect size table Study Measures Number of Participants Outcomes Effect Size Descriptors Pre and post measures 1a. Havighurst et al (2009) – pre-post The Eyberg Child Behaviour Inventory 6 (ECBI; Eyberg and Pincus, 1999 1b. Havighurst et al (2010) – pre-follow up data (Same study as above) 218 1a. Behaviour problems (intensity) – researchers found a significant improvement in the intervention group F(1, 181) = 18.39, p<.001 Smallmedium Effect sizes Pre and measures Effect size descriptors post Pre and follow up measures Smallmedium ECBI Intensity PPC SMD = -0.44 Effect sizes Pre and follow up measures ECBI Intensity Overall Weight of Evidence Medium PPC SMD = - 0.41 Small SESBI PPC SMD = -0.2 216 1b. Behaviour problems – Researchers found a significant interaction between condition and time on the EBCI, F(1, 169) = 11.14. p<.001 On the SESBI, there was also a significant interaction between condition and time F(1,150)=6.87, p=.02 Havighurst et al (2013) The Eyberg Child Behaviour Inventory 6 (ECBI) Sutter-Eyberg Student Behaviour Inventory – a 54 ANCOVA at time 2 showed that parents in the intervention condition reported significantly lower child behaviour intensity than the control group, F(1,34) = 6.32, p =.009, partial eta squared =.16 ANCOVAs of teacher reports on child behaviour indicated that ECBI Intensity High ECBI Small Large Large Intensity PPC SMD= -0.76 PPC SMD = -0.13 Problem Problem PPC SMD= -0.83 PPC SMD = -0.42 Smallmedium SESBI 68 teacher’s version of the ECBI. Kehoe et al (2014) Spence anxiety (SCAS; 1998). children’s scale Spence, at follow-up, children in the intervention condition were perceived to have lower behaviour intensity, F(1,26) = 4.87, p=.036, partial eta squared =.16, and fewer behaviour problems F(1,26) = 4.87, p=.036, partial eta squared = .16 225 Spence child anxiety scale for parents (SCAS-P; Nauta et al, 2004). Medium Small PPC SMD = -0.19 SCAS-P Medium PPC SMD = -0.48 CDI:S Small However no significant difference was found for youth reported depressive symptoms, F(1,206.97) = -1.17, p=.244 Child depression Inventory parent report (CDI:P; Garber,1984) Brief Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment Medium Behaviour problem PPC SMD= -0.5 SCAS Parents in the intervention condition reported significantly lower youth anxiety, F(1,215) = -4.92, p<.001. Child Depression Inventory shortform child selfreport (CDI:S; Kovacs, 1981; Kovacs and Beck, 1977). Lauw et al (2014) Youth with parents in the intervention condition reported significantly lower anxiety, F(1,217.36) = -2.17, p<0.31. Medium Behaviour intensity PPC SMD= -0.58 PPC SMD= -0.13 CDI:P PPC SMD =-0.46 Medium Parent reported youth depression was also significantly lower for the intervention group, F(1,215.46) = -4.06, p<.001 34 Behaviour problems Parents reported significantly lower Low Toddler behaviour problems 69 Wilson et al (2012) (BITSEA; BriggsGowan and Carter,2007) The Eyberg Child Behaviour Inventory 6 (ECBI) The short form of the Social Competence and Behaviour Evaluation (SCBE30; LaFreniere and Dumas, 1995,1996) 128 toddler externalising behaviour difficulties t(1,33)=-2.14, p<.05 Compared to the waitlist control, parents in the intervention group reported significantly lower behaviour problems shown by a significant main effect for condition, F(1,123.8) = 4.99, p=.027 However for interaction between time and condition, parent reported behaviour intensity were nonsignificant, F(1,123.46) = 2.80, p = .97, as were parent reports of behaviour problem frequency F(1,123.78) = 2.68, p=.104 Small PP SMD = -0.26 Medium ECBI Small Behaviour intensity Small PPC SMD= -0.22 Behaviour problem PPC SMD= -0.26 SCBE-30 Anger/aggression Small PPC SMD= 0.02 No significant effects were found for teacher reports of anger and aggression, F(1,117.87) = 0.04, p=.843 70 Havighurst et al (2004) The Emotion Regulation Checklist (ERC; Shields and Cicchetti, 1999) The Eyberg Child Behaviour Inventory (ECBI: Eyberg and Robinson,1983). 47 A significant effect was found for Behaviour intensity, F(2,44) = 10.62, p <.001 and behaviour problems, F(2,44) = 9.94, p<.001 Another significant effect was found for Oppositional Defiant symptoms, Attention Deficit Hyperactive Symptoms, and Conduct Symptoms combined, F(2,44) = 4.23, p<.01 Univariate Repeated measures ANOVA showed that for the group with lower scores on the ECBI, there were no significant changes in ECBI intensity post intervention, F(2,22) = 2.79, n.s. However for the group with higher scores on the ECBI, significant improvements were found post-intervention, F(2,22) = 2.79 =9.66,p<.001 ECBI Medium ECBI Small Small Behaviour intensity PP SMD = -0.26 Behaviour problem PP SMD = -0.31 Smallmedium Behaviour Intensity PP SMD = -0.4 Smallmedium Behaviour Problem PP SMD = -0.42 ERC Lability/negativity ERC Lability/negativity Small PP SMD = -0.31 Smallmedium PP SMD = -0.41 Teacher rated behaviour Medium PP SMD= 0.49 71 0