Early Access to Sign Language Inoculates Deaf Children Against Visual Attention Deficits Presentation language: American Sign Language Early profound deafness brings about a cortical reorganization that enhances specific aspects of visual processing. Studies of adult Deaf native signers have demonstrated that they have enhanced attention to visual motion across the visual field (Bosworth and Dobkins, 2002), and enhanced selective attention to the visual periphery (Bavelier et al., 2000; Bottari et al., 2008). These enhancements appear to be driven by uni-modal and cross-modal changes in cortical function, with enhanced processing in posterior parietal cortex (Bavelier et al., 2000), the posterior superior temporal sulcus (Bavelier et al., 2000), and primary (Karns et al., 2012) and secondary (Finney et al., 2001) auditory areas along the superior temporal gyrus. Behavioral studies (Dye et al., 2009; Codina et al., 2011) suggest that these enhancements emerge during early adolescence, although pediatric imaging work has yet to be undertaken. However, these findings of enhanced visual attention are at odds with the developmental literature, where deaf children are characterized as displaying poor sustained attention, high impulsivity, and high distractibility (Horn et al., 2005; Quittner et al., 1994; Smith et al., 1998; Yucel and Derim, 2008). These developmental studies have typically recruited deaf children who use cochlear implants, and who primarily use spoken language or a combination of speech and sign. In the study reported here, measures of sustained attention, impulsivity, and distractibility were administered to 60 hearing children and 37 Deaf children who acquired American Sign Language as an L1 from Deaf parents (aged 6-13 years). Subjects performed a continuous performance test (Gordon and Mettleman, 1987) that required selective responses to sequences of digits, either with or without distractors. The results indicated few differences in sustained attention as a function of deafness (Figure 1). However, younger deaf children (6-8 years) showing higher levels of impulsivity and distractibility than their hearing peers (Figure 2). The data is interpreted in terms of a spatial redistribution of visual attention (Dye and Bavelier, 2010) that requires executive functions to be harnessed in a goal-directed manner. It is argued that a role for EF explains the interaction between age and deafness, and the "cognitive inoculation" provided by early access to natural (sign) language in deaf children. Future studies are proposed to examine the role of executive functions (inhibitory control, task switching, working memory) as mediators of performance gains resulting from cross-modal plasticity in Deaf children. References Bavelier, D., Tomann, A., Hutton, C., Mitchell, T., Liu, G., Corina, D., & Neville, H. (2000). Visual attention to periphery is enhanced in congenitally deaf individuals. The Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 1-6. Bosworth, R. G., & Dobkins, K. R. (2002). The effects of spatial attention on motion processing in deaf signers, hearing signers, and hearing nonsigners. Brain and Cognition, 49(1), 152169. Bottari, D., Turatto, M., Bonfioli, F., Abbadessa, C., Selmi, S., Beltrame, M. A., & Pavani, F. (2008). Change blindness in profoundly deaf individuals and cochlear implant recipients. Brain Research, 1242, 209-218. Codina, C., Buckley, D., Port, M., & Pascalis, O. (2011). Deaf and hearing children: A comparison of peripheral vision development. Developmental Science, 14(4), 725-737. Dye, M. W. G., & Bavelier, D. (2010). Attentional enhancements and deficits in deaf populations: An integrative review. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 28, 181192. Dye, M. W. G., Hauser, P. C., & Bavelier, D. (2009). Is visual selective attention in deaf individuals enhanced or deficient? The case of the Useful Field of View. PLOS One, 4(5), e5640. Finney, E. M., Fine, I., & Dobkins, K. R. (2001). Visual stimuli activate auditory cortex in the deaf. Nature Neuroscience, 4(12), 1171-1173. Gordon, M., & Mettleman, B. B. (1987). Technical guide to the Gordon Diagnostic System (GDS). Dewitt, NY: Gordon Systems. Horn, D. L., Davis, R. A., Pisoni, D. B., & Miyamoto, R. T. (2005). Development of visual attention skills in prelingually deaf children who use cochlear implants. Ear and Hearing, 26(4), 389-408. Karns, C. M., Dow, M. W., and Neville, H. J. (2012). Altered cross-modal processing in the primary auditory cortex of congenitally deaf adults: A visual-somatosensory fMRI study with a double-flash illusion. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(28), 9626-9638. Quittner, A. L., Smith, L. B., Osberger, M. J., Mitchell, T. V., & Katz, D. B. (1994). The impact of audition on the development of visual attention. Psychological Science, 5(6), 347-353. Smith, L. B., Quittner, A. L., Osberger, M. J., & Miyamoto, R. (1998). Audition and visual attention: the developmental trajectory in deaf and hearing populations. Developmental Psychology, 34(5), 840-850. Figure 1 5" Sensi&vity*(d')* 4" 3" 2" Hearing"(computed"d')" Hearing"(observed"d')" 1" 0" 100" 150" Age*(in*months)* Figure 2 200" 1.2# Hearing# 1.0# Deaf# 0.8# 0.6# 0.4# 0.2# 6)8#years# Deaf"(observed"d')" 50" 1.4# 0.0# Deaf"(computed"d')" 0" Distractor)Effect)(d')units)) Yucel, E., & Derim, D. (2008). The effect of implantation age on visual attention skills. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 72(6), 869-877. 250" 9)13#years# Age)Group)