Assessing the “Mismatch” Hypothesis: Differentials in College Graduation Rates by Institutional Selectivity Sigal Alon Tel Aviv University Marta Tienda Princeton University Running Header: Assessing the Mismatch Hypothesis Approximate Word Count: 6,634 We gratefully acknowledge research support from the Mellon Foundation and institutional support from the Office of Population Research at Princeton University. Please direct all correspondence to salon1@post.tau.ac.il June 2004 111 Assessing the Mismatch Hypothesis Assessing the “Mismatch” Hypothesis: Differentials in College Graduation Rates by Institutional Selectivity Abstract: This paper evaluates the “mismatch” hypothesis advocated by opponents of affirmative action, which predicts lower graduation rates for minority students who attend selective postsecondary institutions compared with those who attend colleges and universities where their academic credentials are better matched to the institutional average. Using two nationally representative longitudinal surveys (HS&B and NELS:88) and a unique survey of students enrolled at selective and highly selective institutions (C&B), we test the “mismatch” hypothesis by implementing a robust methodology that jointly considers enrollment in and graduation from selective institutions as interrelated outcomes. Our findings do not support the “mismatch” hypothesis for black and Hispanic students who attended college during the 1980s and early 1990s. Introduction Waning support for affirmative action during the 1990s challenged the research community to justify consideration of race as a legitimate factor in college admissions decisions. Critics of affirmative action allege that treating race and ethnicity as a plus factor for purposes of admission to selective and highly selective institutions sets up minorities for failure because they are putatively unprepared to succeed academically (Graglia 1993; Sowell 2003). Dubbed the “mismatch” hypothesis,1 the basic claim is that the lower average graduation rates of “affirmative admits” result from a mismatch between their academic preparation—indicated by their lower standardized college entrance test scores and high school grades—and the scholastic requirements of the schools that admitted them by taking race into account (Pell 2003). Presumably, “mismatched” minority students become demoralized, underperform, and ultimately fail to graduate (Crawford 2000; Thernstrom and Thernstrom 1999; Lerner and Nagai 2001; Pell 2003). This logic implies that a better match between the academic credentials of minority students with the average of institutions they attend will lead to stronger performance, including higher graduation rates and post-graduate activities (Arkes 1999). These claims contrast with both common knowledge and empirical research based on elementary and secondary school students, which demonstrates that, regardless of prior achievements, students who attend higher tracks and/or better schools make greater scholastic gains (Gamoran 1987; Gamoran and Berends 1987; Hallinan 1996; 2000; Entwisle et. al. 1997; Hoffer, 1992; Gamoran and Mare, 1989). Empirical studies have demonstrated the advantages of placement in higher ability groups with better instruction, 1 less distraction, more time spent on task, more academic role models, and more serious learning climates (Hallinan 2001). These findings indicate that cognitive skill development depends crucially on the opportunities for learning that schools afford (Gamoran 1987). Participation in higher academic tracks and more demanding schools may have offsetting advantages for disadvantaged students (Hallinan 2001). Black students’ postsecondary experiences further support this idea. By demonstrating that for all intervals of the SAT distribution graduation rates of black students increase as institutional selectivity rises, Bowen and Bok (1998) challenged the core of the “mismatch” hypothesis. Not only do their findings dispute allegations that black students can not succeed at selective colleges and universities, but they also demonstrate a consistent positive association between institutional selectivity and several post-graduation outcomes, including completion of advanced degrees, earnings, and overall satisfaction with college experiences (Dworkin 1998). Attending to the same question, Kane (1998) argues that affirmative action narrows rather than widens gaps in college retention rates by race because the net relationship between college selectivity and college graduation rates is positive for all students. While informative, findings based on comparisons of students attending institutions that differ in the selectivity of their admissions fall short of demonstrating a causal link between institutional selectivity and subsequent college outcomes by ignoring student allocation to selective and nonselective college destinations (Winship and Morgan, 1999). Because the determinants of attending a selective institution and graduating from college overlap to a large extent, ignoring students’ institutional 2 assignment can lead to biased inferences about the effect of college selectivity on graduation resulting from unmeasured influences that are correlated both with institutional assignment and college graduation. This is particularly important if the conditional probability of attending selective postsecondary institutions differs for minority and nonminority populations of varying academic qualifications—that is, if institutions practice affirmative action. Therefore, testing the mismatch hypothesis requires joint estimation of the selection regime allocating students to colleges and their graduation probability. Jointly modeling the graduation outcomes and the institutional assignment process not only resolves a methodological weakness of prior studies, but bears important substantive implications for the ongoing debate about affirmative action. Our evaluation of the “mismatch” hypothesis extends prior research in three important ways. First, unlike prior studies, we account for the selection bias stemming from the fact that the allocation of individuals to colleges and universities that differ in the selectivity of their admissions is endogenous to subsequent academic success. Our analytical framework—devised to assess the causal link between institutional selectivity and college graduation—stands to this challenge by combining several statistical methods that model the allocation regime governing assignment to a selective institution based both on observable and unobservable student attributes. Specifically, using propensity score analysis (Rosenbaum and Rubin, 1983) we assess the likelihood of graduation for groups with similar characteristics and propensity to attend a selective institution. Using a dummy endogenous variable model (Heckman, 1978), we simultaneously model enrollment in selective and nonselective institutions on the likelihood of graduation. 3 Second, we compare the experiences of students attending selective and highly selective institutions using the College and Beyond (C&B) database to national collegebound populations. Analyzing the C&B data allows us to extend our analysis to students attending the most competitive institutions in the country, which are the main focus of the controversy about race-sensitive admissions practices. Comparing the experiences of those students to the national college-bound populations yields a broader perspective on the selection processes that allocate students to institutions that vary in the competitiveness of their admissions, and also provides a reliability check on Bowen and Bok’s (1998) claims for blacks based on the C&B data. This is especially important because the C&B institutional selectivity spectrum is truncated, which may limit the generalizability of inferences. Finally, we compare the educational experiences of the four major ethno-racial groups. By emphasizing black-white differences, prior analysts neglect the most rapidly growing segments of the college population, namely Hispanics and Asians (see Bowen and Bok, 1998; Kane, 1998).2 We pay special attention to Hispanics, whose position in higher education has been relatively understudied compared with blacks. Although Hispanics are notorious for their educational underachievement, particularly their persistent and elevated rate of high school non completion (Fry 2003), understanding their success in higher education—both who gains admission to and who graduates from selective institutions—holds promise for the design of policy geared to improve their standing in postsecondary institutions and beyond. Multi-group comparisons based on nationally representative data not only situate Hispanics in the broader terrain of higher 4 education, but also validate findings for blacks based on a subset of highly selective institutions. In what follows, we elaborate on the testable implications of the “mismatch” hypothesis and formulate a strategy for evaluating them. After describing the three data sources used for empirical estimation, we report the statistical results. We find that conditional on admission, all students who attend selective institutions are more likely to graduate within six years of enrollment than their counterparts who attended less selective colleges. We reject the “mismatch” hypothesis for students who enrolled at the most selective institutions during the late 1980s and early 1990s. Assessing the "Mismatch Hypothesis" The controversy about affirmative action in college admissions, and the derivative “mismatch” hypothesis, is about access to the most selective postsecondary institutions in the nation. Although many factors determine the selectivity of admissions, two key indicators are singled out for ranking postsecondary institutions, namely the average SAT score of freshman classes and the percent of applicants admitted (Barron’s 2003; Bowen and Bok 1998; Greenberg 2002; U.S. News and World Report 2003). Based on the 1650 institutions listed in the 2003 Barron’s Guide, only 64 institutions, or 3.9 percent, are classified as “most competitive.” According to Greenberg (2002: 526), the nation’s twenty-five most highly selective universities offer about 50,000 slots annually. Rising demand for a relatively fixed number of slots at the most competitive institutions has certainly fueled growing disapproval of affirmative action in college admissions, but so 5 too has the growing belief that standardized scores on college entrance exams are reliable criteria for establishing admission cutoffs.3 That minority students typically average lower scores on standardized college entrance exams is used to justify claims about their unsuitability to attend selective institutions, where the average SAT score is well above the minority group average (Arkes 1999; Graglia 1993; Lerner and Nagai 2001; Pell 2003). The broad implication of these claims is that Hispanics and blacks enrolled in institutions with selective and highly selective admissions have a lower probability of graduating than their counterparts with similar characteristics who attend post secondary institutions with less selective admission criteria.4 Before developing our analytical strategy, it is important to distinguish between racial and ethnic graduation probabilities within institutions and the predictions of the “mismatch” hypothesis, which focus on same group comparisons between selective and nonselective institutions. Both sources of inequality are of policy interest, but we are concerned with the latter. Assessing the “mismatch” hypothesis requires comparing the graduation likelihood of students attending selective institutions with their same-race counterparts who attend nonselective schools. However, graduation from a selective institution is conditional on being admitted, a highly selective process influenced by many observed and unobserved factors that also influence the likelihood of timely college graduation.5 We use the Counterfactual Account of Causality conceptual and notational framework to describe the problem (for a review see Winship and Morgan, 1999). 6 Each individual is exposed to one of two destination states, i.e. nonselective vs. selective colleges, although each individual may be exposed to either state a priori. Each college destination is characterized by a set of conditions that, upon exposure, affects the likelihood of college graduation. In this framework, college destination states may be considered as treatment and control in a quasi-experiment. A key assumption of the counterfactual framework is that all students have a graduation likelihood in both states: the one in which they are observed and the one in which they are not observed. A student attending a nonselective college has an observable outcome in a nonselective college and an unobservable counterfactual outcome at a selective institution. The causal effect of the treatment (institutional selectively) on the outcome (graduation) of the ith individual is the difference between the two potential outcomes in the treatment and control states. Because only one outcome is observed for each individual, it is not possible to calculate individual-level casual effects. Considering group differences offers a partial solution. Analytically, the mismatch hypothesis has two related, but separable components: (1) group differences in the probability of graduating from institutions that differ in the selectivity of their admissions; and (2) group differences in the probability of attending a selective institution. The following two equations formally summarize the model. Pr( Υ i = 1 | Χ = x i ) = α Τ i + β ′ Χ i + ε i (1 ) Pr( Τ i* = 1 | Ζ = z i ) = δ ′ Ζ i + ν i (2) Let Y be a measure of college graduation of the ith individual; Τi* is a latent continuous variable denoting institutional selectivity of the ith individual's destination 7 college, where Ti=1 if Τi* ≥ 0 (selective college destination) and Ti=0 if Τi* <0 (nonselective college destination); α is its coefficient; Χ is a vector of observed attributes that influence college graduation; β is the vector of their coefficients; and ε is an error term that captures unobserved factors affecting graduation. The parameter α in equation (1) is the coefficient of interest for testing the “mismatch” hypothesis: the hypothesis cannot be rejected when α < 0; that is, if students attending less selective schools ( Τ = 0 ) are more likely to graduate compared with students attending more demanding institutions ( Τ = 1 ). However, comparisons between students who attend colleges that differ in selectivity are prone to bias because of observable and unobservable differences that govern the selection regime. Because our substantive interest is in same group comparisons between selective and nonselective institutions, this equation is estimated separately for each racial-ethnic group. Equation (2) represents the selection regime that determines assignment into college destinations, where Ζ is a vector of observed exogenous covariates that allocate students among postsecondary institutions; δ is a vector of associated coefficients, and ν is an error term that captures unobserved influences in the allocation regime. Selection bias is possible because T and the error term of equation (1), ε , can be correlated in two different ways (Winship and Morgan, 1999; Heckman and Hotz, 1989). First, when Ζ (eq.2) and ε are correlated, but ε and ν are uncorrelated, the selection regime is based on the observable attributes. If students allocated to selective and nonselective institutions are dissimilar in their observed characteristics, meaningful comparisons are not possible 8 (Slavin, 1990). In the extreme case of no overlap between the groups, graduation outcomes for students attending selective and nonselective institutional destinations would differ even had all attended similar type of institution. In this case, some observed set of factors in Ζ is related to the likelihood of graduation. A second source of bias arises when ε and ν , the two error terms, are correlated, which implies selection on unobservable attributes. That is, when unobserved variables influence both the institutional assignment and the graduation outcome, the errors of the prediction equations for both outcomes are likely to be correlated, and hence must be jointly estimated to obtain unbiased estimates (Pindyck and Rubinfeld 1998). Drawing causal inferences about group-specific effects of institutionsl selectivity on graduation outcomes requires addressing both types of selection bias. To assess the bias associated with "selection on the observables" ( Ζ and ε are correlated), we fit equation (2) by calculating a propensity score for each individual (Rosenbaum and Rubin 1983). Propensity scores represent the probability that individuals with observed characteristics Ζ i , are assigned to a selective rather than a nonselective institution. If the likelihood of attending a selective college is purely a function of the observables, then conditional on the Z vector, assignment is random with respect to the graduation outcome. Including the propensity score as a control variable in the graduation equation (equation 1) removes from both Yi and Ti the component of their correlation that is due to the assignment process (Winship & Morgan 1999). To assess the bias stemming from the "selection on the unobservables" ( ε and ν are correlated), we jointly estimate the likelihood of enrollment at selective institutions (equation 2) and the 9 likelihood of graduation (equation 1). Following Heckman (1978), we fit a dummy endogenous variable model to obtain unbiased and consistent estimates. Using appropriate distributional assumptions, this technique estimates error covariances (i.e. the correlation between ε and v) across equations (Pindyck and Rubinfeld, 1998; Greene, 2000). Testing the “mismatch” hypothesis once selection is accounted for requires evidence of lower graduation odds relative to statistically similar group members attending less demanding schools. That is, H 0 : Pr( Υi = 1 | Τi = 1, X = xi ) < Pr(Υi = 1 | Τi = 0, X = xi ) (3) H 1 : Pr(Υi = 1 | Τi = 1, X = xi ) ≥ Pr(Υi = 1 | Τi = 0, X = xi ) (4) The “mismatch” hypothesis (H0) predicts that Hispanic and black students (as well as whites and Asians) are less likely to graduate from selective compared with nonselective institutions, conditional on their propensity to attend a selective institution. The “mismatch” hypothesis cannot be rejected if counterpart students are more likely to graduate from less selective schools ( α < 0 in equation (1)). Alternatively, we can reject the “mismatch” hypothesis (H1) if statistically similar Hispanic and black students are equally or more likely to graduate from selective compared with nonselective institutions (when α ≥ 0). 10 Data and Empirical Estimation To estimate graduation probabilities of students attending selective and highly selective institutions, we analyze the 1989 cohort of the College and Beyond (C&B) database. However, because these data represent the experience of a relatively small share of students attending selective and highly selective four-year institutions, we also analyze two nationally representative data sets—the High School and Beyond (HS&B) and the National Educational Longitudinal Survey (NELS:88). C&B is a restricted-access database constructed by the Andrew W. Mellon Foundation between 1995 and 1997 (Bowen and Bok 1998: Appendix A). Two strengths of these data for analyzing graduation rates at selective colleges and universities are the accurate persistence data derived from college transcripts (rather than students’ selfreports) and the relatively large samples of minority students attending highly selective institutions. The core of the C&B database is an “institutional data file” consisting of undergraduate students who enrolled at one of 28 academically selective colleges and universities in the fall of 1989.6 The institutional file contains information drawn from students’ applications and transcripts, including race, sex, SAT scores, college grade point average, major field of study, and importantly, graduation status (outcome and date). Institutional records were collected for all students who enrolled in the fall of 1989 at all but three of the C&B institutions.7 Individual student records are linked to several other sources, including a survey that collected retrospective data, files provided by the College Entrance Examination Board (CEEB), and data collected by Higher Education Research Institute 11 (HERI) at the UCLA. Limiting the analysis to U.S. residents/citizens with valid racial and ethnic identities and graduation status, the final sample of 29,018 students includes 23,086 white, 2,260 black, 1,235 Hispanic, and 2,437 Asian origin students. The HS&B and NELS:88 surveys are nationally representative samples of the 1982 and 1992 high school graduation cohorts, respectively, collected by the National Center for Educational Statistics (NCES). The detailed education histories provided by these longitudinal surveys make them ideal for studying both the transition to college and the institutional selectivity of matriculants. In addition to over-samples of blacks and Hispanics, these surveys include rich information about test scores and academic high school performance, as well as standard indicators of family background. Transcript data are available for students who attended college. Our substantive interest dictates restricting both samples to students who attended a 4-year postsecondary institution with a valid institutional selectivity ranking. For the NELS survey, the analysis sample includes the 1992 high school graduation cohort respondents who were interviewed in the 2000 follow-up. Of the 4,530 eligible students, 3,326 are white, 386 black, 361 Hispanic, and 457 of Asian origin. The comparable HS&B analysis sample consists of 4,704 students who graduated from high school in 1982 and who were re-interviewed 10 years later. Eligible respondents include 3,260 whites, 644 blacks, 559 Hispanics, and 241 Asians. Descriptive analyses are weighted to adjust for over-sampling, non-response, and attrition. Moreover, multivariate analyses are adjusted to account for the clustered survey design of the data set.8 Flags for missing values are included in all models, but are not 12 reported in the results presented here.9 Appendix A provides detailed definitions and descriptive statistics of variables analyzed from each dataset. Variables: The measure of institutional selectivity for the HS&B and NELS:88 is identified as the mean combined SAT score of entering freshmen in 1982 and 1992, based on the Cooperative Institutional Research Project (CIRP) data. "Selective" institutions include those whose mean classes SAT scores were above 1050; the remainder of 4-year institutions are classified as "Nonselective." The mean combined SAT score of the 1989 entering freshmen exceeded 1050 for all C&B institutions, which qualify as selective under the CIRP classification. The C&B data defined "Highly selective" institutions as those where the average combined SAT score of the entering class was 1300 or higher (Bowen and Bok, 1989, Appendix B). The graduation equation (equation 1) includes a dummy variable for attending a selective school and a vector of covariates that are known to influence college persistence and success (Tinto, 1993); including: social class (parental education and income); ability (high school class rank, individual SAT scores) to isolate student qualifications from institutional selectivity; and sex. The enrollment equation (equation 2) and the propensity score model include covariates known to influence access to selective institutions, namely: background characteristics (parental education and income, dummy variables for public high school attendance, home geographic region, athlete status),10 and academic preparation based on high school class rank and SAT scores (Persell et al. 1992; Davies and Guppy 1997; Hearn 1984; 1990; 1991; Karen 2002; Kingston and Lewis 1990; Davies and Guppy 1997; Carnevale and Rose 2003). Race and ethnicity 13 dummy variables are included only in pooled models to capture minority students’ possible preferential admission advantage. Results Table 1 reveals the diversification of college campuses during the post-Bakke expansion of affirmative action. The relative share of white students attending selective institutions declined since the early 1980s, when the HS&B students began their postsecondary education. Asians witnessed the most substantial gains at selective institutions, nearly doubling their shares between 1982 and 1992. Despite the rapid growth of the Hispanic college-age population since 1980 and the aggressive recruitment of black and Hispanic students by admissions officers from selective colleges and universities, their shares of the entering cohort rose only slightly during the 1980s. [Table 1 about here] At the most competitive institutions included in the C&B sample, the combined representation of blacks and Hispanics reached 13 percent. Paralleling national trends, the diversification of the most elite institutions largely reflects the rising Asian presence. Cross-data comparisons reveal relatively similar shares of black students attending selective institutions, approximately 6 to 7 percent for NELS and C&B, respectively, but lower shares of Asian and Hispanic enrollees in selective institutions included in the C&B data compared with NELS. Most likely this reflects the exclusion of Texas and California public flagship institutions from the C&B database. 14 Although minority enrollment and graduation from selective postsecondary institutions increased since 1980, racial and ethnic disparities in graduation rates persist. Six-year graduation rates are higher, on average, at selective compared with nonselective institutions, which undermines allegations that lowering admission thresholds to include more minority students lowers overall graduation rates. Table 2 refutes this claim by showing that black and Hispanic student graduation rates increased between 1982 and 1992 at both selective and nonselective institutions, even as their relative shares of the student body rose. Specifically, the graduation rate of black students attending nonselective schools rose from 26 percent in 1982 to 48 percent in 1992, while black graduation rates at selective institutions rose 20 percentage points—from 52 to 72 percent over the decade. The rise in Hispanic graduation rates during this period was more modest, increasing from 26 to 40 percentage points at nonselective institutions compared with an increase of only 7 percentage points at the selective institutions.11 [Table 2 about here] Graduation rates are uniformly higher for selective compared with nonselective institutions (see “ratio” column), although the graduation gap between selective and nonselective colleges and universities narrowed over time for all groups. White students’ 1982 graduation ratio of 1.5, which indicates that students attending selective schools were about 50 percent more likely to graduate than their race counterparts attending nonselective institutions, declined modestly ten years later. For Hispanic students the graduation ratio between selective and nonselective institutions narrowed more substantially, falling from 2.4 to 1.7. That graduation rates are higher for the C&B 15 institutions than the selective schools included in the NELS and HS&B samples is unsurprising given the positive association between institutional selectivity and graduation rates overall. However, these differences also highlight a limitation of the C&B data for testing the “mismatch” hypothesis because the truncated distribution of institutional selectivity greatly restricts variation in both student attributes and graduation rates. That said, two findings are noteworthy for the “mismatch” hypothesis. First, Hispanic and black students’ graduation probabilities are higher at selective compared with nonselective institutions, contrary to the predictions of the “mismatch” hypothesis. In fact, it appears that the racial and ethnic graduation gap narrows as institutional selectivity increases. Second, the graduation gap between selective and nonselective institutions is greater for blacks and Hispanics than it is for whites in all datasets. Although these tabular results appear to challenge the “mismatch” hypothesis, this conclusion is tentative because students attending selective institutions are generally better prepared academically than their counterparts enrolled in nonselective institutions. Thus the higher minority graduation rates at selective institutions may simply reflect their higher average qualifications relative to their group average. The Mistmatch Regime To assess the degree of mismatch between students and their postsecondary institutions, Table 3 reports the deviation of each group SAT and class rank mean from the institutional tier average.12 The top panel reports the corresponding tier averages to 16 which group-specific means are compared. In both 1982 and 1992, average SAT scores for white and Asian students were slightly higher than the institutional tier average at both selective and nonselective institutions. By contrast, and in line with the claims of mismatch proponents, both black and Hispanic students enrolled in selective institutions averaged test scores well below the respective institutional tier averages—162 and 112 points lower, respectively, for black and Hispanic HS&B respondents, and 176 and 95 points lower, respectively, for NELS students. However, the deviation of the black and Hispanic mean SAT scores for the nonselective institutional average was greater still, about 180 and 115 points for blacks and Hispanic HS&B students, respectively, and 155 and 100 points for blacks and Hispanic NELS students, respectively. Racial and ethnic disparities in class rank mirror those based on average SAT scores. [Table 3 About Here] That black and Hispanic mean SAT scores lag behind institutional averages at both selective and nonselective institutions would appear to challenge the “mismatch” hypothesis. Presumably, group-specific disparities in test scores lower the odds that black and Hispanic students will graduate in six years, yet the tabular differences reveal more pronounced disparities among students attending nonselective compared with those attending selective institutions. The C&B results show similar patterns, indicating that Hispanic students are better matched to their institutions' average academic level than are blacks, but less well than either whites and Asians. Hispanic students’ mean SAT scores lag about 83 and 89 points, respectively, behind the tier averages at selective and highly selective schools, but they are better matched to highly selective institutions than to 17 nonselective institutions based on class rank. Hispanic and black students attending both selective and nonselective institutions appear to be less well prepared scholastically, which has direct implications for their persistence in the postsecondary education system and ultimate graduation. Their different starting lines may be more consequential at the most selective institutions compared with less selective schools, according to the “mismatch” hypothesis, because the academic curriculum is more demanding (Bowen and Bok 1998; Massey, et al. 2002). Before addressing the broader implications of this issue, as outline in our analytical strategy, we address this question in the most restricted fashion (see footnote 4), by testing whether the larger the gap between students’ credentials and institutionspecific SAT averages, the greater the likelihood that they will not graduate from college. For this analysis we use the C&B data to fit a probit model to assess the effect of a student's match to institution-specific averages (in terms of SAT and class rank) on the likelihood of graduation.13 Subtracting individual students’ achievements from their institutional averages yields two "match" variables, which we included (along with their product terms with group membership dummies) in the graduation equation. Estimating this model separately for selective and highly selective institutions reveals that students' SAT and class rank matches only predicts graduation at the less selective institutions, but not at the most selective schools in the country. Not only is the mismatch problem is more serious at less selective institutions, but more importantly, the student-institution mismatch has more deleterious outcomes at less selective than at more 18 selective institutions. However, this analysis, which tests the narrow interpretation of the mismatch hypothesis, ignores the selection regime into institutions of varying selectivity. Comparisons between academic preparedness of students attending selective and nonselective institutions underscores the nature of misunderstanding about the “mismatch” hypothesis. Although minority students average lower test scores and class rank than whites and Asians, those enrolled in selective colleges are better prepared than their same-race counterparts who attend nonselective institutions. The “mismatch” hypothesis is not about racial and ethnic differences in graduation within institutions, but rather about same group comparisons across institutions that differ in the selectivity of their admissions. The ultimate question is whether Hispanics and blacks enrolled in institutions with selective and highly selective admissions have a lower probability of graduating than their counterparts with similar characteristics who attend institutions with less selective admission criteria. This is the crux of the controversy about whether race-sensitive admissions legitimately by-pass criteria of “merit” in the interest of diversification. Multivariate Analysis The multivariate analysis is designed to assess the effect of institutional selectivity on six-year graduation status (1 = yes, 0 = no) of white, black, Hispanic, and Asian students for all three surveys. Because the predictions of the “mismatch” hypothesis focus on same group comparisons attending selective and nonselective institutions, estimated models are group-specific (except for one general model for the entire 19 population). The strategy is designed to account for both types of selection bias stemming from the fact that the allocation regime by institutional selectivity is endogenous to subsequent academic success. Because there is no perfect model to assess causality in nonexperimental designs, and because alternative methods may yield different results, our multi-pronged analytical strategy that builds on different assumptions about underlying relationships affords a rigorous standard for testing the mismatch hypothesis (Winship and Mare, 1992). The base model (Model A) is a simple probit that estimates equation (1) ignoring selection. To assess the bias that is associated with "selection on the observables," we add the calculated propensity score as a control variable in the graduation equation (equation 1), effectively "subtracting out" of Yi and Ti the component of their correlation due to the assignment process (Winship & Morgan 1999; Rosenbaum and Rubin 1983). Model B reports these results. Model C assesses possible bias from the "selection on the unobservables" by jointly estimating the likelihood of enrollment at selective institutions (equation 2) and the likelihood of graduation (equation 1). We estimate a bivariate probit technique that fits a dummy endogenous variable model, as suggested by Heckman (1978), to correct for correlated errors across equations (Pindyck and Rubinfeld, 1998; Greene, 2000). The estimated ρ (Rho coefficient) reveals the correlation between the error terms ε and v of equations (1) and (2), and indicates whether the joint estimation is required (i.e. if ε and v are indeed correlated) or whether two independent binary probit models are "nested" in the bivariate probit model (when ρ is not statistically different from zero). Because the 20 determinants of college enrollment and graduation overlap to a large extent, identification is achieved via distributional assumptions (Greene, 2000). For ease of interpretation, Table 4 reports marginal effects associated with the covariate of theoretical interest, namely institutional selectivity [the α in equation (1)].14 Appendix Table B reports the estimated probit coefficients. Results for the entire HS&B cohort indicate that, regardless of the statistical method employed, there is a positive and substantial effect of institutional selectivity on 6-year college graduation status, but particularly once the endogeneity of the institutional allocation regime is modeled to account for both observed and unobserved individual differences. Nonetheless, the group-specific models disclose race and ethnic variation in the benefits associated with attending a selective institution in 1982. For white students the significant ρ coefficient indicates selection on both unobservables and observables, which makes the simultaneous assessment critical for this group. Specifically, after controlling for the selection on the unobservables (model C), the effect of attending a selective school increases white students’ graduation probability by approximately 40 percent. Both the base model that ignores the selection regime altogether and the model that accounts for selection on the observables with propensity scores greatly underestimate the benefits of attending a selective institution for white students. [Table 4 About Here] The ρ coefficient is not statistically significant for minority groups, suggesting either absence of selection bias in the allocation of students across institutions, or bias derived only from observable attributes (thus, obviating the need for joint estimation). 21 Under model B's assumptions (selection bias on the observables), attending a selective school increases Black, Hispanic and Asian students’ graduation probability by 14, 17 and 30 percent, respectively. Our formal test requires that α ≥ 0 to reject the mismatch hypothesis, hence these results are sufficient for the cohort of 1982. Point estimates for the 1992 entering class (NELS:88 data) reveal the same positive relationship between institutional selectivity and college graduation, although the marginal effects are smaller compared with the 1982 cohort. The statistically insignificant ρ coefficient (model C) indicates the absence of selection bias due to unobservables attributes. Therefore, we focus on Model B, which after correcting for propensity scores, reveals a positive causal impact of attending a selective institution on college graduation on the order of 11 percent for the entire 1992 cohort. The group-specific models reveal that this effect is significant for whites and Asians, but not black and Hispanic students. For them, the marginal effect is positive and of approximately similar order of magnitude as the estimates for whites and Asians, but the point estimate not statistically significant. None of the methods produced a statistically significant positive effect for black students, while for Hispanics the point estimates based on models A and B approach the conventional threshold for statistical reliability. This accumulated evidence allows us to safely reject the "mismatch" hypothesis for whites and Asians, but not for blacks and Hispanics. However, even for the latter groups the results do not suggest that there is a merit in the "mismatch" hypothesis, as all the α ≥ 0. To further test the “mismatch” hypothesis, we direct attention to highly selective colleges. Not only are these institutions the focus of controversy about race-sensitive 22 admission policies, but elite institutions also are wealthier and have other resources to support all students they admit, thereby increasing their odds of graduation. In this respect, an examination of the difference between C&B students who attend elite schools and those attending less selective college and universities is particularly instructive. On average, we find a positive causal impact of institutional selectivity on the likelihood of graduation, regardless of the estimation strategy used. The results for the entire cohort do not indicate that selection derives from the unobservables, thus Model B, which corrects for selection on the observables is sufficient. Group-specific models reveal that these results are dominated by white students' experiences. The story for minority groups is somewhat different. Neither model A or B produces a statistically significant effect for institutional selectivity, and the fact the ρ (model C) is significant ( ρ ≤ .05 ) for Hispanics and Asians only ( ρ ≤ .10 for blacks) hints that there is selection bias on the unobservables for minority groups. By accounting for unobserved selection bias, the increased odds of graduation from attending an elite institution school is discernible—on the order of 13, 22 and 10 percent for Blacks, Hispanics and Asians, respectively. Thus, the C&B findings also refute the “mismatch” hypothesis for blacks, as Bowen and Bok (1998) claimed, and our results allow us to generalize this finding to Hispanics and Asians as well. Discussion Our findings from three different datasets provide a robust assessment of the causal effect on graduation associated with attending selective institutions, conditional on 23 admission and enrollment. The results clearly demonstrate that attending a selective or highly selective college increases the likelihood of timely graduation, net of initial differences. The results lend no support for the "mismatch" hypothesis for minority students who attend selective and highly selective institutions. Nevertheless, differences in results obtained for the 1982 and 1992 cohorts are noteworthy because they appear to imply that benefits of attending a selective institution waned during the 1980s. Although we restricted our analysis to students who enrolled in four-year institutions, most of the growth in 4-year postsecondary institutions involves those with less selective admission criteria. Although we cannot ascertain whether these results capture a true trend in this decade or just sampling variability due to the different stratification schemes used for these surveys, the robust C&B results for the 1989 cohort corroborate the latter interpretation. We caution against comparing across groups because by construction, groupspecific models only permit within group comparisons.15 However, among the C&B students, the graduation benefit associated with attending elite schools is larger among Hispanic and black students than among their white and Asian counterparts. The obverse obtains for the national datasets, but the selectivity tiers are far more heterogeneous. This is especially evident for the 1982 cohort, where whites enjoy large graduation advantages from attending selective institutions. Taken together with the weak results for minorities based on the national sample, our findings suggest that the more selective an institution’s admission criteria, the larger the graduation advantages minority students can reap from attending. This finding is not only reassuring, as it supports the conclusions of prior 24 research that ignored selection regimes, but also supports the arguments about the continued value of race-sensitive admissions that were upheld in the recent Supreme Court decision.16 Reflecting on Bowen and Bok’s (1998, p.89) appeal, it appears that selective institutions that attract talented minority students must strive with equal fervor to do whatever they can to ensure that all those admitted realize their full potential. Conclusion In this analysis we sought to test claims that considering Hispanic origin and race in admissions decisions predestined “affirmative admits” for inevitable failure. Our results confirmed claims that minority students thrive in selective schools despite their disadvantaged starting lines (Bowen and Bok 1998; Massey et al 2003). Specifically, the likelihood of graduation increases as the selectivity of the institution attended rises. These findings support the notion that students learn more and are more likely to succeed when they have greater opportunities for learning (Gamoran 1987). In short, institutional selectivity appears to reflect better learning opportunities via better prepared classmates or better teachers (Kane 1998). It also could represent the large institutional endowments and resources that allow for small class sizes and facilitate strong mentoring at the most competitive schools.17 Support mechanisms are especially important for students from socioeconomically and academically disadvantaged backgrounds, particularly first generation college goers, among whom black, but especially Hispanic students are disproportionately represented (Tinto, 1993). Evidence about interaction with faculty 25 members unequivocally suggests that more contact with professors and others on campus is conducive for increasing graduation rates (Pascaralla and Terenzini (1978; 1980); Nettles, 1991; Nettles et. al 1986; Davis, 1991; Von Destinon, 1988; NCES, 1996). Alon (2003) directly links financial aid to college graduation by showing that financial aid, grants in particular, helps equalize black and Hispanic minority students’ college success with that of their white and Asian counterparts. Nevertheless, our results do not speak to the persistent racial and ethnic graduation gap within selectivity tiers. Given the immense efforts and ample resources devoted to attracting and recruiting under-represented minorities to the most selective colleges and universities, evidence that nontrivial shares of blacks and Hispanic students leave these institutions without a college diploma is disconcerting. Even more disturbing are race and ethnic differences in graduation rates among students of comparable academic ability and socioeconomic background (Bowen and Bok 1998; Small and Winship 2002; Vars and Bowen 1998). Racial and ethnic gaps in college graduation rates are of major concern not only because education serves as a gateway to personal financial success and social standing, but also because of the shadow that graduation disparities casts on race-sensitive admission practices. For these reasons, research must continue to explore reasons for minority students' underperformance in both selective and nonselective institutions. Striving to increase college access while narrowing graduation gaps is all the more urgent in light of the changing demographic contours and the dire need to curtail the ethno-racial divide in life chances. In that respect, our results suggest that applying race-sensitive 26 admission criteria aid in achieving this societal goal. Our findings imply that investing institutional resources toward goals that promote college success help minority youth realize their potential. 27 Notes 1 Bowen & Bok use the term “fit” to portray this hypothesis, but we prefer the term “mismatch” because it succinctly captures the essence of the debate, namely that minority students with lower credentials are “mismatched” at selective institutions and thus have worse outcomes. 2 Bowen and Bok were aware of this limitation and were directly responsible for ensuring that the collegegoing behavior of Hispanics was studied with the C&B data. This research derives from that effort. 3 This is manifested by the small industry that developed over the past two decades to prepare students, mainly from middle and upper middle classes, to improve their SAT scores (McDonough, 1994 ). 4 A narrower interpretation of the mismatch hypothesis is that the larger the gap between students’ credentials and institution-specific SAT averages, the greater the likelihood that they will not graduate from college. This narrowly crafted test of the mismatch hypothesis is incomplete because of several problems. First is the complexity of students' profiles: a student may score below the institutional SAT mean but above the institutional class rank mean or she may have another appealing attribute like athletic prowess or mastery of an instrument. Moreover, it is not clear how large a gap in credentials should be to indicate a mismatch. In institutions with diverse student bodies, almost all students are "mismatched" one way or another. However, the most severe drawback to this narrow conception of the mismatch hypothesis is that it ignores the selection regime into institutions of varying selectivity. Although we assess the narrow test, our analytical strategy is designed to deal with the broader implications of the mismatch hypothesis. 5 This is one transition in a multi-stage selection process that includes high school graduation and the decision to continue education after high school. It is necessary to control for prior selection stages so as not to confound current selection with determinants of prior selections (Camron and Heckman, 1998). Unfortunately, like most analysts who do not implement full structural models, we are unable to control for selection prior to high school graduation. 6 Six institutions were excluded from the analysis: four historically black colleges and universities and two universities that did not provide the detailed information needed to measure the timing of graduation. 7 For most institutions, the C&B data files included the entire entering cohorts. However for some institutions the data are derived from samples (Bowen and Bok, 1998). In these cases, sample weights are equal to the inverse of the probability of being sampled. All descriptive statistics presented use appropriate sample weights so that the results accurately represent the entire entering cohort at each institution. These weights allow projections to all C&B institutions (but not to the entire postsecondary universe). 8 This correction affects the estimated standard errors and the variance-covariance matrix of the estimators, but not the estimated coefficients. 9 Using this strategy, a modified zero-order method, we fill all missing data with zeros and add a dummy variable that takes the value one for missing observations and zero for complete ones. These flags provide a useful method for testing whether the pattern of missing observations is random with respect to Y. The modified zero-order strategy is the simplest solution when the proportion of missing data is small (Anderson, Basilevsky and Hum, 1983). However, as with most remedies for missing data, it does not completely eliminate its potential biasing effects. 10 The NELS data does not include a measure for athlete status. 11 That Asians’ graduation probability is lower in 1992 than in 1982 may reflect the greater heterogeneity of the Asian sample in 1992 compared with 1982. The earlier sample includes a higher share of South and East Asian youth, who are known to have very high rates of academic success. In addition, their high graduation rate in 1982 may be inflated due to the small sample size, which increases the error of the estimate. 12 For the C&B we report the percent in the top decile of their class because of the restricted variation on this item in this data set. 13 This analysis cannot be performed using the nationally representative datasets because of small number of students attending each institution. 14 For a dummy variable the marginal effect represents the change in the probability associated with a discrete change in the variable from 0 to 1, holding other variables at their mean (Long, 1997). 28 15 The marginal effects presented in Table 4 summarize the relationship between selective (versus unselective) college attendance and graduation for each group. 16 Grutter v. Bollinger, 123 S. Ct. 2325 (2003); Gratz v. Bollinger, 123 S. Ct. 2411 (2003). 17 Based on U.S. News and World Report, the highest ranking institutions have the lowest student/faculty ratio For example, at the top ranking schools the student/Faculty ratio is 6/1, 8/1, and 7/1 at Princeton, Harvard, and Yale, respectively. At the five selective schools tied for the 47th ranking, the ratio is 12/1, 17/1, 17/1, 19/1, and 11/1 for Pepperdine University, Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute, University of California at Santa Barbara, University of Texas at Austin, and University of Washington, respectively. These factors contribute to the ranking formula (U.S. News and World Report, 2003). 29 References Alon, Sigal. 2003. "Financial Support and Ethnic Disparities in College Graduation." Unpublished manuscript. Arkes, Hal R. 1999. “Review: The Shape of the River: Long-Term Consequences of Considering Race in College and University Admissions.” Liberty, 83, 51-56. Online: http://www.libertysoft.com/liberty/reviews/71arkes.html (Accessed 30 July 2003). Barron’s, 2003. Profiles of American Colleges, 25th Edition. Hauppauge, New York: Bowen, William G. and Derek Bok. 1998. The Shape of the River: Long-Term Consequences of Considering Race in College and University Admissions. Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press. Cameron, Stephen V. and James J. Heckman. 1998. "Life Cycle Schooling and Dynamic Selection Bias: Models and Evidence for Five Cohorts of American Males." The Journal of Political Economy, 106(2):262-333. Carnevale, Anthony P. and Stephen J. Rose. 2003. “Socioeconomic Status, Race/Ethnicity, and Selective College Admissions.” A Century Foundation Paper. Crawford, Curtis. 2000. “Weighing the Benefits and Costs of Racial Preference in College Admissions review of: The shape of the River: Long-Term Consequences of Considering race in College and University Admissions.” Society, 37 (4), 71-80. Online: http://www.ceousa.org/crawford.html (Accessed 1 August 2003). Davies, Scott and Neil Guppy. 1997. “Fields of Study, College Selectivity, and Student Inequalities.” Social Forces, 73 (4), 131-151. Davis, R. B. 1991. Social support networks and undergraduate students' academicsuccess-related outcomes: A comparison of black students on black and white campuses. In R. A. Walter, E. G. Epps, & N. Z. Haniff (Eds.). College in black and white: African American students in predominantly white and in historically black public universities (pp. 143-157). New York: State University of New York Press. Dworkin, Ronald. 1998 (October 22). “Affirming Affirmative Action Review.” The New York Review of Books. Entwisle, Doris R., Karl L. Alexander, and Linda S. Olson. 1997. Children, Schools and Inequality. Westview Press Fry, Richard. 2003. “Hispanic Youth Dropping Out of U.S. Schools: Measuring the Challenge.” Research Report. Washington, D.C.: Pew Hispanic Center. Online: http://www.pewhispanic.org/site/docs/pdf/high%20school%20dropout%20rep ort--final.pdf (Accessed 1 August 2003). Gamoran, Adam. 1987. "The Stratification of High School Learning Opportunities." Sociology of Education, 60:135-155. Gamoran, Adam and Mark Berends. 1987. "The Effect of Stratification in Secondary Schools: Synthesis of Survey and Ethnographic Research." Review of Educational Research, 57:415-435. Gamoran, Adam and Robert Mare. 1989. "Secondary School Tracking and Educational Inequality: Compensation, Reinforcement, or Neutrality?" American Journal of Sociology, 94:1146-83 Graglia, Lino A. 1993. “Racial Preferences in Admission to Institutions of Higher Education.” In Howard Dickman, ed., The Imperiled Academy. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers. Greenberg, Jack. 2002. “Affirmative Action in Higher Education: Confronting the Condition and Theory.” Boston College Law Review, 43, 521-621. Greene, William H. 2000. Econometric Analysis, 4th ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Hallinan, Maureen T. 1996. "Track Mobility in Secondary School." Social Forces, 74 (3):983-1002. Hallinan, Maureen T. 2001. “Sociological Perspectives on Black-White Inequalities in American Schooling.” Sociology of Education, Vol.74, Extra Issue: Current of Thought: Sociology of Education at the Dawn of the 21st Century.” pp. 50-70. Hearn, James C. 1984. “The Relative Roles of Academic, Ascribed, and Socioeconomic Characteristics in College Destination.” Sociology of Education, 57, 22-30. Hearn, James C. 1990. “Pathways to Attendance at the Elite Colleges.” In The High Status Track: Studies of Elite Schools and Stratification, edited by P.W. Kingston and L.S. Lewis. Albany: State University of New York Press. Hearn, James C. 1991. “Academic and Non-Academic Influence on the College Destinations of 1980 High School Graduates.” Sociology of Education, 64, 158-171. Heckman James J. 1978. "Dummy Endogenous Variables in a Simultaneous Equation System." Econometrica, Vol. 46(4):931-959. Heckman, J. J., Hotz, V.J. 1989. “Choosing Among Alternative Nonexperimental Methods for Estimating the Impact of Social Programs: The Case of Manpower Training.” Journal of the American Statistical Association, 84:86274 Hoffer, Thomas. 1992. "Middle School Ability Grouping and Student Achievement in Science and Mathematics." Education Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 14:205-227. Kane, Thomas J. 1998. “Misconceptions in the Debate Over Affirmative Action in College Admissions.” In Gary Orfield and Edward Miller, (ed), Chilling Admissions: The Affirmative Crisis and the Search for Alternatives. Cambridge: Harvard Education Publishing Group. 17-32 Kingston, Paul. W. and Lionel S. Lewis. 1990. Pp. 105-20 in Undergraduates at Elite Institutions: The Best, the Brightest, and the Richest. The High-Status Track. Edited by P.W. Kingston and L.S. Lewis. Albany: University of New York Press. Lerner, Robert and Althea K. Nagai. 2001. "Pervasive Preferences: Racial and Ethnic Discrimination in Undergraduate Admissions across the Nation." Center for Equal Opportunity. http://www.ceousa.org/multi.html (Accessed September 19, 2003). Long, J. S. 1997. Regression Models for Categorical and Limited Dependent Variables. Thousand Oaks,CA:Sage Publications,Inc. Manski, Charles F. and David A. Wise. 1983. College Choice in America. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Massey, Douglas S., Camille Z. Charles, Garvey Lundy and Mary J. Fischer. 2003. Source of the River: The Social Origin of Freshmen at America’s Selective Colleges and Universities. Princeton NJ: Princeton University Press. McDonough, Patricia M. 1994. “Buying and Selling Higher Education: The Social Construction of the College Applicant.” The Journal of Higher Education, 65 (4): 427-446. National Center for Education Statistics. 1996. The Condition of Education. Table 10-1. http://www.nces.ed.gov/pubsold/ce96/c9610d01.html Nettles, M. T. 1991. Racial similarities and differences in the predictors of college student achievement. In R. A. Walter, E. G. Epps, & N. Z. Haniff (Eds.). College in black and white: African American students in predominantly white and in historically black public universities (pp. 75-91). New York: State University of New York Press. Nettles, M. T., Thoeny, A.R., & Gosman, E. J.1986. Comparative and predictive analyses of black and white students’ college achievement and experiences. Journal of Higher Education, 57(3), 289-318. Pascarella, E.T., & Terenzini, P. T. 1980. Predicting freshman persistence and voluntary dropout decisions from a theoretical model. Journal of Higher Education, 51(1), 60-75. Pascarella, E.T., Terenzini, P. T. & Hibel, J. 1978. Student-faculty interactional settings and their relationship to predicted academic performance. Journal of Higher Education, 49(5), 450-463. Pell, Terrance J. 2003. “Racial Preferences and Formal Equality.” Journal of Social Philosophy, 342, 309-325. Persell, Caroline Hodges, Sophia Catsambis, and Peter W. Cookson, Jr. 1992. “Differential Asset Conversion: Class and Gendered Pathways to Selective Colleges.” Sociology of Education 65:208-25. Pindyck, Robert S. and Daniel L. Rubinfeld. 1998. Econometric Models and Economic Forecasts (4th ed.). Boston: Irwin/McGraw-Hill. Rosenbaum, Paul R. and Donald B. Rubin. 1983. "The Central Role of the Propensity Score in Observational Studies for Casual Effect." Biometrika, 70(1):41-55. Small, Mario Luis and Christopher Winship. 2002. “Better Environments or Better Students? Why Black Graduation Rates Vary Across Elite Colleges and Universities.” Working Paper presented at Mellon Foundation Diversity in Higher Education Conference. Sowell, Thomas. 2003 (February 8). “Damaging Admissions: Increasing Faculty Diversity,” Capitalism Magazine. Online: http://www.capmag.com/article.asp?ID=2448 (Accessed 31 July 2003). Slavin, Robert E. 1990. "Achievement Effects of Ability Grouping in Secondary Schools: A Best Evidence Synthesis," Review of Educational Research 60(3), 471-499. Thernstrom, Stephan, & Thernstrom, Abigail. 1999. “Reflections on The Shape of the River Book Review.” UCLA Law Review, 46, 1583-1631. Tinto, Vincent. 1993. Leaving College: Rethinking the Causes and Cures of Student Attrition. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Second Edition. U.S News and World Report. 2003. Best Colleges. http://www.usnews.com/usnews/edu/college/rankings/rankindex.htm (Accessed: 24 June 2003) Vars, Fredrick E. and William G. Bowen. 1998. “Scholastic Aptitude Test Scores, Race, and Academic Performance in Selective Colleges and Universities.” In Christopher Jencks and Meredith Phillips (eds.), The Black-White Test Score Gap. Washington, D.C.: Brookings Institution Press. 457-479. Von Destinon, M. (1988). Chicano student persistence: The effects of integration and involvement. Tucson: University of Arizona Press. Winship, Christopher and Robert D. Mare. 1992. “Models for Sample Selection Bias.” Annual Review of Sociology, Vol.18. pp. 327-350. Winship, Christopher and Stephen L. Morgan. 1999. "The Estimation of Causal Effects from Observational Data." Annual Review of Sociology, 25:659-706. Table 1 Racial and Ethnic Composition of Post-secondary Institutions by Admission Selectivity Tier and Entry Cohort HS & B, 1982 Tier: White Black Hispanic Asian % Mean SAT N Non-selective C & B, 1989 NELS, 1992 Selective Non-selective Selective Selective Highly Selective 82.5 11.4 4.4 1.7 85.4 5.3 4.4 4.9 79.4 10.3 6.2 4.1 78.2 6.2 6.0 9.6 82.7 6.9 3.3 7.1 75.4 7.2 5.5 11.8 83.2 918 (171) 3,847 16.8 1082 (174) 857 72.2 911 (167) 3,172 27.9 1083 (162) 1,358 79.2 1185 (149) 22,253 20.8 1333 (122) 6,765 Table 2 Graduation Probabilities by Race, Institutional Selectivity Tier and Entry Cohort HS & B, 1982 Tier: NELS, 1992 C & B, 1989 Non-selective (1) Selective (2) Ratio (2)/(1) Non-selective (1) Selective (2) Ratio (2)/(1) Selective (1) Highly selective (2) Ratio (2)/(1) White Black Hispanic Asian 53.4 26.4 25.7 50.9 82.0 51.6 62.1 90.9 1.5 2.0 2.4 1.8 62.5 47.6 40.5 53.6 81.2 71.7 69.0 82.9 1.3 1.5 1.7 1.5 86.5 73.4 81.0 89.2 94.8 85.1 90.6 95.9 1.1 1.2 1.1 1.1 Average 49.0 80.0 1.6 59.3 80.0 1.3 85.0 94.0 1.1 3,847 857 3,172 1,358 22,253 6,765 N Table 3 The Mismatch Regime: Deviations from Mean Institutional Scholastic Achievement and Class Rank by Selectivity Tier, Entry Cohort and Race HS & B, 1982 Tier: Nonselective NELS, 1992 Selective Nonselective C & B, 1989 Selective Selective Highlyselective 1333 (122) Tier Means: Tier SAT mean (s.d.) 930.7 (182.2) 1112.6 (179.3) 912.7 (176.7) 1090.2 (172.0) 1185 (149) Tier class rank mean (s.d.) 64.6 (25.47) 78.5 (19.81) 67.4 (23.09) 81.4 (18.32) 63.7 21.8 10.0 21.8 13.5 13.2 1.5 2,680 0.1 580 1.0 2,387 -0.4 939 2.3 17,983 -180.0 -162.3 -155.1 -176.0 -170.7 -9.5 549 -7.0 95 -6.9 298 -3.2 88 -25.3 1,770 -115.2 -112.0 -100.1 -95.1 -83.3 -10.6 467 -3.1 92 -0.1 278 1.5 83 -11.9 861 Asian SAT mean Deviation 7.4 37.0 -19.3 32.6 49.7 Class rank mean Deviation N 9.1 151 9.3 90 -5.7 209 4.5 248 2.8 1,639 a 83.8 a Deviations from Tier Means: White SAT mean Class rank mean N Black SAT mean Deviation Class rank mean Deviation N Hispanic SAT mean Deviation Class rank mean Deviation N a For C&B data, classrank represents percent ranked in top decile. b The number reported is the deviation from the corresponding tier average percentage in HS top ten percent. 13.3 b 1.0 5,103 b -154.7 b -18.9 490 b -89.0 b -3.1 374 b 51.1 b 7.1 798 b Table 4 Group-Specific Marginal Effects (Discrete Change)a of Attending a Selective Institution on 6-Year Graduation Status HS & B, 1982 NELS, 1992 Model Ab Model Bc Model Cd Probit Probit with Control for Propensity Score Bivariate Probit Model A Probit C & B, 1989 Model B Model C Probit with Control for Propensity Score Bivariate Probit Model A Probit Model B Model C Probit with Control for Propensity Score Bivariate Probit All Students 0.20 ** 0.18 ** 0.37 ** ρ = -0.37 ** 0.11 ** 0.11 ** 0.04 ρ = 0.12 0.06 ** 0.05 ** 0.11 ** ρ = -0.25 Group-Specific Models White 0.19 ** 0.17 ** 0.40 ** ρ = -0.52 ** 0.10 ** 0.10 ** 0.00 ρ = 0.17 0.06 ** 0.05 ** 0.10 * ρ = -0.19 Black 0.12 * 0.14 * -0.03 ρ = 0.23 0.11 0.11 -0.07 ρ= 0.27 0.05 † 0.03 0.13 * ρ = -0.24 † Hispanic 0.17 ** 0.17 ** 0.14 ρ = 0.04 0.13 † 0.14 † 0.12 ρ= 0.03 0.07 † 0.05 0.22 * ρ = -0.56 * Asian 0.31 ** 0.30 ** 0.37 ρ = -0.15 0.13 ** 0.13 ** 0.10 ρ= 0.06 0.03 0.01 † 0.10 * ρ = -0.43 ** Note: Appendix B presents complete results * P ≤ .05; ** P ≤ .01; † P ≤ .10 (two-tailed: null hypothesis b = 0) a) For a dummy variable the marginal effect represents the change in the probability associated with a discrete change in the variable from 0 to 1, holding other variables at their mean (Long, 1997). b) Model A does not control for nonrandom assignment into selective institutions (the "treatment"). c) Model B controls for students' propensity to attend a selective institution by controling for "propensity scores." The specification of the model predicting propensity scores is identical to that estimated in the bivariate probit assignment equation (i.e. attending a selective institution). d) Model C is a group-specific bivariate probit (dummy endogenous variable model) of enrollment and 6-year graduation status. Table Appendix A: Descriptive Statistics of HS&B, NELS:88 and C&B Samples Data: College Entry Cohort: Variable Definition Grad6 White Black Hisp Mexican Puerto Rican Cuban Asian 6-year graduation proportion White, not of Hispanic origin Black, not of Hispanic origin Hispanic, regardless of race Hispanic of Mexican origin Hispanic of Puerto Rican origin Hispanic of Cuban origin Asian or Pacific Islander 0.54 0.83 0.10 0.04 Selective Tresholds for selective Parents, B.A. Parental income Class rank SAT Athlete Female Public South Midwest West a (Std. Dev.) NELS 1992 Mean (Std. Dev.) C&B 1989 Mean (Std. Dev.) 0.02 0.65 0.79 0.09 0.06 0.47 0.11 0.42 0.06 0.87 0.81 0.07 0.04 N/A N/A N/A 0.08 If attend a selective institution HS&B and NELS:88: above 1050 C&B: Selective :above 1050; Highly Selective: above 1250 0.17 0.28 0.21 At least one parent with a BA degree In categories (HS&B:6; NELS:15; C&B:17 ) HS class rank (C&B: in Top 10%) SAT score If student was an athlete Female=1, Male=0 If attend a public HS If home region in South If home region in Midwest If home region in West 0.19 0.10 50.01 960.47 0.12 0.53 0.83 0.29 0.30 0.14 0.22 0.10 55.19 (35.79) 963.25 (192.81) N/A 0.55 0.75 0.31 0.27 0.15 0.09 0.19 0.55 1217.03 (156.07) 0.12 0.51 0.44 0.26 0.23 0.08 4,530 29,018 Observations a HS & B 1982 Mean C & B: if attend a highly selective institution 0.45 0.13 0.42 4,704 (36.20) (193.83) Appendix Table B1: Estimates of 6-Year College Graduation: HS&B 1982 Entry Cohort All students White Black Hispanic Asian Model C Model C Model C Model C Model C Model A Model B Model A Model B Model A Model B Model A Model B Model A Model B Bivariate Probit Probit Bivariate Probit Probit Bivariate Probit Probit Bivariate Probit Probit Bivariate Probit Probit Control Control Control Control Control Propensity Propensity Propensity Propensity Propensity Score (1) (2) Score (1) (2) Score (1) (2) Score (1) (2) Score (1) (2) Grad6 Selective Grad6 Selective Grad6 Selective Grad6 Selective Grad6 Selective Selective institution Est. propensity score Parents, B.A. Parental income Class rank SAT Black Hispanic Asian 0.524** 0.489** 1.121** (0.060) (0.062) (0.190) 0.552* (0.242) 0.279** 0.248** 0.234** (0.046) (0.048) (0.048) 0.046** 0.041** 0.039** (0.012) (0.012) (0.012) 0.013** 0.012** 0.012** (0.001) (0.001) (0.001) 0.001** 0.001** 0.001** (0.000) (0.000) (0.000) -0.235** -0.281** -0.284** (0.062) (0.065) (0.063) -0.234** -0.276** -0.278** (0.074) (0.077) (0.075) -0.017 -0.107 -0.124 (0.087) (0.097) (0.090) 0.252** (0.055) 0.043** (0.016) 0.008** (0.002) 0.002** (0.000) 0.419** (0.084) 0.413** (0.089) 0.679** (0.113) 0.557** 0.496** 1.354** (0.078) (0.079) (0.192) 0.858** (0.283) 0.291** 0.248** 0.228** (0.054) (0.056) (0.057) 0.030+ 0.017 0.014 (0.015) (0.016) (0.016) 0.014** 0.014** 0.013** (0.001) (0.001) (0.001) 0.001** 0.000 0.000 (0.000) (0.000) (0.000) 0.307* (0.153) 0.358* (0.163) -0.537 (0.532) 0.249** 0.296* 0.338** (0.064) (0.122) (0.127) 0.082** 0.061* 0.057+ (0.021) (0.030) (0.031) 0.007** 0.011** 0.012** (0.002) (0.003) (0.003) 0.002** 0.001** 0.002** (0.000) (0.000) (0.000) -0.071 (0.465) 0.423* (0.167) 0.429* (0.169) -0.088 (0.653) 0.328** 0.287+ 0.212 0.211 (0.125) (0.161) (0.154) (0.156) 0.057+ -0.053 0.078* 0.078* (0.031) (0.040) (0.036) (0.036) 0.012** 0.010* 0.008** 0.008* (0.003) (0.004) (0.003) (0.003) 0.002** 0.003** 0.002** 0.002** (0.000) (0.000) (0.000) (0.001) Mexican -0.090 (0.145) -0.238 (0.212) 0.387** (0.132) Puerto Rican Female Athlete Public South Midwest West Pseudo R2a Log pseudo-likelihood 0.103* (0.045) 0.106* (0.045) 0.105* (0.043) 0.033 (0.053) 0.235** (0.069) -0.226** (0.079) -0.931** (0.095) -0.573** (0.084) -0.374** (0.113) 0.036 (0.052) 0.037 (0.049) 0.110 (0.119) 0.217** (0.083) -0.236** (0.091) -1.051** (0.118) -0.515** (0.089) -0.412** (0.152) 0.096 (0.119) 0.099 (0.118) 0.250 (0.190) -0.269+ (0.154) -1.080** (0.175) -0.697** (0.195) -0.155 (0.200) -0.090 (0.144) -0.231 (0.214) 0.388** (0.131) 0.360 (0.870) 1.022** 1.004** 1.258 (0.261) (0.272) (0.848) 0.343 (0.775) 0.211 -0.028 0.049 0.007 0.018 (0.156) (0.199) (0.218) (0.231) (0.231) 0.078* 0.021 0.133* 0.139* 0.137* (0.036) (0.042) (0.055) (0.056) (0.054) 0.008* 0.009* 0.014** 0.014* 0.014* (0.004) (0.004) (0.005) (0.006) (0.006) 0.002** 0.002** 0.002** 0.002+ 0.002 (0.001) (0.001) (0.001) (0.001) (0.001) -0.090 (0.145) -0.233 (0.211) 0.388** (0.131) 0.380+ (0.220) -0.035 (0.064) 0.013+ (0.007) 0.004** (0.001) 0.270 (0.183) -0.047 (0.250) 0.329 (0.207) 0.426 (0.333) -0.042 (0.195) -0.974** (0.250) -1.306** (0.353) -0.675* (0.264) 0.329 (0.207) 0.328 (0.206) 0.178 (0.420) -0.234 (0.278) -0.902* (0.413) -1.165** (0.306) -0.729** (0.267) 0.15 0.15 0.13 0.13 0.13 0.13 0.15 0.15 0.31 0.31 -2734.08 -2730.93 -4519.37 -1901.66 -1896.23 -3103.15 -378.61 -378.07 -589.47 -322.79 -322.78 -531.32 -107.98 -107.91 -228.34 ρ -0.37 ** -0.52 ** 0.23 0.04 -0.15 Constant -2.040** -1.748** -1.640** -3.791** -1.831** -1.362** -1.227** -3.950** -2.317** -2.527** -2.463** -3.273** -2.776** -2.811** -2.801** -3.256** -3.754** -3.467** -3.528** -4.201** Observations 4704 4704 4704 4704 3260 3260 3260 3260 644 644 644 644 559 559 559 559 241 241 241 241 Robust standard errors in parentheses + significant at 10%; * significant at 5%; ** significant at 1% a) The Pseudo R2 is the McFadden's R2, also known as the "likelihood rario index",compares the model with just the intercept to a model with all parameters (Long, 1997). Appendix Table B2: Estimates of 6-Year College Graduation: NELS 1992 Entry Cohort All students White Black Hispanic Asian Model C Model C Model C Model C Model C Model A Model B Model A Model B Model A Model B Model A Model B Model A Model B Bivariate Probit Probit Bivariate Probit Probit Bivariate Probit Probit Bivariate Probit Probit Bivariate Probit Probit Control Control Control Control Control Propensity Propensity Propensity Propensity Propensity Score (1) (2) Score (1) (2) Score (1) (2) Score (1) (2) Score (1) (2) Grad6 Selective Grad6 Selective Grad6 Selective Grad6 Selective Grad6 Selective Selective institution 0.314** 0.324** 0.119 (0.055) (0.056) (0.374) Est. propensity score -0.346 (0.329) Parents, B.A. 0.266** 0.289** 0.278** (0.048) (0.053) (0.052) Parental income 0.063** 0.068** 0.066** (0.011) (0.012) (0.012) Class rank 0.010** 0.011** 0.011** (0.001) (0.001) (0.001) SAT 0.001** 0.001** 0.001** (0.000) (0.000) (0.000) Black -0.020 0.029 0.008 (0.078) (0.088) (0.092) Hispanic -0.307** -0.273** -0.286** (0.079) (0.085) (0.091) Asian 0.084 0.153 0.124 (0.080) (0.107) (0.110) Mexican 0.210** (0.052) 0.065** (0.012) 0.006** (0.002) 0.003** (0.000) 0.587** (0.087) 0.322** (0.092) 0.608** (0.083) 0.290** 0.298** -0.001 (0.065) (0.066) (0.533) -0.248 (0.388) 0.273** 0.287** 0.288** (0.057) (0.062) (0.063) 0.080** 0.085** 0.086** (0.013) (0.015) (0.016) 0.011** 0.011** 0.011** (0.001) (0.001) (0.001) 0.001** 0.001** 0.001** (0.000) (0.000) (0.000) 0.278 (0.183) 0.279 (0.185) -0.028 (0.927) 0.196** 0.497** 0.498** (0.064) (0.181) (0.180) 0.096** 0.048 0.048 (0.016) (0.031) (0.034) 0.004* 0.009* 0.009* (0.002) (0.004) (0.004) 0.003** 0.001+ 0.001 (0.000) (0.001) (0.001) -0.171 (3.406) 0.340+ (0.191) 0.351+ (0.192) -0.268 (0.934) 0.503** -0.033 0.258 0.303 (0.181) (0.224) (0.179) (0.229) 0.051 0.057 0.043 0.040 (0.041) (0.052) (0.033) (0.034) 0.010* 0.010* 0.011** 0.012** (0.004) (0.005) (0.004) (0.004) 0.002 0.003** 0.000 0.000 (0.003) (0.001) (0.000) (0.001) Public South Midwest West 0.437** 0.454** 0.336 (0.153) (0.158) (1.315) -0.381 (0.654) 0.267 0.664** -0.036 -0.020 -0.032 (0.271) (0.212) (0.168) (0.169) (0.174) 0.042 -0.043 0.035 0.035 0.035 (0.036) (0.039) (0.028) (0.028) (0.028) 0.011** 0.010 0.004 0.006 0.005 (0.004) (0.007) (0.004) (0.004) (0.006) 0.000 0.003** 0.002** 0.002** 0.002 (0.001) (0.001) (0.000) (0.001) (0.001) 0.147 (0.155) 0.015 (0.027) 0.013** (0.004) 0.003** (0.000) -0.328* -0.324* -0.328* -0.047 (0.161) (0.164) (0.163) (0.182) -0.342 -0.332 -0.341 0.212 Puerto Rican Female 0.295 (1.189) 0.198** 0.198** 0.198** (0.044) (0.044) (0.044) 0.163** 0.163** 0.164** (0.052) (0.052) (0.051) -0.145 (0.091) -0.200** (0.067) -0.064 (0.081) 0.247** (0.095) 0.488** 0.488** 0.485** (0.138) (0.138) (0.155) -0.142 (0.126) -0.158* (0.081) -0.076 (0.093) 0.183 (0.121) 0.241+ (0.139) -0.037 (0.397) -0.290 (0.467) 0.075 (0.450) 0.636 (0.711) 0.241+ (0.139) 0.241+ (0.139) 0.241+ (0.137) 0.032 (0.221) 0.016 (0.313) 0.348 (0.336) 0.470 (0.362) 0.242+ (0.137) 0.241+ (0.137) 0.067 (0.274) -0.567** (0.202) 0.057 (0.286) 0.264 (0.193) Pseudo R2 0.13 0.13 0.12 0.12 0.15 0.15 0.10 0.10 0.14 0.14 Log pseudo-likelihood -2516.72 -2516.10 -4652.02 -1824.05 -1823.84 -3378.95 -226.73 -226.73 -384.60 -224.93 -224.89 -375.07 -220.53 -220.37 -460.40 ρ 0.12 0.17 0.27 0.03 0.06 Constant -2.244** -2.549** -2.415** -5.018** -2.448** -2.684** -2.716** -5.397** -2.302** -2.322* -2.605 -4.789** -1.534** -1.685* -1.560+ -4.426** -2.226** -2.512** -2.305* -4.427** Observations 4530 4530 4530 4530 3326 3326 3326 3326 386 386 386 386 361 361 361 361 457 457 457 457 Robust standard errors in parentheses + significant at 10%; * significant at 5%; ** significant at 1% a) The Pseudo R2 is the McFadden's R2, also known as the "likelihood rario index",compares the model with just the intercept to a model with all parameters (Long, 1997). Appendix Table B3: Estimates of 6-Year College Graduation: C&B 1989 Entry Cohort All students White Black Hispanic Asian Model A Model B Model A Model B Model A Model B Model A Model B Model A Model B Model C Model C Model C Model C Model C Bivariate Probit Probit Bivariate Probit Probit Bivariate Probit Probit Bivariate Probit Probit Bivariate Probit Probit Control Control Control Control Control Propensity Propensity Propensity Propensity Propensity Score (1) (2) Score (1) (2) Score (1) (2) Score (1) (2) Score (1) (2) Grad6 Selective Grad6 Selective Grad6 Selective Grad6 Selective Grad6 Selective Selective institution Est. propensity score Parents, B.A. Parental income Class rank - top 10% SAT Black Hispanic Asian Female Athlete Public South Midwest West Pseudo R2a Log pseudo-likelihood 0.347** 0.278* (0.097) (0.112) 0.381+ (0.197) 0.174** 0.172** (0.023) (0.024) 0.003** 0.003** (0.001) (0.001) 0.258** 0.238** (0.047) (0.049) 0.056** 0.026 (0.016) (0.021) -0.256** -0.318** (0.070) (0.076) -0.099 -0.154* (0.073) (0.073) 0.092 0.079 (0.080) (0.083) 0.108** 0.108** (0.038) (0.037) 0.684** (0.230) 0.169** (0.024) 0.003** (0.001) 0.231** (0.054) 0.019 (0.032) -0.330** (0.076) -0.164* (0.076) 0.077 (0.084) 0.106** (0.037) 0.381** 0.333** 0.642* (0.105) (0.121) (0.281) 0.269 (0.233) -0.021 0.179** 0.174** 0.172** (0.055) (0.034) (0.035) (0.036) 0.001 0.003** 0.003** 0.003** (0.001) (0.001) (0.001) (0.001) 0.340** 0.235** 0.224** 0.220** (0.117) (0.057) (0.060) (0.063) 0.464** 0.051** 0.030 0.023 (0.060) (0.015) (0.024) (0.036) 0.915** (0.156) 0.636** (0.158) 0.081 (0.119) 0.087* 0.087* 0.086* (0.043) (0.042) (0.042) 0.517 (0.425) 0.014 (0.095) 0.560* (0.258) -0.171 (0.243) 0.664* (0.286) 0.163+ (0.089) 0.108 (0.096) 0.496+ (0.275) 0.044 0.248** 0.251** (0.063) (0.054) (0.055) 0.002 0.003 0.003 (0.001) (0.002) (0.002) 0.300* 0.310** 0.261** (0.120) (0.068) (0.074) 0.481** 0.080** 0.042 (0.064) (0.024) (0.028) 0.481* (0.207) 0.339* (0.152) 0.208 (0.168) 0.754** (0.271) 0.246** -0.118 0.012 0.051 (0.053) (0.085) (0.089) (0.094) 0.003 0.000 0.005** 0.006** (0.002) (0.002) (0.002) (0.002) 0.270** 0.564** 0.293** 0.180 (0.075) (0.107) (0.099) (0.111) 0.050 0.361** 0.009 -0.053 (0.031) (0.076) (0.056) (0.059) 0.168** 0.175** 0.167** (0.053) (0.052) (0.053) 0.516 (0.431) 0.010 (0.107) 0.567* (0.263) -0.145 (0.236) 0.669* (0.289) 1.085** (0.319) 0.208 (0.181) 0.054 (0.201) 0.686* (0.272) 0.073 0.012 0.041 0.029 (0.086) (0.132) (0.132) (0.136) 0.006** -0.004 0.001 0.001 (0.002) (0.003) (0.002) (0.002) 0.122 0.457* 0.437** 0.378** (0.140) (0.196) (0.112) (0.099) -0.081 0.398** 0.126** 0.065 (0.065) (0.059) (0.038) (0.047) 0.210** 0.218** 0.201** (0.080) (0.079) (0.076) 0.525 (0.402) -0.080 (0.078) 0.427 (0.298) -0.300 (0.338) 0.734+ (0.402) 0.161 (0.098) 0.293 (0.429) 0.025 (0.118) 0.348 (0.322) -0.274 (0.341) 0.492 (0.407) 0.168+ (0.095) 0.778** (0.147) 0.023 (0.136) 0.001 (0.002) 0.349** (0.109) 0.045 (0.052) 0.010 (0.205) -0.003+ (0.002) 0.417* (0.199) 0.570** (0.096) 0.163+ (0.094) 0.510 (0.511) 0.104 (0.083) 0.625** (0.219) -0.195 (0.241) 0.657** (0.204) 0.05 0.05 0.04 0.04 0.05 0.05 0.04 0.05 0.07 0.08 -10359.1 -10344.5 -20984.3 -7951.6 -7946.2 -16228.1 -1185.1 -1183.4 -1931.9 -521.1 -517.8 -1028.9 -674.9 -666.9 -1649.0 ρ -0.25 -0.19 -0.24 † -0.56 * -0.43 ** Constant -0.044 0.284 0.366 -7.148** 0.009 0.245 0.324 -7.432** -0.591* -0.278 -0.339 -4.903** 0.308 0.839 1.089 -5.573** -0.737 -0.093 0.134 -8.575** Observations 29018 29018 29018 29018 23086 23086 23086 23086 2260 2260 2260 2260 1235 1235 1235 1235 2437 2424 2437 2437 Robust standard errors in parentheses + significant at 10%; * significant at 5%; ** significant at 1% a) The Pseudo R2 is the McFadden's R2, also known as the "likelihood rario index",compares the model with just the intercept to a model with all parameters (Long, 1997).