Educational inequalities in childhood Luke Sibieta, Institute for Fiscal Studies, luke_s@ifs.org.ukss.org.u (joint work edited by Alissa Goodman and Paul Gregg, other contributors from IFS and CMPO) (also based on a review undertaken by Paul Howard-Jones and Liz Washbrook as part for Centre for Understanding Behaviour Change for DfE ) k © Institute for Fiscal Studies Background and Motivation • Children growing up in poor families end up with lower educational attainment than children growing up in rich families • Strong contributor to patterns of social mobility – Low income poor attainment low income • Gaps start very early in life, but tend to widen throughout school © Institute for Fiscal Studies Outline • Perspectives on early intervention • Chart socio-economic gradient in attainment across childhood • Investigate contribution of parent and child behaviours, attitudes to education and aspirations to the evolution of this gradient: – Early years: home learning environments and parenting styles – Primary school: maternal aspirations, child’s own ability beliefs – Teenage years: YP’s own attitudes and behaviours; material resources – Intergenerational factors: parents’ and grandparents’ attitudes; transmission of ability • Summarise and assess implications for policy Perspectives on early intervention (1) • Models developed by James Heckman suggest that intervention in the early years has highest return for policymakers • Wide body of empirical evidence points to importance of early investment – e.g. Perry Pre-School Programme and Nurse-Family – Many later interventions have also been shown to be less effective. • Neuroscience suggests first years of life are important period for neural and cognitive organisation and could be “foundational”. • Normal brain development is discontinuous in some important respects, and this may inform the types of provision provided for different age groups in the future. © Institute for Fiscal Studies Perspectives on early intervention (2) • But, no evidence from neuroscience for a “critical period” of learning and development from 0-3 • Human capital models suggest early investment must be followed up in order to be effective. • Some later interventions may well prove effective, e.g. private job training, mentoring and increasing school choice. • Optimal investment should be tailored to specifics that create adversity and to the likely productivity of investment © Institute for Fiscal Studies Preview of findings from analysis • The gaps between rich and poor children is already large at age 3 continues to widen until age 14 • The following factors seem to have an important role in explaining the perpetuation of these gaps: – – – – – Early home learning environment Expectations/ aspirations for education Beliefs in own actions making a difference Behaviour Material factors • Suggests a potentially important role for policy if it can be shown that: – More positive attitudes and behaviours cause higher attainment AND – Attitudes and behaviours can be influenced Results: by Socio-Economic Quintile Widens through primary Gap at age 3 Widens by age 5 © Institute for Fiscal Studies (Bristol sample not nationally comparable) Widens to 14 ‘Narrows’ again to 16 Explaining the socio-economic gradient in the early years • Define set of family background and possible transmission mechanisms (“early childhood caring environment”) • Family background – Socioeconomic position (SEP) – Parental education – Demographic, and other family background • Early childhood caring environment – Family Interactions (mother-chid and between parents) – Health and Well-being (birth-weight, gestation, post-natal depression) – Childcare usage – Home-learning environment (reading, ABCs, numbers, nursery rhymes) – Parenting Style/Rules (bed-times, meal-times) © Institute for Fiscal Studies 32.2 35 30 2 Number of siblings Mother's age at birth Selected differences in characteristics at age 3 & 5 25.0 25 20 15 10 5 0 1.6 1.5 1.1 1 0.5 0 Mother's age at birth Number of siblings at age 5 100% 92% 79% Per cent 80% 98% 85% 68% 60% 42% 40% 22% 14% 20% 0% Highest HLE Quintile at age 3 © Institute for Fiscal Studies Read to everyday at age 3 Regular bed times at age 3 Regular meal times at age 3 How much of the socio-economic gap in cognitive outcomes at age 3 is explained by these factors? 0% 1% Residual Gap Parental Education 16% Family Background/Demographics 1% 34% 3% Family Interactions 4% Health and Well-Being Childcare Home-Learning Environment 25% 16% Parenting Style/Rules Missing Data Total gap to be explained: 23 percentile points © Institute for Fiscal Studies How much of the socio-economic gap in cognitive outcomes at age 5 is explained by these factors? • Gap widens at age 5 (from 23 to 27 percentile points) • Half of the gap is explained by prior cognitive ability • Less than 1% from the early childhood caring environment • What role for the Home-Learning Environment? – HLE at age 3 explains age 5 cognitive outcomes through its impact on age 3 cognitive outcomes – No independent impact of age 5 HLE on age 5 cognitive outcomes • Overall, results suggest policies to improve parenting skills and home learning environments in isolation cannot possibly eliminate the cognitive skills gap between rich and poor young children. © Institute for Fiscal Studies Gaps in educational attainment in the primary school years • Average percentile score gap between richest and poorest quintiles widens between KS1 and KS2 • Parenting activities and family interactions may continue to matter, but new potential mechanisms come into play as children age: – Parents’ values, beliefs and aspirations for their children 81% of the highest SEP mothers hope their child will go to university, compared with 37% of the lowest SEP mothers – Children’s own values and beliefs 67% of the highest SEP children believe school results are important in life, compared with 51% of the lowest SEP children – Children’s activities and patterns of behaviour – Experience of schooling © Institute for Fiscal Studies How much of the socio-economic gap at age 11 is explained by these factors? -1% Residual gap 14% 14% 3% 29% 20% 19% Total gap to be explained: 31 percentile points © Institute for Fiscal Studies Parental education and family background Child attitudes and behaviours Parental attitudes and behaviours Pre-school environments Schools Missings How much of the socio-economic gap at age 11 is explained by these factors net of prior ability? Residual Gap 7% 13% 4% 6% 63% 6% 0% -1% Parental Education and Family Background Child's attitudes and behaviours Parent's attitudes and behaviours Pre-school environments Schools Missing Data Prior Ability Total gap to be explained: 31 percentile points © Institute for Fiscal Studies Summary of primary years study • Poor children are much more likely to begin primary school behind their better-off peers, but also fall further behind by age 11 • Important factors in explaining the gap include: – Mother’s hopes that the child will go to university – The belief that one’s own actions can make a difference (among both parents and children) – Socio-emotional difficulties such as inattention and conduct problems • Explain some of the widening of the gap, to a lesser degree Outcomes in the secondary school years: evidence from the LSYPE • Now focus on educational attainment at 16 • See if gap can be explained by the following characteristics: – Prior attainment (at 11 and 14) – Parental education and family background – School characteristics – Children’s attitudes and behaviours • Beliefs and values about being at school • Aspirations/expectations towards future education • Behavioural problems – Parental attitudes and behaviours • Provision of educational material resources • Aspirations/expectations about child’s future education • Home relations and educational interactions with the child © Institute for Fiscal Studies How much of the socio-economic gap in cognitive outcomes at age 16 is explained by these factors? Residual Gap 7% 6% 15% 59% 8% 1% 4% Total gap to be explained: 33 percentile points © Institute for Fiscal Studies Parental Education and Family Background Child's attitudes and behaviours Parent's attitudes and behaviours Schools Missing Data Prior Ability Summary of secondary years analysis • Attainment gap at 16 a continuation of earlier gaps • But might be reduced if poorest children: – Have access to computer/internet – Avoid problematic/risky behaviour (in and outside school) – Expect to go to HE, or have parents who expect them to go – Believe that they do well in school • What role for policies to raise education aspirations? – Aspirations are strongly associated with educational attainment – Poorest children have lower expectation of going to HE than rich children, even after taking into account prior attainment – Suggests an ‘aspirations deficit’ that ought to be alleviated © Institute for Fiscal Studies Summary of secondary years analysis • However: – HE expectations are already very high across all SEP groups – Poor children most likely to over-estimate chances of going to HE 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 78% 51% 50% 52% 37% 29% 13% Poorest 20% 2 Expectations at age 14 © Institute for Fiscal Studies 64% 58% 3 4 Reality at age 18/19 Richest An intergenerational picture • British Cohort Study (BCS): everyone born in Great Britain in one week in April 1970 interviewed every few years. • In age-34 wave, half those who had children were randomly selected for parent-and-child questionnaires and children took cognitive tests (BAS). • So we have: • Info about the environment children are growing up in. • Their cognitive test scores. • Info about the cognitive ability, social skills and attitudes of their parents when they were children. © Institute for Fiscal Studies The SEP gap in cognitive test scores: adding new information about parents Other As and Bs 8% Young Person's Behaviours 10% Residual gap 16% CM's gender 0% Parental Education 9% © Institute for Fiscal Studies Parental Cognitive Ability 16% Social Skills 11% Other As and Bs 4% Educational Attitudes and Aspirations 23% Homelearning Environment 4% Residual gap 6% CM's gender Parental 4% Attitudes and Parental Social Skills Education 8% 5% Social Skills 14% Family Background 16% Family Background 12% Young Person's Behaviours 10% Educational Attitudes and Aspirations 20% Home-learning Environment 4% Summary of intergenerational conclusions • Adding usually unobserved information about parents is important. – Predicts about ¼ of SEP gap in cognitive skills. • Mainly due to parental cognitive ability. • But reassuringly it does little to change our impression of relative (predictive) importance of other factors. • Attitudes and aspirations towards education, family background, socio-emotional development still important. © Institute for Fiscal Studies Overall conclusions • Socio-economic gap in attainment could be reduced by improving attitudes and behaviours amongst poor children – Optimistic take would suggest 25% reduction in GCSE attainment gap • • But not a causal analysis. More robust evidence needed to establish that: – Attitudes and behaviours can be changed – Such changes cause improvements in attainment Our work suggests that trials may be best focused on: – Raising educational aspirations and expectations (for both parents and children) – and at an earlier stage than e.g. Aim Higher. – Supporting the home-learning environment (e.g. pre-school reading). – Helping parents and children to believe that their own actions and efforts can help to improve attainment (locus of control). © Institute for Fiscal Studies